Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 5

Personality and Individual Differences 31 (2001) 13751379

www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Color preferences according to gender and sexual orientation


Lee Ellis *, Christopher Ficek
Minot State University, Minot, ND 58707, USA
Received 30 June 2000; received in revised form 13 November 2000; accepted 30 November 2000

Abstract
The present study was undertaken to verify ndings surrounding sex dierences in color preferences, and
to extend this realm of inquiry by looking for possible dierences in color preferences associated with sexual
orientation. Based on a large sample of North American college students, signicant gender dierences were
found, with the main dierence being a greater preference for shades of blue by males than by females.
Females, on the other hand, tended to be more evenly divided than males between preferring both green
and blue. Regarding sexual orientation, we found no signicant dierences between heterosexuals and
homosexual/bisexuals of either gender. In other words, male and female homosexuals/bisexuals exhibited
essentially the same conguration of color preferences as did their heterosexual counterparts. # 2001
Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Studies have arrived at dierent conclusions regarding the existence of gender dierences in color
preferences. In a study of United States college students, Silver et al. (1988) found that greater
proportions of males chose blue as their favorite color than was true for females. In a subsequent
study of elderly persons in the United States, Silver and Ferrante (1995) reported four signicant
dierences in color preferences, with males again being more likely than females to prefer blue. Males
were also more likely to choose red, whereas females were more likely to prefer black and pink.
Three additional studies, all among elderly persons, concluded that there were no signicant
dierences between men and women in their preferences for various colors. Two of these studies
were conducted in the United States (Mather, Stare & Breinin, 1971; Tate & Allen 1985), and the
other was carried out in Sweden (Wijk, Berg, Sivik & Steen, 1999).
The present study was undertaken to verify the existence of gender dierences in color preference. In addition, we decided to look for evidence that color preference might also vary
according to sexual orientation.

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-701-858-3241; fax: +1-701-839-6933.


E-mail address: ellis@misu.nodak.edu (L. Ellis).
0191-8869/01/$ - see front matter # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S0191-8869(00)00231-2

1376

L. Ellis, C. Ficek / Personality and Individual Dierences 31 (2001) 13751379

1. Methods
The present study derived its data from a large investigation of gender-typical behavior patterns
in the United States and Canada conducted at 22 universities during the 1990s. A total of 1924
males and 3766 females provided answers to a question regarding their color preference. Their
ages ranged from 17 to 63, with a mean of 22.82 (S.D.=5.32) for the males and 22.54
(S.D.=5.34) for the females.1
To measure sexual orientation, we asked subjects if they considered themselves to be ``heterosexual, homosexual, bisexual, or undecided.'' Of those who provided answers to the question on
color preference, 53 (2.86%) of the males and 62 (1.71%) of the females declared themselves to be
either homosexual or bisexual.
To our question regarding the subjects' favorite color, subjects were given a brief space in
which to write their choice. For comparing males and females, we consolidated the responses into
11 dierent color categories, including one category for those who stated that they have no particular preference.
Regarding our analysis of males and females according to sexual orientation, several of the
color categories were chosen by fewer than ve homosexuals/bisexuals, thus making meaningful
statistical comparison impossible. Therefore, when we compared sexes by sexual orientation, only
three specic color categories were used, with the remaining choices of colors subsumed under the
category of ``other'' (including no preference).
2. Results
The color preferences expressed by males and females are shown in Table 1, with the colors
arranged roughly in the order of their appearance along the color spectrum. A chi-square test
revealed very signicant dierences between the proportional distributions of male and female
subjects in color preferences (2=435.202, d.f.=10, P=0.000).2 The greatest dierences had to
do with preferences for blue and green. Whereas nearly half (45%) of all males chose various
shades of blue as their favorite color and less than 20% chose some shade of green, only 25% of
females chose blue, while about 28% chose green. Other noteworthy gender dierences were that
females were more likely to choose pink and purple, while males were more partial to black (this
latter nding is contrary to Silver and Ferrante (1995), who found females preferring black more
than males).
In the case of sexual orientation, Table 2 presents consolidated proportions of males and
females according to sexual orientation who chose each color (or shades thereof) as their favorites. To assess statistical signicance, we again used chi square. However, because our samples of
homosexual/bisexuals of both genders were much smaller than the samples of heterosexuals of
both genders, there were fewer than ve observations for several of the colors as far as the

