Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

The Prefrontal Cortex
The Prefrontal Cortex
The Prefrontal Cortex
Ebook1,178 pages20 hours

The Prefrontal Cortex

Rating: 3 out of 5 stars

3/5

()

Read preview

About this ebook

The Prefrontal Cortex, Fifth Edition, provides users with a thoroughly updated version of this comprehensive work that has historically served as the classic reference on this part of the brain.

The book offers a unifying, interdisciplinary perspective that is lacking in other volumes written about the frontal lobes, and is, once again, written by the award-winning author who discovered "memory cells," the physiological substrate of working memory.

The fifth edition constitutes a comprehensive update, including all the major advances made on the physiology and cognitive neuroscience of the region since publication in 2008.

All chapters have been fully revised, and the overview of prefrontal functions now interprets experimental data within the theoretical framework of the new paradigm of cortical structure and dynamics (the Cognit Paradigm), addressing the accompanying social, economic, and cultural implications.

  • Provides a distinctly interdisciplinary view of the prefrontal cortex, covering all major methodologies, from comparative anatomy to modern imaging
  • Unique analysis and synthesis of a large body of basic and clinical data on the subject (more than 2000 references)
  • Written by an award-winning author who discovered "memory cells," the physiological substrate of working memory
  • Synthesizes evidence that the prefrontal cortex constitutes a complex pre-adaptive system
  • Incorporates emerging study of the role of the frontal lobes in social, economic, and cultural adaptation
LanguageEnglish
Release dateMay 22, 2015
ISBN9780124080607
The Prefrontal Cortex
Author

Joaquin Fuster

Dr. Joaquin M. Fuster was born in Barcelona, Spain, in 1930. Studied medicine at the University of Barcelona. In Barcelona and Innsbruck (Austria), he specialized in psychiatry. In 1957 Fuster emigrated to the United States for a career in neuroscience at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA). In 1962-64, he worked as a visiting scientist at the Max-Planck Institute for Psychiatry in Munich. He received his PhD. in neuroscience at the University of Granada, Spain. Dr. Fuster is Professor Emeritus of Psychiatry and a member of the Brain Research Institute and the Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior at the UCLA's School of Medicine. Dr. Fuster's major honors and awards include: the title of Member of Honor of the Spanish Royal Academy of Medicine (1997); Signoret Prize (Université de La Sorbonne, Paris) (2000); Fyssen International Science Prize (2000); Doctor Honoris Causa, Universidad Miguel Hernández, Alicante, Spain (2003); Goldman-Rakic Prize for Cognitive Neuroscience (NARSAD) (2006); George Miller Prize of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society (2006); Doctor Honoris Causa, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (2008); Geschwind Lecturer, Harvard University (2009); Woolsey Lecturer, University of Wisconsin (2010); Elected Member, American Academy of Arts and Sciences (2010); Segerfalk Lecturer, University of Lund, Sweden (2010); Doctor Honoris Causa, Universidad Francisco Marroquín, Guatemala (2014). Dr. Fuster is the author of more than 200 articles and 8 books.

Related to The Prefrontal Cortex

Related ebooks

Biology For You

View More

Related articles

Related categories

Reviews for The Prefrontal Cortex

Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
3/5

1 rating1 review

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    I appreciate the way this book is organized, although I would not necessarily suggest reading it linearly.

Book preview

The Prefrontal Cortex - Joaquin Fuster

The Prefrontal Cortex

Fifth Edition

Joaquín M. Fuster, MD, PhD

Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California

Table of Contents

Cover image

Title page

Front-matter

Copyright

Dedication

Preface

References

Chapter 1. Introduction

I Introduction

References

Chapter 2. Anatomy of the Prefrontal Cortex

I Introduction

II Evolution and Comparative Anatomy

III Development

IV Microscopic Architecture

V Aging

VI Connections

VII Summary

References

Chapter 3. Chemical Neurotransmission

I Introduction

II Development and Aging

III Transmitters in the Prefrontal Cortex

IV Neuropsychiatric Implications

V Summary

References

Chapter 4. Animal Neuropsychology

I Introduction

II Historical Background

III Motility

IV Emotional Behavior

V Cognitive Function

VI Reversible Lesions

VII Development

VIII Aging

IX Summary

References

Chapter 5. Human Neuropsychology

I Introduction

II Historical Background

III Affect, Emotion, and Social Behavior

IV Executive Function

V Language

VI Intelligence

VII Prefrontal Syndromes

VIII Development

IX Aging

X Summary

References

Chapter 6. Neurophysiology

I Introduction

II Historical Background

III Sensory Function

IV Motor Function

V Visceral and Emotional Functions

VI Value and Reward

VII Executive Functions

VIII Summary

References

Chapter 7. Neuroimaging

I Introduction

II Value and Limitations of Imaging

III Imaging Prefrontal Functions In Cognition

IV Prefrontal Imaging in Neuropsychiatric Illness

V Summary

References

Chapter 8. Overview of Prefrontal Functions: E Pluribus Unum – Coordinating New Sequences of Purposeful Action

I Conceptual Introduction

II Hierarchical Organization of Cognitive Networks (Cognits)

III Frontal Action Domains

IV Executive Functions

V Emotional Functions

VI Temporal Organization of Action

VII Prediction and Novelty in the Human Brain

VIII Consciousness and Free Will

IX Summary

References

Index

Front-matter

"Joaquín Fuster has again done the field an invaluable service by synthesizing current scientific knowledge in the new edition of his classic book, The Prefrontal Cortex. Fuster’s splendid book has been an essential reference for neuroscientists since its initial publication in 1980, elucidating the organization and function of the brain region that most makes us human. The new 5th edition has been updated with a wealth of new material from neurophysiology and neuroimaging on the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex, synthesized under the notion that the driving function of prefrontal cortex is to coordinate new sequences of purposeful action. It is a masterful accomplishment."

William Newsome, Director, Stanford Neurosciences Institute, and Harman Provostial Professor of Neurobiology Stanford University, USA.

Joaquín Fuster has produced an extraordinarily insightful compendium of information about how the frontal cortex lets us develop new plans and sequences of thought and action— the heart of the behavior that makes us ourselves. Fuster makes us realize how important time is to the organization of our behavior, and that our mental and physical actions are guided by brain networks that have the frontal cortex as the master hub. Fuster gives us a rich and deep view of how our frontal cortex guides our mental life and lets us develop action sequences over time.

Ann Graybiel, Professor, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA.

"The Prefrontal Cortex is not only a classic, but also an essential book for anyone interested in higher brain function. One might think there’s nothing left to say but Fuster proves that wrong. The latest edition is fresh and vibrant, and itself essential, destined to be a classic in its own right."

Joseph LeDoux, University Professor, New York University, USA, and author of The Emotional Brain and Synaptic Self.

Once again, Dr. Fuster, the world’s pre-eminent expert on the frontal lobes, has delivered an instant classic that should be read by everyone interested in understanding the link between brain and behavior.

Mark D’Esposito, Director, Henry H. Wheeler Jr. Brain Imaging Center, Professor of Neuroscience, University of California, Berkeley, USA.

A classic that has graced the library of many of us since its first edition appeared 35 years ago now returns, splendidly updated. As a result, this book will continue to be the go-to reference on the prefrontal cortex as we strive to understand more fully its critical role in brain function.

Marcus E. Raichle, Professor of Radiology and Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, USA.

The frontal lobe serves the highest cognitive functions of the brain, including symbolic representation of the world, decision making, and planning for the future. Arguably, the enormous development of these functions distinguishes the human from other species. Joaquín Fuster has devoted his life to studying the many complex roles of the frontal cortex in behavior and cognition. This book is the product of his efforts to make these issues comprehensible in an exciting and fast-growing field. Even if you possess earlier editions of his book you should have this one to stay informed about the brain structure that makes us human. For that, this vastly updated edition is a must-have, whether you are a specialist or not.

Pasko Rakic, Chairman, Department of Neurobiology, and Director, Kavli Institute for Neuroscience, Yale Medical School, USA.

"The Prefrontal Cortex is a classic, and the classic has just been updated, expanded and thought anew, with the depth and wisdom that characterize Fuster’s work. As before, this is an indispensable volume for neuroscientists."

Antonio Damasio, Dornsife Professor of Neuroscience and Director, Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, USA.

The frontal lobes are central to cognitive neuroscience. The revision of this important volume provides the crucial background needed to grasp their role. In the final chapter Fuster brings together all that is known by emphasizing the temporal course of brain networks in a way which serves to illuminate action, consciousness and free will.

Michael I. Posner, Professor, Department of Psychology, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, USA.

Joaquín Fuster is one of the leading scientists in the field of cognitive neuroscience. He is famous not only for his discovery of memory cells" in the frontal lobe of the monkey, but also for his excellent books. Among them the most famous and influential is The Prefrontal Cortex. I remember the first edition of it back in 1980. It was a mere intellectual pleasure reading it. The book reviewed an amazing amount of data from anatomy, ontogeny and physiology to the effect of lesions on innate and conditioned behavior and synthesized them in a coherent theory. In this new completely re-written edition of the book, Fuster has been able to repeat the enterprise. In spite of the enormous amount of new data, many coming from the rather messy field of brain imaging, he has been able to review them and put them in a clear theoretical frame. I am sure the new generation of neuroscientists will be influenced by this book in the same way as I was more than 20 years ago and will receive, by reading it, the same intellectual pleasure."

Giacomo Rizzolatti, Professor of Human Physiology, Department of Neuroscience, Section of Physiology, University of Parma, Italy.

This is a superb, fresh, in-depth, review of one of the most complex and fascinating topics in cognitive neuroscience. In this, the latest edition of his excellent book, Fuster further substantiates the general proposition that the prefrontal cortex serves the organization of goal-directed actions in the most human of all action domains. Based on a wealth of recent empirical evidence, he places the temporal integrative functions of the frontal lobe – including working memory and planning – at the summit of the perception-action cycle. There, with access to myriad signals from the internal and external milieus, the prefrontal cortex attends to the orderly pursuit of rational, linguistic, and social goals. It is a must read for all interested in neuroscience.

Richard Thompson, William M. Keck Chair in Biological Sciences and Professor of Psychology and Biological Sciences, University of Southern California, USA.

Copyright

Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier

32 Jamestown Road, London NW1 7BY, UK

525 B Street, Suite 1800, San Diego, CA 92101-4495, USA

225 Wyman Street, Waltham, MA 02451, USA

The Boulevard, Langford Lane, Kidlington, Oxford OX5 1GB, UK

Fifth edition 2015

Copyright © 2015, 2008, 1997, 1989, 1980 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Details on how to seek permission, further information about the Publisher’s permissions policies and our arrangements with organizations such as the Copyright Clearance Center and the Copyright Licensing Agency, can be found at our website: www.elsevier.com/permissions.

This book and the individual contributions contained in it are protected under copyright by the Publisher (other than as may be noted herein).

Notices

Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experience broaden our understanding, changes in research methods, professional practices, or medical treatment may become necessary.

Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge in evaluating and using any information, methods, compounds, or experiments described herein. In using such information or methods they should be mindful of their own safety and the safety of others, including parties for whom they have a professional responsibility.

To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or editors, assume any liability for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of any methods, products, instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein.

ISBN: 978-0-12-407815-4

British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data

A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress.

For Information on all Academic Press publications visit our website at http://store.elsevier.com/

Typeset by MPS Limited, Chennai, India www.adi-mps.com

Printed and bound in the United Kingdom

Dedication

To the memory of my father – physician, educator, historian, and man of infallible common sense

Preface

Joaquín M. Fuster, Los Angeles, California

Since 2007, when the previous edition of this book went to the printer, the electronic database of PubMed has accumulated more than 14,000 additional articles related in one way or another to the book’s subject matter. To bring the book up to date, I had to review much of that massive material, although I did it with a critical discriminating eye, attempting to highlight the substantive new knowledge. I also made use of recently published reviews by others. Naturally, the interpretation of new data may have been subject to my own theoretical biases. Indeed, Friedrich Hayek is reputed to have wisely said that, without a theory the facts are silent. But, of course, theories are based on facts, which can oblige us at any time to change or discard an outdated theory. This is not always easy, especially when the interpretations of facts contradict one another or a faulty theory is deeply entrenched in collective thinking. In any case, as a result of my effort I had to modify some of my previous ideas.

Since the last edition, what is new in the field of the prefrontal cortex seems to be novelty itself, or at least a renewed emphasis on it and on the future orientation of prefrontal functions. There is a growing recognition that the cardinal function of this part of the brain, which is the latest to develop in evolution and in ontogeny, is the design and implementation of novel, complex, goal-directed or purposeful actions. In other words, we are dealing primarily with a prospective function of creating new forms of action. This is especially true for the cortex of the lateral convexity of the frontal lobe, which is the one to have developed to a maximum in the human brain.

In previous editions, novelty of action was couched in the more general concept of a prefrontal role in the temporal organization of all modes of behavioral action. The latest studies, however, oblige us to place the emphasis on what for the organism is not only new behavior, but also new perception, new language, and new reasoning. By so doing, we attribute to the prefrontal cortex imagination in addition to predictive and creative capacity. Indeed, it appears that this cortex opens the brain to the future, giving it the ability both to predict and to invent that future. If the human brain is the ultimate adaptive system to emerge from evolution, the human prefrontal cortex, which is the latest structure to evolve within it, gives it the power to preadapt the organism to its environment and to prepare it for future adaptive actions.

That environment is not only external, made of the world that surrounds us, but internal as well, consisting of what has been called the internal milieu. The latter is the aggregate of biological conditions of the body served by biodrives (hunger, sex, avoidance of pain, and others); these drives ensure internal chemical equilibrium or homeostasis, physical pleasure, defense, survival, and procreation. All biodrives are led or accompanied by emotion and closely intertwined with social behavior. A large body of recent evidence implicates further the prefrontal cortex, especially its internal or medial and inferior (orbital) aspects, in emotion and social behavior, thus complementing and expanding much of the evidence previously inferred from clinical and neuropsychological observations.

Because of its key position in making us agents of free choice, planning, and decision-making, the prefrontal cortex has lately entered the debate on issues of free will and ethical responsibility. In this latest edition these issues had to be dealt with somewhat more extensively than in previous ones, although I have treated them separately in another text (Fuster, 2013).

Animals possess intelligence, working memory, perception, attention, and practically every other cognitive function known to humans. It is by studying the prefrontal cortex of animals that we have come to understand some of the principles of operation of these functions in the human prefrontal cortex as well as the cortex at large. For example, we would know next to nothing about the mechanisms of working memory and the role of the prefrontal cortex in it had it not been for the research of these matters in the non-human primate. Meanwhile, however, we have been neglecting the fact that working memory is a prospective function like all other so-called executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. It has a future dimension that is part of its definition: working memory is short-term memory for a prospective action.

Language is a uniquely human form of communication and behavior. It is also a vehicle of cognitive expression as well as social and emotional interaction. All novel and rich spoken language is a most complex form of organized action. On these grounds alone, the prefrontal cortex plays a critical role in language. The latest data, mainly from neuroimaging studies, point to a dual basis for that role. One is the capacity of the prefrontal cortex to predict (from Latin praedicere, to foretell), and thus to make new proposals, to propositionise, as John Hughlings Jackson (1958) called it; language serves the formulation of new plans of future action, a basic prefrontal function. The other is the temporally organized nature of language; like all forms of organized goal-directed action, it depends critically on the prefrontal cortex, especially if the language is novel and elaborate. A plausible argument can be made for considering the syntax of language a special case of the syntax of action, and as such, dependent on the lateral prefrontal cortex. For that syntax, working memory is essential.

There are two fundamental principles in the previous edition that the present one not only upholds but also strengthens. One is the intimate hierarchical cooperation of the prefrontal cortex with other cortical and subcortical regions of the brain in the structuring of behavior, reasoning, and language. The other is the controlling position of the prefrontal cortex at the summit of the perceptionaction cycle, the cybernetic loop of information processing between the cortex at large and the environment, which adapts and preadapts the organism to that environment. What needed emphasis before, and now receives it, is that much of that environment is internalized in the cortex, in the form of widely distributed cognitive networks or cognits that represent the memories, knowledge, and culture of the individual. All of that, forming part of the perception–action cycle, has been acquired by prior experience in the course of life and is ready for recall to be engaged in that cycle at any new round of adaptation.

Now, more than in 2007, the prefrontal cortex is penetrating our clinical reasoning and agenda. A prefrontal disorder is implicated in several pathological conditions with psychiatric manifestations, ranging from the attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder of childhood to drug addiction, obsessive–compulsive disorder, autism, schizophrenia, depression, and dementia. Rarely is the prefrontal cortex disturbed alone in any of these conditions, which usually also affect other brain structures and several neurochemical systems. Furthermore, some of these conditions are subject to genetic factors, the influence of which is likely to transcend the prefrontal structure or functions. There is no doubt, however, that all of them manifest as cognitive, social, or emotional disorders that are squarely attributable to prefrontal dysfunction.

This foreword cannot come to a close without my recognition of the help that many fellow scientists have extended to me in the writing of the various editions of this book, including the present one: Amy Arnsten, Lewis Baxter, Susan Bookheimer, Carmen Cavada, Norman Geschwind, Patricia Goldman-Rakic, Patricia Greenfield, Eric Kandel, David Lewis, Donald Lindsley, James Marsh, John Mazziotta, Mortimer Mishkin, Walle Nauta, Carlos Otero, Karl Pribram, Javier Quintana, Donald Stuss, and John Warren. To all of them, I am deeply indebted. I also owe special thanks to Carmen Cox, who assisted me in assembling an exceptionally copious bibliography.

December 2014

References

1. Fuster JM. The Neuroscience of Freedom and Creativity: Our Predictive Brain Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2013.

2. Jackson JH. Selected Writings New York: Basic Books; 1958; pp. 155–204.

Chapter 1

Introduction

This chapter introduces the main subjects of the book. The essential function of the prefrontal cortex is the coordinated execution of new forms of organized goal-directed action. The chapter highlights the hierarchical organization of the cortex. At the summit of that organization, the prefrontal cortex controls the perception–action cycle, a complex system of cognitive and emotional operations that adapts the organism to its environment. At that high cortical level the system becomes future oriented. In the pursuit of future objectives, the prefrontal cortex, in cooperation with other brain structures, enters the cycle with its prospective functions of planning, attentional set, working memory, and decision-making. These functions make the prefrontal cortex a proactive, predictive, creative, and preadaptive organ.

Keywords

cortical hierarchy; goal-directed action; perception–action cycle; preadaptation; prediction; prospective prefrontal function; temporal organization

Outline

I. Introduction 1

References 8

I Introduction

The prefrontal cortex is the cortex of the anterior pole of the mammalian brain. In characterizing the anterior part of the frontal lobe with the adjective prefrontal, we make loose, if not improper, use of the prefix pre (literally we place that cortex in midair!). Nevertheless, that designation has been condoned by so much usage that it seems unwarranted to discard it for semantic reasons.

Here, the prefrontal cortex is defined as the part of the cerebral cortex that receives projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. This anatomical definition is applicable to all mammalian brains. It takes into consideration the possibility that the relationship with a well-defined thalamic nucleus reflects an identifiable function or group of functions. Of course, such reasoning is based on analogy with specific thalamic nuclei and their cortical projection areas, an analogy that may not be entirely appropriate. Furthermore, the functions of the mediodorsal nucleus are not well known, and the prefrontal cortex is also connected to many other cerebral structures.

On the other hand, the definition by relationship has here the merit of obeying the reasonable principle that the physiology of a cortical region can be meaningfully studied and understood only in the context of its anatomical connections with other structures (Creutzfeldt, 1977). In this respect, however, the connectivity of the prefrontal cortex with other parts of the cortex may be more important than its thalamic connectivity.

The basic function of the prefrontal cortex is the representation and execution of new forms of organized goal-directed action. All the so-called executive functions of the prefrontal cortex serve that superordinate function in one way or another.

The goals of an organism, especially the human organism, can vary immensely, and so do the timescale and means to achieve them. Also variable are the motives for action and the emotions that accompany it, as well as their influence at any step in the pursuit of a goal. Depending on these factors, each of the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex may be brought into play at one time or another. In the human and non-human primate, as in other animals, each one of these functions has a dominant if not specific regional location in the prefrontal cortex. In any event, the diversity of regional commitments, as well as of the connectivity of different prefrontal areas, has to be analyzed in depth, because therein lies the key to their function. We shall never understand fully the functions of the prefrontal cortex if we neglect the operations of its components.

At the same time, we must keep in mind the wider structural and dynamic context in which those operations take place. This context is defined by two basic biological principles that set the background for any discussion of this cortex. One is the evolutionary hierarchy of cortical and subcortical structures dedicated to the organization of goal-directed actions. The other is the perception–action cycle; that is, the cybernetic circulation of information processing that governs the interactions of the organism with its environment. Both principles are structurally and dynamically intertwined.

The first, the hierarchical vision of the neuroscience of action, has its origin in the writings of John Hughlings Jackson (1958), a scholarly physician who practiced neurology in London’s Queen Square Hospital at the end of the nineteenth and beginning of the twentieth century. Based on his studies of motor functions and their disorders, Jackson advanced the idea that the structures of the central nervous system, its motor structures in particular, were hierarchically organized in the order determined by evolution: structures representing and coordinating simple movements at the bottom (basal ganglia, pyramidal system, cerebellum), and those representing and coordinating new complex behavior at the top (prefrontal and premotor cortices). As clinical observations clearly demonstrate, lesions at a particular level of the motor hierarchy lead to paralyses of movements organized at that level and, at the same time, to the release of simpler, automatic movements from lower levels of the hierarchy. Jackson characterized such a pathological disorder as dissolution, a term he coined as an opposite to evolution, since the disorder indicated that upon failure of higher levels of the hierarchy, the nervous system regressed to the performance of movements that are more primitive from an evolutionary point of view.

An evident corollary of Jackson’s theory, which he drew quite early (Jackson, 1882), is that the evolutionarily lower structures and their functions are nested under the higher structures and functions, which they normally serve. When the higher ones fail, the lower are released from their control. This is true in all cortical hierarchies of action, but is most obvious in the hierarchy of areas dedicated to the phylogenetically most advanced cognitive activity, the spoken language. At the lowest cortical level of the speech hierarchy is the sensorimotor cortex, which controls the representation and articulation of simple speech utterances. By high-resolution methods in humans, precise temporal patterns can be recorded in that cortex that correspond to the successive activations of oral and laryngeal muscles during the articulation of vowels and consonants (Bouchard et al., 2013). Words are made of phonemes and morphemes, which are organized into sentences in hierarchically higher cortical regions, such as the premotor cortex and Broca’s area.

As a result of his clinical research, Jackson reserved for the prefrontal cortex the representation and organization of what he called propositional language. It should not escape us that, in logic and linguistics, the term propositional implies novelty, complexity, and even a future dimension – which all proposals have. At the same time, these are the characteristics that make language a uniquely human activity (Berwick et al., 2013) and place its most novel and complex aspects in the prefrontal cortex, at the summit of the evolutionary hierarchy of neural structures for action.

The concept of the perception–action cycle also has a deep root in biology. Prefrontal areas, networks, and functions are not simply interdependent; they are cooperative. The temporal organization of complex and novel actions toward their goal is the product of the dynamics of the perception–action cycle, which consists of the coordinated participation of neural structures in the successive interactions of the organism with its environment in the pursuit of a goal. Thus, the perception–action cycle is the cortical substrate for the processing of information between the organism and its environment; the prefrontal cortex constitutes the highest stage of neural integration in that cycle. In the course of a goal-directed sequence of actions, signals from the internal milieu and the external environment are processed through hierarchically organized neural channels and lead into the prefrontal cortex (internal signals into orbitomedial, external signals into lateral prefrontal cortex). There, the signals generate or modulate further action, which in turn causes changes in the internal and external environments, changes which enter the processing cycle toward further action, and so on until the goal is reached. At each hierarchical level of the cycle, there is feedback to prior levels. At the highest level, there is re-entrant feedback from the prefrontal cortex to the posterior association cortex, which plays a critical role in working memory, set, and monitoring.

Those two general concepts, hierarchy and the perception–action cycle, mark the theoretical backdrop in the cerebral cortex at large against which the functions of the prefrontal cortex must be viewed. Both cortices, posterior and frontal, are hierarchically organized. Whereas the posterior cortex is devoted to perceptual and mnemonic functions, the entirety of the frontal cortex, including its prefrontal region, is devoted to action of one kind or another, whether it is skeletal movement, ocular movement, the expression of emotion, speech, or visceral control. The action can even be mental and internal, such as reasoning. The frontal cortex is therefore doer cortex, much as the posterior cortex is sensor cortex (both reflecting up in the cortex the polarity of functions existing in the anterior and posterior horns of the spinal cord). In sum, the posterior cortex and the frontal cortex constitute the cortical infrastructure for the perception–action cycle.

The frontal cortex does nothing by itself. It works in the perception–action cycle with other cortices, with subcortical structures, and with certain sectors of the sensory and motor apparatus and of the autonomic system. There is, however, considerable specialization of action within it. Accordingly, there are frontal areas for eye movement, for skeletal movement of various body parts, for speech, for emotional expression, and so on. More importantly in what concerns us here, the specialized areas within the prefrontal cortex, whatever the action domain they represent, contribute their share to the common cognitive and emotional functions that drive the neocortex as a whole. Those functions are essentially integrative and goal directed. They are also, as we will see, new for the organism; they are new as that organism has to meet new circumstances, now or in the future, and has to adapt to them. In that sense, the prefrontal cortex is not only adaptive, but also preadaptive.

As organisms evolve, their actions become more complex and idiosyncratic, their goals more remote in space and time, and their reasons or motives for attaining them less transparent, more based on probability and prior experience than on peremptory instinctual need. Furthermore, action in general becomes more deliberate and voluntary. With this evolution of biological action, and presumably because of it, the most anterior sector of the frontal cortex, which we call the prefrontal cortex, grows substantially – in relative size – as evolution progresses, and so does its functional role. Its growth reaches a maximum in the human primate. The prefrontal cortex of the lateral or outer frontal convexity, which is essential for cognitive functions and intelligent behavior, undergoes greater development than that of the medial and inferior (orbital) surfaces, which are critically involved in emotional behavior. Although their functions are interdependent and integrated in the behavior of the organism, lateral and orbitomedial cortices require somewhat different methodologies for their study.

In this book, we shall examine the prefrontal cortex by systematically reviewing data from each of the contributing methodologies. As we proceed from the basic facts of anatomy to neuropsychology, to neurophysiology, and to neuroimaging, my own conceptual point of view will become progressively more explicit. This introduction outlines it in broad strokes and the last chapter describes it in detail.

In the 33 years since the first edition of this book, my theoretical position on the prefrontal cortex has changed considerably as new facts have demanded it, but some of the basic elements of my initial view are still valid. To begin with, there is now wide agreement with the concept I held then, that the lateral prefrontal cortex is critical for the cognitive functions that mediate the temporal organization of actions. These functions include planning, decision-making, and top–down attention, the latter with its three subcomponents of working memory, set, and inhibitory control. Another surviving view is that the limbic, dysgranular, cingular, medial, and orbital areas of the prefrontal cortex, while also involved in those functions, modulate their emotional and affective components. Those areas can even initiate goal-directed actions in the emotional perception–action cycle, which runs parallel to the cognitive one and interacts with it – in orbital prefrontal cortex. We also know, as we knew then, that two specialized regions at the transition between prefrontal and premotor cortex serve the coordination of eye movements (area 8) and speech (areas 44–45, Broca’s area).

With continuing research, other views on the prefrontal cortex have emerged in later years, which, without substantially modifying the previous ones, add to them two essential accents: one on novelty and the other on the future. Any series of purposive actions that is new and thus deviates from rehearsed or automatic routine or instinctual order necessitates the lateral prefrontal cortex. The longer the series, and therefore the further it extends into the future, the greater the need for that cortex. Time is only one factor, however, among those determining that need; other factors include the complexity of the actions and of the information on which they are based, and still another the uncertainties or ambiguities in that information. There is considerable trade-off between those factors. For example, a monkey with a prefrontal deficit may fail at a simple and thoroughly rehearsed task, such as delayed response, not only because of the interval of time between cue and response, but also because of the competitive interference – a source of uncertainty and ambiguity – between two alternative cues that succeed each other at random from one trial to the next. Still, the question may be asked, what is new for the prefrontal cortex in a task as stereotypical as that one? The answer to that question is that the cue for every trial, although part of an old repertoire, is unpredictable and new for that trial. This critical element of built-in novelty is ignored by many studies using delay tasks.

Yet time is probably the single most important attribute placing a complex and novel sequence of behavior under the physiological purview of the lateral prefrontal cortex (Fuster, 2001). Only this part of the cerebral cortex can provide that temporal gestalt with the coherence and coordination of actions that are essential for the organism to reach its new goal. Both coherence and coordination derive from the capacity of the prefrontal cortex to organize new goal-directed actions in the time domain, which in my view is the most general and characteristic of all prefrontal functions in the primate. The importance of this temporal-organizing function in mammalian behavior cannot be overstated. Without it, there is no execution of novel, elaborate behavior, no speech fluency, no higher reasoning, and no creative activity with more than a minimal temporal dimension; only temporal concreteness is left, the here and now.

For it to function properly, however, the overarching prefrontal function of organizing new goal-directed actions in time necessitates two essential elements: (1) a cortex-wide infrastructure for the representation of knowledge and memory in the form of distributed cognitive networks, which I have called cognits (Fuster, 2009); and (2) a number of executive integrative functions that will manipulate those cognits in temporal integration. There are essentially five of these functions: planning, decision-making, working memory, preparatory set, and inhibitory control. Note that they all have a future perspective and that they interact with one another in the organization of goal-directed actions.

Indeed, all the cognitive functions of the cortex take place on a neural substrate of neural representation. That substrate is made of a vast neuronal network that is the repository of permanent, though modifiable, long-term memory, and knowledge (knowledge is semantic memory). That substrate extends to the hippocampus, which is a portion of ancient cortex that is essential for the consolidation of all explicit/declarative memory and knowledge. Individual memories or cognits are subcomponents, also network-like, of that vast network. They can be perceptual, acquired through the senses and spread in posterior cortex, or executive, acquired through action and spread in frontal cortex; or they can be mixed and spread in both cortices. Cognits overlap, intersect, and interact profusely. One neuron or group of neurons almost anywhere in cortex of association can be part of many cognits, part of many memories or items of knowledge.

Cognits are made by association of simultaneous stimuli or actions, stored in networks by connective associations, and retrieved by association with environmental or internal events that have been previously associated with them. All cognitive functions – attention, perception, memory, language, and intelligence – consist of neural transactions within and between cognits. Because these are made of associations, the cognitive code is essentially a relational code.

The representational substrate of the prefrontal cortex, in particular its lateral sector, is made of networks or cognits of executive memory, which have been formed by prior experience and extend into other cortical areas. The executive functions or operations of the prefrontal cortex essentially consist of the utilization of that substrate (1) for the acquisition of new perceptual and executive memory; and (2) for planning, decision-making, and organizing new behavior, reasoning, or language.

My use of the preposition for in the last sentence points to the central position of teleology in the physiology of the prefrontal cortex. Teleology is anathema in any scientific discourse, if nothing else because it blatantly defies the logic of causality. Yet in the discourse about prefrontal physiology goal, like purpose, is of the essence. All cognitive functions of the lateral prefrontal cortex are determined, we might say caused, by goals. If there is a unique and characteristic feature of that part of the brain, it is its ability to structure the present in order to serve the future, in this manner inverting the temporal direction of causality. Of course, this inversion is not real in physical terms. It is only real in cognitive terms inasmuch as the representations of goals antecede the actions to pursue them through the agency of the prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, all representations of the future, in the brain as in the mind, are reconfigurations of the past.

Teleology or teleonomy (Monod, 1971), thus understood, is at the basis of planning and the temporal organizing cognitive functions of the prefrontal cortex. In the human, as arguably in the large ape (Osvath and Osvath, 2008), the prefrontal cortex can create new plans of behavior. This implies a degree of reasoning and experience. New plans are new configurations of old memory for novel short- or long-term adaptive needs. By definition, a plan has a temporal dimension and a goal. That plan may be represented in a schema of action (a new prefrontal cognit) with its goal. The incipient plan or schema of action may be represented with varying degree of detail, but its implementation requires a temporal organization, the orderly activation of its component networks, some of which may extend into posterior (perceptual) cortex. That implementation requires top–down executive attention (see below).

Teleology also lies at the foundation of decision-making, another executive function of the prefrontal cortex. Decision-making is usually prompted by a new set of environmental circumstances, external or internal. It is essentially a choice between alternatives of action (including non-action), and thus bears heavily on the issues of individual freedom and responsibility (Fuster, 2013). It is multifactorial, as it is subject to the influences and appraisal, much of it unconscious, of a large variety of biological and historical events, such as the individual’s memory, culture, ethical principles, and experience from similar circumstances in the past. Because it is frequently based on many factors, some of which are imponderable and updatable, a decision is usually to some extent Bayesian, which means based on incompletely or imperfectly educated probability (Jaynes, 1986).

A third major prefrontal function is executive attention, which in many respects, as we will see, is indispensable to the first two, planning and decision-making. Executive top–down attention has three critical components, all three direct participants in the temporal organization of action: (1) working memory; (2) preparatory set; and (3) inhibitory control. All three have somewhat different, though partly overlapping, frontal topographies and a different cohort of neural structures with which the prefrontal cortex cooperates to implement them. Strictly speaking, none is localized in this cortex, but all three need their prefrontal base to operate. Furthermore, the prefrontal cortex performs its executive control of temporal organization by orchestrating activity in other neural structures that participate in executive attention. Executive attention, like all forms of attention, implies the optimal use of limited available resources for a given function, wherever they may be in the nervous system.

Working memory is active memory that the animal needs for the performance of acts in the short term. This is why it is often called also short-term memory. It is not to be confused, however, with short-term memory as the precursor stage of long-term memory. According to the dualistic concept of memory, before memories become established they pass through a short-term store. In any case, that concept, which is based on the assumption of separate neural substrates for the two forms of memory, is being replaced by a better supported unitary view of memory with a common cortical substrate. In accord with this view, working memory is the temporary activation of updated long-term memory networks for organizing actions in the near term. That prospective aspect is essential to the definition of working memory.

The content of working memory may be sensory, motor, or mixed; it may consist of a reactivated perceptual memory or the motor memory of the act to be performed, or both. It may also consist of the representation of the cognitive or behavioral goal of the act. Inasmuch as the content is selective and appropriate for current action, working memory is practically inextricable from attention. In fact, working memory is essentially sustained attention focused on an internal representation. In primates, working memory, depending on its content, engages a portion of lateral prefrontal cortex and, in addition, related areas of posterior (i.e., postcentral, postrolandic) cortex. The selective activation of posterior cortical areas by the prefrontal cortex, in the process of internal attention that we call working memory, is a major aspect of the neural basis of what has been called cognitive control (Miller and Cohen, 2001).

Preparatory set is the readying or priming of sensory and motor neural structures for the performance of an act contingent on a prior event, and thus on the content of the working memory of that event. Set may be rightfully viewed as motor attention. In the primate, preparatory set also engages a portion of lateral prefrontal cortex – depending on the act – and, in addition, structures below the prefrontal cortex in the hierarchy of motor structures (e.g., premotor cortex and basal ganglia). The modulation of those lower neural structures in the preparation for action is also part of the so-called cognitive control exerted by the prefrontal cortex.

In functional terms, working memory and preparatory set have opposite and symmetrical temporal perspectives, the first toward the recent past and the second toward the near future. The two of them, operating in tandem through their respective neural substrates and under prefrontal control, mediate cross-temporal contingencies. That means that the two functions together reconcile past with future: they reconcile a sensory cue or a reactivated memory with a subsequent – and consequent – act; they reconcile acts with goals, premises with conclusions, subjects with predicates. Thus, the prefrontal cortex, with its two temporal integrative functions of set and working memory, manages to bridge for the organism whatever temporal distances there may be between mutually contingent elements in the behavioral sequence, the rational discourse, or the construct of speech.

Inhibitory control complements those two temporal integrative functions of the lateral prefrontal cortex (working memory and set). In emotional behavior, it has an impact on the functions of the orbitomedial prefrontal cortex. Throughout the central nervous system, inhibition plays the role of enhancing and providing contrast to excitatory functions. A pervasive role of inhibition is evident in sensory systems (e.g., the retina) as well as motor systems (e.g., the motility of the knee). Inhibition is a critical component of attention; selective attention is accompanied by the suppression, by the inhibition, of whatever cognitive or emotional contents or expressions may interfere with the focused attention. In the prefrontal cortex, inhibition is the mechanism by which, during the temporal organization of actions in the pursuit of goals, sensory inputs and motor or instinctual impulses that might impede or derail those actions are held in check. In sum, an important aspect of the executive and controlling role of the prefrontal cortex is to suppress whatever internal or external influences may interfere with the sequence currently being enacted. In primates, this function seems to be represented mainly, though not exclusively, in orbitomedial prefrontal cortex and to engage other cortical and subcortical structures. The orbitomedial prefrontal cortex is known to be involved also in reward; it contains important components of neurotransmitter systems (e.g., dopamine) activated by rewards.

Each of the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex that we consider components of executive attention – working memory, set, and inhibitory control – finds support in a different category of data. For example, the working-memory function is strongly supported by neurophysiological data from single-unit studies, not only in frontal cortex but in other cortices as well. Ever since 1971, when the first demonstration of prefrontal memory cells was published, there has been a tendency to identify working memory, by whatever name, as the cardinal executive function of the prefrontal cortex. This ignores the evidence that working memory has an ancillary role, along with the other functions, under the superordinate function of temporal organization. The same can be said of preparatory set and inhibitory control.

The subdivision of prefrontal function into its components is made reasonable not only by the apparent specialization of prefrontal areas in different subfunctions of temporal organization, but also by the specialization of those areas in different forms of action (action domains). That subdivision, however, often results in the conceptual Balkanization of the prefrontal cortex into a topographic quilt of areas dedicated to a seemingly endless succession of supposedly independent cognitive or emotional functions, without regard for the two principles that I will try to outline in the subsequent chapters of this book: first that all prefrontal functions and areas are to some degree interdependent; and second that the various functions have areas and networks in common. Without these principles in sight, we are easily led to a sterile compartmentalization of functions. Thus, for example, to attribute only eye-movement control to area 8 and speech to Broca’s area ignores that both these functions depend also on other neural structures. It also ignores the evidence that both areas participate in the more general prefrontal function of temporal organization (syntax of action), which transcends both ocular movement and the spoken language. Nonetheless, a useful empirical approach is first to use whatever degree of specialization may be discernible in a given prefrontal area to investigate the basic mechanisms that support it, and then to examine that specialization within the superordinate organizing function. This approach respects the basic physiological principles of prefrontal function while also respecting areal or domain specificity where there is one. The approach allows for specificity but puts it under the overarching umbrella of temporal organization.

To sum up, this book emphasizes the role of the prefrontal cortex in coordinating cognitive functions and underlying neural structures in the temporal organization of new behavior; that is, in the formation of novel and coherent behavioral sequences toward the attainment of goals. My purpose in making this case is both deductive and inductive, goes often from the general to the particular and vice versa. It is my hope that this work will continue to generate new research, which in turn will provide us with a more solid basis of empirical knowledge than we now have of the neural mechanisms underlying that temporal integrative function of the prefrontal cortex.

References

1. Berwick RC, Friederici AD, Chomsky N, Bolhuis JJ. Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:89–98.

2. Bouchard K, Mesgarani N, Johnson K, Chang EF. Functional organization of human sensorimotor cortex for speech articulation. Nature. 2013;495:327–332.

3. Creutzfeldt OD. Generality of the functional structure of the neocortex. Naturwissenschaften. 1977;64:507–517.

4. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex – an update: time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30:319–333.

5. Fuster JM. Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:2047–2072.

6. Fuster JM. The Neuroscience of Freedom and Creativity: Our Predictive Brain Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2013.

7. Jackson JH. On some implications of dissolution of the nervous system. Medical Press Circular. 1882;ii:411–426.

8. Jackson JH. Selected Writings New York, NY: Basic Books; 1958.

9. Jaynes ET. Bayesian methods: general background. In: Justice JH, ed. Maximum-Entropy and Bayesian Methods in Applied Statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986:1–12.

10. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202.

11. Monod J. Chance and Necessity New York, NY: Knopf; 1971.

12. Osvath M, Osvath H. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Anim Cogn. 2008;11:661–674.

Chapter 2

Anatomy of the Prefrontal Cortex

The prefrontal cortex is among the last regions of the brain to develop, in evolution and in individual maturation. In the human, it constitutes nearly one-third of the totality of the neocortex. The most recent prefrontal cortex (pole and lateral convexity of the frontal lobe) is layered and granular, with a distinct layer IV which is heavily populated by granular cells. The more primitive, phylogenetically earlier, ventral and medial prefrontal cortex is dysgranular, poorly layered, and structurally and functionally akin to limbic cortex. Anatomically defined by its connectivity with the nucleus medialis dorsalis of the thalamus, the prefrontal cortex is the best connected of all neocortical regions. It is reciprocally connected, directly or indirectly, with the posterior association cortex, thalamus, hypothalamus, amygdala, basal ganglia, insula, hippocampus, cerebellum, and brainstem.

Keywords

afferent; comparative anatomy; connectivity; development; dorsolateral cortex; efferent; evolution; thalamus; ventromedial cortex

Outline

I. Introduction 9

II. Evolution and Comparative Anatomy 9

III. Development 15

IV. Microscopic Architecture 20

V. Aging 26

VI. Connections 30

A. Afferents 31

B. Efferents 40

VII. Summary 46

References 48

I Introduction

This chapter is devoted to the anatomy and developmental neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex. It begins with the discussion of issues related to the phylogenetic development and comparative anatomy of the neocortex of the frontal lobe. After that, the chapter deals with its ontogenetic development. Then, the chapter deals with the anatomy and microscopic architecture of the prefrontal cortex in the adult organism, and with the morphological changes it undergoes as a result of aging. Finally, the chapter provides an overview of the afferent and efferent connections of the prefrontal cortex in several species. This overview of connectivity of the prefrontal cortex, which is arguably the most richly connected of all cortical regions, opens the way to subsequent chapters, where connectivity is found to be the key to all its functions.

II Evolution and Comparative Anatomy

The prefrontal cortex increases in relative size with phylogenetic development. This can be inferred from the study of existent animals’ brains as well as from paleoneurological data (Papez, 1929; Grünthal, 1948; Ariëns Kappers et al., 1960; Poliakov, 1966b; Radinsky, 1969). It is most apparent in the primate order, where the cortical sector named by Brodmann (1909, 1912) the regio frontalis, which approximately corresponds to what we call the prefrontal cortex, constitutes, by his calculations based on cytoarchitectonics, 29% of the total cortex in humans, 17% in the chimpanzee, 11.5% in the gibbon and the macaque, and 8.5% in the lemur (Brodmann, 1912). For the dog and the cat, the figures are, respectively, 7% and 3.5%.

The use of values such as those to estimate differences in evolutionary growth has pitfalls and limitations, however (Passingham, 1973). The old notion that the entirety of the frontal lobe is relatively larger in humans than in other primates has been challenged by the results of brain imaging in several primate species (Semendeferi, 2001). Furthermore, by calculating the volume of the prefrontal cortex and plotting it against the total volume of the brain (in rat, marmoset, macaque, orangutan, and human), some authors have come up with a linear relationship, thus belying the volumetric prefrontal advantage of the human (Uylings and Van Eden, 1990). Others, however, have utilized sound empirical reasons to argue that in the course of evolution the prefrontal region per se, strictly defined cytoarchitectonically, grows more than other cortical regions (review by Preuss, 2000). No one has persuasively denied that in the human, as Brodmann showed, the prefrontal cortex attains the greatest magnitude in comparison with those other regions. That greater relative magnitude of the human prefrontal cortex presumably indicates that this cortex is the substrate for cognitive functions of the highest order, which, as a result of phylogenetic differentiation, have become a distinctive part of the evolutionary patrimony of our species. It has even been proposed that certain cortical areas, such as Broca’s area – which is arguably prefrontal – have developed by natural selection with the development of language, a distinctly human function (Aboitiz and García, 1997).

It is always difficult to draw evolutionary conclusions from neuroanatomical comparisons between contemporaneous species in the absence of common ancestors (Hodos, 1970; Campbell, 1975). Such comparisons commonly fail to establish the homology of brain structures (Campbell and Hodos, 1970), and this is a particularly vexing problem when dealing with cortical areas. Ordinarily, for lack of more reliable guidelines, the neuroanatomist uses structural criteria to determine cortical homology. The principal criteria for defining the prefrontal cortex and for establishing its homology across species are topology, topography, architecture, and fiber connections (hodology). The same criteria have been utilized in attempts to elucidate its evolutionary development.

The neocortex of mammals has emerged and developed between two ancient structures that constitute most of the pallium in non-mammalian vertebrates: the hippocampus and the piriform area or lobe (Figure 2.1). The process is part of what has been generally characterized as the evolutionary neocorticalization of the brain (Jerison, 1994). What in the brain of the reptile is a sheet of simple cortex-like structure bridging those two structures is replaced and outgrown by the multilayered neocortex of the mammalian brain (Crosby, 1917; Elliott Smith, 1919; Kuhlenbeck, 1927, 1929; Ariëns Kappers et al., 1960; Nauta and Karten, 1970; Aboitiz et al., 2003). Because the growth of the newer cortex takes place in the dorsal aspect of the cerebral hemisphere, the evolutionary process has been characterized as one of dorsalization of pallial development. Strictly speaking, however, it is inaccurate to consider the reptile’s general cortex as the homologous precursor of the mammalian neocortex (Kruger and Berkowitz, 1960). Moreover, there are other plausible alternate theories of neocortical evolution in addition to the above (Northcutt and Kaas, 1995; Butler and Molnar, 2002). In any case, it appears that the mammalian neocortex is phylogenetically preceded by certain homologous subcortical nuclei in the brains of reptiles and birds.

Figure 2.1 Phylogenetic development of the cortex in several species. (A) Parasagittal brain sections in four vertebrate classes. Abbreviation: P, pallium, generic term for both paleocortex and neocortex. (From Creutzfeldt, 1993, after Eddinger, modified.) (B) Coronal sections of amphibian Necturus, tortoise, opossum, and human. (From Herrick, 1956, modified.)

Studies of cortical architecture in aplacental mammals, such as those by Abbie (1940, 1942), have been helpful to trace neocortical development. They reveal that the neocortex is made of two separate components or moieties, one adjoining the hippocampus and the other the piriform area, that develop in opposite directions around the hemisphere and meet on its lateral aspect. Both undergo progressive differentiation, which consists of cortical thickening, sharpening of lamination, and, ultimately, emergence of granular cells. In higher mammals the two primordial structures, the hippocampus and the piriform lobe, have been outflanked, pushed against each other, and buried in ventromedial locations by the vastly expanded cortex (Sanides, 1964, 1970). Around the rostral pole of the hemisphere, the two phylogenetically differentiated moieties form the prefrontal neopallium.

The external morphology of the frontal region varies so much from species to species that it is difficult to ascertain the homology of its landmarks. Within a given order of mammals, certain sulci can be identified as homologous and used as a guide for understanding cortical evolution; across orders, however, all comparisons are hazardous. Nevertheless, some general principles of prefrontal evolution seem sustainable. One such principle is that, like the rest of the neopallium, the frontal cortex becomes not only larger but also more complex, more fissurated and convoluted, as mammalian species evolve. In primates, the process reaches its culmination with the human brain.

We should note, however, that the phyletic increase in gyrification and fissuration can be attributable to mechanical factors and not only to such factors as functional differentiation. The cortex folds and thus gains surface, keeping up with the three-dimensional expansion of subcortical masses (Bok, 1959). Thus, the overall number of gyri and sulci that form with evolution is largely a function of brain size, as stated by the law of Baillarger–Dareste (Ariëns Kappers et al., 1960). However, where the gyri and the sulci are formed is determined, at least in part, by functional differentiation. Gyri appear to mushroom as functions develop (Welker and Seidenstein, 1959) and, as Clark (1945) first postulated, sulci develop perpendicular to the lines of stress determined by fast area-growth. Not surprisingly, some of the most highly differentiated neuronal functions can be found in the cortex lining sulci (e.g., principalis, central, intraparietal, lunate, superior temporal). At the same time, and as a consequence of those developments, sulci and fissures generally separate areas of different functional significance. Electrophysiological studies corroborate this finding, although they also reveal several notable exceptions (Welker and Seidenstein, 1959; Woolsey, 1959; Welker and Campos, 1963). It should also be noted that, in the ontogenetic development of the monkey, sulci develop shortly after midgestation, long before functions (Goldman and Galkin, 1978).

With respect to the prefrontal cortex, homologies can be established with confidence only for the furrows that approximately mark its lateral boundary. This boundary is marked in the cat and the dog by the presylvian fissure, a fissure already present in marsupials and one of the most constant in carnivores (Ariëns Kappers et al., 1960). It is homologous to the vertical limb of the arcuate sulcus of monkeys and to the inferior precentral fissure of the larger apes and humans. The large expansion of the presylvian (prearcuate) area is one of the most remarkable developments of mammalian evolution.

Although by hodological and other criteria rodents have been determined to possess a prefrontal cortex (Preuss, 1995), it is difficult to find in it, or around it, distinctive anatomical landmarks that could be deemed homologous to those found in carnivores or primates. In the anterior pole of the brain of some carnivores, between the presylvian fissure and the midline, there is a short furrow called the proreal or intraproreal fissure. According to Ariëns Kappers et al. (1960), this furrow may be the equivalent of the sulcus rectus of prosimians, more commonly designated the principal sulcus (sulcus principalis) in the monkey. In the human and anthropoids, the principal sulcus is represented, rostrally, by the sulcus frontomarginalis of Wernicke. It is uncertain whether, in the human and anthropoids, it is the medial or the inferior frontal fissure that represents the posterior extremity of the sulcus principalis (Connolly, 1950; Ariëns Kappers et al., 1960). Cytoarchitecture suggests that it is the inferior frontal fissure (Sanides, 1970).

Sanides (1964, 1970) carried out a remarkable effort to read phylogenetic history into the architecture of the prefrontal cortex. His studies essentially uphold for primates the principle of the dual evolutionary development of the neocortex formerly upheld in lower mammals (Abbie, 1940, 1942). By analysis of frontal architectonic zones, the two primordial trends of cortical differentiation mentioned above can be followed in dorsad progression. Studies of corticocortical connectivity in the adult primate demonstrate, within the prefrontal cortex, two trends of connections: one in ventrolateral and orbital cortex, presumably following the lateral (amygdala–piriform) developmental trend; and the other dorsomedial, presumably following the medial (hippocampal) trend (Yeterian et al., 2012).

A third and later trend seems to have occurred in primates, a trend originating in the more recently differentiated motor cortex and proceeding forward from there. This trend is suggested by the cytoarchitectonic gradations from areas 4 to 6 and from areas 6 to 9 that the Vogts first noted (Vogt and Vogt, 1919). Consequently, as Sanides (1970) points out, the prefrontal cortex of the primate seems to have resulted from the growth and convergence of three differentiating fields over the polar region: the two primordial fields from cingular (parahippocampal) and insular (parapiriform) areas and the third, more recent field, from the motor cortex. Maximal differentiation can be observed in the frontier zones of the three developing fields. These zones show at its best the granularization of layer IV that is characteristic of the prefrontal cortex of humans and other primates.

As a result of these developments, the granular prefrontal cortex of the mature primate is bordered by a fringe of transitional paralimbic mesocortex, at least in its medial and ventral aspects (Reep, 1984). The cortex of those regions is distinctly agranular, thus phylogenetically more primitive than the granular dorsolateral cortex of the primate. It is on the basis of such differences that Passingham and Wise (2012) conclude that, in the primate brain, the medial and ventral cortex is the remnant of the limbic frontal regions of earlier species, whereas the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex represents the truly new prefrontal cortex of primates.

In any case, Pandya and his colleagues adopted Sanides’ concept of developmental architectonic trends and, as noted above, complemented it with evidence of the corticocortical connectivity underlying those trends (Barbas and Pandya, 1989; Pandya and Yeterian, 1990a; Yeterian et al., 2012). Those connective trends clearly follow gradients from paralimbic, agranular, to granular prefrontal cortex.

The robust anatomical relationship between the prefrontal cortex and the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus has been known since the late nineteenth century (Monakow, 1895). Many studies demonstrate that the fiber

Enjoying the preview?
Page 1 of 1