For details on the universities sampled for this study, see Ellis and Robb (2000).
The computer program utilized in this study (SPSS, Version 9) only carried its calculations of the degree of statistical signicance to three decimal points. Therefore, any probability with a 0.0001 or lower appears simply as 0.000.
2

L. Ellis, C. Ficek / Personality and Individual Dierences 31 (2001) 13751379

1377

homosexual/bisexual subjects were concerned. We compensated for this sampling shortfall by


collapsing all of the least frequently chosen color categories (plus ``no preference'') into an
``other'' category. This left the following four color categories for comparison: green, blue, black,
and other.
The resulting chi square for comparing heterosexual males and homosexual/bisexual males was
w2=1.959, d.f.=3, P=0.581. For the females, the chi square dierence between heterosexuals
and homosexual/bisexuals was w2=1.234, d.f.=3, P=0.745. Thus, in neither the case of males
nor females did sexual orientation have a signicant link to color preference. For comparison
purposes, we also performed a chi square in which all males were compared to all females. This
again yielded extremely signicant dierences (w2=310.955, d.f.=3, P=0.000)2, although not
quite as strong as when all eleven color categories were dierentiated.
Table 1
Distribution of consolidated color preferences by gender
Color preference

Males

Females

Frequency
No preference
Pink
Red
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Purple
Brown
Gray
Black
White
Total

Frequency

19
9
233
16
19
367
866
98
13
22
233
29

1.0
0.5
12.1
0.8
1.0
19.1
45.0
5.1
0.7
1.1
12.1
1.5

95
199
447
66
100
1051
938
459
19
7
306
79

2.5
5.3
11.9
1.8
2.7
27.9
24.9
12.2
0.5
0.02
8.1
2.1

1924

100.0

3666

99.9

Table 2
Distribution of consolidated color preferences by gender and sexual orientation
Color preference

Males

Females

Heterosexual
Frequency

Homo/bisexual
%

Heterosexual

Frequency

Frequency

Homo/bisexual
%

Frequency

Green
Blue
Black
Other

340
817
220
424

18.9
45.4
12.2
23.5

14
21
6
12

26.4
39.6
11.3
22.6

999
896
228
1389

28.0
25.1
8.1
38.9

17
13
7
25

27.4
21.1
11.3
40.3

Total

180

100.0

53

99.9

3512

100.1

62

100.0

1378

L. Ellis, C. Ficek / Personality and Individual Dierences 31 (2001) 13751379

3. Conclusion
The present study conrms Silver and Ferrante's (1995) observation that males are signicantly
more likely than females to prefer variations on the color of blue. Given the very high degree of
statistical signicance for our ndings, we are surprised that three studies have failed to nd any
signicant gender dierences in color preference (Mather et al., 1971; Tate & Allen, 1985; Wijk et
al., 1999). This could possibly be due to the fact that all of these studies were of elderly populations, and perhaps gender dierences dissipate with age. We were unable to investigate this possibility since our sample was limited to college students.
Why would color preference vary by gender? Without ruling out any possibility at this point,
we are inclined to suspect the involvement of neurohormonal factors. Studies of rats have found
average sex dierences in the number of neurons comprising various parts of the visual cortex
(Reid & Juraska, 1992, 1995). Also, gender dierences have been found in rat preferences for the
amount of sweetness in drinking water (Lichtensteiger & Schlumpf, 1985; Shapiro & Goldman, 1973; Valenstein, Kakolewski & Cox, 1967). One experiment demonstrated that the sex
dierences in rat preferences for sweetness was eliminated by depriving males of male-typical
testosterone levels in utero (Lichtensteiger & Schlumpf, 1985). Perhaps, prenatal exposure to
testosterone and other sex hormones operates in a similar way to ``bias'' preferences for certain
colors in humans.
Focusing on some of the actual mechanisms that may be involved in humans, research by
Hoyenga and Wallace (1979) and McGuinness and Lewis (1976) found sex dierences in
retinal biochemistry and in how the brain processes color information. According to both
studies, females seemed to be more sensitive to pinks, reds, and yellows. In addition, the
Hoyenga and Wallace study identied certain conditions under which males appeared to be
more sensitive to colors in the blue-green light spectrum. Nevertheless, caution must be exercised
in any attempts to explain preferences for various colors on the basis of sensitivity to those colors.
None of the above conjecture is meant to imply that cultural factors might not also
operate, although we are not aware of any cultural training or expectations regarding which
gender is supposed to prefer one color over another. There is, of course, a tendency in several
Western cultures to dress baby boys in blue and baby girls in pink, and one could argue that
dressing baby boys in blue might increase their preferences for blue later in life. If this were true,
we would expect to nd women exhibiting a corresponding bias toward shades of red. There
was a substantial tendency for greater number of women to choose pink as their favorite color
when compared to men, but women were actually less likely to prefer darker shades of red
(Table 1).
Cross-cultural research could shed light on these issues by determining how varied gender differences are in color preferences. Also, insight could come from comparing males and females of
other species as to any color preferences they might harbor.
Acknowledgements
We thank Katharine Hoyenga and anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on drafts of
this manuscript.

L. Ellis, C. Ficek / Personality and Individual Dierences 31 (2001) 13751379

1379

References
Ellis, L., Robb, B. (2000). Sexual orientation in a large sample of United States and Canadian college students: toward
rened measurement (submitted for publication).
Hoyenga, K. B., & Wallace, B. (1979). Sex dierences in the perception of autokenetic movement of an afterimage.
Journal of General Psychology, 100, 93101.
Lichtensteiger, W., & Schlumpf, M. (1985). Prenatal nicotine aects fetal testosterone and sexual dimorphism of saccharin preference. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, & Behavior, 23, 439444.
Mather, J., Stare, C., & Breinin, S. (1971). Color preferences in a geriatric population. Gerontologist, 11, 311313.
McGuinness, D., & Lewis, I. (1976). Sex dierences in visual persistence: experiments on the Ganzfeld and afterimages.
Perception, 5, 295301.
Reid, S. N. M., & Juraska, J. M. (1992). Sex dierences in neuron numbers in the binocular area of the rat visual
cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 321, 448455.
Reid, S. N. M., & Juraska, J. M. (1995). Sex dierences in the number of synaptic junctions in the binocular area of the
rat visual cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 352, 560566.
Shapiro, B. H., & Goldman, A. S. (1973). Feminine saccharin preference in the genetically androgen insensitive male
rat pseudohermaphrodite. Horm Behavior, 4, 371375.
Silver, N. C., & Ferrante, R. (1995). Sex dierences in color preferences among an elderly sample. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 80, 920922.
Silver, N. C., McCulley, W. L., Chambliss, L. N., Charles, C. M., Smith, A. A., Waddell, W. M., & Wineld, E. B.
(1988). Sex and racial dierences in color and number preferences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 295299.
Tate, F. B., & Allen, H. (1985). Color preferences and the aged individual: implications for art therapy. Arts in Psychotherapy, 12, 165169.
Valenstein, E. S., Kakolewski, J. W., & Cox, V. C. (1967). Sex dierences in taste preference for glucose and saccharin
solutions. Science, 156, 942943.
Wijk, H., Berg, S., Sivik, L., & Steen, B. (1999). Color discrimination, color naming and color preferences in 80-year
olds. Aging: Clinical and Experimental Research, 11, 98106.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi