Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 215

HDO1-71-edzard.

qxd 09/04/2003 10:03 Page i
SUMERIAN GRAMMAR
HDO1-71-edzard.qxd 09/04/2003 10:03 Page ii
HANDBOOK OF ORIENTAL STUDIES
HANDBUCH DER ORIENTALISTIK
SECTION ONE
THE NEAR AND MIDDLE EAST
EDITED BY
H. ALTENMÜLLER · B. HROUDA · B.A. LEVINE · R.S. O’FAHEY
K.R. VEENHOF · C.H.M. VERSTEEGH
VOLUME SEVENTY-ONE
HDO1-71-edzard.qxd 09/04/2003 10:03 Page iii
SUMERIAN GRAMMAR
BY
DIETZ OTTO EDZARD
BRILL
LEIDEN • BOSTON
2003
HDO1-71-edzard.qxd 09/04/2003 10:03 Page iv
This book is printed on acid-free paper.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Edzard, Dietz Otto.
Sumerian grammar / by Dietz Otto Edzard.
p. cm. – (Handbook of Oriental studies. Section 1, Near and Middle East ; v. 71 =
Handbuch der Orientalistik)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 90-04-12608-2
1. Sumerian language–Grammar. I. Title. II. Handbuch der Orientalistik. Erste
Abteilung, Nahe und der Mittlere Osten ; 71. Bd.
PJ4013.E38 2003
499'.955-dc21 2003049567
ISSN 0169-9423
ISBN 90 04 12608 2
© Copyright 2003 by Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, translated, stored
in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic,
mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without prior written
permission from the publisher.
Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use is granted
by Brill provided that the appropriate fees are paid directly to
The Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive,
Suite 910, Danvers MA 01923, USA.
Fees are subject to change.
printed in the netherlands
v
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page v
CONTENTS
Preface ........................................................................................ xi
Abbreviations .............................................................................. xiii
Cn\r+rn Oxr The Sumerian Language .............................. 1
1.1. General Characteristics .................................................. 1
1.2. The (hopeless) question of the linguistic affiliation of
Sumerian ........................................................................ 2
1.3. The linguistic environment of Sumerian .................... 3
Cn\r+rn Tvo How we read Sumerian ................................ 7
2.1. General ............................................................................ 7
2.2. Spelling of Sumerian .................................................... 8
2.2.1. Classification of cuneograms .............................. 8
2.2.2. Combination of cuneograms; spelling proper .... 10
2.2.3. Evolution of Sumerian spelling .......................... 10
Cn\r+rn Tnnrr Minimalia of Sumerian phonology and
syllabic structure .................................................................... 13
3.1. Phonology ...................................................................... 13
3.1.1. Vowels .................................................................. 13
3.1.2. Consonants .......................................................... 14
3.2. Syllable structure ............................................................ 22
Cn\r+rn Fotn The “word” in Sumerian, parts of
speech ...................................................................................... 23
4.1. “Word” ............................................................................ 23
4.2. Parts of speech .............................................................. 23
Cn\r+rn Fiyr Substantives .................................................... 29
5.1. General ............................................................................ 29
5.2. Possession ........................................................................ 29
5.3. Number .......................................................................... 31
5.4. Case ................................................................................ 33
5.4.1. Notes on the phonetic and spelling behaviour
of the case particles ............................................ 35
5.4.2. Notes on the individual case particles .............. 35
5.4.2.1. Absolutive .............................................. 35
5.4.2.2. Ergative .................................................. 35
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page vi
vi cox+rx+s
5.4.2.3. Genitive ................................................ 36
5.4.2.4. Locative ................................................ 39
5.4.2.5. Dative .................................................. 40
5.4.2.6. Comitative ............................................ 40
5.4.2.7. Ablative-instrumental .......................... 41
5.4.2.8. Terminative .......................................... 42
5.4.2.9. Directive .............................................. 43
5.4.2.10. Equative .............................................. 44
5.4.2.11. Recapitulation of the case particles .... 44
Cn\r+rn Six Adjectives .......................................................... 47
Cn\r+rn Sryrx Determination, specification: demonstrative
particles .................................................................................. 49
7.1. -ne(n) ............................................................................ 49
7.2. -bi .................................................................................. 50
7.3. -e(?) ................................................................................ 50
7.4. -ri(?) .............................................................................. 50
7.5. -“e .................................................................................. 51
7.6. Recapitulation .............................................................. 51
Cn\r+rn Eion+ Overview of the sequence of particles for
possession, number and case ................................................ 53
Cn\r+rn Nixr Pronouns ........................................................ 55
9.1. Personal pronouns ........................................................ 55
9.2. Possessive pronouns ...................................................... 56
9.3. Demonstrative pronouns .............................................. 57
9.3.1. ur
5
........................................................................ 57
9.3.2. ne-e(n) ................................................................ 57
9.4. Interrogative pronouns ................................................ 57
9.5. Alleged pronouns .......................................................... 58
Cn\r+rn Trx Numerals .......................................................... 61
10.1. General .......................................................................... 61
10.2. Cardinal numbers ........................................................ 61
10.3. Ordinal numbers .......................................................... 66
10.4. Fraction and measure terminology ............................ 67
10.5. Distributional relations of numbers ............................ 67
Cn\r+rn Erryrx Adverbs ...................................................... 69
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page vii
cox+rx+s vii
Cn\r+rn Tvrryr The verb .................................................. 71
12.1. Preliminaries .............................................................. 71
12.2. ¢am†u and marû .......................................................... 73
12.3. Plural verbs ................................................................ 74
12.4. Verbal base ¢am†u/marû/sg./pl. grids ...................... 74
12.5. Sorts of plurality (¢am†u reduplication) .................... 79
12.6. Note on verbal reduplication .................................... 80
12.7. Conjugation ................................................................ 81
12.7.1. Conjugation pattern 1: Intransitive
(and passive) ................................................ 81
12.7.1.1. The verb me .............................. 82
12.7.2. Conjugation pattern 2a: Transitive .......... 83
12.7.3. Conjugation pattern 2b: Transitive .......... 87
12.7.4. Prefixless finite verbal forms ...................... 90
12.7.5. Was Sumerian a language with “split
ergativity”? .................................................. 90
12.8. Dimensional indicators .............................................. 92
12.8.1: 1–26. Non-ventive indicators ...................... 94
12.8.2: 27–62. Ventive indicators .......................... 103
12.8.3. Dimensional indicators without reference
to a person? ................................................ 109
12.9. Prefixed indicator [e, i] ............................................ 109
12.10. Prefixed indicator [a(1)] ............................................ 111
12.11–12. Modal and connecting indicators ........ 112
12.11. Modal indicators ........................................................ 113
12.11.1. Indicative .................................................... 113
12.11.2. Negative indicative .................................... 113
12.11.2.1. Suffixed -nu ............................ 114
12.11.2.2. [nu] as a separate verbal base 115
12.11.3. Cohortative ................................................ 115
12.11.4. Negative cohortative .................................. 116
12.11.5. Precative .................................................... 116
12.11.6. Vetitive (negative precative) ...................... 117
12.11.7. Affirmative 1 .............................................. 117
12.11.8. Negative affirmative .................................. 118
12.11.9. Prohibitive .................................................. 118
12.11.10. Affirmative 2 .............................................. 119
12.11.11. Affirmative 3 .............................................. 120
12.11.12. Frustrative .................................................. 120
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page viii
viii 
12.12. Connecting indicators ................................................ 121
12.12.1. Prospective ................................................ 121
12.12.1.1. A precedes B ............................ 122
12.12.1.2. Polite imperative ........................ 122
12.12.2. Connecting indicator [inga] ...................... 123
12.12.2.1. [inga] .......................................... 124
12.12.2.2. [nu-nga] ...................................... 124
12.12.2.3. [ga-nga] ...................................... 124
12.12.2.4. [he-nga] ...................................... 125
12.12.2.5. [na-nga] ...................................... 125
12.12.2.6. [“i-nga] ........................................ 125
12.12.2.7. [nu“-inga] .................................. 126
12.12.2.8. ‘Irregular’ [inga-na] .................. 126
12.12.2.9. Summary of [inga] .................... 127
12.13. Imperative .................................................................. 127
12.13.1. Unextended imperative .............................. 128
12.13.2. Extended imperative .................................. 129
12.14. Non-finite verbal forms .............................................. 130
12.14.1. B-[Ø], B-B-[Ø]: unextended bases ............ 131
12.14.2. B-[a], B-[ed], with or without copula ...... 132
12.14.2.1. B-[a], mes-Ane-pada
construction ................................ 132
12.14.2.1.1. B-[a] ...................... 132
12.14.2.1.2. mes-Ane-pada
construction .......... 133
12.14.2.2. B-[ed], B-[edam] ...................... 134
12.14.2.2.1. B-[ed] .................... 134
12.44.2.2.2. B-[ed + copula] .... 134
12.14.3. B-[ede], B-[eda], B-[ada] ............................ 134
12.14.3.1. B-[ede] ...................................... 135
12.14.3.2. B-[eda] ...................................... 136
12.14.3.3. B-[ada] ...................................... 136
12.14.4. Conjugated participle or “pronominal
conjugation” ................................................ 137
12.14.4.1. 1
st
sg. ¢am†u ................................ 138
12.14.4.2. 2
nd
sg. ¢am†u .............................. 138
12.14.4.3. 3
rd
sg. person class ¢am†u .......... 139
12.14.4.4. 3
rd
sg. non-person class ¢am†u .... 139
12.14.4.5. 3
rd
pl. person class ¢am†u .......... 139
12.14.4.6. 1
st
sg. marû ................................ 139
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page ix
cox+rx+s ix
12.14.4.7. 2
nd
sg. marû .............................. 139
12.14.4.8. 3
rd
sg. person-class marû .......... 140
12.14.4.9. 3
rd
sg. non-person class marû 140
12.14.4.10. 3
rd
pl. person-class marû .......... 140
12.14.4.11. Irregular behaviour of the
140 verb ∞gen/du ............................
12.14.4.12. deli-∞ gu
10
-ne etc. ........................ 141
12.14.4.13. Conjugated participles: open
questions .................................. 141
12.15. Compound verbs ........................................................ 142
12.15.1. Compound verbs: free formations ............ 143
12.15.1.1. Extended nominal element .... 145
12.15.1.2. “u ti/te(-∞g) and other
compound verbs and the
special behaviour of their
“objects” .................................. 145
12.15.1.3. Complete incorporation of the
nominal element ...................... 147
12.15.2. Compound verbs with a verbalizer .......... 148
12.15.2.1. Clear cases of verbalization .... 148
12.15.2.2. Verbalization no longer
recognizable to us .................. 149
12.15.3. Difficult case: artificial splits? .................... 149
12.16. Nominalization of finite verbal forms ...................... 150
12.16.1. The nominalized phrase is in the
absolutive .................................................... 151
12.16.2. The nominalized phrase is in the genitive 152
12.16.3. The nominalized phrase is in the locative 152
12.16.4. The nominalized phrase is in the ablative 153
12.16.5. The nominalized phrase is in the
terminative .................................................. 154
12.16.6. The nominalized phrase is in the
equative ........................................................ 154
12.16.7. Nominalized phrase with a possessive
particle ........................................................ 154
Cn\r+rn Tnin+rrx Post-nominal and/or post-verbal
particles other than case particles (5.4) ................................ 157
13.1. [(e)“e] ............................................................................ 157
ge“en] ([∞ 13.2. [∞ gi“en]) ............................................................ 158
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page x
x cox+rx+s
13.3. [nanna] ........................................................................ 158
13.4. [“uba] .......................................................................... 159
13.5. [ri] .............................................................................. 160
Cn\r+rn Fotn+rrx Conjunctions and subjunctions ............ 161
14.1. Conjunctions .............................................................. 161
14.1.1. [u] .................................................................. 161
14.1.2. [ma] ................................................................ 162
14.2. Subjunctions ................................................................ 162
14.2.1. [uda] .............................................................. 162
14.2.2. [tukumbi] ...................................................... 163
14.2.3. [ena] .............................................................. 164
14.2.4. [mu] ................................................................ 164
14.2.5. [iginzu] .......................................................... 165
Cn\r+rn Fir+rrx Exclamations .............................................. 167
15.1. [a] ................................................................................ 167
15.2. [alala] .......................................................................... 167
15.3. [alulu] .......................................................................... 167
15.4. [allili] .......................................................................... 168
15.5. [aua] ............................................................................ 168
15.6. [ellu, elala, ilu, ilulamma] ........................................ 168
15.7. [gana] .......................................................................... 168
15.8. [heam] ........................................................................ 168
15.9. [inu] ............................................................................ 169
15.10. [i Utu] ........................................................................ 169
15.11. [mel(i)ea] .................................................................... 169
15.12. [ua] .............................................................................. 170
15.13. [ulili] ............................................................................ 170
Cn\r+rn Six+rrx Emesal ........................................................ 171
Cn\r+rn Sryrx+rrx The Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area 173
Cn\r+rn Eion+rrx Summary—and what is still missing? 179
Bibliography ................................................................................ 181
Index .......................................................................................... 187
xi
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xi
PREFACE
The present Sumerian Grammar with which the Publisher Brill kindly
entrusted the author is essentially based on introductory classes of
Sumerian offered at the Institut für Assyriologie und Hethitologie of
Munich University over the last twenty years, as well as on a two
semester course on “Geschichte der sumerischen Sprache” (winter
1996/97 and summer 1997). Part of the “History” was also pre­
sented in lectures at the University of London and at Oxford University
between October 15 and November 5, 1998. While offering my own
personal ideas—some of which will no doubt be considered highly
subjective—I have tried to discuss, or at least quote, differing opin­
ions as often as possible.
In order to avoid footnotes, the main text has been interspersed
with numerous “notes” where secondary comments and explanations
are to be found.
As a non-English speaker, I was in need of someone to correct
my grammar, style, spelling, and punctuation. Nicholas Postgate of
Trinity College, Cambridge University, proved to be the ideal adviser,
himself not unfamiliar with the problems of Sumerian grammar. He
not only offered innumerable corrections but quite often also guid­
ance, by pointing out that an argument was unclear, that a descrip­
tion was lacking evidence, or even that some paragraph was misplaced.
These corrections were made partly by mail and to a considerable
degree during a three day stay by the author at Trinity College and
at the home of the Postgates. For all this, my most sincere gratitude
is due to Nicholas.
The author gratefully acknowledges that he made frequent use of
Steve Tinney’s lexical “Index to the Secondary Literature. A col­
lated list of indexes and glossaries to the secondary literature con­
cerning the Sumerian Language” (Philadelphia 1993 ff.).
The Publishing House Brill and their Editors, Mevr. Tanja Cowall
and Mevr. Patricia Radder, have been extremely patient with the
author’s self-indulgent interpretation of the deadline originally set for
the publication of the book. They are, therefore, entitled to my heart­
felt gratitude.
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xii
xii 
Frau Ursula Hellwag MA of Munich most kindly took upon her­
self the trouble to compose the final draft of the manuscript which
the author, unused to the world of computers, had typed on his
beloved “Olympia”.
München, December 2002
xiii
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xiii
AfO
AHw.
AIUON
Ali Letters
AOAT
ARET
ARM
ARN
AS
ASJ
AWL
Bau B
BWL
CAD
CIRPL
CLAM
Copper and
Silver
Curse Akkade
Diri
DP
ABBREVIATIONS
= Archiv für Orientforschung, Berlin, 1923 ff.
= W. von Soden, Akkadisches Handwörterbuch, 1959–
1981.
= Annali dell ’Istituto Universitario Orientale di Napoli,
sezione linguistica, 1959 ff.
= F. A. Ali, Sumerian Letters: Two Collections from
the Old Babylonian Schools. (diss. Univ. of Pennsyl­
vania, 1964).
= Alter Orient und Altes Testament, 1969 ff.
= Archivi reali di Ebla, Testi, 1985 ff.
= Archives royales de Mari, 1950 ff.
= M. Çı</H. Kızılyay/F. R. Kraus, Altbabylonische
Rechtsurkunden aus Nippur, 1952.
= Assyriological Studies, 1931 ff.
= Acta Sumerologica, 1979 ff.
= J. Bauer, Altsumerische Wirtschaftstexte aus Lagasch
(= StudPohl 9, 1972).
= CT 36, 40 = ETCSL 4.02.2.
= W. G. Lambert, Babylonian Wisdom Literature,
1959.
= The Assyrian Dictionary of the University of
Chicago, 1956 ff.
= E. Sollberger, Corpus des inscriptions “royales”
présargoniques de Laga“, 1956.
= M. E. Cohen, The Canonical Lamentations of
Ancient Mesopotamia, 1988.
= Disputation, see ETCSL 5.3.6.
= J. S. Cooper, The Curse of Agade, 1983; = ETCSL
2.1.5.
= lexical series diri SI.A siàku = (w)atru.
= M. F. Alotte de la Fuyë, Documents présargoniques,
1908–1920.
Ean. = Eanatum in CIRPL.
EG = The Epic of Gilgame“.
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xiv
xiv \nnnryi\+ioxs
ELA
En. I
=
=
Enmerkar and the Lord of Aratta (S. Cohen,
1973); = ETCSL 1.8.2.3.
Enanatum I in CIRPL.
Enlil and Ninlil 2
EnmEns.
=
=
Enlil and Sud; = ETCSL 1.2.2.
Enmerkar and Ensu¢ke“edana, A. Berlin
1979; = ETCSL 1.8.2.4.
Ent.
Enz.
=
=
Entemena (Enmetena) in CIRPL.
Enentarzi in CIRPL.
Farmer’s Instructions = M. Civil, Aula Orientalis, Suppl. 5, 1984;
= ETCSL 5.6.3.
Father and Son = Å. Sjöberg, JCS 25 (1973) 105–169; =
ETCSL 5.1.2.
Forde Nebraska = N. W. Forde, Nebraska Cuneiform Texts of
GEN =
the Sumerian Ur III Dynasty, 1967.
Gilgame“, Enkidu and the Nether World; =
ETCSL 1.8.1.4.
Gilgame“ and Agga = D. Katz, Gilgamesh and Akka, 1993; =
ETCSL 1.8.1.1.
Gilgame“, Enkidu and the Netherworld s. GEN
Gilgame“ und
Huwawa A = D. O. Edzard, ZA 80 (1990) 165–203; id.,
Gilgame“ and
Huwawa B =
ZA 81 (1991) 165–233; = ETCSL 1.8.1.5.
D. O. Edzard, Bayer. Akad. der Wiss., Phil.-
Hist. Klasse, Sitzungsbericht 1993/4; =
ETCSL 1.8.1.5.1.
Gungunum = D. R. Frayne, RIME 4 (1990) 114 ff.
HSAO = Heidelberger Studien zum Alten Orient,
1967 ff.
HSM = Harvard Semitic Museum (tablet signature).
HSS = Harvard Semitic Studies, 1912 ff.
Inanna/Enki = G. Farber-Flügge, Der Mythos “Inanna und
Enki” unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der
Liste der me (= StudPohl 10, 1973); =
ETCSL 1.3.1.
Inanna’s Descent = W. R. Sladek, Inanna’s Descent to the Nether
World (diss. Baltimore, 1974); = ETCSL
1.4.1.
Innin “ag. = ¿. W. Sjöberg, in-nin-“à-gur
4
-ra. A Hymn
to the Goddess Inanna by the en-Priestess
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xv
xv \nnnryi\+ioxs
En¢eduanna, ZA 65 (1975) 161–253; = ETCSL
4.07.3.
Iraq = ( journal) Iraq, 1934 ff.
ITT = Inventaires des Tablettes de Tello, 1910–1921.
Jaques = M. Jaques, Le vocabulaire des sentiments dans
les textes sumériens (diss. Univ. de Genève n.d.
[1999]).
JCS = Journal of Cuneiform Studies, 1947 ff.
JNES = Journal of Near Eastern Studies, 1942 ff.
Ke“ Hymn = G. B. Gragg, The Ke“ Temple Hymn, in: ¿. W.
Sjöberg, The Collection of the Sumerian Temple
Hymns (= TCS 3, 1969) 157–188; = ETCSL
4.80.2.
LamSumUr = P. Michalowski, The Lamentation over the
Destruction of Sumer and Ur (= MesCiv. 1,
1989); = ETCSL 2.2.3.
Lament of Ur = S. N. Kramer, Lamentation over the Destruction
of Ur (= AS 12, 1940); = ETCSL 2.2.2.
LamUr I s. Lament of Ur.
Lugal = J. van Dijk, LUGAL UD ME-LÁM-bi NIR-
GÁL, 1983; = ETCSL 1.6.2.
Lugalbanda I s. C. Wilcke, RIA 7 (1987–90) 121–125; ETCSL 1.8.2.1.
Lugalbanda II s. C. Wilcke, ibid., 125–129; ETCSL 1.8.2.2.
Lugalbanda and Enm. see Lugalbanda I.
MAD = Materials for the Assyrian Dictionary, 1952 ff.
Martu A = A. Falkenstein, SGL I (1959) 120–140; = ETCSL
4.12.1.
MBI = G. A. Barton, Miscellaneous Babylonian Inscrip­
tions I, 1918.
MCS = Manchester Cuneiform Studies, 1951 ff.
MDP = Mémoires de la Délégation en Perse, 1900 ff.
MEE = Materiali epigrafici di Ebla, 1979 ff.
MSL = Materialien zum sumerischen Lexikon/Materials
for the Sumerian Lexicon, 1937 ff.
Nanna’s Journey = A. J. Ferrara, Nanna-Suen’s Journey to Nippur
(= StudPohl SM 2, 1973); = ETCSL 1.5.1.
NATN = D. I. Owen, Neo-Sumerian Archival Texts pri­
marily from Nippur, 1982.
NBGT = Neo-Babylonian Grammatical Texts, in: MSL 4
(1956) 129 ff.
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xvi
xvi \nnnryi\+ioxs
NG = A. Falkenstein, Die neusumerischen Gerichtsurkunden
(= Bayer. Akademie der Wiss., Philos.-hist. Klasse,
Abhandl. NF 39, 40, 44, 1956–1957).
Nik. = M. V. Nikolskij, Drevnosti Vosto‘nyja III/2, 1908.
Ninme“ara = A. Zgoll, Der Rechtsfall der En-hedu-Ana im Lied
nin-me-“ara (= AOAT 246, 1997).
Ninurta G = M. E. Cohen 1975 (see bibl.); = ETCSL 4.27.07.
NRVN = M. Çı</H. Kızılyay, Neusumerische Rechts- und Ver­
waltungsurkunden aus Nippur, 1965.
NS = Neue Serie, New Series, Nova Series.
NS = Neo-Sumerian.
OB = Old Babylonian.
OBGT = Old Babylonian Grammatical Texts, in: MSL 4 (1956)
46 ff.
OLZ = Orientalistische Literaturzeitung, 1898 ff.
Or. = Orientalia (NS), 1932 ff.
OrSuec. = Orientalia Suecana, 1952 ff.
OS = Old Sumerian.
PBS = University of Pennsylvania, Publications of the Baby­
lonian Section, 1911 ff.
PN = Personal Name.
PSD = The Sumerian Dictionary of the University Museum
of the University of Pennsylvania, 1984 ff.
RA = Revue d’assyriologie et d’archéologie orientale, 1886 ff.
RGTC = Répertoire géographique des textes cunéiformes, 1974 ff.
RIME = The Royal Inscriptions of Mesopotamia, Early Periods,
1990 ff.
Rìm-Sin D = D. Charpin, Le clergé d’Ur au siècle d’Hammurabi,
1986, 282–86; = ETCSL 2.6.9.4.
RIA = Reallexikon der Assyriologie (und Vorderasiatischen
Archäologie), 1928 ff.
RSO = Rivista degli Studi Orientali, 1907 ff.
SBH = G. Reisner, Sumerisch-babylonische Hymnen . . ., 1896.
Schooldays = S. N. Kramer, JAOS 69 (1949) 199–215; = ETCSL
5.1.1.
Scribe and Son see Father and Son.
Sin-kà“id = D. R. Frayne, RIME 4, 440–64.
SKIZ = W. H. Ph. Römer, Sumerischen “Königshymnen” der
Isin-Zeit, 1965.
SLTNi. = S. N. Kramer, Sumerian Literary Texts from Nippur,
1944.
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page xvii
\nnnryi\+ioxs xvii
S – O – V = Subject – Object – Verb.
SP = Series Prior.
SP = E. I. Gordon, Sumerian Proverbs, 1959.
SR = D. O. Edzard, Sumerische Rechtsurkunden
des III. Jahrtausends . . . (= Bayer. Akademie
der Wissenschaften, Abhandlung NF 67,
1968).
Stat. = Statue.
SRT = E. Chiera, Sumerian Religious Texts, 1924.
StudPohl = Studia Pohl, 1967 ff.
STVC = E. Chiera, Sumerian Texts of Varied Contents
(= OIP 16, 1934).
”ulgi A = Klein 1981, 167–217; = ETCSL 2.4.2.01.
”ulgi B = G. R. Castellino, (StudSem. 42, 1972); =
ETCSL 2.4.2.02.
”ulgi D = Klein 1981, 50–123; = ETCSL 2.4.2.04.
”ulgi F = M. G. Hall, A Study of the Sumerian Moon-
God . . . (diss. Philadelphia, 1985); = ETCSL
2.4.2.06.
”ulgi X = Klein 1981, 124–66; = ETCSL 2.4.2.24.
TCL = Textes cunéiformes du Louvre, 1910 ff.
TCS = Texts from Cuneiforms Sources, 1966 ff.
Three Men of
Adab = Alster 1981–1983; = ETCSL 5.6.5.
TIM = Texts in the Iraq Museum, 1964 ff.
TLB = Tabulae Cuneiformes a F. M. Th. de Liagre
Böhl collectae, 1954 ff.
TM = Tell Mardikh (Ebla) (tablet signature).
TMH (NF) = Texte und Materialien der Frau Professor
Hilprecht Collection . . ., 1932–1934; NF =
1937, 1961 ff.
TrDr. = H. de Genouillac, La trouvaille de Dréhem,
1911.
TS” = R. Jestin, Tablettes sumériennes de ”urup­
pak . . . , 1937.
TUT = G. Reisner, Tempelurkunden aus Telloh,
1901.
UET = Ur Excavations, Textes, 1928 ff.
Ukg. = Urukagina (Erikagina) in CIRPL.
Ur Lament see Lement of Ur.
Ur-Namma Code = Wilcke 2002.
EDZARD_f1_v-xviii 4/30/03 10:42 AM Page xviii
xviii \nnnryi\+ioxs
Ur-Namma’s Death = E. Flückiger-Hawker, Urnamma of Ur in
Sumerian Literary Tradition (= OBO 166,
1999), 93–182; = ETCSL 2.4.1.1.
Ur-Ninurta = D. R. Frayne, RIME 4 (1990) 64–68.
VS = Vorderasiatische Schriftdenkmäler der (könig­
lichen) Museen zu Berlin, 1907 ff.
WO = Die Welt des Orients, 1947 ff.
WF = A. Deimel, Wirtschaftstexte aus Fara (=
WVDOG 45, 1924).
WVDOG = Wissenschaftliche Veröffentlichungen der
Deutschen Orient-Gesellschaft, 1900 ff.
YOS = Yale Oriental Series, Babylonian Texts,
1915 ff.
ZA = Zeitschrift für Assyriologie und verwandte
Gebiete, 1886–1938, or: und Vorderasiatische
Archäologie, 1939 ff.
1
EDZARD_f2_1-5 4/28/03 9:36 AM Page 1
CHAPTER ONE
THE SUMERIAN LANGUAGE
Für Olympia, die mir seit 1954 alle meine Arbeiten
geschrieben hat und die mir auf drei Kontinenten
gefolgt ist.
1.1. Grxrn\r Cn\n\c+rnis+ics
Sumerian was called eme-ki-en-gi-ra “tongue of Kiengir (Sumer)” or
eme-gi
7
(-r) in Sumerian proper and li“àn ”umeri(m) “tongue of ”umeru”
in Akkadian.
The replacing of [“] by [s] in most modern languages (but not
Russian) has its analogue in the change of [“] for [s] in names of
the Hebrew bible.
Sumerian is characterized by the interaction of a word base (nom­
inal, verbal, other) which may be invariable or subject to variation
(e.g., change of vowel, reduction, extension), and an intricate system
of prefixed and suffixed morphemes. The word base itself is impen­
etrable by other morphemes. Unlike Semitic, no infixes occur. Cf.
ha-ma-ab-“úm-mu [ha-m+a-b-“um-e] “he should give it to me” (WO
8, 173: 11b2): precative-to-me-it-base give-ergative (3
rd
person sing.
person class). The number of prefixed morphemes varies between
zero and six for the verb, zero and one for the noun; the number
of suffixed morphemes between zero and three for the verb, zero
and three for the noun. Words of considerable length may be built
up that way, e.g., hu-mu-na-ni-ib-gi
4
-gi
4
“let him return it to him
there” (6 syllables, not comparable, however, with Akkadian
ittanablakkatùnikkunù“im “they will, over and again, revolt against you”,
10 syllables).
In both strings of morphemes, prefixed or suffixed, the sequence of
the individual elements is unchangeable. The morphemes are mostly
monofunctional, as is the rule in agglutinating languages, and very
rarely multifunctional as the morphemes of Semitic or Indo-European.
Instead of gender, Sumerian distinguishes a “person” and a “non­
person” class. The case system includes an ergative, and the verbal
EDZARD_f2_1-5 4/28/03 9:36 AM Page 2
2 cn\r+rn oxr
inflection is characterized by ergativity (whether there was, or evolved,
“split ergativity” is still a matter of debate—see below 12.7.5). There
is well developed, but far from perfect, concord between dimen­
sional suffixes of the noun and dimensional indicators prefixed to
the verb, e.g., DN-ra object mu-na(-n)-§rú “to god NN, object, he-
built-to-him”.
As for number, singular and non-singular may be opposed; there
are different ways to express plurality.
The general word order of Sumerian is S – O – V, unless some
part of speech is taken to the front for focus.
The nominal or verbal base is the essential carrier of meaning,
and only bases are listed as entries in the lexical lists. Occasionally,
meaning is modified by the occurrence of a “frozen” morpheme. Also,
the composition of two (rarely more) nominal word bases may lead
to a new meaning beyond the sum of the meanings of the individ­
ual parts of the compound, e.g., é-gal “house big” = “palace” (Akk.
èkallu) or má-tur “boat small”, a special type of boat (Akk. maturru).
In the light of these general aspects, Sumerian may be compared
to such languages as Georgian, Basque, or Itelmen and many oth­
ers. Such comparison is, however, purely structural and of no con­
sequence for the question of the linguistic affiliation of Sumerian.
1.2. Tnr (norrrrss) otrs+iox or +nr rixotis+ic
\rriri\+iox or Stvrni\x
Scholars have wasted much effort looking for living cognates of
ancient Sumerian, not realizing that the problem is practically insol­
uble for the following reasons:
Sumerian must have separated from a hypothetical language fam­
ily of which it was part in the middle or late fourth millennium B.C.
at the latest. We know next to nothing about the sound and struc­
ture of Sumerian before the middle of the third millennium. Thus
there is a gap of at least two thousand years between that time and
the oldest reconstructible form of any of the languages which have
been compared to Sumerian (e.g., Turkish, Hungarian, Sino-Tibetan).
Efforts to find cognates have been exclusively based on the sound of
individual words. Yet according to W. Deeters (1963, 76) who dis­
cussed the problem of Basque-Caucasian affinities, any words in lan­
EDZARD_f2_1-5 4/28/03 9:36 AM Page 3
3 +nr stvrni\x r\xot\or
guages A and B that sound alike today are more likely to be unre­
lated than related because they are the product of phonetic evolu­
tion over several millennia—not to mention the possible diachronical
change of meaning. While according to Deeters, if contemporane­
ous language X were really a descendant from a language Y, related
to *Proto-Sumerian more than five or six millennia ago, the sound
structure and vocabulary of that hypothetical language Y are liable
to have become altered beyond recognition.
The only essay going beyond the comparison of Sumerian and
another language by way of vocabulary is G. Steiner’s “Sumerisch
und Elamisch: Typologische Parallelen”, ASJ 12 (1990) 143–76.
Steiner stresses structural similarities in the case system, pronominal
system, verb (“intransitiv-passivische Verbalauffassung”), word order
(S – O – V), nominalization of the verbal complex, and in his sum­
mary he arrives at the cautious statement that “diese beiden Sprachen
trotz ihrer sehr unterschiedlichen morphologischen Struktur zu einer
‘Sprachgruppe’ zusammengefaßt werden können”. An essential differ­
ence between Sumerian and Elamite is, however, the fact that the
Sumerian verb base is, as a rule, embedded in a string of prefixes
and suffixes whereas Elamite almost exclusively uses only suffixes.
In any case, even if Sumerian and Elamite were really (remote)
relatives, the general problem of the linguistic affinity of Sumerian
would remain unresolved.
1.3. Tnr rixotis+ic rxyinoxvrx+ or Stvrni\x
From since at least the end of the fourth millennium, Sumerians
were neighbours of the Elamites in Elam and of Semites, both seden­
tary and nomadic, in Mesopotamia proper. Any attempt at extend­
ing this picture must rest on speculation for lack of solid proof. The
earliest evidence for the Elamite language stems from clay tablets
with “Proto-Elamite” script whose find spots extend as far as Tepe
Ya˙yà, ca 900 km ESE of Susa (corresponding to the distance between
Uruk and Damascus as the crow flies). Although the “Proto-Elamite”
script has not yet been convincingly deciphered, it seems plausible
to assume that it was a predecessor of the Elamite linear script of
the Akkade period which has been shown to represent the Elamite
language.
EDZARD_f2_1-5 4/30/03 10:43 AM Page 4
4 cn\r+rn oxr
It cannot be excluded that, within Mesopotamia proper, Sumerian
had neighbours who spoke a language—or languages—that were,
step by step, superseded by Sumerian, but which left their traces in
Sumerian proper names (gods, places) and vocabulary. Thus, e.g.,
divine names such as Nan“e or Ω Gatumdu, goddesses at home in the
ΩGirsu/Laga“ region, may belong to a substratum, or adstratum,
because these names defy all efforts to explain them by way of
Sumerian etymology. Our judgement in this matter is, however,
highly subjective because we know nothing of the early history of
Sumerian and its sound structure. In fact, our first tentative iden­
tification of Sumerian “sound” hardly goes farther back than 24th
century B.C., and the publication of the Ebla glosses for Sumerian
lexical items brought more than one surprise.
For some time, a “monosyllabic myth” has been popular among
Sumerologists, relegating words of more than one syllable to a “Proto-
Euphratian” substratum.
Cf., e.g., B. Landsberger 1944, apud A. Salonen 1968, 31.
See, however, the very sobering discussion of G. Rubio 1999, 1–16,
“On the alleged ‘pre-Sumerian’ substratum” where the author arrives
at the conclusion (p. 11), “Thus, there is no monolithic substratum
that would have left, in a sort of primeval age, its vestiges in Sumerian
lexicon. All one can detect is a complex and fuzzy web of borrow­
ings whose directions are frequently difficult to determine”.
Nan“e and ª Gatumdu (and others) may be pre-Sumerian names,
as may many place names. But we have no means at our disposal
to prove such a supposition. It is a well known fact that proper
names are specially prone to changes of all kind (slurring, abbrevi­
ation, deformation by analogy, popular etymology).
Hurrians—with a language of their own—first appear in cuneiform
sources toward the end of the third millennium B.C. They most
probably never were immediate neighbours of the Sumerians, and
so direct language contact can be excluded, at least during the cen­
turies before the Third Dynasty of Ur.
In general, it may be said that the Mesopotamian plain was not
conducive to a great variety of languages, as against Iran, Anatolia
or the Caucasus which, until our days, has been a veritable lan­
guage museum.
As a consequence of early close contacts between Sumerians and
Semites, a situation arose which greatly stimulated mutual influences.
Sumerian was heading for a Sumero-Akkadian “linguistic area”—
EDZARD_f2_1-5 4/28/03 9:36 AM Page 5
5 +nr stvrni\x r\xot\or
and so was Akkadian. Quite a few occurrences of apparent “de-
Sumerization” or Akkadization of the language have led some schol­
ars to the opinion that Sumerian had ceased to be a living spoken
language as early as the end of the third millennium. The “areal”
situation will be discussed more in detail below in Chapter 17.
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
7
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/30/03 10:23 AM Page 7
CHAPTER TWO
HOW WE READ SUMERIAN
2.1. G
One may say that we see Sumerian through an Akkadian glass darkly,
because the values (“Lautwerte”) of nearly all signs used in the
Sumerian syllabaries of different places and periods have been identified
by way of Akkadian syllabic spellings or—additionally—from the so-
called tu-ta-ti syllabaries. Since Akkadian did not express in its explana­
tory glosses more than four vowel phonemes, each short or long, i.e.,
[a, à, e, è, i, ì, u, ù], we are restricted to that set also for Sumerian.
Whilst, as regards vowel quantity, some arguments may be adduced
for the existence of an opposition between short and long vowels
[v : 9v], no means have so far been found to achieve a precise recon­
struction of Sumerian vowel qualities. The minimal set would be A,
E/I, U, with A and U clearly distinguished in spelling while for E/I
very often there is the same ambivalence as in Akkadian spelling.
Note: A. Poebel suggested the existence of Akkadian [o] (AS 9, 1939, 116 f. with
fnn. 1 and 1(!)), followed by St. Lieberman “The Phoneme /o/ in Sumerian”
(Fs. T. B. Jones [1977] 21–28, = AOAT 203) and Aa. Westenholz who extended
the evidence for “The Phoneme /o/ in Akkadian” (ZA 81 [1991] 10–19).
Being unable, however, to reconstruct Sumerian [o] from spelling or
spelling variants, we disregard it throughout the present grammar.
Spelling variants in parallel texts (synchronic or diachronic) some­
times prove a precious source for phonology, as do scribal “errors”.
Apart from lexical glosses, an important source for the sounds of
Sumerian are loanwords in Akkadian and in a restricted number of
case sign names: kar > kàrum (not *karrum) “quay, mooring place”
points to a long [à] in kar, i.e., [kàr], whereas é-gal > èkallum (not
*èkàlum) suggests short [a] in gal, i.e., *[kal]. The sign name of HI,
DÙG is du-ú-gu [dùgu], not *du-ug-gu *[duggu] which makes us
prefer a long [ù] in Sumerian dùg, du
10
(-g), i.e., [dùg]. See more in
detail below, 3.1.
Just as for the vowels, the identification of Sumerian consonants
depends essentially on Akkadian evidence: glosses in syllabaries and
vocabularies, the behaviour of loanwords and, partly again, sign
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 8
8 cn\r+rn +vo
names. The reconstruction of the hypothetical inventory of Sumerian
consonantal phonemes is, however, much more difficult than in the
case of the vowels. Comparison of èkallu (é-gal) “palace” (above) and
unetukku (ù-ne-e-dug
4
) “letter” show that, at least in our Latin translit­
erations, unvoiced stops (K, T) in the Akkadian loanwords corre­
spond to voiced stops (G, D) in our rendering of the Sumerian basic
expressions. This difference in our transliteration is due to the fact
that the oldest Akkadian syllabary known at present, Proto-Ea (MSL
14 [1979] 3–81), offers voiced instead of unvoiced stops: du-ú: KA
(p. 44:308), ga-la: GAL (p. 50:471).
The complicated phonetic relation between Sumerian and Akkadian
consonants (again: as we transliterate them) has given rise to much
speculation as to whether there was a double or a triple set of stops
in Sumerian: D : T or D : T
1
: T
2
, etc., the unvoiced part being
either a single phoneme or split into two, e.g., simple and post-glot-
talized (T, T’). This question will be discussed in more detail, and
with tables, below pp. 15 f.
2.2. Srrrrixo or Stvrni\x
2.2.1. Classification of cuneograms
Sumerian is written, in its “classical” form of the Gudea and Ur III
period, by means of five classes of cuneiform signs. This distinction
is not, though, visible, but only based on context.
a) Logograms or word signs, expressing a nominal or verbal word
base, e.g., lú “person”, mu “name”, “year”, dùg “good”, “sweet (said
of water)”, -zu “your” (sing.), ba “to attribute”.
b) Syllabograms or syllabic signs, used to convey a sound only,
without primary reference to meaning, e.g., ba- (verbal prefix), mu-
(verbal prefix), -ke
4
(nominal suffix, comprising the final [k] of the
genitive morpheme [ak] and the [e] of the ergative case); gu-za
“chair”.
c) Phonetic indicators, a sub-class of (b), i.e., syllabograms used to
specify the reading of a single sign (or of a sign group). So, in
GI”.TÚG.PI = ∞gé“tug, the signs GI” und TÚG yield the reading
ge“tug], namely ∞ of PI [∞ gé“tug. It is a matter of convention whether
gé“tug or as
gi“-túg
∞ we transliterate GI”.TÚG.PI as ∞ ge“tug with two
phonetic indicators raised.
d) Signs for number or the combined notation of measuring unit +
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 9
9 nov vr nr\r stvrni\x
number, e.g., min “two”, banmin “two ban” = 2 × 1 bán = 2 × 10
sila (1 sila = about 1 litre).
e) Determinatives: these are signs which precede or follow words
or names in order to specify them as belonging to semantic groups.
Determinatives can be proven not to have been pronounced (although
doubt may exist in specific instances), and they are raised in our
transliteration: AN.EN.LÍL =
d
En-líl “the god Enlil”; EN.LÍL.KI =
EN.LÍL
ki
= Nibru
ki
“the city Nippur” (Enlil’s main cult centre). The
determinatives are:
AN (or DIªGIR), preceding divine names; abbreviated as raised
d
for d(eus), d(ea).
KI (ki “place, earth”), following place names, e.g., Unug
ki
“Uruk”.
ÍD (íd “river, canal”), preceding names of rivers or main canals, e.g.,
íd
Idigina “Tigris”; but note íd-éren-na “army canal” where íd is part
of the name.
GE” (∞ ª ge“ “wood, tree”), preceding terms for wooden objects or
∞ ∞
names of trees, e.g.,
ge“
ban“ur = pa““ùru “table”,
ge“
al = allu “hoe”.
The Akkadian loanwords clearly show that ª GE” cannot be part of
the word. But note in contrast ∞ ge“-ùr “beam” with the Akkadian
loanword form gu“ùru showing that ∞ ge“ is part of the word.
URUDU (erida, eridu, urudu “copper”), preceding terms of met­
als and metal objects, e.g.,
urudu
za-rí-in (zarinnu) (mediocre quality of
metal, mainly of copper).
DUG (dug “vessel”), preceding terms of earthenware, e.g.,
dug
a-da =
gur
5
= adagurru (vessel with pointed bottom).
KU
6
(ku
6
“fish”), following the names of fish, e.g., suhur-má“
ku
6
=
su¢urmà“u “goat-fish” (mythological being), “capricorn” (constellation).
MU”EN (mu“en “bird”), following the names of birds, e.g., tu
mu“en
“pigeon”.
LÚ (lú “person”), preceding names of some (male) professions, e.g.,

nu-kiri
6
= nukaribbu “gardener”.
“úm
SAR (sar “vegetable”), following the names of garden plants, e.g.,
sar
= “ùmu “garlic”.
Ú (ú “grass”, “plant”), preceding names of plants, e.g.,
ú
búr-da =
urnû “mint”(?).
GI (gi “reed”), preceding reeds and objects made of reed, e.g.,
ge“
.

gi
pisan = pis/“annu “box”, “container”, also determined by
NA
4
(na
4
“stone”), preceding names of stones and stone objects,
e.g.,
na
4
nunuz = erimmatu (egg-stone, a bead).
TÚG (túg “textile, garment”), preceding names of cloth or gar­
ments, e.g.,
túg
NÍG.LÁM = lam¢u““û, (a long or knee-length skirt).
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 10
10 cn\r+rn +vo
KU” (ku“ “skin, hide, leather”), preceding leather objects, e.g.,
ku“
lu-úb = luppu “(leather) bag”.
”E (“e “barley”) and ZÍD (zíd “flour”) are occasionally used as
determinatives for types of grain or flour respectively.
Whereas the use of some determinatives is rather consistent and more
or less predictable (at least from the Akkade period onward), this
ki ge

“ mu“en ku
6
. does not apply to others. To the first category belong
d
, , , ,
To resume the five sign classes, we have to stress once more that
their distinction is nearly exclusively based on context: ba or mu
may belong to (a) or (b), ∞ge“ to (a), (c), or (e). Even number signs (d)
are not exempt from ambiguity, because, e.g., E” (3 × U) may mean
“thirty” (d), but also be used as a syllabogram (b) in (mainly Old
Bab.) -me-e“ [me“]; note also i (= ia = 5 × A”) or à“ (= 6 × DI”).
2.2.2. Combination of cuneograms; spelling proper
In “standard” (Ur III, Old Bab.) Sumerian spelling, the nominal or
verbal base is frequently noted by a logogram (type a), whereas
accompanying (prefixed or suffixed) morphemes are expressed by syl­
labograms (see 2.2.1, type b), e.g., al-tu“ “he was sitting there” where
prefix al- is a syllabogram, base tu“ a logogram.
A special feature of Sumerian spelling is the “repetition” of the
final consonant of a logogram by the initial consonant of a follow­
ing syllabogram to indicate simply the addition of a vowel. So, “in
Ur”, [Urim-a] is not spelled *Úrim
ki
-a, but Úrim
ki
-ma. The [m] of
the syllabogram [ma] is redundant, the sign conveying simply the
[a] of the locative case. Here there is no reason to suppose conso-
nantal length. The phenomenon is purely orthographic.
Note: This way of spelling has, occasionally, found its way into Akkadian. When,
in Old Akk. or Old Bab., the scribe turned the verbal form i-din [iddin] “he
gave” into ventive [iddinam] “he gave (it) to me”, he wrote i-din-nam. See F. R.
Kraus, RSO 32 (1957) 103–108.
We traditionally read the main temple complex of Uruk, É-an-na
“House of Heaven/An”, as Eanna where Sumerian spelling is reflected
in our Latin transcription. There is, in fact, no reason to read -nn-.
Eana would correspond to what is meant in Sumerian.
2.2.3. Evolution of Sumerian spelling
The progress achieved during a period of at least one millennium
may be roughly described as a continuous advance towards phonetic
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 11
11 nov vr nr\r stvrni\x
exactitude, i.e., the exact rendering of every spoken syllable. Writing
Sumerian started from only noting bases, numerals, and combina­
tions of numbers + measure.
This “nuclear” writing (Th. Jacobsen, ZA 52 [1957] 91 ff. fn. 1)
still disregarded any additional morphemes (nominal and verbal
prefixes and suffixes). Phonetic abstraction (writing, e.g., gi both for
“reed” and the syllables [gi, ge] and, in slightly varied form (GI­
gunû) for the notion of “return” [ge
4
]) opened the way for noting
syllables of the types [V], [CV], [VC], [CVC]; see 2.2.1, type b.
“Syllabaries” (= inventories of syllabograms) came into being. Some
syllabograms were freely applicable, i.e., they could occur in any
position of a word (initial, medial, final) whereas others were of
restricted use; é“ is mainly used, in Ur III and early Old Bab., to
denote the 3
rd
p. pl. suffix on certain verbal forms.
Note: Neither Sumerian nor Akkadian syllabaries offer a clear 1 : 1 relation of
signs and sounds. On the one hand, one sign may denote different syllables, e.g.,
NE = ne, dè, bí, and on the other hand, identical or minimally different sylla-
ble-sounds could be noted by different signs, e.g., [a“] = a“ or á“, [en] = en or
èn (LI).
One of the main problems was the notation of syllable-closing con­
sonants in syllables of the type CVC. Here, the inventory was
insufficient (signs like bam, mag, nal, etc. were never created). At
first, a syllable-closing consonant was just disregarded, e.g., ba-ug
7
-
ge “they died” stood for [ba’uge“, ba-u-ge“]. With lugal-me, only
context could show whether lugal-me(“) “they are kings” or lugal-
me(n) “I am/you are king” was meant. Until Ur III, and partly still
in OB, the person or non-person class ergative or absolutive markers
-n- or -b-, placed immediately before the verbal base, were left un-
noted, because they always were found in a close syllable; mu-na-
rú “he/she built for him/her” stood for [mu-na-n-§ § rú]. Therefore,
reconstruction of a given verbal form often depends on our—sub-
jective—interpretation.
The decisive invention to remedy the situation was made by a
scribe—or scribal school—of pre-Sargonic times, who combined CV
1
+
V
1
C to denote CV
1
C, e.g., mu-un for [mun]. The Akkadian rather
than Sumerian scribal world must be credited with this invention—
unique in the world history of writing—because in Akkadian with
its frequent three-consonantal roots non-notation of a syllable-closing
consonant would have led to much more ambiguity than in Sumerian.
12
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 12
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
13
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/30/03 10:23 AM Page 13
CHAPTER THREE
MINIMALIA OF SUMERIAN PHONOLOGY AND
SYLLABIC STRUCTURE
3.1. P
3.1.1. Vowels
[a]: al (Akk. allu) “hoe”
gag (Akk. kakku) “peg, nail”, “club, weapon”
gala (Akk. kalû) “singer (in cult), cantor”
bárag (Akk. parakku) “dais”
(hé-)∞gál (Akk. he(n)gallu) “let it be” = “abundance”
[à]: kàr (Akk. kàru) “quay, mooring wall, harbour”
nàr (Akk. nàru, Ass. nuàru) “singer, musician”
àn (Akk. ànu) “sky, heaven”, “God of Heaven”
[e]: mu“en, mu-“e-en-nu [mu“ennu] “bird”, sign name for HU,
MU”EN
[è]: èn (Akk. ènu) “en priest”
[i]: apin (Akk. epinnu) “plough”
bukin (Akk. bukinnu) “trough”
sikil, in “úm-sikil (Akk. “umsikillu) “garlic”
[ì]: kìd (Akk. kìtu) “reed mat”
[u]: dub (Akk. †uppu) “tablet”
hu“, ¢u-u“-“u [¢u““u], sign name for HU”
[ù]: nùn,
d
E
4
-nun-na (Akk. *E/Anùnakù)
Note: Not
d
A.NUN.NA = *Anunnakù; the Old Bab. contracted form Enukkù can
be explained only as the product of -n(a)k- > -kk-, and not of *-nn(a)k- > -kk-.
bùr (Akk. pùru) “(watertight) vessel”
The question of Akkadian (Old Akk. and Old Bab.) [o] and [ò]
has been discussed by Westenholz (see above 2.1 note).
The existence of an (original) diphthong [ay] in Sumerian may be
inferred from the Hebrew loan form of Akkadian èkallu (< é-gal): hè§al
<*haykal (also Arabic haykal “temple, structure”). The Sumerian word
for “father”, OS A (as in A-kur-gal “the father is the great mountain”),
OB a-a or a-ya (cf. PSD A/1, 32 ff.), leads one to suppose the form
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 14
14  
[ay], [aya] (only the first, however, being a diphthong strictly speak­
ing). The minimal inventory of Sumerian vocalic phonemes would
then read: a, à; e, è; i, ì; u, ù; (o, ò)
Two notes are, however, needed with regard to our reconstruction:
(a) It is not always clear, from the spelling, whether [e] or [i] was
intended. In the (non-standard) spelling of a curse formula (copy
of a ”ulgi inscription), en hé-a “whether he can be an en” is
replaced by in he-a (TIM 9, 35:19). We are often at a loss
whether we should transliterate NI = ni or né, BI = bi or bé.
(b) Instead of a quantitative opposition short: long, there may have
been an opposition of open and closed vowels (such as is the
case in modern German).
Ideas of how Sumerian should be pronounced certainly changed dia­
chronically among the learned community of scribes. So, e.g., KU =
dab
5
(“to seize”) is glossed da-ab in Proto-Ea 19 (MSL 14, 30), but
di-ib in Ea I 156 (MSL 14, 184), yielding our transliteration díb.
Cf. correspondingly, PA = sàg (“to strike”), glossed sà-ag in Proto-
Ea 490 (MSL 14, 51), but s[i]-ig in Ea I 298 (MSL 14, 191).
It is not clear to the author how this change of vowel came about.
3.1.2. Consonants
When trying to establish the (minimal) set of Sumerian consonantal
phonemes, we will once more base ourselves on loanwords in Akkadian
and on sign-names as the most reliable sources.
For stops (labial, dental, velar), there are three possible types of
relation between a Sumerian word (as rendered in the translitera­
tion we derive from Proto-Ea and later lexical sources) and the cor­
responding Akkadian loanword:
a) labial stops
a1) P' : P apin : epinnu “seeder plough”
pisan : pis/“annu “box”
a2) B : P barag : parakku “dais”
bala : palû “term of office”
dub : †uppu “tablet”
Note: For P' etc. see Gelb 1961
2
, 39 (below).
a3) B : B not attested
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 15
15 vixiv\ri\ or stvrni\x rnoxoroov
b) dental stops
b1) T' : T not attested
b2) D : T du : tuppu “tablet”
kid : kìtu “reed mat”
ù-ne(-e)-dug
4
: unetukku “letter”
b3) D : D é-duru
5
(A) : edurû “village”
sa∞g-dili : SAG-di-lu-û “(single head =) bachelor”
c) velar stops
c1) K' : K kar : kàru “quay”
kiri
6
: kirû “palm grove”
kid : kìtu “reed mat”
c2) G : K gala : kalû “cantor”
engar : ikkaru “tenant farmer”
é-gal : èkallu “palace”
barag : parakku “dais”
ù-ne(-e)-dug
4
: unetukku “letter”
c3) G : G ga-na : gana “hey, now then”
aga : agû “tiara”
Gelb 1961
2
, 39 dealt with the relation of Sumerian stops and Akkadian
spelling before, in, and after the Old Bab. period. He started from
a binary system, b/p : p', d/t : t', g/k : k', assuming an original
opposition of indiscriminate voiced/unvoiced versus unvoiced post­
glottalized stops. Gelb supposed a “sound shift” to have taken place
in Old Bab., and he visualized his theory in a chart:
Before Old Bab. Old Bab. Old Bab. and Later
Written Phoneme Sound Sound Shift Phoneme Sound
BA b/p p p > b b b
PA p' p' p' > p p p
DA d/t t t > d d d
TA t' t' t' > t t t
GA g/k k k > g g g
KA k' k' k' > k k k
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 16
16  
Gelb’s chart does not take into account the more complicated, triple,
relations revealed by Sumerian words and their loans in Akkadian.
While we will not be able definitely to prove our theory, we may at
least point to the fact that a triple (or even more ample) set of stops
is well attested in world languages, e.g., unvoiced : voiced : unvoiced
post-glottalized/unvoiced velarized; or unvoiced unaspirated : voiced
unaspirated : unvoiced aspirated. We will only give two examples:
1) Akkadian: p/b/-; t/d/ˇ; k/g/˚ (Q ).
Here, ˇ and Q cannot be defined more closely, because we are
unable to say whether ˇ and ˚/Q were post-glottalized stops (as
in modern Ethiopic languages) or velarized stops (as in Arabic).
2) Georgian: p/b/p’; t/d/t’; k/g/k’.
p’, t’, k’ are here post-glottalized stops.
For a more than quadruple set, cf. Sanskrit p/ph/b/bh; t/th/d/dh;
k/kh/g/gh; with additionals cerebral stops †/†h/∂/∂h.
Despite this demonstration of possibilities, we have to admit that
most aspects of the phonetics and phonology of Sumerian stops
remain subject to doubt.
Apart from stops, we depend to a very high degree on Akkadian
evidence for the other phonemes. What is offered below may only
be part of a more developed system.
There are three nasals: labial [m], dental [n], and palatal(?) [∞g]:
m: zà-mi : sammû (“praise”) “harp”
lu-lim : lulìmu “stag” (Kulturwort?)
n: na­§rú-a : narû “erected stone” (with inscription), “stele”
ù-ne(-e)-dug
4
: unetukku “letter”
eren : erènu “cedar” (Kulturwort)
Note: As for final [M] and [N], the loanword evidence is sometimes in contra­
diction with Sumerian spelling and/or lexical glosses. In spite of ezeN : isinnu
“festival”, the Ur III genitive of ezeN is indicated by -(m)a; cf. ní∞ g-ezeN-ma “fes­
tival accessories”. AN is used both for the name of the “sky (god)”, An, genitive
an-na, and for the copula, -am
6
, spelled A.AN = àm from Sargonic times onward
(A.AN being contracted from contextual -a’am = nominalizer -a + copula [m]).
∞g: The transliteration of this phoneme was first proposed by J. Krecher,
HSAO 1 (1967) 87 fn.*, and further by him in Fs. L. Matou“ II
(1978) 7–73: “Das sumerische Phonem ∞g”. Its spelling characteristics
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 17
17 vixiv\ri\ or stvrni\x rnoxoroov
are mainly use of the syllabograms GÁ (∞ ge
26
), ÁG (á∞ gi
6
). gá, ∞ g), MI (∞
Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian often reflect [∞g] by spelling -n- or
-ng- (see below). Moreover, there is a [∞ g]: [M] correspondence in
gá-ra “to me” : Ma-ra; ∞ Emesal words, e.g., ∞ gál “exist” : Ma-al.
[∞ g] occurs in all three positions of the words: initial, medial, final,
thus behaving in complete accordance with other consonantal
phonemes.
∞guru“ “adult male” : Ebla sign name nu-rí-“úm (MEE 3 [1981]
198: 46)
gal “wide”, glossed da-∞ da∞ gál
bala∞g “lyre(?)” : balangu, balaggu
hur-sa∞g “mountain range” : ¢ur“ànu
g], there also is the combination [∞ Besides [∞ g+g], and there may
g] and [∞ have been a difference between [∞ gg] or [∞gg] comparable to ∞
that between English singer and finger.
Cf. engar “tenant farmer”, glossed en-ga-ar, Akkad. ikkaru; nan =
gar “carpenter”, glossed na(-an)-ga-ar, Akkad. nangàru, naggàru, where
the gloss each time has GA = ga.
The identification of [∞g] as a palatal nasal would logically com­
plete a nasal series m, n, x (= ∞g).
A different description was offered by Th. Jacobsen, ZA 52 (1957)
92 f.: “a nasalized velar pronounced with rounded lips (nasalized
labio-velar), approximately c ‘
w
”.
There are two liquids: [1] and [r]; they are, once more, seen through
Akkadian “glasses”. Both occur in all three positions.
1: lú-u
18
-lu : lullû “man”
la-ha-an : la¢annu “bottle”
bala : palû “term of office”
kisal : kisallu “court (primarily of a temple)”
hé-∞gál : he(n)gallu “abundance”
As for final [1], classical Sumerian spelling distinguishes between con­
tinuation with -la or—more rarely—-lá, e.g.:
∞gál-la gibil-lá (also -la); see Krecher 1966, 113 with fnn. 328 f.
lugal-la
lul-la líl-lá
si-il-la (“u-)pe-el-lá (also -la); see Attinger 1993, 710–14.
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 18
18  
sikil-la
“ul-la
til-la
If in view of this distribution one might be tempted to posit two
kinds of [1] : [1
1
] and [1
2
], it must be admitted, on the other hand,
that there is no counterpart in the [e] sector: when sikil or líl are
continued by [e], spelling is in both cases [LI] = -le, e.g.,
d
En-líl-le
“Enlil” (ergative).
An as yet unsolved problem of Sumerian phonetics is found in
the variation of initial [NU] with [Lu] or [La]. The Ebla spelling
of lugal “(big person =) king” is nu-gal ARET 5, 24 ii a; iii 1–5; iv
1–3 paralleled by lugal in 24 i a, ii 1–4, iii 1–4; 26 i a. The negative
prefix nu- has a variant la- before prefix ba- (and, secondarily, li­
before bí-). The Akkadian loan of nu-banda “foreman” is laputtû
(luputtû) with oldest attestation in Old Bab. (CAD L 98 c 2’)
r: rab : rappu “clamp”
barag : parakku “dais”
kar : kàru “quay”
Note: Apart from rappu, Sumerian loanwords beginning with r- are probably
absent in Akkadian (cf. Edzard, ZA 90 [2000] 292 with fn. 2).
A Sumerian phoneme to be distinguished from [r] has been sup­
posed to exist first by Th. Jacobsen, ZA 52 (1957) 93 fn. 1 (d), and
then by J. Bauer, “Zum/dr/-Phonem im Sumerischen”, WO 8 (1975)
1–9; the ensuing discussion has been summarized by J. Black, RA
84 (1990) 108 f., 111, and note, with more literature, Attinger 1993,
143. The argument is the presence of spelling (or glossation) variants
with either D or R-syllabograms, e.g., na-RÚ-a “implanted stone” =
“stele”, with Akkadian loanword narû, but na-DI-a TIM 9, 35:2, 12
(cf. CAD N/l, 364 lex.).
The symbol [§ r] has been introduced (in order not to confuse it with
Czech ®), but—in contrast to [∞g], no general agreement has so far
been found: is it a single phoneme or rather a consonantal cluster
(DR)? We tentatively adopt [§ r]:
r: § rú). § rú “to implant, build” (formerly separated as dù and §
kur§
5
(TAR) “to separate”
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 19
19 vixiv\ri\ or stvrni\x rnoxoroov
The (original) existence of a phoneme (or of two phonemes?) [H]
in Sumerian may, again, be inferred for various reasons. [H] is here
used as a symbol rather than an exactly defined sound, but [h] has
a good chance.
a) é-gal, èkallu, “palace” is reflected by hkl in Ugaritic and by hè§al
in Hebrew (see also above 3.1.1). This leads us to suppose an
original *hè or *hay for Sumerian “house”. In Mesopotamian Old
Akkadian É = "à is found for Semitic [˙] (I. J. Gelb, MAD 2
2
[1961] 88 f.), in Ebla both for [h] and [˙] (M. Krebernik, ZA
72 [1982] 220 f.).
b) The Hebrew name of the Tigris, quite evidently a Mesopotamian
loan, is Óiddeqel with initial [˙], whereas we only know an Akkadian
form Idiglat. The town
d
ÍD
ki
, I-da
ki
(Old Bab.), URU I-di
( Middle Bab.) ( RGTC 2 104 f.; 3, 135 f.), classical Is, still
exists as modern Hìt on the Euphrates. Apparently, initial [h]
survived in the modern name. Whatever the etymology of the
name, there is some chance that Sumerian “river” originally
sounded [hid].
c) The Akkadian syllabogram ú is used, in Ugaritic, for [hu] as in
Ú-PI = hu-wa “he” in a Sumero-Akkadian-Hurrian-Ugaritic quadri­
lingual lexical text: J. Nougayrol, Ugaritica 5 (1968) 245: no. 137
ii 28’. This usage of Ú for [hu] probably goes back to the spelling
of Amorite PNs (cf. M. P. Streck 2000, 241), and it may reflect
Sumerian [Hu] for ú “grass”.
d) Sumerian nominal and verbal bases ending (at least in our translit­
eration) in a vowel absorb a following -e (of the ergative), -e(-ne)
(personal pl.), or -e-dè (verbal suffix), sometimes noting a plene
vowel instead of [e]: ama-a “mother” (ergative) instead of *ama­
e; lú-ù “person” (erg.), ama-ne “mothers”, ugula-ne “overseers”.
There are, however, cases where this rule does not hold: gala-e-
ne “cantors”, ì-lá-e(-ne) “he (they) will pay” (note the unconven­
tional spelling i-la-i in Mari, ARM 8, 48:9).
When Irikagina of Laga“ contrasts the “women of the past” (munus-
u
4
-bi-ta-ke
4
-ne) with the “present/nowadays women” (munus-u
4
-da-e-
ne) (Ukg. 6 iii 20’ and 23’), we have a regular genitive compound in the
first case: [munus-ubita-(a)k-ene], but an adjectival compound in the
second case: [munus-uda-ene], lit. “women-(in the day =)today-pl.]”.
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 20
20  
Here, u
4
-da (locative) is used like an adjective. After the locative
[uda], the [e] of [ene] is not absorbed or elided. This can only mean
that there was some kind of hiatus between the locative designation
[a] and the pers. pl. ending [ene]: [munus-uda’ene].
This admittedly slender evidence may lead to the conclusion that
after a vowel the [e] of certain morphemes was not absorbed or
elided, if there was a “barrier”, audible, but not visible in our translit­
erations. We propose to note that supposed barrier by [H]: galaH­
ene, ì-láH-e, munus-udaH-ene.
Akkadian preserved the common Semitic unvoiced velar fricative [¢],
as in a-¢u-um [a¢um] “brother”. We transliterate [h] in Sumerian
whenever words are glossed by syllabograms ¢a, ¢é, ¢i, ¢u, a¢/i¢,
ú¢, disregarding the crescent below the [¢]. Still, we may be sure
that the [¢] of the Akkadian loanword in Sumerian, pu¢rum ( pu-ú¢-
ru-um) “assembly” was pronounced like, or very close to, Akkadian
[¢]. We may, furthermore, rely, as usual, on Sumerian loanwords
in Akkadian, where Sumerian [h] is rendered by Akkadian [¢]:
hur-sa∞g : ¢ur“ànu “mountain range”
he-∞gál : ¢e(n)gallu “abundance”
bahar : pa¢àru, pa¢¢àru “potter” (the -¢¢- of Akkadian is due to the
assimilation of the word to the Akkadian pattern parràs-
(cf. Arabic fa¢¢àr)
ki-mah : kimà¢u, kima¢¢u “(greatest place =) grave”
These correspondences do not imply, however, that in all Sumerian
occurences our transliterated [h] was identical in sound (or close to
it) with Akkadian [¢]. There may have been voiced and unvoiced
variants which escape us.
We will be brief on Sumerian sibilants, because this group of
sounds is already difficult for us to define in Akkadian, where we
have no exact idea about the identity (and possible diachronic change)
of [z, s, ß, “, ≤]. It is all the more difficult even to approximate the
Sumerian values.
Z: zà-mi : sammû “lyre”
ezen : isinnu “festival”
a-zu : asû “physician”
S: kisal : kisallu “courtyard”
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 21
21 vixiv\ri\ or stvrni\x rnoxoroov
ús : ùsu “guideline, behaviour”
hur-sa∞g : ¢ur“ànu “mountain range”
sa∞gga ∞ : “angû “main temple administrator”
énsi(-k) : i““iakku “city ruler, governor” (the Akkadian -akku end­
ing is most probably due to the restitution of the
Sumerian genitive particle [ak]; see also below nu-è“ :
nê“akku)
dub-sar : tup“arru “scribe”
“à-tam : “atammu “temple administrator”
nu-è“ : nê“akku (a high-ranked priest) (for the Akkadian ending
-akku see above, énsi(-k)
The rather complicated correspondences between Sumerian forms
and their rendering in Akkadian loanwords reminds us of the stops:
z : z d : d
z : s and d : t
s : s t : t
The evidence becomes even more difficult to judge because of word-
initial or word-medial correspondence s : “ as in hur-sa∞ g : ¢ur“ànu.
From this, A. Falkenstein, ZA 42 (1934) 152–54, had concluded there
had been (diachronically) different “Lehnwortschichten”, and he also
noted, p. 153, fn. 2, cases where word-initial sibilants of identical
words were registered with [s] or [“] by scribes. Later, in 1959, he
proposed the existence of Sumerian [≤] in order to explain “irregu­
lar” sibilant correspondences (1959, 24).
If accepted this would yield this tentative chart of Sumerian conso­
nantal phonemes:
p' p b m
t' t d n l (1
2
) r §r
k' k g ∞g
“ (≤?)
s z
*H h
Our transliteration in this grammar will, however, follow traditional
values and will not note p', t', or k'.
EDZARD_f4_12-22 4/28/03 2:51 PM Page 22
22 cn\r+rn +nnrr
3.2. Svrr\nrr s+ntc+tnr
Here again we are dependent on Akkadian, because we are only
able to reach Sumerian syllabograms through their Akkadian pronun­
ciation. As a Semitic language has no initial consonantal clusters
(tra-, stra-), word-internal clusters of more than two consonants
(-astra-, -abstra-) or word-final clusters of two or more consonants
(-art, -arst, -arbst) and, consequently, there are no syllabograms serv­
ing comparable purposes, we cannot identify Sumerian syllables with
a structure differing from Akkadian. Therefore, while we may ask
whether Sumerian syllables of the type bra-, pli-, sku-, -arp, -urps
actually existed, there is no way to prove them.
Note: A. Falkenstein, starting from the assumption that the verbal prefix ba- can
be segmentized as b+a-, concluded that, by analogy, ba-ra- might have been a
spelling for intended *b+ra-, i.e. [bra], see 1949, 190; 1950, 185 with fn. 2. This
assumption has been refuted by J. N. Postgate, JCS 26 (1974) 18.
Taking our transliteration at face value, Sumerian had the follow­
ing types of syllables:
V: e.g., a
CV: e.g., ba, ri, ru
VC: e.g., ab, e“, u∞g
CVC: e.g., bar, min, mun
Note: The Ur III unorthodox spelling nam-bi-ri (NRVN I 4:4; see also 2:4 f.)
stands for expected *nam-(é)ri(m) [namri] and suggests a pronunciation [nambri]
with [b] as a glide between [m] and [r]. We are reminded of comparable glides
(between a nasal and a liquid) in Greek an-d-rós (genitive of énÆr “man”), French
(and English) hum-b-le, etc.
So, even if a Sumerian internal cluster -mbr- may have occurred in
spoken language, it was—until proof of the contrary—a secondary
phonetic phenomenon.
It goes without saying that in the spoken language there must have
been free variants of pronountiation, depending on speed, with all
such universal features as slurring, elision, assimilation, dissimilation.
23
EDZARD_f5_23-27 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 23
CHAPTER FOUR
THE “WORD” IN SUMERIAN, PARTS OF SPEECH
We will try to define Sumerian parts of speech not by applying clas­
sical models, but by observing the morpho-syntactic behaviour of
Sumerian “words”.
4.1. “Wonr”
A “word” in Sumerian is an entity that can convey meaning on its
own without anything spoken before or after. In é-“è “towards the
house”, é is a word whereas -“è, the terminative postposition, is not.
We define [“e] as a nominal particle.
gál “it is there/available”, ∞ In ì-∞ gál “to exist” may occur indepen­
dently, but the prefixed element ì- may not. We define [i] as a ver­
bal particle.
gu
Thirdly, in é-zu “your house”, -zu “your” (sg.) does not qualify
as a “word”, because to the question “whose house is this, mine or
10
or *-zu, but only ∞ yours?” the answer cannot be *-∞ gá(-a)-kam,
za(-a)-kam “it is (of me =) mine”, “it is (of you =) yours”. [∞ gu], [zu]
and others are connectible with both a nominal and a verbal base.
We define them as common particles.
Note: I owe the distinction of nominal, verbal, and common particles to G. B.
Milner, Fijian Grammar (1956) 130 f. (M. uses “general” instead of “common”.)
The Sumero-Akkadian lexicographers were apparently aware of the
idea “word”. Their lexical entries exactly correspond to our idea of
a “word” (or of a compound); it is only in the grammatical series
(OBGT, NBGT) that we see entries which we would define as “par­
ticles” (or rather syllables through which a morpheme boundary ran,
e.g., un, an, in, en listed to denote consonantal preverbal -n-).
4.2. P\n+s or srrrcn
We may distinguish eight parts of speech: (1) nouns, (2) pronouns,
(3) numerals, (4) verbs, (5) adverbs, (6) exclamations, (7) subjunctions,
EDZARD_f5_23-27 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 24
24 cn\r+rn rotn
conjunctions, (8) interrogations. All these occur as “words” (see our
definition above) and may be found as entries in lexical texts. (1) to
(4) may be combined with bound particles (prefixes, suffixes): nom­
inal, verbal, and common. (5) to (8) do not combine with particles.
(1) to (3) may be opposed to (4) as nonverbal to verbal categories.
Parts of speech can be identified exclusively on context. There is
no way to tell by the form of the base alone whether we are faced
with a noun (dur, tur), a verb (gur), or some other part of speech.
A further subdivision of (1) nouns into (1a) substantives and (1b)
adjectives is not unproblematic. On the one hand, sikil “pure” can­
not form a plural *sikil-e-ne “the pure ones” whereas it may be fol­
lowed by the plural of the copula: sikil-me-e“ “they are pure”. For
“the pure ones” a nominal head would be needed: lú-sikil-e-ne “the
pure persons”. On the other hand, sikil following a nominal head,
e.g., ki-sikil “(pure place =) girl, young woman” behaves exactly like
an apposition and, as such, may take on all nominal particles.
For practical reasons, we will make the distinction between sub­
stantives and adjectives. As a guide-line of high antiquity we may
again take the fact that certain Sumerian nouns are entered in lex­
ical lists as Akkadian substantives while others are rendered by
Akkadian adjectives. In Akkadian itself, the differentiation of sub­
stantives and adjectives is unproblematic on both morphological and
syntactical grounds.
2
(1a) The substantive has the following grammatical categories: class
(person, non-person), number (singular, non-singular, plural, collec­
tive, detailed, etc.), case (absolutive, ergative, genitive, dative, loca­
tive, ablative, comitative, terminative, directive, equative), possession
singular: 1
st
, 2
nd
, 3
rd
person: person and non-person class; plural: 1
st
,
nd
, 3
rd
person: person class only.
These grammatical categories—apart from class—are realized by
the suffixation of particles.
The substantive may form part of quite intricate appositional and
genitive constructions. It may be followed by adjectives. Some sub­
stantives may be repeated (“reduplicated”, e.g., énsi-énsi “all of the
city rulers”), but we cannot as yet establish whether reduplication
was open to any substantive or subject to restrictions.
Substantives and adjectives may take the prefixed particle nam-,
serving to express an abstract concept: nar “musician”, nam-nar
“music”, mah “very big”, nam-mah “greatness”.
EDZARD_f5_23-27 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 25
25 +nr “vonr” ix stvrni\x, r\n+s or srrrcn
(1b) The adjective rarely occurs on its own. It is usually connected
with a (preceding—rarely following) substantive. Adjectives may serve
as the base of adverbs, e.g., dirig-bi “(its excessive =) excessively”.
A few adjectives which express dimension take a reduplicated form
when the preceding substantive is meant to be in the plural, e.g.,
di∞ gir-gal-gal(-e-ne) “the great gods” (see also 5.3.7).
Note: This usage has been imitated in Akkadian where an equally restricted class
of adjectives expressing dimension has bases with a lengthened middle radical,
e.g., ilù rabbûtu “the great gods” (Reiner 1966, 64).
Note: There is a conspicuous similarity between Akkadian nominal formations in
-ùtu which express both the masc. plural of adjectives (e.g., damqùtu “good ones”)
and an abstract notion (e.g., ßìrùtu “majesty”, or “arrùtu “kingship”), and Sumerian
formations with nam- (e.g., nam-mah “greatness”, nam-lugal “kingship”). This
similarity has most probably to be seen as another symptom of a Sumero-Akkadian
linguistic area.
When an adjective is attached to a substantive, all suffixed particles
pertaining to the substantive are placed after the following adjective
so that one might argue, formally, that the adjective behaves like a
substantive. In reality, however, the whole complex (substantive +
adjective) has been substantivized.
Adjectives, like substantives, may take the prefixed particle nam-,
e.g., nam-mah “quality of being the greatest” (but it is not yet known
how far this was a productive feature); cf. p. 24 bottom.
Thomsen 1984, 64 f., quotes Gragg 1968, 9 who considers adjec­
tives as a sub-class of the verb. In fact, adjectival bases can often
be turned into verbal bases, but this can hardly be stated as a gen­
eral overriding rule.
Black 2003.
(2) Pronouns have the categories of person (1
st
, 2
nd
, 3
rd
), class (per­
son, non-person), number (sg., pl.), and case.
(2.1) Personal pronouns: As 1
st
and 2
nd
persons can, by nature,
only be person class (unless non-person class becomes a “person” in
a literary context), the differentiation of person : non-person is
restricted to the 3
rd
person; moreover, it does not operate in the 3
rd
pl., where only person class occurs.
As for case, personal pronouns have a restricted system of case
inflection as compared with substantives: absolutive and ergative are
identical in form; there is no ablative or directive.
We do not include as parts of speech the bound forms of per­
sonal pronouns in their quality of possessors; cf. below 5.2.
We may include, among pronouns, the question words “who”,
EDZARD_f5_23-27 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 26
26 cn\r+rn rotn
“what”, because the answer may not only be a substantive (who: my
father), but also a person (who: me, you). Sumerian, like most of the
world’s languages, distinguishes “who” (a-ba) and “what” (a-na).
The system of deictic pronouns is still poorly explored in Sumerian.
Bound forms (suffixed particles) are not included here; see below 7.
(3) Numerals. Cardinal numerals are written in the sexagesimal
system (see below, 10.2) following the item counted. In this respect,
they resemble adjectives, and as with adjectives if a complex of sub­
stantive + cardinal number is provided with a suffixed particle, it is
shifted from the substantive to the numeral, the whole complex, sub­
stantive + numeral, being turned into one noun.
Numerals occur in the genitive, after a substantive, in order to
denote ordinal numbers: mu-3-kam “(it is the year of three =) third
year”.
(4) The verb is the most complex part of speech in Sumerian. It
has an extremely variable set of prefixed particles as well as a num­
ber of suffixed particles. The verbal base in itself may be subject to
variation.
The verb may express the following categories: person, class, num­
ber, action, direction, tense/aspect, mood.
Except for lexical lists where the (simple or reduplicated) verbal
base is regularly equated with Akkadian infinitives, the verbal base
extremely rarely occurs alone, without any prefixed or suffixed particles.
A verb(al complex) may be turned into a noun, i.e., be nominalized,
by the addition of the suffixed particle [a] (see 12.16). Such a newly
created noun may then be subject to nominal inflection (receiving
suffixed case particles). Nominalized verbal complexes frequently have
to be rendered by English dependent clauses. The very productive
system of Akkadian (nominal) infinitive constructions may be seen as
an Akkadian share in the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area (see 17).
(5) Adverbs. Words which describe in a more precise way the idea
contained in a verb are traditionally defined as adverbs: “he arrived”,
“he recently arrived”. In Akkadian, this category is usually formed
with an adjective as base + the ending -i“, e.g., e““u “new”, e““i“
“anew”, damqu “good”, damqi“ “nicely”. This way of forming adverbs
has two Sumerian counterparts:
gibil-bi “(its new =) anew”,
zi-dè-e“ [zid-e“] = zi(d) “true” with the adverbiative particle [e“]
(Attinger 1993, 253–56), “truly”, “in a sincere, reliable way”.
EDZARD_f5_23-27 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 27
27 +nr “vonr” ix stvrni\x, r\n+s or srrrcn
Note: The obvious similarity of Sumerian [e“] and Akkadian [i“] may not be due
to pure chance. Was the Sumerian adverbiative a loan from Akkadian?
Less frequently, -bi + “è (or -é“?) are combined: mah-bi-”È “in a
most exalted way”; gibil-bi-”È “anew”.
Cf. Thomsen 1984, 66 f.
The adverb being already a derived form, it is frozen in itself and
not subject to further change.
(6) Exclamations. They express joy, fear, pain, surprise, doubt, etc.
i (
d
Utu) “woe (o Sungod)”, u
8
-ú-a [way] “alas, woe”, a-la-la “hey”,
and others.
Here also belong the expressions for “yes”, hé-àm (lit. “let it be”)
and “no”, in-nu (where the negative particle [nu], given the status
of a base, is preceded by the neutral motion particle [i]).
(7) Because Sumerian mainly uses nominalized verbal phrases (to
which postpositions may be added—see 12.16) instead of subordi­
nate clauses, it essentially lacks subjunctions and conjunctions. Note
ì-gi-in-zu “as if ”, tukumbi “if ” (for which Old Sum. has [uda], see
14.2.1).
As a conjunction, ù “and” has been borrowed from Akkadian
[wa, u].
Note: Borrowing “and” is well known in agglutinating languages: ve (Arabic wa)
in Turkish, ja (Old Germanic jah) in Finnish, eta in Basque. But Sumerian u
only occurs to connect phrases, not parts of speech; insofar it is not a replace­
ment of Sumerian -bi(-da) which may connect parts of speech (see hereafter).
As a replacement for a conjunction, -bi or -bi-da is attested: áb
amar-bi-da “the cow (with its =) and the calf ”;
d
Nin-∞gir-su,
d
”ára-bi(-r) “to Ningirsu and ”ara” Ent 28/29 i 5–6
(see 5.4.2.6. b).
(8) Interrogations. We can only partly translate into Sumerian the
famous hexameter quis quid ubi quibus auxiliis cur quomodo quando.
For “who” and “what” see 9.4; “where” is me-a; “why” is a-na-a“
(-àm) [ana“’am] “for what (is it)?”.
[ J. Black, Sumerian lexical categories, ZA 92 (2002) 60–77, came to the author’s
attention only after the preceding chapter had been written.]
28
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 28
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
29
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 29
CHAPTER FIVE
SUBSTANTIVES
5.1. G
Sumerian substantives lack gender, but are strictly divided into two
classes: person and non-person. A. Falkenstein used “Personen- und
Sachklasse”, M.-L. Thomsen “animate and inanimate”. We follow
Attinger because animals (unless they are personified in literary con­
text) are “animate”, but “non-person”, and they are not “Sachen”.
The form of the substantive does not betray its class. It is shown by
agreement: lugal-a-ni “his owner” (said of a slave), lugal-bi “its owner”
(said of a garden). Only person class substantives may take the erga­
tive plural (-e)-ne or form a dative; only non-person substantives may
form an ablative. Class is also expressed in verbal morphology.
nin
With a limited number of substantives, gender (masc., fem.) is
expressed lexically: ninta “male”, munus “woman”; ses “brother”,
(9)
“sister”; an“e “male donkey”, éme(AN”E.MÍ) “jenny”, etc.
Apart from class, the categories of substantives are possession,
number, and case: ses-∞ gu
10
-(e)ne-da “with my brothers” where the
particles serving the respective functions occur in a predictable and
invariable order.
5.2. P
Ownership of something or someone by something or someone is
expressed by a set of suffixed particles, closest in rank to the owner
(substantive, nominal compound, or nominalized verbal form in -a).
No distinction is made between alienable and inalienable possession
(sa∞ gu
10
“my head”, a-“à-∞ g-∞ gu
10
“my field”) or, in the 1
st
person pl.,
between the categories inclusive (“ours” = “mine and yours present”)
and exclusive (“ours” = “mine and yours absent”, “mine and theirs”).
For the independent personal pronouns see 10.
For reasons of morphological behaviour we have dissociated pos­
sessive particles from free personal pronouns.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 30
30 cn\r+rn riyr
Note: Jacobsen 1965, 100
19
had proposed to see a distinction between “inclusive”
and “exclusive” in such forms as ga-an-“i-su
8
-dè-en “let us proceed toward it” (!)
(incl.) and ga-ba-ab-túm-mu-dè “let us carry (him) off ” (excl.), but his examples
do not fit the general definition of these two pronominal categories.
Note: Kienast 1980, 54, explained each possessive particle as “enttontes Enklitikon”
ga would become -∞ (following Falkenstein 1959, 33): 1
st
sg. *-∞ gu (under the influence
of the preceding consonant), and -∞gu would, then, have engendered 2
nd
sg. -zu.
Falkenstein’s explanation of lugal-ani as “König er” (for “his king”) cannot be
proven (see also 9.2).
There are three persons each in sg. and pl.; the 3
rd
sg. distinguishes
person and non-person class. All other persons are restricted to per­
son class.
1
st
sg. -∞gu
10
(MU)
2
nd
sg. -zu
3
rd
sg. person (-a)-ni
3
rd
sg. non-person -bi
1
st
pl. -me
2
nd
pl. -zu-ne-ne
3
rd
pl. (-a)-ne-ne
While there are clear morphological sg.: pl. relations in 2
nd
and 3
rd
persons, 1
st
sg. and 1
st
pl. are not related (nor are they in Akkadian)
because “ours” cannot be *“mine + mine”, but only “mine + yours”
or “mine + his/hers/its”.
Morphological behaviour:
1
st
sg. is -∞
3
2
gá in gen. and loc.
nd
sg. is -za in gen. and loc.
rd
sg. p. -a-ni follows a consonant, -ni follows a vowel.
1
3
(-a)-ni is (-a)-na in gen. and loc.
rd
sg. non-p. is -ba or -bi-a in gen. and loc.
st
to 3
rd
pl.: with -me, -zu-ne-ne, (-a)-ne-ne, no overriding of final
-e by -a of gen. takes place.
Note: 1
st
to 3
rd
sg. differ from nouns ending in a vowel. Whereas in ab-ba-eri-
(a)ke
4
-ne “city elders” the [a] of [ak] is elided, with the possessive particles sg. it
is their final vowel that yields—at least graphically—to the [a] of genitive [a(k)]
or of locative.
When the possessor is another noun, use of a genitive compound is
made whereby the relation between possessor and possessed depends
on meaning: ká-é-gal-la(-k) “the gate (possessed) of the palace (pos­
sessor)”; but lugal-Unu
ki
-ga(-k) “the king (possessor) of Uruk (pos­
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 31
31 stns+\x+iyrs
sessed)”. Much more rarely, the inverted genitive construction is used:
é-a(k) lugal-bi “of the house, its (king =) owner”.
5.3. Ntvnrn
Sumerian number differs fundamentally from that in Akkadian where
practically all substantives may form a plural and a restricted num­
ber may form a dual. Sumerian has no dual (but cf. note to 5.3.5).
5.3.1.
A plural properly speaking (implying two or more counted objects)
and marked by a special suffixed particle is restricted to person class:
[(e)ne] with the distribution [ene] after consonant, including [H],
and [ne] after vowels. The first [e] thus behaves like the [e] of the
ergative or directive case particle: lugal-e-ne “kings”, galaH-e-ne “can­
tors”, ugula-ne “overseers”.
Orthographically, the first [e] may be contained in a Ce syllabo-
gir-ré-ne (OS), di∞ gram, e.g., di∞ gir-re-e-ne (OB), ab-ba-eri-ke
4
-ne “city
elders”.
If the substantive is extended by an adjective, an apposition or by
another noun in the genitive, [ene] follows the extended complex:
di∞ gir-gal-gal-e-ne “the great gods”, ama-a-a-An-na-ke
4
-ne “the mothers
and fathers of An”.
5.3.2.
Plurality may be expressed by reduplication of the nominal base:
gi“-gi-∞ a-gàr-a-gàr “fields”, ∞ gi“-gi “cane-brakes”, bára-bára-Ki-en-gi
énsi-kur-kur-ra(-k) “the (daises =) rulers of Sumer, the city-rulers of
the other lands”, gur
7
-gur
7
“(many) piles of grain/silos”.
Reduplication here, apparently, serves the need to express the idea
of plural where the simple sg. base which may sometimes also func­
tion to express a collective (see 5.3.8) would lack clarity.
Reduplication very rarely occurs with extended nouns: ses-gal-ses-
gal “(elder brothers =) school elders”.
It is not known to us whether reduplication of substantives may
sometimes have resulted in variant pronunciation, as it did with, e.g.,
deli “one”, deli-deli > de/idli “single ones”.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 32
32 cn\r+rn riyr
5.3.3.
A rare type of partial reduplication is ku-li-li “mutual friends”, prob­
ably to be compared to Akkadian itbàru “mutual friends” as against
ebrù “friends”.
5.3.4.
dedli (see also 5.3.2. end) indicates a detailed plurality: ki“ib-dedli
“the individual sealed documents”, bàd-dedli-gal-gal “the individual
big fortresses”.
Note: dedli is closer in rank to the substantive than the regular adjective. dedli
may be followed by [(e)ne]: lú-dedli-ne “the individual persons”.
5.3.5.
hi-a “mixed” indicates a plurality of heterogeneous items: túg-hi-a
“diverse textiles”, u
8
-udu-hi-a “sheep”.
Note: J. Krecher, OLZ 73 (1978) 28 with fn. 2, referred to hi-a indicating
a duality through mention of only one member: mer-hi-a “(the diverse south
(-winds) =) north and south”. This is reminiscent of Arabic al-Furàtàn “(the two
Euphrates =) E. and Tigris”, al-qamaràn “(the two moons =) sun and moon”.
5.3.6.
The exact quantity may be expressed by a cardinal number follow­
ing a substantive. Here, no extra plural word or particle is needed:
me-umun
7
“the seven ordinances”, é-ninnû “Fifty Houses”, Nin∞ girsu’s
temple complex and ziggurat at ªGirsu, *lugal-ussu “eight kings”.
Note: Texts frequently write 8 lugal instead of lugal 8. The inverted notation has
been borrowed from accountancy texts where figures, for the sake of visual clarity
are noted at the beginning of a line, and so written exactly one below the other.
5.3.7.
A few adjectives appear in reduplicated form when the preceding
substantive is plural: di∞gir-gal-gal “the great gods”; na
4
-di
4
(TUR)-di
4
“small stones”.
Here, one is reminded of the Akkadian pl. form of some adjec­
tives: rabbûtu, ße¢¢erùtu, arrakùtu. For more discussion, see above 4.2.1(b).
Note: Against Falkenstein 1949, 72, Thomsen 1984, 65, supposes that the redu­
plication of adjectives could express superlatives. However, in the case of na
4
-gal-
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 33
33 stns+\x+iyrs
gal na
4
-di
4
-di
4
there is hardly the idea of “biggest, smallest stones”, but simply
“big, small stones”. Moreover, as gal-gal, di
4
-di
4
virtually only occur when the
noun is plural, there is no reason to assume a superlative, or elative, function.
5.3.8.
Finally, plurality may be present in a simple unextended substantive
with collective meaning: ku
6
, udu, túg are “fish”, “sheep (and goats)”,
“textiles/garments”.
5.3.9.
Summing up, we may state that number in Sumerian nouns and
adjectives has a totally different significance from that in Akkadian:
apart from an opposition singular : plural, we see different non-num-
erical notions such as totality, collectivity, detailed quantities, mixed
quantities.
5.4. C\sr
Sumerian has ten cases: (1) absolutive, (2) ergative, (3) genitive,
(4) locative, (5) dative, (6) comitative, (7) ablative(-instrumental),
(8) terminative, (9) directive, (10) equative. A second locative is only
attested with the verb; see 12.8.1.26.
Of these, ergative (2) and directive (9) are identical in spelling,
and their original identity has often been suggested; cf. 5.4.9.
For (4) to (9) our enumeration corresponds to the sequence of the
directional prefixed particles of the verb.
Cases (4), (5) and (7) to (9) may be summarized as directional.
The equative (10) stands apart: it is restricted to sg. and it may
override any of the cases (4) to (9), so that ses-gin
7
not only means
“like a brother”, but also, e.g., “as with a brother”, depending on
context.
Note: to some degree this feature of (10) is shared by Akkadian: kìma eleppim “like
a boat” may also mean “as in a boat”; cf. also Turkish hasta gibi ona bakmı{tı
“he looked at him as (at a sick person =) if he were a sick person”.
E
D
Z
A
R
D
_
f
6
_
2
8
-
4
5


4
/
2
8
/
0
3


2
:
4
0

P
M


P
a
g
e

3
4
3
4

“king” “mother” “tree” “city” “your mother” “his/her tablet” “kings” “mothers”
(1) abs.
(2) erg.
(3) gen.
(4) loc.
(5) dat.
(6) com.
(7) abl.
(8) term.
(9) dir.
(10) equ.
lugal ama ∞ge“ eri ama-zu dub-ba-ni — —
lugal-e ama ∞ge“-e eri ama-zu dub-ba-ni lugal-e-ne ama-ne
lugal-la(-k) ama(-k) ∞ge“-a(k) eri(-k) ama-za(-k) dub-ba-na(-k) lugal-e-ne(-k) ama-ne(-k)
(—) — ∞ge“-a eri-a — dub-ba-na — —
lugal-ra ama-r(a) — — ama-zu-r(a) — lugal-e-ne-r(a) ama-ne-r(a)
lugal-da ama-da ∞ge“-da eri-da ama-zu-da dub-ba-ni-da lugal-e-ne-da ama-ne-da
— — ∞ge“-ta eri-ta — dub-ba-ni-ta — —
lugal-“è ama-“è ∞ge“-“è eri-“è ama-zu-“è dub-ba-ni-“è lugal-e-ne-“è ama-ne-“è
(cf. 2) (cf. 2) ∞ge“-e eri (cf. 2) dub-ba-ni (cf. 2) (cf. 2)
lugal-gin
7
ama-gin
7
∞ge“-gin
7
eri-gin
7
ama-zu-gin
7
dub-ba-ni-gin
7
— —
c
n
\
r
+
r
n

r
i
y
r

EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 35
35 stns+\x+iyrs
5.4.1. Preliminary notes on the phonetic and spelling behaviour
of the case particles
In general, a number of (a) phonotactic and (b) spelling rules apply
when case particles are added to a noun. For [e] of the ergative or
directive see 5.4.2.2 and 5.4.2.9. For the extremely complicated
behaviour of the genitive case particle [ak] see 5.4.2.3. Case parti­
cles consisting of a vowel ([e], [a]) or beginning with a vowel ([ak])
may be written either with a vowel sign or with a syllabogram CV,
representing both the consonant belonging to the preceding—nom-
inal or verbal—base and the vowel of the respective case particle,
e.g., kur-ra [kur-a] “in the foreign country”. For more details, see
the paragraphs on the individual case particles.
5.4.2. Notes on the individual case particles
5.4.2.1. Absolutive
The absolutive which is unmarked denotes—in our modern inter-
pretation—both the ‘subject’ of an intransitive verbal form and the
‘object’ of a transitive verbal form. In Sumerian “father (absolutive)
sleeps” and “mother (ergative) cooks dinner (absolutive)”, father and
dinner are in the identical unmarked case.
The absolutive being the unextended form is the form cited in
lexical lists. Absolutive is also used, as a vocative, for calling a god,
person, or animal. Nominal pl. in [(e)ne] does not occur with abso­
lutive.
5.4.2.2. Ergative
The ergative marks the subject of a transitive (‘object’-taking) verbal
form. Its usage is consistent in classical Sumerian, and it is only
under 2
nd
millennium Akkadian influence that absolutive and erga­
tive may be used “incorrectly”.
The [e] of the ergative (as well as the directive—see 5.4.2.9) is
not graphically shown when the preceding substantive ends in a
vowel. It is either disregarded in spelling or replaced by a “plene”
vowel sign, corresponding to the last vowel of the preceding noun:
ama or ama-a “mother”, lú or lú-ù “person”. It is unknown whether
the “plene” vowel sign served (a) to indicate a pronunciation of a
single vowel different from the absolutive form, e.g., amâ instead of
ama, or (b) to denote a syllable to be pronounced separately, e.g.,
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 36
36 cn\r+rn riyr
ama’a, or whether it was (c) a mere visual mark for guiding the
reader without relevance for pronunciation. (a) appears to be the
most plausible option.
Apart from -e, the ergative (or directive) may be noted by -Ce,
e.g.,
d
En-líl-le [Enlil-e].
While it is still disputed whether the Sumerian verb is construed
in pure or in “split” ergativity (cf. the discussion in 12.7.5), it must
be stressed that, judging by the distribution of absolutive and erga­
tive in classical Sumerian syntax, there is no “split” whatsoever in
the nominal part of Sumerian SOV phrases.
Note: The question which verbs were actually considered transitive or intransitive
is often difficult to answer, the more so as the modern interpreter cannot help
being influenced by his or her native language. Sumerian “to enter” (ku
4
-r/ku
4
-
ku
4
) is introduced by the ergative as if it were “to make an entry”. In English,
“to enter”, may be used both as a transitive and an intransitive verb, whereas
in German there is a strict distinction between “eintreten” (intr.) and “eintreten
lassen, hinein bringen, etc.” (trans.).
When the head of a genitival (regens-rectum) compound, e.g., lugal-
kur-kur-ra(-k) “king of all countries”, is in the ergative, the particle
-e is shifted from the regens to the end of the rectum: lugal-kur-kur-
ra-ke
4
instead of *lugal-e kur-kur-ra.
Note: The syllabogram KID, in our transliteration -ke
4
, is, strangely enough, not
attested in the series Proto-Ea; cf. MSL 14, 41:243–254: gi-i (var. ge-e, g[i
4
-. . .]),
su-úh, li-il. Therefore, some scholars in the past preferred -gé instead of -ke
4
which has been inferred from our idea of the grammatical structure.
-ke
4
may, however, be proven by spelling variants. Cf. SRT 11:68
sipa
d
Ur-
d
Namma-ke
4
“the shepherd Ur-Namma” paralleled by TCL
15, 38:10 si-pa Ur-
d
Na-ma-KI, and see Å. Sjöberg, OrSuec. 10 (1961)
11; or Ninme“ara 116 ki su-ub ma-ra(-ab)-AG-ne//-a
5
(AG)-ke
4
-ne
“they kiss the ground before you”.
A reading -gé (instead of -ke
4
) would contradict the fact that nouns
ending in -g and prolonged by [e] are regularly spelled -ge or -ge
4
,
but not *-gé.
For the homophony and—probably—common origin of ergative
and directive (see also 5.4.2.9) cf. Steiner 1976.
5.4.2.3. Genitive
The genitive is the most complicated of the case particles, both in
morphology and in syntax. Starting from the basic morpheme [ak]
we can note four allomorphs:
(a) [ak] in the position -C-ak-V, e.g., lugal-Úrim
ki
-ma-ke
4
“the king
of Ur” (erg.).
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 37
37 stns+\x+iyrs
(b) [k] between -V and -V, e.g., ab-ba-eri-ke
4
-ne “the city elders”
(erg.).
(c) [a(k)] between -C and -Ø, e.g., lugal-Úrim
ki
-ma(-k) “the king of
Ur” (absol.).
(d) [(V)(k)] between -V and -Ø, e.g., ab-ba-eri(-k) “the city elder”
(absol.).
There are four exceptions from the above rule (b): When -∞gu
10
“my”
(see 5.2) -zu “yours”, (-a)-ni “his/hers”, or -bi “its” (non-p. class)
stand in the genitive, the final [u] or [i] is overlaid by the [a(k)] of
genitive or by the [a] of locative, yielding -∞ gá, -za, (-a)-na, -ba.
Note: Pronouns, cross-linguistically, exhibit peculiarities of their own: in mor­
phology, plural formation, and other respects.
As regards the allomorphs (c) [a(k)] and (d) [(k)], it remains unknown
what happened, phonetically speaking, to the final [k] of the geni­
tive morpheme.
That it did not disappear totally becomes clear from the OS
spelling rule for the dative case particle [r(a)]. It is not noted in writ­
ing after a preceding vowel, e.g.,
d
Nanna(-r) “to the Moongod”,
because syllable-closing consonants were still (mostly) disregarded in
spelling up till and during Ur III. [ra] is noted after a consonant,
e.g.,
d
En-líl-ra “to Enlil” and also after [a(k)], e.g.,
d
Nin-∞gír-su(-k)-
ra. Quite apparently, there was not one syllable *[sur], but two,
[su(k)-ra].
Also, Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian resulting from genitival
compounds, display an ending -Vkku, e.g., áb-zà-mi(-(a)k) “cow (as
part) of the lyre” > apsamikku, a geometrical shape, most probably
“trapezium”.
With a genitival compound X-Y-a(k) any possessive, number, or
case particle pertaining to X has to follow the whole compound,
e.g., lugal-Úrim
ki
-ma(-k)-da “with the king of Ur”; cf. already above,
ad 5.4.2.2 (ergative).
However, either X or Y of the compound may be extended by
the addition of adjectives or of another dependent substantive (the
latter again to be put in the genitive). This results in an intricate,
but logical, genitive “algebra”:
(a) X (= A-B) – Y-a(k): ur-sa∞ g-kal-ga—
d
En-líl-lá(-k) “the mighty war­
rior of Enlil”.
(b) X – Y (= A-B)-a(k): eri-bar—abul-tur-ra(-k) “suburb of the small
city-gate”.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 38
38 cn\r+rn riyr
(c) X (= A-B-a(k)) – Y -a(k): ab-ba-eri(-k)—Unug
ki
-ga-ke
4
-ne “the city
elders of Uruk”.
(d) X – Y (= A-B-ak) -a(k): sa∞gga- ∞
d
Nin-∞ gír-su-ka(-k) “the temple admin­
istrator of the Lord-of-ªGirsu”.
Probably for reasons of euphony, no more than two genitive parti­
cles were allowed on a string, even if three or more genitival rela­
tionships are involved.
What is more, in X – Y -a(k), both X and Y may be extended
by appositions: X
1
= X
2
– Y -a(k) or X – Y
1
= Y
2
-a(k). In such
cases the string of noun + apposition(s) functions like a simple noun
so that there will be no further consequence for the genitive “alge­
bra”. Cf. for X
1
= X
2
,
d
Nin-∞ g-kal-ga—
d
En-líl-lá(-k)-ra gír-su(-k) ur-sa∞
“to N., mighty warrior of Enlil”. Here, Ningirsu(k) and mighty war­
rior are in apposition.
For Y
1
= Y
2
: suhu“-an-ki(-k) “the foundations of Heaven (and)
Earth”, where an-ki “Heaven and Earth” represent a string (though
additive instead of appositive).
Possessive particles may equally be incorporated. In X – Y-a(k),
both X and Y may carry a possessive particle:
ká—é-gal-la(-k)-∞ gu
10
“my (gate of the palace =) palace gate”.
ká—é-gal-la-ka-ni “his (gate of the palace =) palace gate”.
Here possession applies to the whole compound.
Contrast ká—é-gal-∞gá(-k) “the gate of my palace”.
ká—é-gal-la-na(-k) “the gate of his palace”.
Here only Y (the rectum) is marked by a possessive particle.
OB scribes sometimes became confused with their more elaborate
constructions: bàd-gal-[BÀ]D
ki
eri-nam-”AGIN(-a(k))-nam-dumu-na-
ka-ni “the great wall of Der, the city of governorship (nam-”AGIN)
of his (son-ship =) state of crown-prince”, i.e., “. . . of the city where
he had been governor when he was still a crown-prince”.
Here, X = bàd-gal and Y = the city-name with a complicated
string of apposition and genitives: A
1
-A
2
(-ak)—B-ak. The possessive
particle -ani “his” should also have been in the genitive, -a-na. We
may understand the scribe’s ‘mistake’ by re-translating the whole
complex into Akkadian: *dùram rabiam “a Der àl “akkanakkùtim “a màrùti-
“u the last -“u of which was mechanically rendered by the scribe by
Sumerian -a-ni.
Apart from the X-Y-a(k) syntagma with a bound genitive, there
is free, unbound, genitive with no regens expressed occurring much
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 39
39 stns+\x+iyrs
more rarely, at least in the documentation of historical times: ∞gá
(-a)-kam, za(-a)-kam “it is of me, you” = “it is mine, yours”; An-na-
kam “it is of An (or: of heaven)”. A free genitive may also appear
as a rectum before its regens: In such cases, the regens has to refer
back to the rectum by adding a possessive particle sharing the class
(person or non-person) of the rectum: é-a(-k) lugal-bi “of the house,
its owner” = “the owner of the house”.
Note: This construction represents a universal type. Whether it was more fre-
quent—or even predominant—in prehistoric Sumerian cannot be said.
Note: It is generally difficult to find etymologies for case particles (but see 5.4.2.6 for
the comitative). Th. Jacobsen, “Notes on the Sumerian genitive”, JNES 32 (1973)
161–66, made a tentative proposal to connect genitive [ak] with the verb ak
*“to gather” and to explain (p. 166) é-lú-ak “from ‘house gathering in the man’,
i.e., connecting with him, to ‘house of the man’”. In his last paragraph, though,
Jacobsen mentions the possibility of an underlying “verb ak of not recoverable
specific meaning”.
G. Zólyomi, “Genitive constructions in Sumerian”, JCS 48 (1996) 31–47.
5.4.2.4. Locative
The locative is rendered by -(C)a. We presented an argument for
an original form [aH] (above 3.1.2), but we cannot determine the
vowel quantity: -a(H) or -à(H). Locative -a, like the [a] of genitive
[ak] supersedes the final vowel of the possessive particles of the sg.:
gu
10
, -zu, -a-ni, -bi : -∞ -∞ gá, -za, -a-na, -ba.
Note: Besides u
4
-ba “at (its day =) that time”, there also is u
4
-bi-a (Akkadian in
ùmi“u). The distinction of the two temporal adverb and the reason for the—
uncontracted—u
4
-bi-a are not yet clear, but note below, 7.2.
The main function of the locative is to indicate rest and arrest at a
goal, not movement toward or into something (which would rather
be rendered by the terminative, see 5.4.2.8); there is no insistence
on direct contact (for which see directive, 5.4.2.9).
eri-me-a “in our city”; an-ki-a “in heaven (and) on earth”.
As in many languages, local rest may be transferred to the tem­
poral sphere: “à-mu-ba-ka “(in the middle of =) during that year”;
u
4
-bi-a “at that time”.
The locative, as is evident from its functions, is essentially restricted
to the non-person class, and as such it stands in complementary dis­
tribution with the dative (see 5.4.2.5), which is exclusively used with
nouns of the person class. It does not occur with pl. [(e)ne].
gá (instead of lugal-∞ Note: Ali Letter B 2:1 lugal-∞ gu
10
-ra) “to my lord”, quoted in
Thomsen 1984, 98, is an exception to the above stated rule of complementary
distribution.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 40
40 cn\r+rn riyr
5.4.2.5. Dative
The dative case particle [ra] has the allomorphs [ra] and [r] where
the original distribution was probably between [ra] after a conso­
nant (including -H and genitive -a(k)) and [r] after a vowel. In OS,
no dative is marked graphically when the preceding noun ends in
a vowel (see above 5.4.2.3). In NS and OB Sumerian both -V-Vr
and -V-ra occur. Cf. lugal-/nin-a-ni-ir “to his/her lord/lady”, ubi­
quitous in royal inscriptions, as against lugal-∞gu
10
-ra “to my lord” in
the opening of letters.
Note: Cf. Michalowski 1993 nos. 87–92, 94 Na-ni(-r) “to Nani”, but 93 Na-ni-
ra. Rarely lugal-∞ gu
10
-úr,
d
Nanna-ar “to my lord”, “to the Moongod”.
Cases where, after a vowel, -ra is written instead of -Vr, may either
be regarded as purely orthographical variants or as some kind of
grammatical hyper-correction.
Dative -r(a) with person class nouns almost always stands in com­
plementary distribution with locative -a, occuring with non-person
class nouns.
Note: Zólyomi 1999 proposed a complementary distribution of personal dative
and “inanimate” directive in the prefixed string of verbal directional particles.
As for the relationship between case particles and verbal dimensional
prefix particles, the case of the concord of -r(a) and verbal infixed
-na- “to him/her” is one of the most clearly marked.
The function of dative -r(a) is, generally, to indicate that an action
or state is in a person’s favour; that speech is addressed to a per­
son; or that a certain state of mind (e.g., love, hatred) is felt towards
a person.
5.4.2.6. Comitative
The comitative is expressed by [da]. There is no restriction to either
person or non-person class. The general function is to express com­
pany, doing something or being with someone or something; being
in someone’s neighbourhood; mental and intellectual participation;
ability to do something. The particle may, originally, have been iden­
tical with the noun da “side”; but this cannot be proved.
an-da gú-lá-a “(hang the neck with =) embracing heaven” (said
of the height of a building).
ama-a [d]umu-da gù n[u]-mu(-n)-da(-n)-dé “a mother did not (talk
with =) nag her child” Gudea Cyl. A xiii 3.
Commodity (grain, silver) PN
1
(ergative) PN
2
-da an-da-tuku “PN
1
(creditor) (has with =) is owed by PN
2
(debtor)” NRVN I 61:7, and
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 41
41 stns+\x+iyrs
often in pre-Sargonic and Ur III debt documents (for the verbal
prefix a(l)- see 12.10).
An
d
Inana-da húl-a-e “An, rejoicing over Inana” Inana and Ebih 61.
Note: For var. see Jaques n.d. II 216.
bala-nam-lugal-la-∞ gá 3 “e:gur-ta . . . kù-babbar 1 gín-e hé-eb-da
(-n)-sa
10
“during my royal office one shekel of silver (bought with
it =) could buy three kor of barley each” Sin-kà“id 13:11–19, and
see nos. 8, 10, etc. (RIME 4, 460).
There are two special functions of the comitative:
(a) Our idea of “without” is expressed by X-da nu-me-a “not being
with X”, e.g., ∞gá-da nu-me-a “not being with me” = “without
me” in the sense of “without my permission”; cf. the PN Nin-
da-nu-me-a “Without the Lady?”, being a hypothetical question,
to be continued by something like “is there anything possible?”
(e.g., Struve 1984, 138).
gir-da-nu-me-a is noted as Di∞ Note: In early OS, the PN Di∞ gir-nu-me, e.g., WF
p. 26* n.v. “
d
Nu-me”.
Note: A similar idea underlies English “with-out”, Russian vne “in not”, or
German children’s “mit ohne” for “ohne”.
(b) áb amar-bi-da “the cow with its calf ” expresses an idea closely
related to, or even identical with, “the cow and (the) calf ” (note
the interchangeability of “chicken with/and rice”). Sumerian lacks
a word for “and” and, at the latest in NS, borrowed Akkadian ù.
Note: For the X Y-bi-da construction cf. Turkish Leyla ile Mecnun “with L.,
M.” = “Mecnun and Leyla”. For the borrowing of “and” see above 4.2 (7).
The combination -bi-da has a variant -bi which also occurs in lex­
ical lists (cf. bi = ù MSL 4, 195:159; bi = ù, qa-[du] MSL 4, 175:215
(NBGT); bi, bi-da = ù MSL 4, 137:203 f. (NBGT).
gír-su
d
”ara-bi(-r) “to Nin∞ OS Ent. 28–29 i 5–6
d
Nin-∞ girsu and
”ara”.
Poebel 1923, 147 f.
5.4.2.7. Ablative-instrumental
The ablative-instrumental case particle is expressed by [ta] which,
at least in our Latin transliteration, is phonologically clearly differ­
entiated from comitative [da]. It only occurs with non-person class
nouns and, therefore, cannot occur with pl. [(e)ne].
Its functions are to indicate the starting point from which some­
one or something comes from or by means of which (instrumentally)
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 42
42 cn\r+rn riyr
something is realized or manufactured (cf. French produire qc. à
partir de qc.).
an-gal-ta ki-gal-“è “from the great Above to the great Below”
Inana’s Descent 1.
The local aspect was easily transferred to the temporal: u
4
-bi-ta
“from that day” = “since that time”.

ge“
tukul-kal-ga
d
En-líl-le mu-na(-n)-“úm-ma-ta “with the mighty
weapon which Enlil had entrusted to him”.
The ablative, placed after a nominalized phrase, serves to express
what is a temporal subordinate clause in Akkadian: sa∞g-ki-gíd-da-
d
En-líl-lá-ke
4
Ki“
ki
gu
4
-an-na-gin
7
im-ug
5
-ga-ta “after Enlil’s frown had
killed Ki“ like the bull of Heaven” Curse of Akkade 1.
Note: Or “as by means of the Bull of Heaven”; see above 5.4 for the equative
overriding other cases.
Ablative -ta cannot be applied to person class nouns so that where
“from (a person)” is needed the circumpositional version ki-N-a(k)-
ta, lit. “from the place of N”, is often used. Cf. the ubiquitous ki-
PN
1
(-ak)-ta PN
2
(erg.) “u ba-an-ti “PN
2
received (object) from PN
1
”.
A special function of -ta is to indicate distribution, such a quan­
tity “each”: see, e.g., Sin-kà“id 13:11–19, above 5.4.2.6.
5.4.2.8. Terminative
The terminative case particle owes its designation to its main func­
tion: to indicate that someone or something reaches a goal.
It is usually written with the syllabogram “è, but sometimes as
-V“. It is unknown how the pronunciation of “è differed from [“e]
in “e(-ga) “favoured”.
Note: We owe to Attinger 1993, 253–56, the strict distinction between the “termina­
tive” case particle and the adverbial particle [e“(e)], for which he proposed “adver­
biative” (“adverbiatif ”). A common origin of both particles is not improbable.
Note that Proto-Ea (MSL 14, 33:61) with e-“e É” (var. “e, “i) is
inconsistent as is Ea I (MSL 14, 186:180–182) which by offering the
equivalences e-e“ = a-na, “e-e = ki-ma, and e-e“ = eb-lu, at least to
our modern interpretation, seems to have switched “terminative” (e­
e“ = a-na) and “adverbiative” (“e-e = ki-ma).
The original distribution of the allomorphs [“e] and [“] most prob­
ably has its counterpart in dative [ra] and [r] (see 5.4.2.4), -“è would
then have stood after a consonant, whereas -“ (either written or
latent) would be found after a vowel. However, as regards the allo­
morph [“], either spelling is inconsistent or our present understand­
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 43
43 stns+\x+iyrs
ing is still inadequate. In the ubiquitous dedicatory formula “for
his/her life”, the spelling is consistently nam-ti-la-ni-”È and never
*-ni-i“ oder *-ni-e“. Either there were two syllables [ni“e] or -ni-”È
stood for one syllable [ni“].
Falkenstein 1949, 113 f., referred to the spelling sa∞g-bi“(PE”) “to
its head” Gudea Cyl. B ii 18 with contrasting sa∞g-bi-”È in Cyl. A
iv 11, and see Falkenstein 1949, 30.
The terminative is most obviously opposed to the ablative in such
phrases as an-gal-ta ki-gal-“è (see 5.4.2.7) or Úrim
ki
-ta Nibru
ki
-“è
“from Ur to Nippur”.
Local use is, again, transferred to temporal: u
4
-ul-lí-a-“è “until
(those days =) all future”.
Moreover, the terminative may be used to indicate abstract ways
of reasoning: cause, reason, purpose. Cf. mu-bi-“è “(to its name =)
therefore”; a-na-a“(-àm) “for what is it” = “why”; ní∞ g-ba-a“ “as (the
purpose of ) a present”.
The terminative, set after a nominalized verbal phrase (whether
introduced by u
4
or not) serves to express what is a subordinate tem­
poral clause in Akkadian (with adi ): “until . . .”.
Both the terminative and adverbiative particles have to be kept
separate from the quotation particle [e“e] which the Gudea corpus
equally spell -É” (in OB e-“e). Cf. below 13.1.
5.4.2.9. Directive
The directive (locative-terminative) case particle [e] was, most prob­
ably, identical in sound with the ergative [e] and because both par­
ticles share the same spelling and phonotactic behaviour, the origin
of the ergative particle has been sought in the directive. See above
5.4.2.2.
Be that as it may, by the time we have some insight into the pho­
netic realities hidden behind Sumerian spelling habits, both cases
have clearly established themselves as separate entities and, most
tellingly, the concord of the two case particles with verbal prefix par­
ticles is also different. Cf. Gudea Cyl. A iv 4: kar-Ni∞ gin
ki
-na-ke
4
(dir.)
má (absol.) bí(-n)-ús “he (= Gudea, erg.) had the boat moor at the
quay of N.”.
The main function of the directive is to express movement arriv­
ing (into contact with) an object, or position adjacent to (in contact
with) an object.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 44
44 cn\r+rn riyr
The directive is mainly used with nouns of the non-person class,
and it is only rarely found with person class, e.g., ama dumu-ni
[dumu-(a)ni-e] ní∞ g nu-ma-ni-ra [nu-mma-ni-n-ra] “no mother beat
her child”, lit. “let something hit at her child” Gudea Cyl. A xiii 3.
We may try to visualize the directional functions of locative, ter­
minative, and directive in the following graphs:
o
o
Locative: or motion into, position inside.
Terminative: motion towards.
Directive: or motion arriving at, position next to.
5.4.2.10. Equative
Noun + -gin
7
(GIM) formations may be taken as cases because the
particle has the same position as the case particles for ergative through
directive. Equative stands apart, however, in that it may, virtually,
contain the notion expressed by a dimensional case (cf. above, 5.1
and see 5.4.2.7 end).
The equative expresses comparison and corresponds to Akkadian
kìma (which may equally imply a dimensional case). Equative may
be used with nouns of both classes: person or non-person, but it is
not attested with plural.
GIM = gin
7
shares with some substantives the -m/n variant of its
final consonant. In Proto-Ea 530 (MSL 14, 53), GIM/DÍM is glossed
gi-in, ge-en, and gi-im. When -GIM is followed by the copula [am]
the spelling is GIM-nam, thus clearly leading to -gin
7
-nam.
Note, however, ur
5
-GIM-ma-àm in Lugalbanda and Enm. 163 (cf. Thomsen 1984,
109 [249]) where -ma- has been confirmed by collation (Wilcke 1969, 107, note
to line 163).
It cannot be excluded that the variant form GIM = gim was influenced
by the [m] of Akkadian kìma.
Note: For the PN A-ba-DN/ses-∞gu
10
-GIM, there are variant spellings with -KI
and -KID = -gé). The first var., A-ba-
d
En-líl-KI (YOS 4, 302:27) may be dis­
missed as a simple case of dittography: line 28 offers “à-EN.LÍL.KI “in Nippur”.
For A-ba-ses-∞gu
10
-gé see ITT 4, 7450:8.
5.4.2.11. Recapitulation of the case particles
The addition of case particles 2 and 4 to 10 is mutually exclusive,
this also being the reason for the non-existence of such expressions
as *lugal-ra-gin
7
“like for a king”.
EDZARD_f6_28-45 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 45
45 stns+\x+iyrs
Note: a-ab-ba-sig-ta-ta “from the Lower Sea” in Lugal-zagesi 1 ii 4–5 (Steible
1982/II 317) is no exception to the rule because -ta is added to a frozen expres­
sion, sig-ta “from-below” = “lower”. See the discussion by Wilcke 1990, 471–475,
who also offers comparable examples for . . . -ta-(a)k and . . . -ta-“è.
On the other hand, any of the case particles 2 to 10 may be added
to genitive (3) [(a)(k)], yielding the sequences [ake], [aka(k)], [aka],
[a(k)ra], [a(k)da], [a(k)ta], [a(k)“e], [ake], [a(k)gin].
The case system is more restricted in the plural of person class
where as well as the locative (4) and the ablative (7), which are
incompatible with person class, the absolutive (1) and equative (10)
are unrepresented.
46
EDZARD_f7_46-48 4/28/03 9:37 AM Page 46
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
47
EDZARD_f7_46-48 4/28/03 9:37 AM Page 47
CHAPTER SIX
ADJECTIVES
Adjectives accompany substantives which they qualify; or, as a pre­
dicate, they occur with the verbal copula: udug-hul “evil demon”;
é-gibil “new house”.
Once a substantive + adjective compound has been formed it is
impenetrable and any particle has to follow the complex as a whole:
ses-gal-ra “to the (big brother =) school overseer”.
An apparent exception occurs when a substantive is followed by
the individualizing plural element didli (see 5.3.4). Here, dedli vir­
tually also functions as an adjective as it is in origin, but it is closer
to the substantive: bàd-dedli-gal-gal “the individual big fortresses”.
There has been much debate on whether adjectives should be
considered a part of speech of their own (cf. 4.2. (1b)). Gragg 1968,
91, proposed to classify them as a sub-category of the verb, by which
he is—tentatively—followed by Thomsen 1984, 64. Attinger 1993,
148 f., distinguishes between primary and secondary adjectives, the
criterion being whether or not they may be provided with the suffixed
particle [a]. Krecher 1978, 376–403 (esp. 382–85), had proposed
determinant force to -a: zi(-d) “true”, zi-da “right (not left)”. Formally,
zi-da is indistinguishable from du
11
-ga “said”, i.e., the so-called ¢am†u
participle of the verb (see 12.14.2). Krecher’s study was taken up by
Klein 1993, 81–98, “The suffix of determination /a/”.
We owe to Black 2003 the first extensive study of the Sumerian
adjective, and he offers morphological and syntactical criteria for the
identification of adjectives. Much of the following is based on his
study.
The respective criteria are not applicable to each and every adjec­
tive, but rather apply in a variety of situations: ability to reduplicate
(shared with substantives and verbs); suffix -a (shared with the ¢am†u
participle of the verb); negation with nu- (shared with verbs); posi­
tion after a substantive; “nominal predicate of copular clauses”.
Black sees no “watertight” category of adjective and, therefore,
submits ‘adjectives’ to scrutiny in terms of both their formation and
EDZARD_f7_46-48 4/28/03 9:37 AM Page 48
48 cn\r+rn six
semantic type (dimension, physical property, colour, human propen­
sity, age, value, speed, uncertain).
In view of the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area (see 17) and the
bilingual lexical entries, we might, however, venture a rather prag­
matic approach: what did the scribes, in their Akkadian translations,
consider as adjectives (for in Akkadian, the adjective as a part of
speech is not contested)? Their approach was probably very close to
ours.
The adjective occurs in simple form: mu-gibil “new year”, udug­
hul “evil demon”, kisal-mah “greatest (= main) court”.
It occurs in reduplication: na
4
-gal-gal “big stones”, me-
d
kal-
d
kal
“highly valued rites”.
Note: For reduplicated adjectives as a means to express the pl. of the preceding
substantive see 5.3.7.
A special feature is the reduplication of some adjectives denoting
colours: bar
6
-bar
6
“white”, from Ur III on spelled by single UD, but
pronounced babbar; sig
7
-sig
7
“green”.
An adjective occurs as a base + particle -a: á-zi-da “right (arm =)
side” (see Krecher 1978, above); ka“ ní∞g-du
10
-ga “(beer, good thing =)
beer of specially good quality”.
Note: ní∞g-du
10
-ga = *dummuqu “improved”; is there a correspondence with D-
stem (“quttulu”) adjectives of Akkadian? ( J. N. Postgate)
There is a considerable number of compound nouns which Black
treats, according to their syntactical position, as extended adjectives;
we quote only three examples from Black’s catalogue: á-∞ gál “(strength
available =) strong”, ní-tuku “(having awe =) reverent”; gal-zu “(know­
ing great =) skilful”.
For the adjective as a base for adverbial expressions see ch. 11.
49
EDZARD_f8_49-51 4/28/03 9:38 AM Page 49
CHAPTER SEVEN
DETERMINATION, SPECIFICATION:
DEMONSTRATIVE PARTICLES
Certain particles suffixed to a substantive may serve to mark it as
something set apart. These particles have traditionally been treated
as dependent forms of the demonstrative pronoun.
See Falkenstein 1949, 55 f.; 1959, 34; Thomsen 1984, 80–82; Attinger 1993, 175 f.
As with possession (see 5.2) we prefer to separate “pronouns” as
independent words from particles attached to a noun. In the case
of determination or specification there is another reason to keep
demonstrative pronouns and demonstrative particles separated. Whereas
free ne(n) “this” only occurs as non-person class, dependent -ne(n)
may refer to substantives of both the person and non-person classes.
There are five particles to be discussed: -ne(n), -bi, -e, -ri, and
-“e. The evidence for some of these is not uncontroversial, partly
because of orthographical ambiguity or scarcity of references.
7.1. -ne(n)
-ne(n): u
4
-ne-na “on this very day” Gudea Stat. B ix 7.
Note: For this passage Falkenstein 1949, 55 fn. 8, had a translation “an diesem
seinen Tag”, combining a demonstrative and a possessive particle. Thomsen 1984,
80, has “on this day”.
u
4
-ne má“-∞ gi
6
-ka “on that day, in a night time vision” Gudea Cyl.
A i 17.
Note: Pace Edzard 1997, 69, who reads u
4
-dè. A sequence directive [e] and loca­
tive [a] for describing the time of the day would, however, be quite unusual. u
4
­
ne is virtually in the locative, too, but the case particle [a] is only noted at the
end of the string.
lú-ne is so far only attested in lexical context: lú-ne-da = itti annîm
“with this one” OBGT I 307 (MSL 4, 48); ki-lú-ne(-k)-ta = itti annîm
ibid. 308, and see lines 309–341.
Note: It is hard for modern grammarians to judge whether these examples should
be considered—at least partly—as a learned exercise or as a reflex of late OB
spoken Sumerian. Already ibid. lines 309 f. lú-ne-da-me-e“, lú-ne-me-e“-da = itti
annûtim raise our suspicion, because the pl. of the copula, me-e“ “they are”, is
EDZARD_f8_49-51 4/28/03 9:38 AM Page 50
50 cn\r+rn sryrx
mechanically inserted to express nominal, not verbal, plurality. On the other
hand, most of the sg. examples meet the standards we are setting for “our”
Sumerian grammar.
7.2. -bi
-bi is most probably identical with the possessive particle of the 3
rd
sg. non-person class, and one easily sees a transition in meaning
from “its, relating to something” to “that”, “the aforementioned”.
u
4
-bi-a “(in its day =) at that time”, sometimes referring to pri­
mordial days.
u
4
-bi-a already in Ukg. 4 ii 4 = 5 ii 5. The distribution of u
4
-bi-a
and u
4
-ba (cf. 5.2) is not yet clear. Was the [i] of demonstrative [bi]
not overridden by the locative case particle [a] as was the [i] of pos­
sessive [bi]? In that case one would, perhaps, have to distinguish
between [biH] and [bi].
7.3. -e(?)
It is difficult to establish the separate existence of a demonstrative
particle [e], since it would be identical in spelling with the ergative
and directive case particles.
Gudea Stat. B vii 49–53 is often quoted: alan-e ù kù-nu za-gìn
g-∞ nu-ga-àm [nu-(i)nga-am] ù erida-nu ù nagga-nu sipar-nu ki∞ gá lú
gá-∞ nu-ba-∞ gá “for the/this statue nobody was supposed to apply in
work silver or lapis lazuli, or copper or tin or bronze”. Here, it is
not clear at all whether alan-e is “statue + demonstrative particle”
or simply the directive case (“to apply in work material to the statue”).
-e is attested in lexical context: lú-e-ra, lú-e-me-e“-a OBGT I 326 f.
(MSL 4, 49).
For the second form, cf. the note at the end of 7.1.
Woods 2003, sees the demonstrative -e as the origin of the erga­
tive case particle. See also Yoshikawa, end of 7.4.
7.4. -ri(?)
With -ri it is uncertain whether it should be considered a demon­
strative or an adjective meaning “far away, remote”; cf. CAD N/2, 186
EDZARD_f8_49-51 4/28/03 9:38 AM Page 51
51 rr+rnvix\+iox. srrciric\+iox
nesû adj. lex. for Nabnìtu X 36 (MSL 16, 118: IX (= X) 36 where
it is read dal) and Aa II/7: 11' (MSL 14, 293: ri-i = né-su-ú).
-ri is mainly found in the phrase u
4
-/∞gi
6
-/mu-ri(-a) “in those
days/nights/years” when the poet refers to beginning in primordial
time; cf. already OS TS” 79 i 1–5 (UD.GAL.NUN text). -ri-a may
be analysed either as [ri] + locative case particle -a or as [ri] + -a
of ¢am†u participle, with the locative not further noted in spelling.
Note: Shaffer 1963, 122, referred to SBH no. 4:162 f. etc. u
4
-RI-LI-na (= ina
ùmè ullùti “in those days”) and he preferred reading u
4
-re-èn-na instead of u
4
-dal-
le-na, from which Attinger 1993, 176, deduces a pair “-re/i, -re-èn”. The topic
must be left to further research.
Note: Yoshikawa 1993, 185–92, tried to show there was a “Spatial Deictic System
in Sumerian”, with -e “something in the space which is close to the speaker and
addressee” and -ri “something which is remote from the space occupied by the
speaker or addressee”. Yoshikawa essentially bases himself on lexical texts, not
on context examples.
7.5. -“e( ?)
-“e occurs in lú-“e in Gilgame“ and Agga 69–71, 91–92 referring to
Gilgame“ seen on the Uruk city wall by beleaguering Agga. Jacobsen
1965, 177 fn. 55, tentatively proposed “anyone from here” basing
himself on NGBT II i 11–15 (MSL 4, 158 f.) where, however, only
line 11 [lú-“]e = animmamû can be restored more or less reliably. Katz
1993, 43, 45 offers “that person”; Römer 1980 lines 91 f. lú-“e “der
Mann da”.
So, due to the extreme scarcity of attestation, -“e is not a certain
candidate for inclusion among the demonstrative particles.
7.6. Recapitulation
To sum up, out of the five elements discussed here, -ne(n), -bi, -e,
-ri, and -“e, only two: -ne(n) and -bi, may be convincingly classified
as demonstrative particles.
52
EDZARD_f9_52-53 4/28/03 9:38 AM Page 52
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
53
EDZARD_f9_52-53 4/28/03 9:38 AM Page 53
CHAPTER EIGHT
RESUMING THE SEQUENCE OF PARTICLES FOR
POSSESSION, NUMBER AND CASE
In Sumerian ses-∞ gu
10
-(e)ne-r(a) “to my brothers” exhibits a sequence
possessive—number—case whose particles cannot switch in rank one
with another.
Note: Demonstrative particles (see 7) are not included here. They most proba­
bly share the rank with possession.
For the sake of comparison, examples are given for Turkish, Mongolian,
Hungarian, and Finnish:
Turkish: karde{-ler-im-e “to my brothers” (brother—pl.—poss.—case).
Mongolian: (minu) aqa-nar-dur “to (my) brothers” (brother—pl.—case).
Hungarian: barát-a-im-nak “to my brothers” (brother—pl.—poss.—
case).
Finnish: talo-i-ssa-ni “in my houses” (house—pl.—case—poss.).
These few examples may show that there is no “universal” rule, in
agglutinative languages, for the hierarchy of the suffixed morphemes
used to express possession, number, and case.
54
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 54
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
55
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 55
CHAPTER NINE
PRONOUNS
9.1. Prnsox\r rnoxotxs
The system is still incomplete for us in the 1
st
and 2
nd
pl. This cor­
responds to the strange fact that many inflected forms of the verb
in the 1
st
and 2
nd
pl. cannot be safely reconstructed and/or, when
they occur in different manuscripts of OB literary texts, present incon­
gruous variants.
3
2
1 gá-e (or contracted ∞
st
sg. ∞ ge
24
-e)
nd
sg. za-e (but note contracted NS zé)
rd
sg. person cl. OS a-ne, NS, OB e-ne
3
rd
sg. non-person class: no independent pronoun is safely attested
although one would expect it, given the existence of possessive -bi.
ur
5
“this, that” may be considered a substitute. ur
5
, however, does
not seem to occur alone, only in combination with case particles
and/or the copula: ur
5
-gin
7
“like this”, ur
5
-“è-àm “for this (purpose)”.
Note to 1
st
and 2
nd
sg.: It is hard to ascertain the exact phonetic nature of these
gá-ra, za-ra may point to bases [∞ pronouns. The dative forms ∞ ga], [za]. But regres­
ge] ¡ [∞ sive assimilation is equally possible. Moreover, [∞ gara], [ze] ¡ [zara] would
not be inconceivable. Cf. modern Turkish ben, sen with datives bana, sana.
1
st
pl. me, secondarily me-en-dè-en
Note: me = ni-i-nu is clearly offered by Proto-Ea vocabulary 71:5 (MSL 14, 19),
and there is no need to attribute me of Proto-Ea to the possessive particles, -me
“our” (see 5.2). Admittedly, however, independent me “we” is so far unattested
in context, but the chances of meeting such a me are—statistically—quite small,
because the personal pronouns only occur when emphasis is needed.
The lexical equation me-da-nu = i-na ba-l[u-ni ] “without us” OBGT
I (vii) 484 (MSL 4, 53) is obviously derived from me “we”.
The (secondary) form me-en-dè-en looks identical to the inflected
copula, “we are”: [me-(e)nden]. For me-en-dè(-n) in OB literary con­
text, cf. Lam. Ur and Sumer 240.
2
nd
pl. me-en-zé-en, za-e-me-en-zé-en
Attested lexically only.
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 56
56 cn\r+rn xixr
Note: me-en-zé-en looks identical to the inflected copula, “you are”: [me-(e)nzen].
By analogy with the relation -zu : -zu-ne-ne in the set of possessive particles, a
form *za-ne(-ne) might be reconstructed, because “you” (pl.) often is “thou + pl.”.
For secondary pronouns of the 1
st
and 2
nd
pl. cf., e.g., Spanish
nosotros, vosotros, or French nous autres (français) “we (French)”.
3
rd
pl. person class a-ne-ne (OS, NS), e-ne-ne (OB).
Here, the relation between 3
rd
sg. and pl. clearly is one of a pri­
vate opposition: “they” = “he/she + pl.”, [ane-(e)ne], [ene-(e)ne].
As in many languages, Sumerian personal pronouns have a reduced
declension pattern, compared to that of the substantive. There is just
one form, at least judging by orthography, for absolutive and erga­
tive; then there are genitive, dative, comitative, and equative. Locative
and ablative are not to be expected because they only occur with
non-person class. Terminative is so far unattested, and directive, if
it existed, would have been identical in spelling with the ergative.
As with substantives, an ablative can, however, be constructed with
the help of the circumpositional syntagma ki- . . . -ta. So ki-zu-ta
“from your place” would equal “from you” (cf. 5.4.2.7 end).
The free genitive ∞ gá(-a)-kam, za(-a)-kam “it is (of me, you =) mine,
yours” functions as a free possessive pronoun (see 9.2).
gá(-a)-ra, ∞ Dative: ∞ gá(-a)-ar, zá(-a)-ra, e-ne-ra, e-ne-er “to me, you,
him/her”.
Comitative: ∞gá-e-da-nu-me-a “not being with me” = “without me”,
“without my permission” (cf. 5.4.2.6).
Equative: ∞gá-e-gin
7
-nam “it is like me”.
Note: We do not quote the rich lexical evidence for 1
st
to 3
rd
pl. pronouns, as
offered in OBGT I 376–556 (MSL 4, 50–54), because much of the Babylonian
scribe’s interpretation looks dubious in the light of modern scholarship. Com­
pare e-ne-ne-ra and e-ne-ne-er (ibid. 554 and 556) both of which we would
translate as “to them” [enene-r(a)], but are rendered by “u-nu-ti and e-li-“u-nu
respectively.
9.2. Possrssiyr rnoxotxs
Strictly speaking, these exist only in the form of the genitive of the
personal pronoun combined with the copula, e.g., ∞gá(-a)-kam “it is
(of me =) mine”.
We have treated the possessive particles by themselves under “pos­
session” (see 5.2). While the morphological relation between those
particles and the free forms is obvious, it is still difficult to explain
the origin of the possessive particles unless we move back into pre­
history (cf. Poebel 1923, 76).
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 57
rnoxotxs 57
At any rate, the high antiquity of possessive particles is suggested
by the fact that of the categories of possession, number, and case
particles, the possessive particles rank closest to the substantive (see
above ch. 8).
9.3. Drvoxs+n\+iyr rnoxotxs
The Sumerian system is more difficult to describe and define than the
very clearly developed system of Akkadian demonstrative pronouns.
There are, so far, only two words known that would deserve the
definition of (independent) demonstrative pronouns:
9.3.1. ur
5
ur
5
(already mentioned 9.1): ur
5
hé-na-nam(-ma-àm) “let it really be
this”, ur
5
-gin
7
“like this”, ur
5
-ra-ke
4
-é“ “therefore” (lit. [ur-ak] as a
free genitive, with the adverbiative particle [e“(e)]).
ur
5
is restricted to the non-person class and has, therefore, been
considered a substitute for the—non-existing—personal pronoun, 3
rd
sg. non-person class.
9.3.2. ne-e(n)
ne-e(n) is equally restricted to the non-person class, at least as far
as it occurs independently.
∞gá-e za-kam ne-e a-na-àm ì-til-l[e] “I am yours, this—what is it?—
will (live =) stay” Innin “ag. 246 (ZA 65, 198).
lú gùd-∞ gá ne-en ba-e(-a)-AG-a “you, the person (who) did this to
my nest” Lugalb. 105, and see Wilcke 1969, 164.
ne nam-[di]-
d
En-líl-lá “this is [a judgement] of Enlil” Ur III PN,
NG no. 40:9, reflected by ne-en-nam di-ku
5
-
d
Nanna-kam = annûm
dìnu “a Sin “as (regards) this, it is a judgement of the Moongod” van
Dijk 1953, 129:42 f. (PBS 10/1, 1 iv 18).
9.4. Ix+rnnoo\+iyr rnoxotxs
Here, the universal pair “who” and “what” prevails, the first [aba]
asking about someone of the person class, the second [ana] about
something of the non-person class. The opposition b : n (instead of
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 58
58 cn\r+rn xixr
*n : b) is surprising, at least if one expects a morphological corre­
spondence between interrogative and personal pronouns as, e.g., in
Latin quis : is, quid : id or German wer : er, was : das.
3
For a-ba, ergative a-ba-a is attested, and both a-ba and a-na, like
rd
person class pronoun e-ne, may be combined with the copula
-àm. Generally speaking, the restrictions as to the affixation of case
particles affecting the personal pronouns apply equally to the inter­
rogative pronouns (see 9.1).
There is no adjectival interrogative, as a possible correspondence
to Akkadian ajjû, ajjìtu “which”. For na-me “any one” see 9.5.2.
Note: CAD A/1, 234 ajû lex., quotes a late reference where na-me is rendered
by ajjû “which”; here, however, the Sumerian text has the well-known syntagma
na-me + negated verb, perhaps as a question, so that the Akkadian translation
“which one” would actually render “(has) no one”.
9.5. Arrrorr rnoxotxs
9.5.1. Reflexive pronoun?
The Sumerian base ní-(te-) serves to express, along with possessive
particles, notions which we are used to translate by “myself ”, etc.
i.e., by a reflexive pronoun: “ám-ní-te-na “purchase price of him­
self ” (= for his own person) NG no. 38:7; ní-zu-“è ∞gé“tu-zu “to your­
self your ear!” (= “be aware of yourself ”) Angim 81, and see more
examples in Thomsen 1984, 78 f.
There seems to have been a general distribution rule: ní-te-V ver­
sus ní-C, i.e., ní-∞gu
10
/-zu versus ní-te(-a)-ni, ní-te(-a)-ne-ne for
“my/your/itself ”, “him/herself, themselves”. But note ní-te-∞ gu
10
-“è
“to myself ” UET 6/2, 144:36 f., quoted by Attinger 1993, 174.
NS or OB ní-te is preceded by OS ME-te, most probably to be
read ni
x
-te (Attinger 1993, 174): Nite(TE:ME)-ga-i “Let me praise
(my)self ” DP 73 ii 2 (PN).
For bilingual contexts where ní-te = ramanu see CAD R 117 lex.
ní-(te-) + possessive particle is a nominal phrase not different from,
e.g., sa∞g + poss. particle, and therefore, strictly speaking, ní-(te-) can­
not be considered a “pronoun”. One would rather associate ní-(te)
with parts of the body.
Alster 1974, 178–80; Cavigneaux 1978, 177–85; Thomsen 1984, 78 f.; Attinger
1993, 174 f. with more lit.
EDZARD_f10_54-59 4/28/03 2:40 PM Page 59
 59
9.5.2. Indefinite pronoun?
na-me is found both as a substantive and as an adjectival base, inde­
pendent of person or non-person class. In the majority of references
it occurs along with a negative verbal form, so that it means either
(positive) “any (one)” or (negative) “no (one)”.
lú-na-me, níg-na-me, u
4
-na-me “anyone”, “anything”, “any (day =)
º
time”; na-me nu-kúr-ru “nobody will alter it” SKIZ 83 i 15.
Note: In the last example as in parallel cases, na-me may be an abbreviated form
of lú-na-me.
[me] of na-me should at least until we have better knowledge not
be associated with the verbal base me “to be”.
Thomsen 1984, 78; Attinger 1993, 174.
60
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 60
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
61
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 61
CHAPTER TEN
NUMERALS
10.1. Grxrn\r
The Sumerian language had a “sexagesimal” system in which numer­
ation proceeded in alternating steps of 10 and 6: 1–10, 10–60, 60–
600, 600–3600, 3600–36000, 36000–216000. With sixty as the “hun­
dred”, Sumerian numeration had the enormous advantage of being
able to divide by 3 or 6 without leaving a remainder. The sexa­
gesimal system has left its traces in our modern divisions of the hour
or of the compass. It permeates the metrological systems of the
Ancient Near East.
Powell 1971; 1989.
Since cardinal and ordinal numbers, including fractions, as well as
notations of length, surface, volume, capacity, and weight are next
to exclusively written with number signs, we are poorly informed on
the pronunciation and the morpho-syntactical behaviour of numbers
in Sumerian. To what degree were numbers subject to case inflection?
If 600 was pronounced ∞ ge“-u “sixty ten”, i.e., “ten (times) sixty”,
what was the pronunciation of “sixty (plus) ten”, i.e., 70? Was there
a difference of stress or some other means of intonation, e.g., *[∞gé“(d)u]
versus [∞ ge“(d)ú]?
Syntactically, persons or things counted were followed, not pre­
ceded, by numerals so that the position of a numeral corresponds
to that of an adjective. For—purely graphic—exceptions to this rule
see 5.3.6, note.
10.2. C\nrix\r xtvnrns
The oldest pronunciation guide for the Sumerian cardinal numbers
2 to 10 is an exercise tablet from Ebla: TM 75.G. 2198: Edzard
1980; 2003b.
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 62
62  
Ebla
1. (slanted vertical wedge)
2. mì-nu [min]
3. i“
11
-“a-am [i“/e“ + copula]
4. li-mu [lim(m)u]
5. i [ia]
6. A-“u [A“]
7. ù-mi-nu [umin]
8. ù-sa-am [us + copula]
9. ì-li-mu [ilim(m)u]
10. u
9
-wa-mu [*haw(?) + copula]
later tradition
a“, deli, di“ (ge
(4)
)
min
e“
5
limmu
ía
à“
umun
5
, imin
ussu
ilimmu
u
Diakonoff 1983, 83–93, cf. G. Pettinato, AIUON 41 (1981) 141–43 (“inspired” by
Edzard 1980).
Notes on the individual numbers 1 to 10:
For 1 and the following cardinal numbers, the most exhaustive
treatment still is that by Powell 1971, 13 ff.
1: for deli (di-li, de-e-li, du-li), a“ and di“ so far only an approxi­
mate distribution pattern can be offered:
deli is “single, unique” (wèdum); cf. lú-sa∞g-deli, glossed sagdilû “(per­
son, single head =) bachelor”. Reduplicated deli: dedli indicates
detailed plurality (see 5.3.4). A”-ni “he/she all alone” is read deli­
ni or a“(a)-ni by individual authors. The OS PN [A“ani “he alone”]
is spelled A-“a
4
(DU)-ni (DP 125 iv 6), or A“
x
(GE
23
)-“a
4
-ni (DP 124
iv 2; 126 iv 3).
See J. Bauer, RA 64 (1970) 188, partly corrected by J. N. Postgate,
AfO 24 (1973) 77.
However, deli-du-ni “he is coming alone” might also be proposed.
A”(deli)-di∞ gir-re-ne “the only one of the gods” VS 10, 199 iii 4 (cf.
Falkenstein 1959, 66). A similar uncertainty is found with the epi­
thet of the Moongod,
d
A”-im
4
/im-bar
6
-bar
6
/babbar (discussed by
M. Krebernik, RlA 8, 362 f. § 2.3; there is no var. “á“”).
It is difficult to differentiate a“ from deli. If 6 really were 5+1,
Ebla A-“u might be explained as *[ia+a“]. This is, however, quite
uncertain.
di“ may have been the regular counting word (“one, two, three”),
not bound to either person or non-person class. bandi“ “1 seah” (fol­
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 63
 63
lowed by banmin), munus di“-àm “there was a (certain) woman”
Gudea Cyl. A iv 23. There is, however, no lexical correspondence
di“ = i“tèn, and, strangely enough, di“ is not found in Proto-Ea.
For the Emesal form [did] see Schretter 1990, 164 f.
2: It is unclear whether Ebla mì-nu stands for [min] (which we
adopted) or for bi-syllabic [minu]. Ebla spelling does not yet clearly
denote syllable-closing consonants and often resorts to CV(-C) or
CV-C(V). Note, however, that 4 li-mu definitely stands for the bi-
syllabic numeral.
Apart from min, there is tab “double, parallel”. tab and min are
combined in gú-tab-min-bi “its two parallel banks”.
3: Ebla i“
11
-“a-am can only be explained as 3 [i“] or [e“] + copula
[am]; note also the copula in Ebla 8 and 10.
4: Ebla li-mu may stand for single or lengthened [m]. Post-Eblaic
lexical glosses note lim-mu, rarely LAM-mu. For ní∞g-úr-limmu “(thing
four limbs =) quadrupeds” an emesal writing has ne-mu-li-mu (TCL
15, 3:4; Falkenstein, ZA 53 [1959] 101 fn. 34).
5: Ebla i is ambiguous as it may stand for [ya] or [yi] (cf. Krebernik,
ZA 72 [1982] 191). i early found its way into Sumerian and Akkadian
syllabaries. Powell 1971, 35 pointed to the OS spelling “u-IÁ for
“u-i “barber”. 5 is part of numerals 7 and 9; see below.
6: Ebla A-“u (for the -u ending see above 2) is graphically as ambigu­
ous as is Ebla 5. A may stand for [a, ay, aw, ya] (Krebernik, ZA
72 [1982] 180 f.).
6 has been analysed as 5+1 by analogy with the evident cases of
5+2 and 5+4. So, *ia+a“ “5+1” might have yielded [ya’a“], [yà“].
The post-Eblaic gloss a-á“ may, but does not have to, stand for [à“];
a in a-á“ may also be taken as a simple vocalic indicator:
a
á“. If 6
really were a compound of 5+1, then the element “1” could only
be [a“] and not [di“].
7: Ebla ù-mi-nu (for the ending -u see above 2) does not display the
expected form 5+2 because of its Anlaut [u]. This, however, is most
probably due to anology with the Anlaut of following 8, [us] or
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 64
64  
[us(s)a]. Later, [umin] turned into [umun]. The etymologically “cor­
rect” imin, used by most Assyriologists, is not attested at all in lex­
ical glosses, but occurs in sign-names; see Powell 1971,40; Gong
2000, 139.
Note: Rhyme and assonance are universally attested in neighbouring digits: cf.
Latin quattuor, quinque, Turkish altı, yedi, sekiz, dokuz, Finnish yksi, kaksi, viisi,
kuusi, or, most noteworthy, Slavic 9 whose initial n- turned into d- by analogy
with 10, e.g., Russian dev’at’, des’at’.
8: Ebla ù-sa-am, i.e., [us + copula] or [us(s)a + copula], is definitely
not *5+3, as is still assumed in Thomsen 1984, 82 who derives “ussu
< *iá+e“
5
(5+3)”.
Powell 1971, 37 with fn. 2 and 38 fnn. 1–4 (with previous literature) already
clearly dismissed the 5+3 theory.
For the difficulties we encounter with the exact form of 8, see Powell
1971, 41 f.
9: Ebla ì-li-mu offers the earliest and clearest example of a Sumerian
digit compound 5+n. Note, however, that the first element, 5, is
spelled NI = ì and not I as with 5. Maybe ì (versus I) noted a
reduced form of 5 in the compound.
10: Ebla U
9
-PI-mu is difficult to “normalize”. Anlaut U
9
with val­
ues [ha] or [˙a] (cf. Edzard 1980, 126) would be compatible with
two of the five glosses preserved for U “ten”: u
4
, ú, a, hu-u, ha-a
(Powell 1971, 43 f.). Since there is no trace of [m] in later glosses,
Ebla -mu may be again part of the copula [am]: *ha-wa-m(u), leav­
ing us with *haw+am. *[haw], in the course of time, may have
turned into *[hò] *[hù].
At this juncture it must be remembered that U, in earlier OB
Akkadian spelling, was used to indicate [o], as Poebel 1939, 116 f.
and Westenholz 1991 have shown.
No evidence is so far available for the pronunciation of numbers
11 to 19. We may guess at juxtaposition of 10 + digit with possi­
ble reduction or contraction in the compounds.
20: [ni“], [ne“]. For lexical evidence see Powell 1971, 48; CAD E
367 e“rà lex. No references are available before the 1st mill.
30: [u“(u)]. Powell 1971, 48; CAD ”/l, 234 “alà“à lex. No references
are available before the 1st mill.
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 65
 65
Note: In view of 50 = 40 + 10, one might venture an evolution *ni“-u 20+10
> *(n)u“u. But an independent word is possible as well. For independent 30 cf.,
e.g., Turkish otuz.
40: [nimin], [nìn]: Powell 1971, 48, CAD E 255 erbà lex. No ref­
erences are available before the 1st mill.
[nimin], glossed ni-mìn, ni-mi-in, ni-in, may be explained as a
reduced form of *[ni“-min] “two twenties”.
50: [ninnû], Powell 1971, 49; CAD ›, 81 ¢am“à lex.
[ninnû], glossed ni-/ni-in-/nin-nu-u is a short form for *[ni(“)min-u]
“forty + ten”. No references are available before the 1st mill.
Note: ninnû (not “ninnu”, pace Edzard 1997 passim in “Eninnu”) is certainly a
far shot from the contemporary—and virtually unknown—pronunciation of Nin-
Girsu’s ziggurat complex at ª ª Girsu, é-ninnû “50 houses” during the 24th to 21st
centuries B.C.
60: [∞ge“(d)]. The reading of 60 was definitely settled by Steinkeller
1979, 176–87. For lex. evidence see Powell 1971, 50–53; CAD ”/3,
380 “u“i lex.
Whereas multiples of 60 have been glossed, e.g., 120 = ∞ge“-min,
we do not know how 61, 62 etc. were pronounced. It is hardly pos­
sible in a society permeated with calculation and accountancy, that
ge“+min “sixty (plus) two” and *∞ 62 *∞ gé“-min “two sixties” should
have been homophonous.
600: [∞ ge“(d)u] “ten sixties”. Again, we may ask how 600 was dis­
tinguished in fast speech from 60 + 10 = 70.
Akkadian had an individual word for 600, nèru of unknown ori­
gin, remembered by Greek n∞row.
600 became a new basic number so that 1200 was *∞ ge“(d)u-min
“two six-hundreds”.
Note: Let it be asked how 2002 (3 × 600 + 3 × 60 + 22) was pronounced. Was
ge“(d)u-e“ ∞ it something close to *[∞ ge“-e“ ni“-min]?
3600: [“ar] “ár “circle” represents both the concrete figure and the
“myriad”. It was borrowed by Akkadian as “àr and, finally, by Greek
as sãrow. See Powell 1971, 78; CAD ”/2, 36.
36,000: [“aru] is spelled ”ÁR × U until at least Ur III and later on
”ÁR.U.
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 66
66 cn\r+rn +rx
Unlike 600, 36,000 has not become a new basic number. 72,000
is “ár-ni“ “twenty 3600” and not *“two 36,000”. 180,000 is “ár-ninnû
“fifty 3600”. See Powell 1971, 73–77.
ge“(d)] = “sixty 3600, also “ár-gal “big 3600”. [“ar∞ 216,000: [“ar∞ ge“(d)]
is written ”ÁR × ª GÉ” or ”ÁR × U-gunû until Ur III and later on
”ÁR.ªGÉ”. See Powell 1971, 73–78.
The highest number attested is 216,000 × 60 = 12,960,000, “ár-gal-
“u-nu-tag-ga “big 3600 that has not been touched”. See Powell 1971,
76–78.
In practice, “ár-gal occurs in Ur III administrative documents with
the count of reed-bundles; cf., e.g., “ár-gal, 4 ”ÁR × U, 2 “ár 4 600,
7 60, 27 = 216,000 + 144,000 + 7200 + 2400 + 420 + 27 =
367,647 gi-sa “reed-bundles” P. Artzi/S. Lewy, Atiqot 4 (1965) no.
8 iv 3 = 10–11.
Note: It is interesting to note the increase of the number 3600 −> 36,000 −>
216,000 in royal inscriptions from ªGirsu in the phrase “when god’s hands seized
(the ruler) from among n people”: [“à-l]ú-“ár-ta (Enmetena, CIRPL Ent. 32 i 2''),
“à-lú-”ÁR × U-ta (Erikagina, CIRPL Ukg. 4/5 vii 18), “á-lú-”ÁR × U-gunû-ta
(Gudea, Stat. B iii 10). It would, of course, be absurd to refer this diachronic
increase of symbolic figures to a corresponding growth of the population.
Sumerian numerals, as to be expected, had special forms in Emesal.
Emesal voc. III 131–133 (MSL 4, 39) notes [did], [imma] and
[ammu“] for “1, 2, 3”; for [mu“] “60” see ibid. 134–138. More
details in Powell 1971, 51–53; Schretter 1990, 154 ff. nos. 53, 70,
71, 190, 331, 395, 397.
A peculiar system, rising in groups of three, for 1 to 7 is found
in the (reconstructed) series me-er-ga “1”, TAKA “2”, pe“ “3”; pe“-
bala “beyond three” “4”, pe“-bala-ge
4
“beyond three + one” “5”,
pe“-bala-ge
4
-ge
4
“beyond three + one + one” “6”, pe“-pe“-ge
4
“three
three one” “7”: NBGT IV 33, 41–45 (MSL 4, 164 f.), and see
Powell 1971, 28–32 with lit. Origin (and application) of this system
are still unknown.
10.3. Onrix\r xtvnrns
In principle, cardinal numbers may also function as ordinal num­
bers, cf. Gudea Cyl. A xxi 1 é-a(k) sá-min-nam nam-mi(-n)-sì “(Gudea)
verily laid the second square (of the ziggurat)”, followed by sá-e“
5
­
àm (xxi 5) up to sá-umun
7
(xxi 11).
EDZARD_f11_60-67 4/28/03 2:41 PM Page 67
xtvrn\rs 67
Note: For purely rhythmical reasons, one-syllable numbers are followed by the
copula [am], two-syllable numbers are not (see W. Heimpel, Or. 39 [1970]
492–95). For the general interpretation (as against Edzard 1997, 82) see C. Suter,
ZA 87 (1997) 1–10.
A secondary means to form ordinal numbers was to put the num­
ber in the genitive followed by the copula [am]: u
4
-2-kam [u(d)-min-
ak-am] “it is of day two” = “the second day”. This construction,
already towards the end of the OS period, was extended by the
addition of a second genitive case particle so that, practically,
[(a)kama(k)] became the ending to mark an ordinal number. Cf. the
locative u
4
-umun
7
-kam-ma-ka “on the seventh day” Gudea Cyl. A
xxiii 4. Since the number sign is never explicitly followed by the [a]
of the genitive, it is possible that the ordinal number ending was in
fact [kama(k)] and not [(a)kama(k)].
For an OB sequence “first” to “seventh” cf. Gilg. and Huwawa
A 37–43 di“-àm, min-/e“
5
-/limmu
5
-/ía-/à“-/umun
7
-kam-ma, refer­
ring to “the first (demon)” etc.
10.4. Fn\c+iox \xr vr\stnr +rnvixoroov
For the extremely diversified Sumerian system of fractions and mea­
surements, see Powell 1971, 84–248, and 1989 throughout.
10.5. Dis+nint+iox\r nrr\+ioxs or xtvnrns
For expressing “each”, the (measure +) number are followed by the
ablative case particle, e.g., gín-7-ta “(rings of ) 7 shekels each”; to
-ta, the copula [am] may still be added: -ta-àm.
Poebel 1923, 113–14.
munus-u
4
-bi-ta-ke
4
-ne ninta-2-ta ì(-n)-tuku-am
6
“the women of yore
took two men each” (CIRPL Ukg. 6 iii' 20'–22').
10.6. Mtr+irric\+iox
For “times”, a-rá “course” is placed before the numeral: a-rá-3(-kam)
“three times”.
Poebel 1923, 114–16.
Note: Semantically, cf. gang, gång in Scandinavian languages.
68
EDZARD_f12_68-69 4/28/03 9:39 AM Page 68
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
69
EDZARD_f12_68-69 4/28/03 9:39 AM Page 69
CHAPTER ELEVEN
ADVERBS
We here restricted the term “adverb” to derivations from adjectives
or non-finite verbal forms in -a (¢am†u participles), Attinger’s “adverbes
de manière” (1993, 168–70), excluding expressions indicating modal­
ity or time, Attinger’s “adverbes de modalité et de temps” (1993,
170); see below 14.2.5. Most of the “adverbes de manière” corre­
spond to Akkadian adverbs in -i“, e.g., damqi“ “in a good way”.
For literature see Attinger 1993, 168–70; see also above 4.2.(5).
Formally, there are three types of adverbs: a) R-bi, b) R-bi-e“(e), c)
R-e“(e).
a) “úr-bi = ezzi“ “furiously”, mah-bi = ßìri“ “in a magnificent way”
(CAD Í 207), gal-bi “greatly”, and see Attinger 1993, 169 (b) for
many more examples (“type très productif ”).
b) gibil-bi-e“/-é“ “anew”. This is type (a) with the addition of the
adverbiative ending.
c) zi-dè-e“/-é“(-e/-“è), i.e., [zid-e“(e)] “in a true, correct, righteous
way”.
According to Attinger, this type, combined with adjectives, is “peu
productif ”.
[e“(e)] may, on the other hand, also be combined with substan­
tives, yielding a “type très productif ” (Attinger 1993, 168); cf. only
“ul-le-é“ “in the manner of a strong young man”, u
4
-dè-e“/-é“(e), i.e.,
[ud-e“(e)] “like daylight”.
70
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 70
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
71
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 71
CHAPTER TWELVE
THE VERB
12.1. P
Describing the Sumerian verb is the most difficult part for the gram­
marian. One still admires A. Poebel’s work of 1923 who practically
“conquered the unconquerable” with his Grundzüge der sumerischen
Grammatik. Since Poebel, Sumerian grammars, partly for the language
as a whole, partly for chosen sectors, but always with the focus on
the verb, have appeared in a considerable number and in chrono­
logical density: Deimel 1924,
2
1939; Jestin 1943, 1946, 1951; Falkenstein
1949, 1950, 1959; Sollberger 1952; Römer 1982; Thomsen 1984;
Jacobsen 1956, 1965, 1988; Black 1984,
2
1991; Attinger 1993; Edzard
1995; Kaneva 1996.
The Sumerian verb consists of a base, e.g., gu
7
“to eat”, and a
series of prefixed and/or suffixed particles. The base is quoted in lexi­
cal texts and is regularly translated by an Akkadian infinitive (gu
7
=
akàlu). One may distinguish between finite verbs, expressing person,
and non-finite verbs: infinitives, participles, or the base alone.
The base is impenetrable, i.e., no infixes may be inserted such as
-t(a)- or -ta(n)- in Akkadian. The base is either invariable in form or
subject to variation, e.g., extended or reduced form, reduplication
gar/∞ (full or partial), ‘Ablaut’, or heteronymy, e.g., è/è-d, gi
4
/gi
4
-gi
4
, ∞ gá-
gá, ti/te-∞ ∞ g, dug
4
/e.
Frequently the same spelling is used for two variants, e.g., na∞ g(NAG)
“to drink”, has to be read na
8
-na
8
(NAG.NAG) when reduplicated.
But in quite a few cases one cannot be certain whether B (= base)
has but one reading or two.
Note: A specially ambiguous sign is DU which, as a verbal base, may be read
(depending on context) du, ∞gen “to go”, gub “to stand”, “put”, túm, de
6
“to
bring”, rá, not to mention the different readings of reduplicated DU.DU or
DU
.
DU
Variants of the base of a given verb may concern the categories
¢am†u and marû and/or singular and plural.
For the sake of clarity we introduce, for verbal bases, grids with
horizontal and vertical divisions, into which the varying forms of the
base can be entered (as far as they are already known):
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 72
72 cn\r+rn +vrryr
sg. pl.
¢.
m.
For details on the categories ¢am†u and marû see 12.2.
The surrounding (i.e., prefixed and suffixed) verbal particles pre­
sent an enormous variety, and they may be classified, in diminish­
ing distance, or rank, from the base:
a) the particle [ed], suffixed, compatible only with the marû base.
b) ergative and absolutive markers, both prefixed and suffixed.
c) prefixed markers of dimensional reference, ranked in the sequence
(from right to left) locative 2, directive, terminative, ablative(-instru-
mental), comitative, dative, locative. These partly have person or
non-person reference. The first-to-the-left of these, moreover, may
imply motion or absence of motion.
d) prefixed markers of connection, negation, and of diverse posi­
tive or negative modalities.
e) and f ) nominalizer [a] and copula [am], both suffixed.
Whereas the variation of the string of suffixed particles is limited, the
“prefix string” offers an astounding number of variants, since up to
5 particles may appear in a row, e.g., enim hu-mu-na-ni-ib-ge
4
-ge
4
[hu-mu-na-ni-b-gege] “the word—verily—ventive—dative (to him/
her)—directive/causative—absolutive (it)—make return (marû base)—
(ergative: he/she)” = “he/she verily answers him/her thereupon”.
Heimpel, in his 1974 count of OB “prefix chain” variants, added
up to 1264; this figure may now easily be raised to about 1300–1400.
For the pre-Ur III period Heimpel offered 122, for the Gudea cor­
pus 154 “prefix chains”.
Note: For the sake of comparison, compare the “prefix chains” in modern Basque
(Guipuzcoan), a language structurally comparable to Sumerian: 537 Variants are
listed in J. L. Mendizabal, La lengua vasca,
2
1959 [Buenos Aires] 353–69.
Let it be borne in mind, however, that in modern French, if we
arrange all possible combinations of bound elements je with me, te,
le, la, nous, vous, les, lui, leur, en, y, each in the positive and neg­
ative, e.g., je te le dis etc., we arrive at about 80 forms. In fact, je-
ne-lui-en-ai-rien-dit “I did not tell him/her anything about it” is as
complex a verbal form as many in Sumerian.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 73
+nr yrnn 73
12.2. ›AMˇU \xr MARÛ
These Akkadian adjectives meaning, “quick” and “fat, slow” respec­
tively, were used as grammatical terms by the compilers of bilingual
lexical lists. They have been the cause of lengthy learned debates,
since Yoshikawa 1968.
¢am†u (¢an†u) or marû is found as an addition to the Akkadian trans­
lation of a Sumerian (verbal) entry, e.g.,
igi-zu = uddû ¢an†u, igi-zu-zu = II marû
“to mark, indicate (when ¢.)”, to mark, indicate (when m.)”
Edzard 1971, 209–12, gave a survey of the successive efforts of
scholars to explain ¢. and m. from 1885–1968; the discussion between
1971 and 1984 has been summarized by Thomsen 1984, 115–23;
thereafter, Black
2
1991, 99–119; Lambert 1991, 7–9, brought argu­
ments that ¢. and m., found in the right hand Akkadian column of
lexical lists referred to the left hand, Sumerian, column (and not to
the right hand col., as Steiner 1981, 1–14, as well as Jacobsen 1988,
173, had seen it).
See now also Attinger 1993, 185–87, and Krecher 1995, 142 with
fn. 1, with more lit.
If ¢am†u and marû refer to the Sumerian verbal base, they cannot
in all cases be describing its form. For while, e.g., igi-zu is, in fact,
“quick (short)” as opposed to igi-zu-zu, “slow (long)”, ∞gen “to go” (¢.)
versus du (m.) presents no such opposition. The same formal dilemma
we would encounter in Akkadian where, e.g., iprus : iparras would cor­
respond to “short” : “long” whereas ìkul : ikkal, i“ìm : i“àm would not.
Be that as it may, Krecher’s decisive argument (1995, 142) was
that the references “¢. and m.” in the right, Akkadian, column should
be seen in the same way as the reference EME.SAL, which can only
refer the Sumerian expression given in the left, Sumerian, column.
It has in fact proved practical to apply the terms ¢am†u and marû
to those complementary variants of the Sumerian verbal base, occur­
ring in two different, transitive, conjugation patterns: “preterite” in-
gar “he set (something)” and “present” ib-∞ gá “he sets/will set it”; ∞ gá-∞
gar-ra [∞ and to two non-finite uses of the verbal base: ∞ gar-a] “set”
(past participle) and ∞ gá-(d) [∞ ga-ed] “setting” (present participle). gá-∞ ga-∞
We will, therefore, retain those two terms both for different ver­
bal bases and for the conjugation patterns in which they occur.
Note: The two conjugation patterns have, alternatively, been named “Präteritum”:
“Präsens-Futur” (Poebel 1923; Falkenstein 1949–50; Römer 1982, 55 f.); “achevé”:
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 74
74 cn\r+rn +vrryr
“inachevé” ( Jestin 1943, 209); “perfective” : “imperfective” (Steinkeller 1979, 54;
Jacobsen 1988, 174 with fn. 15); “standard construction” : “secondary construc­
tion” (Black
2
1991, passim and spec. 130 f.); “¢am†u” : “marû” (Attinger 1993).
The ¢am†u variant provides the base for a participle in -a (most prob­
ably not identical with the nominalizer [a], see 12.16):
d
Mes-lam-ta-
è-a “(the god) who has come out of the Meslam (sanctuary)”. It is,
furthermore, used in the transitive conjugation pattern 2b (12.7.3)
which most often corresponds to the Akkadian preterite iprus. It is
also used in the intransitive conjugation pattern 1 (12.7.1).
The marû variant is used to form a participle in -e(d), -(ed): eme
èd-dè-da-ni [ed-ed-ani] “while sticking out its tongue” Gudea Cyl.
B xiv 7. It is, furthermore, used in the transitive conjugation pattern
2a (12.7.2) which most often corresponds to the Akkadian iparras.
Note: A verb is transitive if it displays the two conjugation patterns 2a and 2b.
A large group of verbs have only one conjugation pattern which
may correspond to both Akkadian iprus and iparras. We tradition­
ally call these verbs intransitive. For a restricted number of intran­
sitive verbs, nevertheless, both a ¢am†u and a marû base are found,
and their distribution corresponds, once more, to Akkadian iprus and
iparras: ba-∞gen “I went away”, ì-du-un [i-du-(e)n] “I go”.
12.3. Prtn\r Vrnns
Apart from the ¢am†u : marû dichotomy, some verbs distinguish indi­
vidual bases for whether the ‘subject’ (with intr. verbs) or ‘object’
(with trans. verbs) is in the sg. or pl. If one person is registered to
“stay, live” somewhere, tìl is used; if there are two or more, the verb
will be se
12
.
12.4. Vrnn\r n\sr ¢\v†t⁄v\nt⁄so.⁄rr. onirs
The Sumerian verbal base in ¢am†u/marû/sg./pl. grids.
As noted above (12.1) it proves practical to enter the different
attested base forms in a grid. We can offer eight variants. There
may, however, be more because—most probably—the correct read­
ings of some ambiguous cuneograms still escape us.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 75
  75
12.4.1.
sg. pl.
¢. a a
m. a a
“úm “úm gu
7
gu
7
sar sar
“úm “úm gu
7
gu
7
sar sar
“give” “eat” “write”
This is the inalterable class. It has not yet been estimated how great a
percentage the class represents in the total of (attested) Sumerian verbs.
12.4.2.
sg. pl.
¢. a a
m. a' a'
è è ti/te ti/te
è(-d) è(-d) ti(-∞ g) ti(-∞ g) g)/te(-∞ g)/te(-∞
“come out” “approach”
Here, “vertically”, a simple and an extended base are opposed. ti/te
etc. mainly occurs in the compound verb “u ti “to bring the hand
close (to something)” = “to receive” (see 12.15.1.2).
Note: In occasional Ur III administrative texts a reduplicated TI.TI stands for
marû ti(-∞ g), e.g., “u ha-ba-an-“i-íb-TI.TI “he will certainly ([ha]) receive it ([b])
from him ([n-“i])”; see Krecher 1995, 161, 163–73, for similar cases of pseudo-
reduplication of a verbal base to indicate the marû.
12.4.3.
sg. pl. sg. pl.
¢. a a
a-a a-a
and ¢.
m. m.
a a
a'-a' a'-a'
ge
4
ge
4
zi(-g) zi(-g)
ge
4
-ge
4
ge
4
-ge
4
zi-zi zi-zi
“return” “rise, raise”
na∞g na∞g kur
9
kur
9
gar ∞ ∞ gar
gá-∞ gá na
8
-na
8
na
8
-na
8
ku
4
-ku
4
ku
4
-ku
4
∞ gá ˆ gá-∞
“drink” “enter” “set”
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 76
76 cn\r+rn +vrryr
In this class, “vertically”, the opposition is between a simple ¢am†u
and a reduplicated marû base. If the simple form ends in a conso­
nant, this final consonant is omitted in reduplication; note, however,
class 4. If the simple form ends in a vowel, it is reduplicated with­
out alteration.
gar/∞ gá. Unfortunately, different cuneograms are only used with ∞ gá-∞
In the other cases the morphological evidence has to be inferred from
the behaviour of the suffixed particles or from non-orthodox spellings.
12.4.4.
sg. pl.
sùh sùh tuku
4
tuku
4
¢. a a
m. a'+a" a'+a" [sish] [sish] [tutk] [tutk]
“confuse, trouble” “tremble”
For si-is-he [si(h)s(i)h-e] or tu-ut-ke [tu(k)t(u)k-e] see Falkenstein 1959
b, 99 f. This type of reduplication where C
1
VC
2
(V)-C
1
VC
2
(V) turns
into C
1
VC
1
C
2
, recalls the nominal reduplication *deli-deli > dedli
(see 5.3.4).
Type 4 is, strictly speaking, a variant of type 3. But we are unable
to state why *zig-zig became zi-zi instead of *zizg.
Krecher 1995, 173–77.
12.4.5.
sg. pl. non-fin.
¢. a b
b b c
dug
4
e
e e di m.
“speak, do”
This verb most probably forms a unique class and we have termed
it “irregular”. There is a heteronymic differentiation between ¢am†u
and marû in sg., but marû [e] also intruded in to ¢am†u pl. Moreover,
there is a third heteronymic base for non-finite marû.
Although dug
4
/e is a transitive verb, the pl. base is used for plural
‘subjects’, not ‘objects’ (see below 12.4.7), this being another irregularity.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 77
  77
M. Civil and R. D. Biggs proposed that DUG
4
should occasion­
ally be read as e
x
(apud Steinkeller 1979, 61). But there is no proof
for such an assumption.
Exhaustive information on dug
4
/e/di is found in Attinger 1993
where also the astounding number of over 210 compound verbs is
treated in detail.
12.4.6.
In types 1–4 there was a “horizontal” ¢am†u : marû division of the
grid. In types 6–9 the “vertical” sg.: pl. division becomes relevant.
Unfortunately, some of the grids can only be partially filled out as yet.
sg. pl.
¢. a b
m. a b
gub “u
4
-(g)/su
8
(-g) ú“ ug
7
gub “u
4
-(g)/su
8
(-g) – –
“stand” (intr. only) “die”
tìl se
12
(sig
7
)
– –
“live, dwell”
For gub, see Krecher 1967, 8–11; for tìl : se
12
(SIG
7
) see Steinkeller
1979, 55 with fn. 5 (with previous lit.). When someone died it was
noted as ba-ú“; in the case of two or more dead, OS ba-ug
7
-ge(-“)
is found.
Note: See Steinkeller 1979, 55 fn. 4, for the graphic difference between (OS)
Ú” and BAD.
It is important to observe that the above verbs belong to class 6
only as long as they are intransitive. Transitive gub “to put, place”
is turned into the non-changing class 1: cf. mu-gub-ba-∞gu
10
ma-an-
gub-bu-u“ [ma-n-gub-e“] “they set before me my ‘standing lines’”
Schooldays 6. The same holds for ug
5
“to kill”.
Note: A widespread (universal?) rule is that verbs which display morphological
pecularities in their primary form become “regular” in derived forms. Cf. the
uniform vocalization of (derived) D stem verbs in Akkadian and other Semitic
languages; German sitzen ¡ setzen, English sit ¡ set, etc.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 78
78 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.4.7.
sg. pl.
¢. a c
m. b c
DU
tu“ durun de
6
lah
5
(DU.DU) lah
4
(
DU
)
DU
dúr durun tùmu lah
5
(DU.DU) lah
4
(
DU
)
“sit” “carry away”
Here, the grid is divided both “horizontally” and “vertically”. The
sg. has different bases for ¢am†u and marû, and there is a base for
the plural of both ¢am†u and marû (formally related to m. sg.?).
Steinkeller 1979, 55 f. fn. 6, noted that OS TU”.TU” stands for
durun
x
(not *durun-durun, as was supposed by Yoshikawa 1981, 115).
Note also Krecher 1995: TU”.TU” = durun
x
. Cf. above 12.4.2 for
Ur III TI.TI = marû [ti∞g-e].
ki-dam-A-NE-a-ti-ka ì-dú-ru-né-“a-àm [i-durun-e“-am] “they reside
with A.’s wife” NG no. 214:41.
For lah
5
and lah
4
see Steinkeller 1979, 54–67, esp. 57–60.
Whereas tu“ etc. is intransitive, de
6
etc. is transitive. With de
6
, there­
fore, the absolutive to which the plurality refers is the ‘object’.
However, apart from being transitive, [lah] may occur as a passive:
ba-lah
5
“were carried away” (Steinkeller 1979, 59 f.).
Note: Steinkeller 1979, 65 compared Caucasian (Georgian, Svan, etc.) and North
American Indian languages (Athapascan, etc.) for pl. verbs implying a pl. object.
12.4.8.
sg. pl.
¢. a c
m. b d
∞gen [ere]
du su
8
(-b)
“go”
The verb “to go” presents pecularities and irregularities in so many
languages that there is no need to quote examples. It is no surprise
that “to go” holds the record in Sumerian for base variants and
that, what is more, all four are heteronymic.
The decisive study of these variants is Krecher 1967, 1–11 “Die
pluralischen Verben für ‘gehen’ und ‘stehen’ im Sumerischen”.
Pl. ¢am†u DU.DU and
DU
DU
represent the same evolution of spelling
as with the pl. of gub “to stand” (see 12.4.7).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 79
+nr yrnn 79
[ere] is noted, in non-orthodox spelling, as e-re and e-re
7
(DU) (or
e
ere).
ì-im-e-re-“a “who had come here” UET 3, 1633:11; see, for more
forms, Wilcke 1988, 32.
As in classes 6 and 7, plurality refers to the absolutive case, the
‘subject’ of the intransitive verb.
12.5. Son+s or rrtn\ri+v (›AMˇU nrrtrric\+iox)
Apart from the ¢am†u : marû and sg. : pl. distribution patterns as shown
in 12.4.1–8, the unaltered ¢am†u base can also be reduplicated for
a variety of reasons: to denote different sorts of plurality or totality
of the ‘subject’ or ‘object’; to stress its size, strength, or general
importance; or to stress the durative or iterative character of the
action (Edzard 1971, 226–32; Steinkeller 1979, 63 f.).
The difference between this type of reduplicated base and the marû
reduplication is transparent in the spellings ∞ gá (marû) and ∞ gar. gá-∞ gar-∞
This kind of “free reduplication” of the ¢am†u base may occur
both in the ¢am†u and marû conjugation patterns:
igi hé-mu-e-“i-bar-bar-re “may Utu—again and again—look on
you” Enm. 95
igi nam-bar-bar-re-en “you are not supposed to stare at every­
thing” Scribe and Son 31.
gar-∞ siki-udu-¢ganÜ-na-kam “u-a mi-ni(-n)-∞ gar “(instead of the whip)
he put wool of ewes in the hand (of all the overseers)” Gudea Cyl.
A xiii 2.
dam dumu-Ku-li, dumu-Ba-ba-∞gu
10
-ke
4
-ne(-r), ba-an-da-zàh-zàh-é“,
dumu-Ba-ba-∞gu
10
-ke
4
-ne mu-un-dab
5
-dab
5
-bé-é“ “the wife and chil­
dren of Kuli had all fled from the children of Baba∞gu, (but) the chil­
dren of Baba∞gu seized them all” NG no. 41:11'–13'.
gar-∞ iku-zi-dam é“e ì(-n)-∞ gar “(Gudea) laid—along all sides—the
measuring rope to (a square corresponding) exactly to an iku” Gudea
Cyl. A xviii 26.
Note: Not “gána”, as transliterated in ZA 61, 229.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 80
80 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.6. No+r ox yrnn\r nrrtrric\+iox
The form a verbal case takes when it is reduplicated cannot be estab­
lished beforehand. It differs from one verb to the other. We may
generally distinguish between two ways of information: (1) standard
orthography: repetition of the sign used to render the simple form or,
very rarely, of a variant form (e.g., ∞ gá instead of ∞ gar); (2) un- gá-∞ gar-∞
orthodox spelling: the scribe tried, to the best of his ability, to render
in syllabic script the actual pronunciation of the reduplicated form.
12.6.1
a) Full reduplication: gi
4
-gi
4
, bad-bad (”ulgi A 45 with var. ba-ad-
ba-ad in Susa MDP 57 no. 200).
12.6.2
b) Loss of final consonant: ∞ gá-∞ gar ¡ ∞ gá; kur
9
¡ ku
4
-ku
4
; na∞g ¡
na
8
-na
8
, and see above 12.4.2.
c) Loss of final consonant in first base: bi-bi-z(é) < *biz-biz (Gudea
Cyl. A xxviii 11, 24; “u ba-ba-r(a-da) < BAR-BAR Gudea Cyl. B
xv 7; la-la-ah < LAH
4
-LAH
4
, see ZA 53, 99 with fn. 16.
d) Assimilation of final consonant in first base to following initial
consonant: bar
6
-bar
6
¡ UD (babbar), ba(-ab)-ba-ar Proto-Ea 157
(MSL 14, 38).
e) Loss of consonant in second base: gen ¡ (*gen-gen) ge-en-ge-
te [genge(n)-(e)d-e] “make firm” Innin “ag. 118 (var. to ge-en-ge-en),
see Å. Sjöberg, ZA 65 (1975) 188.
f ) VC-VC > VCCVC (loss of hiatus): ul
4
¡ ul-lu-ul “to hasten”
ibid. (as e) 116 (var. ul
4
-ul
4
).
g) C
1
VC
2
(V) ¡ C
1
VC
1
C
2
(V) (loss of consonant in first, of vowel
in second base): bir-bir CT 15, 22:8 // bi-ib-ri VS 2, 2 iv 20 (see
Falkenstein 1959 b, 99 with fn. 17).
tuku
4
¡ tu-ut-ke see above 12.4.3.
h) C
1
VC
2
VC
3
¡ C
1
V-C
1
VC
2
VC
3
(loss of second syllable in first
base): ù-bi-GALAM-GALAM
ga-ga-la-am
: gana utlellì “rise high (O Inanna)”
TCL 6, 51:37 (Hru“ka 1969, 483 f.).
zu-lu-un ¡ zu-zu-lu-un, sù-sù-lu-un = rußßunu “roaring” see CAD
R 183 raßànu lex.
i) C
1
VC
2
VC
3
¡ C
1
VC
2
-C
1
VC
2
VC
3
(loss of the end of the second
syllable in first base): zalag ¡ za-al-zu-le-g(a) = nuwwurum “illumi­
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 81
+nr yrnn 81
nate” (var. to ZALAG-ga) Innin “ag. 160, see Å. Sjöberg, ZA 65
(1975) 194.
Note: za-al-za-le-bi ibid. 124 does not belong here according to Sjöberg, ibid. p. 235.
The change of vowel in [zalzuleg(a)] is noteworthy and it is proba­
ble a phenomenon occurring much more frequently than may be
guessed from standard spelling.
Besides reduplication of the verbal base, triplication and even
quadruplication may be observed, but only much less frequently. Cf.
Attinger 1993, 190 (with previous lit.).
12.7. Cox¡to\+iox
As the Sumerian verb is essentially person-oriented it is appropriate
to speak of its “conjugation” (or of “verbal inflection”).
There are three patterns, (1) one for intransitive (and passive)
verbal forms, exhibiting absolutive person elements only; (2) two pat­
terns for transitive verbs showing both ergative (“subject”) and abso­
lutive (“object”) person elements.
Forms of pattern (1) are rendered in Akkadian by both iprus and
iparras. For (2) there is one (2a) normally translated by iprus and one
(2b) by iparras. This distribution has led former grammarians to speak
about (1) the “intransitive standard form” (“Normalform”) and (2a)
“preterite” and (2b) “present-future” (see above, p. 73 f. n.).
The three patterns may be reconstructed with relative certainty
during and after Ur III. In OS, and to some degree still in Ur III,
spelling only partly represents the full form because of a marked ten­
dency to neglect syllable-closing consonants in writing: Note the ubi­
quitous “u ba-ti for [“u banti] or the above-quoted OS ba-ug
6
-ge(-“)
[ba-ug-e“].
12.7.1. Conjugation pattern 1: Intransitive (and passive)
sg. 1
st
ba-zah-en (Ur III -èn) [ba-zah-en] “I run away”
sg. 2
nd
ba-zah-en [ba-zah-en]
sg. 3
rd
ba-zah [ba-zah]
pl. 1
st
ba-zah-en-dè-en [ba-zah-enden]
pl. 2
nd
ba-zah-en-zé-en [ba-zah-enzen]
pl. 3
rd
ba-zah-e“ (Ur III -é“) [ba-zah-e“]
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 82
82 cn\r+rn +vrryr
The pattern may be extended by adding -e(d) to the base: [ba-zah-
ed-en] “I will flee” in the oath ba-ra-ba-zah-e-dè-en “I swear I will
not run away (another time)” (said by a captured runaway slave)
(negative affirmative particle [bara], see 12.11.4). [ed] here serves to
express more precisely the temporal situation (future).
Note: The homophony—at least according to spelling—of 1
st
and 2
nd
person sg.
is striking. Was there originally only an opposition of “I” or “you” : “he, she, it”?
Phonetic pecularities: The [e] of [en, enden, enzen, e“] may yield
to the final vowel of the preceding base: ì-du-un [i-du(e)n] “I/you go”.
Or this [e] may assimilate to a non-final vowel of the base: *ba-
“úm-mu-u“ [ba-“um-e“] “they were given” (example made up from
corresponding active forms of pattern 2b).
Assimilation of the vowel is not yet predictable for us: cf., e.g.,
ga-ba-húl-le-en-dè-en [ga-ba-hul-enden] “let us rejoice over it”, not
*[-hul-unden].
Conjugation pattern 1 is neutral as for tense or aspect (except
when the base is extended by [e(d)]). This is easily explained by the
fact that it has only one participant (absolute) while patterns 2a and
2b, with their distinction of perfective (¢am†u based pattern) and imper­
fective (marû based pattern) are essentially characterized by the pres­
ence of two participants: absolutive and ergative.
12.7.1.1. The verb me
The verb [me] “to be” and the copula [me] have conjugations which
differ only in the 3
rd
sg., where there is both a free and a bound
form (copula).
sg. 1
st
ì-me-en -me-en [men] “I am”
sg. 2
nd
ì-me-en -me-en [men]
sg. 3
rd
ì-me(-àm) (OS -am
6
) -àm (OS -am
6
) [am]
pl. 1
st
ì-me-en-dè-en -me-en-dè-en [menden]
pl. 2
nd
ì-me-en-zé-en -me-en-zé-en [menzen]
pl. 3
rd
ì-me-e“ (Ur III -é“) -me-e“ (Ur III -é“) [me“]
In the majority of cases, the bound form occurs, e.g., lugal-me-en
“I am king”, “I, the king”.
For the free form cf. ba-ra-me “he certainly will not be” NG no.
18:34; ì-me-àm “he was” NG no. 63:12, 70:9; ì-me-àm [i-me-a-am]
“who is” NG no. 6:2, 26:2, 75:2; ì-me-“a-ke
4
-é“ [i-me-(e)“-a-(a)k-e“]
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 83
  83
“pertaining to the fact that they are” = “because they are (. . .)”
Gudea Cyl. A xxvi 15.
Thomsen 1984, 273–78; Attinger 1993, 312 f.
The free form is mainly used when [me] is understood as a “preterite”
or “future” and when the form is nominalized by the particle [a].
The [a] of [am] may be superseded by a preceding vowel: -bi-im
[bi-(a)m]” “it is its (. . .)”; -zu-um [zu-(a)m] “it is your (. . .)”. It is
unlikely that there was an original distribution of two forms: [am]
after C and [m] after V because we find, e.g., hé-àm.
[am] was written by AN in OS, transliterated as am
6
. From the
frequent combination of the nominalizing particle [a] + copula [am],
-a-am
6
[a’am] the more recent (Ur III) spelling A.AN = àm arose,
probably after the hiatus originally present in [a’am] had disap­
peared, resulting in [âm].
After the genitive particle [ak] and after [d] or [n], the CVC
cuneograms kam, dam, and nam are normally used: za-a-kam [za(e)-
(a)k-am] “it is yours”, min-nam [min-am] “it is two”.
For enclitic [am] derivation from *i-m(e) (or *a-m(e)), i.e., a reduced
free form with prefixed particle [i] or [a] has been proposed: Poebel
1923, 72 ff., followed by Falkenstein 1949, 147. A heteronymous
form is, however, more probable, because heteronymy in the cop­
ula conjugation is also found in other languages, e.g., Indo-European.
12.7.2. Conjugation pattern 2a: Transitive
Given its great similarity to pattern 1, we describe pattern 2a, marû,
imperfective, before pattern 2b, ¢am†u, perfective.
sg. 1
st
ì-lá-en [i-laH-en] “I pay”
sg. 2
nd
ì-lá-en [i-laH-en]
sg. 3
rd
ì-lá-e [i-laH-e]
pl. 1
st
ì-lá-en-dè-en [i-laH-enden]
pl. 2
nd
ì-lá-en-zé-en [i-laH-enzen]
pl. 3
rd
(person class only) ì-lá-e-ne [i-laH-ene]
The pattern offered here has been simplified because only one par­
ticipant has been noted, the suffixed ergative markers; the second
participant, the ‘object’ denoted by absolutive markers, has been dis­
regarded here (see below p. 84 f.).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 84
84 cn\r+rn +vrryr
Pattern 2a is formally identical with pattern 1 in the 1
st
and 2
nd
persons; for the 3
rd
p. pattern 2a replaces absolutive (-Ø, -e“) by
ergative (-e, -ene).
In verbs with differing ¢am†u : marû bases pattern 2a is linked to
the marû base(s) (cf. above 12.4).
It is probable that 3
rd
sg. [e] and 3
rd
pl. [ene] have the same ori­
gin as the nominal ergative particles sg. [e] and pl. [ene].
Phonetic details: As in pattern 1, the [e] of [en, e, enden, enzen,
gá-∞ ene] may yield to the final vowel of the preceding base: ì-∞ gá-ne
ga∞ [i-∞ ga-(e)ne] “they set, place (something)”; or it may be assimilated
to the non-final vowel of the base: -“úm-mu-un, -“úm-mu [“um-en],
[“um-e] “I/you give, he gives”, also pl. -“úm-mu-un-dè-en, -“úm-mu-
ne [“um-enden], [“um-ene]; ba-tar-ra-a“ [ba-tar-e“] “they were split”
Lugale 72. But, as with pattern 1, assimilation cannot be predicted.
Discussion has arisen about whether the [e] of [en] etc. is part of
an autonomous marker of the conjugation pattern, or should be con­
sidered a separate “marû” marker so that -en of ì-lá-en [i-laH-en]
would have to be segmentized as -e-n. Such a “marû” [e] would,
then, only occur in verbs with a non-alternating base (cf. 12.4.1).
Put another way round, we would have to ask whether in 3
rd
sg.
-ge
4
-ge
4
[gege], [gege] would represent *gege + e, with [e] super­
seded by the final vowel of the base; or whether [gege] would have
been self-sufficient to indicate 3
rd
p. sg.
Note: Krecher 1995, 183 § 43, joined Yoshikawa 1974, 18, and Jacobsen 1988,
180, in asserting that [e] is not part of the person elements.
Since the great majority of—reduplicated or unreduplicated—marû
bases end in a vowel, there is the danger of a circular argument. How­
ever, in case of è (¢am†u)/e(-d) (marû) “come/bring out”, -è-dè [ed­
e] can only represent the marû base + additional, autonomous, [e].
The same holds for the verb te/ti (¢.)/te(-∞ g) (m.) “to come/bring
close (to something)”, with -te-∞ge
26
(GÁ) as marû base + [e]. Here,
some authors have posited, ad hoc, the marû base to be [te∞ga].
Verbal forms of conjugation pattern 2a normally express—or at
least imply, if graphically unrealized—absolutive elements for the
person or non-person class ‘objects’. The system runs:
sg. 1
st
-(e)n-B(ase)
sg. 2
nd
-(e)n-B
sg. 3
rd
person - n-B
sg. 3
rd
non-person - b-B
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 85
  85
pl. 1
st
?
pl. 2
nd
?
pl. 3
rd
person -ne- B
d
En-líl-le im-ma-“i-in-gi
4
-gi
4
[imma-“i-(e)n-gigi] “Enlil sends me back
(against the rebel lands)” Römer 1969, 298:109 (see Attinger 1985,
166); more references for 1
st
sg. “me” in Attinger 1993, 163–67.
Ha-ba(-n)-zi-zi [ha-ba-(e)n-zizi] “May (the personal god) raise you
here” (= PN) Limet 1968, 311 f.; more references for 2
nd
sg. “you”
see Attinger 1985, 167–75.
Note: Until—and partly including—Ur III the consonants representing the abso­
lutive elements are disregarded in spelling (because of their position at the end
of a closed syllable), and their restitution depends entirely on our (subjective)
understanding of the context.
ha-ra-ab-“úm-mu [ha-(e)ra-b-“um-e] “let him give it to you”.
ha-mu-ra-ne-“úm-mu [ha-mu-(e)ra-ne-“um-e] “let him give them
(pers.) to you”. ITT 1,1100:16.
[For Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pat­
tern 2a see p. 86.]
In literary texts of OB date, -(e)n- before the base often alternates
with -e- (Attinger 1985, 163 ff. passim).
The fact that the absolutive markers for the 1
st
, 2
nd
, and 3
rd
sg.
person class, i.e., -(e)n-, -(e)n-, -n-, may have been homophonous
(they are so at least in our Latin transliteration), can hardly have
contributed much to clarity. The coexistence of homophonous mor­
phemes has, however, never been an obstacle to practical under­
standing, as is shown by English multifunctional [s] in (1) wings,
(2) sings, (3) king’s, (4) kings’, (5) it’s, (6) he’s (has).
The 1
st
and 2
nd
pl. forms of the absolutive marker are still unknown.
If they existed they may have been replaced over time by periphras­
tic expressions by means of the free forms of the personal pronoun
(see 9.1).
It is still open to research whether Sumerian, in the transitive
verb, distinguished between determinate action implying an object
(e.g. “he fights an enemy”) and indeterminate action not implying
an object (e.g., “he fights”).
Final note to 12.7.2.: We have described conjugation pattern 2a
in terms of ergativity although, since Michalowski 1980, the opinion
has been voiced that Sumerian was a language with “split ergativ­
ity” (accepted by Attinger 1993, 150–52) where only conjugation
e
d
z
a
r
d
_
f
1
3
_
7
0
-
1
5
5
/
P
D
F


4
/
2
8
/
0
3


2
:
4
5

P
M


P
a
g
e

8
6
8
6

Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pattern 2a
absolutive
ergative
1
st
sg. 2
nd
sg. 3
rd
sg. p. 3
rd
sg. non-p. 1
st
pl. 2
nd
pl. 3
rd
pl. p.
1
st
sg. -(e)n-B-en -n-B-en -b-B-en ? -(e)n-B-en (?)
2
nd
sg. -(e)n-B-en -n-B-en -b-B-en ? -(e)n-B-en (?)
3
rd
sg. p. -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-e -n-B-e -b-B-e ? ? -(e)n-B-e (?)
3
rd
sg. non-p. -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-e -n-B-e -b-B-e ? ? -(e)n-B-e (?)
1
st
pl. -(e)n-B-enden -n-B-enden -b-B-enden ? ?
2
nd
pl. -(e)n-B-enzen -n-B-enzen -b-B-enzen ? ?
3
rd
pl. p. -(e)n-B-ene -(e)n-B-ene -n-B-ene -b-B-ene ? ? -(e)n-B-ene (?)
Crossed-out boxes would imply reflexive forms (I-myself, etc.) which the Sumerian verbal system does not express.
Boxes with a question mark refer to forms where the absolutive element cannot be safely stated or reconstructed.
c
n
\
r
+
r
n

+
v
r
r
y
r

edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 87
  87
pattern 2b (¢am†u) would be termed ergative, while pattern 2a as
well as the imperative and cohortative would have to be considered
as functioning on a nominative-accusative level. The question will
be discussed in more detail in 12.7.4.
12.7.3. Conjugation pattern 2b: Transitive
sg. 1
st
-(V-)lá
sg. 2
nd
-e-lá
sg. 3
rd
p. -n-lá
sg. 3
rd
non-p. -b-lá
pl. 1
st
-lá-enden (see note)
pl. 2
nd
-e-lá-enzen (see note)
pl. 3
rd
p. -n-lá-e“
In pattern 2b the ergative markers for sg. and 3
rd
pl. immediately
precede the verbal base (in its ¢am†u variant if such exists). 3
rd
p.
-n- and -b- appear identical with the absolutive markers of pattern 2a.
Note to 1
st
sg.: The ergative marker can hardly have been zero,
but its original vocalic quality can no longer be ascertained. Some
rare OB verbal forms have [e] before the base, thus suggesting iden­
tity with the 2
nd
sg. [e], but this may be a late analogy formed on
the identity of the absolutive/ergative markers [en] of 1
st
and 2
nd
sg.
Note Falkenstein 1949, 159 fn. 2: “Vielleicht ist das Personenzeichen
der 1. ps. sg. mit dem der 2. ps. sg. -e- identisch”.
Jacobsen 1988, 198, preferred “Poebel’s first suggestion [1923, 175]
of a mark zero”.
Extensive discussion by Attinger 1993, 217 § 139a, without a definite
result.
Note to 2
nd
sg.: Krecher 1985, 144, proposed identifying the erga­
tive marker as [e(r)] because he analysed the dative and directive
elements -e-ra- and -e-re- “to you” as [er-a/e] instead of [e-r-a/e].
He is followed by Attinger 1993, 217–20 and passim.
Before Krecher, -r- had been explained as a Hiatustilger (the [r]
being borrowed from the dative case particle [ra]). Now since *[er]
only occurs between vowels and would otherwise have to be recon­
structed as *[e(r)], Krecher’s argument clearly is circular. Moreover,
g [mu-e+da-e-a∞ in such cases as á mu-e-da-a-á∞ g] “you (moved the
arm with me =) ordered me” one would have expected [*r] to be
indicated before the vowel of á∞ g.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 88
88 cn\r+rn +vrryr
Note to 1
st
pl.: A form -*me-lá, postulated by Poebel 1923, 173
n. 176; Falkenstein 1949, 160 fn. 2; 1959, 40; Thomsen 1984, 148
(but with caution in fn. 43), has not yet been demonstrated. The
form -lá-enden is rarely attested in OB; cf. Attinger 1993, 22. It
looks like a ‘late’ suppletive form.
It is probably just by chance that very few texts record the speech
of more than one person. Letters and depositions of witnesses always
happen to be in the sg. There was no pluralis maiestatis (“we” for “I”).
We hesitate to posit the ergative 1
st
pl. marker as (sg.) [(V)] + . . .
-enden because cross-linguistically “we” is rarely expressed as “I” +
pl. (cf. above 5.2). However, formation by analogy with 2
nd
pl., (sg.)
[e] + . . . -enzen, cannot be excluded.
Note: For exceptions cf. Maghrebi (and Maltese) Arabic 1
st
pl. niqtlu (1
st
sg. =
niqtil ) or Chinese wo-men “we” = “I” + pl.
Note to 2
nd
pl.: A form -e-ene-B was offered by Thomsen 1984, 148,
mechanically appending pl. [(e)ne] to sg. [e]. However, the Enlil­
bàni (of Isin) ‘key text’ HSM 1384 has in line 11 nu-mu-¢eÜ-“[úm-
m]u-un-zé-en [nu-mu-e-“um-e-nzen] “you did not give (it)” (Edzard
1976, 160; 165).
The absolutive markers of conjugation pattern 2b are placed after
the verbal base in suffix rank 2 (after [ed]):
sg. 1
st
-B-en
sg. 2
nd
-B-en
sg. 3
rd
p. -B-Ø
sg. 3
rd
non-p. -B-Ø
pl. 1
st
-B-enden
pl. 2
nd
-B-enzen
pl. 3
rd
p. -B-e“(?)
These elements are identical with the absolutive markers of conjuga­
tion pattern 1. Since one ‘slot’ can only receive either an ergative or
an absolutive marker, in cases where both might be wanted a selection
had to be made, and in such cases it is ergative that prevails: in-túd-
dè-en [i-n-tud-en] “he (erg.) beat me (abs.)” cannot be transformed into
“they beat me”, because a form *[i-n-tud-e“-en] would require to fill
one ‘slot’ with both [e“] (erg. 3
rd
pl.) and [en] (abs. 1
st
sg.). Therefore,
would such a phrase have to be expressed differently? The same
applies to forms such as *“we beat you (pl.)”, “they beat us”, etc.
e
d
z
a
r
d
_
f
1
3
_
7
0
-
1
5
5
/
P
D
F


4
/
2
8
/
0
3


2
:
4
5

P
M


P
a
g
e

8
9
Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pattern 2b
absolutive
ergative
1
st
sg. 2
nd
sg. 3
rd
sg. p. 3
rd
sg. non-p. 1
st
pl. 2
nd
pl. 3
rd
pl. p.
1
st
sg. -V-B-en – – -V-B-enzen ?
2
nd
sg. -e-B-en – – -e-B-enden ?
3
rd
sg. p. -n-B-en -n-B-en – – -n-B-enden -n-B-enzen ?
3
rd
sg. non-p. -b-B-en -b-B-en – – -b-B-enden -b-B-enzen ?
1
st
pl. – – – – –
2
nd
pl. – – – – –
3
rd
pl. p. – – – – – – –
Crossed-out boxes would imply reflexive forms (I-myself etc.) which the pattern does not serve.
Boxes with a dash refer to forms where one ‘slot’ would have to be filled by two elements at the same time, so that one
must have been selected at the expense of the other.
Boxes with a question mark refer to forms where the absolutive marker cannot be securely reconstructed.









8
9

edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 90
90 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.7.4. Prefixless finite verbal forms
Old Babylonian literary texts occasionally offer prefixless verbal forms
which still have to be considered finite because their suffixes evi­
dently represent ergative or absolutive elements of conjugation pat­
tern 1, 2a and 2b. The motivation for these forms is still unknown
nor do we know how far they were productive.
Examples known as of 1965 have been collected and commented
on by Römer 1965, 220–23.
èn-“è nú-dè-en [nud-en] “how long (O Gilgame“ . . .) will you (lie =)
sleep?’’ Gilg. Huw. A 76, 80 (var.s ì-nú-dè, ì-nú-na).
u
4
-da kur-“è àm-e
11
-dè-en-na (var.s mu-un-e
11
-[. . .], e
11
-dè-en) “today
I am about to descend to the (Foreign Country =) Netherworld”
Inana’s Descent 32.
har-ra-an lú-du-ù-bi nu-gi
4
-gi
4
-dè “à-zu a-gim túm-mu-un “how
did your heart (bring =) move you to (take) the road whose trav­
eller will not return?” Inana’s Descent 84.
a-re-e“ dug
4
-ge-e“ “they praised” Ur-Ninurta B 28.
12.7.5. Was Sumerian a language with “split ergativity”?
The question has been raised whether Sumerian was a fully-fledged
ergative language (with both the nominal and the verbal inflections
displaying definite ergative characteristics) or whether it belonged—
at least from a given period onward—to the type of language defined
by “split ergativity”. In the latter case only part of the system is
ergative (with ergative and absolutive cases and ergative behaviour
of the verbal inflexion) whereas another part of the system has a
nominative-accusative construction.
“Split ergativity” may affect both the nominal a n d the verbal
syntax; or the noun may go on exhibiting ergative and absolutive
case while only the verbal system suffers the “split”.
On “split ergativity” cf. R. M. W. Dixon 1994, 70–110: “Types of split system”.
Michalowski 1980 passim, Thomsen 1984, 51, and Attinger 1993,
150–52, define Sumerian as a language with “split ergativity”. The
nominal system is definitely ergative, and only during the OB period
some scribes lost the notion of when to apply a “subject case” in
-e and when not—an evolution that happened in the final stage of
the development of the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 91
  91
For the verb, conjugation pattern 2b (Black’s “standard construc­
tion”, see 12.3) is ergative. However, the imperative (see 12.13) and
cohortative (see 12.11.3), both using the ¢am†u base, are claimed to
show non-ergative, nominative-accusative, construction.
A non-ergative, nominative-accusative, construction is equally claimed
by the authors quoted for conjugation pattern 2a (transitive, marû).
In forms like in-na-an-du
11
[i-na-n-dug] “he (erg.) said (it) to him”
“it” has to be understood, the “slot” for absolutive [ b] before the
base, being already filled by ergative [n].
du
11
-ga-na-ab [dug-a-na-b] “say it to him/her”. Here the ergative
of the person who is commanded to speak, is implied in the imper­
ative itself; a “slot” has become open for absolutive [ b] “it”.
The same holds true for the cohortative ga-na-ab-dug
4
[ga-na-b-
dug] “let me tell it to him/her”. Here again, because the cohorta­
tive particle [ga] automatically implies a 1
st
person subject, a “slot”
has become free for absolutive [ b] “it”.
This explanation may be viewed as more plausible than the over­
all proposal to “split” conjugation patterns 2a (marû) and 2b (¢am†u,
plus imperative and cohortative) into individual patterns, one nom-
inative-accusative and the other ergative-absolutive.
Do we have to see conjugation pattern 2a (marû) as evidence for
“split ergativity” at all? Compare two examples:
(a) ses-gal-e dumu-é-dub-ba-a in-túd [i-n-tud] “the (big brother =)
school overseer hit the pupil”. Here, [n] indicates the ergative; there
is no “slot” for the absolutive which would equally be [n], before
the base [tud].
(b) ses-gal-e dumu-é-dub-ba-a in-túd-dè [i-n-tud-e] “the school over-
seer will hit the pupil”. Here, both ergative [-e] and absolutive [n]
are noted in their individual “slots”. We do not have to re-name
them “nominative” and “accusative” while the nominal subject of
the phrase, ses-gal-e, is in the ergative case.
Either way, the question of “split ergativity” does not seem to be
of any importance in Sumerian.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 92
92 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.8. Divrxsiox\r ixric\+ons
The Sumerian verb is characterized by a series of up to six prefixed
particles. Their main and original—but no longer exclusive—function
is to indicate arrest and movement, direction, separation, company
and related notions.
These indicators frequently correspond to case particles 4–9 (see
5.4.2.4–9) to suffixed nouns in the same phrase. The dimensional indi­
cators are arranged in a string within which the individual elements
have their fixed and inalterable rank.
Many dimensional indicators can be segmentized into a pronom­
inal head and a case element, e.g., [na] “for, to him/her”, consist­
ing of 3
rd
sg. person-class [n] and the (dative-)locative element [a],
or -(e)ne-da [(e)ne-da] “with them”, where pl. [(e)ne] is combined
with comitative [da].
Note to -da-: Due to the uncertainties of Sumerian spelling, esp.
with regard to the frequent non-notation of syllable-closing conso­
nants, we sometimes cannot know whether -da- was meant for [nda],
[bda], [mda], or [da] alone, and similarly for -“i-, -ta-, etc.
See 12.8.3 for the question whether dimensional indicators could
also occur without reference to a person.
For each dimensional indicator there exists a variant with prefixed
[m(u)], e.g., -na- : (-)mu-na-; (-)ba- : (-)V-m-ma-. Following the gram­
matical terminology of Akkadian, Foxvog 1974 and 1975, 400 f. with
fn. 17, proposed “Sumerian ventive” for the m-forms; see Attinger
1993, 270–80 with lit. p. 270.
If one wonders about the coincidence of both languages using an
[m] element for ventive, it should be borne in mind that the Akkadian
ventive marker is (historically) [aC] (before -k, -n, -“ ), and it is only
[am] before -m or in word-final position. There is no need, in
Akkadian, to construe ad hoc assimilation rules like “am-kum” > akkum
etc. Rather, the ventive (as opposed to affirmative) function of Akkadian
[aC/am] may have been a re-interpretation of this morpheme under
Sumerian influence, at the beginning of the Sumero-Akkadian areal
interaction. See Pedersén 1989, 434 for derivation of the Akkadian
ventive from common-Semitic affirmative (energetic) [an].
Note: The [m] of the Akkadian dative pronouns -kum, -“um, -nim, etc., has, most
probably, to be kept separate from the [m] of the ventive final form -am.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 93
  93
It seems practical to arrange the dimensional indicators in numbered
grids and to refer to the numbers for reference. We will present a
grid each for “non-ventive” and “ventive” indicators. The grids have
a horizontal division by cases and a vertical division by person. Boxes
left blank reflect forms that the system did not allow, but have been
numbered for the sake of completeness.
1
Grid 1 (non-ventive) has no entries for the 1
st
(sg. or pl.), because
st
person would automatically require ventive.
Dimensional indicators non-ventive
Dative-loc. Comitative Ablative Terminative Directive Locative 2
2
nd
sg. era (1) eda (6) (11) (e)“i (16) (e)ri (21) –
3
rd
sg.
person
na (2) nda (7) nta (12) n“i (17) ni (22) –
3
rd
sg.
non-person
ba (3) bda (8) bta (13)
ra
b“i (18) bi (23) ni (26)
2
nd
pl. (4) (9) (14) (19) (24) –
3
rd
pl.
person
(e)nea (5) (e)neda (10) (15) (e)ne“i (20) (e)ne (25) –
Dimensional indicators: ventive
Dative-loc. Comitative Ablative Terminative Directive Locative 2
1
st
sg. ma (27) muda (34) (41) mu“i (48) mu (55) –
2
nd
sg. mura (28) mueda (35) (42) mue“i (49) muri (56) –
3
rd
sg.
person
muna (29) munda (36) (43) mun“i (50) muni (57) –
3
rd
sg.
non-person
mma (30) mda (37) mta (44)
mmara
m“i (51) mmi (58)
mmeri
mini (62)
mmini
1
st
pl. mea (31) mueda (38) (45) (52) me (59) –
2
nd
pl. (32) (39) (46) (53) *muri-. . .(60)
-enzen

3
rd
pl.
person
munea (33) muneda (40) (47) mune“i (54) mune (61) –
Note: Variants are not indicated; see the individual paragraphs (ns. 1–62) below.
In many cases where [i] is shown, [e] may be preferable. In view
of our general reluctance to propose a clear phonemic distinction
between Sumerian [e] and [i], we have left NI as ni and RI as ri
instead of né and re.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 94
94 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.8.1. Non-ventive indicators (1–26)
12.8.1.1. [era, (V)ra]:
e-ra-an-du
11
“he told (it) to you” Inanna/Enki, StPohl 10, 32 II i 15.
Note: Spelling does not allow us to decide whether [era] was preceded by the
indicator [e, i] (12.9), but non-ventive e-na-, ì-na- “to him/her” (12.8.1.2) rather
favours a positive answer.
ga-ra-ab-“úm “let me give it to you”.
ki a-ra-du
11
-ga : a“ar aqbûkum “where I told you” MSL 5, 196:12.
For mu-ra- and ma-ra- see (28)
Note: [era] has mostly been analysed as [e] + [a] with [r] as a Hiatustilger. See
the discussion above, 12.7.3, note to 2
nd
sg.
12.8.1.2. [na]:
“to him/her” is the most easily predictable dimensional indicator when
the phrase contains a noun in the dative (with case particle -ra).
e-na(-n)-“úm “he/she gave (it) to him/her”.
na-e-a [na-b-e-a] “what you will say to him/her (is this)”, OS let­
ter opening formula, written na-ab-bé-a from Ur III on.
Note: It is interesting to note, diachronically, that the verbal-base [’e] turned into
[e], the latter no longer causing a hiatus between absolutive [b] and the base.
ù-na-a-du
11
[u-na-e-dug] “(after you said to him/her =) please say
to him/her” (polite imperative, see 12.11.2.2).
For mu-na- see (29).
12.8.1.3 [ba] : [ba] is treated here on the assumption that it is com­
posed of [b] and [a], in accordance with the general pattern of non­
ventive and ventive dimensional indicators.
Doubts have been raised about this interpretation, e.g., by M.
Civil apud Postgate 1974, 20 fn. 11: “(while) the prefix ba- has no
connection with a locative element /a/”, and Thomsen 1984, 176
ff., treats ba- (as well as bí-) as “conjugation prefixes”.
OS texts from ”uruppag ( Fàra) and Abu Íalàbì¢ as well as
Sumerograms used in Ebla lexical and administrative texts use both
BA (ba) and GÁ (ba
4
) as verbal prefixes in a distribution still imper­
fectly understood. They do not seem to be mere graphic variants:
“u ba
4
-ti “he received” has no correspondence “u *ba-ti and ∞ ge“ ba­
tuku “he heard” is not paralleled by ∞ge“ *ba
4
-tuku.
Note: See d’Agostino 1990, 77–82 (monolingual and bilingual lexical entries
ba-B) and 83–87 (monol. and bil. lex. entries ba
4
-B) with no overlap (with the
seeming exception of ba-DU and ba
4
-DU where we should possibly reckon on
two different readings of DU). See also Krebernik 1998, 287.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 95
  95
At present, the diachronic evidence cannot be said to support a
hypothesis that the two spellings go back to two different morphemes,
[ba
x
] and [ba
y
].
For general discussion see Attinger 1993, 204 and 280–84.
ki-a-na∞g-e ha-ba-gub “let (the stela) stand at the libation place”
Gudea Stat. B vii 55.
“u ba(-n)-ti “he (brought the hand to it =) received (it)”.
Note: The compound verb “u ti offers a typical example of the frozen use of a
directive indicator.
Apart from indicating locative proper, [ba] may also, with verbs of
motion, denote moving away, e.g., picking up something here and
taking it there. Note the contrast between ba(-n)-de
6
“he carried (it)
away” and mu(-n)-túm “he brought (it) here”, where non-ventive
[ba] is in clear opposition with ventive [mu].
Note: In the second example, the ventive function of [mu] is general, not linked
to a specific goal (person, non-person).
A special function of [ ba], first attested in Ur III, is to denote pas­
sive voice. The origin of this usage is not entirely clear. Edzard 1976,
169 f., tried to derive it from ocurrences such as ba-an-“úm [ba-n-
“um] “he (gave him to =) ordered him to (do something)” where
without ergative [n] a neutral form ba-“úm “he was given to (. . .)”
would have arisen.
Note the frequent variants in Ur III year formula: mu-un-hul “he destroyed” and
ba-hul “(a city) was destroyed”.
More research on the origin of passive [ba] is, however, very desir­
able.
Passive [ba], being disconnected from dimensional notation, has
no counterpart in ventive [mma] (30).
Although, in principle, locative [ba] and dative [na] are found in
complementary distribution, ba-na- is attested when [ ba] denotes
passive, e.g., ba-na-ge(-n) “(someone/something) was confirmed/awarded
to him” NG III p. 114.
12.8.1.4: A dative “to you (pl.)” is not attested as a dimensional
indicator.
12.8.1.5 [(e)nea]:
hé-ne-ab-“úm-mu [he-(e)ne-a-b-“um-e] “let him give it (= the rent
for the hired boat) to them (= PN
1
and PN
2
)” TCS 1 no. 135:7; cf.
no. 365:6 (Ur III).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 96
96 cn\r+rn +vrryr
The dative “to them” is more frequently replaced by the direc­
tive [(e)ne] (25).
g e-da-lu ù∞ gal “people will multiply, peo- 12.8.1.6 [eda]: ù∞ g e-da-da∞
ple will spread (with you =) during your reign” SKIZ 211:55.
ba-ra-ba-a-da-gi
4
-gi
4
-dè(-n) [bara-ba-eda-gigi-(e)d-en] “I will not, I
swear it, come back (with =) against you (with a claim)” Genava
NS 8 (1960) 306 no. 20.
For mu-e-da-, mu-ù-da- see (35).
12.8.1.7 [nda]: in-da-∞gál-àm “(PN’s tablet) is (with =) at the dispo­
sition (of PN
2
)” NG no. 208:14.
di in-da-tuku-àm “it is that he had a lawsuit with him” PBS 13,
32:3.
Note: Before [ni], [da] may be assimilated to -dì(TI)-; see (26) for Ukg. 34:1.
For mu-un-da- see (36).
12.8.1.8 [bda]: bala-nam-lugal-la-ka-ni 3 “e:gur-ta . . . kù-babbar 1:gín:e
hé-eb-da-sa
10
[he-bda(-n)-sa] “when he ruled as a king, one shekel of
silver bought, so I affirm, 3 kor of barley . . . each” Sin-kà“id 8:13–22
(and see 10:13–20, etc., see RIME 4, 454 ff.).
Note: bala- . . . -ka-ni is virtually in the comitative. D. Frayne, RIME 4, 545,
translated “3 gur of barley . . . cost one shekel of silver”. But 1:gín:e, i.e., *gín-
di“-e, can only be the subject (ergative) of the sentence.
For -b-da- cf. -e-da- in 12.8.1.6.
12.8.1.9: I cannot offer an example for “with you” (pl.).
12.8.1.10 [(e)neda]: e-ne-da-tu“ “he is residing with them” Nik. 309
r. 4.
It is impossible to decide whether or not [(e)neda] was preceded
by the neutral, non-ventive, indicator [i/e].
Poebel 1931, 16–19, suggested that -PI- might have been an allo­
graph for -be(da)- “with them” in OS ªGirsu, with non-person class
[b-e] serving in lieu of person class 3
rd
pl. [(e)ne]. Falkenstein, resum­
ing the question in AfO 18 (1957/58) 94 (d) f., preferred [(e)ne-da],
and so did Sollberger 1961, 39:233, though with a question mark.
Cf. DP 621 iii 1 e-PI-∞gál “it is with them”, and see Poebel 1931,
16 fnn. 1–4, for more examples.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 97
  97
12.8.1.11: No example available.
12.8.1.12 [nta]: Although ablative is generally incompatible with per-
son class, an exception may be found in the Ur III PN In-ta-è-a
“Having come out from her” (i.e., from the mother’s womb, whether
it refer to the real mother of the child or to the protective deity);
see Limet 1968, 438 who, however, prefers to see -nta- as a vari­
ant of -bta- (p. 79 ad 237).
Note: J. N. Postgate suggests in-ta “from the stalk” (i“innu) (referring to the
ancestors).
In the frequent OS PN E-ta-è (Struve 1984, 63–66) incomplete
spelling conceals from us whether -ta-, -bta- or -mta- (44) is intended.
12.8.1.13 [bta], [ra]: íb-ta-ni(-n)-è “he let (the boundary ditch) go
out from . . . to . . .” Ent. 28 ii 3.
iti-ta u
4
-n-kam ba-ta-zal “the n
th
of the month had ended” is a
frequent note at the end of Ur III administrative documents; it varies
with simple ba-zal “had ended” and ba-ra-zal (for [ra] see below), and
the—pleonastic?—form ba-ta-ra-zal; cf. Falkenstein 1949, 215 fn. 5.
Note: -(b)ta- or -ra- have been inserted into the frozen ba-zal. Falkenstein’s pro­
posal to explain ba-ta- as [bta] has not been maintained since Postgate 1974, 17 f.
[ta] has a variant [ra] which looks out of place in a beautifully reg­
ular system. Attinger 1993, 256–58, showed that the two morphemes
are in contemporary distribution: [ta] follows a consonant and [ra]
a vowel, [ra], therefore, being incompatible with a consonantal
pronominal element ([n], [b]). This observation does not, however,
explain the phonetic situation; for [t] and [r] (at least in our Latin
transliteration) do not form a pair.
á-sàg . . . é-ta ha-ba-ra-è “the asakku (demon) definitely left the
house”.
ní-te-ne-ne ba-ra-an-sa
10
-a“ [ba-ra-n-sa-(e)“] “They sold themselves”
TMH NF 1/2, 53: 8' (cf. Falkenstein, NG I 84 f.).
Falkenstein 1939, 180–94; id., NG III 152 f; Steinkeller 1989,
153–62, “Excursus: the verb sa
10
and the noun (níg-)sám”.
12.8.1.14: No example available.
12.8.1.15: No non-ventive example available; see (47).
12.8.1.16 [(e)“i]: “u ba-a-“i-íb-TI [“u ba-e“i-b-te∞ g-e] “he will receive
it from you” Gudea Cyl. A vii 3.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 98
98 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.8.1.17 [n“i]: in-“i(-n)-sa
10
“(purchaser) bought (object) from (seller)”
Falkenstein, NG III 153 b. The action of buying is made “in the
direction” of the seller.
12.8.1.18 [b“i]: igi-zi ba-“i-bar [ba-b“i-n-bar] “he looked friendlily at
it”.
igi na-“i-bar-re [na-b“i-bar-e] “let him not look at it” Gudea Stat.
B ix 18.
12.8.1.19: No example available to me.
12.8.1.20 [(e)ne“i]: in-ne-“i-sa
10
[i-(e)ne“i-n-sa] “(purchaser) bought
(object) from (sellers)” UET 3, 27:6; cf. 41:6.
Note: in- of in-ne- has been taken over mechanically from the much more fre­
quent sg. in-“i- (see 17).
12.8.1.21–26.
The complex of directive and locative 2, closest to the absolu-
tive/ergative elements or directly before the base, is the most difficult
in the system of directional indicators. It has been exhaustively dealt
with by Wilcke 1988, 1–49, Attinger 1993, 234–47 and, most recently,
by Zólyomi 1999 (as well as B. Jagersma apud Zólyomi), 215–53.
We owe to Wilcke the decisive breakthrough.
Both person and non-person class 3
rd
sg. [ni] (22) and [bi] (23) as
well as locative 2 [ni] (26) have variants [n] and [b] which might
easily be confused with the absolutive or ergative elements [n] and [b].
These variants occur only in a position immediately before the base:
bi - X - base -ni - X - base
Vb - Ø - base -Vn - Ø - base
Moreover, comparable to [ba] (3) with its secondary function of a
passive indicator, 3
rd
sg. person class [ni] (22) and non-person class
[bi] (23) also developed a secondary function: they may denote a
causative where [ni] or [bi] are turned—at least in our view—into
a secondary ‘subject’. Here, the parallel with [”] in the causative
formation of the Akkadian verb leaps to the eye: u“àkil“u “he (subj.
1) saw to it that he (subj. 2) ate (it)” = “he made him eat (it)”. If
we acknowledge the existence of a Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area
(see 17) we cannot help seeing a correlation between Sumerian [ni],
[bi] and the Akkadian causative markers [“u], [“a], [“].
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 99
  99
Note: The bilingual grammatical lists abound in ‘equations’ between a Sumerian
verbal form with -ni- or bi- and an Akkadian ”-form. See, e.g., MSL 4, 79 ff.,
gar-ma-ab: “uknam “set (it) to me”, ∞ OBGT VI 19 ∞ gar-ma-ni-ib: “u“kinam “have (it)
set to me”, and passim; see, generally, Black 1991, 27–35. Black, however, admits,
p. 34, that “All in all, the Sumerian of the causative sections . . . is complicated
and awkward. We can only say that the Babylonian grammarians seem to have
tried to force Sumerian into a straight jacket by devising un-idiomatic forms
which would correspond to those of the complex and productive causative sys­
tem possessed by their own language”.
It is uncertain whether the directional indicator for locative 2, [ni]
(26), is secondary in the system and a deviant of the directive set
(21–25), or whether it is original to the system and there was per­
haps homography (not necessarily homophony) between [ni
a
] (22)
and [ni
b
] (26). See the discussion below.
12.8.1.21 [(e)ri]:
d
Utu-eri(URU)-è “the Sungod has risen next to you”
(PN).
Note: In OS, URU = eri occurs as one of the extremely rare bisyllabic syl­
labograms; see also Sollberger 1952, 78.
nam ge
4
-rí-íb-tar
ar
nam-dùg gú-mu-rí-íb-tar
ar
“let me make a firm
promise for you, let me make a benevolent firm promise on your
behalf ” ”ulgi D 384 f. (Klein 1981, 88).
Note: The contrast of non-ventive [eri] and ventive [muri] implies growing
intensity.
∞gá-e ga-ri-ib-zu-zu “let me make it all known to you” TMH NF 3,
!
25:13 f. (Akkadian gloss lu-uq -bi-ki ); see C. Wilcke, AfO 23 (1974)
84–87.
Note: [eri] may be analyzed as e+r-i/e with -r- as a Hiatustilger. Cf. above [era]
(1), and ibid. for Krecher’s interpretation of [er] as an indissoluble unit.
12.8.1.22 [ni]: (PN
1
-e PN
2
-ra object) al ì-ni(-n)-du[g
4
] “(PN
1
) demanded
(object) [to =] from PN
2
” NRVN 1, 247:3.
túg ì-ni-in-dul “he draped a garment over her” ZA 55 (1962) 70,
1:10.
I cannot offer non-ventive examples for person class [ni] in causative
function; for ventive mu-ni- see (57).
12.8.1.23 [bi]: [bi] is, next to [mu] and [ba], the most frequent
opener of Sumerian verbal forms.
In Pre-Sargonic Girsu, there was an orthographic distribution of
bé- [BI] and bí- [NE] which Kramer 1936, following Poebel 1931,
explained by way of regressive “vowel harmony”: V
1
occurs if the
following syllable contains V
a
; and V
2
if there is V
b
. bé-, according
to Poebel, stood before a syllable with a vowel close to the opening
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 100
100 cn\r+rn +vrryr
grade of [a], and bí- before a syllable with a vowel close to the
opening grade of [i]: e.g., bé-ra, bí-ri. It goes without saying that
we often risk a circular argument based on our Latin transliteration.
Still, there is some chance that the distributional pattern worked,
more or less, along the lines described by Poebel and Kramer. See
also below, 12.9.
Be that it as it may, although “vowel harmony” is ubiquitous in
languages of the agglutinative type, it would completely upset our
ideas about Sumerian phonetics, if we applied such a phenomenon
not to the language as a whole, but only to a very restricted sector.
OS ªGirsu spelling usage should, therefore, probably be seen as an
isolated phenomenon which realized in writing peculiarities that were
perhaps more widely present in the spoken language.
An-né Ki-en-gi-ra nam bí-in-tar
ar
“An made a firm promise to
Sumer” ”ulgi F 30 (see Zólyomi 1999, 249:74).
Note: Here again, as in (21), nam tar governs the directive.
“à-kù-ge bí(-n)-pà “(the lord Ningirsu called =) chose (Gudea) unto
his pure mind” Gudea Cyl. A. xxiii 22 f.
Note: The directive expresses motion close to, but not into, something.
“u-a bí(-n)-ge
4
“he (returned into the hand =) delivered (objects)”
AWL 81 v 3 f.
Cf. “u-na ì-ni(-n)-ge
4
“he delivered (object) (into her hand =) to
her” AWL 178 v 4–5 (see Zólyomi 1999, 231:26 f.).
Eridu
ki
-gin
7
ki-sikil-la bí(-n)-§ rú “(Gudea) built (the temple) on a
place as pure as Eridu” (or: “pure as if (in) Eridu) Gudea Stat. B
iv 8–9.
gin
ki
-“è ka-Ni∞ eri-ni Ni∞ gin
ki
-ke
4
má bí(-n)-ús “(Gudea, on his way)
to her city N. (let the boat come close =) moored the boat at the
harbour of N.” Gudea Cyl. A iv 4.
Note: This example is especially instructive because it shows that ergative (Gudea)
and directive (harbour) may co-occur as two cases marked by [e]. See 5.4.29.
For the shortened form [b] preceded by [i] or [a] note: ki“ib-PN
2
. . .
-a(k) íb-ra [i-b(i)-ra] “(instead of PN’s seal) PN
2
’s seal has been (struck =)
rolled (on the document)” ITT 2/1, 3470 r. 1–2.
Note: The directive is used to denote immediate contact of the seal with the sur­
face of the tablet.
ì-íb-∞gál “there is (. . . on the field)” CT 7, 18 r. 2 (Zólyomi 1999,
227:15).
Cf. ì-in-∞gál “there is (in the basket)” UET 3, 153:4 (Zólyomi, ibid.
16; with locative 2).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 101
  101
Note: Wilcke 1988, 35, suggested that ì-íb-∞ gál means “is found on (the surface
of ) something” and ì-in-∞gál “is found in it”, thus explaining the contrastive use
of directive [b(i)] and locative 2 [n(i)].
ab-ús-sa [a-b(i)-us-a] “(house) which is bordering on (. . .)” Ukg. 4/5
xi 3.
ab-§ rú-a “(pedestals) which had been erected (at . . .)” Ent. 28/29
ii 41.
Parallel to person class [ni], non-person class [bi] may assume the
function of expressing causative:
ud
5
-dè ní∞g-àr-ra bí-íb-gu
7
-en “I will (make eat =) feed the goats
with groats” Nanna’s Journey 271.
ní∞g-àr-ra “groats” is in the absolutive (‘object’); the ergative sub­
ject of the one who feeds is implied in the verbal form. The second
‘subject’, the one being fed, cannot stand in a second absolutive case
(in Akkadian: double accusative); it is in the directive.
Edzard 1976b, 72 f., attempted an explanation of the Sumerian
causative construction: The Akkadian sentence “arrum “akkanakkam
àlam u“èpi“ “the king had the general build a city” cannot be ren­
dered in Sumerian with two absolutive (“accusative”) cases. The per­
son caused to act (“akkanakkum) has to stand in a dimensional case,
preferably in the dative if it is a person: lugal-e (ergative) “agin-ra
(dative) eri (absolutive). The dative “agin-ra cannot, however, be
resumed by the infixed particle of the dative, -na-, because a sen­
tence *lugal-e “agin-ra eri mu-na-an-§ rú would mean “the king built
a city for the general”; instead of -na-, directive -ni- is used.
Zólyomi 1999, 219 with fn. 9, quotes, but rejects Edzard’s expla­
nation, but the author would still maintain his position.
Note: Zólyomi’s example (1999, 318:6) É-an-na-túm-ra lú ti mu-ni(-n)-ra “some­
one (struck =) sent an arrow against E.” has no causative implication at all and
cannot serve as an argument.
12.8.1.23a: The imperative “úm-me-eb “give it to us” has most prob­
ably to be interpreted as a ventive form; see (59).
12.8.1.24: No examples available to the author for “to you (pl.)”.
gar [i-ene-e-n-∞ 12.8.1.25 [(e)ne]: gú-ne-ne-a e-ne(-n)-∞ gar] “he (put (it)
on their neck =) charged (it) to their account”.
Note: More frequent in the sg. gú-na e-na(-n)-∞gar where dative -na- (2) is used.
While the sg. offers a clear locative : dative correspondence, in the pl. it is loca­
tive : directive. For reasons still unknown to us *[i-ene-a-n-∞gar] has been replaced
by [i-ene-e-n-∞gar].
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 102
102 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.8.1.26 [ni]: Falkenstein 1949, 206 f. (also 1959
a
, 48), had defined
[ni], reduced [n], as a positional variant of [bi] in ba-ni- or mu-ni-,
each time allegedly caused by dissimilation of the second of two labi­
als (*ba-bi-, *mu-bi-). However, Sumerian has no general aversion
against bab(V)-, mub(V)-; cf. only mu-bi “this year” or gub-ba-bi,
“ub-ba-bi “its being placed, having fallen”.
Falkenstein was criticized by Gragg 1973, 69–90, and Postgate
1974, 21 f. Gragg 1973, 73 ff., has extensive material for locative
[ni], and Postgate showed that [ni] in mi-ni- (< *bi-ni-) and im-mi-
ni- (not = im-ma-ni-!) cannot be identical with the directive element.
Also, mi-ni- and mu-ni- have to be kept strictly apart; see for more
discussion (62).
An autonomous locative element [ni] had already been noted by
Jacobsen, MSL 4 (1956) 39* (“allocative”), Sollberger 1952, 90–96
(“locatif ”), again Jacobsen 1965, 95 (“neuter allative-illative”), and
see most definitively Wilcke 1988, 40 f.
We prefer to write “locative 2” in order not to have to rename
the locative dimensional indicators [ba] (3) and [(m)ma] (30).
At any rate, the directive dimensional indicator and locative 2
may follow each other and do not fall into the same “slot”, so that
they definitely have to be kept apart.
tu
mu“en
-min-nam igi-ba “embi ba-ni(-n)-∞ gar “he put kohl on the eyes
of two pigeons” Ean. 1 xviii 2–3.
e-bi íd-nun-ta gú-eden-na-“è íb-ta-ni(-n)-è “(Enmetena) made his
ditch-and-dyke come up from the ‘High Canal’ to the ‘Border-of-
the-Steppe’” Ent. 28–29 ii 1–3.
e(-n)-da(-n)-dug
4
-ga-a [e-n-da-n-dug-a-a] “u nu-dì-ni(-b)-bala-e [nu
(-b)-da-ni-b-bala-e] “he (Erikagina) will not (turn the hand over (-a) =)
change what Ningirsu had (spoken with =) agreed upon with
(Erikagina)” Ukg. 34:1.
Note: Comitative [da] (8) has become dì(TI) by regressive assimilation to [ni].
ì-in-∞gál “it is contained therein” Wilcke 1988, 24 f.
ba-an-ku
4
“it has been brought in there” Wilcke 1988, 27 n. 97
bottom.
Note: [ban], reduced from [bani], forms a closed syllable. In this case, in OS
spelling, the syllable-closing consonant is most often omitted in writing. Therefore,
OS ba- may stand for (1) ba-Ø-, e.g., ba-ú“ “he died”; (2) ba-n- (-n- being the
absolutive or ergative element) and (3) ba-niØ-, reduced to ba-n(i)Ø/-. This poly­
valence of spelling does not facilitate the modern interpreter’s task.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 103
  103
12.8.2. Ventive indicators (27–62)
In the following series of ventive dimensional indicators the trans­
lations do their best to render the nuance given to the verbal forms
by these indicators.
12.8.2.27 [ma]: Dimensional indicators for the 1
st
p. sg. (and pl.) are
to the best of our knowledge exclusively ventive. For whereas in the
2
nd
or 3
rd
person there may always have been a choice between “to
you/him here” and “to you/him there”, the 1
st
person “to me/us”,
“with me/us”, etc., always implies motion towards the person (or
object).
ma-a-dug
4
[ma-e-dug] “you told me”.
d
En-líl-le-ma-an-ba “Enlil attributed to me (the child)” (PN, cf.
Akkadian Enlil-iqì“am).
Note: The verb ba, in spite of Akkadian qâ“u “to give as a present”, is primar­
ily “to allocate (something to someone)”, and the idea of a gift is secondary.
ma-an-∞gál “he let (it) be there for me”.
gá-e mu-zu-“è ∞ 12.8.2.28 [muera]: ∞ gi
6
an-bar
7
-ba
gi“
gag-ti mu-e-ra-
TAR.TAR “(as for) me: (for your name =) on your behalf, day and
night, (the ribs are being cut for you =) I get upset because of you”
Father and Son 121.
nam-ti ha-mu-ra-sù(-d) “may life be long for you”.
“u-zi ma-ra-a-∞gar “I (put a faithful hand to you =) did faithfully
obey you” Gudea Cyl. B ii 20.
1
Note: mu-ra- and ma-ra-, both for [muera], may have been synchronic options.
The regressive vowel assimilation in ma-ra- was most probably conditioned by
st
person [ma] “to me”.
12.8.2.29 [muna]: mu-na- is one of the most frequent sequences
among linked dimensional indicators. This is mainly due to its ubi­
quitous occurrence in building inscriptions: mu-na(-n)-§rú “he built for
(DN)”, in dedications: a mu-na(-“è)(-n)-ru “he dedicated (object) to
(DN) (for the life of PN)”, or in dialogues: gù mu-na-dé-e “he says
to him”.
ha-mu-na-ab-“úm-mu [ha-mu-na-b-“um-e] “let him give it to him”
Ur III, see Sollberger 1966 glossary p. 169 f.: 640 s.v. sumu.
Since its publication, OS RTC 19 has been used to demonstrate
the contrast of (non-ventive) e- and (ventive) mu-:
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 104
104 cn\r+rn +vrryr
“u mu-na(-n)-taka “(visiting lady) conveyed (gifts) to her (= person
visited)” iii 2.
mu(-n)-da-∞ gen-na “(PN) who had come with her (on her way to
person visited)” iii 6 (seen from the side of the visiting person).
mu-na(-n)-“úm “(visiting lady) gave (gifts) to her (= person visited)”
iv 3.
And contrast “u e-na(-n)-taka “(receiver of gifts) conveyed (gifts in
return) to her (= visiting person)” r. ii 3.
e(-n)-da-∞ gen “(PN) had come with her (on her way to person vis­
ited)” r. ii 5 (seen from the side of the person visited).
e-na(-n)-“úm “(receiver of gifts) gave (gifts in return) to her (= vis­
iting person)” r. iii 5.
Note: The document was drawn up at the residence of Bara-namtara, wife of
ensi Lugal-anda of Laga“. All actions and motions in her direction are noted as
ventives; all actions and motions referring to the visiting person, Nin-∞gi“kimti,
wife of the ensi of Adab, are noted as neutral non-ventives.
12.8.2.30 [mma]: An zà-gal-la mu-na(-n)-tu“, An-ra
d
En-líl im-ma-
ni(-n)-ús [-mma-ni-n-us],
d
En-líl-ra,
d
Nin-mah mu-ni(-n)-ús “(Gudea)
seated An for him (= Ningirsu) at the (big side =) seat of honour,
he made Enlil sit there next to An, (and) he seated Ninmah at Enlil(’s
side)” Gudea Cyl. B xix 18–21.
Note: The seating at the banquet was Ningirsu-An-Enlil-Ninmah. The change
from [muna] to [immani] to [muni] was probably made for reasons of style,
avoiding repetition, rather than for morpho-syntactic subtlety.
dumu-ù ama-ni-ra KA-§rú-a nu-ma-na-dug
4
[nu-mma-na-n-dug] “no
child would (say a ‘stuck-in’ word on something to =) disobey its
mother” Gudea Cyl. A xiii 4–5.
For more Gudea examples see Falkenstein 1949, 202.
Umma
ki
e-ma-zi(-g) [e-mma-zig] “Umma rose against it” Ukg. 6
iv 10'–11'.
Íl-e nam-énsi Umma
ki
-a “u e-ma(-n)-ti [e-mma-n-ti] “Il seized for
himself the ensi-ship at Umma” Ent. 28/29 iii 37.
12.8.2.31 [mea]: Alongside the frequent Ur III PN (in Umma)
d
Ӈra-
mu(-n)-túm “”ara brought (the child)” (e.g., Limet 1968, 528), there
are variants of the name:
d
”ára-me-a-túm “”ara brought us (the
child)” Johnes/Snyder 1961, 270:53, and DN-me-túm (see 59).
Note: The pronominal element of the indicator, [me], is also found as the 1
st
pl.
of the possessive particle (cf. 5.2).
Is [me] self-sufficient, or is it the result of haplological ellipsis: *[mume]
> [me]? See (38) for [mueda], probably resulting from hypothetical
*[mumeda].
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 105
  105
12.8.2.32: No example available.
12.8.2.33 [munea]: No example available; see (61).
12.8.2.34 [muda]: ha-mu-da-gub “let (Nan“e) stand by me” Gudea
Cyl. A i 25.
g [mu-eda-V-á∞ 12.8.2.35 [mueda]: á-“è mu-e-da-a-á∞ g] “I (passed (it)
to the arm with you =) ordered you”
“à-bi nu-mu-ù-da-zu [nu-mu-e-da-V-zu] “I did not learn (with =)
from you (about) (its heart =) the matter” Gudea Cyl. A viii 22.
Note: [e] of [eda] assimilated to preceding [u].
12.8.2.36 [munda]: á mu(-n)-da(-n)-á∞g “he (passed the arm with =)
ordered him” Gudea Cyl. A xv 11.
eri-ni ki-Lagas
ki
-e si
11
-lí-a u
4
mu(-n)-dì-ni-íb-zal-e [mu-nda-ni-b-
zal-e] “his city (and) the land of Lagas spend the day here with him
in rejoicing”
Note: [a] of [da] assimilated to following [i] of [ni]; cf. above (26) Ukg. 34:1.
ad mu-un-di
1
-ni-íb-ge
4
-ge
4
“(the king) takes council with her” Three
Men of Adab 17, see Alster 1991–93, 32.
12.8.2.37 [mda]: ad im-dab
5
-ge
4
-ge
4
[i-mda-b-gege] “(Nisaba) was
consulting with (the tablet) for herself (ventive)” Gudea Cyl. A v 1.
d
Utu im-da-húl “the Sungod rejoiced over (the brick)” Gudea Cyl.
A xix 19.
Note: In hé-em-“i-húl “let him rejoice over it” the object of joy is noted in the
terminative (51).
12.8.2.38 [mueda](?): a nu-mu-e-da-∞ gál “there was no water (with =)
for us” Three Men of Adab 6, see Alster 1991–93, 31.
Note: Is [mueda] a dissimilated form of *[mumeda]? Or is it literally “with you
(sg.)” as a rhetorical form used by the story-teller?
me-e-de-en-ha-ze “he will seize him (= Dumuzi) from us (= the
demons)” VS 2, 2 ii 44, 46.
12.8.2.39: No example available.
12.8.2.40 [muneda]: No example available.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 106
106 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.8.2.41–43: No examples available.
3
Note: Ablative is generally incompatible with 1
st
and 2
nd
persons as well as with
rd
person person-class; see, however, discussion on possible [nta] in 12.8.1.12.
12.8.2.44 [mta], [mmara]:
a) [mta]: lú E-ninnû-ta im-ta-ab-è-è-a “he who would remove (the
statues) here from the Eninnû” Gudea Stat. B viii 7.
Note: The parallel version of Stat. C iv 6 has the non-ventive form íb-ta-ab-è-
è-a. This is hardly a free variant, but has to do with the fact that in Statue C
the temple is Inana’s Eana and not Nin∞ girsu’s own Eninnû. So, imta- refers to
her own, ibta- to another, environment.
b) im-ma-ra-an-dú-ud [i-mmara-n-dud] “she gave birth from (her)
own (vulva)” Enki-Ninh. 253, etc.
Note: We follow Attinger’s translation proposal (b), see ZA 74 (1984) 46; his
alternative (a) was “(X) est né(e) pour toi” where -ra- would be taken as the
directive indicator [era] (1).
“à-bi-ta nu-ù-ma-ra(-n)-è-a [nu-mmara-n-e-a] “(he swore) he had not
removed (anything) from (the storehouse)” NG no. 205:53.
Note: An alternative interpretation would be [mma + ra], i.e., locative + ablative.
12.8.2.45–46: No examples available (see note on 12.8.2.41–43).
12.8.2.47: No example available.
NG no. 51:15 mu-ne-ra-è “(two witnesses) came forth (against two
other persons)” only seemingly contains [(e)nera] “from (among)
them”. [(e)ne] is in fact virtually a dative, replaced by the directive
(cf. 12.8.2.5, 25, and 61). This becomes clear from NG no. 127:16
mu-na-ra-ni-è-e“ “they came forth (as witnesses) against him (-na-)”;
see also no. 129:13. In cases without dative (or directive) of the per­
son, è(-d) “to come forth” is construed with im-ta- or íb-ta- (see NG
III 105) s.v. è(-d) 3. The ablative is used here either to refer to the
group of persons from which the witnesses rose or it has become
“frozen” to denote the special meaning of è(-d), “to come forth”.
See Falkenstein, NG I 68 f. fn. 4.
Note: [ra] probably never has a preceding pronominal element. See below 12.8.3
for the question whether other dimensional indicators could occur only with gen­
eral reference to the verb’s semantic meaning and with no (even suppressed)
pronominal element.
12.8.2.48 [mu“i]: mu-“i-in-“e “he favoured me”.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 107
  107
12.8.2.49 [mue“i]:
d
En-líl-le igi-zi mu-e-“i-in-bar [mu-e“i-n-bar] “Enlil
looked at you full of joy” Iddin-Dagan Hymn: SKIZ 209:5.
Ki-en-ge-re
6
kur-kur igi-bi ha-mu-“i-∞ gál “all the countries, let their
eyes be directed to (you, O) Sumer” Gudea Cyl. B xxii 20.
Note: [Ki-engere] is taken here as a vocative. If it were an ergative and if 2
nd
sg. were not intended, one would expect rather *hé-em-“i-∞ gál (51).
G.KIª 12.8.2.50 [mun“i]: ki mu-un-“i-KIª G “he looks for (someone)”
Lugalbanda I 270 f.
12.8.2.51 [m“i]: hé-em-“i-húl “let him rejoice over it”.
12.8.2.52: No example available.
12.8.2.53: No example available.
12.8.2.54 *[mu-ne-“i]: No example available.
12.8.2.55 [mu]: mu-“è mu-“e
21
(SA
4
) [mu-V-n-“e] “he gave me as a
name (‘. . .’)” Gudea Cyl. A x 5, 14.
12.8.2.56 [muri]: nam ge
4
-rí-íb-tar
ar
nam-dùg gú-mu-rí-íb-tar
ar
”ulgi
D 384 f., see above (12.8.1.21).
12.8.2.57 [muni]: mu-“è mu-ni-“e
21
“he gave him as a name (‘. . .’)”
Note: mu-“è mu-na(-n)-“e
21
means “he gave (the statue) as a name (‘. . .’) for
his/her (= the deity’s) sake” Gudea Stat. A iv 3; D v 7; etc. Here, -na- refers
to the deity to whom the statue is dedicated, not to the person or object named.
na-ám-zé-eb du
10
-mu-ni-ib-tar
ar
“let me make a firm promise for him”
(Emesal) TLB 2, 2:36, 38, 41 (”ulgi).
Note: Only a short excerpt of the vast and complicated morphological evidence
for the verb nam tar could be offered; see also 12.8.1.23, 2:56.
mu-ni-ús see above (12.8.2.30).
12.8.2.58 [mmi], [mmeri]:
a) [mmi]: e-ba na-§
tu
rú-a e-me-sar-sar [e-mmi-n-sarsar] “at the dyke
here (on this side) he inscribed stelae” Ent. 28/29 ii 4–5.
mu“en
-min-nam sa∞g-ba eren ì-mi-du
8
[i-mmi-n-du] “he stuck ‘cedar’
(twigs) at the heads of two pigeons (of his)” Ean. 1 obv. xviii 4, xxi
16, etc.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 108
108 cn\r+rn +vrryr
é . . . an-né im-ús “the House . . . borders on Heaven” Gudea Cyl.
B i 6.
Note: [mmi] here is reduced to [m(mi)] immediately before the base. For more
Gudea examples see Falkenstein 1949, 207 f.
lú GN-ta ì-im-e-re-“a [i-m(mi)-ere-(e)“-a] “(persons) who had come
here from GN” UET 3, 1633:11, and see Wilcke 1988, 32.
Note: Many more examples for ì-im-B in Wilcke 1988, 15; 19–32.
b) [immeri]: hur-sa∞g-umun
7
-e im-me-ri-BAL.BAL [i-mmeri-BALBAL]
“he crossed seven mountain ranges” Enm. 171, and cf. Lugalbanda
I 252.
Note: The form is difficult to explain. Jacobsen 1965 (= 1970), 265 analysed it
as containing “(b)ri ‘beyond it’, ‘over it’” (= “Neuter Superlative”).
[immeri] is hardly imaginable without the model of [immara] (see
12.8.2.44.b), for which an alternative interpretation as [mma+ra],
locative + ablative, was suggested (see 12.8.2.44 end). Was, there­
fore, [immeri] just a “rhyming” form of [immara]?
Note: In OB Sumerian, apparently, no fixed form is used when “crossing, pass­
ing over (something)” is to be expressed. Traditionally, one would expect [bta]
(13) or [mta] (44), e.g., ∞ge“-gán-na ib-ta(-n)-bala “he made (someone) step over
the wooden rod” (cf. Edzard 1970, 8–53).
When the hero crosses mountain-ranges, OB texts offer a variety of
forms: in-ti-bala/bal-lam Gilg. Hu. A 61 ff.; bé-ri-bala, in-TE-bala
(ibid.); see Edzard 1991, 187 f.
12.8.2.59 [me]:
d
Nanna-me(-n)-túm “the Moongod brought us (the
child)” Limet 1968, 75.
Note: [me] is probably a (free?) variant of [mea], for which see above (12.8.2.31).
“úm-me-eb [“um-me-b] “give it to us” Inana’s Descent 278.
Note: See above (12.8.2.31) note on whether [me] is self-sufficient or the result
of haplological ellipsis.
12.8.2.60: No example available.
gir-gal-gal-Lagas
ki
-ke
4
-ne(-r) é-a-ne-ne mu-ne-§ 12.8.2.61 [mune]: di∞ rú
[mu-(e)ne-e-n-§ ru] “he built here their houses for the great gods of
Lagas” Gudea Stat. I iii 6; P iii 7.
15.8.2.62 [mini], [mmini]:
g-gin
7
. . . sa∞ a) [mini]: é-e hur-sa∞ g an-“è mi-ni-íb-íl “the House
(raised the head =) proudly looked up to Heaven . . . like a moun­
tain range” Gudea Cyl. A xxi 23.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 109
+nr yrnn 109
b) [mmini]: (é . . .) DUGUD-gin
7
an-“à-ge im-mi-ni-íb-DIRI.DIRI-
ne “they let (the House) soar into the midst of Heaven as if it were
a cloud” Gudea Cyl. A xxi 20.
12.8.3. Dimensional indicators without reference to a person?
The question must be asked whether in principle a dimensional indi­
cator could occur with general reference to the verb’s semantic mean­
ing, but with no (even suppressed) pronominal element. Were there
-da- “with”, -ta- “from”, or -“i- “toward” indicating that the verb
implied the general idea of having something with it, stemming from,
moving towards a goal? The question is extremely hard to answer
because of our well-known difficulties interpreting Sumerian syllabic
spelling where, until the beginning of the 2
nd
millennium B.C., syl-
lable-closing consonants could be neglected in writing: did, e.g., -da­
always stand for [nda], [bda], [mda] or could [da] alone be intended?
So far the only possible exception seems to be ablative [ra] (see
12.8.1.13, 12.8.2.44) which is never found with a person element,
e.g., *[bra] or *[mra]. But [ra] is an extraneous element anyhow in
the system of the dimensional indicators.
12.9. Pnrrixrr ixric\+on [r. i]
Prefixed [e] or [i], spelled e- or ì-, is difficult to define. It has chal­
lenged Sumerologists for more than a century. If a basic function
once existed it may well have vanished over the long period during
Sumerian was spoken and written.
Note: Our transliteration NI = i is confirmed by allograph i- from Ur III onward.
Vanstiphout 1985, 1–2, resumed the Forschungsgeschichte; see, there­
after, Wilcke 1988, 2–4.
It is advisable to start from forms where [e]/[ì] precedes the ver­
bal base as the only element and where no suffixes occur, i.e., 3
rd
sg. forms of the intransitive conjugation pattern 1 (see 12.7.1): e-∞gen
“he went”, e-∞ gál “it is/was present”, ì-til “he lived, stayed”.
The function of [e]/[i] may be defined here as an element moving
the verbal base out of its neutral (lexical) mode into a finite verbal
mode, i.e., indication—or implication—of a pronominal participant.
[e]/[i], seen this way, is left without an oppositional mark. It could
not be defined as, e.g., “non-ventive” in a general way because it may
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 110
110 cn\r+rn +vrryr
occur at least with a ventive directional indicator of the 3
rd
person
non-person class, cf. above [mma] 12.8.2.30, [mda] 12.8.2.37, etc.
The idea of two allegedly opposed elements [mu] and [e, i], termed
“Konjugationspräfixe” by B. Landsberger apud R. Scholtz 1934, 2,
which was essential to Falkenstein 1950, 158–181; 1959, 45 f., 58
f.; Römer 1982, 65–69; Yoshikawa 1979, 185–206 and others, lost
its relevance since the discovery of the Sumerian “ventive” (above
12.8, p. 92) of which [mu] is but part.
Falkenstein supposed ì- to constitute a nasalized vowel (still main­
tained in Thomsen 1984, 162 f. and passim) because he analysed
im-ma- as *˜ı-ba-. This theory, involving a circular argument, has
been disproved by Gragg 1973, and Postgate 1974.
Poebel 1931 demonstrated that in the pre-Sargonic texts from Girsu
(Lagas) e- and ì- show complementary distribution, each form deter­
mined by the quality of the vowel in the following syllable; 12.8.1.23
cf. e.g., e-sar, e-da-, e-ta-, e-“è- versus ì-gu
7
(“kú”), ì-rí-, ì-mi- etc.
In a number of cases, Poebel begged the question. Because of
e-ur
4
(instead of *i-ur
4
) “he sheared”, Poebel postulated e-or
4
; see
also his e-sor, e-ne-sor for -sur.
See above, 12.8.1.23, on alleged “vowel harmony”.
Krecher 1985 proposed to reduce—or even partially eliminate—the
existence of a Sumerian verbal prefix [e]/[i]. He claimed that the
vowel was mainly part of the pronominal element constituting or
opening a verbal prefix (series). So, he proposed to define pronom­
inal elements (-)n-, (-)b- as original in-, ib-, thus including an alleged
prefix [e]/[i] as part of the pronominal prefix. Krecher’s view was
refuted by Wilcke 1988 who brought ample evidence of pre-OB
spellings ì-íb-, ì-in-, ì-im- (as well as a-ab-, see below 12.10) consti­
tuting a prefix series which could not possibly have been rendering
merely a non-functional [i].
Even if we cannot recover the original function of [e]/[i], we may
repeat that the prefix served, before a simple base, to indicate
“finalization” or “pronominalization” of the base. By extension, it
may then also have been used when absolutive, ergative and dimen­
sional indicators preceded the base: in-sar [i-n-sar] “he wrote”, in­
gál [i-nda-∞ da-∞ gal] “it is/was present with him”, etc.
There is a distribution rule limiting the use of [e]/[i]: (1) It occurs
with all non-ventive indicators except with [ba] (3); there is no *[iba].
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 111
+nr yrnn 111
(2) It does not occur with ventive indicators except with the 3
rd
sg.
non-person class series [mma] (30), [mda] (37), [mta], [mmara] (44),
[m“i] (51), [mmi], [mmeri] (58), and [mmini] (62). There is no *[ima],
*[imuda] etc.
For peculiarities of [ba] (3) which are still in need of further elu­
cidation see above 12.8.1.3.
12.10. Pnrrixrr ixric\+on [\(r)]
a- and al- both indicate the notion of state (not necessarily passive)
or habitualness, as against the notion of action, mobility, or becoming.
enim-bi al-til “the respective matter is in the state of having being
settled” (ubiquitous in pre-Ur III and Ur III court documents).
x y-“è ab-“i-∞ gar “(x is set in relation to y =) x equals y”, e.g.,
NRVN I 202:3.
al- was until recently seen as unique among Sumerian verbal
prefixes because of its alleged inability to combine with any other
morpheme—apart from prospective [u]: *ù-al- > ù-ul-.
Attinger 1993, 267–69 (a-) and 269 f. (al-), both with extensive
literature, clearly states (p. 269) that “[al] semble être le pendant de
[a] dans le cas où la base n’est pas précédé d’un préf. III (i.e., the
group next to the base: absolutive, ergative, dimensional indicator)
ou d’un préfixe II (i.e., the group next-but-one to the base, [i], ven­
tive indicators)”; he stresses that a- before the base must always go
back to a-x-B and that “la seule fonction de -l-” (i.e., in [al]) “est
d’indiquer que [a] est directement suivi de la base, que donc al-B
représente morphématiquement [a+B]”.
Thus Attinger implicitly stated that a- and al- are found in com­
plementary morphemic distribution.
Since an element [l] encountered exclusively in [al] would be hard
to explain, it seems preferable to posit with Attinger just the one
morpheme [a(l)], with the allomorphs [a] and [al] depending on
Sumerian syllable structure: *[alb] > [ab], *[alnda] > [anda], etc.
Note: As a matter of fact, a circular argument is involved: The Sumerian sys­
tem of syllable writing which we can only see through “Akkadian glasses”, had
no room for such notations as [alb], [bla], [albra]. We, therefore, discount the
possible occurrence of such consonantal clusters in Sumerian syllable structure.
Therefore this line of reasoning only really holds good for a Semitic language
such as Akkadian where such clusters do not occur.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/30/03 10:23 AM Page 112
112 cn\r+rn +vrryr
a- *[a(l)-n(i)]: é me-lim
5
-bi kur-kur-ra a-dul
5
“the awe of the House
is spread over all the countries” Ent. 8 vi 2.
For the formula u
4
an-§ gál see Edzard 2003a, 89. rú enim an-∞
ab- *[a(l)-b(i)]: bára-§ gir-ré-ne nam-nun-da-ki-∞ rú-a rú-a-di∞ gar-ra ab-§
“the pedestals of the gods, which had been erected there in Namnunda-
ki∞gara” Ent. 28–29 ii 39–41.
ab-da- [*a(l)-b-da]: (silver) má“-bi-“è dam-[a-ni] . . . ab-da-gub “for
its interest PN, [his] wife, (stands =) guarantees” TMH NF 1–2,
32:1–4.
gen igi-∞ àm- *[a(l)-m(mi)]: en-na àm-∞ gu
10
-“è enim-bi a-bala-e na-ba-
an-§rú “until he came here to report before me, he (= the addressee)
should not be (stuck in =) kept” TCS 1 no. 125:8–9.
an- *[a(l)-n]: a-na-gin
7
an-AG “(the one who had one son, . . .)
how does he fare?” Gilgame“, Enkidu and the Netherworld 255 and
passim.
an-da- *[a(l)-n-da]: an-da-tuku “(creditor, erg.) has (a credit) with
him (= debtor)” SR 99 iv 12–15.
ú-[ni al]-bar a-ni al-bar ú-gíd al-gu
7
-e a-gíd al-na
8
-na
8
eri-bar-ra-a
al-tu“ “[his] (grass =) food is separate, his (water =) drink is sepa­
rate, he is supposed to eat (long =) faraway food, he is supposed to
drink faraway water, he sits at the suburb” Gilgame“, Enkidu and
the Netherworld. (UET 6/1, 58:6).
Note: For more evidence, see Edzard 2003a, 87–98.
12.11–12. Mor\r \xr coxxrc+ixo ixric\+ons
It seems advisable to treat these two kinds of indicators together
because their functions partly interlink. The following chart is divided
into a positive left and a negative right part.
Ind. Ø- (1
st
–3
rd
p.) 12.11.1 Neg NU- (1
st
–3
rd
p.) 12.11.2
Ø- 12.13.1 INGA Ind. NU- 12.13.2 NGA
Coh. GA- (1
st
p.) 12.11.3 Neg BARA- (1
st
p.) 12.11.4
GA- 12.13.3 Coh.
Prec. HÉ-
HÉ-
(2
nd
, 3
rd
p.)
12.13.4
12.11.5
NGA
Vet. BARA- (2
nd
, 3
rd
p.) 12.11.6
Aff. 1 HÉ- (1
st
, 3
rd
p.) 12.117 Neg.Aff. BARA- (1
st
, 3
rd
p.) 12.11.8
Aff. 2 NA
II
- (1
st
, 3
rd
p.) 12.11.10 Prohib. NA
I
- (2
nd
, 3
rd
p.) 12.11.9
NA
II
- 12.13.5 NGA
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 113
+nr yrnn 113
Table (cont.)
Aff. 3 ”I- (1
st
, 3
rd
p.) 12.11.11
”I- 12.13.6 NGA
Frustr. NU”- 12.11.12
NU” 12.13.6 INGA
Prosp. Ù 12.12.1
Note to the chart: NA
I
and NA
II
are, very probably, of the same origin, but have
diverged, NA
I
referring to future, NA
II
to past events. NA
II
as an affirmative may
go back to a negative rhetorical question (see 12.11.9). For allomorphs see below.
The table “Construction of Finite Forms” in Thomsen 1984, 139 is
an over-simplified presentation of the “Modal prefixes”.
12.11. Mor\r ixric\+ons
12.11.1. Indicative
Simple statements, in the “indicative mood”, are not specially marked.
12.11.2. Negative indicative
Its mark is [nu] (for allomorphs see below). [nu] is used indepen­
dent of conjugation pattern, and it may also be prefixed to non-
finite verbal forms such as participles or infinitives.
“à-bi nu-zu “I did not understand its (heart =) meaning” Gudea
Cyl. A i 28.
“à-ga-ni nu-mu-zu “I did not understand what he meant with
respect to me” Gudea Cyl. A iv 21.
From NS onward, [nu] has the allomorph [la] before the dimen­
!
gu
10
la-ba-ta-è “nobody escaped sional indicator [ba] (3): á (DA)-bad-∞
my outstretched arm” Gudea Cyl. A iv 26.
g-∞ ‘Archaizing’ nu-ba- instead of *la-ba- is found in alan-e . . . ki∞ gá
gá-∞ nu-ba-∞ gá “for this statue . . . nobody was supposed to use (sil­
ver . . .) as a working material” Gudea Stat. B vii 53. nu-ba- may
have been conditioned by the fourfold occurrence of suffixed -nu in
preceding lines 50–52.
In OB, the allomorph [la] before [ba] was extended to [li] before
[bi]: za-e “u-∞gar-lugal-zu li-bí-in-gi
4
“you(!) have not avenged your
king” Ali Letters B 5:7, as compared to earlier ér nu-bí(-n)-dug
4
“she
uttered no laments thereon” Gudea Stat. B v 4.
It is an open question whether allomorph [la] has been influenced
by the existence of Akkadian là “not”. There is a parallel, equally
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 114
114 cn\r+rn +vrryr
unexplained, in the relation between Sumerian nu-banda “foreman”
and the Akkadian loanword laputtûm < *la-pant-.
[nu] may be spelled nu-ù-, probably when followed by [i]: nu-ù-
me-en “I am not” NG no. 32:3.
[nu] is not used if a modal form is to be negated, as with veti­
tive (12.11.6) or prohibitive (12.11.9).
Note: Similarly Babylonian Akkadian would not use ul except for a negated
indicative in a main clause; là is used in a dependent clause or in prohibitive,
ay, è in vetitive.
[nu] is used, however, in negative infinitive constructions even where
they may have a modal connotation, e.g., final “in order not to . . .”,
nu-B-(e)de/-(e)da (see 12.14.3).
12.11.2.1. Suffixed -nu
[nu] occurs suffixed to a noun as the negation of the copula [am]
(see 12.7.1.2):
gu
10
-nu dumu-∞ gu
10
-nu im-me “he exclaims ‘my dam-∞ gu
10
-nu é-∞
wife/my child/my house is no more’ ” LamSumUr 95–97.
Note: Michalowski 1989, 43 understood the passage as “he says not, Oh, my
wife!”, assuming a verbal form nu-im-me.
Nin-nu-nam-“ita
x
“(There) being no Lady, (would there be) a prayer?”
(PN) DP 113 iv 4; 114 iv 3; HSS 3, 17 iii 10; 23 ix 26.
munus-bi a-ba me-a-nu a-ba me-a-ni “that woman: who being
(was she) not, who being (was) she?” = “that woman, whoever was
she?” Gudea Cyl. A iv 23.
Note: me-a-ni is an example of the “pronominal conjugation” (see 12.14.4).
ù kù-nu za-gìn nu-ga-àm ù erida-nu nagga-nu sipar-nu “it is not sil­
ver nor is it lapislazuli, and it is neither copper nor tin nor bronze”
Gudea Stat. B vii 50–52.
Note: nu-ga-àm [nu-(i)nga-am] contains the connecting indicator [inga] for which
see 12.12.1.
OB grammatical and lexical texts show suffixed -nu with pronouns,
e.g., me-en-dè-nu [menden-nu] : ul nìnu “it is not us” MSL 4, 51:419
(OBGT); a-ne-da-nu [ane-da-nu] : balu““u “it is (not with =) without
him” MSL 13, 86: E 31 (Proto-Kagal).
In practice, suffixed -nu has often become a substitute for the syn­
tagma X-da nu-me-a “(not being with =) without X” (see 5.4.2.6).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 115
  115
12.11.2.2. [nu] as a separate verbal base
dub-sar “u nu-a nar míli (KA × LI) nu-a “a scribe having no hand
(is like) a cantor having no voice” SP II 43.
u
ud
5
má“ nu-a “a goat having no kid” TrDr. 26:9.
8
sila
4
nú-a “a ewe having no lamb” TCL 2, 5621:1.
Note: The lexical passage u
8
sila
4
nú-a: “a pu¢àdsa nì[lu] “a ewe whose lamb is
resting” (see CAD L 42 la¢ru lex.) most probably has to be kept separate from
the Ur III reference (TrDr. 26:9) where -nu-a would not be an abbreviated
spelling for *-nú-a.
in-nu [i-nu] has become the expression for “No!” by OB.
nu may also be considered a base of its own in the phrase a-ba-
àm lugal a-ba-àm nu lugal “who is it (that was) king, who is it (that
was) not king?” = “whoever was king?” Sum. Kinglist vii 1.
For nu in OS Ean. 1 r. x 2 see 12.11.11.
12.11.3. Cohortative
The cohortative mood occurs only in the 1
st
sg. and pl. It is denoted
by prefixed ga-, with allomorphs (Ur III) ge
4
-(rí-) and gú-(mu-). The
cohortative is found in complementary distribution with the preca­
tive, which supplies the 2
nd
and 3
rd
persons, see 12.11.5.
ga- is essentially linked to verbal forms with the ¢am†u base and,
therefore, is not marked with a suffix (denoting person) after the base:
ga-tìl(
il
) “(let me live =) ex-voto object”.
nam gé
4
-rí-íb-tar
ar
etc. ”ulgi D 384 f. (*NS), see above 12.8.1.21.
Although combined with conjugation pattern 1 in the case of an
intransitive verb, there is no ending -en after the base. Apparently,
ga- alone was sufficient to mark the 1
st
sg. In the pl., however,
-enden is suffixed: ga-ba-húl-húl-le-en-dè-en [ga-ba-hulhul-enden] “let
us mightily rejoice over it” ZA 45 (1939) 119.
The (irregular) verb dug
4
/e “to speak, do” behaves in a special
way in OB: silim-ma ga-na-ab-bé-en [ga-na-b-e-(e)n] “let me say
‘hail’ to her” Iddin-Dagàn B 1–3, 6, etc. where we do not find the
expected ga-na-ab-dug
4
[ga-na-b-dug] as in Gudea Cyl. A iii 22 f.
!
e-ne-sù-ud ga-da-e [ga-eda-e-(e)n] “(let me play with you =) let
us have sex together” TMH NF 3, 25:20.
Note: Here the pl. base [e] may have been chosen because the intended act
implied two (active) participants.
See Attinger 1993, 222 f.; 476.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 116
116 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.11.4. Negated cohortative
Negative exhortation, “let me not . . .”, “I will certainly not . . .”, is
expressed with the bi-syllabic prefix [bara], a multi-functional mor­
pheme which is also found in the vetitive (12.11.6) and in the neg­
ative affirmative (12.11.8).
The negative cohortative is restricted by definition to the 1
st
per­
son and found with the marû verbal base and personal suffixes if the
form is transitive.
ki-sur-ra-
d
Nin-∞gír-su-ka-ke
4
ba-ra-mu-bala-e(n) “I swear I will not
transgress, as regards me, Nin∞girsu’s boundary” Ean. 1 xx 19.
di ba-ra-a-da-ab-bé-en
6
[bara-eda-b-e-(e)n] “I promise I will not
go to court (with =) against you” NG no. 28:8.
12.11.5. Precative
The precative mood is used with 2
nd
and 3
rd
person, “you may . . .”,
“he may . . .”, “let him/her . . .”, etc. It is found in complementary
distribution with the cohortative (12.11.3).
Precative is marked by the prefix HÉ-, graphically realized as hé­
or ha-, as well as (OB) hu- before mu-.
Precative forms are found with the marû form if the verb is tran­
sitive.
The distribution of [he] and [ha] is not entirely clear, but, accord­
ing to Attinger 1993, 292 f. § 191, hé- is “sans hésitation possible
la forme de base”, being found in a wider range of phonetic con­
texts than is [ha].
In Ur III letter-orders hé-na-ab-“úm-mu and ha-na-ab-“úm-mu
“let him/her give it to him/her” (Sollberger 1966, 169 f. s.v. sumu)
the relative frequency of the two forms is 125:15. As both hé-na-
and ha-na- occur at ª Girsu, local variation should be discounted.
Moreover, there are ‘mixed’ spellings HÉ-ab-, HÉ-an- (see below),
but none of a type *HA-eb-, *HA-en-.
Note: For another allograph, hi- (Ur III, ”ulgi hymns), see Attinger 1993, 293
with fn. 838, where also reference is made to Ebla, d’Agostino 1990, 128–34.
gìri-bi ha-ma-∞ gá “may (Nan“e) show me the way there” Gudea ∞ gá-∞
Cyl. A ii 19.
gi“kim-∞ ∞ gu
10
hé-sa
6
“let my sign be favourable” Gudea Cyl. A iii 18.
E
4
-nun-na bar-∞gu
10
-a “ùd hé-mi(-b)-sa
4
(-n)-za(-n) [he-mmi-b-sa-
(e)nzen] “O Enùna (pl.), may you say a blessing thereat on my
behalf ” Gudea Cyl. B ii 6.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 117
  117
kalam-e hé-∞gál-la “u HÉ.A-da-pe“-e [he-eda-b-pe“-e] “let the Land
(stretch out the hand =) gain in abundance (with you =) under your
rule” Gudea Cyl. A xi 9.
mu-ni . . . dub-ta hé-em-ta-∞gar “may his name . . . be (set =) taken
off the tablet” Gudea Stat. B ix 16.
hé-àm “let it be” became in effect an expression for “yes”, in con­
trast to in-nu “no” (cf. 12.11.2.2).
Note: See also Edzard 1971, 213 f. (note that in TCS 1 no. 82:4 there is 3
rd
, not
2
nd
person).
hé is used as an independent verbal base in the expression A
hé-a B hé-a (= lù A lù B), (“A WISH-ing, B WISH-ing =) be it A,
be it B”, e.g., in blessing or curse formulae, en hé-a lugal hé-a
“whether he be a lord or a king”.
12.11.6. The vetitive (negative precative)
It is used with the 2
nd
and 3
rd
person, “you/he should not”, “please
do not”, etc. It is found in complementary distribution with the neg­
ative cohortative (1
st
person, see 12.11.4).
Like the negative cohortative, vetitive is marked by the prefixed
particle [bara]. It is found with the marû verbal base if the verbal
form is transitive.
mí-ús-sá-zu mí-ús-sá-∞gu
10
ba-ra-me “the son-in-law you (had in mind)
should certainly not be(come) my son-in-law” NG no. 18:24 (oath).
Note: Since the copula is intransitive and has no opposed ¢am†u : marû forms, it
is difficult to decide whether this is a vetitive or a negative affirmative “will cer­
tainly not . . .”; cf. 12.11.8.
12.11.7. Affirmative 1
Like the precative (see 12.11.5) it is marked by the prefixed parti­
cle [he] and its allomorphs. As against precative (occurring with 2
nd
and 3
rd
persons), affirmative 1 occurs in the 1
st
and 3
rd
persons. It
refers to something in the past and, therefore, is found with conju­
gation pattern 2b (see 12.7.3) and the ¢am†u verbal base if the ver­
bal form is transitive.
Edzard 1971, 213–25.
“Affirmative 1” is a catch-all term. The main function of this mood
is to remove doubt, on the side of the listener, about what is being said.
Note: Cf. “honest!” in colloquial English, which serves to remove doubt.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 118
118 cn\r+rn +vrryr
Affirmative 1 is extremely popular in royal statements about what
the ruler did, achieved, performed. It might then be rendered by a
pluralis maiestatis, “we . . .”, instead of “I verily . . .”.
Note: In the royal hymn ”ulgi A nearly all statements of the king’s activities
made in the 1
st
sg., are preceded by hé-, with the exception of subordinate clauses
and of lines where the copula -me-en “I am” is used.
Affirmative 1 does not occur with the 2
nd
person.
l2.11.8. Negative affirmative
The negative affirmative is marked by the prefixed particle [bara]
which also serves the negative cohortative (12.11.4) and the vetitive
(12.11.6). It occurs in the 1
st
and 3
rd
person. As it refers to some­
thing in the past, it is found with the ¢am†u verbal base and with
conjugation pattern 2b (see 12.7.3) if the verbal form is transitive.
Edzard 1971, 218 f.
As with positive affirmative 1 (12.11.7), the main function of nega­
tive affirmative is to rule out doubt, on the side of the listener, about
what is being said.
nu-mu-un-su lú-á-tuku-ra ba-ra-na-an-∞gar “he did not—do not
doubt it—(set =) pledge (as a security) a widow to a wealthy per­
son” Ur-Namma Code 115.
ní ba-ra-ba-da-te su ba-ra-ba-da-zi “Yeah! I (the king) was not afraid
of it, did not let (my) hair stand on end because of it” ”ulgi A 70.
12.11.9. Prohibitive
The prohibitive mood is prefixed by the particle [na] graphically
(and phonetically?) identical to the affirmative 2 particle [na]. Directed
at a 2
nd
person it expresses prohibition, and as such it is the negation
of the imperative. With a 3
rd
person form it equally implies strict
interdiction. As against the negative cohortative [bara] (see 12.11.4)
it does not occur with the 1
st
person (nor does the vetitive, 12.11.6).
túg-tán-tán-na na-an-mu
4
-mu
4
-un [na-ni-mumu-(e)n] “do not wear
a clean garment there” Gilg., Enkidu and the Nether World 185.
ì-du
10
-ga bur-ra na-an-“e
22
-“e
22
-en “do not anoint (yourself ) there
with sweet oil from a jar” ibid. 187.
Edzard 1971, 219 f.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 119
  119
We summarize the negative moods:
(1) negative cohortative [bara] (1
st
person, 12.11.4): strict instruc­
tion not to act or to repeat an action.
(2) vetitive [bara] (2
nd
, 3
rd
persons, 12.11.6): interdiction, or rec­
ommendation not to act.
(3) negative affirmative [bara] (1
st
, 3
rd
persons, 12.11.8): assertion
that something has not occurred, or that someone has not done
something.
(4) prohibitive [na] (2
nd
, 3
rd
persons, 12.11.9): negative imperative;
interdiction (theoretically).
See also table on p. 112 f.
12.11.10. Affirmative 2
Affirmative 2, prefixed by particle [na], has been amply treated by
Falkenstein 1942, 181–223. It serves to draw attention to the impor­
tance of something that was there or happened, but is still mean­
ingful for what is to come. For that reason, it is usually found with
the ¢am†u base and conjugation pattern 2b (see 12.7.3), if the ver­
bal form is transitive. It is often found at the beginning of tales:
∞gé“tu-ga-ni na-an-gub [na-n-gub] “You should know that (Inana)
(set her ear =) decided to move (from the great Heaven to the great
Earth)”. Inana’s Descent 1.
é ur
5
-gin
7
dím-ma énsi-deli-e
d
Nin-∞ rú na-mu(-n)- gír-su-ra nu-na(-n)-§
rú “a house built that way no single ensi had (ever) built for Nin∞ § girsu;
(now, however) he (= Gudea) did build it” Gudea Stat. B vii 4.
Edzard 1971, 220–21.
Affirmative na- can rarely be confused with prohibitive na- (12.11.9)
because the latter uses the marû base and conjugation pattern 1 (see
12.7.2) in the case of transitive verbal forms.
For affirmative 2 na- and prohibitive na-—instead of chance
homophony—a common origin is plausible. Affirmative 2 na- may
go back to a rhetorical negative question: “has it not . . . by all
means?” turning into “it definitely has . . .”.
Note: Phonetic assonance or even identity of elements denoting negation, ques­
tion (and affirmation) are found in diverse languages: Latin ne and enclitic ques­
tion marker -ne; Turkish -mV- (negation) and -mV (question), e.g., bil-mi-yorum
“I do not know”, evin-mi “your house?”; Semitic là “not” and affirmative and/or
optative elements containing la (see D. Testen, JSS 38 [1993] 1–13); cf. also in
colloquial English the negative tag question “. . . isn’t it?” meaning “it is really . . .”.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 120
120 cn\r+rn +vrryr
hé-àm “it is really so” (= “yes”, see 12.11.5 end and 15.8) has a
counterpart in na-nam “it definitely was, and still is, so”. It is a
pleonastic formation of *na-àm [na-am] turned into [na-na-(a)m] and
could be extended still further to (ur
5
) hé-na-nam-ma(-àm).
Note: Pleonastic extensions frequently occur in languages; cf., e.g., French le
lendemain “the next day”, starting out from *main (< mane), then extended as
demain “tomorrow”, en-demain, l’endemain.
For the UD.GAL.NUN correspondence ”A = NA see below, 12.11.11.
12.11.11. Affirmative 3
Affirmative 3 “a-, “i-, with allomorph (OB) “u- before mu-, is found
only in literary contexts, and it is unknown whether it also occurred
in colloquial Sumerian.
Beside the extensive treatment of the morpheme by Falkenstein
1944, 73–118, cf. the note of M. Civil apud Heimpel 1974, 44 and
48 fn. 25; Edzard 1971, 222; Attinger 1993, 294 f. (with more lit.).
The main function of [“a] is to reconfirm something that already
had been stated or had occurred (Civil 1974: “Main event precedes”),
as against affirmative 2 [na] which stresses the importance of some­
thing in the past, still meaningful for the future.
na-§rú-a mu-bi lú-a nu(-àm) mu-bi “i-e “the name of the statue—
it is not a person’s (name)—its name (says =) reads (as we already
know): ‘. . .’” Ean. 1 r. x 1–3.
di-ku
5
ka-a“ bar-re-da [z]a-a-da “a-mu-e-da-∞ gál “Pronouncing deci­
sions rests (as it always has) with you (= Nergal)” SRT 12.:20, see
Römer 1965, 91.
Note: A. Ganter (Zgoll) contrasted [“a] and [na]: ““a- weist mit Nachdruck—
affirmativ—auf etwas schon Vorliegendes, Vorauszusetzendes zurück; im Gegensatz
zu na-, das primär auf etwas Kommendes, zu Erwartendes verweist” (Sumerian
Grammar Discussion Group, Oxford 1993; see Zgoll 2003).
12.11.12. Frustrative
The prefixed particle nu-u“- (var. nu-ú“- and nu-“V-) expresses a
hypothetical wish: something should be or have been, occur or have
occurred. The term “frustrative” was proposed by Jacobsen 1965,
74 (= 1970, 249). [nu“] was thoroughly analysed by Römer 1976,
371–78. See Attinger 1993, 297 with more lit.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 121
+nr yrnn 121
[nu“] is so far only found in literary and lexical contexts. Ebla
NU.U” = lu-wu-um, ( l)a-wu-um represents, in its Akkadian column,
precative [luw] and hypothetical [law] in substantivized form. Cf.
Krebernik 1983, 23 f. n. 78; 45:1439, as well as apud Edzard 1984,
115 n. 1.
!
sipa-∞gu
10
hi-li-a-ni nu-u“-ma (-da)-an-ku
4
-ku
4
[nu“-ma/mada-n(i)-
kuku] ∞gá-e ba-ra-ku
4
-ku
4
-dè-en “Would that my shepherd (= the dead
Ur-Namma) (could still) bring in unto me (var. with me) his beau­
tiful features; as for me (= lamenting Inana), I will certainly not
enter (there again)” Ur-Namma’s Death 213.

gu
10
é-na∞ gál-la-àm = ùma pukku u
4
-ba
ge“
ellag-∞ gar-ra-ka nu-u“-ma-da-∞
ina bìt naggàri lù ezib “If only on that day my hoop(?) had stayed
with me in the carpenter’s house” = “had the hoop(?) but stayed
on that day in the carpenter’s house” Gilgame“, Enkidu and the
Netherworld 172 (= EG XII 1).
[nu“] may perhaps be segmentized into negative [nu] + [”], but the
nature of the second element would remain quite uncertain. Civil 1983,
51, proposed *nu-“è-; but *nu-“i-, i.e., a—rhetoric?—negation of affir­
mative 3, cannot be ruled out, nor can some other unknown origin.
For rare spelling variants nu-ú“-, né-e“-, and ni-i“- see Attinger
1993, 297.
12.12. Coxxrc+ixo ixric\+ons
12.12.1. Prospective
Prospective [U] has two main functions: (1) it serves to indicate that
the verbal idea expressed by form A precedes (in time or argument)
the verbal idea expressed by a subsequent form B, e.g., “after I had
done A, I did B”. A syntagma [U]- . . . -A, . . . -B sometimes comes
close to a conditional clause, “if A, then B”.
(2) [U] expresses a polite imperative: “after you did A (I would
be grateful)” = “would you please do A”.
Note: Cf. English “if you would (kindly) close the window” which equals “close
the window, please”.
[U] is written ù- (also u- in post-OB or in Emesal); a- before ba-
(a-ba-), and ì- before bí- (ì-bí-).
The prospective cannot, most probably, be negated: *“after I did
not see him, I went home”; at least, no examples are known.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 122
122 cn\r+rn +vrryr
Attinger 1993, 295 f. with lit., esp. Heimpe1 1974, 229–233 with
over 70 strings of prefixed particles starting with [U].
12.12.1. A precedes B
kalam-e zi-“à-∞gál ù-ma-“úm [u-mma-n-“um], lú-deli lú-min-da ki∞g
mu-da-ak-ke
4
“after he (= the Northwind) has given life to our Land,
one single person will (do work with a second person =) work as
much as two” Gudea Cyl. A xi 24–25.
gá-∞ 2 “e gur a-“à ù-gíd, má“-a-“à-ga a-ba-ra-zi, úgu-ba má“ ì-íb-∞ gá
“(when =) if he (= the tenant) (only) tilled (the amount of ) 2 kor of
barley; when the interest of the field will have been raised he (= the
lessor) will set his rent/interest thereon” TCL 5, 6170:13–15 (NG
no. 144, and see Wilcke 1988, 10 with fn. 39).
12.12.2. Polite imperative
There is a universal tendency to moderate the harsh form of a direct
imperative: “pass me the salt”—“would you please pass me the salt”.
In OS, the author of a letter addresses the scribe by dug
4
-ga-na
(-b) “tell it to him” in order to convey the message to the addressee,
e.g., Michalowski 1995, 11 no. 1:6 [dug
4
]-ga-¢naÜ (= CIRPL Enz. 1).
Later on this quite direct formula was generally replaced by a ver­
bal form using the prospective: ù-na-dug
4
[u-na-e-dug] “after you
said to him/her” = “would you please tell him/her”. The formula
must often have been pronounced [unêdug] in order to account for
the Akkadian loanword form unetukku (see above 3.1.2).
A typical example of polite imperatives are Nin∞girsu’s instructions
given to Gudea to tell him how he should construct his (= Nin∞girsu’s)
chariot and accessories:
ge“ ù-ma-ta-∞ gar] . . . kì“ib ù-mi-kúr [u-mbi-e-kur] ∞ gar [u-mba-(b)ta-e-∞
∞ge“
gigir ù-mu-silim [u-mu-e-silim] ANSE.DUN.ÙR ù-“i-lá [u-b“i-e-
laH] . . . “u ù-ma-ni-tag [u-mba-ni-e-tag] “would you please break
the seal (on your storehouse), lay out the wood from there, . . . fit
together a chariot, harness to it a donkey stallion . . ., decorate (that
chariot)” Gudea Cyl. A vi 15–19.
An Ur III (or earlier) collection of medical prescriptions nearly
twenty times offers prospective verbal forms which should be con­
sidered as suggestions, i.e., polite imperatives: Civil 1960, 61:54
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 123
  123
ù-dé [u-e-de] “would you pour”, ibid. 58 ù-gaz “would you crush”,
and see 66, 80, and passim.
A frozen form ù-/u-me-ni-B “would you please . . .” is found in
post-OB incantations, in prescriptions to perform a ritual, e.g., e
4
. . .

ù-me-ni-dé,
ge“
“inig . . . ù-me-ni-“ub “would you pour water (in a jug),
throw a (twig of ) tamarisk into it” Falkenstein 1931, 90:28 f.
12.12.2. Connecting indicator [inga]
The preposed particle [inga] occurs in its full form, inga-, in verb
initial position (see 12.12.2.1) as well as after [nu“] (see 12.12.2.7);
it is -Vn-ga- [nga] after a preceding vowel (see 12.12.2.2–6).
The question arises whether [inga] might be segmentized into [i]
+ [nga]. In such a case we would have to define [i] (see 12.9) as
the modal indicator for the indicative (cf. table on p. 112 f.), instead
of giving it the value [ø].
Thomsen 1984, 169–72, only notes an element [ga] instead of
[inga]. In her theory, [i], preceding [ga], is nasalized, so that [inga]
would represent *[˜ı-ga]. See above, 12.9, for arguments militating
against the existence of a nasalized element *[Û].
As a connecting indicator, [inga] stands out by its ability to com­
bine with at least five positive modal particles (GA-, HÉ-, NA
II
-,
”A-. NU”-) as well as with negative NU-.
The functions of [inga] have been summed up by Attinger 1993,
297 f., as indicating “et alors”, “et par conséquence”, “(et) aussi . . . que”,
“et de plus”, “(et) de nouveau”, “non seulement . . . mais encore”.
See 12.12.2.1 ff. [inga], as a rule, only occurs with a second (or
third . . .) verbal form in a series, and it is only found in first posi­
tion when it is followed by a second verbal form with [inga].
[inga] was given much attention in the grammatical lists, both
OBGT and NBGT. Cf., e.g., in-ga-me-en-dè-en, in-ga-me-en-da-
nam—nìnuma “it is we; we too” OBGT I 410 f. (MSL 4, 51), also
lines 412–418 where, each time, [inga] is rendered by suffixed -ma
in Akkadian.
The NBGT 205–208 (MSL 4, 137) list un-ga, an-ga, in-ga, en-
ga (in the well-known u-a-i(-e) sequence) = ù “and”, preceded by
lines 202–204 ù, bi, bi-da = ù “and”. un-ga etc. were treated sep­
arately from such strings as nu-un-ga-; na-na-ga-, ga-an-ga-; in-ga-,
“i-in-ga-; hé-en-ga-—an example of the efforts of cuneiform scribes
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 124
124 cn\r+rn +vrryr
to define grammatical elements whose structure cannot be rendered
with syllabic signs.
See also MSL 4, 199:21; 145:395–398; 150:37–40; 163:12–15 (new:
MSL 5, 198).
l2.12.2.1. in-ga-
Laga“
ki
gaba-bi “u e-ma-ús . . . gaba-bi “u e-ga-ma-ús [inga-mma-n-us]
“(Umma) (came close to the breast of =) defied Laga“, . . . it (also =)
once more defied it” Ean. 1 ii 27–iii 22.
gal mu-zu gal ì-ga-túm-mu “(Gudea) is wise (and) (also brings forth
great (things) =) able, too, to realize things” Gudea Cyl. A vii 10.
eri
ki
in-ga-àm eri
ki
in-ga-àm “à-bi a-ba mu-zu “it is (such) a city,
it is (such) a city, (who knows its heart =) that no one can know its
heart” Ke“ Hymn 59.
lugal-∞ gu
10
za-gin
7
a-ba an-ga-kal a-ba an-ga-a-da-sá “my lord, who
is as powerful as you are, who would also (= [anga]) be able to
compete with you?” ”ulgi D (MBI 3 i 23–25).
This passage may be interpreted in two ways: (1) a-ba an-ga- is
a contraction of a-ba (i)n-ga-. (2) (i)nga is preceded by indicator [a(l)]
(see 12.10).
a-a-∞ gu
10
a-a-zu-gin
7
in-ga-dím “my father is (made =) as good as
your father” Sefati 1998, 195:11.
12.12.2.2. [nu-nga]
a-na gur-si-sá-ta, àm-á∞ gá], ù ∞ g [nu- g[-∞ gá-[e], (1) gur ki-su
7
-ta, nu-ga-á∞
nga-V-a∞g] “what(ever) may have been measured out from the reg­
ular kor, as for me, I did not measure out (one) kor from the threshing
floor” BIN 8, 156:7–11 (letter, unclear context).
. . . me-zu mah-àm . . .,
d
Nan“e me-zu me-na-me nu-un-ga-an-da-
sá “your ordinances are the greatest . . ., and, O Nan“e, any other
ordinances could certainly not rival with you(rs)” Nan“e Hymn 251.
For Gudea Stat. B vii 50 nu-ga-àm “nor is it” see 12.11.2.1.
[. . .] mu-tar, Ur-
d
Nin-∞gír-su, ù nam-érim, nu-ga-ma-tar “[. . .] has
sworn, as for U., he did not swear an (assertive) oath in my favour”
ITT II 5758 r. 1'–4'.
12.12.2.3. [ga-nga]
gan ga-ni-re
7
-en-dè-en gan ga-an-ga-àm-gi(!)-dè-en [ga-nga-mm(i)-
gigi(!)-(n)den] “come, let’s go there, come, let’s also return here” The
Three Ox Drivers 13.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 125
  125
12.12.2.4. [he-nga]
é ki-hur-sa
6
-ga-ù∞ g-∞ g-bi-ta íb-ba su- g-sa∞ gi
6
-ga ì-me-en-na-ke
4
-e“, ì-ze-è∞
mu-ug-ga hé-en-ga-mu-e-da-TAB.TAB-e-e“ “although, O House, you
used to be the place of joy for the Black-headed ones, instead of its
festivals (emesal for ezem) they (doubled with you =) gave in return
to you both wrath and disaster” Lament of Ur 116–117.
12.12.2.5. [na-nga]
en-e ní∞g-du
7
-e pa na-an-ga-àm-mi-in-è (var. naM-ga-), . . ., an ki-ta
baD-rˆe
6
-dè sa∞g na-an-ga(-àm)-ma-an-“úm “the Lord, you should know
(na-), made appear here what is due, in bright fashion, . . . and, more­
over, he in fact gave heed to separate heaven from earth” Creation
of the Pickaxe 1 . . . 5.
Note: The actions described in lines 1 and 5 are connected by (i)nga . . . (i)nga,
lit. “both . . . and . . .”. na- in line 1 is the affirmative na
II
- frequently found at
the beginning of tales (see 12.11.10), and it is repeated in line 5, most probably
because of rhyme.
en-na W. ù N. na-an-ga-ti-la-a“ igi-ni-ne-“è ì-gub-bu(-d) as long as
either W. or N. will actually be alive, he (= the adopted person) will
(stand before them =) be at their service” ARN 7:8–11.
É-an-na-túm-me gal na-ga-mu-zu “Eanatum in fact also knows
this: . . .” Ean. 1 xxi 12–13, r. i 31–32, etc. (the phrase each time
introduces the magic ritual performed with pigeons).
ki “à-∞gu
10
na-an-ga-ma-ab-bé-a “(I will loosen my sandals) at
whichever place my heart tells me” Lugalbanda I 178 (this is the
climax after a series of cohortative verbal forms introduced by ga-).
Mu-ni-na-ga-me “Whichever be his/her (= the divinity’s) name”
Old Sum. PN, DP 95:4, etc.; see Struve 1984, 124.
na-an-gaba(!)-ti-[l]a-da [na-nga-ba-til-ed-a] “as long as he lives”
NRVN 1, 236:4.
[nanga] occurs in a frozen form in u
4
na-an-ga-ma [na-nga(-i)-
me-a?] “whichever day it actually was” in the sense of “formerly”
(see Wilcke 1969, 159 ad line 74).
Note: For the ‘inverted’ sequence inga-na- see below, 12.12.2.8.
12.12.2.6. [ “i-nga]
gé“tu-diri . . . ki-tu“-a-ni-ta “à-di∞ u
4
-ba ∞ gir-re-e-ke
4
“i-in-ga-zu-a . . . á im-
ma-an-á∞g “then the very wise one . . . who even from his residence
finds out about the (heart =) intentions of the gods . . . gave instruc­
tion” Inanna and Enki SLTNi. 32:9–10 (Farber-Flügge 1973, 18).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 126
126 cn\r+rn +vrryr
gu
10
ama-∞ nin
9
-∞ gu
10
“i-ga-mi-in-na “my sister who, as we know (“i-),
are also my mother” VS 2, 27 v 7.
. . . me-e “i-in-ga-m[èn-na], . . . me-e “i-in-ga-mèn-[na] “I who, as
one knows, am . . ., and who, as one knows, also am . . .” SBH 56:18 f.
and 22 f. (see Falkenstein 1944, 92).
Kè“
ki
-gin
7
rib-ba lú “i-in-ga(-an)-túm-mu, . . . ama “i-in-ga-an-ù-dú
“will someone bring forth somebody who would be as overwhelm­
ing as Ke“, or will a mother bear somebody (who would be as over­
whelming as A“gi, the warrior (of Ke“))?” Ke“ Hymn 18–19 (as well
as at the end of each ‘House’ section).
12.12.2.7. [nu“-inga]
lugal-∞gu
10
ní-hu“-rib-ba-za, nu-u“-in-ga-zu-àm, . . . ní-mah-a-za, nu-u“-
in-ga-¢zu-àmÜ “My lord, would that your overwhelming fierce awe
were known, . . . that your unsurpassed awe were known” Ninurta G
174–177 (and see 179, 181, 183), M. E. Cohen 1975, 29.
Note: The fivefold repetition of [inga] might be rendered by “firstly”, “sec­
ondly”, etc.
nu-u“-in-ga- is spelled with ‘hiatus’ above, but note Ur Lamentation
101 nu-“i-in-ga- with var. nu-u“-in-ga- in UET 6/2, 136:96.
12.12.2.8. ‘Irregular’ [inga-na]
Attinger 1993, 297: “Que [(i)nga] n’est ni un préf. I [“préformatif ”]
ni un préf. II [“préfixe de conjugaison”], mais une sorte de “prédi­
cat conjonctif ” (§ 90.e [p. 149]), ressort clairement du fait qu’il peut
être aussi bien précédé que suivi du préf. I assertif (na) (comp. in-
ga-na(m)-mu-na-be
2
(-en) et na-an/nam-ga-)”.
According to Attinger who rather associates [inga] with -bi, -bi-
da, ù “and” (p. 149 f.), [inga] should not find its fixed position in
table on p. 112 f. In fact, [inga] might be isolated as “also” in GilgHuw.
I 91 mìn-kam-ma-“è in-ga(-)nam-mu-na-ab-bé “a second time, still, he
definitely addresses him”. But in most of the other occurrences, [(i)nga]
appears inseparably embedded in the prefix string of the verb.
Note: Only a living informant would be able to say whether or not [inga-na-
mu-na-b-e] and *[na-nga-mu-na-b-e] were freely exchangeable.
At any rate, Attinger’s position will have to be submitted to further
research, in a comprehensive study of the particle [inga].
For bibl., see Attinger 1993, 297.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 127
+nr yrnn 127
12.12.2.9. Summary of [inga].
After Attinger’s definition (1993, 297; see above 12.12.2), the main
functions of [inga] may be tentatively summed up as follows:
1. Simple [inga] denotes “also”, “too”, with an antecedent not
provided by [inga]; [inga] in consecutive verbal forms means
“both . . . and . . .”.
2. X Y-gin
7
[inga] means “X equals Y (nominal antecedent)”.
3. [nu-nga] “not even”.
4. [he-nga] “also really/verily”, “instead of ”.
5. [na-nga] “also in fact”.
6. [“i-nga] “also, indeed, as we already know”.
7. [nu“-inga] “let/may also . . .” (rhetorical question).
12.12a. [ini]
A verb-connected element [iri], spelled i-ri, i-rí, iri, is so far attested
only before [(i)nga]. We have not entered it in table on p. 112 f.,
because we are at a loss to define its function. See, for discussion
and bibl., Attinger 1993, 296 f. (“Ni la fonction ni même la caté­
gorie grammaticale de {iri} ne peuvent être déterminées”.)
12.13. Ivrrn\+iyr
The Sumerian imperative is regularly formed with the verbal ¢am†u
(alternatively the reduplicated ¢am†u) base without any prefixed par­
ticles. There are basically two types: 1) the unextended imperative
consisting of the base and an additional vowel whose quality is unpre­
dictable to us, i.e., B-V; 2) the extended imperative; here, one or
more (up to four) particles, otherwise prefixed to the base, follow
either the base itself or the base+V.
The element V following the base has, since Poebel 1923, 278 f.
§§ 676–678, been interpreted as a reflex of the prefixed indicator
[e, i] (see 12.9); cf. Attinger 1993, 298. This explanation does not,
however, account for the vocalic diversity. Therefore, we prefer to
see V as a mark of imperative as such.
We are unable so far to predict the imperative of a given Sumerian
verb—as against many languages (Indo-European, Semitic, and others).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 128
128 cn\r+rn +vrryr
The plural of the Sumerian imperative is formed by the addition
of [nzen] after V or [zen] after C; correlation with 2
nd
pl. [(e)nzen]
is obvious.
In the extended imperative, as in the cohortative (see 12.11.3), the
absolutive ‘object’ of the 3
rd
sg. is noted by [n] (extremely rarely) or
[b]. This fact has been claimed as an argument for Sumerian being
a language with “split ergativity”; but see our alternative explana­
tion above in 12.7.4.
So far, no context examples are available for the 1
st
(sg. or pl.)
absolutive ‘object’, i.e., e.g., *“push me”, *“push us”. The 2
nd
(sg.
and pl.) may be safely excluded, because forms like *“put yourself
(in my position)” would be reflexive and require ní-zu(-ne-ne) “your­
self ”, “yourselves”.
The OBGT in their ∞gar = “akànum paradigm, offer two examples
for an imperative + “me”: ∞gar-mu-un = “uknanni “set me (here)”
MSL 4, 80 OBGT VI 46; [∞gar]-mu-ub = “u“kinanni “cause me to
be set (here)” ibid. VI 51. The first of the two forms may be seg­
mentized into [∞gar-mu-(e)n] with [(e)n] as the absolutive “me” of the
transitive conjugation pattern 2b (see 12.7.3). As for the second exam­
ple, we are at a loss to explain the relation existing between the
Sumerian and Akkadian forms.
Note: “Help me” which in our language would imply a 1
st
person absolutive
‘object’, occurs as dah-ma-ab, lit. “add it to me” in GilgHuw. A 110 za-e ∞ gá-e
dah-ma-ab “(as for) you, help m e ”. The construction is with the dative.
12.13.1. Unextended imperative (base +V)
gi
4
-a “go back”, “return” Gilg. Huw. A 117, 118.
ér “é“-a “shed tears” Inana’s Descent 42, 51, 59.
ge“
“udun gú-ba ∞

gar-ì “set the yoke on their neck” Ur-Ninurta B r.
gar-ì “put into her hands” Message of Ludi∞ 6; “u-ni-“è ∞ gira 7; but
gar-ra [∞ note ∞ gar-a] “ukun “set” OBGT VI 1 (MSL 4, 79).
gál-ù (var. ∞ é ∞ gál-lu) “open the house” Inana’s Descent 75 f.; Nanna’s
Journey 260–264, etc.
gál taka; either [∞ Note: The verb “to open” is ∞ gal-u] is an irregular imperative, or
the imperative still preserves a verbal form that was otherwise extended to ∞ gál taka.
ki-gub-za nú-ì “lie (down) at your resting place” Copper and Silver
33 (SRT 4:33).
ní-za ∞ gé“tu AG-ì “heed yourself ” Grain and Sheep” 162 (ASJ 9,
26).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 129
  129
∞gen-na “go” Inana’s Descent 70.
gá-nu (or ∞ ∞ ge
26
-nu?) “come on” Lugalbanda I 135.
Note: The two imperatives of ∞ gen (¢am†u)/du (marû) “to go” have been gener­
ally discussed by Wilcke 1969, 172 f., who also dissociated the exclamation [gana]
(see 15.7).
The plural of the unextended imperative is relatively rare; cf. e.g.,
gen-na-an-zé-en [∞ ∞ gen-a-(e)nzen] “go” Inana’s Descent 227.
gu
4
-ud-an-zé-en [gud-a-(e)n-zen] “jump, dance” SRT 5:42, 44.
12.13.2. Extended imperative
The extended imperative may contain such additional information
as ventive, dimensional indicators, or absolutive. As a rule, one to
three of these elements occur (the plural marker not counted), excep­
tionally as many as four (see 12.13.2.4).
12.13.2.1. One element
∞gìri kur-“è nú-ba-an-zé-en [nu-ba-nzen] “(lay down the feet =) slide
down (on your behinds) toward the Nether World” Inana’s Descent
227.
12.14.2.2. Two elements
má-a dab
5
-dab
5
-ba-ab [dabdab-ba-b] “take (all the cattle) on the boat”
SR 87:5.
dah-ma-ab [dah-ma-b] “add it to me” Gilg. Huw. A 110.
tuku-ba-an [tuku-ba-n] “marry her” NG no. 6:8.
g-mu-ub-zé-en [na∞
Note: ba- is here a frozen indicator, referring to implied nam-dam-“è “for marriage”.
a na∞ g-mu-b-zen] “give me water to drink” Schooldays
13.
zi-mu-ub-zé-en [zi-mu-b-zen] “wake me” Schooldays 16.
Note: [b] is difficult to explain here. Does it ‘rhyme’ with [b] in the preceding
imperatives?
ní te-ba-ab té“ tuku-ba-ab “be devoted, be reverent (in face of my
words)” Father and Son 101.
du
11
-ga-na(-b) [dug-a-na-b] “say it to him (there)” CIRPL N 12
iii 1.
“úm-me-eb “give it to us” Inanna’s Descent 245, 266, 269.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 130
130 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.13.2.3. Three elements
ur
4
-ur
4
-u
4
-mu-na-ab [ururu-mu-na-b] “collect it for him” SRT 12:60.
é-di∞gir-re-e-ne nigin-na-ma-ni-ib [nigin-a-ma-ni-b] “go around the
houses of the gods for my sake” Inana’s Descent 36.
∞gi“kim-a-ni [e-ne-ra] du
11
-mu-na-ab [dug-mu-na-b] “give [him] a
sign (or: an identification) of his” ELA 496.
12.13.2.4. Four elements
¢sá du
11
Ü-ga-àm-mu-¢na-ni-íbÜ [dug-a-mu-na-ni-b] = ¢“utak“i Üda““um
“cause him
1
to be brought in close contact with him
2
” OBGT IX
52 (MSL 4, 106).
[sá du
11
]-¢ga-ba-naÜ-ni-íb [dug-a-ba-na-ni-b] = “utak ¢“idsumÜ “cause
him
1
to be brought close to him
2
” OBGT IX 40 (MSL 4, 105).
Note: Neither AHw. nor CAD K s.vv. ka“àdu (“utak“udu) has offered a convinc­
ing translation of these Akkadian imperatives. Both the Sumerian and the Akkadian
forms look like learned scribal efforts—as do some other forms in the sá-du
11
(-g)
= ka“àdu section of OBGT.
Attinger 1993, 298 f. with lit.
As in many languages, there is no negated imperative in Sumerian.
Instead, vetitive [bara] (12.11.8) or prohibitive [na
I
] (12.11.9) are used.
12.14. Nox-rixi+r yrnn\r ronvs
Non-finite verbal forms lack any of those prefixes or suffixes by which
state or action can be linked to a ‘subject’ of 1
st
, 2
nd
, or 3
rd
person,
sing. or pl. Cf. 12.7.1, 12.7.2, and 12.7.3 for the conjugation pat­
terns of the Sumerian verb.
There are three types (see 1–3) of non-finite verbal forms. Moreover,
there is a ‘hybrid’ type (see 4) where particles of possession (see 5.2)
occur.
In all except (1), the distribution of the ¢am†u or marû bases is
relevant.
(1) B-[Ø], B-B-[Ø].
(2) B-[a] and B-[e(d)], without or with copula.
(3) B-[ede], B-[eda], B-[ada].
(4) ‘Conjugated’ participle or ‘pronominal conjugation’.
It seems practical to call (2) participles, (3) infinitives, and (4) a con­
jugated participle.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 131
  131
12.14.1. B-[Ø], B-B-[Ø]: unextended bases
The unextended verbal base (single or reduplicated) occurs by itself
as an abstract lexical unit and as such it is the form regularly used
for entries in the lexical lists.
Note: In Innin “agura, the section
d
Inana za-a-kam “it is yours, O Inana” (Sjöberg
1975, 188 ff. lines 115 ff.) contains verbs cited both as B(-e)-dè and as unex­
tended forms, e.g., “ár-“ár (= “utàbulum) “to interchange” (p. 192: 140 f.). The
unextended bases are cited here in the manner of entries in a lexical list.
The unextended base may be joined to a preceding substantive (in
the absolutive case). In our translation, the substantive may be seen
as an object dependent on the verb.
dub-sar “tablet write” = “(who writes tablets =) scribe”.
kù-dím “silver form” = “(who forms silver =) silver-smith”.
The loanwords in Akkadian, tup“arru or kuttimmu, show that the
verb actually stands as the base by itself, without any further affix.
kù nu-zu(-ù)-ne “(people) not knowing silver”.
kur gul-gul “destroying the (foreign) countries”.
ní∞g-kur-du
11
-du
11
= ni
5
-in-kur-du-tu “constantly saying hostile things”,
“deriding” (= epè“ namûtim “derision”) Inin “agura 159.
ù∞ g-ge-en-ge-en “establishing the people”.
Note: Reduplicated B-B (base-base) has to be understood as the ‘free’ (non-marû)
reduplication of the base (cf. 12.5) which occurs in the ¢am†u variant (if there is
any). The clearest example is supplied by du
11
-du
11
from the ¢am†u base of the
verb du
11
(-g)/e/di.
For more examples see Edzard 1972, 5 f.:12 “(Der mah-di-Typus
und) der dub-sar-Typus”; 6.8 “Der kur-gul-gul-Typus (freie Redup­
likation)”.
In some compounds of the type noun—B, the noun cannot be
interpreted as an object depending on B:
ki-bala “place transgressing” = “rebellious land”.
Note: bala has neither the suffix -a nor -e(d). This becomes clear from the spelling
ki-bala-e sá-di “conquering the rebellious land” Gungunum 1:7. See Edzard 1972,
9 fn. 108.
sá-du
11
(-g) = sattukku “arrival” = “(regular) delivery” (cf. Krecher 1978,
388), with a pre-verbal element sá “be equal, comparable” (cf. Attinger
1993, 642 f.).
∞ge“-hur = gi“¢urru “wood carve” = “engraving, design, ground
plan”. This is not the dub-sar type, because ∞ge“ is not ‘governed’
by the verb hur.
ki-gub “place stand” = “position”, ki-tu“ “place sit” = “dwelling
place”, ki-nú “place lie down” = “bed”.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 132
132 cn\r+rn +vrryr
All these compounds appear to be ‘archaic’ formations which had
already ceased to be productive in Old Sumerian.
Edzard 1972, 8 f. “Der sá-du
11
(-g)-Typus”.
12.14.2. B-[a], B-[ed], without or with copula
In view of the function which they assume in Sumerian syntax,
B-[a] and B-[ed] may be called participles. B in B-[a] is regularly
represented by the ¢am†u base of the verb, B in B-[ed] by the marû
base. Both formations remained productive throughout.
Note: The “mah-di-Typus”, proposed by Edzard 1972, 2–5, and contrasted there
with the “dub-sar-Typus” (see above 12.14.1), is not a formation of its own, but
simply corresponds to B-[ed].
Whereas [a] undergoes no variation, [ed] has four allomorphs: (1)
[ed] after a consonant (including H, cf. 3.1.2 p. 19f.) and before a
vowel, e.g., nam tar-re-dè [tar-ed-e] “to make a binding promise”;
rú-dè [§ (2) [(e)d] after a vowel and before a vowel, e.g., § ru-(e)d-e] “to
build”; (3) [e(d)] after a consonant in final position, e.g., “úm-mu(-d)
[“um-e(d)] “someone who will give”; (4) [(e)(d)] ge
4
-ge
4
[gege(e)(d)]
“someone who will return”. Moreover, the [e] of [ed] may be assim­
ilated to a preceding (first) vowel in the same way as the [e] of the
verbal particles [en], [enden], [enzen], [e“], and the first [e] of [ene].
See above “úm-mu [“um-e(d)].
[ed] marking the marû participle is most probably identical with
the [ed] occurring in the extended conjugation patterns 1 and 2a
(see 12.7.1 and 12.7.2), e.g., ba-ra-ba-zah-e-dè-en [bara-ba-zah-ed-
en] “I swear I will not run away (again)”.
12.14.2.1. B-[a]; mes-Ane-pada construction
12.14.2.1.1. B-[a]
d
Mes-lam-ta-è-a [e-a] “the one having come out of Meslam (sanc­
tuary)” (name of a god). Compare eme è-è(-d) [ed-e(d)] “(snake) dart­
ing (its) tongue in and out” Gudea Cyl. A xxvi 25.
g-∞ g-a] “who has drunk beer” Lugalbanda B 24. ka“ na∞ gá [na∞
An-gin
7
dím-ma [dim-a] “made like An” (first line and title of
Return of Ninurta to Nibru).
kù lá-a-bi-im [laH-a-bi-(a)m] “(NN) was the one weighed the silver”
Forde Nebraska 63:13.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 133
  133
As shown above, the verb in the B-[a] participle may in principle
be either transitive or intransitive. However, seen statistically, exam­
ples of intransitive participles by far outweigh transitive ones.
Edzard 1972, 10–12; Thomsen 1984, 255 ff. passim.
12.14.2.1.2. The mes-Ane-pada construction
A special use of B-[a] is found in the syntagma labelled “mes-Ane-
pada” construction by Falkenstein 1949, 135, and 1950, 35 f. with
fn. 2. Mes-An-né-pà-da [mes an-e pad-a], name of an Early Dynastic
ruler of Ur, literally means “young male chosen by (the agency of )
An (ergative)”. It is a nominalized form of the phrase *An-e (erg.)
mes (absol.) . . . -n-pà(-d) “An chose the young male”.
Note: The closeness (and original identity?) of ergative and directive seems to
become clear from the syntactical ambiguity of the construction which we may
translate either “whom An chose” or “chosen (next to =) by An”. It should be
stressed, however, that the directive case in no other instance can be shown to
express the ‘ablative of agent’ (“done by someone”).
é-ninnû An-né ki ∞gar-ra “the Eninnû founded by An” Gudea Cyl.
A ix 11.
ninta-zi
d
Utu-ù [Utu-e] ní∞ g-si-sá sagˆ -e-e“ rig
7
-ga “the trustworthy
man whom the Sun god presented with righteousness” ”ulgi D 5.
dumu Eridu
ki
-ge dú-da “child born by Eridu” Gudea Cyl. A ii 16.
The full mes-Ane-pada construction may be reduced by the sup­
pression of one or two of its components:
1) Suppression of the absolutive: hur-sa∞ g-e dú-da “born by the
Mountain” Gudea frag. 8+3+5+4 iv' 3' (RIME 3/1, 103).
2) Suppression of the ergative: Lú-pà-da “person chosen (by . . .)”
Ur III PN, see Limet 1968, 486; see also pre-Sargonic Lú-pà (for
Lú-pad-a) in Struve 1984, 117.
gé“tu “úm-ma in ∞ 3) Suppression of absolutive and ergative: ∞ gé“tu-
“úm-ma-
d
En-ki-ka-ke
4
“the one gifted with (ear =) wisdom of Enki” =
“the one whom Enki gifted with wisdom” Ean. 2 ii 6–7. The originator
of the gift, Enki, is expressed in the genitive, and the whole complex
is in the ergative. The participial verbal element of the construction,
“úm-ma, is extended by the addition of an ‘object’ (∞ gé“tu) in the
absolutive case. ∞gé“tu-“úm-ma, therefore, is not “given wisdom”, but
someone gifted with wisdom although, formally, [∞ge“tu “um-a] is
g-∞ g-a] “beloved child”. identical with dumu-ki-á∞ gá [dumu kia∞
Edzard 1972, 12–14, where transformational rules are given.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 134
134 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.14.2.2. B-[ed]; B-[ed+copula]
12.14.2.2.1. B-[ed]
Nam-tar-re(-d) “(the god) is making a firm promise”, abbreviated PN,
SR 65 iii 4.
The same person occurs in the ergative in the same document:
Nam-tar-re-dè [namtar-ed-e] ibid. ii 3.
Note: In Yang Zhi 1989 no. A 661:3 an ergative Nam-tar-DU-e is noted (courtesy
W. Sallaberger). It is uncertain whether a secondary ergative formation of Namtare
is implied: Nam-tar-ré-e, or whether we have to do with a different name.
ma“kim-di(-d) “functioning as a inspector” Ukg. 4–5 vi 35.
gà-la nu-dag-ge(-d) “not dilatory” I“mè-Dagàn A 33.
12.14.2.2.2. B-[ed+copula]
d
En-líl húl-le(-d)-me-en “you are one who pleases Enlil” ”ulgi D 31.
ní∞g-nu-kúr-ru-dam “it is something that will not be altered” Ninurta
C iii 9.
kù má“ ∞ gá-dam [∞ ga-(e)d-am] “it is silver yielding interest” gá-∞ ga∞
NG no. 135:2.
B-[edam] not unfrequently has a modal connotation: “has to . . .”,
“is to . . .”, but it sometimes is a matter of subjective interpretation
how a given form should be classified.
èn-bi tar-re-dam “to be (asked =) re-examined”, final note in Ur
III accounts when the calculation made by the scribe still has to be
checked.
su-su-dam “to be replaced” UET 3, 37:6.
dah-he-dam “to be added” YOS 4, 18:10.
dam-A. ù M. ge-né-dam “A.’s wife and M. will (have to) prove
it” NG no. 214:42.
Note: [gen-ed-am] is in the sing. in spite of the pl. subject. There probably was no
form *ge-né(-d)-me-é“ *[gen-ed-me-(e)“]. Maybe usage of the conjunction ù “and”
caused the form to be understood in a distributive way: “A.’s wife and M. will
each (have to) prove it”. Or there is an anakoluthon: “A.’s wife and M.—(by
them) it is to be established” (suggestion of J. N. Postgate). Edzard 1967, 36–40.
12.14.3. B-[ede], B-[eda], B-[ada]
The Sumerian verb has no infinitive form which serves for citation,
corresponding to the Akkadian pattern paràsum. In lexical lists, verbs
are quoted with their base, simple or reduplicated (cf. above 12.14.1).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 135
  135
There are, however, at least in NS and OB, bases with suffixed
[ede], [eda], and [ada] which are rendered by Akkadian infinitive
constructions in (OB) bilingual texts, e.g.,
rú-ù-da sa∞ bàd-Zimbir
ki
§ g-bi íl-i-da = dùr Z. epè“am rè“ì“u ullâm “(he
definitely ordered me) to build the wall of Sippar (and) to raise its
head” RIME 4, 335 Hammu-ràpi 2:22–23 // 23–25.
The three Sumerian forms, B-[ede], B-[eda], and B-[ada] are
treated together here because of their similar syntactic behaviour,
although the proposed morphological analysis would not favour a
parallel arangement of the respective forms.
12.14.3.1. B-[ede]
B is always represented by the marû base of the verb. A morpho­
logical analysis B-ed-e is obvious. This would, formally, constitute a
marû participle B-[ed] with the addition of the directive particle [e].
sa
10
-sa
10
-dè [sasa-(e)d-e] “in order to buy” would, then, literally mean
“at being buying”, “at being a buyer”.
The reading of -NE as -dè is substantiated by spellings -te (e.g.,
Sjöberg 1975, 190:120) or -de
1
(e.g., NATN 1, 702:2).
lú-Umma
ki
-a . . .
a-“à
a“a
5
tùmu-dè an-ta bala-e-da . . .
d
En-líl-le hé-
ha-lam-me “may Enlil destroy (any) person of Umma . . . who would
cross over here from up there in order to (carry away =) seize fields”
Ent. 28–29 A vi 9–20.
l ma-na kù-luh-ha igi-nu-du
8
-a sa
10
-sa
10
-dè U. dam-gàr- . . . -ke
4
ba­
de
6
“U., the . . . merchant, took with him one mina of refined silver
in order to buy (blind men =) garden workers” Nik. 293 i 1–5.

10 ì-nun : sìla
ge“
gigir-e AG-dè “10 pounds of butter to (make =)
use for (lubricating) (at) the chariot (PN received)” BIN 8, 320:7–9.
d
E
4
-nun-na ù di-dè im-ma-“u
4
-“u
4
-ge-é“ “the Enùna stopped here
to gaze in awe” Gudea Cyl. A xx 23; see Cyl. B i 11 (var. u
6
).
u
4
“u-bala AG-dè ∞ ge“-hur ha-lam-e-dè . . . An
d
En-líl
d
En-ki
d
Nin-
mah-bi nam-bi ha-ba-an-tar-re-e“ “An, Enlil, Enki, and Ninmah have
definitely decreed . . . that they would overturn the (appointed) time,
forsake the (preordained) plans” Lament Sumer and Ur 1–55.
Note: There are in this passage 51 more B-[ede] forms which depend on nam-
bi ha-ba-an-tar-re-e“, and there is no B-[eda] variant.
In all these examples and others, the subject implied in B-[ede] and
the subject of the finite verb following B-[ede] are identical. Moreover,
the subjects implied are all in the ergative. Identity of subject of
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 136
136 cn\r+rn +vrryr
B-[ede] and following verb does not—as a rule—occur with B-[eda]
for which see 12.14.3.2.
Edzard 1967, 43; but see the reservations made by Attinger 1993, 307.
12.14.3.2. B-[eda]
As with B-[ede], B is always represented by the marû base of the verb.
In analogy to B-[ede], therefore, a morphological analysis B-ed-a is
obvious. B-[eda] would, formally, constitute a marû participle B-[ed]
with the addition of the locative particle [a].
é-a-ni §rú-da ma-an-du
11
“(Ningirsu) told me (to build =) I should
build his house” Gudea Cyl. A iv 20.
gidri-u
4
-sù-§ gá-∞ gír-su-ra me-ni- ∞ rá “u ∞ gá-da . . .
d
Ig-alim . . . en
d
Nin-∞
da mu-na-da-dib-e “(Gudea) brings Ig-alim along with himself (and
introduces him) to the lord Ningirsu . . ., that he (Ig-alim) hand over
a sceptre for long days” Gudea Cyl. B vi 16–23.
Note: Many parallel examples for B-[eda] in Gudea Cyl. B vi 11–x 17. They
were listed in Edzard 1972, 25–27; see Edzard 1997, 92–94, for a recent trans­
lation. In all the examples (numbering 25) the subject implied in B-[eda] and the
subject of the following verb are different, so that B-[eda] is clearly set off against
B-[ede] (see 12.14.3.1).
Edzard 1967, 43; Thomsen 1984, 266. However, as with B-[ede], note Attinger’s
reservations (1993, 307).
In OB Sumerian literary texts, different manuscripts are occasion­
ally inconsistent in their use of B-[ede], B-[eda], or even B-[edam],
so that the awareness of the distinction of the respective forms may
have been lost.
12.14.3.3. B-[ada]
Although B-[ada] occurs in contexts where it is clearly syntactically
paralleled with B-[eda] [B-ed-a], it cannot be analysed as *B-ad-a
because we know of no morpheme *[ad].
B in B-[ada] is always represented by the ¢am†u base of the verb.
B-[ada] forms are always intransitive or passive and never have an
ergative subject.
Since we explain B-[ede] and B-[eda] as marû participle con­
structions, it would only be logical to define B-[ada] as the ¢am†u
participle with the addition of a case particle. Here, then, comita­
tive [da] offers itself. Krecher 1978, 401 f. fn. 21, opened the way
to this explanation.
Note: The analysis of B-[ada] as B-a-da may challenge our analysis of B-[eda]
as [B-ed-a] (12.14.3.2), with locative [a], and lead to propose *[B-ed-da] instead
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 137
  137
of [B-ed-a]. While we cannot exclude such an analysis, the close contact between
B-[ede] and B-[eda] rather favours the interpretation given above.
gú-eden-na eden-du
10
-ge na-de
6
“úm-ma-da “that the Edge-of-the-
Steppe, the best part of it, be subject to inspection” Gudea Cyl. B
xii 8–9.
Contrast (a) eri-ni . . . na-de
6
“úm-mu-da [“um-ed-a] “that he might
inspect his city . . .” Gudea Cyl. B vi 14 with (b) eri § rú-a-da ki-tu“
∞gar-ra-da “that cities be built, settlements be founded” Gudea Cyl.
B xii 19.
Here, in (a) “úm-mu-da there is a (hidden) ergative subject whereas
rú-a-da and ∞ (b) § gar-ra-da have none.
Edzard 1972, 25–29, with many more examples for B-[ada], but still lacking a
convincing morphological analysis; Wilcke 1990, 496 with fn. 84.
12.14.4. Conjugated participle or “pronominal conjugation”
Falkenstein 1949, 149, introduced the term “pronominale Konjugation”
for participles which are ‘conjugated’ by the addition of particles ex­
pressing possession. The term has found wide acceptance in spite of
the argument of Jacobsen 1988, 130 f., who took the term to be a
misnomer, because “conjugation” should be restricted to the finite verb.
Note: F. Thureau-Dangin, RA 32 (1935) 108 f., to whom Falkenstein refers treats
none of the examples quoted below.
The forms in question consist of (1) a ¢am†u or marû participle,
B-[a] or B-[ed], plus (2) an additional [a] after the marû participle
plus (3) a particle of possession (cf. 5.2), and (4) in the 1
st
and 2
nd
persons, of an element -NE of unknown origin or function, with var.
spelling -ni (see 12.14.2 ff. and esp. 12.14.4.13) suggesting -ne rather
than -dè as had been assumed by most authors until recently.
The paradigm reads:
¢am†u
1
st
sg. ku
4
-ra-∞gu
10
-ne [kur-a-∞gu-ne] “when I entered”
2
nd
sg. ku
4
-ra-zu-ne [kur-a-zu-ne]
3
rd
sg. p. ku
4
-ra-ni [kur-a-(a)ni]
3
rd
sg. non-p. ku
4
-ra-bi [kur-a-bi]
1
st
pl. not attested
2
nd
pl. not attested
3
rd
pl. ku
4
-ra-ne-ne [kur-a-(a)nene]
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 138
138 cn\r+rn +vrryr
marû
1
st
sg. ku
4
-ku
4
-da-∞gu
10
-ne [kuku-(e)d-a-∞gu-ne]
2
nd
sg. ku
4
-ku
4
-da-zu-ne [kuku-(e)d-a-zu-ne]
3
rd
sg. p. ku
4
-ku
4
-da-ni [kuku-(e)d-a-(a)ni]
3
rd
sg. non-p. ku
4
-ku
4
-da-bi [kuku-(e)d-a-bi]
1
st
pl. not attested
2
nd
pl. not attested
3
rd
pl. p. not attested, but see 12.14.4.10
for du-ne-ne
In general, the forms with ¢am†u participle express the idea of
something having happened, occurred, and with marû participle of
something about to happen, occur, or in course of happening, occur-
ring—each time in anticipation of a following finite verbal form or
a noun with copula.
A much-quoted contrastive example is ku
4
-ra-ni “when she had
entered (her bedroom)” Gudea Cyl. B v 12 versus ku
4
-ku
4
-da-ni
“when (the warrior) was about to enter (or: was entering) (his house)”
ibid. v 4.
12.14.4.1. 1st sg. ¢am†u: nígin-na-∞ gu
10
-ne “when I had turned around
(heaven, turned around the earth)” Inana/Ebe¢ 25 (see 26 f.).
gen-na-∞ u
4
-da kur-“è ∞ gu
10
-ne “when today I will have gone away
to the Nether World” Inana’s Descent 33.
g-gin
7
KI-LUGAL.GUB-ta ní íl-la-∞ piri∞ gu
10
-ne “having presented
myself, clad in awe, (from =) on the royal dais” ”ulgi A 56.
12.14.4.2. 2nd sg. ¢am†u:
nú-a-zu-ne ù∞g “i-mu-e-da-nú-dè
zi-zi-da-zu-ne ù∞g “i-mu-e-da-zi-zi
“when you (O Sungod) have (lain down =) gone to sleep,
the people will also go to sleep with you,
(and) when you will rise again, the people will also rise
with you” Lugalbanda I 235 f.
za-e ∞ gál-la-zu-ne “now that you have been (back) again” Lugalbanda
I 227.
mí du
11
-ga-zu-ni “when you praised” Cohen CLAM II 503:33.
Note: The late version has mè
!?
-a du
11
-ga-zu-ne = [t]à¢aza ina lapàtika “when you
(touch =) engage in the battle” (Akk.); cf. Attinger 1993, 604.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 139
  139
12.14.4.3. 3rd sg. person-class ¢am†u: an-gin
7
ri-ba-ni ki-gin
7
ri-ba-ni
[rib-a-(a)ni] “(someone who) was enormous as the skies, enormous
as the earth” Gudea Cyl. A iv 15–16.
enkara . . . zà-ga-na lá-a-ni “having tied the e.-weapon to (the king’s)
side” Martu A 49.
a-ba me-a-nu a-ba me-a-ni “(who not being, who his/her being =)
who was it at all?” Gudea Cyl. A iv 23.
ku
4
-ra-ni “when she entered” (see above p. 138).
The verb “to come, go” in Ur III administrative (messenger) texts
merits special attention because of its irregular behaviour; see below
12.14.4.11.
12.14.4.4. 3rd sg. non-person class ¢am†u: sig
4
é-“è sa∞g íl-la-bi “as
(more and more) bricks raised (their) heads =) were piled up for
the house” Gudea Cyl. A xix 17.
[gú
íd
Bu]ranuna-kù-ga-ka §rú-a-bi [
íd
Bu]ranuna a na
8
-na
8
-da-bi “when
(the tree) had been planted at [the bank] of the sparkling Euphrates,
when it was drinking water (in =) from the Euphrates” Gilg., Enkidu
and the Netherworld 28–29.
12.14.4.5. 3rd pl. person class ¢am†u: gen-na-ne-ne “when they
had gone (to take away . . .)” TUT 213 r. 7; see RTC 330:7;
335:8.
12.14.4.6. 1st sg. marû: u
4
zal-la-∞ gu
10
-ne “when I gu
10
-ne e-ne di-da-∞
spent the day, when I was playing around” = “when I spent the
day playing around” TMH NF 3, 25:1 (cf. C. Wilcke, AfO 23
[1970] 84 ff.).
Note: For discussion of the element -ne see 12.14.4.13.
zi-zi-da-∞gu
10
-ne “when I get up” Schooldays 18.
ge
26
-e-da-∞ te-∞ gu
10
-ne “when I was approaching” Letter Coll. A 1:12.
kur-“à-ga du
7
-du
7
-da-∞ gu
10
-ne “when I am goring at the innermost
part of the foreign country” Inana/Ebe¢ 28.
12.14.4.7. 2nd sg. marû: ku
4
-ku
4
-da-zu-ni “when you are about to
enter” CT 42 no. 3 ii 17, see D. Charpin, Le clergé d’Ur (1986)
282 f.: 7 (= Rìm-Sin D).
Note: For the element -ni (var. of -ne) see 12.14.4.13.
me-lim
5
-nam-lugal-la mu
4
-mu
4
-da-zu-ne “when you robe yourself with
the splendour of kingship” Samsu-iluna 50.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 140
140 cn\r+rn +vrryr
e-sír-ra dib-bé-da-zu-ne “when you walk along the street” Father
and Son 31.
a-“à ur
4
-ru-da-zu-ne “when you are working the field with the
seeder-plough” Farmer’s Instructions 48.
12.14.4.8. 3rd sg. person class marû: um-mi-a . . . èn tar-re-da-ni
“when the headmaster . . . was asking” Schooldays 28.
ku
4
-ku
4
-da-ni “when he was about to enter” (see above p. 138).
For marû du-ni (instead of *du-da-ni) see below 12.14.4.11.
12.14.4.9. 3rd sg. non-person class marû: e-bi bala-e-da-bi “when
(Umma) is about to cross this dyke-and-canal” Ean. 1 r. v 38.
Note: This example, which would be the oldest available, is not unambiguous
because OS spelling prevents us from knowing whether a further (unwritten)
syllable/particle followed bala-e-da-bi.
na
8
-na
8
-da-bi see above 12.14.4.4.
dal-le-e-da-bi [dal-ed-a-bi] “when (my sling-stones) are flying” ”ulgi
B 37.
12.14.4.10. 3rd pl. person class marû: hur-sa∞g-umun
7
-kam-ma bala­
!
e-da-n[e-ne . . .] “when they were crossing the seventh mountain
range” Gilg. Huwawa A 62.
Note: This is the var. of ex. NiAA (see Edzard 1991, 188:62). The other ex.s
have bala-e-da-bi, bala-e-d[a-n]i, bala-da-ni, bi-ri-bala.
12.14.4.11. Irregular behaviour of the verb gˆen/du
The verb ∞ gen (¢am†u)/du (marû) “to come, to go” has a partially
irregular behaviour with the conjugated participle. Whereas ∞gen-na-
gu
10
-ne (12.14.4.1) or ∞ ∞ gen-na-ne-ne (12.14.4.5) are regular ¢am†u for­
mations, we find du-ni instead of *du-(e)da-ni and du-ne-ne instead
of *du-(e)da-ne-ne.
Ki-ma“
ki
-ta du-ni “when he was arriving from Kima“” HSS 4,
58:9 (see 12, r. 3 and 6).
”u“ana
ki
-“è du-ni “when he was leaving for Susa” HSS 4, 58:3
(see 6, r. 9 and 12).
du presents here an unextended marû participle du, i.e., without
the addition of [ed]. Clear evidence for such a marû participle is
offered by Enmerkar 157: lú-kin-gi
4
-a kur-“è du-úr [du-r(a)] “to the
messenger travelling to the foreign country”.
du-ne-ne “when they were leaving (for ”ima“gi)” HSS 4, 56 r. 10.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 141
  141
Note: There are spelling variants du-ni-ni ITT 3, 6332 r. 4; HLC 2, 58 85 r.
8; du-ni-ne RA 19, 43 XCII 7; in the latter form, the scribe probably added—
mechanically—pl. -(e)ne to sg. du-ni.
For plenty more Ur III examples (as of 1955) see T. Fish, MCS 5,
13–26; also Edzard 1972, 17–19.
12.14.4.12. deli-gˆu
10
-ne etc.
The formations B-a-∞gu
10
-ne etc. have an unexpected parallel in a
nominal compound where number “one” occurs in the position of
a verbal base:
deli-ni “he/she alone”, e.g., Angin dima 197.
deli-∞ gu
10
-ne “I all alone” Lugalbanda II 328.
deli-zu-ne “you all alone” Lugalbanda II 356.
Most interestingly, the variant spelling -ne/-ni is shared by B-(d)a-
gu
10
-ne/-ni and deli-∞ ∞ gu
10
-ne/-ni. For discussion see 12.14.4.13.
deli-zu-ne mah-me-en “(Suen, . . .) (of/in) your sole self, you are
the greatest” ZA 63 (1973) 32 no. 5:20.
deli-zu-ni mah-me-en “(Nuska, . . .) (of/in) your sole self you are
the greatest” ZA 63, 17 no. 3a:11.
Note: Å. Sjöberg, not yet realizing the parallel between -ne and -ni, read dili-zu
ì-mah-me-en.
!
deli
li
-zu-ni mah-me = e-di-“e-ka ßí-ra-ta VS 2, 89 obv.(?) 7' // 9'.
Note: I owe these—and more—examples to the late H. Behrens.
12.14.4.13. Conjugated participles: unresolved questions
Taken by themselves, 3
rd
sg. and pl. person class B-[a]-(a)ni, B-[a]-
(a)nene as well as B-[ed]-ani, B-[ed]-anene would most easily be
analysed as the ¢am†u or marû participles + possessive particles [ani]
or [anene].
Such an analysis is not compatible, however, with 1
st
and 2
nd
sg.
marû B-[ed]-a-∞ gu
10
/-zu-ne or with 3
rd
sg. non-person class marû B-
[ed]-a-bi. Here, before the possessive particle an element [a] is inserted
which would remind us, at first glance, of the supposed locative par­
ticle in B-[ed]-a (see 12.14.3.2). But addition of a possessive particle
after a (dimensional) case particle would disagree with the supposed
hierarchy of suffixed nominal morphemes in Sumerian which is: pos-
session—number—case, e.g., ses-∞gu
10
-(e)ne-da “with my brothers” (see
above, 8). Therefore, the element [a] in question might as well be
of a different origin, with a different function. Or are B-[ed]-a-∞ gu
10
­
ne, B-[ed]-ani, B-[ed]-a-bi, of different origin, converging to form a
common paradigm, disregarding rules of suffix hierarchy?
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:45 PM Page 142
142 cn\r+rn +vrryr
The second problem is the suffix -ne (var. -ni) added after pos­
sessive particles -∞gu
10
, -zu. The former reading -dè instead of -ne has
been disproved by the variant spelling -ni, attested both synchroni­
cally with -ne and diachronically as a later variant.
Note: C. J. Gadd, Iraq 22 (1960) 161 f., note to line 7, (“-ni must arise from
reading NE (regular here) as ne instead of dè”). The variant was also noted by
Aro 1961, 327 fn. 1 (“Auffälligerweise ni statt NE (dè) geschrieben”). It was dis­
cussed by Attinger 1993, 107 and 311, but without definite conclusion.
To sum up, the function of this [ne, ni] is still unknown, and we
cannot explain why it occurs only in forms of the 1
st
and 2
nd
person.
A completely analogous distribution of presence or absence of
[ne/ni] is found in deli-∞gu
10
/zu-ne “on my/your own”, but deli-ni
“on his/her own” (see 12.14.4.12).
12.15. Covrotxr yrnns
By “compound verbs” Sumerian grammars traditionally understand
frozen combinations of a noun (mostly a substantive, rarely an adjec­
tive) and a verbal base. The meaning of the compound is not a sim­
ple addition of the meanings of the individual elements, but a new
one: A+B = C or A+B ≠ A+B. This very often becomes clear from
the Akkadian translation, e.g., ki á∞g, equated with râmu “to love”,
cannot be explained by combining the meanings “earth, ground” (ki)
and “to measure out” (á∞g).
Note: A. Falkenstein, orally, suggested a gesture of reverence, making a gener­
ous move of the hand/arm towards the ground, as the origin of ki á∞g.
Postgate 1974, 35, proposed as “the most specific criterion for dis­
tinguishing a compound verb from a ‘simple’ one” the position of
the nominal element immediately before the verbal complex. The
nominal element may be extended by an adjective—or, occasionally,
by a dependent genitive—but no independent word may enter between
the nominal element and the verbal complex. Thus, ki mu-ra-á∞g-en
“I love you” would not allow insertion of za-ra as in *ki za-ra mu-
ra-á∞ g-en “It is you I love”.
Civil 1976, 148 f., and Attinger 1993, 179, have been sceptical
about the criterion A+B ≠ A+B, “difficilement utilisable dans le cas
d’une langue morte aux catégories de pensée étrangère aux nôtres”.
In fact, ∞gé“tu(-ga-ni) gub may be taken either literally as “to set
(one’s) (ear =) mind” or, on a more developed meaning, “to intend/plan
to do”.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 143
  143
Attinger 1993, 179, showed that there are two types of compound
verbs: 1) Both the nominal and the verbal element have an origi­
nally independent status; 2) the verbal element serves as a verbal­
izer; the latter function is essentially restricted to du
11
(-g)/e/di “to
say, do” and AG “to do, make”. While (1) may be said to be a
closed class, limited by practical usage, (2) may be called an open
class. So, e.g., the type nam-na∞gar AG “to (do carpentry =) act as
a carpenter” could theoretically be extended to as many nam-x for­
mations as there are.
Note: In Akkadian corresponds . . . -ùta(m) epè“u(m) for which CAD E 201–225
offers over 50 examples.
Attinger 1993 listed as many as 213 examples for the verbalizer x(-
y) du
11
(-g)/e/di.
Karahashi 2000, 2–10, has reviewed former discussions of the
topic, between 1908 and 1993; add Postgate 1974, 35–40.
See, moreover, Thomsen 1984, 269–72; Attinger 1993, 178–82; Krecher 1993,
107–18.
12.15.1. Compound verbs: free formations
Karahashi 2000 treated the subject with body-part terms as nomi­
nal elements, excluding formations with du
11
(-g)/e/di and AG (see
12.15.2). On pp. 72–108 she listed over 130 compound verbs.
The main problem with the syntactical interpretation is the func­
tion of the nominal element: is it in the absolutive or may it also
stand in a different case? Among Karahashi’s examples, there are
91 (out of 130, i.e., 70%) with a nominal element which (in our
Latin transliteration) ends in a vowel: á “arm, side”, gaba “breast,
chest”, ∞ gìri “foot”, gú “neck”, gù “voice”, igi “eye, front”, ki “place”,
ní “self ”, su “body, flesh”, “u “hand”, zi “breath”, zú “tooth”. All
these may, theoretically, be in the absolutive, or in the directive (with
the [e] assimilated). So, “u ti/te(-∞g) “to receive, to take” could be
interpreted either as “to make (an object) (absolutive) come close to
the hand (directive)” or as “to make the hand (absolutive) come close
(to an object)”. See below, p. 145f., for “u ti/te(-∞g).
On the other hand, a nominal element ending in a consonant,
gé“tu(-g) “ear”, há“ “thigh”, sa∞ e.g., dùg “knee”, ∞ g “head”, “à(-g)
“heart, middle part”, is clearly recognizable as an absolutive if it
g “to soothe the heart” can only have [“a∞ stands by itself. “à(-g) hu∞ g-
Ø] as the nominal element, to the exclusion of *[“age] or *[“aga].
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 144
144 cn\r+rn +vrryr
But a more important criterion for determing the syntactical char­
acter of the nominal element is the form taken by an “object” depend­
ing on a compound verb, whether as an independent part of speech
or as an infixed particle.
If the nominal element is in the absolutive, there cannot be a sec­
ond absolutive as a second “object” (or “subject” where the mean­
ing of the verb is intransitive). It will have to stand in a dimensional
case. Put the other way round, this means that if a second “object”
(or “subject”) is in fact in a dimensional case, it may be taken as
proof that the nominal element of the compound verb is in the abso­
lutive. This argument applies equally to dimensional indicators in
the verbal chain (see 12.8).
tukumbi “u mu-ri-bar-re(-n) “if I set you free” Prov. Coll. 5.55:3
(E. I. Gordon, JCS 12 [1958] 46).
Here, the “object” of “u bar “to set free” is in the directive [(e)ri],
and “u stands in the absolutive.
Note: For “u bar (< ba§r) see Krecher 1993, 113.
ninda [ninda-e] ∞ge“ ha-ba-ni-tag “I (”ulgi) indeed offered (bread =)
food” ”ulgi A 55.
Whatever the original meaning of ∞ge“ tag (touching wood in a
ceremony?), ∞ ge“ is in the absolutive.
nam ge
4
-rí-íb-tar nam-du
10
(-g) gú-mu-rí-íb-tar “let me make a firm
promise for you, let me make a benevolent firm promise for you”
”ulgi D 384 f.
nam is in the absolutive, the “object” in the directive [(e)ri].
é-e ∞ gál ba-an-taka “he (loosened the . . . at the house =) he opened
the house”.
∞gál (meaning unknown) is in the absolutive, the “object” in the
directive.
Note: The nominal element ∞gál is—graphically—the same as ig “door”. It was
also used for the abstract notion ∞ gál “to be, to exist”.
urdu-dè lugal-ni [lugal-(a)ni-e] zà(-g) mu-da-DU-àm [mu-n-da-n-DU-
am] “the slave (had it with him that he set the side next to his mas­
ter =) was allowed to go side by side with his master” Gudea Stat.
B vii 32–33.
zà(-g) “side” is in the absolutive and lugal-ni in the directive.
si-a
d
Inana me-kur-ra-ke
4
“u al-du
7
-du
7
“be silent, Inana, the ordi­
nances of the Netherworld are absolutely perfect” Inana’s Descent
132, etc.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 145
  145
The original meaning of “u du
7
is probably “to move the hand
directly (towards a goal)” (with du
7
“to make a direct movement
(towards a goal)”). The “object” me-kur-ra(-k) is in the directive.
Gù-dé-a gal mu-zu gal ì-ga-túm-mu “Gudea knows much, and he
(also brings great =) is able, too, to accomplish great things” Gudea
Cyl. A vii 9–10.
Here, the nominal element is an adjective. It is probably to be
understood in the absolutive case, but an unmarked adverb cannot
be excluded.
12.15.1.1. Extended nominal element
The nominal element of a compound verb is occasionally extended
by an adjective; it is uncertain, however, whether such an extension
was a productive feature of the system or whether it was restricted
in use by phraseology.
Beside igi bar “to look (at)” there is igi-zi bar:
d
En-líl-e en
d
Nin-∞ gír-su-“è igi-zi mu-“i-bar [mu-n-“i-n-bar] “Enlil
had directed his (true =) meaningful gaze toward the lord Ningirsu”
Gudea Cyl. A i 3.
Only an informant would still be able to tell whether igi-zi bar
was tantamount to zi-dè-e“ igi bar “to look in a true, reliable, mean­
ingful way”. Be that as it may, the addition of zi after igi adds a
shade of meaning to the verbal expression and may, therefore, well
be called adverbial.
gu
10
[ezem-An-a(k)-∞ me-gal-gal ezem-An-na-∞ gu-e] “u-gal ma-du
7
-du
7
“all the great performances, my ‘festival of An’, are performed for
me in ritual perfection” Gudea Cyl. A x 18.
12.15.1.2. “u ti/te(-ˆ g) and other compound verbs and the special
behaviour of their “objects”
“u ti/te(-∞g) “to receive, to take” is by far the most frequently
attested compound verb, due to its ubiquitous usage in legal and
administrative documents.
The subtle difference between OB Akkadian ma¢àru “to receive”
(without any ensuing consequence) and leqû “to receive” (with ensu­
ing consequences, e.g., the obligation to repay a loan) has no coun­
terpart in Sumerian where “u ti/te(-∞g) is used in both senses.
For examples from literary texts, see Karahashi 2000, 168–71.
As a rule, the “object” of “u ti/te(-∞g) is in the directive or locative,
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 146
146 cn\r+rn +vrryr
and the person from whom something is received is originally in the
terminative:
lugal-a-ni (erg.) siskur ara
x
(DU)-zu-ni [arazu-(a)ni-e] Gù-dé-a-á“ en
d
Nin-∞gír-su-ke
4
(erg.) “u ba-“i(-n)-ti “his master (accepted) from Gudea,
the lord Ningirsu accepted from him prayer and rite” Gudea Cyl.
A ii 21–22; cf. iv 1–2; Cyl. B iii 3–4.
Occasionally, mostly in texts of OB tradition, the “object” of “u
ti/te(-∞ g) is in the absolutive as if—contrarily to what one would
expect—there actually were a second absolutive:
g-∞ . . .
d
Nuska á-á∞ gá-
d
En-líl-lá “u ba-an-ti-a-ta “after . . . Nuska had
received Enlil’s instructions” Enlil and Ninlil 2:44.
ga-Enlil-a(k)] instead of *[a’a∞ The “object” is [a’a∞ ga-Enlil-ak-e].
Falkenstein 1950, 134 n. 1 judged this as “der klarste Fall für die
Beeinflussung der Konstruktion eines sumerischen Verbums durch
die seines akkadischen Gegenstücks”.
A comparable—secondary—construction is also found with si sá
“to direct, adjust, etc.”:
gi“
ù-“ub-kù(-g) [s]i íb-sá “a (clean =) brand-new brick-mould was
adjusted” Gudea Cyl. A A v 6.
The “subject”, in the passive construction, would be expected to
be *[u“ub-kuge].
For an exhaustive treatment of the compound verb si sá see C.
Wilcke 2003.
In the above cases, the nominal element and the verbal base have
practically coalesced so as to behave like a simple transitive verb.
For this phenomenon two reasons may be adduced: (1) It was the
result of an independent, internal evolution of Sumerian morpho-
syntax. (2) Or—more probably in view of the Sumero-Akkadian lin­
guistic area—it was due to the influence of the syntax of the underlying
Akkadian verb, e.g., leqû in the case of “u ti/te(-∞g), or e“èru, “ù“uru in
the case of si sá (Falkenstein’s position).
Note: In the case of “u ti/te(-∞g), by Ur III also the notation of the person from
whom something was received had undergone a change—at least in juridical and
administrative contexts: Instead of PN-“è (see above, Gudea Cyl. A ii 21–22), ki-
PN(-a(k))-ta “from (the place of ) PN” or only ki-PN, became more customary.
Here, the influence of itti PN leqû is obvious.
This change of syntax has a clear parallel in the verb “to buy”,
sa
10
/sa
10
-sa
10
, where the seller was originally noted with the termi­
native, PN-“è, but by Ur III times with ki-PN(-a(k))-ta, or simply ki-
PN; see J. Krecher, RIA 5 (1976–80) 496 r., and C. Wilcke, ibid.
505 r.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 147
  147
12.15.1.3. Complete incorporation of the nominal element
In a final—Old Bab.—stage of the evolution of the morpho-syntactic
behaviour of compound verbs, the nominal element could be trans­
ferred to stand immediately before the verbal base, in finite and non-
finite forms. Sometimes, in such cases, the nominal element was
simultaneously kept in place, redundantly, before the string of prefixed
verbal particles, e.g., si . . . -si-sá (see below).
gi
6
-pàr-en-na . . . mu-un-ki-∞ é-∞ gar “I(!) founded . . . the abode of the
en (priestess)” RIME 4, 472 f. no. 2:16–19 (Anam of Uruk).
A form *ki mu-un-∞ gar “he laid the ground” has been transformed,
but the reason remains unknown.
ga-bi nu-mu-ra-“u-gíd-e “(the sheep) no (longer) offer their milk to
you” LamUr I 362.
So ms. C = STVC 17 v 2'; ms. N = MBI 6 ii 48' has ga nu-mu-ra-AG-e.
The underlying verb is “u gíd “to stretch out the hand (in either
direction)” = “to offer, to receive” (cf. Karahashi 2000, 161 f.).
A post-OB example is hé-nir-∞gál “may he be authoritative” (PN)
A. Moortgat, Rollsiegel
3
(1988) 554.
dumu-An-na An-x-la-ta té“ mu-u
8
-da-mah-di “daughter of An
(= Bau), . . ., you alone are able to be as outstanding as An(?)” CT
36, 40 (r.) 7 (Bau B).
Note: Does Ur III PN Íb-ta-ab-PA-è RA 8 (1911) 184 no. 1:10' already repre­
sent a case of incorporated pa è “to come forth radiantly”? In view of Íb-ta-ab-
è and Íb-ta(-b)-è (see Limet 1968, 431), Íb-ta-ab-PA-è has perhaps to be seen as
a graphic variant.
The nominal element is sometimes found twice with the verb si sá:
g-ur-sa∞ “à-bi(-a) ur-sa∞ g-e-ne si mu-si-sá-e-ne “all the warriors go
straightway into the interior (of the sanctuary)” Ke“ Hymn 60 (OB).

?
a ní∞g-izi-te-na si mi-ni-in-si-sá
∞garza me-mah “u mi-ni-ib-“u-du
7
“. . . water which quenches fire has been directed there, the rites
and sublime ordinances have been performed there in the most per­
fect way (for us) Flood i 44–45.
Cf. Krecher 1987, 69; Karahashi 2000, 4; in general Wilcke 2003.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 148
148 cn\r+rn +vrryr
12.15.2. Compound verbs with a verbalizer
Attinger 1993, 179, noted that there is a group of compound verbs
with the bases du
11
(-g)/e/di or AG essentially serving the function
of turning a noun into a verb, i.e., serving as verbalizers.
Note: Verbalization is a wide-spread phenomenon. It is realised, morphologically,
either (a) by a special ending attached to the noun or (b) by a special verb. For
(a) note, in Indo-European, -ize (English), -(is)ieren (German), -iser (French), etc.,
and for (b) etmek “to do” (Turkish), kardan “to do” (Persian). Semitic languages
have not developed a productive way to verbalize nouns: here examples such as
Akkadian ruggubu “to provide with a roof/an upper storey (rugbu)” or Arabic tal­
fana “to telephone” (extremely unpopular) are rather exceptions to the rule.
Sumerian would belong to category (b).
Attinger 1993 collected 213 examples for x du
11
(-g)/e/di. A count
for x AG has not yet been made. While verbalization applies to the
majority of verbal compounds of the types x AG or x du
11
(-g)/e/di,
there certainly is a number of verbs which should rather be classified
under 12.15.1, “free formations”; however, our modern interpreta­
tion is often arbitrary.
We will only quote a few examples each for (a) evident verbal­
ization and (b) uncertain cases. In general, Attinger 1993 should be
consulted for du
11
(-g)/e/di.
12.15.2.1. Clear cases of verbalization
á“ du
11
(-g)/e/di “to say a curse” = “to curse”; di du
11
(-g) “to say,
make legal case” = “to judge”; A-nir du
11
(-g) “to say a lamentation”
= “to lament”; gù du
11
(-g) to say, make a voice” = “to shout, yell,
roar”; ∞gì“ du
11
(-g) “to do the penis” = “to have intercourse with a
woman”; ma“kim du
11
(-g) “to do the bailiff ” = “to act as a bailiff ”;
tu
6
du
11
(-g) “to say an incantation” = “to conjure”, etc.
PN
Note: No references are offered because all the evidence is amply available in
Attinger 1993, 414–765 (alphabetical catalogue).
[e
4
] pa
5
-bi “u-bala ba-ra-ak-ke
4
(-n) “I will not—I swear—change (the
course) of (Girsu’s) canals and dykes” Ean. 1 r. i 20–21.
“u bala “to make the hand go across something” is a compound
in itself; it has been verbalized—or re-verbalized—by the addition
of AG “to make”; “to effect a change”.
1–5
enim PN
6
-ra in-na-an-e“-a [i-na-n-e-(e)“-a] “u-bala bí-in-AG-
é“ “PN
1
. . . PN
5
(changed =) contradicted what they had said to
PN
6
” NG no. 113:17–24.
sa-gaz lú mu-na [mu-n-a(k)] “someone committed a robbery here”
NG no. 121:3.
Note: There is no need to restore mu-na<-AG>, as Falkenstein did in his edition.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 149
  149
12.15.2.2. Verbalization no longer recognizable to us
al du
11
(-g)/e/di “to wish, to strive for” has a nominal element al
with unknown etymology: see Attinger 1993, 443 with fn. 1179.
sá du
11
(-g)/e/di “to reach, arrive” is derived from sá “être égal à”
by Attinger 1993, 641; in fact, “to say ‘equal’” = “to measure up to
something”, may be the appropriate etymology; but it is not certain.
a-ne/e-ne du
11
(-g)/e/di “to enjoy, play” has a nominal element
which Landsberger (see Attinger 1993, 472 fn. 1294) equated with
the personal pronoun “he, she”; Landsberger saw the origin in count-
ing-out rhymes: “it is him, her”; but this is uncertain.
12.15.3. Difficult cases: artificial splits?
sa∞g(-[e“]) [rig] “to give as a present” has been claimed as a pre-Old
Akkadian loan from Akkadian “aràkum “to present” in its stative form
“arik, with an artificial split of the Semitic root ”RK into [sa(∞g)] and
[rig].
Cf. Karahashi 2000, 140 with fn. 93, and see already I. J. Gelb,
MAD 3 (1957) 284.
Note: While the spelling of the first, ‘nominal’, element, [sa(∞g)], is consistently
SAG, [rig] offers several—diachronical—spelling variants, each one of the diri(g)
type: PA.KAB = rig
8
(see below, Ean. 2 vii 6); KAB.[SA]G.DU = rig
9
(see below,
Steible 1982 b 201); PA.KAB.DU = rig
7
in Ur III and OB. These spellings still
remain unexplained.
gír-su-ra Lum-ma-gin
7
-du
10
sa∞
d
Nin-∞ g-é“ mu-ni-rig
8
“(Eannatum) gave
(the canal) L. as a present to Nin∞girsu” Ean. 2 vii 3–6.
Note: Like with nam tar, sa∞g(-[e“]) [rig] uses the directive for the (god or) per­
son in whose favour the action is made.
[sa]∞g r[i]g
9
(KAB.[SA]G.DU Steible 1982 b 201: AnAgr. 4 ii 2' (OS,
in broken context).
gˆ e“
gu-za
d
En-ki-k[e
4
] sa∞g ha-ba-ra-PA.KA[B.DU]-ga-a suhu“-bi ha-
ra-ab-ge-ge-in “I (= Ninlil) will certainly consolidate for you the
throne which Enki has already given you as a present” ”ulgi R 87.
Note: sa∞g rig
7
here governs the dative (verbal infix [(e)ra]) instead of the direc-
tive—perhaps in parallel with the following [ha-(e)ra-b-gege-n].
d
En-ki-ke
4
Mar-tu má“-an“e sa∞g-e-e“ mu-ni-rig
7
“Enki gave the cat­
tle as a gift to the Amurru (nomad)” EWO 249.
Summing up, sa∞g [rig] as a split of *“arik would be quite an
unusual type of borrowing. At least two arguments may be raised
against a—direct—Akkadian origin of the verb in question: (1) Why
was [sa∞g] chosen to represent the first syllable of alleged “arik, instead
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 150
150 cn\r+rn +vrryr
of, e.g., sá? (2) Why was [sa∞g rig] most often construed with the
addition of the adverbiative ending [e“(e)] (cf. above 4.2.(5))?
For (2) one could refer to the parallel—post-OB—ní∞g-ba-a“ ba =
ana qì“ti “aràku “to give as a present” (see CAD Q 156 qâ“u lex.).
But given the late attestation of [ni∞ gba-“ ba], one would rather sug­
gest that this idiom was formed by analogy with [sa∞g-e“ rig].
The question has to be left to further research.
A noun rig
7
“enclosed room(?)” was discussed by Krecher 1966,
153 f. It is not clear whether the spelling of the verbal element of
[sa∞g(-e“) rig] was derived from the respective noun or vice versa.
Some phonetic similarity has been noted between the noun ∞ gé“tu
(-g) (GI”.TÚG.PI and similar spellings) “ear” and the verb ∞ge“ tuku
“to hear”. Karahashi 2000, 84 f.: “in gi“-tuku, gi“ is probably chosen
not because of its literal meaning but because of its phonological
proximity to ge“tú ‘ear’”; see her excursus on “gi“-tuku” ibid. p. 86 f.
gé“tu(-g) ¡ ∞ There is hardly good reason to propose a “split” ∞ ge“
[tuG], and such was not Karahashi’s intention either. For ∞gé“tu(-g)
gé“tu-ga-ni [∞ “ear” is regularly prolonged by -gV (e.g., ∞ ge“tug-ani]
whereas tuku in ∞ ge“ tuku is, without exception, prolonged by -a,
-àm (see Karahashi 2000, 86 f.).
Whatever the etymology of ∞ge“ tuku, it cannot be directly related
to [∞ge“tug] “ear”.
12.16. Novix\riz\+iox or rixi+r yrnn\r ronvs
Sumerian finite verbal forms may be turned into nouns by the addi­
tion of a morpheme [a], the nominalizer. Once such a change from
verb to noun took place, the newly attained nominal complex behaves
like a noun: it may be inflected for case; possessive particles may be
added. A nominalized complex cannot, however, be put in the plural.
Nominalization is important in Sumerian syntax because inflected
complexes often stand for what we would interpret as subordinate
clauses. Sumerian nominalization most probably influenced Akkadian
which stands out, among other Semitic languages, by its great vari­
ety of infinitive constructions.
J. Aro, Die akkadischen Infinitivkonstruktionen (= StudOr. 26, 1961), esp. pp.
323–32, “Die sumerischen Entsprechungen und Vorbilder der akkadischen
Infinitivkonstruktionen”; Thomsen 1984, 241–50; Attinger 1993, 305.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 151
  151
The origin of nominalizer [a] is unknown. It may be added to
both ¢am†u and marû bases along with the respective conjugation pat­
terns. For this reason, identity of particle [a] with the ending of the
¢am†u participle (§ rú-a, see 12.14.2) is quite improbable.
lú . . . di-ku
5
-a-na “u ì-íb-bala-e-a “he who . . . will (let the hand
pass over =) disregard his (= Gudea’s) judgment” Gudea Stat. B viii
42–43 (marû).
lú É-ninnû . . . in-§rú-a “who (had) built the Eninnû” Gudea Stat.
A caption 4–6 and passim in the Gudea Corpus (¢am†u).
The nominalized phrase has, as a rule, a head or antecedent on
which it depends, e.g., lú “person”, ní∞g “thing”, u
4
“day, time”, mu
“year”, etc.; but often the antecedent has to be supplied, because it
is not explicitly expressed.
It seems most practical to arrange the material by the case in
which the nominalized phrase occurs, usually indicated by a case
particle following the nominalizer [a], though we shall not provide
examples for every case.
12.16.1. The nominalized phrase is in the absolutive
(or “casus pendens”), unmarked (-Ø)
ní∞g Ki-en-gi-ra ba-a-gu(-ul)-la kur-ra ga-àm-mi-íb-gu-ul “what has
been destroyed in Sumer, I will destroy in the foreign land” ”ulgi
D 219; cf. line 335: ní∞g Ki-en-gi-ra ba-a-gu-ul-la kur-ra ì-mi-in-gu-ul.
The head of the phrase is ní∞g, “thing, what”, and the nominal­
ized phrase is, morpho-syntactically, in apposition to, or an adjec­
tive added to, ní∞g.
d
E
4
-nun-na . . . a-gin
7
ba-e-ne-diri-ga “the Enùna . . . among whom
you (= Inana) are so much superior” Ninme“ara 115.
Note: The phrase is to be understood as a vocative which is unmarked.
. . . dam-“è ha-tuku bí-in-du
11
-ga PN
1
PN
2
nam-érim-àm “PN
1
(and)
PN
2
swore that (PN
3
) declared: ‘I will take (PN
4
. . .) as a wife’” NG
no. 15:6–9; cf. ibid. nos. 16:6–11, 17:7–10, etc., for similar state­
ments made under oath.
Note: It is interesting to note that the quotation of direct speech is not made
with the addition of the quotation particle [(e)“e], but with nominalized [bi-n-
dug] “he declared”.
Note: Here, a deposition is confirmed, under oath, by witnesses. nam-érim-àm
literally means “it is an evil (which should befall me [= the oath-taking party]
in case of perjury)”. It is used for an oath concerning something that already
happened (“assertory oath”), as against the oath taken as a promise way that one
would do or not do—or refrain from—something (“promissory oath”).
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 152
152 cn\r+rn +vrryr
PN
1
PN
2
-na-an-na lú nu-ù-da-nú-a nam-érim-àm “PN
1
swore that
apart from PN
2
nobody slept with me” NG no. 24:9'–12' (for =
nanna see 13.3).
The syntactical relation between the nominalized statement and
the following nam-érim-àm is difficult to establish. One might say
the nominalized phrase is in a casus pendens.
See Falkenstein 1956, 63 ff. for the different formulations of “assertorischer Eid”
and “promissorischer Eid”.
12.16.2. The nominalized phrase is in the genitive
gir-Gù-dé-a . . . lú E-an-na in-§ ru-a-(a)k-am] “it is the di∞ rú-a-kam [i-n-§
(personal) god of Gudea . . . the one who built the Eana” Gudea
Stat. C i 2–5.
ur-sa∞ g-ug
5
-ga ì-me-“a-ke
4
-é“ [i-me-(e)“-a-(a)k-e“] “because they (= the
aforementioned mythological beings) are warriors (once) slain (by
Nin∞girsu)” Gudea Cyl. A xxvi 15.
Note: [ime“ak] is a free standing genitive to which the adverbiative ending [e“]
has been added. We cannot give an answer to how [ime“ake“] was distinguished
from a construction mu . . . -a-“è (see 12.16.5), i.e., *mu . . . ì-me-é“-a-“è.
enim-ama-ne-ne nu-ub-kúr-ne-a [nu-b-kur-(e)ne-a-a(k)] mu-lugal-bi in-
pà-dè-e“ “they invoked the royal name that they would not change
the (word =) disposition of their mother” NG no. 99:44–46.
Note: In this and in many parallel cases it is not certain whether the nominalized
phrase should really be understood as standing in the genitive, resumed by the
-bi of mu-lugal-bi, or whether it stands in the absolutive, as a “casus pendens”.
12.16.3. The nominalized phrase is in the locative
u
4
d
Nin-∞ girsu gír-su-ke
4
“à-ge ba-pà-da-a [ba-n-pad-a-a] “when Nin∞
had (called him in the heart =) chosen him” En. I 2 i 7–ii 1.
The nominalized phrase is in the locative, and there is no geni­
tival (regens-rectum) relation between the head (u
4
) of the phrase and
the phrase itself. Therefore, the phrase can only be defined—mor-
pho-syntactically—as an apposition or an adjective joined to u
4
. If
we see the phrase as an apposition the head (u
4
) would virtually be
in the locative, too, with the locative case particle deleted following
a rule that in a sequence of parallel syntactical members only the
last one will be marked by a case particle: u
4
(-da) . . . -a. Cf. the
sequence an ki-a “in heaven (and) on earth” where the locative after
an (*an-na = [an-a]) has been deleted: an//ki-a.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 153
  153
If, on the other hand, we see the relation between head (u
4
) and
phrase as a connection of substantive (u
4
) + adjective (nominalized
phrase), there is no need to postulate the deletion of a case particle
after u
4
.
By the time of the Ur III dynasty, the separate notation of nom­
inalizer [a] and locative case particle [a] often coalesced so that a
single written [a] may stand for both. But note still Hammu-rapi bí-
in-du
11
-ga-a RIME 4, 335 no. 2:20.
The u
4
. . . -a construction is extremely popular. In Akkadian it
has an exact parallel in the sequence inu/inùma . . . Moreover, the
sometimes very elaborate construction u
4
. . . (1) . . . -a, (2) . . . -a,
(3) . . . -a (etc.) u
4
-ba . . . “when . . ., (at that day =) then . . .” (e.g.,
En. I 20 i 9–iii 2) has its counterpart in Akkadian inu . . . inùmi“u . . .
“when . . ., then . . .”, e.g., Hammu-rapi RIME 4, 335 no. 2:1–
25//1–28.
mu bala∞g u“umgal-kalam-ma ba-dím-ma [ba-dim-a-a] “the year
(when) the harp (called) ‘Dragon of the Land’ was fashioned”, year
name Gudea of Laga“ 3; see Edzard 1997, 27.
The construction of the year date formulae exactly follows the
pattern u
4
. . . -a (see before). The nominalized phrase is in the loca­
tive and so is—virtually—the head mu.
However, as against the u
4
. . . -a construction, the mu . . . -a con­
struction was often reduced to a formula where [a] has been deleted
!
after the verbal form, e.g., mu Lugal-Ba-gára é-a ba-ku
4
“the year
(when) the Lord of Bagara entered (his) house” Gudea year 16; see
Edzard 1997, 27. The head mu here behaves like a subjunction.
bar “e-bi nu-da-sù-sù-da-ka [nu-n-da-n-su(d)sud-a-(a)k-a] “because
he (= Ur-LUM-ma of Umma) no longer was willing to (lengthen,
extend =) restore that grain (with =) to him” Ent. 28–29 A ii 27.
Note: The syntagma is bar-Noun-ak-a, lit. “at the side of Noun” with the noun
here represented by a verbal phrase + nominalizing [a], which we have to under­
stand as “because of . . .”; see PSD B 109 f. bar C, esp. p. 110: 3.
12.16.4. The nominalized phrase is in the ablative
ba-dú-ud-dè-en-na-ta [ba-dud-en-a-ta] ninta-kal-ga-me-en “I am a
mighty king since I was born” ”ulgi A 2.
Ba-gára . . . im-ti-a-ta “after he had come close to the Bagara (sanc­
tuary)” Gudea Cyl. A ii 7.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 154
154 cn\r+rn +vrryr
d
Nin-a-zu-ù . . . “ùd-da mi-ni-in-pà-pà-da-ta “after Nin-azu . . . had
pronounced many blessings thereon” ”ulgi X 126.
di “u la-ba-an-ti-en-za-na [la-ba-n(i)-ti-enzen-a] enim-“è la-ba-dúr-
ru-ne-en-za-na-ta [la-ba-durun-enzen-a-ta] “because you (pl.) did not
accept the verdict (and) did not abide by the word” Enki-hegal/Enkitalu
191 (see Karahashi 2000, 169 no. 12a).
e∞ger ba-ùr-ra-ta “after (the flood) had swept thereover” SKL 40.
12.16.5. The nominalized phrase is in the terminative
mu ∞ ge“-nú-PN ba-an-zuh-a-“è “because he had stolen PN’s bed” NG
no. 203:3–4.
Note: mu “name” and the following nominalized phrase may be defined—mor-
pho-syntactically—as a head noun with an apposition, both standing in the same
case, but with the case particle deleted after the head noun: mu[-“è] . . . -a-“è.
See the discussion in 12.16.5 locative under u
4
. . . -a.
mu PN-e mu-lugal pà-da du
11
-ga-na ba-ni(-n)-ge-na-“è PN
2
PN
3
ba-
an-tuku “because PN
1
had (confirmed on his saying =) admitted that
(the royal name had been invoked =) an oath (invoking) the king
had been sworn, PN
3
married PN
2
” NG no. 14:18–21.
Sumerian mu . . . -a-“è constructions correspond to Akkadian sub­
ordinate clauses with a““um “because”.
12.16.6. The nominalized phrase is in the equative
lugal-ra . . . e
4
mu-na-a-tu
5
-a-gin
7
[mu-na-V-tu-a-gin] “as I (= Inana)
bathed for the king” ”ulgi X 14–15; see 16, and cf. lines 19, 21,
24, etc.
Note: The comparative function of -gin
7
“in the same way as . . .”, “like . . .” here
coalesced with a temporal function.
Cf. Lugal 390–392, 419–422, 483, 617.
12.16.7. Nominalized phrase with a possessive particle
u
4
An-né kur-kur-ra sa∞g-ki ba-da-an-gíd-da-ba [ba-bda-n-gid-a-b(i)-a]
“when An had frowned upon all the lands” LamSumUr 22; see ibid.
23–26.
Note: We have to do with an extension of the u
4
. . . -a construction (see 12.16.3).
The locative supposed to follow the nominalized verbal form has been shifted
after the possessive particle -bi.
edzard_f13_70-155/PDF 4/28/03 2:46 PM Page 155
  155
d
En-líl lú-nam-tar-tar-re-dè a-na bí-in-AG-a-ba
d
En-líl-le Elam
ki
. . .
kur-ta im-ta-an-è “what Enlil who makes inescapable decisions actu­
ally did is that he, Enlil, brought the Elamites . . . down against us
from their highlands” LamSumUr 165 f.
[ana binAGaba] lit. is “in its ‘what-he-did thereby’”.
Note: lú here functions as a ‘relative pronoun’; hardly “in order to decide the
fate of mankind” (Michalowski’s translation), because lú “person” must be kept
separate from (nam-)lú-u
18
-lu [(nam)lulu] “man(kind)”.
156
EDZARD_f14_156-160 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 156
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
157
EDZARD_f14_156-160 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 157
CHAPTER THIRTEEN
POST
-
NOMINAL AND/OR POST
-
VERBAL PARTICLES
OTHER THAN CASE PARTICLES (5.4)
This heading is a catch-all for different kinds of particles, both nom­
inal and nominal/verbal (general), lacking a collective denominator.
We kept them apart from the (nominal) case particles in spite of
Attinger 1993, 260 “les postpositions ‘isolantes’” (Krecher’s term).
The particles described here cannot even be said to follow a com­
mon morpho-syntactical behaviour pattern, because some of them may
be affixed directly to a noun, pronoun, or finite verbal form ([(e)“e],
ge“en], [nanna]), and some to a nominalized verbal form ([∞ [∞ ge“en],
[ri]), while [“uba] stands apart in that it follows a noun standing in
a dimensional case (but also may be the head of a copula).
The matter is in need of more thorough treatment.
13.1. Tnr r\n+icrr [(r)“r]
Sumerian has a suffixed quotation particle corresponding to Akkadian
-mi. [(e)“e] is a particle which may be added to both nouns and
verbs (see 4.1). It is first found in the Gudea corpus, spelled é“e(”È);
-“i occurs in Ur III, and OB and later texts (including the lexical
evidence) have -e-“e.
e-“e: mi-i KI.TA (“mi, suffixed”) NBGT I 461 (MSL 4, 147); II
13 (ibid. 149).
lú an-gin
7
ri-ba ki-gin
7
ri-ba-é“e “the person who, as you said, was
as enormous as the skies, enormous as the earth” Gudea Cyl. A v
13 (see ibid. 15).
Note: Instead of é“e, -“è was read until Thomsen 1984, 102, and it was inter­
preted as the terminative case particle (“concerning . . .”).
Ur-ni
9
-∞ gar-ra Ab-ba-sa
6
-ga-a kù in-da-tuku-“i mu-lugal . . . ga-ab-su
bí-in-dug
4
“A declared: ‘name of the king, I will repay it by . . .’,
having said (-“i) that (U. has silver with him =) he owes U. silver”
TCL 2,2557:1–6.
ge“-nú-∞ ∞ gu
10
gub-ba-ab-zé-en ga-ba-nú-e-“e “‘set up my bed, I want
to sleep’ I said” Schooldays 15; see ibid. 26, 29, . . . 41.
EDZARD_f14_156-160 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 158
158 cn\r+rn +nin+rrx
The origin and etymology of [(e)“e] are unknown; there can hardly
be a connection with the adverbiative particle (see 11).
See Falkenstein 1952, 113–130, who still thought of [e“e] as a “Potentialis- und
Irrealissuffix”.
13.2. [gˆr“rx] ([gˆi“rx])
ge“en] ([∞ The suffixed particle [∞ gi“en]) is rarely attested in context and,
therefore, its function may only be described in a preliminary way.
ge“-en, me-en-zé-en-∞ ge“-en = nìnu-man, me-en-dè-∞ ge“-en, e-ne-ne-∞
attunu-man, “unu-man “were it we, you (pl.), they” OBGT I 460–462
(MSL 4, 52).
∞ge“-en = ma-an (KI.TA) (i.e., used as a suffix) NBGT I 460 (MSL
4, 147).
Both AHw. (following GAG § 152d) and CAD M/1, 202, define
Akkadian man as a particle denoting the irrealis mood; this would
also suit the Sumerian context of [∞ge“en].
It is not yet possible to propose an etymology for [∞ge“en] ga-nam
ga-ug
5
-g(a)-en-dè-en [ga-ug-enden] ∞ ge“-en ga-an-gu
7
, ga-nam ga-ti-le-
ge“-en ga-bí-íb-∞ en-dè-en [ga-til-enden] ∞ gar “well, we might die (Akkad.
sg.), (so) let me eat (it all); (or) well, we might live (Akkad. sg.), (so)
let me store (food)” BWL 244 f.: 42–45.
Note: ∞ge“-en is on both occasions written at the beginning of a new line, as if
it were a separate word. This, however, must be a misunderstanding, since the
Babylonian grammarians defined [∞ge“en] as a suffix (KI.TA), see above.
á mu-e-da-á∞ ge“-“e-en á-á∞ gá ma-ab-“úm-mu-un-e-“e “if I would g-∞ g-∞
tell you to do something you would say ‘(how come) you are giving
me orders’” Father and Son 43 f.
rú-§ ge“-“e-en ki-ad-da-∞ g-∞ igi-§ rú-a na-an-gig-ga-∞ gá mu-e-(var. ì-)ki∞ gá-
ge“-“e-en ∞ ∞ gá-e mu-zu-“è gi
6
-an-bar
7
(NE)-ba ù-du
10
nu-mu-e-da-ku-ku-
u[n] “. . . because of you I am unable to sleep and rest at day or
at night” Father and Son 119–121.
13.3. Tnr strrixrr r\n+icrr [x\xx\]
In addition to the syntagma -X-da nu-me-a “not being with X” =
“without X” (see 5.4.2.6.a), another Sumerian expression rendering
the idea of “without” is suffixed -[nanna]. It occurs with pronouns,
nouns, and nominalized verbal forms.
EDZARD_f14_156-160 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 159
159 ros+-xovix\r \xr/on ros+-yrnn\r r\n+icrrs
me-en-dè-na-an-na = “a là n[iàti ] “without us”
me-en-zé-en-na-an-na = “a là [kunùti ] “without you”
e-ne-ne-na-an-na = “a là [“unùti ] “without them”
OBGT I 488–490 (MSL 4, 53)
It is not yet possible to propose an etymology for the particle [nanna],
but it would not be to bold to propose that the initial element
[n(a)-] is related with the [n] of negative [nu] or prohibitive [na].
PN-na-an-na lú nu-ù-da-nú-a . . . (see 12.16.1).
dub-lá-mah u
4
-ul-lí-a-ta ki-“u-tag “uku-UD “ub-ba ì-me-a-na-an-na
é-bi nu-§rú-àm “apart from the D. (where) from ancient days offerings
(and) . . . which had been neglected had not been there (and) (apart
from) that house which had not been built (Amar-Suena did build . . .)”
RIME 3/2, 254, Amar-Suena 9:3–8.
13.4. Tnr r\n+icrr [“tn\]
“ub-ba [“uba] is originally either a ¢am†u participle of “ub “to throw,
leave (behind), neglect”: “left neglected”, or an imperative: “leave
(it)” (suggestion of J. N. Postgate). In the latter case, “ub-ba would
closely follow Akkadian ezib/ezub. [“uba] secondarily became a par­
ticle expressing the idea “apart from”. [“uba] is not, however, directly
suffixed (see note below), thus not strictly fulfilling the features of
the particles collected under 13.
me-en-dè-a “ub-ba = ez[ib niàti ] “left apart from us”
“ub-ba-me-en-zé-en = ezi[b kunùti ] “you (pl.) are set apart”
e-ne-ne-a “ub-ba = ezi[b “unùti ] “left apart from them”
“ub-ba-me-en-da-nam = ezi[b niàtima] “it is we who are set
apart”
“ub-ba-me-en-za-nam = ezi[b kunùtima] “it is you (pl.) who are
set apart”
e-ne-ne-a “ub-ba-kam = ezi[b “unùtima] “it is of being left apart
from them”
OBGT I 491–496 (MSL 4, 53)
Note: In none of the six occurrences does [“uba] function as an immediately suffixed
particle. It is only the Akkadian translations that treat [“uba] on the same level
as [∞ge“en] (13.2) or [nanna] (13.3). [“uba] cannot even be defined as a suffixed
particle, because it may also stand as the head of a phrase containing a copula.
EDZARD_f14_156-160 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 160
160 cn\r+rn +nin+rrx
For more lexical evidence see CAD E 429 ezib (prep.) lex. sect.
gá-ka-e(!) “ub-ba é-
d
Nanna-kam ¢∞ mu
d
Ba-ú nin-∞ garzaÜ ki-gub-ba
nu-tuku-a = a““um Bau bèltia ezub ina bìt Suen parßa u mazzàzam là
i“ûma “that apart from that which relates to my mistress Bau, I have
no office (or) position in the house of the Moongod” (transl. of CAD
E 429) PBS 1/2, 135: 13–15 (see Falkenstein 1952, 122; van Dijk
1953, 128).
Note: [“uba] here, as in some of the lexical entries quoted above governs a dimen­
sional case: [mu-Bau nin∞g(u)-ak-a-e(!)].
13.5. Tnr strrixrr r\n+icrr [ni]
-ri occurs after nominalized verbal forms ending in -a. Krecher 1965
defined it as an “isolierende Postposition”, i.e., an affixed particle
highlighting the meaning of the verbal form. Attinger 1993, 260,
who preliminarily adopted Krecher’s term, showed that -ri may stand
in variation with ablative -ta “after”.
Note: It is unlikely that the respective particle [ri] should be identical in mean­
ing and function with the deictic particle [ri], treated above, 7.4. For lexical
occurrences of the latter see OBGT I 785–786, and 789–791 (MSL 4, 58) and
OBGT XIV 4–5 (ibid. 122), and see above, 7.4.
mu-5-àm mu-10-àm ba-e-zal-la-ri “after five, ten years had passed”
GEN 40 = 83 = 127; see Enmerkar 430 and Green 1978, 145.
“à-zu im-mi-ib-du
11
-ga-ri//“à-zu mi-ni-ib-du
11
-ga-ta “after your heart
spoke about it” Attinger 1993, 260.
All of Krecher’s (1965) and Green’s (1978, 145) examples favour
an ablative interpretation of suffixed [ri].
If that be the case, the choice of -ri rather than -ta may have
been a matter of style rather than grammar.
At any rate, more research is needed.
161
EDZARD_f15_161-165 4/28/03 9:40 AM Page 161
CHAPTER FOURTEEN
CONJUNCTIONS AND SUBJUNCTIONS
Sumerian as an agglutinative language has few conjunctions or sub-
junctions, and some of them are actually loans from Akkadian. In
fact, Sumerian has no original word for “and”. As for subjunctions,
their number is restricted, too, because what is expressed by a con­
junction in a non-agglutinative language such as Semitic or Indo-
European is mostly represented by a nominalized verbal form plus
a dimensional particle in Sumerian, i.e., Ki“ . . . im-ug
5
-ga-ta “after
Ki“ . . . had been killed” (Curse of Akkade 2) where the nominalized
verbal form [im-ug-a] is put in the ablative, with [ta], in order to
denote temporal distance.
In the following, conjunctions and, specially, subjunctions are
treated without assigning them to strict grammatical categories, because
they are essentially atypical for Sumerian.
14.1. C
14.1.1. [u]
Sumerian borrowed Semitic [wa] “and” in its Akkadian form ù [u].
Akkadian [u] has not so far been the subject of a detailed study,
nor has Sumerian [u]. [u] occurs much less in Akkadian than it does
in its cognate Semitic languages, partly because it was replaced—for
linking sentences—by [ma]. Therefore, Sumerian ù can hardly be
expected to occur more frequently.
Note: wa (or ù) is still much more frequent in the Ebla (24
th
cent. B.C.) variant
of Old Akkadian.
The allegedly oldest occurrences of ù in pre-Sargonic Sumerian (cf.
Thomsen 1984, 16 and 83) are actually found in Akkadian contexts
(Ebla and Abu Íalàbì¢): IAS no. 326//ARET 2 no. 6). It should be
stressed, however, that the Ebla scribe used ù instead of the wa (PI)
otherwise preferred for “and” in literary Ebla-Akkadian texts. The
EDZARD_f15_161-165 4/28/03 9:40 AM Page 162
162 cn\r+rn rotn+rrx
scribe may, therefore, have used Ù as a Sumerogram which would,
at least indirectly, attest to ù as an Akkadian loan in pre-Sargonic
Sumerian. The question needs more attention.
ù “and, and also” definitively gained currency in Sumerian by the
time of Ur III, becoming as Sumerian as, more recently, Arabic wa
“and” became Turkish in the form of ve.
ù frequently occurs connecting two personal names (e.g., NG no.
7:17–18, and see Sollberger 1966, 182 s.v. u 1:2–4), but it is not
used in the enumeration of the names of judges or witnesses. By Ur
III times, PN
1
ù PN
2
practically replaced former PN
1
PN
2
-bi(-da) (see
4.2.(7) and 5.4.2.6).
Moreover, ù may connect PN and a pronoun, two nouns, or two
verbal forms (see Sollberger 1966 ibid. 1.27).
ù e∞ger-ab-ba-ne-ne ì-ba-e-ne “and they will also divide the estate
of their father” NG no. 7:20.
The relation of sentence-connecting ù to the more traditional asyn­
detic construction is in need of further elucidation, especially with
regard to diachrony.
14.1.2. [ma]
By the OB period, apart from ù Akkadian connective -ma has also
been borrowed.
dub-∞ rú ì-[sar] ì-til-ma mu-gub-ba-∞ gu
10
ì-§ gu
10
ma-an-gub-bu-u“ “I
set up my tablet, [wrote] (it), finished (it), and then they put before
me (my standing lines =) the model lines (to be copied by me)”
Schooldays 5–6.
Cf. A. Falkenstein, WO 1/3 (1948) 176.
14.2. Stn¡txc+ioxs
14.2.1. [uda]
[uda] is spelled ù-da or u
4
-da in Pre-Sargonic Laga“ inscriptions. If
the etymology of the subjunction were actually the locative of the
word u
4
(-d) “day”, i.e. u
4
-da “at the day; today”, the word would
have become a subjunction secondarily. It would be difficult to
explain, however, why in that case a variant ù-da should have
occurred which is graphically more complex.
EDZARD_f15_161-165 4/28/03 9:40 AM Page 163
163 cox¡txc+ioxs \xr stn¡txc+ioxs
[uda] introduces a clause with the verb in conjugation pattern 1
(intransitive) or 2a (transitive) (see 12.7), both translated by us as pre­
sent tense. Such a clause is followed by a second clause equally with
the verb in pattern 1 or 2a. The whole complex may be interpreted
as a conditional sentence construction consisting of a protasis (“if A”)
and an apodosis (“then B”).
u
4
-da ka-ka-na níg-érim ba-∞ gá

gá-∞
ge“
gag ka-ka-na “è-gaz “if she (the
seller) (puts evil in her mouth =) behaves dishonestly/treacherously
a wooden peg will be driven into her” SR no. 43 vi 1–2.
Note: The exact meaning of the sanction is unknown.
ù-da (var. u
4
-da) mu-“è-sa
10
-sa
10
[mû“e-sasa-(e)n] kù “à-∞ gá a-sa
6
-ga lá­
ma “if you (want to) sell it to me, pay me silver (which is pleasant
for my heart =) which I find appropriate” Ukg. 4–5 xi 7–10//xi
38–xii 3.
Note: Thomsen 1984, 85, offers two OB occurrences of u
4
-da for “when”, “if ”;
for both, however, a translation “today” would fit, especially so in EnmEns. 26
where the verb following u
4
-da is in conjugation pattern 2b (preterite).
14.2.2. [tukumbi]
[tukumbi] “if ” graphically is one of the most elaborate “dirig” com­
pounds in the spelling of Sumerian words. Apart from final -bi, it
is usually written with four elements: ”U.GAR.TUR.LÁ, whose ori­
gin has not yet been explained.
tu-ku-um-bi ”U.GAR.TUR.LÁ-bi = “u-um-mu Diri V 120 see CAD
”/3, 275 “umma lex. sect.
tukum alone occurs in èn tukum-“è “how long still; until when?”
(note LamSumUr 451) which is formed after more frequent èn-“è
(adi mati ) “until when?”.
tukum, instead of tukum-bi, is found in tukum “e ì-∞gál, RA-na-
an-“úm-mu, tukum na-∞ gál, é-ZAR-ta, ha-mu-na-ra-pà-dè “if there is
barley, let him give it to him; if there is not, let him find it for him
from the . . .” Sollberger 1966 no. 367 r. 2'–6'.
Note: tukum alone may just be a scribal idiosyncrasy (note, too, the unusual
RA-, instead of *ha-, in 3') because otherwise the corpus of Ur III letters regu­
larly has tukum-bi (see Sollberger 1966 p. 181).
Beginning with the Code of Ur-Namma (Ur III), tukum-bi regularly
introduces conditional sentences. The verb of the protasis is in con­
jugation pattern 1 (intransitive) or 2 b (transitive preterite), exactly
corresponding to the preterite (more rarely perfect) found with Akka­
dian “umma “if ”, clearly another Sumero-Akkadian areal feature.
EDZARD_f15_161-165 4/28/03 9:40 AM Page 164
164 cn\r+rn rotn+rrx
tukum-bi lú-ù sag∞ ∞ge“ bí-in-ra lú-bi ì-gaz-e-dam “if a person com­
mits a murder that person will have to be killed” Ur-Namma Code
C 52–54 (§1), see Wilcke 2002, 311.
14.2.3. [ena]
en-na is parallel to Akkadian adi in its dual functions of (a) a sub-
junction and (b) a preposition.
There is no strict rule observable for the morphological behav­
iour of the verb dependent on the subjunction.
(1) en-na + finite verbal form: en-na àm-du igi-∞ gu
10
-“è enim-bi
a-bala-e “until he will have come here (and) the respective matter
will have been brought to me” Sollberger 1966 no. 125:8–9; see
ibid. no. 68:4 (i-im-du).
(2) en-na + nominalized verbal form: gu
4
. . . en-na ab-lah
5
-a “oxen . . .
(up to =) as many as could be brought” Ur-Namma A 87.
éren en-na ba-ug
5
-ga “soldiers (up to =) . . . as many as have
died” ibid. 139.
Note: Susa var. has in-na ba-¢“ubÜ-e; see Flückiger-Hawker 1999, 125.
(3) en-na + nominalized verbal form with terminative case particle:
PN
1
ù PN
2
en-na igi-∞gu
10
-“è di in-da-an-du
11
-ga-a“ [i-n-da-n-dug-
a-“(e)] “until PN
1
will have had (his) lawsuit with PN
2
in my pres­
ence” Sollberger 1966 no. 113: 3–5,
en-na na-an-ga-ti-la-a“ ARN 7:8–11, see above 12.12.2.5.
en-na may be used with a noun, thus virtually functioning as a
preposition: en-na sukud-§ rá-bi “(the walls) to their highest point”
Curse Akk. 227.
Since the etymology of [en] in en-na (or of [ena] as such) is
unknown, it is difficult to describe the difference (if any) between
[ena] “until” and en/èn-“è “until when, how long?”.
14.2.4. [mu]
mu “year” in Sumerian year formulae may, from Gudea of Laga“
onward, be followed by a finite, not nominalized, verbal form. In such
a case, mu may be taken as a subjunction; see above 12.16.3 end.
EDZARD_f15_161-165 4/28/03 9:40 AM Page 165
165 cox¡txc+ioxs \xr stn¡txc+ioxs
14.2.5. [iginzu]
igi-zu, i-gi
4
-in-zu (and other variant spellings, see Wilcke 1968, 229)
“as if ” mainly introduces hypothetical comparisons.
alam igi-zu
d
Nin-∞ gír-su-ka-kam “a statue, as if it were one of
Ningirsu” Gudea Stat. B vii 58–59.
Note: See (Th. Jacobsen, apud) Wilcke 1968, 232, and correct Edzard 1997, 36.
[d]ur
9
“u àm-kar-kar-re, i-gi
4
-in-zu ní-te-a-ni-“è, lugal-a-ni-“è-àm “the
donkey stallion runs along as if it were for himself, (but) it is for his
master” UET 6, 275; see J. Bauer, Fs. W. W. Hallo (1993) 39.
Note: The etymology of [iginzu] is still unknown, as Wilcke’s discussion of alleged
“the eye noticed” makes clear (Wilcke 1968, 238 f.).
C. Wilcke, Das modale Adverb I-GI
4
-IN-ZU im Sumerischen, JNES 27 (1968)
229–242, with additions added to offprint; Attinger 1993, 170.
166
EDZARD_f16_166-170 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 166
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
167
EDZARD_f16_166-170 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 167
CHAPTER FIFTEEN
EXCLAMATIONS
“Exclamations” should be understood as a catch-all term for calls,
interjections, or even such expressions as “yes” or “no”.
Although exclamations are, strictly speaking, part of the lexicon
rather than of grammar, we will still briefly quote the most impor­
tant ones. The exact meaning of an exclamation being highly depen­
dent on its cultural context, it is difficult today to arrive at a strict
definition (and delimitation from related terms). Therefore, many
translations have to be taken as approximations only.
The following list of transliterated terms is arranged alphabetically.
15.1. []
gu
For a “woe”, “ouch”, see Krecher 1966, 145.
10
. . . a-nir im-∞ gá-ne “ouch, my neck, . . . they are wail- a gú-∞ gá-∞
ing” GEN 155.
15.2. []
a-la-la, exclamation of a positive character, also used as a work cry.
See PSD A/1, 100.
Note: [1] in exclamations (see also 15.6) is a universal phenomenon. Cf. only
Classical Greek élalã, Hebrew hallelùyà, Spanish olè, French hélas, olàlà, German
hallo, halali; ululation, etc., whatever the etymology of the individual expression.
15.3. []
a-lu-lu “woe”, see PSD A/1, 107.
EDZARD_f16_166-170 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 168
168 cn\r+rn rir+rrx
15.4. [\rriri]
al-li-li(-àm-ma), interjection of unclear meaning, found in context
with a-ù-àm-ma (see 15.5) and ù-li-li (15.13).
S. N. Kramer, Or. 54 (1985) 120 i 23–24, and p. 128; see PSD A/3, 153.
15.5. [\t\]
!
a-ù-a, a-ù-u
8
-a, a-ù-àm-ma are exclamations partly of a lamenting,
partly of a soothing character; see PSD A/1, 199, a-u
3
-a A, and cf.
above, 15.4, and below, 15.12.
15.6. [rrrt, rr\r\, irt, irtr\vv\]
e-el-lu, el-lu, el-lú, e-el-lum, e-la-lu, i-li(-a), i-lu-lam-ma, etc. Krecher
1966, 148, with fn. 433, collected references to exclamations all based
on the voiced lateral [1]; see note to 15.2.
15.7. [o\x\]
ga-na is an encouraging, often self-encouraging, exclamation, “come
on now”, “let’s get moving”, etc. The first syllable [ga] is most prob­
ably identical with the modal cohortative indicator [ga] (see 12.11.3).
ga-na ga-na-ab-du
11
“come on then, I will have to tell it to her”
Gudea Cyl. A iii 22 f.
15.8. [nr\v]
hé-àm “let (it) be”, i.e., the copula (3
rd
sg.) prefixed by the modal
precative indicator [he] (see 12.11.5), has become the Sumerian term
for “yes” (Akkadian annu(m)).
For hé-àm du
11
(-g)/e “to assent, to say ‘yes’” see Attinger 1993,
552 f. with bibl.
Another (more emphatic?) assertion is na-nam; see 12.11.10.
EDZARD_f16_166-170 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 169
rxcr\v\+ioxs 169
15.9. [ixt]
in-nu “no” most probably may be explained as the negative indi­
cator [nu] (see 12.11.2) prefixed by indicator [i/e] (see 12.9); [nu]
would in this case have been treated as a verbal base.
in-nu ní te-ba-ab in-nu té“ tuku-ba-ab “no, be respectful to them
(= the words I recited to you), no, be ashamed to oppose them”
Enkita and Enki-hegal 102 (see Wilcke 1969, 82 fn. 76).
15.10. [i U+t]
i
d
Utu “woe, O Sungod” is found in contexts where someone desper­
ately turns for help to the Sun-god as the supreme god of justice.
lú-bi ì
d
Utu ì-e “that person will cry ‘woe, O Sungod’” Ukg. 6
ii 14'.
Strangely enough, the expression is found, thereafter, only as a
noun, i
d
Utu “complaint”. It may not even be excluded that the
above quoted Ukg. passage has already to be understood as “that
person will lay a complaint”.
níg-érim i
d
Utu ∞gìri bí(-n)-ús “(Gudea) (set (his) foot on the neck
of =) eliminated evil (and) complaint” Gudea Cyl. B xviii 11.
i-
d
Utu was eventually understood a genitival compound: lú-i-
d
Utu-
ka [lu-i-Utu-(a)k-a(k)] “person of complaint” = “oppressor” as in Ur-
Namma C 35 (together with ní-zuh “thief ”.).
15.11. [vrr(i)r\]
me-li-e-a (pronounced as three or four syllables?) “alas”, = Akkadian
inimma, inimmu (CAD I s.v.). No etymology available for the expres­
sion in either language. me-li-e-a may stand at the beginning or the
end of a phrase.
me-li-e-a me-a tu“-ù-dè-en me-a gub-bu-dè-en “woe is me, where
shall I sit down, where shall I stand?” Ur Lament 294, and see 295,
304, etc.
enim-du
11
-ga-“e-“a
4
-∞gu
10
me-li-e-a: inimmu amat iqbû u“admimanni “the
bitter word pronounced for me, alas” (Akkadian “alas, the word he
said has made me moan”), see CAD I 148 inimma bil. sect.
EDZARD_f16_166-170 4/28/03 1:01 PM Page 170
170 cn\r+rn rir+rrx
15.12. [t\]
The great variety of spellings for [ua] “woe” has been collected by
Krecher, 1966, 114 f.: ù-a, ú-a, u
5
-a, ù-u
8
-a, etc. While [ua] is essen­
tially a sound of lamentation (with its labial element comparable to
Latin vae, English woe, German o weh, Yiddish auvay, etc.), it also
occurs as a soothing sound in [ua aua].
ù-a erim
6
-ma-∞gu
10
“alas for my treasure house” VS 1, 25 ii 6 f.
(Krecher 1966, 54).
u
5
-a a-ù-a “sleep baby”, beginning of the Sumerian lullaby (see
Attinger 1993, 49 s.v. “Lullaby”).
The different sounds which probably lie behind the different nota­
tion of the two [u] sounds as u
5
and ù, cannot be reconstructed.
Note: B. Landsberger, MSL 2 (1951) 29 f., tentatively suggested for u
8
“ein
langgedehntes offenes o” (as in English paw, law).
The onomatopoeia of [ua aua] in lullabies may easily be rendered
by corresponding soothing sounds in other languages, such as, e.g.,
Russian bayubayubayubáy or German eiapopeia.
15.13. [triri]
munus-ù-li-li = zammirtu “ulili woman” (Akkadian “songstress”) Lu
III ii 16 (MSL 12, 124).
Note: The lexical context has to do with singing and performance, but the exact
meaning of [ulili] cannot be reconstructed.
171
EDZARD_f17_171-172 4/28/03 9:41 AM Page 171
CHAPTER SIXTEEN
EMESAL
Emesal (eme-sal) is a sociolinguistic variety of Sumerian attested for
the speech of women or goddesses and of the “cantor” (gala). The
pronunciation is supported by the Akkadian loanword ummisallu (AHw.
s.v.; see also emesallu, CAD E). The meaning of the term is not com­
pletely clear; maybe “thin, fine tongue” (sal = raqqu) referred to a
highpitched voice; cf. Russian tonkij golos “thin (high-pitched) voice”;
see also Krecher 1967b, 87 fn. 1).
Note: The fact that the “cantor” used a form of speech otherwise attested for female
persons has caused much speculation: Was the gala a eunuch or an effeminate
male? See the discussion in Schretter 1990, 124–36. It is, however, well known that
the modern counter-tenor or even altus is sung by sexually normal male persons.
Emesal is first attested in the early OB period. It is found in a one-
to-one relation with eme-gi
7
(-r) in phonology, morphology, vocabu­
lary, and syntax. The main difference between the standard form of
Sumerian (eme-gi
7
(-r)) and emesal regards sounds and lexicon.
Emesal is spelled phonetically (e.g., zé-eb [zeb] = dùg “good,
sweet”) or semi-phonetically (e.g.,
d
Mu-ul-líl [Mullil] = Enlil where
the second element líl is never given as *li-il). It is quite probable
that also traditional eme-gi
7
(-r) spelling could be read in emesal when
required by context.
Only a few examples out of the very ample evidence for emesal
will be offered here, the whole material having been collected by
Krecher 1967b and Schretter 1990, with special attention to the
phonetic evidence.
eme-gi
7
(-r) eme-sal
a-ga a-ba “back, rear”
dùg zé-eb “good, sweet”
ga- da- (modal indicator: cohortative)
enim e-ne-è∞g “word”
nir-∞gál “e-er-ma-al “person of authority”
di∞gir dìm-me-er “deity”
EDZARD_f17_171-172 4/28/03 9:41 AM Page 172
172 cn\r+rn six+rrx
lú mu-lu “person”
nin, ere“ ga-“a-an, ga“an “lady”
en ù-mu-un, umun “lord”
túm ir “to bring”
a-na-àm ta-àm “what (is it)?”
ma-an-“úm ∞gá-ba-zé-em “he gave me”
im-ma-da-te in-ga-da-te “he approached me”
Apart from heteronymy (e.g., [lu] : [mulu], [nin] : [ga“an]) there are
consonantal and vocalic correspondences (e.g., [g] : [b], [g] : [d],
[∞ g] : [m], [g] : [z], [u] : [e]). In spite of parallel occurrences of some
of those correspondences, emesal forms are not predictable for us.
The two verbal forms quoted last show that also the string of prefixed
morphemes may have been affected by emesal variation.
Apart from the usage of emesal in literary texts, individual eme­
sal words and name variants are also found in lexical lists (see
Schretter 1990, 17–30). A special “Emesal Vocabulary” of post-OB
date has been edited in MSL 4 (1956) 3–44. It consists of three
“Tablets” the first of which contains divine names and the second
and third offer nouns, a few verbal bases and verbal forms as well
as a very restricted number of pronouns and numerals.
Krecher 1967b; Schretter 1990 (see also the review of J. Black, OLZ 87 [1992]
382–85); Langenmayr 1992.
173
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 173
CHAPTER SEVENTEEN
THE SUMERO
-
AKKADIAN LINGUISTIC AREA
Sumerian and Akkadian coexisted in a region of about the size of
modern Belgium during most of the 3
rd
millennium B.C. and during
the first quarter of the 2
nd
millennium. Many speakers of the two
languages then lived in closest proximity. The final result of mutual
contacts was—apart from extensive borrowing of vocabulary—a con­
siderable divergence of Akkadian from the traditional “Semitic” lan­
guage type; and—on the other hand—non-Sumerian, “Semitic”, traits
in Sumerian. These have often been interpreted as faulty treatment
of “classical” Sumerian by scribes who—allegedly—no longer would
have been used to, and aware of, the typical grammatical categories
of Sumerian. Consequently, some scholars have maintained that
Sumerian had already ceased to be a living spoken language as early
as the time of the Third Dynasty of Ur.
But what actually happened was the building up of a “linguistic
area” or “convergence area” where “speakers can switch from one
code to another with a minimum of additional learning” (Gumperz/
Wilson 1971, 154). Seen in such a context, many features of both
Sumerian and Akkadian may be easily explained.
17.1.
The following diagram tries to demonstrate what happens when lan­
guages A and B enter such a “convergence area”. A here stands for
Sumerian and B for Akkadian. The example illustrated is the com­
mon Semitic conjunction [wa], borrowed by Sumerian as [u] (ù).
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 174
174 cn\r+rn sryrx+rrx
*Pre-Sumerian Semitic
(0
Sumerian Akkadian
)|| ù ù ||
(usage restricted, (usage restricted as
not productive) compared with other
Semitic languages)
wa
Note: Common Semitic wa “and”, “and also”, often used as a “sentence opener”
(Satzweiser, see Richter 1970, 39.84), is still attested in its full set of functions in
Ebla Akkadian. In Mesopotamian proper, its usage is restricted, and as a con­
nector of phrases it has mainly been replaced by enclitic -ma. Akkadian wa in its
(pronunciation and) spelling ù has been borrowed into Sumerian where it is
mainly used to connect nouns (see 14.1.1). Sumerian itself lacks a connecting par­
ticle “and”. The relatively rare usage of ù in Sumerian then most probably had
its reverse effect on Akkadian where ù is of limited application as compared to
the usage of wa in the other Semitic languages. This, then, may be called a real
areal feature.
We will enumerate a number of other such features attesting to the
Sumero-Akkadian “convergence area”.
17.2.
The subject-object-verb (SOV) word order is common to both
Sumerian and Mesopotamian Akkadian as against more traditional
Eblaite Akkadian where we often encounter VOS (cf. Edzard 1984,
115 f.). The older V(O)S order is still found—as an ‘archaism’—in
Ur III and OB personal names, e.g., iddin-Sin “the Moongod gave
(the child)”. When, however, the Akkadian verbal form of a PN is
set in the ventive, the word order S(O)V prevails: Sin-iddinam “the
Moongod gave me (the child)”.
The SOV word order is also maintained in Akkadian when the
subject is extended by the addition of a relative clause, this leading
to veritable “Schachtelsatz” constructions (see Poebel 1947, 23–42).
The same holds for the insertion of infinitive constructions which
stand between S and V. Therefore, most probably Akkadian word
order was influenced by Sumerian.
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 175
175 +nr stvrno-\kk\ri\x rixotis+ic \nr\
17.3. Vrx+iyr
The formation of the Akkadian ventive, permeating the whole ver­
bal system except for the non-finite forms, has not developed on its
own. The ventive : non-ventive dichotomy of the finite verbal system
of Sumerian has left its impact on neighbouring Akkadian where a
post-verbal morpheme [an], inherited from common Semitic affirmative
[an], was given a new function (cf. Pedersén 1989, 433–36). The
personal names Nanna-man“um “the Moongod gave (the child) to
me” and Sin-iddinam (ditto) may be translated back and forth in
word order (SV) and morpheme by morpheme.
Note: Common Semitic [an] became [am] in Akkadian in word-final position
only, e.g., iddin-am. Otherwise [an] maintained its identity except that the [n]
was assimilated to a following consonant: iddin-aN-kum ¡ iddinakkum “he gave to
you”, etc.
17.4. Loss or Stvrni\x [H] \xr or Akk\ri\x [n], [˙], \xr [']
It may be assumed that the reduction of the Akkadian consonantal
phoneme inventory, as inherited from common Semitic, was due to
Sumerian substratum. By Ur III times at the latest, [h], [˙], and [']
can no longer be substantiated for Akkadian phonology.
Dead loss in Hilgert 2002.
One cannot, on the other hand, escape the conclusion that Sumerian
originally had an unvoiced laryngeal phoneme which we tentatively
transliterated as [H] (see 3.1.2, p. 20). So if phoneme reduction in
Akkadian is being considered as due to Sumerian substratum, loss
of [H] on the Sumerian side would be said to follow a similar ten­
dency. Nothing could be more eloquent for an areal situation.
17.5. Pnn\sroroov or Stvrni\x \xr Akk\ri\x n\s vtcn
ix covvox
gú “ub “to throw the neck” = a¢am nadûm “to throw the arm” =
“to be neglectful”.—∞gé“tu gub “to set the ear” = uznam “akànum (ditto)
= “to be attentive”.—igi ∞gar “to set the eye” = pànam “akànum “to
set the face” = “to prepare to (. . .)”.—a-“à è “to bring out =) rent a
field” = eqlam “ùßûm (ditto).—nam-X AG = X-ùtam epè“um “to exercise
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 176
176 cn\r+rn sryrx+rrx
the X-ship” = “to follow a profession, trade”.—“à-“è gíd “to draw
to the heart” = “to consider carefully” = ana libbim “adàdum (ditto).—
enim-ma tu“ “to (sit to the word =) obey” = ana awàti wa“àbum
(ditto).—ù-ma-a-du
11
“would you please say” (polite imperative, see
12.12.1.2) = qibima “say and . . .”.
17.6. Arrric\+iox or +nr Akk\ri\x s+\+iyr cox¡to\+iox
r\++rnx +o stns+\x+iyrs
In Akkadian, the conjugation pattern ßabtàku, ßabtàta “I am, you are
holding” may also be applied to substantives: belèku, “arràta “I am
lord”, “you are king”. The pattern as such has its counterpart in
the “perfect” of, e.g., Hebrew, Arabic, or Ethiopic, where it is
restricted to verbs. The application to substantives in Akkadian is
most probably due to influence of the Sumerian copula (me, see
12.7.1.1) which is mainly used with substantives: dub-sar-me-en “I
am/you are a scribe”.
Note: Application of the Sumerian copula has not, however, been completely
g-∞ gu
10
-me-en “you are my beloved” has no reflected in Akkadian. So, e.g., ki-á∞ gá-∞
such Akkadian correspondence as *naràmtì-àti.
17.7. C\sr svs+rv
There is a conspicuous phonetic similarity between Sumerian [(e)“(e)],
mark of the terminative case particle (5.4.2.8), and the ending of the
Akkadian terminative-adverbial [i“]. Although there can be no doubt
of the Semitic background of Akkadian [i“], the fact that the Akkadian
case was maintained throughout the millennia may be due to the
parallel existence of a case with comparable function(s) in Sumerian.
Pedersén 1989, 430–33.
Note: The theory of an original ergativity of Semitic (cf. Diakonoff 1965, 1988;
Kienast 2001, 179; see also p. 141) rests on the misinterpretation of the Akkadian
locative-adverbial case in -um, seemingly identical with the nominative in -um.
The two authors did not consider the important evidence of the construct state
used for Akkadian nominative and locative-adverbial. In the first case -um is elim­
inated or replaced by a Stützvokal: bèlum ¡ bèl, wardum ¡ warad, napi“tum ¡
napi“t(i ); in the second case, -um (or more recent -u) is maintained in the con­
struct state: ßillum Sin “in the shade of the Moongod”, or the [m] is assimilated
to a following consonant: ßilu““u “in his shade”. So, the two cases have to be
strictly kept apart.
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 177
177 +nr stvrno-\kk\ri\x rixotis+ic \nr\
17.8. x\v- \xr -ù+tv
Sumerian and Akkadian both have a nominal device to express the
abstract of title, profession, or adjective: nam-lugal = “arrùtum “all
that has to do with a ruler” = “king-ship”. Both in Sumerian and
Akkadian, the respective pattern is fully productive.
17.9. Tnr rrtn\r or \r¡rc+iyrs
In Akkadian, a restricted number of adjectives (mostly indicating
some kind of dimension) form a plural where the middle radical of
the root is lengthened: rabûm “big” ¡ rabbûtum, arkum “long” ¡ arrakù-
tum, ße¢rum “small” ¡ ße¢¢erùtum, etc.; see Reiner 1966, 64. One may
easily compare this type of ‘internal’ plural with the reduplicated
Sumerian adjectives which, following a substantive, indicate plural
(5.3.7, and see 4.1.1.b), e.g., di∞gir-gal-gal-(e-ne) = ilù rabbûtum “the
great gods”.
Note: Both AHw. and CAD R offer an entry rabbû (sg.), conceding, however,
that it mainly occurs in the plural.
17.10. Vrnn\r voor
Akkadian has a set of verbal moods which it is easier to compare
to the system of Sumerian moods (cf. chart on p. 112 f.) than to
the moods known from other Semitic languages. To quote but one
example: There is a complete correspondence between Sumerian
positive affirmative 1, occurring in the 1
st
and 3
rd
p. and built on
the (¢am†u) conjugation patterns 1 or 2b (see 12.11.7) and the Akkadian
positive affirmative 1: lù aprus, lù iprus, equally occurring in the 1
st
and 3
rd
p. and built on the preterite (iprus) conjugation pattern.
For more details see Edzard 1973, 121–141, specially pp. 140 f.
Note: Edzard’s article has a considerable number of misprints because the author
had no opportunity to read and return proofs.
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 178
178 cn\r+rn sryrx+rrx
17.11. Mt+t\r nonnovixo or rrxic\r i+rvs ix Stvrni\x
\xr Akk\ri\x
This topic, worth a whole book, can only be dealt with here very
briefly. It should also be kept in mind that (mutual) lexical borrowing
may be—but is not necessarily—a symptom of an areal situation.
The CAD volumes A, B, D, E, G, H, I/J, K, L, M, N, Q , R,
S, Í, ”, and Z (with P, T, ˇ, and U/W still missing) include ca.
13.630 Akkadian lemmata. Of these, about 980 are Sumerian loan­
words (ca. 320 of which are only attested in lexical lists). This count
yields a little above 7% of Sumerian loanwords in the Akkadian
vocabulary.
Note: The above figures have been added from the author’s CAD reviews in ZA
53 (1959) to 90 (2000) (see M. Krebernik/M. P. Streck, ZA 91 [2001] 13–20).
In spite of the author’s own skepticism on the exactitude of these figures, they
have never so far been challenged.
Unfortunately, no corresponding data may so far be offered for
Akkadian loanwords in Sumerian because Sumerian lexicography is
still in its infancy.
Note: PSD B was published in 1984 whereas PSD A (1992 ff.) is still incomplete.
The following data of PSD B, while not negligible, are in no way eligible for a
projection on the situation of Akkadian loanwords in Sumerian in general. There
are only about 13 words of Akkadian origin among ca. 350 entries in PSD B; of
these 13, 5 occur only in lexical lists. This count yields 3,7% of Akkadian loan­
words for Sumerian words beginning with the letter B. We must repeat that the
figure can in no way be regarded as representative for the vocabulary as a whole.
179
EDZARD_f18_173-179 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 179
CHAPTER EIGHTEEN
SUMMARY—AND WHAT IS STILL MISSING?
The present grammar, in its essential aspects, is traditionally descrip­
tive. It is not under the obligation of a more recent method—gen-
erative, structural, or transformational. It is far from perfect as, e.g.,
in contrast to A. Falkenstein’s monumental “Gudea” grammar (1949,
1950), it lacks a section—or chapters—on syntax, the stepchild of
more recent grammars. However, quite a few features that would
traditionally figure in a “syntax” (the description of how parts of
speech as well as phrases are “set together”, but also of the way by
which parts of speech and phrases are morphologically marked for
their functions), have been included in the above chapters: e.g., func­
tions and morpho-syntactical behaviour of the case particles (5.4),
the system of the modal and connecting indicators (12.11, 12.12),
the vast complex of the non-finite verbal forms (12.14), or the nom­
inalisation of finite verbal forms (12.16).
On the other hand, the author may much too often have left the
impression of Sumerian as one uniform block instead of a living
being with a marked diachronic evolution. At any rate, a historical
grammar of Sumerian still has to be written.
Thirdly, the author has endeavoured, as often as possible, not to
describe Sumerian as a completely isolated body, but in connection
with Akkadian, the language of those inhabitants of Mesopotamia
who were the immediate neighbours of the Sumerians for a whole
millennium or even longer.
The author will not enumerate here topics that would have mer­
ited inclusion in a grammar of Sumerian, such as, e.g., word for­
mation and nominal compounds, morphological variation (both
nominal and verbal) in different manuscripts of a given line of a lit­
erary composition, or literary style in general. There is no end of
addenda—and corrigenda.
Finally, according to the witty remark of the late I. M. Diakonoff
that there are as many kinds of Sumerian as there are authors of
Sumerian grammars, I freely admit the highly subjective character
of my own effort.
180
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 180
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
181
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 181
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Note: Bibliographical items that have been quoted above unabridged are not repeated
here.
B. Alster 1974: The Instructions of Suruppak. A Sumerian Proverb Collection
(= Mesopotamia 2).
id. 1991–93: The Sumerian Folktale of the Three Ox-Drivers of Adab, JCS 43–45,
27–38.
J. Aro 1961: Die akkadischen Infinitivkonstruktionen (= StudOr. 26).
P. Attinger 1993: Eléments de linguistique sumérienne. La construction de du
11
/e/di
“dire” (= OBO Sonderband).
id. 1995: Les préfixes absolutifs de la première et de la deuxième personne sin­
gulier dans les formes marû ergatives, ZA 75, 161–78.
J. Bauer 1993: Ein sumerisches Sprichwort, in: (ed. M. E. Cohen et al.) Fs. W. W.
Hallo, 39–41.
J. Black 1984: Sumerian Grammar in Babylonian Theory (= StudPohl SM 12); id.
2
1991.
id. 2003: (in press) (on Sumerian adjectives) in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa.
A. Cavigneaux 1978: L’essence divine, JCS 30, 177–85.
M. Civil 1974 see W. Heimpel 1974 and J. N. Postgate 1974.
id., 1976: Lexicography, in: (ed. St. Lieberman) Fs. Th. Jacobsen (= AS 20) 123–57.
M. E. Cohen 1975: ur.sag.me.“ár.ur
4
. A ”irnam“ubba of Ninurta, WO 8, 22–36.
F. d’Agostino 1990: Il sistema verbale sumerico nei testi lessicali di Ebla (= StudSem.
NS 7).
W. Deeter 1963: Armenisch und die kaukasischen Sprachen, HdOr. 1. Abt. Bd. 7,
1–79.
A. Deimel 1924: sumerische Grammatik (= Or. SP 9–13); id.
2
1939.
I. M. Diakonoff 1983: Towards a History of Mathematical Speculation, JAOS 103,
83–93.
J. J. A. van Dijk 1953: La sagesse suméro-accadienne. Recherches sur les genres
littéraires des textes sapientiaux.
R. M. W. Dixon 1994: Ergativity.
D. O. Edzard 1967: Das sumerische Verbalmorphem /ed/ in den alt- und neu-
sumerischen Texten, HSAO 1, 29–62.
id. 1970: Die bukànum-Formel der altbabylonischen Kaufverträge und ihre sumerische
Entsprechung, ZA 60, 8–53.
id. 1971: ¢am†u, marû und freie Reduplikation beim sumerischen Verbum, ZA 61,
208–32.
id. 1972: (as 1971) ZA 62, 1–34.
id. 1973: Die Modi beim älteren akkadischen Verbum, Or. 42, 121–41.
id. 1976a: (as 1971) ZA 66, 45–61.
id. 1976b: Zum sumerischen Eid, AS 20 (Fs. Th. Jacobsen), 63–98.
id. 1976c: “Du hast mir gegeben”, “ich habe dir gegeben”. Über das sumerische
Verbum sum, WO 8, 159–77.
id. 1980: Sumerisch 1 bis 10 in Ebla, StudEb. 3, 122–27.
id. 1984: Zur Syntax der Ebla-Texte, in: (ed. P. Fronzaroli) Studies on the Language
of Ebla (= QuadSem. 13) 101–16.
id. 1990: Gilgame“ und Huwawa A. I. Teil, ZA 80, 165–203.
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 182
182 ninrioon\rnv
id. 1991: Gilgame“ und Huwawa A. II. Teil, ZA 81, 160–233.
id. 1995: The Sumerian Language, in: (ed. J. M. Sasson) CANE IV 2107–16.
id. 1997: Gudea and His Dynasty (= RIME 3/1).
id. 2003a: Zum sumerischen Verbalpräfix a(1), in: (ed. W. Sallaberger et al.) Fs.
C. Wilcke, 87–98.
id. 2003b: Sumerisch 1 bis 120, in: (ed. Y. Sefati) Fs. J. Klein.
id. 2003c: Wann ist Sumerisch als gesprochene Sprache ausgestorben, in: (ed.
K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa.
A. Falkenstein 1939: Untersuchungen zur sumerischen Grammatik 1. Der Plural
des Kohortativs; 2. Das richtungsanzeigende Infix -ra-, ZA 45, 169–94.
id. 1949–1950: Grammatik der Sprache Gudeas von Laga“. I. Schrift- und Formen­
lehre. II. Syntax (= AnOr. 28–29).
id. 1952: Das Petentialis- und Irrealissufix -e-“e des Sumerischen, Indogerm.
Forschungen 66, 113–30.
id. 1956: NG I.
id. 1959a: Das Sumerische (= HdOr. I/2/1–2/1).
id. 1959b: Untersuchungen zur sumerischen Grammatik 5. Zum Akzent des Sumer­
ischen, ZA 53, 98–105.
G. Farber-Flügge 1973: Der Mythos “Inanna und Enki” unter besonderer Berücksich-
tigung der me (= StudPohl 10).
D. A. Foxvog 1974: apud Heimpel 1974.
id. 1975: The Sumerian Ergative Construction, Or. 44, 395–425.
I. J. Gelb
2
1961: Old Akkadian Writing and Grammar (= MAD 3).
Y. Gong 2000: Die Namen der Keilschriftzeichen (= AOAT 268).
G. B. Gragg 1968: The Syntax of the Copula, in: (ed. J. W. M. Verhaar) The
Verb ‘Be’ and its Synonyms 3 (= Foundations of Languages, Suppl. Series 8).
id. 1973: Sumerian Dimensional Infixes (= AOAT 5).
M. Green 1978: The Eridu Lament, JCS 30, 127–67, esp. pp. 145 f. for -ri.
J. J. Gumperz/R. Wilson 1971: Convergence and Creolization. A Case for the
Indo-Aryan/Dravidian Border in India, in: (ed. D. Hymes), Pidginization and
Creolization, 151–67, esp. p. 154.
W. Heimpel 1974: The Structure of the Sumerian Prefix Chain (ms.).
M. Hilgert 2002: Akkadisch in der Ur III-Zeit (= IMGULA 5).
B. Hru“ka 1969: Das spätbabylonische Lehrgedicht “Inannas Erhöhung”, ArOr. 37,
473–521.
Th. Jacobsen 1956: Introduction to the Chicago Grammatical Texts, MSL 4, 1*–50*.
id. 1965: About the Sumerian Verb, in: (ed. H.G. Güterbock/Th. Jacobsen) Fs.
B. Landsberger (= AS 16) 71–101); reprinted in Jacobsen 1970, 245–70 + 430–66.
id. 1970: (ed. W. L. Moran) Toward the Image of Tammuz and other Essays on
Mesopotamian History and Religion (= HSS 21).
id. 1988: The Sumerian Verbal Core, ZA 78, 161–220; the reader is warned for
a number of regrettable misprints.
B. Jagersma 1999: apud Zólyomi 1999.
M. Jaques n.d. [1999]: Le vocabulaire des sentiments dans les textes sumériens
(= thèse de doctorat, Université de Genève).
R. Jestin 1943: Le verbe sumérien (I).
id. 1946: Le verbe sumérien (II).
id. 1951: Abrégé de grammaire sumérienne.
T. B. Jones/J. W. Snyder 1961: Sumerian Economic Texts from the Third Dynasty.
I. T. Kaneva 1996: ”umerskij jazyk.
F. Karahashi 2000: Sumerian Compound Verbs with Body-part Terms (diss. Univ.
of Chicago).
D. Katz 1993: Gilgamesh and Akka.
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 183
ninrioon\rnv 183
B. Kienast 1980: Probleme der sumerischen Grammatik 4. Bemerkungen zu ¢am†u
und marû im Sumerischen, ZA 70, 1–35.
J. Klein 1981. Three ”ulgi Hymns. Sumerian Royal Hymns Glorifying King ”ulgi
of Ur.
id. 1993: The Suffix of Determination /a/, ASJ 15, 81–98.
S. N. Kramer 1936: The Sumerian Prefix Forms be- and bi- in the Time of the
Earlier Princes of Laga“ (= AS 8).
M. Krebernik 1983: Zu Syllabar und Orthographie der lexikalischen Texte aus Ebla
II, ZA 73, 1–47.
id. 1998: Die Texte aus Fàra und Tell Abù Íalàbì¢, in: (ed. P. Attinger/M. Wäfler)
Mespotamien. Späturuk-Zeit und Frühdynastische Zeit (= OBO 160/1), 237–
427.
J. Krecher 1965: Zur sumerischen Grammatik 1. Isolierende Postpositionen, ZA 57,
12–29.
id. 1966: Sumerische Kultlyrik.
id. 1967a: Die pluralischen Verben für “gehen” und “stehen” im Sumerischen, WO
4, 1–11.
id. 1967b: Zum Emesal-Dialekt des Sumerischen, HSAO 1, 87–110.
id. 1978: Die Form und der Gebrauch der nominalen Verbalformen und die
Determination im Sumerischen, Or. 47, 376–403.
id. 1985: Die /m/-Präfixe des sumerischen Verbums, Or. 54, 133–81.
id. 1987: Morphemeless Syntax in Sumerian as Seen on the Background of Word-
Composition in Chukchee, ASJ 9, 67–88.
id. 1993: Über einige ‘zusammengesetze Verben’ im Sumerischen, in: (ed. A. F.
Rainey et al.) Mem. R. Kutscher, 107–118.
id. 1995: Die marû-Formen des sumerischen Verbums (= AOAT 240) 141–200.
W. G. Lambert 1991: The Reference of ¢am†u and marû in Lexical Lists, ZA 81,
7–9.
B. Landsberger 1944: Die Anfänge der Zivilisation in Mesopotamien (Ankara Üniv.
Dil ve Tarih-Co<rafya Fak. Dergisi II/3, 431–37); see also (transl. and edited by
M. de J. Ellis) The Beginnings of Civilisation in Mesopotamia (1974), Monographs
of the Ancient Near East 1/2, 8–12.
A. Langenmayr 1992: Sprachpsychologische Untersuchung zur sumerischen “Frauen-
sprache”, ZA 82, 208–11.
H. Limet 1968: L’anthroponymie sumérienne dans les documents de la 3
e
dynas­
tie d’Ur.
P. Michalowski 1980: Sumerian as an Ergative Language, I, JCS 32, 86–103.
id. 1898: The Lamentation over the Destruction of Sumer and Ur (= MesCiv. 1).
id. 1993: Letters from Early Mesopotamia.
O. Pedersén 1989: Some Morphological Aspects of Sumerian and Akkadian Linguistic
Area, in: (ed. H. Behrens et al.) Fs. Å. W. Sjöberg, 429–38.
A. Poebel 1923: Grundzüge der sumerischen Grammatik.
id. 1931: The Sumerian Prefix Forms e- and i- in the Time of the Earlier Princes
of Laga“ (= AS 2).
id. 1939: Studies in Akkadian Grammar (= AS 9).
id. 1947: Miscellaneous Studies (= AS 14), esp. pp. 23–42: The “Schachtelsatz”
Construction of the Narâm-Sîn Text RA XVI 157 f.
J. N. Postgate 1974: Two Points of Grammar in Gudea, JCS 26, 16–54.
M. A. Powell 1971: Sumerian Numeration and Metrology (= diss. Univ. of Minnesota).
id. 1989: Maße und Gewichte, RIA VII (1987–90) 457–517.
W. Richter 1970: Exegese als Literaturwissenschaft. Entwurf einer alttestamentlichen
Literaturtheorie und Methodologie.
E. Reiner 1966: A Linguistic Analysis of Akkadian.
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 184
184 ninrioon\rnv
W. H. Ph. Römer 1965: Sumerische “Königshymnen” der Isin-Zeit.
id: 1969: Einige Beobachtungen zur Göttin Nini(n)sina . . ., in: (ed. W. Röllig) Fs.
W. von Soden (= AOAT 1). 279–305.
id. 1976: Kleine Beiträge zur Grammatik des Sumerischen 1. Das modale gram­
matische Element nu-u“-, in: (ed. B. L. Eichler et al.) Fs. S. N. Kramer (= AOAT
25) 371–78.
id. 1980: Das sumerische Kurzepos “Bilgame“ und Akka” (= AOAT 2009/1).
id. 1982: Einführung in die Sumerologie (esp. pp. 27–97, “Einiges zur Sprache”).
G. Rubio 1999: On the alleged ‘Pre-Sumerian’ substratum, JCS 51, 1–16.
W. Sallaberger 1993: Der kultische Kalender der Ur III-Zeit (= UAVA 7/1 und
7/2).
A. Salonen 1968: Agricultura Mesomotamica.
R. Scholtz 1934: Die Struktur der sumerischen engeren Verbalpräfixe (Konjugations­
präfixe) speziell dargelegt an der I. und II. Form (E- und Mu-Konjugation) (=
MVAG 39/2).
M. Schretter 1990: Emesal-Studien.
Y. Sefati 1998: Love Songs in Sumerian Literature. Critical Edition of the Dumuzi-
Inanna Songs.
A. Shaffer 1963: Sumerian Sources of Tablet XII of the Epic of Gilgame“ (= Ph.D.
diss. Philadelphia).
¿. W. Sjöberg 1975: in-nin “à-gur
4
-ra. A Hymn to the Goddess Inanna by the en-
Priestess En¢eduanna, ZA 65, 161–253.
E. Sollberger 1952: Le système verbal dans les inscriptions “royales” présargoniques
de Laga“.
id. 1961: Le syllabaire présargonique de Laga“, ZA 54, 1–50.
id. 1966: The Business Administration Correspondence under the kings of Ur
(= TCS 1).
H. Steible 1982: Die altsumerischen Bau- und Weihinschriften I. II (= FAOS 5/
I–II).
G. Steiner: Intransitiv-passivische und aktivische Verbalauffassung, ZDMG 126,
230–80.
id. 1981: ›am†u und Marû als verbale Kategorien im Sumerischen und im Akkadischen,
RA 75, 1–14.
P. Steinkeller 1979: Notes on Sumerian Plural Verbs, Or. 48, 54–67.
M. P. Streck 2000: Das amurritische Onomastikon der altbabylonischen Zeit I,
Die Amurriter. Die onomastische Forschung. Orthographie und Phonologie.
Normalmorphologie (= AOAT 271/1).
V. V. Struve 1984: (ed. G. Kh. Kaplan) Onomastika rannedinasti‘eskogo Laga“a.
M.-L. Thomsen 1984: The Sumerian Language. An Introduction to its History and
Grammatical Structure (= Mesopotamia 10);
3
2001, 3rd unchanged and unex­
tended edition.
H. Vanstiphout 1985: On the Verbal Prefix /i/ in Standard Sumerian, RA 79,
1–15.
Aa. Westenholz 1991: The Phoneme /o/ in Akkadian, ZA 81, 10–19.
C. Wilcke 1969: Das Lugalbandaepos.
id. 1968: Das modale Adverb I-GI
4
-IN-ZU im Sumerischen, JNES 27, 229–42.
id. 1988: Anmerkungen zum ‘Konjugationspräfix’ /i/- und zur These vom “silbi­
schen Charakter der sumrischen Morpheme” anhand neusumerischer Verbalformen
beginnend mit ì-, íb-, ì-im- und ì-in-, ZA 78, 1–48.
id. 1990: Orthographie, Grammatik und literarische Form. Beobachtungen zu der
Vaseninschrift Lugalzaggesis (SAKI 152–156), in: (ed. Tz. Abusch et al.) Fs.
W. L. Moran, 455–504.
id. 2002: Der Kodex Urnammu (CU): Versuch einer Rekonstruktion, in: (ed. Tz.
Abusch) Mem. Th. Jacobsen, 291–333.
EDZARD_Bib_180-185 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 185
ninrioon\rnv 185
id. 2003: (in print) (the verb si sá), in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa.
C. Woods 2003: (in print) (Sumerian demonstratives), in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs.
M. Yoshikawa.
Yang Zhi 1989: Sargonic Inscriptions from Adab (= The Institute for the History
of Ancient Civilizations. Periodic Publications of Ancient Civilizations 1).
M. Yoshikawa 1968a: On the Grammatical Function of -e- of the Sumerian Verbal
Suffix -e-dè/-e-da(m), JNES 27, 251–61.
id. 1968b: The Marû and ›am†u Aspects in the Sumerian Verbal System, Or. 37,
401–16.
id. 1979: The Sumerian Verbal Prefixes mu-, i- and Topicality, Or. 48, 185–206.
id. 1981: Plural Expressions in Sumerian Verbs, ASJ 3, 111–24.
id. 1993a: Spatial Deictic System in Sumerian, ASJ 15, 185–92.
id. 1993b: Studies in the Sumerian Verbal System (= ASJ SS 1), a reprint of 24
articles published between 1968 and 1993.
A. Zgoll 2003: . . ., in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa.
G. Zólyomi 1999: Directive infix and oblique object in Sumerian: An account on
the history of their relationship, Or. 68, 215–53.
186
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 186
6
EDZARD_f3_6-11 4/28/03 2:39 PM Page 6
This page intentionally left blank
187
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 187
[a] (exclamation) 167
a > i (e.g., dab
5
> díb)
diachronically 14
ablative(-instrumental) (case) 33 f.,
41 f.
absolutive (case) 33–35
achevé (verbal aspect) 73
adjectives 25, 47–48
adverbiative 26, 42
adverbs 26 f., 69
affirmative 1 (verbal mood) 117 f.
affirmative 2 (verbal mood) 119 f.
affirmative 3 (verbal mood) 120
a(l)- (prefixed verbal indicator) 111 f.
[alala] (exclamation) 167
[allili] (exclamation) 167
[alulu] (exclamation) 167
Anatolia 4
“and” (lack of ) 41
areal situation (Sumero-Akkadian)
4 f., 173 ff.
[aua] (exclamation) 167
b (variant of verbal indicator [bi])
100 f.
B = base
B-[Ø] (verb) 130–132
[ba] (non-ventive directional indicator)
94 f.
B-[a] 132 f., 137 f.
B-[ada] (verb) 134 f., 136 f.
[bara] (modal verbal indicator: vetitive)
117
[bara] (modal verbal indicator: negative
affirmative) 118
base (verb) 71 and passim
Basque (prefix chains) 72
B-B-[Ø] (verb) 130 f.
[bda] (non-ventive directional indicator)
B-[ed] 130, 134
B-[eda] 134–136
B-[ede] 134–136
[bi] (demonstrative) 50
[bi] (non-ventive directional indicator)
98–101
-bi(-da) “and” 41
INDEX
*[bra] 22
[b“i] (non-ventive directional indicator)
98
[bta] (non-ventive directional indicator)
97
cardinal numbers 61–66
case 33–45
Caucasus 4
causative 101
causative (Akkadian) 98 f.
circumpositional syntagma 56
class (person, non-person) 1, 24
clusters (syllabic) 22
cognates of Sumerian (alleged) 2 f.
cohortative (verbal mood) 115 f.
comitative (case) 33 f., 40 f.
compounds (nominal) 2
compound verbs 142–150
conjugated participle 137–142
conjugation 81
conjugation patterns 81–88
conjugation pattern 1 (intransitive and
passive) 81–83
conjugation pattern 2a (transitive)
83–87
conjugation pattern 2b (transitive)
87–90
conjunctions 27, 161–162
conjunctions and subjunctions
161–165
connecting indicators (verb) 112,
121–127
consonants 7–8, 14–21
copula 55, 82–83
cuneograms, classification of 8–10
dative (case) 33 f., 40
dedli 31 f.
deli 62, 141 f.
demonstrative particles
demonstrative pronouns
determination (of nouns)
determinatives 9 f.
dili see deli
49–51
57
49–51
dimensional indicators (verb) 92–109
diphthongs 13 f.
96
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 188
188 
directive (case) 33 f., 43 f. genitive (case) 33 f., 36–39
distribution 41 f., 67 Georgian (stops) 16
dug
4
/e/di 76 f. Girsu/Laga“ 4
grids (for verbal bases) 71 f., 74–78
e/i (difficulty to distinguish them) 14 [gu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator:
[e] (prefixed verbal indicator)
109–111
-e (demonstrative?) 50
Ebla glosses 4
[ed] 74, 82
[eda] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 96
[elala] (exclamation) 168
Elamite 3
[ellu] (exclamation) 168
eme-gi
7
(-r) 1
eme-Ki-en-gi-ra 1
Emesal 66
(numerals) 171 f.
[ena] (subjunction) 164
[enea] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 95 f.
[ene“i] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 98
equative (case) 33 f., 44
ergative (case) 33 f., 35 f., 90 f.
ergativity 1 f., 35 f.
[era] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 94
[eri] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 99
[e“] (adverbiative) 26, 42
[e“i] (non-ventive dimensional indicator)
97
exclamations 27, 167–170
exclusive 30
finite verb 71
focus 2
fractions 67
free genitive 38 f.
“frozen” morpheme 2
frustrative (verbal mood) 120 f.
cohortative) 115
H (Sumerian consonantal phoneme)
19 f., 50, 175
-H 40
¢ 20
[ha] (modal verbal indicator: precative)
116 f.
[ha] (modal verbal indicator:
affirmative 1) 117 f.
¢am†u 71 ff. passim
¢am†u participle 47, 132 f.
[he] (modal verbal indicator: precative)
116 f.
[he] (modal verbal indicator:
affirmative 1) 116 f.
[heam] (exclamation, “yes”) 27, 168
heteronymy (verbal base) 71
hi-a 32
Hiatustilger (r) 94, 99
[hu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator:
precative) 116 f.
[hu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator:
affirmative 1) 117 f.
Hurrian 4
[i] (prefixed verbal indicator)
109–111
[ilu] (exclamation) 168
[ilulamma] (exclamation) 168
[(i)mmeri] (ventive verbal indicator)
108
imperative 127–130
extended i. 129 f.
polite i. 122 f.
unextended i. 128 f.
imperfective (verbal aspect) 74
inachevé (verbal aspect) 74
inalterable (class of verbs) 75
∞g 17
[ga] (modal verbal indicator:
cohortative) 115
[gana] (exclamation) 168
Gatumdu 4
“-gé” 36
[ge] (modal verbal indicator:
cohortative) 115
inclusive 30
indefinite pronoun (?) 59
indicative (verbal mood) 113
infinitive (Akkadian) 71
[(i)nga] (connective verbal indicator)
123–127
interrogative pronouns 57 f.
“intransitive” 36
§gen/du (irregular behaviour in [inu] “no” (exclamation) 169
non-finite verb) 140 f. inverted genitive construction 31
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 189
 189
Iran 4
[i Utu] (exclamation) 169
ke
4
36
KID 36
l
l
, l
2
17 f.
lexical lists 2, 23 f., 26
linear script (Elamite) 3
linguistic affiliation (of Sumerian) 2 f.
linguistic area (Sumero-Akkadian) 4 f.,
25, 173–178
linguistic environment (of Sumerian)
3–4
liquids 17 f.
loan-words (Sumerian in Akkadian) 7,
16, 37
locative (case) 33 f., 39, 44
locative 2 see [ni]
logograms 8
[ma] (ventive dimensional indicator)
103
-ma (conjunction, borrowed from
Akkadian) 162
marû 73 f.
[mda] (ventive dimensional indicator)
105
[me] 1st p. pronoun) 55 f.
[me] (ventive dimensional indicator)
108
[mea] (ventive dimensional indicator)
104
measures (terminology) 67
[mel(i)ea] (exclamation) 169
mes-Ane-pada construction 133
[mini] (ventive dimensional indicator)
108
[mma] (ventive dimensional indicator)
104
[mmara] (ventive dimensional indicator)
106
[mmeri] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107 f.
[mmi] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107 f.
[mmini] (ventive dimensional indicator)
109
modal indicators (verb) 113–121
“monosyllabic myth” 4
[m“i] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107
[mta] (ventive dimensional indicator)
106
[mu] (= mu-V-) (ventive dimensional
indicator) 107
[mu] (“year”, as subjunction) 164
[muda] (ventive dimensional indicator)
105
[mueda] (ventive dimensional indicator)
105
[mueda(?)] (ventive dimensional indica­
tor) 105
[muera] (ventive dimensional indicator)
103
[mue“i] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107
[muna] (ventive dimensional indicator)
103 f.
[munda] (ventive dimensional indicator)
105
[mune] (ventive dimensional indicator)
108
[muni] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107
[mun“i] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107
[muri] (ventive dimensional indicator)
107
[mu“i] (ventive dimensional indicator)
106
[n] (variant of [ni], locative 2) 98
[na] (non-ventive dimensional indicator)
94
[na] (modal verbal indicator:
affirmative 2) 119 f.
[na] (modal verbal indicator:
prohibitive) 118 f.
[nam] (nominal prefix) 25
Nan“e 4
nasals (consonants) 16 f.
[nda] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 96
[ne(n)] (demonstrative) 49 f., 57
[(e)ne] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 101
[(e)nea] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 95 f.
[(e)neda] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 96
negative affirmative (verbal mood) 118
negative precative see vetitive
negative verbal indicators 113–118
[(e)ne“i] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 98
[ni] (non-ventive dimensional indicator:
locative 2) 98 f., 102
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 190
190 
[ni] (non-ventive dimensional indicator:
3rd p. sg. person class) 99
“no” 27, 169
nominalization (of finite verbal forms)
150–155
non-finite verb 71
non-finite verbal forms 130–142
non-ventive dimensional indicators 93
(grid) 94–102
[n“i] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 98
[nta] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 97
[nu] (modal indicator: negative
indicative, negation of non-finite
verbal forms) 113–115
“nuclear” writing 11
number 2, 24, 31–33
numerals 26, 61–67
[nu“] (modal verbal indicator:
frustrative) 120 f.
o (in Sumerian) 7
o (in Akkadian) 7, 13
ordinal numbers 66 f.
particles 23
parts of speech 23–27
passive 95
perfective (verbal aspect) 74
person 30 f.
personal pronouns 55 f.
phonetic indicators 8
phonology 13–21
vowels 13–14
consonants 14–21
plural bases (verb) 74, 78
polite imperative 122 f.
possession 24, 29–31, 56 f.
possessive pronouns 56 f.
post-nominal and/or post-verbal
particles other than case particles
157–160
Präsens-Futur 73
Präteritum 73
precative (verbal mood) 116 f.
prohibitive (verbal mood) 118 f.
pronominal conjugation 137–142
pronouns 25 f., 55–59
pronouns (alleged) 58 f.
prospective (connective verbal indicator)
121–123
Proto-Ea 8
“Proto-Elamite” script 3
quadruplication (of verbal base) 81
quotation particle 157 f.
§r 18 f.
[ra] (non-ventive dimensional indicator)
97, 109
redundancy of spelled consonants 10,
35
reduplication 24 f.
r. (number) 31
r. of adjectives 25, 31, 48
r. of verbal base 75 f., 79–81
pseudo r. of verbal base 75
reflexive pronoun (?) 58
[(e)ri] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 99
Sanskrit (stops) 16
secondary construction (verb) 74
sequence (of post-nominal particles) 53
sexagesimal system 26, 61–66
sibilants 20 f.
sign names (Akkadian) 7, 14
specification (of nouns) 49–51
spelling of Sumerian 8–11
spelling variants 7
“split ergativity” 2, 90 f.
‘Sprachgruppe’ 3
standard construction (verb) 74
stops 8, 14–16
subjunctions 27, 162–165
subordinate temporal clause 42 f.
substantivation 25
see also nominalization
substantives 24
substratum (pre-Sumerian, alleged) 4
Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area 4 f.,
173–178
syllabaries 11
syllabic spelling (in Akkadian, to
identify Sumerian) 7
syllable-closing consonants (spelling of )
11
syllable structure 22
syllabograms 8
[“a] (modal verbal particle: affirmative
3) 120
[“e] (demonstrative) 51
[“i] (modal verbal particle: affirmative
3) 120
[(e)“i] (non-ventive dimensional
indicator) 97
EDZARD_Index_186-191 4/28/03 1:02 PM Page 191
 191
[“u(mu)] (modal verbal particle: ventive (Sumerian) 92
affirmative 3) 120 ventive (Akkadian) 92
“u ti/te(-∞g) (behaviour of the “object”) ventive dimensional indicators 93
145 f. (grid) 103–109
verb 26, 71–155
[ta] (non-ventive verbal indicator) 109 verbalizer (in compound verbs) 143,
Tepe Ya˙yà 3 148 f.
terminative (case) 33 f., 42 f. vetitive (verbal mood) 117
ternary system (of numerals) 66 vocative (case) 35
“transitive” 36 “vowel harmony” 99 f., 110
triplication (of verbal base) 81 vowels 7, 13 f.
[tukumbi] 163 f. phonemes 7
quality 7, 13 f.
[u] (verbal connective indicator, quantity 7
prospective 121–123
see also polite imperative “without” 41
[u] (conjunction) 161 f. “word” 23
[ua] (exclamation) 170 word order (S–O–V) 2
[uda] (subjunction) 162 f.
[ulili] (exclamation) 170 “yes” 27, 168
ur
5
(demonstrative) 55, 57
-ùtu (Akkadian) 25 zero 87
HDO1-serie.qxd 29-4-2003 10:10 Page i
HANDBOOK OF ORIENTAL STUDIES
(HANDBUCH DER ORIENTALISTIK)
Section I: NEAR AND MIDDLE EAST
Abt. I: DER NAHE UND MITTLERE OSTEN
ISSN 0169-9423
Band 1. Ägyptologie
1. Ägyptische Schrift und Sprache. Mit Beiträgen von H. Brunner, H. Kees, S. Morenz, E. Otto, S.
Schott. Mit Zusätzen von H. Brunner. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1959). 1973. ISBN 90
04 03777 2
2. Literatur. Mit Beiträgen von H. Altenmüller, H. Brunner, G. Fecht, H. Grapow, H. Kees, S.
Morenz, E. Otto, S. Schott, J. Spiegel, W. Westendorf. 2. verbesserte und erweiterte
Au age. 1970. ISBN 90 04 00849 7
3. Helck, W. Geschichte des alten Ägypten. Nachdruck mit Berichtigungen und Ergänz-ungen.
1981. ISBN 90 04 06497 4
Band 2. Keilschriftforschung und alte Geschichte Vorderasiens
1-2/2. Altkleinasiatische Sprachen [und Elamitisch]. Mit Beiträgen von J. Friedrich, E. Reiner, A.
Kammenhuber, G. Neumann, A. Heubeck. 1969. ISBN 90 04 00852 7
3. Schmökel, H. Geschichte des alten Vorderasien. Reprint. 1979. ISBN 90 04 00853 5
4/2. Orientalische Geschichte von Kyros bis Mohammed. Mit Beiträgen von A. Dietrich, G. Widen­
gren, F. M. Heichelheim. 1966. ISBN 90 04 00854 3
Band 3. Semitistik
Semitistik. Mit Beiträgen von A. Baumstark, C. Brockelmann, E. L. Dietrich, J. Fück, M.
Höfner, E. Littmann, A. Rücker, B. Spuler. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1953-1954). 1964.
ISBN 90 04 00855 1
Band 4. Iranistik
1. Linguistik. Mit Beiträgen von K. HoVmann, W. B. Henning, H. W. Bailey, G. Morgen­
stierne, W. Lentz. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1958). 1967. ISBN 90 04 03017 4
2/1. Literatur. Mit Beiträgen von I. Gershevitch, M. Boyce, O. Hansen, B. Spuler, M. J.
Dresden. 1968. ISBN 90 04 00857 8
2/2. History of Persian Literature from the Beginning of the Islamic Period to the Present Day. With
Contributions by G. Morrison, J. Baldick and Sh. Kadkan¯. 1981. ISBN 90 04 06481 8
3. Krause, W. Tocharisch. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1955) mit Zusätzen und Berich­
tigungen. 1971. ISBN 90 04 03194 4
Band 5. Altaistik
1. Turkologie. Mit Beiträgen von A. von Gabain, O. Pritsak, J. Benzing, K. H. Menges, A.
Temir, Z. V. Togan, F. Taeschner, O. Spies, A. Caferoglu, A. Battal-Tamays. Reprint with
additions of the 1st (1963) ed. 1982. ISBN 90 04 06555 5
2. Mongolistik. Mit Beiträgen von N. Poppe, U. Posch, G. Doerfer, P. Aalto, D. Schröder, O.
Pritsak, W. Heissig. 1964. ISBN 90 04 00859 4
3. Tungusologie. Mit Beiträgen von W. Fuchs, I. A. Lopatin, K. H. Menges, D. Sinor. 1968.
ISBN 90 04 00860 8
Band 6. Geschichte der islamischen Länder
5/1. Regierung und Verwaltung des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. Mit Beiträgen von H. R. Idris
und K. Röhrborn. 1979. ISBN 90 04 05915 6
5/2. Regierung und Verwaltung des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. 2. Mit Beiträgen von D.
Sourdel und J. Bosch Vilá. 1988. ISBN 90 04 08550 5
6/1. Wirtschaftsgeschichte des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. Mit Beiträgen von B. Lewis, M.
Rodinson, G. Baer, H. Müller, A. S. Ehrenkreutz, E. Ashtor, B. Spuler, A. K. S. Lambton,
R. C. Cooper, B. Rosenberger, R. Arié, L. Bolens, T. Fahd. 1977.
ISBN 90 04 04802 2
Band 7
Armenisch und Kaukasische Sprachen. Mit Beiträgen von G. Deeters, G. R. Solta, V. Inglisian.
1963. ISBN 90 04 00862 4
Band 8. Religion
1/1. Religionsgeschichte des alten Orients. Mit Beiträgen von E. Otto, O. Eissfeldt, H. Otten, J.
Hempel. 1964. ISBN 90 04 00863 2
HDO1-serie.qxd 29-4-2003 10:10 Page ii
1/2/2/1. Boyce, M. A History of Zoroastrianism. The Early Period. Rev. ed. 1989.
ISBN 90 04 08847 4
1/2/2/2. Boyce, M. A History of Zoroastrianism. Under the Achaemenians. 1982.
ISBN 90 04 06506 7
1/2/2/3. Boyce, M. and Grenet, F. A History of Zoroastrianism. Zoroastrianism under Macedonian
and Roman Rule. With a Contribution by R. Beck. 1991. ISBN 90 04 09271 4
2. Religionsgeschichte des Orients in der Zeit der Weltreligionen. Mit Beiträgen von A. Adam, A. J.
Arberry, E. L. Dietrich, J. W. Fück, A. von Gabain, J. Leipoldt, B. Spuler, R. Strothman, G.
Widengren. 1961. ISBN 90 04 00864 0
Ergänzungsband 1
1. Hinz, W. Islamische Maße und Gewichte umgerechnet ins metrische System. Nachdruck der
Erstausgabe (1955) mit Zusätzen und Berichtigungen. 1970. ISBN 90 04 00865 9
Ergänzungsband 2
1. Grohmann, A. Arabische Chronologie und Arabische Papyruskunde. Mit Beiträgen von J. Mayr
und W. C. Till. 1966. ISBN 90 04 00866 7
2. Khoury, R. G. Chrestomathie de papyrologie arabe. Documents relatifs à la vie privée, sociale et
administrative dans les premiers siècles islamiques. 1992. ISBN 90 04 09551 9
Ergänzungsband 3
Orientalisches Recht. Mit Beiträgen von E. Seidl, V. Korosc, E. Pritsch, O. Spies, E. Tyan, J. Baz,
Ch. Chehata, Ch. Samaran, J. Roussier, J. Lapanne-Joinville, S. S¸ . Ansay. 1964.
ISBN 90 04 00867 5
Ergänzungsband 5
1/1. Borger, R. Einleitung in die assyrischen Königsinschriften. 1. Das zweite Jahrtausend vor Chr.
Mit Verbesserungen und Zusätzen. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1961). 1964.
ISBN 90 04 00869 1
1/2. Schramm, W. Einleitung in die assyrischen Königsinschriften. 2. 934-722 v. Chr. 1973.
ISBN 90 04 03783 7
Ergänzungsband 6
1. Ullmann, M. Die Medizin im Islam. 1970. ISBN 90 04 00870 5
2. Ullmann, M. Die Natur- und Geheimwissenschaften im Islam. 1972. ISBN 90 04 03423 4
Ergänzungsband 7
Gomaa, I. A Historical Chart of the Muslim World. 1972. ISBN 90 04 03333 5
Ergänzungsband 8
Kornrumpf, H.-J. Osmanische Bibliographie mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der Türkei in Europa.
Unter Mitarbeit von J. Kornrumpf. 1973. ISBN 90 04 03549 4
Ergänzungsband 9
Firro, K. M. A History of the Druzes. 1992. ISBN 90 04 09437 7
Band 10
Strijp, R. Cultural Anthropology of the Middle East. A Bibliography. Vol. 1: 1965-1987. 1992.
ISBN 90 04 09604 3
Band 11
Endress, G. & Gutas, D. (eds.). A Greek and Arabic Lexicon. (GALex ). Materials for a Dictionary
of the Mediæval Translations from Greek into Arabic.
Fascicle 1. Introduction—Sources— { – {-kh-r. Compiled by G. Endress & D. Gutas, with the
assistance of K. Alshut, R. Arnzen, Chr. Hein, St. Pohl, M. Schmeink. 1992.
ISBN 90 04 09494 6
Fascicle 2. {-kh-r – {-s
.
-l. Compiled by G. Endress & D. Gutas, with the assistance of K. Alshut,
R. Arnzen, Chr. Hein, St. Pohl, M. Schmeink. 1993. ISBN 90 04 09893 3
Fascicle 3. {-s
.
-l – {-l-y. Compiled by G. Endress, D. Gutas & R. Arnzen, with the assistance of
Chr. Hein, St. Pohl. 1995. ISBN 90 04 10216 7
-
Fascicle 4. Ila – inna. Compiled by R. Arnzen, G. Endress & D. Gutas, with the assistance of
Chr. Hein & J. Thielmann. 1997. ISBN 90 04 10489 5.
Band 12
Jayyusi, S. K. (ed.). The Legacy of Muslim Spain. Chief consultant to the editor, M. Marín. 2nd
ed. 1994. ISBN 90 04 09599 3
Band 13
Hunwick, J. O. and O’Fahey, R. S. (eds.). Arabic Literature of Africa. Editorial Consultant:
Albrecht Hofheinz.
Volume I. The Writings of Eastern Sudanic Africa to c. 1900. Compiled by R. S. O’Fahey, with the
assistance of M. I. Abu Salim, A. Hofheinz, Y. M. Ibrahim, B. Radtke and K. S. Vikør.
1994. ISBN 90 04 09450 4
Volume II. The Writings of Central Sudanic Africa. Compiled by John O. Hunwick, with the
HDO1-serie.qxd 29-4-2003 10:10 Page iii
- -
assistance of Razaq Abubakre, Hamidu Bobboyi, Roman Loimeier, Stefan Reichmuth and
Muhammad Sani Umar. 1995. ISBN 90 04 10494 1
Volume III. The Writings of the Muslim Peoples of Northeastern Africa. Compiled by R.S. O’Fahey
with the assistance of Hussein Ahmed, Lidwien Kapteijns, Mohamed M. Kassim,
Jonathan Miran, Scott S. Reese and Ewald Wagner. 2003. ISBN 90 04 10938 2
Volume IV. The Writings of Western Sudanic Africa. Compiled by John O. Hunwick with the
assistance of Ousmane Kane, Bernard Salvaing, Rudiger Seesemann, Mark Sey and Ivor
Wilks. 2003. ISBN 90 04 12444 6
Band 14
Decker, W. und Herb, M. Bildatlas zum Sport im alten Ägypten. Corpus der bildlichen Quellen zu
Leibesübungen, Spiel, Jagd, Tanz und verwandten Themen. Bd.1: Text. Bd. 2: Ab-bildungen.
1994. ISBN 90 04 09974 3 (Set)
Band 15
Haas, V. Geschichte der hethitischen Religion. 1994. ISBN 90 04 09799 6
Band 16
Neusner, J. (ed.). Judaism in Late Antiquity. Part One: The Literary and Archaeological Sources.
1994. ISBN 90 04 10129 2
Band 17
Neusner, J. (ed.). Judaism in Late Antiquity. Part Two: Historical Syntheses. 1994.
ISBN 90 04 09799 6
Band 18
Orel, V. E. and Stolbova, O. V. (eds.). Hamito-Semitic Etymological Dictionary. Materials for a
Reconstruction. 1994. ISBN 90 04 10051 2
Band 19
al-Zwaini, L. and Peters, R. A Bibliography of Islamic Law, 1980-1993. 1994.
ISBN 90 04 10009 1
Band 20
Krings, V. (éd.). La civilisation phénicienne et punique. Manuel de recherche. 1995.
ISBN 90 04 10068 7
Band 21
Hoftijzer, J. and Jongeling, K. Dictionary of the North-West Semitic Inscriptions. With appendices
by R.C. Steiner, A. Mosak Moshavi and B. Porten. 1995. 2 Parts.
ISBN Set (2 Parts) 90 04 09821 6 Part One: { - L. ISBN 90 04 09817 8 Part Two:
M - T. ISBN 90 04 9820 8.
Band 22
-
Lagarde, M. Index du Grand Commentaire de Fahr al-DÊn al-Ra Ê. 1996.
-
z
ISBN 90 04 10362 7
Band 23
Kinberg, N. A Lexicon of al-Farra " ’s Terminology in his Qur"an Commentary. With Full Definitions,
English Summaries and Extensive Citations. 1996. ISBN 90 04 10421 6
Band 24
Fähnrich, H. und Sardshweladse, S. Etymologisches Wörterbuch der Kartwel-Sprachen. 1995. ISBN
90 04 10444 5
Band 25
Rainey, A.F. Canaanite in the Amarna Tablets. A Linguistic Analysis of the Mixed Dialect used by
Scribes from Canaan. 1996. ISBN Set (4 Volumes) 90 04 10503 4
Volume I. Orthography, Phonology. Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Pronouns, Nouns,
Numerals. ISBN 90 04 10521 2 Volume II. Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Verbal
System. ISBN 90 04 10522 0 Volume III. Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Particles and
Adverbs. ISBN 90 04 10523 9 Volume IV. References and Index of Texts Cited. ISBN 90
04 10524 7
Band 26
Halm, H. The Empire of the Mahdi. The Rise of the Fatimids. Translated from the German by
M. Bonner. 1996. ISBN 90 04 10056 3
Band 27
Strijp, R. Cultural Anthropology of the Middle East. A Bibliography. Vol. 2: 1988-1992. 1997.
ISBN 90 04 010745 2
Band 28
Sivan, D. A Grammar of the Ugaritic Language. 1997. ISBN 90 04 10614 6
Band 29
Corriente, F. A Dictionary of Andalusi Arabic. 1997. ISBN 90 04 09846 1
HDO1-serie.qxd 29-4-2003 10:10 Page iv
Band 30
Sharon, M. Corpus Inscriptionum Arabicarum Palaestinae (CIAP). Vol. 1: A. 1997.
ISBN 90 04 010745 2 Vol.1: B. 1999. ISBN 90 04 110836
Band 31
Török, L. The Kingdom of Kush. Handbook of the Napatan-Meroitic Civilization. 1997.
ISBN 90 04 010448 8
Band 32
Muraoka, T. and Porten, B. A Grammar of Egyptian Aramaic. 1998. ISBN 90 04 10499 2
Band 33
Gessel, B.H.L. van. Onomasticon of the Hittite Pantheon. 1998.
ISBN Set (2 parts) 90 04 10809 2
Band 34
Klengel, H. Geschichte des hethitischen Reiches 1998. ISBN 90 04 10201 9
Band 35
Hachlili, R. Ancient Jewish Art and Archaeology in the Diaspora 1998. ISBN 90 04 10878 5
Band 36
Westendorf, W. Handbuch der altägyptischen Medizin. 1999.
ISBN Set (2 Bände) 90 04 10319 8
Band 37
Civil, M. Mesopotamian Lexicography. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11007 0
Band 38
Siegelová, J. and Sou´ek, V. Systematische Bibliographie der Hethitologie. 1999.
ISBN Set (3 Bände) 90 04 11205 7
Band 39
Watson, W.G.E. and Wyatt, N. Handbook of Ugaritic Studies. 1999.
ISBN 90 04 10988 9
Band 40
Neusner, J. Judaism in Late Antiquity, III,1. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11186 7
Band 41
Neusner, J. Judaism in Late Antiquity, III,2. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11282 0
Band 42
Drijvers, H.J.W. and Healey, J.F. The Old Syriac Inscriptions of Edessa and Osrhoene. 1999.
ISBN 90 04 11284 7
Band 43
Daiber, H. Bibliography of Philosophical Thought in Islam. 2 Volumes.
ISBN Set (2 Volumes) 90 04 11347 9
Volume I. Alphabetical List of Publications 1999. ISBN 90 04 09648 5
Volume II. Index of Names, Terms and Topics. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11348 7
Band 44
Hunger, H. and Pingree, D. Astral Sciences in Mesopotamia. 1999. ISBN 90 04 10127 6
Band 45
Neusner, J. The Mishnah. Religious Perspectives 1999. ISBN 90 04 11492 0
Band 46
Neusner, J. The Mishnah. Social Perspectives 1999. ISBN 90 04 11491 2
Band 47
Khan, G. A Grammar of Neo-Aramaic. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11510 2
Band 48
Takács, G. Etymological Dictionary of Egyptian. Vol. 1. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11538 2
Takács, G. Etymological Dictionary of Egyptian. Vol. 2. 2001. ISBN 90 04 12121 8
Band 49
Avery-Peck, A.J. and Neusner, J. Judaism in Late Antiquity IV. 2000. ISBN 90 04 11262 6
Band 50
Tal, A. A Dictionary of Samaritan Aramaic. (2 Volumes) 2000. ISBN 90 04 11858 6 (dl. 1)
ISBN 90 04 11859 4 (dl. 2) ISBN 90 04 11645 1 (set)
Band 51
Holes, C. Dialect, Culture, and Society in Eastern Arabia. Vol. 1 : Glossary 2001.
ISBN 90 04 10763 0
Band 52
Jong, R.E. de. A Grammar of the Bedouin Dialects of the Northern Sinai Littoral. Bridging the
Linguistic Gap between the Eastern and Western Arab World. 2000.
ISBN 90 04 11868 3
HDO1-serie.qxd 29-4-2003 10:10 Page v
Band 53
Avery-Peck, A.J. and Neusner, J. Judaism in Late Antiquity III,3. Where we stand: Issues and
Debates in Ancient Judaism. 2000. ISBN 90 04 11892 6
Band 54
Krahmalkov, Ch. R. A Phoenician-Punic Grammar. 2001. ISBN 90 04 11771 7
Band 55
Avery-Peck, A.J. and Neusner, J. Judaism in Late Antiquity III,4. Where we stand: Issues and
Debates in Ancient Judaism.. The Special Problem of the Synagogue. 2001.
ISBN 90 04 12000 9.
Band 56
Avery-Peck, A.J., Neusner, J., and Chilton, B. Judaism in Late Antiquity V,1. The Judaism of
Qumran: A Systemic Reading of the Dead Sea Scrolls. Theory of Israel. 2001.
ISBN 90 04 12001 7
Band 57
Avery-Peck, A.J., Neusner, J., and Chilton, B. Judaism in Late Antiquity V,2. The Judaism of
Qumran: A Systemic Reading of the Dead Sea Scrolls. World View, Comparing Judaisms.
2001. ISBN 90 04 12003 3
Band 58
Gacek, A. The Arabic manuscript tradition. A Glossary of Technical Terms and Bibliography.
2001. ISBN 90 04 12061 0
Band 60
Marzolph, U. Narrative illustration in Persian lithographed books. 2001.
ISBN 90 04 12100 5
Band 61
Zammit, M.R. A Comparative Lexical Study of Qur’§nic Arabic. 2002.
ISBN 90 04 11801 2
Band 62
Grossmann, P. Christliche Architektur in Ägypten. 2002.
ISBN 90 04 12128 5
Band 63
Weipert, R. Classical Arabic Philology and Poetry. A Bibliographical Handbook of Important
Editions from 1960 to 2000. 2002
ISBN 90 04 12342 3
Band 64
Collins, B.J. A History of the Animal World in the Ancient Near East. 2002.
ISBN 90 09 12126 9
Band 65
Avery-Peck, A.J. and Neusner, J. The Mishnah in Contemporary Perspective. Part I. 2002.
ISBN 90 09 12515 9
Band 66
Muffs, Y. Studies in the Aramaic Legal Papyri from Elephantine. 2003.
ISBN 90 04 12868 9
Band 67
del Olmo Lete, G., and Sanmartín, J., edited and translated by Wilfred G.E. Watson.
A Dictionary of the Ugaritic Language in the Alphabetic Tradition (2 vols)
ISBN 9004128913 (set)
Band 68
Melchert, H.C. The Luwians. 2003.
ISBN 90 04 13009 8
Band 69
Ritter, H, translated by John O’Kane with Editorial Assistance of Bernd Radtke. The Ocean of the
Soul. Man, the World and God in the Stories of FarÊd al-DÊn #Aãã§r
ISBN 90 04 12068 8
Band 70
Borg, A. A Glossary of Cypriot Maronite Arabic (Arabic-English). With an Introductory Essay. 2003.
ISBN 90 04 13198 1
Band 71
Edzard, D.O. Sumerian Grammar. 2003.
ISBN 90 04 12608 2
Band 72
Westbrook, R. A History of Ancient Near Eastern Law.
ISBN 90 04 12995 2

SUMERIAN GRAMMAR

HANDBOOK OF ORIENTAL STUDIES

HANDBUCH DER ORIENTALISTIK
SECTION ONE

THE NEAR AND MIDDLE EAST
EDITED BY

H. ALTENMÜLLER · B. HROUDA · B.A. LEVINE · R.S. O’FAHEY K.R. VEENHOF · C.H.M. VERSTEEGH

VOLUME SEVENTY-ONE

SUMERIAN GRAMMAR

BY

DIETZ OTTO EDZARD

BRILL

LEIDEN • BOSTON

2003

mechanical. without prior written permission from the publisher.955-dc21 2003049567 ISSN 0169-9423 ISBN 90 04 12608 2 © Copyright 2003 by Koninklijke Brill NV. Title. Near and Middle East . 222 Rosewood Drive. electronic. PJ4013. No part of this publication may be reproduced. Sumerian grammar / by Dietz Otto Edzard. Dietz Otto. – (Handbook of Oriental studies. The Netherlands All rights reserved. Fees are subject to change. II. Suite 910. ISBN 90-04-12608-2 1. v. cm. Leiden. Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use is granted by Brill provided that the appropriate fees are paid directly to The Copyright Clearance Center.E38 2003 499'. recording or otherwise. 71. Section 1. Danvers MA 01923. Bd. I.This book is printed on acid-free paper. 71 = Handbuch der Orientalistik) Includes bibliographical references and index. p. Erste Abteilung. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Edzard. Handbuch der Orientalistik. Nahe und der Mittlere Osten . photocopying. Sumerian language–Grammar. USA. or transmitted in any form or by any means. stored in a retrieval system. printed in the netherlands . translated.

................ Classification of cuneograms .................. Notes on the phonetic and spelling behaviour of the case particles ............. parts of speech ....2......... 1. 5............... 1.........1.............................. The linguistic environment of Sumerian ................... 5...... Phonology .......................................................... 5.............. spelling proper ................ 5....................2..........2...... Number .... General ...................1...........................................2.........1.. 5.. Ergative ........................................................4........ 3. Absolutive .........................2... 2... Notes on the individual case particles ............................. Consonants ...........1....................................4..... 2.....................................................1............ 2. 3................. General Characteristics ................ 4.... Syllable structure .. Possession ........... Spelling of Sumerian ...............................................2..........................1... General .............. The (hopeless) question of the linguistic affiliation of Sumerian .......................1.. Vowels .....................1.......... C T How we read Sumerian ...... Abbreviations ..... “Word” ..................................... Parts of speech ........................................... Evolution of Sumerian spelling ...............................................................2........................2......................................................... 4...........................4.............................................................................4................... Combination of cuneograms.................. 5............................ xi xiii 1 1 2 3 7 7 8 8 10 10 13 13 13 14 22 23 23 23 29 29 29 31 33 35 35 35 35 ........................................................................1..4........3....................................................2..2......3.... 5..............................1.................... C F The “word” in Sumerian................. 5..2.3...1. Case ...........................2................ C F Substantives ....... 2................. 1......................................... 3........................... 3.... C O The Sumerian Language ..................2...................................................2................................ C T Minimalia of Sumerian phonology and syllabic structure .............. 2....CONTENTS Preface ......

........................ .......................................3......... Personal pronouns ...... Directive ...............................11.4..........................4............4............1.............2... Recapitulation of the case particles ........... C S  Genitive ...... 7............................2.................................................... Comitative .... ur5 ........7........................................................2... Demonstrative pronouns .............................................. 7...............5............... 9..................1...................................2......3.. Terminative ...................................................................................... ne-e(n) .................. 9...............................2............................4...........................................2..... C N Pronouns .........2.....................4...... 9. C E Overview of the sequence of particles for possession........... specification: demonstrative particles ...................................................... Recapitulation .... Ablative-instrumental .............. -“e ..........2..............5.................................. C S Determination..........8................ Alleged pronouns ............3.......................................3.................. C T Numerals ............................................. Fraction and measure terminology .............4.................................. 5.. 7....5... number and case ....1......9............ 10......2...............2......... 10... 5................................................ Distributional relations of numbers ...2... 5.......................... Locative .. 5...... Interrogative pronouns .............................. 7. 36 39 40 40 41 42 43 44 44 47 49 49 50 50 50 51 51 53 55 55 56 57 57 57 57 58 61 61 61 66 67 67 69 Adjectives ..........................................2........ 9........................... 7.........4.3.......... 5.............3..vi 5...................................................... Ordinal numbers ..4.....................4..........................10.................. -ne(n) ............... 10. 10.... 9.......4................ Cardinal numbers ........................... 9..2...... C E Adverbs ...............5.......................... 10......................4.... 5....4.......... -bi .............................................. 5......... -ri(?) ...... Equative .. General ...................................................... Possessive pronouns ......................... 9.................6................6. 7.....................................4... -e(?) .. 5...............................1.... Dative ....

................................. 12............................ 12..2: 27–62.... Ventive indicators .............. Precative ..... 12.......12...... Non-ventive indicators . 12....................... 12..........5...........................7................4. Dimensional indicators without reference to a person? .......5.....3........../pl............. .................. 12... 12...11................................................. 12........ [nu] as a separate verbal base 12. 12. 12..........11.............4.......2........... Sorts of plurality (¢am†u reduplication) ................ 12..........11...........11.........8....... 12........................11...1...8.. The verb me .... Negative indicative .. 12.7..11......1..................... Verbal base ¢am†u/marû/sg.............11................ 12..... 12......3....... Prefixless finite verbal forms ................................. Cohortative .8...11........ Preliminaries ............... Prohibitive .....................7...................... grids ..................1......11...3....................10....7.....8...11.....2... Conjugation pattern 1: Intransitive (and passive) .......... Frustrative . Prefixed indicator [e..........9.... Affirmative 2 ......... 12.............. Modal indicators ....................6..8.................11..................2............................ Prefixed indicator [a(1)] ....... 12........................10........................................11.............7.......................................... 12......... Negative affirmative .............................2... 12.1.............. 12.. Affirmative 1 ...............1.. Dimensional indicators ........11............5............. Conjugation pattern 2b: Transitive ............ 12..... 12... Modal and connecting indicators ....... Conjugation ...........3.....6............. Suffixed -nu ................. Affirmative 3 ..7..... Was Sumerian a language with “split ergativity”? ......... 12...................... i] ........2.....1........ 12..2.4... 12.7.............7.... 12......11................... T The verb ....................... Indicative ... C 12............................................... 12................ 12... Conjugation pattern 2a: Transitive ....1: 1–26..9................... 12. 12........11–12.......... 12........... Plural verbs ............................... ¢am†u and marû ...... Note on verbal reduplication ..............11........................... vii 71 71 73 74 74 79 80 81 81 82 83 87 90 90 92 94 103 109 109 111 112 113 113 113 114 115 115 116 116 117 117 118 118 119 120 120 12...... Vetitive (negative precative) . Negative cohortative ......11...... 12..................

3....................44.. B-[eda]...1.......... [na-nga] ...1. ‘Irregular’ [inga-na] ..............12..2... Extended imperative ..14. 12....14..2...... 12........4..2.. B-[edam] . 12... 12. mes-Ane-pada construction ..12.. 12........ 12. 12.................1.... A precedes B ............. 12...........5............... B-[ed] ......... 12..2... Unextended imperative ....2....... [nu“-inga] ..viii  121 121 122 122 123 124 124 124 125 125 125 126 126 127 127 128 129 130 131 132 132 132 133 134 134 134 134 135 136 136 137 138 138 139 139 139 139 12.......................... mes-Ane-pada construction ........12....... Polite imperative . B-[ed].......... 12.4.......................... Prospective .......2..8....1.... 12.............13..... [he-nga] .........................1.14...14...............1........ 3rd sg............. 2nd sg........2. 12.. 12................ B-B-[Ø]: unextended bases ................2. Conjugated participle or “pronominal conjugation” . B-[ada] ................12.. 1st sg..2... B-[ede]............... person class ¢am†u .. 12.9. Summary of [inga] ..2.............6....5...14.......... [inga] ......2.............. ..... 12.12......12.....2... B-[a].6..... 12..4....... marû .....1.....................3....14............................ 12.... 12........ 12.....12... B-[a].......14........ [“i-nga] .2.2..........2.... 12.......12...........2........ 12..3........14.... B-[ed + copula] .14..2...... Connecting indicator [inga] ...13...........12. 12....... 1st sg... ¢am†u .............14.. 3rd pl. 12................ B-[ed]..2...........1............... 3rd sg........ Imperative ....2...2. B-[ede] .1...12........ [nu-nga] .......4.......2.......................2........ with or without copula ......... ¢am†u ..12.......12....14.......... B-[eda] ..3.. 12......2.. 12....14..... B-[ada] ...2............. 12.1.4.......1...........4.....14........4...3.................3..14....... B-[a] ...14......14.... person class ¢am†u ..1....4............ 12. non-person class ¢am†u . 12. B-[Ø].... Connecting indicators .......... Non-finite verbal forms .. 12.......2...............2............ 12... 12..............3.....14........14........14... 12.... [ga-nga] ........ 12................................2.. 12.........7..........12...... 12. 12..........12.4...13...1................................1....

.... Nominalized phrase with a possessive particle ......6............................ 12.2...15.......1.........14............3............ C T Post-nominal and/or post-verbal particles other than case particles (5........ person-class marû .....................7... 12..... Conjugated participles: open questions ....... [∞ge“en] ([∞gi“en]) ... 12....1........................ 2nd sg............. marû ..................16........14............... 12......... 12.................... 12.....2................13.....15. Verbalization no longer recognizable to us ..14.........15...1............16....1.14..15.....................16....16... The nominalized phrase is in the absolutive ...............15......... . Compound verbs: free formations .. Complete incorporation of the nominal element .... 12........14..... Irregular behaviour of the verb ∞ en/du ........2.....4.............................. 12..4) .......4...16.....2..... 12......15...... non-person class marû 3rd pl....9...3. ix 139 140 140 140 140 141 141 142 143 145 145 147 148 148 149 149 150 151 152 152 153 154 154 154 157 157 158 . [(e)“e] ... 12....16..2...4..1.....4.3............. 13.........5..........15..............7. 3rd sg...... 12... The nominalized phrase is in the terminative ........... 12... Extended nominal element ...8......... Difficult case: artificial splits? ............12.......... The nominalized phrase is in the ablative 12.......10....15..... Compound verbs with a verbalizer ....4....... deli-∞gu10-ne etc...11... person-class marû .. Clear cases of verbalization .......1.... 12...2. Nominalization of finite verbal forms .. “u ti/te(-∞g) and other compound verbs and the special behaviour of their “objects” ...15.......14......4....................... 12....................... 3rd sg.... 12... 12.............. 12.4............4.......... The nominalized phrase is in the locative 12........16....... 13..... 12..... 12.. 12...2... g 12....16...........14.1. The nominalized phrase is in the equative .... The nominalized phrase is in the genitive 12.............1... Compound verbs ...

............................................................9.. ilu. 15....................8. [ulili] ..............3..................5...........1.........5................10... 15... 15.......... 13.........13.................................5....................7................................ C S The Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area C E Summary—and what is still missing? Bibliography .................. [“uba] ....... [mel(i)ea] ......................................................................................................................................................................... 15.. [heam] .....................................................................................3.........3................................2.... [ri] ............................1..............2..................... ......................... 15.................................. 15......................................................................................4.............................. [a] ..................... C 15..... F Exclamations ..................................... [uda] ..............................................1..................................................12.......... [mu] ......................................... [tukumbi] ............... 14... 14........................... [ena] ..4..11......................... 14............... 13.......4...2.......1......................................................... [alulu] ...... [ua] . [nanna] .................................................. Conjunctions .............. 158 159 160 161 161 161 162 162 162 163 164 164 165 167 167 167 167 168 168 168 168 168 169 169 169 170 170 171 173 179 181 187 C F Conjunctions and subjunctions ................................. 14.2...... 14....... 14................ 15......... [inu] ........................................................... elala...............2........... 15.. [u] ...2...... [i Utu] .. [gana] .....1. [iginzu] .............................................. 14. 14.............. 15....................... 15.......x  13...... [ma] ..... ilulamma] ......................6............... 15....1.............................................................. [allili] .....................2.......... [aua] .......................................... [ellu......................................................................................................................2......................... 15...... 14...................................................................... Subjunctions ............... [alala] ........ Index ................................................................................2.... C S Emesal .........

and punctuation. In order to avoid footnotes. or even that some paragraph was misplaced. For all this. spelling. The Publishing House Brill and their Editors. my most sincere gratitude is due to Nicholas. Patricia Radder. style. himself not unfamiliar with the problems of Sumerian grammar. by pointing out that an argument was unclear. . Nicholas Postgate of Trinity College. While offering my own personal ideas—some of which will no doubt be considered highly subjective—I have tried to discuss. the main text has been interspersed with numerous “notes” where secondary comments and explanations are to be found. Cambridge University. differing opin­ ions as often as possible. therefore. proved to be the ideal adviser. I was in need of someone to correct my grammar. have been extremely patient with the author’s self-indulgent interpretation of the deadline originally set for the publication of the book. As a non-English speaker. A col­ lated list of indexes and glossaries to the secondary literature con­ cerning the Sumerian Language” (Philadelphia 1993 ff. that a descrip­ tion was lacking evidence. They are.PREFACE The present Sumerian Grammar with which the Publisher Brill kindly entrusted the author is essentially based on introductory classes of Sumerian offered at the Institut für Assyriologie und Hethitologie of Munich University over the last twenty years. These corrections were made partly by mail and to a considerable degree during a three day stay by the author at Trinity College and at the home of the Postgates. 1998. Tanja Cowall and Mevr. He not only offered innumerable corrections but quite often also guid­ ance. Mevr. or at least quote. Part of the “History” was also pre­ sented in lectures at the University of London and at Oxford University between October 15 and November 5. entitled to my heart­ felt gratitude. as well as on a two semester course on “Geschichte der sumerischen Sprache” (winter 1996/97 and summer 1997). The author gratefully acknowledges that he made frequent use of Steve Tinney’s lexical “Index to the Secondary Literature.).

had typed on his beloved “Olympia”. unused to the world of computers.xii  Frau Ursula Hellwag MA of Munich most kindly took upon her­ self the trouble to compose the final draft of the manuscript which the author. December 2002 . München.

= The Assyrian Dictionary of the University of Chicago. Corpus des inscriptions “royales” présargoniques de Laga“.1. Ean. 1972). 40 = ETCSL 4. Kraus. EG = The Epic of Gilgame“. 1931 ff. AIUON Ali Letters AOAT ARET ARM ARN AS ASJ AWL Bau B BWL CAD CIRPL CLAM Copper and Silver = Disputation. Ali. R. = Assyriological Studies. 1908–1920.ABBREVIATIONS = Archiv für Orientforschung. DP = M. sezione linguistica. 1959– 1981. = F. = ETCSL 2. = M. Diri = lexical series diri SI. Berlin. The Canonical Lamentations of Ancient Mesopotamia. 1983. 1969 ff.A siàku = (w)atru. = Annali dell ’Istituto Universitario Orientale di Napoli. = J. of Pennsyl­ vania. Çı</H. The Curse of Agade. = Acta Sumerologica.2. 1985 ff. S. 1988. 1950 ff. Univ. Akkadisches Handwörterbuch.02. Alotte de la Fuyë. = E. 1956 ff. 1956. Bauer. 1959 ff. Altbabylonische Rechtsurkunden aus Nippur. Curse Akkade = J. = Archivi reali di Ebla. Sollberger. AfO AHw. = CT 36. Lambert. G. = W. von Soden. F. . E. 1923 ff. = M. A.6. = Alter Orient und Altes Testament. Kızılyay/F. = Eanatum in CIRPL. see ETCSL 5. Documents présargoniques. Cohen. Altsumerische Wirtschaftstexte aus Lagasch (= StudPohl 9. 1952.3. 1959. Babylonian Wisdom Literature. Cooper. 1964). = W. Sumerian Letters: Two Collections from the Old Babylonian Schools. = Archives royales de Mari. Testi.5. (diss. 1979 ff.

= Entemena (Enmetena) in CIRPL.8.3.3. JCS 25 (1973) 105–169. Enkidu and the Nether World.2. Gilgame“ and Agga = D. = ¿. Gilgame“ and Huwawa B = D. = ETCSL 1.4.xiv ELA  = Enmerkar and the Lord of Aratta (S. 1984. HSS = Harvard Semitic Studies. Enkidu and the Netherworld s. R. O. = ETCSL 1. Gungunum = D.2. R. Frayne. Forde Nebraska = N. Sitzungsbericht 1993/4.1.1. Der Mythos “Inanna und Enki” unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Liste der me (= StudPohl 10. Innin “ag. GEN Gilgame“ und Huwawa A = D. ZA 81 (1991) 165–233. = ETCSL 5. 1967. GEN = Gilgame“. 1912 ff. Sjöberg.6. = ETCSL 1. Nebraska Cuneiform Texts of the Sumerian Ur III Dynasty. = ETCSL 1. Farmer’s Instructions = M. Cohen. 5. Baltimore.Hist. Klasse. 1973). 1974). A Hymn to the Goddess Inanna by the en-Priestess . Aula Orientalis. Inanna/Enki = G. = ETCSL 1.2.2. in-nin-“à-gur4-ra. Bayer.. HSAO = Heidelberger Studien zum Alten Orient. En. Inanna’s Descent to the Nether World (diss.1. = ETCSL 1. der Wiss. Father and Son = Å.5. W. = Enmerkar and Ensu¢ke“edana.8. 1993. Gilgamesh and Akka.8. Berlin 1979. Farber-Flügge. Katz. Sjöberg. Edzard.3. I = Enanatum I in CIRPL. Akad.1. Enz. 1967 ff.8. Gilgame“. Suppl.1.5.1. A. = Enentarzi in CIRPL. 1973). Edzard.1.2. = ETCSL 1. Forde.4.1. Sladek. = ETCSL 5.8. Inanna’s Descent = W. O..4.8. = ETCSL 1. ZA 80 (1990) 165–203. Phil. Ent. id. = ETCSL 1. Civil. Enlil and Ninlil 2 = Enlil and Sud. RIME 4 (1990) 114 ff. EnmEns.1. W. HSM = Harvard Semitic Museum (tablet signature).

The Ke“ Temple Hymn. Nanna’s Journey = A.2. Lugalbanda and Enm.2.80. Barton. Lamentation over the Destruction of Ur (= AS 12. = ETCSL 4. NATN = D.6. 1983. Wilcke. = ETCSL 4. RIA 7 (1987–90) 121–125. Jaques = M. 1979 ff.2. 1982. A. C. W. ITT = Inventaires des Tablettes de Tello. 1951 ff.8. I. Univ. Miscellaneous Babylonian Inscrip­ tions I. in: MSL 4 (1956) 129 ff. = ETCSL 1.1.2.2. Ke“ Hymn = G. 1947 ff. NBGT = Neo-Babylonian Grammatical Texts. 1910–1921. LUGAL UD ME-LÁM-bi NIRGÁL. Kramer. MBI = G.3. LamSumUr = P. 1934 ff. JNES = Journal of Near Eastern Studies. The Collection of the Sumerian Temple Hymns (= TCS 3. Martu A = A. Lugalbanda II s.. de Genève n.07.2. ibid. ETCSL 1.d. C. xv En¢eduanna. 1973). 1918.5. Neo-Sumerian Archival Texts pri­ marily from Nippur. LamUr I s. MDP = Mémoires de la Délégation en Perse. . Jaques.8. Sjöberg.2. see Lugalbanda I. [1999]). ZA 65 (1975) 161–253. 125–129. 1900 ff. MAD = Materials for the Assyrian Dictionary. J. Le vocabulaire des sentiments dans les textes sumériens (diss. = ETCSL 1. = ETCSL 2. Lament of Ur. Wilcke. N.1. MEE = Materiali epigrafici di Ebla. B. Lugal = J. in: ¿. JCS = Journal of Cuneiform Studies. Owen.1. Ferrara. 1942 ff. The Lamentation over the Destruction of Sumer and Ur (= MesCiv. = ETCSL 2. SGL I (1959) 120–140.3. Falkenstein. Iraq = ( journal) Iraq. Nanna-Suen’s Journey to Nippur (= StudPohl SM 2. MSL = Materialien zum sumerischen Lexikon/Materials for the Sumerian Lexicon. Gragg. 1989). 1.2. 1969) 157–188. 1940). Lament of Ur = S. = ETCSL 4. ETCSL 1. Michalowski.12. 1952 ff. Lugalbanda I s. van Dijk. 1937 ff. MCS = Manchester Cuneiform Studies.

1965.-hist. NF 39. = Orientalia Suecana. PN = Personal Name. = ETCSL 5. 282–86. Charpin.1. Der Rechtsfall der En-hedu-Ana im Lied nin-me-“ara (= AOAT 246. SBH = G. 44. .. = ETCSL 2. PBS = University of Pennsylvania. RIME = The Royal Inscriptions of Mesopotamia. 1932 ff. Abhandl. 1974 ff. Cohen 1975 (see bibl. Sin-kà“id = D.4. 1944. Rìm-Sin D = D. 440–64. PSD = The Sumerian Dictionary of the University Museum of the University of Pennsylvania. Sumerischen “Königshymnen” der Isin-Zeit. 1908.). = S. = ETCSL 4. SKIZ = W.07. Publications of the Baby­ lonian Section. RGTC = Répertoire géographique des textes cunéiformes. RIA = Reallexikon der Assyriologie (und Vorderasiatischen Archäologie). Die neusumerischen Gerichtsurkunden (= Bayer. Kramer. Le clergé d’Ur au siècle d’Hammurabi. RSO = Rivista degli Studi Orientali. OB = Old Babylonian. Neusumerische Rechts. = M. Schooldays = S.6. NRVN = M. OLZ = Orientalistische Literaturzeitung.xvi NG  = A. SLTNi. OBGT = Old Babylonian Grammatical Texts. Early Periods. New Series. Kızılyay. 1956–1957). R. 1911 ff. Frayne. Reisner. Nik. 1990 ff.9.27. E. RIME 4. Klasse. Philos. Sumerian Literary Texts from Nippur. Drevnosti Vosto‘nyja III/2. Falkenstein. NS = Neue Serie. = Orientalia (NS).1. Nova Series. Çı</H. N. 1997). Zgoll. Ph. 1965. Kramer. 1928 ff. V. OS = Old Sumerian. . Or. 1896. N. 1898 ff. 1907 ff. RA = Revue d’assyriologie et d’archéologie orientale. . 1886 ff. NS = Neo-Sumerian. H. 1952 ff. OrSuec. 1986. Nikolskij. Sumerisch-babylonische Hymnen . 1984 ff. in: MSL 4 (1956) 46 ff.und Ver­ waltungsurkunden aus Nippur. Ninme“ara = A. JAOS 69 (1949) 199–215.. Akademie der Wiss. Scribe and Son see Father and Son. 40. Römer. Ninurta G = M.

Sumerische Rechtsurkunden des III. Ur-Namma Code = Wilcke 2002. E. G. TM = Tell Mardikh (Ebla) (tablet signature). UET = Ur Excavations. 1932–1934. 1911. Sumerian Proverbs. . Sumerian Religious Texts. (= Bayer.4. 1968). 1985). Gordon. = ETCSL 2. M. = ETCSL 2. = Texts in the Iraq Museum. TMH (NF) = Texte und Materialien der Frau Professor Hilprecht Collection . = ETCSL 2. Studia Pohl. = Urukagina (Erikagina) in CIRPL.4. . = Tabulae Cuneiformes a F. 1954 ff. Abhandlung NF 67. E.2.4.5. 1937. 167–217. 1972). 1966 ff. Jestin.4.06. 1964 ff. S – O – V SP SP SR = = = = xvii Stat. La trouvaille de Dréhem. Jahrtausends . . Series Prior. E.04. Philadelphia.02. SRT StudPohl STVC ”ulgi A ”ulgi B ”ulgi D ”ulgi F = = = = = = = = = Alster 1981–1983. Textes. Statue. 1901. Hall. O. . = ETCSL 2.4. (StudSem. Klein 1981. Tempelurkunden aus Telloh. . Th.2. 1961 ff. G. Ur Lament see Lement of Ur. Castellino. 1928 ff. Klein 1981.24. TrDr. Klein 1981. A Study of the Sumerian MoonGod . Ukg. 1934). . 1967 ff. = ETCSL 5. Akademie der Wissenschaften. Texts from Cuneiforms Sources. (diss. de Liagre Böhl collectae. D.6. Reisner. .01. 1959. 50–123. TUT = G.2. M. TS” = R. de Genouillac.2. Textes cunéiformes du Louvre. R. NF = 1937. Sumerian Texts of Varied Contents (= OIP 16. 124–66. . = ETCSL 2. Tablettes sumériennes de ”urup­ pak . = H. I. Chiera. Edzard. 1924. Chiera.2. . ”ulgi X TCL TCS Three Men of Adab TIM TLB = = = Subject – Object – Verb. . 42.. 1910 ff.

xviii



Ur-Namma’s Death = E. Flückiger-Hawker, Urnamma of Ur in Sumerian Literary Tradition (= OBO 166, 1999), 93–182; = ETCSL 2.4.1.1. Ur-Ninurta = D. R. Frayne, RIME 4 (1990) 64–68. VS = Vorderasiatische Schriftdenkmäler der (könig­ lichen) Museen zu Berlin, 1907 ff. WO = Die Welt des Orients, 1947 ff. WF = A. Deimel, Wirtschaftstexte aus Fara (= WVDOG 45, 1924). WVDOG = Wissenschaftliche Veröffentlichungen der Deutschen Orient-Gesellschaft, 1900 ff. YOS = Yale Oriental Series, Babylonian Texts, 1915 ff. ZA = Zeitschrift für Assyriologie und verwandte Gebiete, 1886–1938, or: und Vorderasiatische Archäologie, 1939 ff.

CHAPTER ONE

THE SUMERIAN LANGUAGE

Für Olympia, die mir seit 1954 alle meine Arbeiten geschrieben hat und die mir auf drei Kontinenten gefolgt ist.

1.1. G C Sumerian was called eme-ki-en-gi-ra “tongue of Kiengir (Sumer)” or eme-gi7(-r) in Sumerian proper and li“àn ”umeri(m) “tongue of ”umeru” in Akkadian. The replacing of [“] by [s] in most modern languages (but not Russian) has its analogue in the change of [“] for [s] in names of the Hebrew bible. Sumerian is characterized by the interaction of a word base (nom­ inal, verbal, other) which may be invariable or subject to variation (e.g., change of vowel, reduction, extension), and an intricate system of prefixed and suffixed morphemes. The word base itself is impen­ etrable by other morphemes. Unlike Semitic, no infixes occur. Cf. ha-ma-ab-“úm-mu [ha-m+a-b-“um-e] “he should give it to me” (WO 8, 173: 11b2): precative-to-me-it-base give-ergative (3rd person sing. person class). The number of prefixed morphemes varies between zero and six for the verb, zero and one for the noun; the number of suffixed morphemes between zero and three for the verb, zero and three for the noun. Words of considerable length may be built up that way, e.g., hu-mu-na-ni-ib-gi4-gi4 “let him return it to him there” (6 syllables, not comparable, however, with Akkadian ittanablakkatùnikkunù“im “they will, over and again, revolt against you”, 10 syllables). In both strings of morphemes, prefixed or suffixed, the sequence of the individual elements is unchangeable. The morphemes are mostly monofunctional, as is the rule in agglutinating languages, and very rarely multifunctional as the morphemes of Semitic or Indo-European. Instead of gender, Sumerian distinguishes a “person” and a “non­ person” class. The case system includes an ergative, and the verbal

2

 

inflection is characterized by ergativity (whether there was, or evolved, “split ergativity” is still a matter of debate—see below 12.7.5). There is well developed, but far from perfect, concord between dimen­ sional suffixes of the noun and dimensional indicators prefixed to the verb, e.g., DN-ra object mu-na(-n)-§rú “to god NN, object, hebuilt-to-him”. As for number, singular and non-singular may be opposed; there are different ways to express plurality. The general word order of Sumerian is S – O – V, unless some part of speech is taken to the front for focus. The nominal or verbal base is the essential carrier of meaning, and only bases are listed as entries in the lexical lists. Occasionally, meaning is modified by the occurrence of a “frozen” morpheme. Also, the composition of two (rarely more) nominal word bases may lead to a new meaning beyond the sum of the meanings of the individ­ ual parts of the compound, e.g., é-gal “house big” = “palace” (Akk. èkallu) or má-tur “boat small”, a special type of boat (Akk. maturru). In the light of these general aspects, Sumerian may be compared to such languages as Georgian, Basque, or Itelmen and many oth­ ers. Such comparison is, however, purely structural and of no con­ sequence for the question of the linguistic affiliation of Sumerian. 1.2. T ()       S Scholars have wasted much effort looking for living cognates of ancient Sumerian, not realizing that the problem is practically insol­ uble for the following reasons: Sumerian must have separated from a hypothetical language fam­ ily of which it was part in the middle or late fourth millennium B.C. at the latest. We know next to nothing about the sound and struc­ ture of Sumerian before the middle of the third millennium. Thus there is a gap of at least two thousand years between that time and the oldest reconstructible form of any of the languages which have been compared to Sumerian (e.g., Turkish, Hungarian, Sino-Tibetan). Efforts to find cognates have been exclusively based on the sound of individual words. Yet according to W. Deeters (1963, 76) who dis­ cussed the problem of Basque-Caucasian affinities, any words in lan­

  

3

guages A and B that sound alike today are more likely to be unre­ lated than related because they are the product of phonetic evolu­ tion over several millennia—not to mention the possible diachronical change of meaning. While according to Deeters, if contemporane­ ous language X were really a descendant from a language Y, related to *Proto-Sumerian more than five or six millennia ago, the sound structure and vocabulary of that hypothetical language Y are liable to have become altered beyond recognition. The only essay going beyond the comparison of Sumerian and another language by way of vocabulary is G. Steiner’s “Sumerisch und Elamisch: Typologische Parallelen”, ASJ 12 (1990) 143–76. Steiner stresses structural similarities in the case system, pronominal system, verb (“intransitiv-passivische Verbalauffassung”), word order (S – O – V), nominalization of the verbal complex, and in his sum­ mary he arrives at the cautious statement that “diese beiden Sprachen trotz ihrer sehr unterschiedlichen morphologischen Struktur zu einer ‘Sprachgruppe’ zusammengefaßt werden können”. An essential differ­ ence between Sumerian and Elamite is, however, the fact that the Sumerian verb base is, as a rule, embedded in a string of prefixes and suffixes whereas Elamite almost exclusively uses only suffixes. In any case, even if Sumerian and Elamite were really (remote) relatives, the general problem of the linguistic affinity of Sumerian would remain unresolved. 1.3. T    S From since at least the end of the fourth millennium, Sumerians were neighbours of the Elamites in Elam and of Semites, both seden­ tary and nomadic, in Mesopotamia proper. Any attempt at extend­ ing this picture must rest on speculation for lack of solid proof. The earliest evidence for the Elamite language stems from clay tablets with “Proto-Elamite” script whose find spots extend as far as Tepe Ya˙yà, ca 900 km ESE of Susa (corresponding to the distance between Uruk and Damascus as the crow flies). Although the “Proto-Elamite” script has not yet been convincingly deciphered, it seems plausible to assume that it was a predecessor of the Elamite linear script of the Akkade period which has been shown to represent the Elamite language.

or adstratum.. step by step. Salonen 1968. 11). goddesses at home in the Ω ΩGirsu/Laga“ region. In general. But we have no means at our disposal to prove such a supposition. as against Iran.. has been a veritable lan­ guage museum. Our judgement in this matter is. within Mesopotamia proper. Anatolia or the Caucasus which. at least during the cen­ turies before the Third Dynasty of Ur. 31. because these names defy all efforts to explain them by way of Sumerian etymology. deformation by analogy. divine names such as Nan“e or Gatumdu. e. it may be said that the Mesopotamian plain was not conducive to a great variety of languages. Thus. and so direct language contact can be excluded. See. e. there is no monolithic substratum that would have left. “Thus.. Landsberger 1944. in a sort of primeval age. the very sobering discussion of G.C.g.C. B. Rubio 1999. Nan“e and Gatumdu (and others) may be pre-Sumerian names. and the publication of the Ebla glosses for Sumerian lexical items brought more than one surprise. however. places) and vocabulary. a situation arose which greatly stimulated mutual influences. They most probably never were immediate neighbours of the Sumerians. For some time. its vestiges in Sumerian lexicon. ª as may many place names.. “On the alleged ‘pre-Sumerian’ substratum” where the author arrives at the conclusion (p.g. Sumerian was heading for a Sumero-Akkadian “linguistic area”— . a “monosyllabic myth” has been popular among Sumerologists. highly subjective because we know nothing of the early history of Sumerian and its sound structure. relegating words of more than one syllable to a “ProtoEuphratian” substratum. superseded by Sumerian. until our days. As a consequence of early close contacts between Sumerians and Semites. Cf. our first tentative iden­ tification of Sumerian “sound” hardly goes farther back than 24th century B.4   It cannot be excluded that. apud A. It is a well known fact that proper names are specially prone to changes of all kind (slurring. 1–16. Hurrians—with a language of their own—first appear in cuneiform sources toward the end of the third millennium B. All one can detect is a complex and fuzzy web of borrow­ ings whose directions are frequently difficult to determine”. abbrevi­ ation. but which left their traces in Sumerian proper names (gods. however. Sumerian had neighbours who spoke a language—or languages—that were. popular etymology). In fact. may belong to a substratum.

   5 and so was Akkadian. Quite a few occurrences of apparent “deSumerization” or Akkadization of the language have led some schol­ ars to the opinion that Sumerian had ceased to be a living spoken language as early as the end of the third millennium. . The “areal” situation will be discussed more in detail below in Chapter 17.

This page intentionally left blank .

i. each short or long. T. à. Poebel suggested the existence of Akkadian [o] (AS 9. ù]. Since Akkadian did not express in its explana­ tory glosses more than four vowel phonemes.1. we are restricted to that set also for Sumerian. 3. [a. followed by St. i. as do scribal “errors”. u. The sign name of HI.. Jones [1977] 21–28. an important source for the sounds of Sumerian are loanwords in Akkadian and in a restricted number of case sign names: kar > kàrum (not *karrum) “quay. the identification of Sumerian consonants depends essentially on Akkadian evidence: glosses in syllabaries and vocabularies.. = AOAT 203) and Aa. *[kal]. The minimal set would be A. Being unable. no means have so far been found to achieve a precise recon­ struction of Sumerian vowel qualities. B. 116 f. Note: A. whereas é-gal > èkallum (not *èkàlum) suggests short [a] in gal. Westenholz who extended the evidence for “The Phoneme /o/ in Akkadian” (ZA 81 [1991] 10–19). E/I. because the values (“Lautwerte”) of nearly all signs used in the Sumerian syllabaries of different places and periods have been identified by way of Akkadian syllabic spellings or—additionally—from the socalled tu-ta-ti syllabaries. to reconstruct Sumerian [o] from spelling or spelling variants. Whilst.e. some arguments may be adduced for the existence of an opposition between short and long vowels [v : 9v]. Apart from lexical glosses. G One may say that we see Sumerian through an Akkadian glass darkly. i.e. however.1. U.CHAPTER TWO HOW WE READ SUMERIAN 2. the behaviour of loanwords and.e. è.e. i.. not *du-ug-gu *[duggu] which makes us prefer a long [ù] in Sumerian dùg. Lieberman “The Phoneme /o/ in Sumerian” (Fs. DÙG is du-ú-gu [dùgu]. with fnn. mooring place” points to a long [à] in kar. See more in detail below. Just as for the vowels. we disregard it throughout the present grammar. e. du10(-g). [dùg]. ì. with A and U clearly distinguished in spelling while for E/I very often there is the same ambivalence as in Akkadian spelling. as regards vowel quantity. 1939. sign .. 1 and 1(!)). [kàr]. partly again. Spelling variants in parallel texts (synchronic or diachronic) some­ times prove a precious source for phonology. i.

. but only based on context. So. 44:308). This distinction is not. It is a matter of convention whether we transliterate GI”.TÚG. This difference in our transliteration is due to the fact that the oldest Akkadian syllabary known at present. namely ∞gé“tug. b) Syllabograms or syllabic signs. and with tables. unvoiced stops (K. The reconstruction of the hypothetical inventory of Sumerian consonantal phonemes is. 15 f. in its “classical” form of the Gudea and Ur III period. Comparison of èkallu (é-gal) “palace” (above) and unetukku (ù-ne-e-dug4) “letter” show that.1. ga-la: GAL (p. mu “name”.8   names. offers voiced instead of unvoiced stops: du-ú: KA (p. a) Logograms or word signs. Proto-Ea (MSL 14 [1979] 3–81). in GI”.. a sub-class of (b). though. Classification of cuneograms Sumerian is written. used to convey a sound only. syllabograms used to specify the reading of a single sign (or of a sign group). etc. “year”.(verbal prefix). the signs GI” und TÚG yield the reading of PI [∞ge“tug]. at least in our Latin translit­ erations. comprising the final [k] of the genitive morpheme [ak] and the [e] of the ergative case).). -zu “your” (sing. -ke4 (nominal suffix. d) Signs for number or the combined notation of measuring unit + . ba. T’). simple and post-glottalized (T. without primary reference to meaning.PI = ∞gé“tug. e. dùg “good”. c) Phonetic indicators.g.g. e.. much more difficult than in the case of the vowels. lú “person”. gu-za “chair”.. i. 2. S  S 2.PI as ∞gé“tug or as gi“-túgge“tug with two ∞ phonetic indicators raised. ba “to attribute”. expressing a nominal or verbal word base. This question will be discussed in more detail. mu(verbal prefix). e. “sweet (said of water)”. D) in our rendering of the Sumerian basic expressions.e.2. visible.TÚG. however. the unvoiced part being either a single phoneme or split into two. by means of five classes of cuneiform signs. T) in the Akkadian loanwords corre­ spond to voiced stops (G. below pp.2.g. The complicated phonetic relation between Sumerian and Akkadian consonants (again: as we transliterate them) has given rise to much speculation as to whether there was a double or a triple set of stops in Sumerian: D : T or D : T1 : T2.. 50:471).

“container”.g. KU6 (ku6 “fish”).LÍLki = Nibruki “the city Nippur” (Enlil’s main cult centre).. lú nu-kiri6 = nukaribbu “gardener”. following the names of birds..g. e.g.. LÚ (lú “person”). e. The determinatives are: AN (or DIªGIR). banmin “two ban” = 2 × 1 bán = 2 × 10 sila (1 sila = about 1 litre). na4nunuz = erimmatu (egg-stone. e. e. garment”).g. duga-da = gur5 = adagurru (vessel with pointed bottom). min “two”...    9 number. following place names.. earth”). EN. GI (gi “reed”). gi ∞ pisan = pis/“annu “box”. d(ea). túgNÍG. ÍD (íd “river. e. DUG (dug “vessel”)..EN.g. tumu“en “pigeon”. preceding divine names. preceding names of cloth or gar­ ments.. íd Idigina “Tigris”.LÁM = lam¢u““û. preceding terms of earthenware. urudu “copper”). (a long or knee-length skirt). Ú (ú “grass”. but note íd-éren-na “army canal” where íd is part of the name. ∞ge“ban“ur = pa““ùru “table”. and they are raised in our transliteration: AN.g.g. The Akkadian loanwords clearly show that GE” cannot be part of ª the word. ge“al = allu “hoe”.g. ª GE” (∞ge“ “wood. mainly of copper). úbúr-da = urnû “mint”(?). preceding names of plants. e.g.. following the names of garden plants. canal”).g. e. But note in contrast ge“-ùr “beam” with the Akkadian ∞ loanword form gu“ùru showing that ge“ is part of the word.LÍL = dEn-líl “the god Enlil”.. eridu. preceding names of rivers or main canals. Unugki “Uruk”. NA4 (na4 “stone”). “capricorn” (constellation). a bead). e.g.KI = EN. e. preceding reeds and objects made of reed.. e. e) Determinatives: these are signs which precede or follow words or names in order to specify them as belonging to semantic groups. . preceding terms for wooden objects or ∞ names of trees. tree”). TÚG (túg “textile. e. KI (ki “place. abbreviated as raised d for d(eus)..g.g.LÍL. e. suhur-má“ku6 = su¢urmà“u “goat-fish” (mythological being). SAR (sar “vegetable”). MU”EN (mu“en “bird”)... following the names of fish. uruduza-rí-in (zarinnu) (mediocre quality of metal.g. preceding names of stones and stone objects. preceding terms of met­ als and metal objects. e. “plant”). Determinatives can be proven not to have been pronounced (although doubt may exist in specific instances). ∞ URUDU (erida. also determined by ge“. “úmsar = “ùmu “garlic”. preceding names of some (male) professions. e.

2. Old Bab. as Eanna where Sumerian spelling is reflected in our Latin transcription.2. type b). ki. Evolution of Sumerian spelling The progress achieved during a period of at least one millennium may be roughly described as a continuous advance towards phonetic . mu“en. É-an-na “House of Heaven/An”. RSO 32 (1957) 103–108.. e. ∞ge“. To the first category belong d. spelling proper In “standard” (Ur III. we have to stress once more that their distinction is nearly exclusively based on context: ba or mu may belong to (a) or (b). R.g. (c). hide. the scribe turned the verbal form i-din [iddin] “he gave” into ventive [iddinam] “he gave (it) to me”.is a syllabogram. found its way into Akkadian. or (e). So. base tu“ a logogram. e.) -me-e“ [me“]. al-tu“ “he was sitting there” where prefix al. The [m] of the syllabogram [ma] is redundant.. 2.. the sign conveying simply the [a] of the locative case. but also be used as a syllabogram (b) in (mainly Old Bab. he wrote i-din-nam. whereas accompanying (prefixed or suffixed) morphemes are expressed by syl­ labograms (see 2. occasionally. Even number signs (d) are not exempt from ambiguity.g. note also i (= ia = 5 × A”) or à“ (= 6 × DI”). in fact. or Old Bab. There is.2. lu-úb = luppu “(leather) bag”.. E” (3 × U) may mean “thirty” (d). the nominal or verbal base is frequently noted by a logogram (type a). ”E (“e “barley”) and ZÍD (zíd “flour”) are occasionally used as determinatives for types of grain or flour respectively. ku6. “in Ur”. A special feature of Sumerian spelling is the “repetition” of the final consonant of a logogram by the initial consonant of a follow­ ing syllabogram to indicate simply the addition of a vowel. preceding leather objects. Eana would correspond to what is meant in Sumerian. e. When. because.10 ku“   KU” (ku“ “skin.1. Combination of cuneograms. [Urim-a] is not spelled *Úrimki-a. Kraus. To resume the five sign classes.2.g. leather”). Here there is no reason to suppose consonantal length. 2. Note: This way of spelling has.) Sumerian spelling. Whereas the use of some determinatives is rather consistent and more or less predictable (at least from the Akkade period onward). but Úrimki-ma. The phenomenon is purely orthographic. We traditionally read the main temple complex of Uruk. See F. this does not apply to others. ∞ge“ to (a). no reason to read -nn-.3. in Old Akk.

in Ur III and early Old Bab. pl. to denote the 3rd p. and combina­ tions of numbers + measure. mu-na§rú “he/she built for him/her” stood for [mu-na-n-§rú]. the inventory was insufficient (signs like bam. gi both for “reed” and the syllables [gi. dè. [a“] = a“ or á“.or -b-. Note: Neither Sumerian nor Akkadian syllabaries offer a clear 1 : 1 relation of signs and sounds. With lugal-me. Phonetic abstraction (writing. fn. in slightly varied form (GI­ gunû) for the notion of “return” [ge4]) opened the way for noting syllables of the types [V]. The decisive invention to remedy the situation was made by a scribe—or scribal school—of pre-Sargonic times.g.e. were left unnoted..g. see 2. é“ is mainly used. Jacobsen. a syllable-closing consonant was just disregarded. e. e. [VC]. and on the other hand. bí. Until Ur III. one sign may denote different syllables. e.1. and partly still in OB. placed immediately before the verbal base. were never created). [CV]. Therefore. “Syllabaries” (= inventories of syllabograms) came into being. type b. the person or non-person class ergative or absolutive markers -n. medial.. NE = ne. only context could show whether lugal-me(“) “they are kings” or lugalme(n) “I am/you are king” was meant.e. mag. final) whereas others were of restricted use.    11 exactitude. This “nuclear” writing (Th.. ZA 52 [1957] 91 ff.. i. The Akkadian rather than Sumerian scribal world must be credited with this invention— unique in the world history of writing—because in Akkadian with its frequent three-consonantal roots non-notation of a syllable-closing consonant would have led to much more ambiguity than in Sumerian. . ge] and. e. identical or minimally different syllable-sounds could be noted by different signs. [CVC].. numerals. reconstruction of a given verbal form often depends on our—subjective—interpretation. suffix on certain verbal forms.2. At first. ba-u-ge“]. because they always were found in a close syllable.g. e.g. they could occur in any position of a word (initial. Here. Some syllabograms were freely applicable. the exact rendering of every spoken syllable.... nal. i. ba-ug7ge “they died” stood for [ba’uge“. mu-un for [mun]. One of the main problems was the notation of syllable-closing con­ sonants in syllables of the type CVC. On the one hand.g. who combined CV1 + V1C to denote CV1C. Writing Sumerian started from only noting bases. etc. [en] = en or èn (LI). 1) still disregarded any additional morphemes (nominal and verbal prefixes and suffixes).

This page intentionally left blank .

kìtu) “reed mat” [u]: dub (Akk. 32 ff. sign name for HU. allu) “hoe” gag (Akk. nàru. “God of Heaven” [e]: mu“en.1. OB a-a or a-ya (cf. nail”. and Old Bab.> -kk-. weapon” gala (Akk.CHAPTER THREE MINIMALIA OF SUMERIAN PHONOLOGY AND SYLLABIC STRUCTURE 3. structure”).). ¢u-u“-“u [¢u““u]. sign name for HU” [ù]: nùn.NUN. OS A (as in A-kur-gal “the father is the great mountain”). ànu) “sky. Ass. contracted form Enukkù can be explained only as the product of -n(a)k.1 note). PSD A/1.> -kk-. nuàru) “singer. harbour” nàr (Akk. kalû) “singer (in cult). the Old Bab. ènu) “en priest” [i]: apin (Akk.NA = *Anunnakù. bùr (Akk. musician” àn (Akk. Vowels [a]: al (Akk. †uppu) “tablet” hu“. *E/Anùnakù) Note: Not dA. in “úm-sikil (Akk. he(n)gallu) “let it be” = “abundance” [à]: kàr (Akk. P 3. heaven”. The Sumerian word for “father”. pùru) “(watertight) vessel” The question of Akkadian (Old Akk. leads one to suppose the form . MU”EN [è]: èn (Akk. kàru) “quay. The existence of an (original) diphthong [ay] in Sumerian may be inferred from the Hebrew loan form of Akkadian èkallu (< é-gal): hè§al <*haykal (also Arabic haykal “temple. parakku) “dais” (hé-)∞gál (Akk.1. mooring wall. kakku) “peg. and not of *-nn(a)k.) [o] and [ò] has been discussed by Westenholz (see above 2. cantor” bárag (Akk. epinnu) “plough” bukin (Akk. mu-“e-en-nu [mu“ennu] “bird”. “umsikillu) “garlic” [ì]: kìd (Akk. “club.1. bukinnu) “trough” sikil. dE4-nun-na (Akk.

i. from the spelling. The minimal inventory of Sumerian vocalic phonemes would then read: a. we will once more base ourselves on loanwords in Akkadian and on sign-names as the most reliable sources.14   [ay].g. yielding our transliteration díb. 39 (below). see Gelb 19612. en hé-a “whether he can be an en” is replaced by in he-a (TIM 9. è. In the (non-standard) spelling of a curse formula (copy of a ”ulgi inscription). correspondingly. but s[i]-ig in Ea I 298 (MSL 14. 191). dental. Consonants When trying to establish the (minimal) set of Sumerian consonantal phonemes. needed with regard to our reconstruction: (a) It is not always clear. ì. It is not clear to the author how this change of vowel came about. 3. For stops (labial. but di-ib in Ea I 156 (MSL 14. 51). ù. however. whether [e] or [i] was intended. e. 184). u. So. [aya] (only the first. 35:19). à. velar). there may have been an opposition of open and closed vowels (such as is the case in modern German). however. being a diphthong strictly speak­ ing). PA = sàg (“to strike”). glossed sà-ag in ProtoEa 490 (MSL 14.1. BI = bi or bé. a3) B : B not attested . ò) Two notes are. (o. (b) Instead of a quantitative opposition short: long. Ideas of how Sumerian should be pronounced certainly changed dia­ chronically among the learned community of scribes. KU = dab5 (“to seize”) is glossed da-ab in Proto-Ea 19 (MSL 14. there are three possible types of relation between a Sumerian word (as rendered in the translitera­ tion we derive from Proto-Ea and later lexical sources) and the cor­ responding Akkadian loanword: a) labial stops a1) P' : P a2) B : P apin pisan barag bala dub : : : : : epinnu “seeder plough” pis/“annu “box” parakku “dais” palû “term of office” †uppu “tablet” Note: For P' etc. We are often at a loss whether we should transliterate NI = ni or né. 30). e.2. Cf..

and Later Phoneme b p d t g k Sound b p d t g k . d/t : t'. assuming an original opposition of indiscriminate voiced/unvoiced versus unvoiced post­ glottalized stops. He started from a binary system. and he visualized his theory in a chart: Before Old Bab. Written BA PA DA TA GA KA Phoneme b/p p' d/t t' g/k k' Sound p p' t t' k k' Old Bab. Gelb supposed a “sound shift” to have taken place in Old Bab. g/k : k'. 39 dealt with the relation of Sumerian stops and Akkadian spelling before. Sound Shift p > b p' > p t > d t' > t k > g k' > k Old Bab. now then” agû “tiara” Gelb 19612. in. b/p : p'.    b) dental stops b1) T' : T not attested b2) D : T du kid ù-ne(-e)-dug4 b3) D : D é-duru5(A) sa∞g-dili c) velar stops c1) K' : K 15 : : : : : tuppu “tablet” kìtu “reed mat” unetukku “letter” edurû “village” SAG-di-lu-û “(single head =) bachelor” c2) G : K c3) G : G kar kiri6 kid gala engar é-gal barag ù-ne(-e)-dug4 ga-na aga : : : : : : : : : : kàru “quay” kirû “palm grove” kìtu “reed mat” kalû “cantor” ikkaru “tenant farmer” èkallu “palace” parakku “dais” unetukku “letter” gana “hey. and after the Old Bab.. period.

-am6. Sanskrit p/ph/b/bh. k’ are here post-glottalized stops. L. with additionals cerebral stops †/†h/∂/∂h. or unvoiced unaspirated : voiced unaspirated : unvoiced aspirated.. Here.AN = àm from Sargonic times onward (A. t/d/ˇ. t’. “stele” unetukku “letter” erènu “cedar” (Kulturwort) Note: As for final [M] and [N]. ∞g: The transliteration of this phoneme was first proposed by J. we have to admit that most aspects of the phonetics and phonology of Sumerian stops remain subject to doubt. k/g/k’. the Ur III genitive of ezeN is indicated by -(m)a.*. For a more than quadruple set. What is offered below may only be part of a more developed system. and for the copula. relations revealed by Sumerian words and their loans in Akkadian. t/d/t’. t/th/d/dh. genitive an-na. Krecher. 2) Georgian: p/b/p’. e.16   Gelb’s chart does not take into account the more complicated. p’. the loanword evidence is sometimes in contra­ diction with Sumerian spelling and/or lexical glosses.AN being contracted from contextual -a’am = nominalizer -a + copula [m]). cf. unvoiced : voiced : unvoiced post-glottalized/unvoiced velarized. k/g/˚ (Q ). ˇ and Q cannot be defined more closely. We will only give two examples: 1) Akkadian: p/b/-.g. ní∞g-ezeN-ma “fes­ tival accessories”. In spite of ezeN : isinnu “festival”. dental [n]. Matou“ II (1978) 7–73: “Das sumerische Phonem ∞g”. While we will not be able definitely to prove our theory. triple. cf. spelled A. Apart from stops. Its spelling characteristics . Despite this demonstration of possibilities. AN is used both for the name of the “sky (god)”. k/kh/g/gh. we depend to a very high degree on Akkadian evidence for the other phonemes. we may at least point to the fact that a triple (or even more ample) set of stops is well attested in world languages. and palatal(?) [∞g]: m: zà-mi lu-lim n: na­§rú-a ù-ne(-e)-dug4 eren : : : : : sammû (“praise”) “harp” lulìmu “stag” (Kulturwort?) narû “erected stone” (with inscription). An. There are three nasals: labial [m]. HSAO 1 (1967) 87 fn. and further by him in Fs. because we are unable to say whether ˇ and ˚/Q were post-glottalized stops (as in modern Ethiopic languages) or velarized stops (as in Arabic).

glossed na(-an)-ga-ar. e. ZA 52 (1957) 92 f. Akkad.(see below). 1: lú-u18-lu la-ha-an bala kisal hé-∞gál : : : : : lullû “man” la¢annu “bottle” palû “term of office” kisallu “court (primarily of a temple)” he(n)gallu “abundance” As for final [1]. and there may have been a difference between [∞g] and [∞gg] or [∞gg] comparable to ∞ that between English singer and finger. 710–14. approximately ‘cw”. Jacobsen. glossed da-∞gál bala∞g “lyre(?)” : balangu. there also is the combination [∞ g+g]. The identification of [∞g] as a palatal nasal would logically com­ plete a nasal series m. thus behaving in complete accordance with other consonantal phonemes. ∞ [∞g] occurs in all three positions of the words: initial.g. ∞guru“ “adult male” : Ebla sign name nu-rí-“úm (MEE 3 [1981] 198: 46) da∞gal “wide”. classical Sumerian spelling distinguishes between con­ tinuation with -la or—more rarely—-lá. 113 with fnn.g. gá-ra “to me” : Ma-ra. there is a [∞g]: [M] correspondence in Emesal words. where the gloss each time has GA = ga. balaggu hur-sa∞ “mountain range” : ¢ur“ànu g Besides [∞g]. ∞gál “exist” : Ma-al. x (= ∞g). n. . they are.. once more.: “a nasalized velar pronounced with rounded lips (nasalized labio-velar). líl-lá (“u-)pe-el-lá (also -la). engar “tenant farmer”. see Krecher 1966.: ∞gál-la lugal-la lul-la si-il-la gibil-lá (also -la). glossed en-ga-ar. naggàru. Moreover. Both occur in all three positions. see Attinger 1993. nan = gar “carpenter”. nangàru. There are two liquids: [1] and [r]. Akkad. ÁG (á∞g). e. Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian often reflect [∞g] by spelling -n. seen through Akkadian “glasses”. Cf.or -ng. MI (∞gi6). ∞ge26). 328 f. medial. final. A different description was offered by Th.    17 are mainly use of the syllabograms GÁ (∞gá. ikkaru.

ii 1–4. 24 ii a. The argument is the presence of spelling (or glossation) variants with either D or R-syllabograms. secondarily. iii 1–4. Jacobsen. Sumerian loanwords beginning with r. 364 lex. li­ before bí-). e. The symbol [§r] has been introduced (in order not to confuse it with Czech ®). 12 (cf.g. WO 8 (1975) 1–9... the ensuing discussion has been summarized by J.(and. that there is no counterpart in the [e] sector: when sikil or líl are continued by [e]. build” (formerly separated as dù and rú). iv 1–3 paralleled by lugal in 24 i a. 2). 26 i a.18 sikil-la “ul-la til-la   If in view of this distribution one might be tempted to posit two kinds of [1] : [11] and [12]. spelling is in both cases [LI] = -le.before prefix ba. it must be admitted. § kur5(TAR) “to separate” § . Attinger 1993. 35:2. dEn-líl-le “Enlil” (ergative). The Akkadian loan of nu-banda “foreman” is laputtû (luputtû) with oldest attestation in Old Bab. and then by J. no general agreement has so far been found: is it a single phoneme or rather a consonantal cluster (DR)? We tentatively adopt [§r]: § r: §rú “to implant. Black. with more literature.. Bauer. na-RÚ-a “implanted stone” = “stele”. “Zum/dr/-Phonem im Sumerischen”. on the other hand. e. but na-DI-a TIM 9. but—in contrast to [∞g].). RA 84 (1990) 108 f. ZA 52 (1957) 93 fn. Edzard.g. iii 1–5. 111. and note. CAD N/l. (CAD L 98 c 2’) r: rab : rappu “clamp” barag : parakku “dais” kar : kàru “quay” Note: Apart from rappu. A Sumerian phoneme to be distinguished from [r] has been sup­ posed to exist first by Th. An as yet unsolved problem of Sumerian phonetics is found in the variation of initial [NU] with [Lu] or [La]. 1 (d). The Ebla spelling of lugal “(big person =) king” is nu-gal ARET 5. The negative prefix nu. ZA 90 [2000] 292 with fn.has a variant la. 143.are probably absent in Akkadian (cf. with Akkadian loanword narû.

). 241). classical Is. ZA 72 [1982] 220 f. This leads us to suppose an original *hè or *hay for Sumerian “house”. In Mesopotamian Old Akkadian É = "à is found for Semitic [˙] (I. Ugaritica 5 (1968) 245: no. cases where this rule does not hold: gala-ene “cantors”. URU I-di (Middle Bab.). This usage of Ú for [hu] probably goes back to the spelling of Amorite PNs (cf. ugula-ne “overseers”.. P. [H] is here used as a symbol rather than an exactly defined sound. Krebernik. èkallu.). ARM 8. d) Sumerian nominal and verbal bases ending (at least in our translit­ eration) in a vowel absorb a following -e (of the ergative). b) The Hebrew name of the Tigris. The town dÍD ki. ì-lá-e(-ne) “he (they) will pay” (note the unconven­ tional spelling i-la-i in Mari. we have a regular genitive compound in the first case: [munus-ubita-(a)k-ene]. in Ebla both for [h] and [˙] (M. 6 iii 20’ and 23’). but an adjectival compound in the second case: [munus-uda-ene]. 3. When Irikagina of Laga“ contrasts the “women of the past” (munusu4-bi-ta-ke4-ne) with the “present/nowadays women” (munus-u4-da-ene) (Ukg. is Óiddeqel with initial [˙]. . sometimes noting a plene vowel instead of [e]: ama-a “mother” (ergative) instead of *ama­ e. in Ugaritic. There are. Apparently. Whatever the etymology of the name. -e(-ne) (personal pl. but [h] has a good chance. 137 ii 28’.    19 The (original) existence of a phoneme (or of two phonemes?) [H] in Sumerian may.) (RGTC 2 104 f. M. lit.). be inferred for various reasons. for [hu] as in Ú-PI = hu-wa “he” in a Sumero-Akkadian-Hurrian-Ugaritic quadri­ lingual lexical text: J.1). or -e-dè (verbal suffix). still exists as modern Hìt on the Euphrates. initial [h] survived in the modern name. ama-ne “mothers”. J. whereas we only know an Akkadian form Idiglat. Gelb. again. I-da ki (Old Bab.). 48:9). c) The Akkadian syllabogram ú is used. Streck 2000.).]”.1. MAD 22 [1961] 88 f. lú-ù “person” (erg. however. Nougayrol. 135 f. and it may reflect Sumerian [Hu] for ú “grass”. there is some chance that Sumerian “river” originally sounded [hid]. quite evidently a Mesopotamian loan. “women-(in the day =)today-pl. “palace” is reflected by hkl in Ugaritic and by hè§al in Hebrew (see also above 3. a) é-gal.

We propose to note that supposed barrier by [H]: galaH­ ene. however. ¢é. ending [ene]: [munus-uda’ene].of Akkadian is due to the assimilation of the word to the Akkadian pattern parràs(cf. Akkadian preserved the common Semitic unvoiced velar fricative [¢]. Arabic fa¢¢àr) ki-mah : kimà¢u. There may have been voiced and unvoiced variants which escape us. Still. “. munus-udaH-ene. pl. ≤]. After the locative [uda]. we may be sure that the [¢] of the Akkadian loanword in Sumerian. a¢/i¢. furthermore. if there was a “barrier”. It is all the more difficult even to approximate the Sumerian values. ì-láH-e. s. kima¢¢u “(greatest place =) grave” These correspondences do not imply. Z: zà-mi ezen a-zu S: kisal : : : : sammû “lyre” isinnu “festival” asû “physician” kisallu “courtyard” . as usual. audible. disregarding the crescent below the [¢]. u4-da (locative) is used like an adjective. because this group of sounds is already difficult for us to define in Akkadian.20   Here. that in all Sumerian occurences our transliterated [h] was identical in sound (or close to it) with Akkadian [¢]. ¢u. on Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian. ú¢. where we have no exact idea about the identity (and possible diachronic change) of [z. or very close to. We transliterate [h] in Sumerian whenever words are glossed by syllabograms ¢a. We will be brief on Sumerian sibilants. rely. This admittedly slender evidence may lead to the conclusion that after a vowel the [e] of certain morphemes was not absorbed or elided. Akkadian [¢]. pa¢¢àru “potter” (the -¢¢. ¢i. but not visible in our translit­ erations. This can only mean that there was some kind of hiatus between the locative designation [a] and the pers. the [e] of [ene] is not absorbed or elided. as in a-¢u-um [a¢um] “brother”. pu¢rum ( pu-ú¢ru-um) “assembly” was pronounced like. We may. ß. where Sumerian [h] is rendered by Akkadian [¢]: hur-sa∞g : ¢ur“ànu “mountain range” he-∞gál : ¢e(n)gallu “abundance” bahar : pa¢àru.

24). If accepted this would yield this tentative chart of Sumerian conso­ nantal phonemes: p' t' k' “ *H p t k (≤?) s h b d g z m n ∞g l (12) r §r Our transliteration in this grammar will. Falkenstein. ZA 42 (1934) 152–54. he proposed the existence of Sumerian [≤] in order to explain “irregu­ lar” sibilant correspondences (1959. A. t'. had concluded there had been (diachronically) different “Lehnwortschichten”. 2. and he also noted. p. follow traditional values and will not note p'. Later. . see also below nu-è“ : nê“akku) dub-sar : tup“arru “scribe” “à-tam : “atammu “temple administrator” nu-è“ : nê“akku (a high-ranked priest) (for the Akkadian ending -akku see above. behaviour” ¢ur“ànu “mountain range” “angû “main temple administrator” i““iakku “city ruler. fn. governor” (the Akkadian -akku end­ ing is most probably due to the restitution of the Sumerian genitive particle [ak]. 153. however. From this. cases where word-initial sibilants of identical words were registered with [s] or [“] by scribes. énsi(-k) The rather complicated correspondences between Sumerian forms and their rendering in Akkadian loanwords reminds us of the stops: z : z z : s s : s d : d d : t t : t and The evidence becomes even more difficult to judge because of wordinitial or word-medial correspondence s : “ as in hur-sa∞g : ¢ur“ànu. or k'. in 1959.    ús hur-sa∞ g sa∞gga ∞ énsi(-k) : : : : 21 ùsu “guideline.

assimilation. even if a Sumerian internal cluster -mbr. by analogy. -arbst) and. JCS 26 (1974) 18. This assumption has been refuted by J.g. e“.g. see also 2:4 f. while we may ask whether Sumerian syllables of the type bra-. mun Note: The Ur III unorthodox spelling nam-bi-ri (NRVN I 4:4.. [bra]. i.g.. because we are only able to reach Sumerian syllabograms through their Akkadian pronun­ ciation.might have been a spelling for intended *b+ra-. there is no way to prove them.) stands for expected *nam-(é)ri(m) [namri] and suggests a pronunciation [nambri] with [b] as a glide between [m] and [r]. 2. We are reminded of comparable glides (between a nasal and a liquid) in Greek an-d-rós (genitive of énÆr “man”). e. sku-. -arp. . starting from the assumption that the verbal prefix ba. it was—until proof of the contrary—a secondary phonetic phenomenon. It goes without saying that in the spoken language there must have been free variants of pronountiation. etc. word-internal clusters of more than two consonants (-astra-.may have occurred in spoken language. 1950. Falkenstein. depending on speed. there are no syllabograms serv­ ing comparable purposes. As a Semitic language has no initial consonantal clusters (tra-. Note: A. e. min. French (and English) hum-b-le. Postgate. -urps actually existed. -arst. ru ab.. 190. pli-. Taking our transliteration at face value.. elision. consequently.22   3.can be segmentized as b+a-. Therefore. ba-ra. 185 with fn. u∞g bar. S  Here again we are dependent on Akkadian. a ba.e. with all such universal features as slurring.g. e.2. we cannot identify Sumerian syllables with a structure differing from Akkadian. see 1949. So. N. -abstra-) or word-final clusters of two or more consonants (-art. dissimilation. concluded that. Sumerian had the follow­ ing types of syllables: V: CV: VC: CVC: e. ri. stra-).

(7) subjunctions.2.CHAPTER FOUR THE “WORD” IN SUMERIAN. P   We may distinguish eight parts of speech: (1) nouns. is not. We define [i] as a ver­ bal particle. in é-zu “your house”. mine or yours?” the answer cannot be *-∞gu10 or *-zu. (M..) does not qualify as a “word”. verbal. PARTS OF SPEECH We will try to define Sumerian parts of speech not by applying clas­ sical models. In ì-∞gál “it is there/available”. Thirdly. Milner.may not. za(-a)-kam “it is (of me =) mine”. e. “W” A “word” in Sumerian is an entity that can convey meaning on its own without anything spoken before or after. [zu] and others are connectible with both a nominal and a verbal base. Note: I owe the distinction of nominal. B. an. un. the terminative postposition. Fijian Grammar (1956) 130 f. (4) verbs. Their lexical entries exactly correspond to our idea of a “word” (or of a compound). We define [“e] as a nominal particle. but only ∞gá(-a)-kam. but by observing the morpho-syntactic behaviour of Sumerian “words”. é is a word whereas -“è. NBGT) that we see entries which we would define as “par­ ticles” (or rather syllables through which a morpheme boundary ran. but the prefixed element ì. 4. “it is (of you =) yours”. uses “general” instead of “common”. . (3) numerals.g. (5) adverbs. en listed to denote consonantal preverbal -n-). We define them as common particles.1. (6) exclamations. [∞gu]. it is only in the grammatical series (OBGT. and common particles to G. because to the question “whose house is this.) The Sumero-Akkadian lexicographers were apparently aware of the idea “word”. ∞gál “to exist” may occur indepen­ dently. (2) pronouns. in. In é-“è “towards the house”. 4. -zu “your” (sg.

3rd person: person class only. number (singular. dative. ki-sikil “(pure place =) girl. non-person). but we cannot as yet establish whether reduplication was open to any substantive or subject to restrictions. etc. énsi-énsi “all of the city rulers”). plural: 1st.). may take on all nominal particles. case (absolutive. young woman” behaves exactly like an apposition and. directive. (1) to (3) may be opposed to (4) as nonverbal to verbal categories. It may be followed by adjectives. comitative. verbal. The substantive may form part of quite intricate appositional and genitive constructions. 2nd. 3rd person: person and non-person class. non-singular. detailed. (5) to (8) do not combine with particles. For “the pure ones” a nominal head would be needed: lú-sikil-e-ne “the pure persons”. and common. or some other part of speech. 2nd. On the other hand. e. possession singular: 1st. Substantives and adjectives may take the prefixed particle nam-. As a guide-line of high antiquity we may again take the fact that certain Sumerian nouns are entered in lex­ ical lists as Akkadian substantives while others are rendered by Akkadian adjectives. .g.g. the differentiation of sub­ stantives and adjectives is unproblematic on both morphological and syntactical grounds. serving to express an abstract concept: nar “musician”. as such. All these occur as “words” (see our definition above) and may be found as entries in lexical texts. loca­ tive.. ablative. a verb (gur). A further subdivision of (1) nouns into (1a) substantives and (1b) adjectives is not unproblematic. e.24   conjunctions. nam-nar “music”. tur). Some sub­ stantives may be repeated (“reduplicated”. (1) to (4) may be combined with bound particles (prefixes. plural. sikil “pure” can­ not form a plural *sikil-e-ne “the pure ones” whereas it may be fol­ lowed by the plural of the copula: sikil-me-e“ “they are pure”. There is no way to tell by the form of the base alone whether we are faced with a noun (dur. nam-mah “greatness”. mah “very big”. suffixes): nom­ inal. These grammatical categories—apart from class—are realized by the suffixation of particles. ergative. we will make the distinction between sub­ stantives and adjectives.. genitive. (8) interrogations. (1a) The substantive has the following grammatical categories: class (person. equative). On the one hand. sikil following a nominal head. Parts of speech can be identified exclusively on context. In Akkadian itself. terminative. For practical reasons. collec­ tive.

(2.g. e. cf... e. below 5. formally. Thomsen 1984. quotes Gragg 1968.(e.. cf.3.7). e. only be person class (unless non-person class becomes a “person” in a literary context). ilù rabbûtu “the great gods” (Reiner 1966. When an adjective is attached to a substantive. 3rd). Adjectives. nam-mah “quality of being the greatest” (but it is not yet known how far this was a productive feature). all suffixed particles pertaining to the substantive are placed after the following adjective so that one might argue. 24 bottom. . class (per­ son. 64). Black 2003.. where only person class occurs..g. and case. it does not operate in the 3rd pl. there is no ablative or directive. damqùtu “good ones”) and an abstract notion (e. but this can hardly be stated as a gen­ eral overriding rule. and Sumerian formations with nam. di∞gir-gal-gal(-e-ne) “the great gods” (see also 5. pl. nam-mah “greatness”.g. however. like substantives. nam-lugal “kingship”). plural of adjectives (e. adjectival bases can often be turned into verbal bases. Adjectives may serve as the base of adverbs. As for case. 64 f. “ ”  . or “arrùtu “kingship”).    25 (1b) The adjective rarely occurs on its own. ßìrùtu “majesty”. (2) Pronouns have the categories of person (1st.2. In fact. dirig-bi “(its excessive =) excessively”.). We may include.. personal pronouns have a restricted system of case inflection as compared with substantives: absolutive and ergative are identical in form. the differentiation of person : non-person is restricted to the 3rd person.1) Personal pronouns: As 1st and 2nd persons can.g. A few adjectives which express dimension take a reduplicated form when the preceding substantive is meant to be in the plural. We do not include as parts of speech the bound forms of per­ sonal pronouns in their quality of possessors. moreover. by nature.. Note: There is a conspicuous similarity between Akkadian nominal formations in -ùtu which express both the masc.g. that the adjective behaves like a substantive. may take the prefixed particle nam-.. e. number (sg. the whole complex (substantive + adjective) has been substantivized. the question words “who”.g. This similarity has most probably to be seen as another symptom of a Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area. p. among pronouns.. Note: This usage has been imitated in Akkadian where an equally restricted class of adjectives expressing dimension has bases with a lengthened middle radical. non-person).g.. It is usually connected with a (preceding—rarely following) substantive. In reality. 2nd. 9 who considers adjec­ tives as a sub-class of the verb.

(4) The verb is the most complex part of speech in Sumerian. without any prefixed or suffixed particles. e““u “new”. zi-dè-e“ [zid-e“] = zi(d) “true” with the adverbiative particle [e“] (Attinger 1993. In this respect. class. Bound forms (suffixed particles) are not included here. Cardinal numerals are written in the sexagesimal system (see below. but also a person (who: me. because the answer may not only be a substantive (who: my father).g. e. reliable way”. 10. be nominalized. Sumerian. Such a newly created noun may then be subject to nominal inflection (receiving suffixed case particles). The system of deictic pronouns is still poorly explored in Sumerian. distinguishes “who” (a-ba) and “what” (a-na). (5) Adverbs. Except for lexical lists where the (simple or reduplicated) verbal base is regularly equated with Akkadian infinitives. damqi“ “nicely”. Nominalized verbal complexes frequently have to be rendered by English dependent clauses.16).26   “what”. after a substantive. this category is usually formed with an adjective as base + the ending -i“. in order to denote ordinal numbers: mu-3-kam “(it is the year of three =) third year”. see below 7. “truly”. (3) Numerals. Numerals occur in the genitive. tense/aspect. This way of forming adverbs has two Sumerian counterparts: gibil-bi “(its new =) anew”. e““i“ “anew”. The very productive system of Akkadian (nominal) infinitive constructions may be seen as an Akkadian share in the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area (see 17). you).2) following the item counted. they resemble adjectives. action. the whole complex.. “in a sincere. In Akkadian. mood. It has an extremely variable set of prefixed particles as well as a num­ ber of suffixed particles. being turned into one noun. The verb may express the following categories: person. it is shifted from the substantive to the numeral. the verbal base extremely rarely occurs alone. A verb(al complex) may be turned into a noun. direction. sub­ stantive + numeral. damqu “good”. 253–56). . Words which describe in a more precise way the idea contained in a verb are traditionally defined as adverbs: “he arrived”.e.. i. “he recently arrived”. and as with adjectives if a complex of sub­ stantive + cardinal number is provided with a suffixed particle. like most of the world’s languages. The verbal base in itself may be subject to variation. by the addition of the suffixed particle [a] (see 12. num­ ber.

Note ì-gi-in-zu “as if ”. gibil-bi-”È “anew”. But Sumerian u only occurs to connect phrases. hé-àm (lit. d Nin-∞gir-su. fear. insofar it is not a replace­ ment of Sumerian -bi(-da) which may connect parts of speech (see hereafter). [ J. For “who” and “what” see 9. u8-ú-a [way] “alas.4. woe”. (6) Exclamations. Here also belong the expressions for “yes”.    27 Note: The obvious similarity of Sumerian [e“] and Akkadian [i“] may not be due to pure chance. doubt. and others.2.6. “why” is a-na-a“ (-àm) [ana“’am] “for what (is it)?”. etc. Sumerian lexical categories.4. Note: Borrowing “and” is well known in agglutinating languages: ve (Arabic wa) in Turkish. see 14. ù “and” has been borrowed from Akkadian [wa. it essentially lacks subjunctions and conjunctions. u].2. surprise. ZA 92 (2002) 60–77. Was the Sumerian adverbiative a loan from Akkadian? Less frequently. “ ”  .] . -bi or -bi-da is attested: áb amar-bi-da “the cow (with its =) and the calf ”. it is frozen in itself and not subject to further change. Thomsen 1984. not parts of speech. They express joy. (7) Because Sumerian mainly uses nominalized verbal phrases (to which postpositions may be added—see 12. is preceded by the neutral motion particle [i]). given the status of a base. tukumbi “if ” (for which Old Sum. Black.1). As a conjunction. (8) Interrogations. d”ára-bi(-r) “to Ningirsu and ”ara” Ent 28/29 i 5–6 (see 5. has [uda]. We can only partly translate into Sumerian the famous hexameter quis quid ubi quibus auxiliis cur quomodo quando. As a replacement for a conjunction. “let it be”) and “no”. a-la-la “hey”. in-nu (where the negative particle [nu]. ja (Old Germanic jah) in Finnish. eta in Basque. d i ( Utu) “woe (o Sungod)”. 66 f. b).16) instead of subordi­ nate clauses. The adverb being already a derived form. “where” is me-a. -bi + “è (or -é“?) are combined: mah-bi-”È “in a most exalted way”. pain. came to the author’s attention only after the preceding chapter had been written. Cf.

This page intentionally left blank .

-L. No distinction is made between alienable and inalienable possession (sa∞g-∞gu10 “my head”. an“e “male donkey”.MÍ) “jenny”. Apart from class.und Sachklasse”. nin(9) “sister”. Thomsen “animate and inanimate”. éme(AN”E. For the independent personal pronouns see 10. M. in the 1st person pl. P Ownership of something or someone by something or someone is expressed by a set of suffixed particles. closest in rank to the owner (substantive. but are strictly divided into two classes: person and non-person. but “non-person”. ses “brother”. A. or nominalized verbal form in -a). between the categories inclusive (“ours” = “mine and yours present”) and exclusive (“ours” = “mine and yours absent”. fem. Falkenstein used “Personen. With a limited number of substantives.1. gender (masc. and they are not “Sachen”.2. “mine and theirs”). munus “woman”. lugal-bi “its owner” (said of a garden).. Only person class substantives may take the erga­ tive plural (-e)-ne or form a dative. the categories of substantives are possession. G Sumerian substantives lack gender. nominal compound. ..) is expressed lexically: ninta “male”. It is shown by agreement: lugal-a-ni “his owner” (said of a slave). and case: ses-∞gu10-(e)ne-da “with my brothers” where the particles serving the respective functions occur in a predictable and invariable order. number. etc. The form of the substantive does not betray its class. only non-person substantives may form an ablative.CHAPTER FIVE SUBSTANTIVES 5. a-“à-∞gu10 “my field”) or. For reasons of morphological behaviour we have dissociated pos­ sessive particles from free personal pronouns. We follow Attinger because animals (unless they are personified in literary con­ text) are “animate”. Class is also expressed in verbal morphology. 5.

pl. pl. it is their final vowel that yields—at least graphically—to the [a] of genitive [a(k)] or of locative.). 1st sg. takes place. and 1st pl. but his examples do not fit the general definition of these two pronominal categories. 3rd sg. sg. st rd 1 to 3 pl. (-a)-ni is (-a)-na in gen. 1st sg. 10019 had proposed to see a distinction between “inclusive” and “exclusive” in such forms as ga-an-“i-su8-dè-en “let us proceed toward it” (!) (incl. no overriding of final -e by -a of gen. and loc. Note: Kienast 1980. (-a)-ne-ne. 2nd sg. Whereas in ab-ba-eri(a)ke4-ne “city elders” the [a] of [ak] is elided. differ from nouns ending in a vowel.: pl. with the possessive particles sg.30   Note: Jacobsen 1965. relations in 2nd and 3rd persons. and loc. explained each possessive particle as “enttontes Enklitikon” (following Falkenstein 1959. is -ba or -bi-a in gen. 54. the 3rd sg. sg. -a-ni follows a consonant. distinguishes person and non-person class. pl. and loc. p.) and ga-ba-ab-túm-mu-dè “let us carry (him) off ” (excl. 33): 1st sg. sg. is -za in gen. and pl.. have engendered 2nd sg.2). There are three persons each in sg. -zu.: with -me. but only “mine + yours” or “mine + his/hers/its”. then. -zu-ne-ne. 3rd sg. -ni follows a vowel. All other persons are restricted to per­ son class. are not related (nor are they in Akkadian) because “ours” cannot be *“mine + mine”. use of a genitive compound is made whereby the relation between possessor and possessed depends on meaning: ká-é-gal-la(-k) “the gate (possessed) of the palace (pos­ sessor)”. When the possessor is another noun. 1st 2nd 3rd 3rd 1st 2nd 3rd sg. and loc. -∞gu10(MU) -zu (-a)-ni -bi -me -zu-ne-ne (-a)-ne-ne person non-person While there are clear morphological sg. but lugal-Unuki-ga(-k) “the king (possessor) of Uruk (pos­ . Morphological behaviour: is -∞gá in gen. and -∞gu would. Note: 1st to 3rd sg. *-∞ga would become -∞gu (under the influence of the preceding consonant). Falkenstein’s explanation of lugal-ani as “König er” (for “his king”) cannot be proven (see also 9. non-p.

deli-deli > de/idli “single ones”. Reduplication here. its (king =) owner”. 5. e. Much more rarely. e. the city-rulers of the other lands”. serves the need to express the idea of plural where the simple sg.3. note to 5. Plurality may be expressed by reduplication of the nominal base: a-gàr-a-gàr “fields”. the inverted genitive construction is used: é-a(k) lugal-bi “of the house. gur7-gur7 “(many) piles of grain/silos”. It is not known to us whether reduplication of substantives may sometimes have resulted in variant pronunciation. deli “one”. [ene] follows the extended complex: di∞gir-gal-gal-e-ne “the great gods”.2.3. an apposition or by another noun in the genitive. di∞gir-ré-ne (OS). the first [e] may be contained in a Ce syllabogram. N Sumerian number differs fundamentally from that in Akkadian where practically all substantives may form a plural and a restricted num­ ber may form a dual. The first [e] thus behaves like the [e] of the ergative or directive case particle: lugal-e-ne “kings”. Reduplication very rarely occurs with extended nouns: ses-gal-sesgal “(elder brothers =) school elders”.g.8) would lack clarity. di∞gir-re-e-ne (OB). galaH-e-ne “can­ tors”. bára-bára-Ki-en-gi ∞ énsi-kur-kur-ra(-k) “the (daises =) rulers of Sumer. Orthographically.5).3. and [ne] after vowels. base which may sometimes also func­ tion to express a collective (see 5. ugula-ne “overseers”. ab-ba-eri-ke4-ne “city elders”.1.. 31 sessed)”. 5. apparently. . 5. A plural properly speaking (implying two or more counted objects) and marked by a special suffixed particle is restricted to person class: [(e)ne] with the distribution [ene] after consonant.. as it did with. gi“-gi-∞gi“-gi “cane-brakes”. including [H].3.3.g. ama-a-a-An-na-ke4-ne “the mothers and fathers of An”. Sumerian has no dual (but cf. If the substantive is extended by an adjective.

5.2. one is reminded of the Akkadian pl. *lugal-ussu “eight kings”. referred to hi-a indicating a duality through mention of only one member: mer-hi-a “(the diverse south (-winds) =) north and south”. 5. 2.3.3.3. hi-a “mixed” indicates a plurality of heterogeneous items: túg-hi-a “diverse textiles”. 65. OLZ 73 (1978) 28 with fn.32   5. 5. The inverted notation has been borrowed from accountancy texts where figures. for the sake of visual clarity are noted at the beginning of a line. Here. na4-di4(TUR)-di4 “small stones”. Nin∞girsu’s temple complex and ziggurat at ªGirsu.3. For more discussion. A few adjectives appear in reduplicated form when the preceding substantive is plural: di∞gir-gal-gal “the great gods”.3. Note: Against Falkenstein 1949. supposes that the redu­ plication of adjectives could express superlatives. This is reminiscent of Arabic al-Furàtàn “(the two Euphrates =) E. A rare type of partial reduplication is ku-li-li “mutual friends”. al-qamaràn “(the two moons =) sun and moon”. dedli may be followed by [(e)ne]: lú-dedli-ne “the individual persons”. Note: Texts frequently write 8 lugal instead of lugal 8. bàd-dedli-gal-gal “the individual big fortresses”. Note: dedli is closer in rank to the substantive than the regular adjective. Here. u8-udu-hi-a “sheep”. Note: J.1(b). in the case of na4-gal- . and Tigris”. é-ninnû “Fifty Houses”. Krecher. 5. see above 4.7. arrakùtu. and so written exactly one below the other. prob­ ably to be compared to Akkadian itbàru “mutual friends” as against ebrù “friends”. 5.3. 72. The exact quantity may be expressed by a cardinal number follow­ ing a substantive.4.2. Thomsen 1984. end) indicates a detailed plurality: ki“ib-dedli “the individual sealed documents”. ße¢¢erùtu. dedli (see also 5.6.3. no extra plural word or particle is needed: me-umun7 “the seven ordinances”. form of some adjec­ tives: rabbûtu. However.

(6) comitative.4.9. “textiles/garments”. A second locative is only attested with the verb. but also.8.4. C Sumerian has ten cases: (1) absolutive. as gal-gal. (5) and (7) to (9) may be summarized as directional. e. but simply “big. . Moreover. 5. and their original identity has often been suggested. (8) terminative. see 12. we see different non-numerical notions such as totality. Finally. 5. and it may override any of the cases (4) to (9). collectivity. so that ses-gin7 not only means “like a brother”. also Turkish hasta gibi ona bakmı{tı “he looked at him as (at a sick person =) if he were a sick person”.1. (4) locative.3. or elative. there is no reason to assume a superlative. di4-di4 virtually only occur when the noun is plural. túg are “fish”. detailed quantities. cf. small stones”. (10) equative.8.3. Cases (4).26. 33 gal na4-di4-di4 there is hardly the idea of “biggest.9. 5. Note: to some degree this feature of (10) is shared by Akkadian: kìma eleppim “like a boat” may also mean “as in a boat”.g. ergative (2) and directive (9) are identical in spelling. we may state that number in Sumerian nouns and adjectives has a totally different significance from that in Akkadian: apart from an opposition singular : plural. (2) ergative. “as with a brother”. Of these. For (4) to (9) our enumeration corresponds to the sequence of the directional prefixed particles of the verb. (7) ablative(-instrumental). 5. depending on context. Summing up. “sheep (and goats)”.. mixed quantities. (5) dative. (3) genitive. cf. plurality may be present in a simple unextended substantive with collective meaning: ku6. function. smallest stones”. (9) directive. udu. The equative (10) stands apart: it is restricted to sg.

(2) erg. (3) gen. 2) ama-zu-gin7 “his/her tablet” dub-ba-ni dub-ba-ni dub-ba-na(-k) dub-ba-na — dub-ba-ni-da dub-ba-ni-ta dub-ba-ni-“è dub-ba-ni dub-ba-ni-gin7 “kings” — lugal-e-ne lugal-e-ne(-k) — lugal-e-ne-r(a) lugal-e-ne-da — lugal-e-ne-“è (cf. 2) ama-gin7 “tree” ∞ge“ ∞ge“-e ∞ge“-a(k) ∞ge“-a — ∞ge“-da ∞ge“-ta ∞ge“-“è ∞ge“-e ∞ge“-gin7 “city” eri eri eri(-k) eri-a — eri-da eri-ta eri-“è eri eri-gin7 “your mother” ama-zu ama-zu ama-za(-k) — ama-zu-r(a) ama-zu-da — ama-zu-“è (cf. (7) abl. (9) dir.34 “king” (1) abs. (4) loc. 2) lugal-gin7 “mother” ama ama ama(-k) — ama-r(a) ama-da — ama-“è (cf. (10) equ. 2) — “mothers” — ama-ne ama-ne(-k)   — ama-ne-r(a) ama-ne-da — ama-ne-“è (cf. lugal lugal-e lugal-la(-k) (—) lugal-ra lugal-da — lugal-“è (cf. (6) com. (5) dat. 2) — . (8) term.

5.2.. Absolutive The absolutive which is unmarked denotes—in our modern interpretation—both the ‘subject’ of an intransitive verbal form and the ‘object’ of a transitive verbal form.4. see the paragraphs on the individual case particles.2. amâ instead of ama.g.1.4. representing both the consonant belonging to the preceding—nominal or verbal—base and the vowel of the respective case particle.9. and it is only under 2nd millennium Akkadian influence that absolutive and erga­ tive may be used “incorrectly”.4. 5.2. or animal.4.4. In Sumerian “father (absolutive) sleeps” and “mother (ergative) cooks dinner (absolutive)”. Case parti­ cles consisting of a vowel ([e]. Ergative The ergative marks the subject of a transitive (‘object’-taking) verbal form. The absolutive being the unextended form is the form cited in lexical lists..g. or (b) to denote a syllable to be pronounced separately. Preliminary notes on the phonetic and spelling behaviour of the case particles 35 In general.4. kur-ra [kur-a] “in the foreign country”. It is either disregarded in spelling or replaced by a “plene” vowel sign. in [(e)ne] does not occur with abso­ lutive. 5. person. For more details.2. . The [e] of the ergative (as well as the directive—see 5.2. as a vocative. [a]) or beginning with a vowel ([ak]) may be written either with a vowel sign or with a syllabogram CV.4. Its usage is consistent in classical Sumerian. For the extremely complicated behaviour of the genitive case particle [ak] see 5. e. Absolutive is also used.4.3.g.2 and 5.. lú or lú-ù “person”. e. a number of (a) phonotactic and (b) spelling rules apply when case particles are added to a noun.9) is not graphically shown when the preceding substantive ends in a vowel.1.2. It is unknown whether the “plene” vowel sign served (a) to indicate a pronunciation of a single vowel different from the absolutive form.2. Nominal pl. corresponding to the last vowel of the preceding noun: ama or ama-a “mother”. e.2. Notes on the individual case particles 5. father and dinner are in the identical unmarked case. for calling a god. For [e] of the ergative or directive see 5.

Cf. MSL 14. 38:10 si-pa Ur-dNa-ma-KI.g. Genitive The genitive is the most complicated of the case particles.9) cf. cf.) and “eintreten lassen. Sjöberg. When the head of a genitival (regens-rectum) compound. the more so as the modern interpreter cannot help being influenced by his or her native language. or Ninme“ara 116 ki su-ub ma-ra(-ab)-AG-ne/ 5(AG)-ke4-ne /-a “they kiss the ground before you”.4.). SRT 11:68 sipa dUr-dNamma-ke4 “the shepherd Ur-Namma” paralleled by TCL 15.g. (a) appears to be the most plausible option.g. g[i4-.. . -ke4 may. For the homophony and—probably—common origin of ergative and directive (see also 5.3.” (trans. and see Å. Apart from -e. Starting from the basic morpheme [ak] we can note four allomorphs: (a) [ak] in the position -C-ak-V. ge-e.2. . Note: The question which verbs were actually considered transitive or intransitive is often difficult to answer. e. Note: The syllabogram KID.5). OrSuec. some scholars in the past preferred -gé instead of -ke4 which has been inferred from our idea of the grammatical structure. e. there is no “split” whatsoever in the nominal part of Sumerian SOV phrases. be proven by spelling variants. not attested in the series Proto-Ea. li-il. the particle -e is shifted from the regens to the end of the rectum: lugal-kur-kurra-ke4 instead of *lugal-e kur-kur-ra. e. whereas in German there is a strict distinction between “eintreten” (intr. A reading -gé (instead of -ke4) would contradict the fact that nouns ending in -g and prolonged by [e] are regularly spelled -ge or -ge4. lugal-Úrimki-ma-ke4 “the king of Ur” (erg. but not *-gé. “to enter”. is. or whether it was (c) a mere visual mark for guiding the reader without relevance for pronunciation. the ergative (or directive) may be noted by -Ce. Steiner 1976. in our transliteration -ke4.). 41:243–254: gi-i (var.]). is in the ergative. In English. hinein bringen.. . dEn-líl-le [Enlil-e]. lugalkur-kur-ra(-k) “king of all countries”.2. su-úh. Therefore. both in morphology and in syntax. it must be stressed that.36   ama’a. etc. Sumerian “to enter” (ku4-r/ku4ku4) is introduced by the ergative as if it were “to make an entry”. strangely enough. 5. judging by the distribution of absolutive and erga­ tive in classical Sumerian syntax.7. While it is still disputed whether the Sumerian verb is construed in pure or in “split” ergativity (cf. the discussion in 12. may be used both as a transitive and an intransitive verb.4. 10 (1961) 11. however..

g.. a geometrical shape.g. exhibit peculiarities of their own: in mor­ phology. lugal-Úrimki-ma(-k) “the king of Ur” (absol. cross-linguistically. cf. plural formation. dNin-∞gír-su(-k)ra. phonetically speaking. -za. dEn-líl-ra “to Enlil” and also after [a(k)]. to the final [k] of the geni­ tive morpheme. ab-ba-eri(-k) “the city elder” (absol. display an ending -Vkku. and other respects. With a genitival compound X-Y-a(k) any possessive.). e. e. Also.. -ba. (d) [(V)(k)] between -V and -Ø. There are four exceptions from the above rule (b): When -∞gu10 “my” (see 5.g. (b) X – Y (= A-B)-a(k): eri-bar—abul-tur-ra(-k) “suburb of the small city-gate”. . e. dNanna(-r) “to the Moongod”. there was not one syllable *[sur]. (-a)-na. e.. e.. but two.). either X or Y of the compound may be extended by the addition of adjectives or of another dependent substantive (the latter again to be put in the genitive). most probably “trapezium”. This results in an intricate. However.g. [su(k)-ra].2) -zu “yours”.g. e. Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian resulting from genitival compounds. Note: Pronouns. the final [u] or [i] is overlaid by the [a(k)] of genitive or by the [a] of locative. [ra] is noted after a consonant. (-a)-ni “his/hers”. e. because syllable-closing consonants were still (mostly) disregarded in spelling up till and during Ur III. or -bi “its” (non-p. ad 5. It is not noted in writ­ ing after a preceding vowel. already above.g. ab-ba-eri-ke4-ne “the city elders” (erg. it remains unknown what happened. (c) [a(k)] between -C and -Ø. number...g. class) stand in the genitive. genitive “algebra”: (a) X (= A-B) – Y-a(k): ur-sa∞g-kal-ga—dEn-líl-lá(-k) “the mighty war­ rior of Enlil”. lugal-Úrimki-ma(-k)-da “with the king of Ur”.2.2 (ergative).. or case particle pertaining to X has to follow the whole compound.g. yielding -∞gá. Quite apparently. e. but logical. áb-zà-mi(-(a)k) “cow (as part) of the lyre” > apsamikku.. 37 (b) [k] between -V and -V.). That it did not disappear totally becomes clear from the OS spelling rule for the dative case particle [r(a)].4. As regards the allomorphs (c) [a(k)] and (d) [(k)].

for X1 = X2. . Possessive particles may equally be incorporated. g ká—é-gal-la-na(-k) “the gate of his palace”. dNin-∞gír-su(-k) ur-sa∞g-kal-ga—dEn-líl-lá(-k)-ra “to N. The possessive particle -ani “his” should also have been in the genitive. unbound. -a-na. . Here only Y (the rectum) is marked by a possessive particle. Here possession applies to the whole compound.. i.. Ningirsu(k) and mighty war­ rior are in apposition.38   (c) X (= A-B-a(k)) – Y -a(k): ab-ba-eri(-k)—Unugki-ga-ke4-ne “the city elders of Uruk”. of the city where he had been governor when he was still a crown-prince”. In X – Y-a(k). mighty warrior of Enlil”. genitive with no regens expressed occurring much . (d) X – Y (= A-B-ak) -a(k): sa∞gga-dNin-∞gír-su-ka(-k) “the temple admin­ ∞ istrator of the Lord-of-ªGirsu”. even if three or more genitival rela­ tionships are involved. What is more. where an-ki “Heaven and Earth” represent a string (though additive instead of appositive). both X and Y may be extended by appositions: X1 = X2 – Y -a(k) or X – Y1 = Y2 -a(k). X = bàd-gal and Y = the city-name with a complicated string of apposition and genitives: A1-A2(-ak)—B-ak.e. Here. there is free. in X – Y -a(k). For Y1 = Y2: suhu“-an-ki(-k) “the foundations of Heaven (and) Earth”. OB scribes sometimes became confused with their more elaborate constructions: bàd-gal-[BÀ]Dki eri-nam-”AGIN(-a(k))-nam-dumu-naka-ni “the great wall of Der. Apart from the X-Y-a(k) syntagma with a bound genitive. the city of governorship (nam-”AGIN) of his (son-ship =) state of crown-prince”. We may understand the scribe’s ‘mistake’ by re-translating the whole complex into Akkadian: *dùram rabiam “a Der àl “akkanakkùtim “a màrùti“u the last -“u of which was mechanically rendered by the scribe by Sumerian -a-ni. Here. no more than two genitive parti­ cles were allowed on a string. both X and Y may carry a possessive particle: ká—é-gal-la(-k)-∞gu10 “my (gate of the palace =) palace gate”. Contrast ká—é-gal-∞ á(-k) “the gate of my palace”. “. Cf. In such cases the string of noun + apposition(s) functions like a simple noun so that there will be no further consequence for the genitive “alge­ bra”. ká—é-gal-la-ka-ni “his (gate of the palace =) palace gate”. Probably for reasons of euphony.

e. but note below.1. -bi : -∞gá. yours”. “Genitive constructions in Sumerian”.4. local rest may be transferred to the tem­ poral sphere: “à-mu-ba-ka “(in the middle of =) during that year”. is an exception to the above stated rule of complementary distribution. An-nakam “it is of An (or: of heaven)”. at least in the documentation of historical times: ∞gá (-a)-kam. Note: Ali Letter B 2:1 lugal-∞gá (instead of lugal-∞gu10-ra) “to my lord”. It does not occur with pl. is essentially restricted to the non-person class.4. As in many languages.2). Th. -zu.6 for the comitative). like the [a] of genitive [ak] supersedes the final vowel of the possessive particles of the sg. Locative The locative is rendered by -(C)a. JCS 48 (1996) 31–47. i. its owner” = “the owner of the house”. Locative -a. but we cannot determine the vowel quantity: -a(H) or -à(H). not movement toward or into something (which would rather be rendered by the terminative. 166) é-lú-ak “from ‘house gathering in the man’.2. Note: It is generally difficult to find etymologies for case particles (but see 5. -a-ni. 39 more rarely. Zólyomi. 5. there is no insistence on direct contact (for which see directive.2. though. -ba. to ‘house of the man’”. We presented an argument for an original form [aH] (above 3. which is exclusively used with nouns of the person class. -a-na. as is evident from its functions.2. Note: Besides u4-ba “at (its day =) that time”.2.5). quoted in Thomsen 1984.2. 7.9). Jacobsen. G. The distinction of the two temporal adverb and the reason for the— uncontracted—u4-bi-a are not yet clear. made a tentative proposal to connect genitive [ak] with the verb ak *“to gather” and to explain (p.8).: -∞gu10. -za. you” = “it is mine. 98. see 5. there also is u4-bi-a (Akkadian in ùmi“u). za(-a)-kam “it is of me. 5.4. [(e)ne].4.4. The main function of the locative is to indicate rest and arrest at a goal. Jacobsen mentions the possibility of an underlying “verb ak of not recoverable specific meaning”. Note: This construction represents a universal type. and as such it stands in complementary dis­ tribution with the dative (see 5. “Notes on the Sumerian genitive”. connecting with him. JNES 32 (1973) 161–66. eri-me-a “in our city”. an-ki-a “in heaven (and) on earth”. Whether it was more fre- quent—or even predominant—in prehistoric Sumerian cannot be said. the regens has to refer back to the rectum by adding a possessive particle sharing the class (person or non-person) of the rectum: é-a(-k) lugal-bi “of the house. . The locative. u4-bi-a “at that time”.4.. A free genitive may also appear as a rectum before its regens: In such cases. In his last paragraph.2.

occuring with non-person class nouns. 94 Na-ni(-r) “to Nani”. Commodity (grain.2. 5. Dative -r(a) with person class nouns almost always stands in com­ plementary distribution with locative -a. The particle may. silver) PN1 (ergative) PN2-da an-da-tuku “PN1 (creditor) (has with =) is owed by PN2 (debtor)” NRVN I 61:7. Note: Zólyomi 1999 proposed a complementary distribution of personal dative and “inanimate” directive in the prefixed string of verbal directional particles. after a vowel. In NS and OB Sumerian both -V-Vr and -V-ra occur. Rarely lugal-∞gu10-úr. ability to do something. originally. being in someone’s neighbourhood. no dative is marked graphically when the preceding noun ends in a vowel (see above 5. lugal-/nin-a-ni-ir “to his/her lord/lady”. In OS. but this cannot be proved. but 93 Na-nira. Dative The dative case particle [ra] has the allomorphs [ra] and [r] where the original distribution was probably between [ra] after a conso­ nant (including -H and genitive -a(k)) and [r] after a vowel.g.4. There is no restriction to either person or non-person class.“to him/her” is one of the most clearly marked. ubi­ quitous in royal inscriptions.. The general function is to express com­ pany. dNanna-ar “to my lord”. 87–92.2. or that a certain state of mind (e.2. Cases where. Michalowski 1993 nos. Cf. an-da gú-lá-a “(hang the neck with =) embracing heaven” (said of the height of a building).4. mental and intellectual participation. hatred) is felt towards a person. -ra is written instead of -Vr.40   5.4. and . ama-a [d]umu-da gù n[u]-mu(-n)-da(-n)-dé “a mother did not (talk with =) nag her child” Gudea Cyl. the case of the concord of -r(a) and verbal infixed -na. As for the relationship between case particles and verbal dimensional prefix particles. generally. Note: Cf. to indicate that an action or state is in a person’s favour.3). doing something or being with someone or something. The function of dative -r(a) is. love. may either be regarded as purely orthographical variants or as some kind of grammatical hyper-correction. A xiii 3. have been iden­ tical with the noun da “side”. that speech is addressed to a per­ son.5.6. as against lugal-∞gu10-ra “to my lord” in the opening of letters. Comitative The comitative is expressed by [da]. “to the Moongod”.

M. Note: A similar idea underlies English “with-out”. at the latest in NS. (RIME 4. 5..g. e. (b) áb amar-bi-da “the cow with its calf ” expresses an idea closely related to.2 (7). The combination -bi-da has a variant -bi which also occurs in lex­ ical lists (cf. (NBGT). WF p. 138). 10. rejoicing over Inana” Inana and Ebih 61. Russian vne “in not”. being a hypothetical question. Note: For var. or even identical with. 137:203 f. 26* n. 175:215 (NBGT). ∞gá-da nu-me-a “not being with me” = “without me” in the sense of “without my permission”. e. or German children’s “mit ohne” for “ohne”. It only occurs with non-person class nouns and.v. An dInana-da húl-a-e “An.2. cf.4. etc.d.see 12. borrowed Akkadian ù. Note: For the X Y-bi-da construction cf. II 216. bala-nam-lugal-la-∞gá 3 “e:gur-ta . to be continued by something like “is there anything possible?” (e.7. For the borrowing of “and” see above 4.10). . the PN Di∞gir-da-nu-me-a is noted as Di∞gir-nu-me. “dNu-me”.g. 28–29 i 5–6 dNin-∞gír-su d”ara-bi(-r) “to Nin∞girsu and ”ara”. “the cow and (the) calf ” (note the interchangeability of “chicken with/and rice”). There are two special functions of the comitative: (a) Our idea of “without” is expressed by X-da nu-me-a “not being with X”.. 195:159. 460). is phonologically clearly differ­ entiated from comitative [da]. qa-[du] MSL 4. Sumerian lacks a word for “and” and. Its functions are to indicate the starting point from which some­ one or something comes from or by means of which (instrumentally) .g. kù-babbar 1 gín-e hé-eb-da (-n)-sa10 “during my royal office one shekel of silver (bought with it =) could buy three kor of barley each” Sin-kà“id 13:11–19. bi-da = ù MSL 4. bi = ù. 147 f. Struve 1984. Poebel 1923. and see nos. therefore. the PN Ninda-nu-me-a “Without the Lady?”.” = “Mecnun and Leyla”. cannot occur with pl. 41 often in pre-Sargonic and Ur III debt documents (for the verbal prefix a(l). bi. see Jaques n. Note: In early OS. bi = ù MSL 4. at least in our Latin transliteration. [(e)ne]. . Ablative-instrumental The ablative-instrumental case particle is expressed by [ta] which... Turkish Leyla ile Mecnun “with L. 8. OS Ent.

the ubiquitous kiPN1(-ak)-ta PN2 (erg. 5. see above 5. “i) is inconsistent as is Ea I (MSL 14. 33:61) with e-“e É” (var. as regards the allo­ morph [“]. It is usually written with the syllabogram “è. French produire qc. above 5. the strict distinction between the “termina­ tive” case particle and the adverbial particle [e“(e)]. However. but sometimes as -V“.2. The original distribution of the allomorphs [“e] and [“] most prob­ ably has its counterpart in dative [ra] and [r] (see 5. either spelling is inconsistent or our present understand­ .4. an-gal-ta ki-gal-“è “from the great Above to the great Below” Inana’s Descent 1. A common origin of both particles is not improbable.. ge“ ∞ tukul-kal-ga dEn-líl-le mu-na(-n)-“úm-ma-ta “with the mighty weapon which Enlil had entrusted to him”. seems to have switched “terminative” (e­ e“ = a-na) and “adverbiative” (“e-e = ki-ma). The ablative.42   something is realized or manufactured (cf.2. at least to our modern interpretation. e. serves to express what is a temporal subordinate clause in Akkadian: sa∞g-ki-gíd-dad En-líl-lá-ke4 Ki“ki gu4-an-na-gin7 im-ug5-ga-ta “after Enlil’s frown had killed Ki“ like the bull of Heaven” Curse of Akkade 1. “e-e = ki-ma. whereas -“ (either written or latent) would be found after a vowel.4 for the equative overriding other cases. Terminative The terminative case particle owes its designation to its main func­ tion: to indicate that someone or something reaches a goal. for which he proposed “adver­ biative” (“adverbiatif ”). and e-e“ = eb-lu. -“è would then have stood after a consonant. Sin-kà“id 13:11–19.4.).8.4. Note that Proto-Ea (MSL 14. A special function of -ta is to indicate distribution. 253–56. such a quan­ tity “each”: see. lit. “e. placed after a nominalized phrase. Ablative -ta cannot be applied to person class nouns so that where “from (a person)” is needed the circumpositional version ki-N-a(k)ta.g. Note: We owe to Attinger 1993. The local aspect was easily transferred to the temporal: u4-bi-ta “from that day” = “since that time”.6. It is unknown how the pronunciation of “è differed from [“e] in “e(-ga) “favoured”. Cf.2. Note: Or “as by means of the Bull of Heaven”. is often used.4). “from the place of N”. à partir de qc.) “u ba-an-ti “PN2 received (object) from PN1”. 186:180–182) which by offering the equivalences e-e“ = a-na.

1.) bí(-n)-ús “he (= Gudea. The main function of the directive is to express movement arriv­ ing (into contact with) an object.. Cf. Both the terminative and adverbiative particles have to be kept separate from the quotation particle [e“e] which the Gudea corpus equally spell -É” (in OB e-“e). and see Falkenstein 1949. or position adjacent to (in contact with) an object. B ii 18 with contrasting sa∞g-bi-”È in Cyl. most prob­ ably.4.2. the concord of the two case particles with verbal prefix par­ ticles is also different. the origin of the ergative particle has been sought in the directive. below 13. purpose.4. Local use is. . the spelling is consistently nam-ti-la-ni-”È and never *-ni-i“ oder *-ni-e“. mu-bi-“è “(to its name =) therefore”. again. erg. ní∞g-ba-a“ “as (the purpose of ) a present”. the terminative may be used to indicate abstract ways of reasoning: cause. . 30. The terminative is most obviously opposed to the ablative in such phrases as an-gal-ta ki-gal-“è (see 5. set after a nominalized verbal phrase (whether introduced by u4 or not) serves to express what is a subordinate tem­ poral clause in Akkadian (with adi ): “until . a-na-a“(-àm) “for what is it” = “why”. identical in sound with the ergative [e] and because both par­ ticles share the same spelling and phonotactic behaviour. Gudea Cyl.7) or Úrimki-ta Nibruki-“è “from Ur to Nippur”. most tellingly. reason. The terminative.9.”.) had the boat moor at the quay of N.2. Cf. Falkenstein 1949. transferred to temporal: u4-ul-lí-a-“è “until (those days =) all future”. by the time we have some insight into the pho­ netic realities hidden behind Sumerian spelling habits.4. Moreover. Directive The directive (locative-terminative) case particle [e] was. A iv 4: kar-Ni∞ginki-na-ke4 (dir. 43 ing is still inadequate. Cf.2. both cases have clearly established themselves as separate entities and.) má (absol.2. Either there were two syllables [ni“e] or -ni-”È stood for one syllable [ni“].”. Be that as it may. A iv 11. . 113 f. referred to the spelling sa∞g-bi“(PE”) “to its head” Gudea Cyl. See above 5. 5. In the ubiquitous dedicatory formula “for his/her life”.

Note: For the PN A-ba-DN/ses-∞gu10-GIM. in that it may. e. but it is not attested with plural. position inside. 5.4. We may try to visualize the directional functions of locative. For A-ba-ses-∞gu10-gé see ITT 4.2. contain the notion expressed by a dimensional case (cf.4. Recapitulation of the case particles The addition of case particles 2 and 4 to 10 is mutually exclusive. ge-en.. A-ba-dEn-líl-KI (YOS 4.KI “in Nippur”. ur5-GIM-ma-àm in Lugalbanda and Enm. Equative Noun + -gin7(GIM) formations may be taken as cases because the particle has the same position as the case particles for ergative through directive. ter­ minative. and gi-im. GIM = gin7 shares with some substantives the -m/n variant of its final consonant. Equative may be used with nouns of both classes: person or non-person.LÍL.g. 5. Equative stands apart. When -GIM is followed by the copula [am] the spelling is GIM-nam.7 end). there are variant spellings with -KI and -KID = -gé). In Proto-Ea 530 (MSL 14. and directive in the following graphs: Locative: Terminative: Directive: or o or o motion into.44   The directive is mainly used with nouns of the non-person class. GIM/DÍM is glossed gi-in. 109 [249]) where -ma. note to line 163).2. motion arriving at. position next to. The first var.has been confirmed by collation (Wilcke 1969. 5. Note.1 and see 5. 107.4. and it is only rarely found with person class. ama dumu-ni [dumu-(a)ni-e] ní∞g nu-ma-ni-ra [nu-mma-ni-n-ra] “no mother beat her child”. above. It cannot be excluded that the variant form GIM = gim was influenced by the [m] of Akkadian kìma. 163 (cf. Thomsen 1984. thus clearly leading to -gin7-nam.11. virtually.2.. . however. “let something hit at her child” Gudea Cyl. motion towards. 302:27) may be dis­ missed as a simple case of dittography: line 28 offers “à-EN.10. A xiii 3. 53). 7450:8. The equative expresses comparison and corresponds to Akkadian kìma (which may equally imply a dimensional case). however. lit. this also being the reason for the non-existence of such expressions as *lugal-ra-gin7 “like for a king”.

See the discussion by Wilcke 1990. . . 471–475. The case system is more restricted in the plural of person class where as well as the locative (4) and the ablative (7). [ake]. which are incompatible with person class. . sig-ta “from-below” = “lower”. [a(k)ta]. any of the case particles 2 to 10 may be added to genitive (3) [(a)(k)]. [aka(k)]. . [aka]. [a(k)gin]. the absolutive (1) and equative (10) are unrepresented. -ta-“è. [a(k)da]. On the other hand. [a(k)ra]. -ta-(a)k and . . [a(k)“e]. yielding the sequences [ake]. 45 Note: a-ab-ba-sig-ta-ta “from the Lower Sea” in Lugal-zagesi 1 ii 4–5 (Steible 1982/II 317) is no exception to the rule because -ta is added to a frozen expres­ sion. who also offers comparable examples for .

This page intentionally left blank .

(shared with verbs).. Formally. “The suffix of determination /a/”. (1b)). but rather apply in a variety of situations: ability to reduplicate (shared with substantives and verbs).14. é-gibil “new house”.3. “nominal predicate of copular clauses”. Much of the following is based on his study. Black sees no “watertight” category of adjective and. Gragg 1968. An apparent exception occurs when a substantive is followed by the individualizing plural element didli (see 5. 4. they occur with the verbal copula: udug-hul “evil demon”. Here. Attinger 1993. i. We owe to Black 2003 the first extensive study of the Sumerian adjective. 81–98.4). dedli vir­ tually also functions as an adjective as it is in origin. proposed to classify them as a sub-category of the verb.CHAPTER SIX ADJECTIVES Adjectives accompany substantives which they qualify. The respective criteria are not applicable to each and every adjec­ tive.2. Krecher’s study was taken up by Klein 1993. Krecher 1978. Once a substantive + adjective compound has been formed it is impenetrable and any particle has to follow the complex as a whole: ses-gal-ra “to the (big brother =) school overseer”. the criterion being whether or not they may be provided with the suffixed particle [a]. and he offers morphological and syntactical criteria for the identification of adjectives. or. submits ‘adjectives’ to scrutiny in terms of both their formation and . suffix -a (shared with the ¢am†u participle of the verb). as a pre­ dicate. 64. distinguishes between primary and secondary adjectives.2). by which he is—tentatively—followed by Thomsen 1984.. 382–85).e. therefore. posi­ tion after a substantive. negation with nu. 148 f. had proposed determinant force to -a: zi(-d) “true”. 376–403 (esp. but it is closer to the substantive: bàd-dedli-gal-gal “the individual big fortresses”. zi-da “right (not left)”. 91. the so-called ¢am†u participle of the verb (see 12. There has been much debate on whether adjectives should be considered a part of speech of their own (cf. zi-da is indistinguishable from du11-ga “said”.

in their Akkadian translations. however. venture a rather prag­ matic approach: what did the scribes. gal-zu “(know­ ing great =) skilful”. ní-tuku “(having awe =) reverent”. we quote only three examples from Black’s catalogue: á-∞gál “(strength available =) strong”. good thing =) beer of specially good quality”. me-dkal-dkal “highly valued rites”. colour. but pronounced babbar. ka“ ní∞g-du10-ga “(beer. the adjective as a part of speech is not contested)? Their approach was probably very close to ours. from Ur III on spelled by single UD. It occurs in reduplication: na4-gal-gal “big stones”.48   semantic type (dimension. kisal-mah “greatest (= main) court”. sig7-sig7 “green”. value. Note: For reduplicated adjectives as a means to express the pl. The adjective occurs in simple form: mu-gibil “new year”. 11. physical property. . speed. age.7. according to their syntactical position. above). A special feature is the reduplication of some adjectives denoting colours: bar6-bar6 “white”. N. of the preceding substantive see 5. we might. human propen­ sity.3. Postgate) There is a considerable number of compound nouns which Black treats. An adjective occurs as a base + particle -a: á-zi-da “right (arm =) side” (see Krecher 1978. udug­ hul “evil demon”. For the adjective as a base for adverbial expressions see ch. uncertain). is there a correspondence with Dstem (“quttulu”) adjectives of Akkadian? ( J. consider as adjectives (for in Akkadian. In view of the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area (see 17) and the bilingual lexical entries. as extended adjectives. Note: ní∞g-du10-ga = *dummuqu “improved”.

1959. The evidence for some of these is not uncontroversial. of the copula.1. A i 17. 34. 7. Already ibid. These particles have traditionally been treated as dependent forms of the demonstrative pronoun. 80–82. -bi. too. dependent -ne(n) may refer to substantives of both the person and non-person classes. 308. u4­ ne is virtually in the locative. lú-ne-da-me-e“. had a translation “an diesem seinen Tag”. be quite unusual. and -“e. Note: For this passage Falkenstein 1949. SPECIFICATION: DEMONSTRATIVE PARTICLES Certain particles suffixed to a substantive may serve to mark it as something set apart. lines 309 f. Thomsen 1984. A sequence directive [e] and loca­ tive [a] for describing the time of the day would. because the pl. u4-ne má“-∞gi6-ka “on that day. lú-ne-me-e“-da = itti annûtim raise our suspicion. lú-ne is so far only attested in lexical context: lú-ne-da = itti annîm “with this one” OBGT I 307 (MSL 4.2) we prefer to separate “pronouns” as independent words from particles attached to a noun.CHAPTER SEVEN DETERMINATION. Thomsen 1984. As with possession (see 5. Attinger 1993. See Falkenstein 1949. -e. is .. In the case of determination or specification there is another reason to keep demonstrative pronouns and demonstrative particles separated. Note: Pace Edzard 1997. B ix 7. 80. -ne(n) -ne(n): u4-ne-na “on this very day” Gudea Stat. 48). however. has “on this day”. but the case particle [a] is only noted at the end of the string. Note: It is hard for modern grammarians to judge whether these examples should be considered—at least partly—as a learned exercise or as a reflex of late OB spoken Sumerian. who reads u4-dè. me-e“ “they are”. 175 f. ki-lú-ne(-k)-ta = itti annîm ibid. in a night time vision” Gudea Cyl. and see lines 309–341. Whereas free ne(n) “this” only occurs as non-person class. 55 f. 8. -ri. partly because of orthographical ambiguity or scarcity of references. 55 fn. combining a demonstrative and a possessive particle. There are five particles to be discussed: -ne(n). 69.

plurality. B vii 49–53 is often quoted: alan-e ù kù-nu za-gìn nu-ga-àm [nu-(i)nga-am] ù erida-nu ù nagga-nu sipar-nu ki∞g-∞gá lú nu-ba-∞gá-∞gá “for the/this statue nobody was supposed to apply in work silver or lapis lazuli.1. not verbal. -ri(?) With -ri it is uncertain whether it should be considered a demon­ strative or an adjective meaning “far away. 186 . since it would be identical in spelling with the ergative and directive case particles. The distribution of u4-bi-a and u4-ba (cf. remote”. Woods 2003. perhaps. -e(?) It is difficult to establish the separate existence of a demonstrative particle [e]. cf. 49). 7. For the second form. it is not clear at all whether alan-e is “statue + demonstrative particle” or simply the directive case (“to apply in work material to the statue”). u4-bi-a “(in its day =) at that time”. examples meet the standards we are setting for “our” Sumerian grammar. 7. Was the [i] of demonstrative [bi] not overridden by the locative case particle [a] as was the [i] of pos­ sessive [bi]? In that case one would. Here. -e is attested in lexical context: lú-e-ra. sees the demonstrative -e as the origin of the erga­ tive case particle. sometimes referring to pri­ mordial days. 5.4. cf. relating to something” to “that”. 7. “the aforementioned”. the note at the end of 7.4. CAD N/2. lú-e-me-e“-a OBGT I 326 f. non-person class. and one easily sees a transition in meaning from “its. most of the sg. or copper or tin or bronze”.50   mechanically inserted to express nominal. end of 7. (MSL 4. See also Yoshikawa. have to distinguish between [biH] and [bi]. Gudea Stat. -bi -bi is most probably identical with the possessive particle of the 3rd sg.3. 4 ii 4 = 5 ii 5.2.2) is not yet clear. On the other hand. u4-bi-a already in Ukg.

118: IX (= X) 36 where it is read dal) and Aa II/7: 11' (MSL 14.. -ri-a may be analysed either as [ri] + locative case particle -a or as [ri] + -a of ¢am†u participle. lex.NUN text). cf. 45 offers “that person”. u4-RI-LI-na (= ina ùmè ullùti “in those days”) and he preferred reading u4-re-èn-na instead of u4-dalle-na.) where. -e. 293: ri-i = né-su-ú). out of the five elements discussed here. already OS TS” 79 i 1–5 (UD. and -“e. 177 fn.GAL. Jacobsen 1965. The topic must be left to further research. -bi. referred to SBH no. 176. Note: Yoshikawa 1993. deduces a pair “-re/i. tentatively proposed “anyone from here” basing himself on NGBT II i 11–15 (MSL 4. not on context examples. Yoshikawa essentially bases himself on lexical texts. due to the extreme scarcity of attestation.  51 nesû adj. . Recapitulation To sum up. however. -“e(?) -“e occurs in lú-“e in Gilgame“ and Agga 69–71. -ri is mainly found in the phrase u4-/∞gi6-/mu-ri(-a) “in those days/nights/years” when the poet refers to beginning in primordial time. with the locative not further noted in spelling. So. 7. 91–92 referring to Gilgame“ seen on the Uruk city wall by beleaguering Agga. Note: Shaffer 1963. tried to show there was a “Spatial Deictic System in Sumerian”. for Nabnìtu X 36 (MSL 16. 55. only two: -ne(n) and -bi. with -e “something in the space which is close to the speaker and addressee” and -ri “something which is remote from the space occupied by the speaker or addressee”. 122. 43. only line 11 [lú-“]e = animmamû can be restored more or less reliably. 7. -“e is not a certain candidate for inclusion among the demonstrative particles. -ne(n). 185–92. from which Attinger 1993. Katz 1993. -re-èn”. -ri. Römer 1980 lines 91 f. etc. 4:162 f.6. may be convincingly classified as demonstrative particles. lú-“e “der Mann da”. 158 f.5.

This page intentionally left blank .

—case). Mongolian. in agglutinative languages.—case—poss.—case).CHAPTER EIGHT RESUMING THE SEQUENCE OF PARTICLES FOR POSSESSION.— case). For the sake of comparison.). examples are given for Turkish.—poss. and Finnish: Turkish: karde{-ler-im-e “to my brothers” (brother—pl. These few examples may show that there is no “universal” rule. and case. Note: Demonstrative particles (see 7) are not included here. NUMBER AND CASE In Sumerian ses-∞gu10-(e)ne-r(a) “to my brothers” exhibits a sequence possessive—number—case whose particles cannot switch in rank one with another. Hungarian: barát-a-im-nak “to my brothers” (brother—pl. number. Hungarian. . They most proba­ bly share the rank with possession.—poss. Mongolian: (minu) aqa-nar-dur “to (my) brothers” (brother—pl. for the hierarchy of the suffixed morphemes used to express possession. Finnish: talo-i-ssa-ni “in my houses” (house—pl.

This page intentionally left blank .

and there is no need to attribute me of Proto-Ea to the possessive particles. 19). The dative forms ∞gá-ra.2). [za]. NS. OB e-ne 3rd sg. 3rd sg. when they occur in different manuscripts of OB literary texts. Lam. Moreover. . ur5 “this. ur5. sana. 1st pl. cannot be safely reconstructed and/or.: It is hard to ascertain the exact phonetic nature of these pronouns. P  The system is still incomplete for us in the 1st and 2nd pl. cf. modern Turkish ben. given the existence of possessive -bi. Note to 1st and 2nd sg. But regres­ sive assimilation is equally possible. ur5-“è-àm “for this (purpose)”. Cf. ∞gá-e (or contracted ∞ge24-e) za-e (but note contracted NS zé) person cl. [ze] ¡ [zara] would not be inconceivable. however. me. me-en-zé-en. independent me “we” is so far unattested in context. 2nd sg. This cor­ responds to the strange fact that many inflected forms of the verb in the 1st and 2nd pl. secondarily me-en-dè-en Note: me = ni-i-nu is clearly offered by Proto-Ea vocabulary 71:5 (MSL 14. that” may be considered a substitute. The lexical equation me-da-nu = i-na ba-l[u-ni ] “without us” OBGT I (vii) 484 (MSL 4. For me-en-dè(-n) in OB literary con­ text. The (secondary) form me-en-dè-en looks identical to the inflected copula. but the chances of meeting such a me are—statistically—quite small. -me “our” (see 5. “we are”: [me-(e)nden]. however. 2nd pl.CHAPTER NINE PRONOUNS 9.1. 1st sg. za-e-me-en-zé-en Attested lexically only. sen with datives bana. Admittedly. [∞ge] ¡ [∞gara]. only in combination with case particles and/or the copula: ur5-gin7 “like this”. 53) is obviously derived from me “we”. because the personal pronouns only occur when emphasis is needed. present incon­ gruous variants. non-person class: no independent pronoun is safely attested although one would expect it. does not seem to occur alone. Ur and Sumer 240. OS a-ne. za-ra may point to bases [∞ga].

comitative. ∞gá(-a)-ar. So ki-zu-ta “from your place” would equal “from you” (cf. As with substantives. Terminative is so far unattested. -ta. 5. you =) mine.4.2).. pronouns.2. then there are genitive. because much of the Babylonian scribe’s interpretation looks dubious in the light of modern scholarship.56   Note: me-en-zé-en looks identical to the inflected copula. Spanish nosotros.g. these exist only in the form of the genitive of the personal pronoun combined with the copula. zá(-a)-ra. be constructed with the help of the circumpositional syntagma ki. For secondary pronouns of the 1st and 2nd pl. dative. e-ne-ne (OB). . 76). 554 and 556) both of which we would translate as “to them” [enene-r(a)].) often is “thou + pl. person class a-ne-ne (OS. Sumerian personal pronouns have a reduced declension pattern. Comitative: ∞gá-e-da-nu-me-a “not being with me” = “without me”. za(-a)-kam “it is (of me. Equative: ∞gá-e-gin7-nam “it is like me”. an ablative can. and equative. clearly is one of a pri­ vate opposition: “they” = “he/she + pl. e. at least judging by orthography. By analogy with the relation -zu : -zu-ne-ne in the set of possessive particles.. 50–54).”.6). NS). [ene-(e)ne]. 9. While the morphological relation between those particles and the free forms is obvious. 3rd pl. e-ne-ra. and pl.”. Here. e-ne-er “to me. Poebel 1923. however. P  Strictly speaking. and directive.. cf. the relation between 3rd sg. Com­ pare e-ne-ne-ra and e-ne-ne-er (ibid. We have treated the possessive particles by themselves under “pos­ session” (see 5. compared to that of the substantive. [ane-(e)ne]. Dative: gá(-a)-ra. would have been identical in spelling with the ergative. The free genitive gá(-a)-kam. ∞ him/her”. ∞ yours” functions as a free possessive pronoun (see 9. “without my permission” (cf. . for absolutive and erga­ tive. but are rendered by “u-nu-ti and e-li-“u-nu respectively. a form *za-ne(-ne) might be reconstructed.2). if it existed. ∞gá(-a)-kam “it is (of me =) mine”.2. 5.4. . “you are”: [me-(e)nzen]. as offered in OBGT I 376–556 (MSL 4.7 end). you. it is still difficult to explain the origin of the possessive particles unless we move back into pre­ history (cf.2.g. e. because “you” (pl.. There is just one form. As in many languages. or French nous autres (français) “we (French)”. Locative and ablative are not to be expected because they only occur with non-person class. vosotros. Note: We do not quote the rich lexical evidence for 1st to 3rd pl.

3. ur5 ur5 (already mentioned 9. 9.4. at least as far as it occurs independently. the second [ana] about something of the non-person class. the first [aba] asking about someone of the person class.3. There are. 9. D  The Sumerian system is more difficult to describe and define than the very clearly developed system of Akkadian demonstrative pronouns. 57 At any rate. 129:42 f. ur5-ra-ke4-é“ “therefore” (lit. 8). reflected by ne-en-nam di-ku5-dNanna-kam = annûm dìnu “a Sin “as (regards) this. therefore. [ur-ak] as a free genitive.2. only two words known that would deserve the definition of (independent) demonstrative pronouns: 9. lú gùd-∞gá ne-en ba-e(-a)-AG-a “you. non-person class. ne nam-[di]-dEn-líl-lá “this is [a judgement] of Enlil” Ur III PN. with the adverbiative particle [e“(e)]). 9. I  Here. 3rd sg. 40:9. 198).1): ur5 hé-na-nam(-ma-àm) “let it really be this”. 105. ur5 is restricted to the non-person class and has. the universal pair “who” and “what” prevails. ur5-gin7 “like this”. 1 iv 18).3. been considered a substitute for the—non-existing—personal pronoun. and see Wilcke 1969. NG no. the person (who) did this to my nest” Lugalb. this—what is it?— will (live =) stay” Innin “ag. ne-e(n) ne-e(n) is equally restricted to the non-person class. number. so far. 246 (ZA 65. the high antiquity of possessive particles is suggested by the fact that of the categories of possession. (PBS 10/1. it is a judgement of the Moongod” van Dijk 1953. and case particles. the possessive particles rank closest to the substantive (see above ch.1. ∞gá-e za-kam ne-e a-na-àm ì-til-l[e] “I am yours. 164. The opposition b : n (instead of .

. like 3rd person class pronoun e-ne. 144:36 f.g. 78 f. at least if one expects a morphological corre­ spondence between interrogative and personal pronouns as. themselves”. A  9. strictly speaking.5. the restrictions as to the affixation of case particles affecting the personal pronouns apply equally to the inter­ rogative pronouns (see 9. as a possible correspondence to Akkadian ajjû. There seems to have been a general distribution rule: ní-te-V ver­ sus ní-C. Reflexive pronoun? The Sumerian base ní-(te-) serves to express. ní-(te-) + possessive particle is a nominal phrase not different from. But note ní-te-∞gu10-“è “to myself ” UET 6/2. . For na-me “any one” see 9. Note: CAD A/1. 178–80. Attinger 1993.58   *n : b) is surprising.. in Latin quis : is.. and therefore. i. here.. quoted by Attinger 1993. ní-(te-) can­ not be considered a “pronoun”. and both a-ba and a-na. Cavigneaux 1978. Thomsen 1984.. 174): Nite(TE:ME)-ga-i “Let me praise (my)self ” DP 73 ii 2 (PN). perhaps as a question. Alster 1974. ní-zu-“è ∞gé“tu-zu “to your­ self your ear!” (= “be aware of yourself ”) Angim 81. along with possessive particles. i. 177–85. most probably to be read nix-te (Attinger 1993. and see more examples in Thomsen 1984.1.e. There is no adjectival interrogative. 174 f.. e. For a-ba. For bilingual contexts where ní-te = ramanu see CAD R 117 lex. Generally speaking. ergative a-ba-a is attested.5. ní-te(-a)-ne-ne for “my/your/itself ”. may be combined with the copula -àm. 38:7.5. One would rather associate ní-(te) with parts of the body.2. quid : id or German wer : er. the Sumerian text has the well-known syntagma na-me + negated verb. 234 ajû lex. 9. sa∞g + poss.. e. was : das. notions which we are used to translate by “myself ”. ní-∞g u 10/-zu versus ní-te(-a)-ni. particle. “him/herself.g. etc. so that the Akkadian translation “which one” would actually render “(has) no one”. ajjìtu “which”. 174. 78 f. NS or OB ní-te is preceded by OS ME-te.1). however. quotes a late reference where na-me is rendered by ajjû “which”.e. with more lit. by a reflexive pronoun: “ám-ní-te-na “purchase price of him­ self ” (= for his own person) NG no.

“anything”. inde­ pendent of person or non-person class. na-me nu-kúr-ru “nobody will alter it” SKIZ 83 i 15. na-me may be an abbreviated form of lú-na-me. .2. lú-na-me.5. u4-na-me “anyone”. so that it means either (positive) “any (one)” or (negative) “no (one)”. [me] of na-me should at least until we have better knowledge not be associated with the verbal base me “to be”. In the majority of references it occurs along with a negative verbal form. Indefinite pronoun? 59 na-me is found both as a substantive and as an adjectival base. Note: In the last example as in parallel cases. 78. Thomsen 1984. “any (day =) º time”. níg-na-me. 9. 174. Attinger 1993.

This page intentionally left blank .

10–60. Sumerian numeration had the enormous advantage of being able to divide by 3 or 6 without leaving a remainder.e. It permeates the metrological systems of the Ancient Near East. 2198: Edzard 1980. 2003b.CHAPTER TEN NUMERALS 10. For—purely graphic—exceptions to this rule see 5.. 10. e. *[∞gé“(d)u] versus [∞ge“(d)ú]? Syntactically. i. With sixty as the “hun­ dred”. . 36000–216000. ∞ what was the pronunciation of “sixty (plus) ten”.G. including fractions. G The Sumerian language had a “sexagesimal” system in which numer­ ation proceeded in alternating steps of 10 and 6: 1–10. 60– 600. 600–3600.e.. The sexa­ gesimal system has left its traces in our modern divisions of the hour or of the compass.1. by numerals so that the position of a numeral corresponds to that of an adjective. 70? Was there a difference of stress or some other means of intonation. not pre­ ceded.6.. 1989.g. persons or things counted were followed. C  The oldest pronunciation guide for the Sumerian cardinal numbers 2 to 10 is an exercise tablet from Ebla: TM 75. capacity. “ten (times) sixty”. note. To what degree were numbers subject to case inflection? If 600 was pronounced ge“-u “sixty ten”. Powell 1971. volume.3. as well as notations of length. i. we are poorly informed on the pronunciation and the morpho-syntactical behaviour of numbers in Sumerian. surface. Since cardinal and ordinal numbers. 3600–36000.2. and weight are next to exclusively written with number signs.

AfO 24 (1973) 77. If 6 really were 5+1. cf. deli-du-ni “he is coming alone” might also be proposed. partly corrected by J. de-e-li. 13 ff. 2.4). See J. A”(deli)-di∞gir-re-ne “the only one of the gods” VS 10. Pettinato.62 Ebla 1. N. unique” (wèdum). 66). two. It is difficult to differentiate a“ from deli. 9. Notes on the individual numbers 1 to 10: For 1 and the following cardinal numbers. Reduplicated deli: dedli indicates detailed plurality (see 5. A”-ni “he/she all alone” is read deli­ ni or a“(a)-ni by individual authors. 7. there is no var. Postgate. 3. 362 f. RlA 8. a“ and di“ so far only an approxi­ mate distribution pattern can be offered: deli is “single. quite uncertain.3. cf. Krebernik. dA”-im4/im-bar6-bar6/babbar (discussed by M. 10. § 2. di“ (ge(4)) min e“5 limmu ía à“ umun5. 5. However. AIUON 41 (1981) 141–43 (“inspired” by Edzard 1980). lú-sa∞g-deli.   later tradition a“. Ebla A-“u might be explained as *[ia+a“]. du-li). 199 iii 4 (cf. imin ussu ilimmu u (slanted vertical wedge) mì-nu [min] i“11-“a-am [i“/e“ + copula] li-mu [lim(m)u] i [ia] A-“u [A“] ù-mi-nu [umin] ù-sa-am [us + copula] ì-li-mu [ilim(m)u] u9-wa-mu [*haw(?) + copula] Diakonoff 1983. however. G. RA 64 (1970) 188. or A“x(GE23)-“a4-ni (DP 124 iv 2. 4. 6. glossed sagdilû “(per­ son. This is. deli. 83–93. The OS PN [A“ani “he alone”] is spelled A-“a4(DU)-ni (DP 125 iv 6). not bound to either person or non-person class. “á“”).3. Falkenstein 1959. three”). A similar uncertainty is found with the epi­ thet of the Moongod. bandi“ “1 seah” (fol­ . 126 iv 3). Bauer. 8. 1: for deli (di-li. single head =) bachelor”. the most exhaustive treatment still is that by Powell 1971. di“ may have been the regular counting word (“one.

A iv 23. note also the copula in Ebla 8 and 10. For ní∞g-úr-limmu “(thing four limbs =) quadrupeds” an emesal writing has ne-mu-li-mu (TCL 15. 2: It is unclear whether Ebla mì-nu stands for [min] (which we adopted) or for bi-syllabic [minu]. stand for [à“]. For the Emesal form [did] see Schretter 1990. see below. there is tab “double. 5 is part of numerals 7 and 9. [yà“]. 164 f. 7: Ebla ù-mi-nu (for the ending -u see above 2) does not display the expected form 5+2 because of its Anlaut [u]. strangely enough. 63 lowed by banmin). 34). 3: Ebla i“11-“a-am can only be explained as 3 [i“] or [e“] + copula [am]. A may stand for [a. munus di“-àm “there was a (certain) woman” Gudea Cyl. So. and. This. Ebla spelling does not yet clearly denote syllable-closing consonants and often resorts to CV(-C) or CV-C(V). however.). 35 pointed to the OS spelling “u-IÁ for “u-i “barber”. however. rarely LAM-mu. 4: Ebla li-mu may stand for single or lengthened [m]. parallel”. tab and min are combined in gú-tab-min-bi “its two parallel banks”. i early found its way into Sumerian and Akkadian syllabaries. a in a-á“ may also be taken as a simple vocalic indicator: aá“. Krebernik. ZA 72 [1982] 191). The post-Eblaic gloss a-á“ may. that 4 li-mu definitely stands for the bisyllabic numeral. There is. then the element “1” could only be [a“] and not [di“]. ya] (Krebernik. Falkenstein. If 6 really were a compound of 5+1. ZA 72 [1982] 180 f. 3:4. Note. 6 has been analysed as 5+1 by analogy with the evident cases of 5+2 and 5+4. Apart from min. 5: Ebla i is ambiguous as it may stand for [ya] or [yi] (cf. *ia+a“ “5+1” might have yielded [ya’a“]. is most probably due to anology with the Anlaut of following 8. no lexical correspondence di“ = i“tèn. however. Post-Eblaic lexical glosses note lim-mu. [us] or . aw. 6: Ebla A-“u (for the -u ending see above 2) is graphically as ambigu­ ous as is Ebla 5. ay. ZA 53 [1959] 101 fn. but does not have to. di“ is not found in Proto-Ea. Powell 1971.

2 and 38 fnn. CAD ”/l. No references are available before the 1st mill. and Westenholz 1991 have shown. No references are available before the 1st mill.turned into d. 37 with fn. ú. is definitely not *5+3. 82 who derives “ussu < *iá+e“5 (5+3)”. sekiz. Note. 10: Ebla U9-PI-mu is difficult to “normalize”. Since there is no trace of [m] in later glosses. is not attested at all in lex­ ical glosses. Powell 1971. Ebla -mu may be again part of the copula [am]: *ha-wa-m(u). hu-u. ha-a (Powell 1971. *[haw]. a. . 1–4 (with previous literature) already clearly dismissed the 5+3 theory. Latin quattuor.40. 234 “alà“à lex. Note: Rhyme and assonance are universally attested in neighbouring digits: cf. kaksi. Maybe ì (versus I) noted a reduced form of 5 in the compound. leav­ ing us with *haw+am. [us + copula] or [us(s)a + copula]. 139. 30: [u“(u)]. We may guess at juxtaposition of 10 + digit with possi­ ble reduction or contraction in the compounds. see Powell 1971. Powell 1971.. 8: Ebla ù-sa-am. i. that the first element. Slavic 9 whose initial n. 20: [ni“]. 48. used by most Assyriologists. [ne“]. 43 f.by analogy with 10. des’at’.. Gong 2000. Edzard 1980. kuusi. Later. dokuz. as is still assumed in Thomsen 1984. in earlier OB Akkadian spelling. 41 f. in the course of time. as Poebel 1939. [umin] turned into [umun].g. CAD E 367 e“rà lex. The etymologically “cor­ rect” imin. 9: Ebla ì-li-mu offers the earliest and clearest example of a Sumerian digit compound 5+n.). may have turned into *[hò] *[hù]. For the difficulties we encounter with the exact form of 8. 116 f. e.64   [us(s)a]. No evidence is so far available for the pronunciation of numbers 11 to 19. was used to indicate [o]. Anlaut U9 with val­ ues [ha] or [˙a] (cf. quinque. yedi. viisi. Turkish altı. Finnish yksi. At this juncture it must be remembered that U. but occurs in sign-names. or. 5. For lexical evidence see Powell 1971. 48.e. Russian dev’at’. 126) would be compatible with two of the five glosses preserved for U “ten”: u4. most noteworthy. see Powell 1971. is spelled NI = ì and not I as with 5. however.

.U. 36.000: [“aru] is spelled ”ÁR × U until at least Ur III and later on ”ÁR. 62 etc. See Powell 1971. ni-in. Again. 50: [ninnû].g. ni-mi-in.C. 48.g.. 49. we may ask how 600 was dis­ tinguished in fast speech from 60 + 10 = 70. nèru of unknown ori­ gin. we do not know how 61. glossed ni-/ni-in-/nin-nu-u is a short form for *[ni(“)min-u] “forty + ten”. For independent 30 cf. [nìn]: Powell 1971. The reading of 60 was definitely settled by Steinkeller 1979. remembered by Greek n∞row. 60: [∞ge“(d)]. glossed ni-mìn. For lex. Powell 1971. were pronounced. that 62 *∞ge“+min “sixty (plus) two” and *∞gé“-min “two sixties” should have been homophonous. CAD ”/3. e. It is hardly pos­ sible in a society permeated with calculation and accountancy. 120 = ∞ge“-min. 65 Note: In view of 50 = 40 + 10. [nimin]. 78. one might venture an evolution *ni“-u 20+10 > *(n)u“u. may be explained as a reduced form of *[ni“-min] “two twenties”. No references are available before the 1st mill. 81 ¢am“à lex. 40: [nimin]. 50–53. 380 “u“i lex. Note: Let it be asked how 2002 (3 × 600 + 3 × 60 + 22) was pronounced. Whereas multiples of 60 have been glossed. evidence see Powell 1971.. é-ninnû “50 houses” during the 24th to 21st centuries B. by Greek as sãrow. 600: [∞ge“(d)u] “ten sixties”. CAD E 255 erbà lex. Was it something close to *[∞ge“(d)u-e“ ∞ge“-e“ ni“-min]? 3600: [“ar] “ár “circle” represents both the concrete figure and the “myriad”. e. But an independent word is possible as well. pace Edzard 1997 passim in “Eninnu”) is certainly a far shot from the contemporary—and virtually unknown—pronunciation of NinªGirsu’s ziggurat complex at ªGirsu. Akkadian had an individual word for 600. finally. No ref­ erences are available before the 1st mill. CAD ›. CAD ”/2. It was borrowed by Akkadian as “àr and. Turkish otuz. Note: ninnû (not “ninnu”.. 176–87. [ninnû]. 36. 600 became a new basic number so that 1200 was *∞ge“(d)u-min “two six-hundreds”.

154 ff.000 + 7200 + 2400 + 420 + 27 = 367. nos. Origin (and application) of this system are still unknown. 73–78.. 28–32 with lit. pe“-pe“-ge4 “three three one” “7”: NBGT IV 33. as to be expected. for 1 to 7 is found in the (reconstructed) series me-er-ga “1”. 39) notes [did]. 72.). 70. 331. 71. [“ar∞ge“(d)] is written ”ÁR × GÉ” or ”ÁR × U-gunû until Ur III and later on ª ”ÁR. 3”. CIRPL Ukg. CIRPL Ent. “ár-gal“u-nu-tag-ga “big 3600 that has not been touched”. for [mu“] “60” see ibid. 32 i 2''). 397.3. . “à-lú-”ÁR × U-ta (Erikagina.66   Unlike 600. [imma] and [ammu“] for “1. pe“bala “beyond three” “4”. 134–138. 36. In practice. Emesal voc. Atiqot 4 (1965) no. e. 41–45 (MSL 4. 164 f. Sumerian numerals. cf. 27 = 216. 76–78. 10. 180. Stat. 7 60. cardinal numbers may also function as ordinal num­ bers. 51–53. O  In principle. 216.000 has not become a new basic number. TAKA “2”. Gudea Cyl. The highest number attested is 216.. More details in Powell 1971. rising in groups of three. Artzi/S. and see Powell 1971.647 gi-sa “reed-bundles” P. “á-lú-”ÁR × U-gunû-ta (Gudea. 2 “ár 4 600. pe“ “3”.000 is “ár-ninnû “fifty 3600”.000 × 60 = 12. See Powell 1971. 395. 4/5 vii 18). pe“-bala-ge4-ge4 “beyond three + one + one” “6”. It would. 73–77. be absurd to refer this diachronic increase of symbolic figures to a corresponding growth of the population. had special forms in Emesal.000 in royal inscriptions from ªGirsu in the phrase “when god’s hands seized (the ruler) from among n people”: [“à-l]ú-“ár-ta (Enmetena. “ár-gal occurs in Ur III administrative documents with the count of reed-bundles. Note: It is interesting to note the increase of the number 3600 −> 36. “ár-gal. cf. III 131–133 (MSL 4. of course. Lewy. B iii 10).000 −> 216.000. Schretter 1990.960. 2. A xxi 1 é-a(k) sá-min-nam nam-mi(-n)-sì “(Gudea) verily laid the second square (of the ziggurat)”. 190. followed by sá-e“5­ àm (xxi 5) up to sá-umun7 (xxi 11). 4 ”ÁR × U.000 is “ár-ni“ “twenty 3600” and not *“two 36. See Powell 1971.000: [“ar∞ge“(d)] = “sixty 3600.ªGÉ”.000”. pe“-bala-ge4 “beyond three + one” “5”. 53. A peculiar system.000 + 144. 8 iv 3 = 10–11. also “ár-gal “big 3600”.g. See Powell 1971.

[(a)kama(k)] became the ending to mark an ordinal number. A secondary means to form ordinal numbers was to put the num­ ber in the genitive followed by the copula [am]: u4-2-kam [u(d)-minak-am] “it is of day two” = “the second day”. For the general interpretation (as against Edzard 1997. and 1989 throughout. 10. and Huwawa A 37–43 di“-àm. A xxiii 4. already towards the end of the OS period. cf. Poebel 1923. Cf. 113–14. the (measure +) number are followed by the ablative case particle. 39 [1970] 492–95)..4. the locative u4-umun7-kam-ma-ka “on the seventh day” Gudea Cyl. see Powell 1971. gín-7-ta “(rings of ) 7 shekels each”. F    For the extremely diversified Sumerian system of fractions and mea­ surements. ZA 87 (1997) 1–10. a-rá “course” is placed before the numeral: a-rá-3(-kam) “three times”.g. practically. Gilg. For an OB sequence “first” to “seventh” cf. 82) see C. gang.5. min-/e“5-/limmu5-/ía-/à“-/umun7-kam-ma. 6 iii' 20'–22'). it is possible that the ordinal number ending was in fact [kama(k)] and not [(a)kama(k)]. 114–16. gång in Scandinavian languages. e. Since the number sign is never explicitly followed by the [a] of the genitive.6. 67 Note: For purely rhythmical reasons. Suter. 84–248. 10. two-syllable numbers are not (see W. Note: Semantically. Poebel 1923. D    For expressing “each”. M For “times”. was extended by the addition of a second genitive case particle so that. Or. . one-syllable numbers are followed by the copula [am]. refer­ ring to “the first (demon)” etc. the copula [am] may still be added: -ta-àm. This construction. munus-u4-bi-ta-ke4-ne ninta-2-ta ì(-n)-tuku-am6 “the women of yore took two men each” (CIRPL Ukg. Heimpel. to -ta. 10.

This page intentionally left blank .

168–70. also be combined with substan­ tives. For literature see Attinger 1993.e. only “ul-le-é“ “in the manner of a strong young man”. [ud-e“(e)] “like daylight”. combined with adjectives. and see Attinger 1993. c) R-e“(e). see also above 4. 168). 170). c) zi-dè-e“/-é“(-e/-“è). u4-dè-e“/-é“(e). 168–70). b) gibil-bi-e“/-é“ “anew”. e. is “peu productif ”. . According to Attinger.. Attinger’s “adverbes de manière” (1993. 169 (b) for many more examples (“type très productif ”). on the other hand. righteous way”. Attinger’s “adverbes de modalité et de temps” (1993. this type. a) “úr-bi = ezzi“ “furiously”.(5). [e“(e)] may. Most of the “adverbes de manière” corre­ spond to Akkadian adverbs in -i“. mah-bi = ßìri“ “in a magnificent way” (CAD Í 207). yielding a “type très productif ” (Attinger 1993.CHAPTER ELEVEN ADVERBS We here restricted the term “adverb” to derivations from adjectives or non-finite verbal forms in -a (¢am†u participles). damqi“ “in a good way”.. Formally. i.2..5. excluding expressions indicating modal­ ity or time.g. gal-bi “greatly”. correct.e. cf. b) R-bi-e“(e). [zid-e“(e)] “in a true. there are three types of adverbs: a) R-bi. This is type (a) with the addition of the adverbiative ending.2. i. see below 14.

This page intentionally left blank .

“put”. P Describing the Sumerian verb is the most difficult part for the gram­ marian.. 1950. e. partly for chosen sectors. partly for the language as a whole. For the sake of clarity we introduce. 1965. 1959.g. 1951. The base is impenetrable. Edzard 1995. Römer 1982.g. Thomsen 1984. na∞g(NAG) “to drink”. ti/te-∞g. into which the varying forms of the base can be entered (as far as they are already known): .. extended or reduced form.. è/è-d. grids with horizontal and vertical divisions. or heteronymy.DU or DU . may be read (depending on context) du. ‘Ablaut’.e.. but always with the focus on the verb. i. The base is either invariable in form or subject to variation. Since Poebel. ∞gen “to go”. ∞ Frequently the same spelling is used for two variants. gar/∞gá∞ gá. e. Jacobsen 1956. expressing person.g. Jestin 1943. 1988. 21991. de6 “to bring”. DU Variants of the base of a given verb may concern the categories ¢am†u and marû and/or singular and plural. e. 21939. and a series of prefixed and/or suffixed particles. and non-finite verbs: infinitives. The Sumerian verb consists of a base. Note: A specially ambiguous sign is DU which. Falkenstein 1949. Black 1984. gi4/gi4-gi4. have appeared in a considerable number and in chrono­ logical density: Deimel 1924. But in quite a few cases one cannot be certain whether B (= base) has but one reading or two. One still admires A. Sollberger 1952. Attinger 1993. The base is quoted in lexi­ cal texts and is regularly translated by an Akkadian infinitive (gu7 = akàlu). or the base alone. túm. One may distinguish between finite verbs. Sumerian grammars. has to be read na8-na8(NAG.NAG) when reduplicated.1. gub “to stand”.g. as a verbal base. no infixes may be inserted such as -t(a). rá. 1946.CHAPTER TWELVE THE VERB 12.. e. not to mention the different readings of reduplicated DU. gu7 “to eat”. Poebel’s work of 1923 who practically “conquered the unconquerable” with his Grundzüge der sumerischen Grammatik. reduplication (full or partial).or -ta(n). participles.in Akkadian. for verbal bases. Kaneva 1996. dug4/e.

¢. both prefixed and suffixed. since up to 5 particles may appear in a row. e. compatible only with the marû base. or rank. en. Mendizabal. The surrounding (i. The first-to-the-left of these. le. pl. dative. La lengua vasca. y.2. in diminish­ ing distance. Note: For the sake of comparison. from the base: a) the particle [ed]. ranked in the sequence (from right to left) locative 2. each in the positive and neg­ ative.. d) prefixed markers of connection. Heimpel. may imply motion or absence of motion.. . je te le dis etc. we arrive at about 80 forms. la. Whereas the variation of the string of suffixed particles is limited. L.72   sg. for the Gudea cor­ pus 154 “prefix chains”. e.g.. nous.e. For the pre-Ur III period Heimpel offered 122. enim hu-mu-na-ni-ib-ge4-ge4 [hu-mu-na-ni-b-gege] “the word—verily—ventive—dative (to him/ her)—directive/causative—absolutive (it)—make return (marû base)— (ergative: he/she)” = “he/she verily answers him/her thereupon”. m. les. ablative(-instrumental). 21959 [Buenos Aires] 353–69. moreover. e) and f ) nominalizer [a] and copula [am]. negation. b) ergative and absolutive markers. both suffixed. locative. These partly have person or non-person reference. the “prefix string” offers an astounding number of variants. added up to 1264. that in modern French. suffixed. if we arrange all possible combinations of bound elements je with me. terminative. For details on the categories ¢am†u and marû see 12.g. in his 1974 count of OB “prefix chain” variants. c) prefixed markers of dimensional reference.. directive. prefixed and suffixed) verbal particles pre­ sent an enormous variety. however. compare the “prefix chains” in modern Basque (Guipuzcoan). Let it be borne in mind. jene-lui-en-ai-rien-dit “I did not tell him/her anything about it” is as complex a verbal form as many in Sumerian. this figure may now easily be raised to about 1300–1400. vous. leur. and they may be classified. In fact. and of diverse posi­ tive or negative modalities. lui. comitative. a language structurally comparable to Sumerian: 537 Variants are listed in J. te.

¢am†u (¢an†u) or marû is found as an addition to the Akkadian trans­ lation of a Sumerian (verbal) entry. thereafter. as Steiner 1981.. transitive. indicate (when ¢. Krecher’s decisive argument (1995. 185–87.” in the right. gave a survey of the successive efforts of scholars to explain ¢.. indicate (when m.) presents no such opposition. 1.). “achevé”: . and m. Note: The two conjugation patterns have. They have been the cause of lengthy learned debates..g.) versus du (m. occur­ ring in two different.)”. Akkadian. to mark. igi-zu is. since Yoshikawa 1968. were used as grammatical terms by the compilers of bilingual lexical lists. The same formal dilemma we would encounter in Akkadian where. 209–12. It has in fact proved practical to apply the terms ¢am†u and marû to those complementary variants of the Sumerian verbal base. iprus : iparras would cor­ respond to “short” : “long” whereas ìkul : ikkal.SAL. igi-zu-zu = II marû “to mark. had seen it). 142) was that the references “¢. Falkenstein 1949–50. in fact. and m. For while. been named “Präteritum”: “Präsens-Futur” (Poebel 1923. e. “slow (long)”. 1–14. with more lit. 7–9.g.2. 55 f. Römer 1982. they cannot in all cases be describing its form. igi-zu = uddû ¢an†u. therefore. ∞gen “to go” (¢. brought argu­ ments that ¢. 99–119. Lambert 1991. column should be seen in the same way as the reference EME. Sumerian. conjugation patterns: “preterite” in∞gar “he set (something)” and “present” ib-∞gá-∞gá “he sets/will set it”. “quick (short)” as opposed to igi-zu-zu. and Krecher 1995.g. and to two non-finite uses of the verbal base: ∞gar-ra [∞gar-a] “set” (past participle) and ∞gá-∞gá-(d) [∞ga-∞ga-ed] “setting” (present participle). ›AMˇU  MARÛ 73 These Akkadian adjectives meaning. 142 with fn. We will. See now also Attinger 1993. and m. slow” respec­ tively. “quick” and “fat. Sumerian. 173. from 1885–1968. the discussion between 1971 and 1984 has been summarized by Thomsen 1984. 115–23. found in the right hand Akkadian column of lexical lists referred to the left hand.. as well as Jacobsen 1988.. Black 21991. alternatively.  12. column. which can only refer the Sumerian expression given in the left. retain those two terms both for different ver­ bal bases and for the conjugation patterns in which they occur. i“ìm : i“àm would not. If ¢am†u and marû refer to the Sumerian verbal base.)” Edzard 1971. e. e. Be that as it may. column (and not to the right hand col.

“standard construction” : “secondary construc­ tion” (Black 21991. if there are two or more. Note: A verb is transitive if it displays the two conjugation patterns 2a and 2b. -(ed): eme èd-dè-da-ni [ed-ed-ani] “while sticking out its tongue” Gudea Cyl. and their distribution corresponds. furthermore. V  ¢†⁄⁄. 12. once more. some verbs distinguish indi­ vidual bases for whether the ‘subject’ (with intr. both a ¢am†u and a marû base are found. nevertheless. see 12.2) which most often corresponds to the Akkadian iparras.⁄. verbs) or ‘object’ (with trans. We tradition­ ally call these verbs intransitive. used in the transitive conjugation pattern 2a (12.4. “¢am†u” : “marû” (Attinger 1993).74   “inachevé” ( Jestin 1943. 130 f. As noted above (12. The ¢am†u variant provides the base for a participle in -a (most prob­ ably not identical with the nominalizer [a]. passim and spec. however. used in the transitive conjugation pattern 2b (12. We can offer eight variants. grids.). There may. 209). It is. P V Apart from the ¢am†u : marû dichotomy.3. If one person is registered to “stay. 54. tìl is used. B xiv 7. verbs) is in the sg. the verb will be se12.7. “perfective” : “imperfective” (Steinkeller 1979. It is.1) it proves practical to enter the different attested base forms in a grid. It is also used in the intransitive conjugation pattern 1 (12. be more because—most probably—the correct read­ ings of some ambiguous cuneograms still escape us. Jacobsen 1988.1). For a restricted number of intran­ sitive verbs. to Akkadian iprus and iparras: ba-∞gen “I went away”.16): dMes-lam-taè-a “(the god) who has come out of the Meslam (sanctuary)”. 174 with fn.7. furthermore. 12. .3) which most often corresponds to the Akkadian preterite iprus. 15). or pl. A large group of verbs have only one conjugation pattern which may correspond to both Akkadian iprus and iparras. The marû variant is used to form a participle in -e(d).  The Sumerian verbal base in ¢am†u/marû/sg./pl.7. live” somewhere. ì-du-un [i-du-(e)n] “I go”.

12. ¢. 163–73. sg. ti/te etc.g. e. see Krecher 1995.15.1. ¢. a a' è è è(-d) è(-d) “come out” ti/te ti/te ti(-∞g)/te(-∞g) ti(-∞g)/te(-∞g) “approach” Here. for similar cases of pseudoreduplication of a verbal base to indicate the marû. a m.3. a'-a' a'-a' zi(-g) zi(-g) zi-zi zi-zi “rise. “u ha-ba-an-“i-íb-TI. sg. ¢. It has not yet been estimated how great a percentage the class represents in the total of (attested) Sumerian verbs.1.2).4. a a m. a m.4.. 12. sg. Note: In occasional Ur III administrative texts a reduplicated TI.2. a simple and an extended base are opposed. a m. 161. a-a pl. “vertically”. pl. a a-a and sg. raise” kur9 kur9 ku4-ku4 ku4-ku4 “enter” ∞ gar gar ∞ ∞gá-∞gá gá-∞gá ˆ “set” ge4 ge4 ge4-ge4 ge4-ge4 “return” na∞g na∞g na8-na8 na8-na8 “drink” .  12.TI stands for marû ti(-∞g).4. a pl. ¢.TI “he will certainly ([ha]) receive it ([b]) from him ([n-“i])”. a' pl. a a “úm “úm “úm “úm “give” gu7 gu7 gu7 gu7 “eat” sar sar sar sar “write” 75 This is the inalterable class. mainly occurs in the compound verb “u ti “to bring the hand close (to something)” = “to receive” (see 12.

. base is used for plural ‘subjects’. the opposition is between a simple ¢am†u and a reduplicated marû base.3. a a m. strictly speaking.4. . a variant of type 3. Krecher 1995.4.5.4). sùh sùh [sish] [sish] “confuse. different cuneograms are only used with gar/∞gá-∞gá. This type of reduplication where C1VC2(V)-C1VC2(V) turns into C1VC1C2. There is a heteronymic differentiation between ¢am†u and marû in sg. sg. there is a third heteronymic base for non-finite marû. this final consonant is omitted in reduplication. If the simple form ends in a conso­ nant. b b non-fin. recalls the nominal reduplication *deli-deli > dedli (see 5. do” This verb most probably forms a unique class and we have termed it “irregular”. c dug4 e e e di “speak. pl. a m. If the simple form ends in a vowel. note.4. but marû [e] also intruded in to ¢am†u pl. 99 f. Unfortunately. “vertically”. trouble” tuku4 tuku4 [tutk] [tutk] “tremble” ¢. it is reduplicated with­ out alteration. ¢. class 4. But we are unable to state why *zig-zig became zi-zi instead of *zizg. 12. the pl. Type 4 is. 12. b pl. Moreover. Although dug4/e is a transitive verb.7).4. however.76   In this class. ∞ In the other cases the morphological evidence has to be inferred from the behaviour of the suffixed particles or from non-orthodox spellings. a'+a" a'+a" For si-is-he [si(h)s(i)h-e] or tu-ut-ke [tu(k)t(u)k-e] see Falkenstein 1959 b. 173–77. this being another irregularity. sg. not ‘objects’ (see below 12.

place” is turned into the non-changing class 1: cf. German sitzen ¡ setzen. 61). for tìl : se12(SIG7) see Steinkeller 1979.). 55 with fn. 4. Exhaustive information on dug4/e/di is found in Attinger 1993 where also the astounding number of over 210 compound verbs is treated in detail. dwell” For gub. the uniform vocalization of (derived) D stem verbs in Akkadian and other Semitic languages. only) tìl se12(sig7) – – “live. OS ba-ug7-ge(-“) is found.4. in the case of two or more dead. mu-gub-ba-∞gu10 ma-angub-bu-u“ [ma-n-gub-e“] “they set before me my ‘standing lines’” Schooldays 6. Note: See Steinkeller 1979. Biggs proposed that DUG4 should occasion­ ally be read as ex (apud Steinkeller 1979. 8–11. But there is no proof for such an assumption. The same holds for ug5 “to kill”. ¢.6. see Krecher 1967. for the graphic difference between (OS) Ú” and BAD. 5 (with previous lit.: pl. 55 fn. some of the grids can only be partially filled out as yet. When someone died it was noted as ba-ú“. English sit ¡ set. ú“ – ug7 – “die” It is important to observe that the above verbs belong to class 6 only as long as they are intransitive. Cf. . Transitive gub “to put. division becomes relevant. etc. Civil and R.  77 M. 12. sg. In types 6–9 the “vertical” sg. In types 1–4 there was a “horizontal” ¢am†u : marû division of the grid. Unfortunately. b b gub “u4-(g)/su8(-g) gub “u4-(g)/su8(-g) “stand” (intr. a pl. a m. Note: A widespread (universal?) rule is that verbs which display morphological pecularities in their primary form become “regular” in derived forms. D.

above 12.4. the grid is divided both “horizontally” and “vertically”. 1–11 “Die pluralischen Verben für ‘gehen’ und ‘stehen’ im Sumerischen”. c d ∞gen [ere] du su8(-b) “go” The verb “to go” presents pecularities and irregularities in so many languages that there is no need to quote examples. object. as was supposed by Yoshikawa 1981. 12. For lah5 and lah4 see Steinkeller 1979. etc. Pl. ki-dam-A-NE-a-ti-ka ì-dú-ru-né-“a-àm [i-durun-e“-am] “they reside with A. the absolutive to which the plurality refers is the ‘object’. sg.’s wife” NG no.) and North American Indian languages (Athapascan. 57–60. 65 compared Caucasian (Georgian. b Here. is intransitive.4. . etc. what is more. With de6.7. m. However. sg. pl. is transitive. 59 f. 55 f.DU and DU represent the same evolution of spelling DU as with the pl. sg. The sg. c c tu“ durun dúr durun “sit” DU de6 lah5(DU. verbs implying a pl.TI = marû [ti∞g-e]. apart from being transitive.). a b pl.4. and there is a base for the plural of both ¢am†u and marû (formally related to m. Svan.78   12. of gub “to stand” (see 12.4. It is no surprise that “to go” holds the record in Sumerian for base variants and that. a m. ¢am†u DU. ¢. all four are heteronymic. Cf.DU) lah4(DU) DU tùmu lah5(DU.TU” = durunx. esp. de6 etc. there­ fore. 54–67.DU) lah4(DU) “carry away” ¢.?). Note also Krecher 1995: TU”. noted that OS TU”.8. 115). Note: Steinkeller 1979. has different bases for ¢am†u and marû.TU” stands for durunx (not *durun-durun.2 for Ur III TI. The decisive study of these variants is Krecher 1967. Whereas tu“ etc. [lah] may occur as a passive: ba-lah5 “were carried away” (Steinkeller 1979. 6.7). 214:41. Steinkeller 1979.) for pl. fn.

dam dumu-Ku-li. As in classes 6 and 7. 1633:11. The difference between this type of reduplicated base and the marû reduplication is transparent in the spellings gá-∞gá (marû) and ∞gar-∞gar. in non-orthodox spelling. the unaltered ¢am†u base can also be reduplicated for a variety of reasons: to denote different sorts of plurality or totality of the ‘subject’ or ‘object’. Steinkeller 1979. Note: Not “gána”. see. 12. 95 igi nam-bar-bar-re-en “you are not supposed to stare at every­ thing” Scribe and Son 31.). or to stress the durative or iterative character of the action (Edzard 1971.1–8.  e 79 [ere] is noted. ba-an-da-zàh-zàh-é“. 41:11'–13'. (but) the chil­ dren of Baba∞gu seized them all” NG no. distribution patterns as shown in 12. plurality refers to the absolutive case. ∞ This kind of “free reduplication” of the ¢am†u base may occur both in the ¢am†u and marû conjugation patterns: igi hé-mu-e-“i-bar-bar-re “may Utu—again and again—look on you” Enm. the ‘subject’ of the intransitive verb. 229. ì-im-e-re-“a “who had come here” UET 3. as transliterated in ZA 61. or general importance. strength.4. 32. for more forms. to stress its size. Wilcke 1988. 63 f. . : pl. A xiii 2. dumu-Ba-ba-∞gu10-ke4-ne(-r). A xviii 26. dumu-Ba-ba-∞gu10-ke4-ne mu-un-dab5-dab5-bé-é“ “the wife and chil­ dren of Kuli had all fled from the children of Baba∞gu. 226–32. S   (›AMˇU ) Apart from the ¢am†u : marû and sg. siki-udu-¢ganÜ-na-kam “u-a mi-ni(-n)-∞gar-∞gar “(instead of the whip) he put wool of ewes in the hand (of all the overseers)” Gudea Cyl. as e-re and e-re7(DU) (or ere).5. iku-zi-dam é“e ì(-n)-∞gar-∞gar “(Gudea) laid—along all sides—the measuring rope to (a square corresponding) exactly to an iku” Gudea Cyl.

24.).6. ZA 65 (1975) 188. 17). 16. (as e) 116 (var. see ZA 53. N    The form a verbal case takes when it is reduplicated cannot be estab­ lished beforehand. 118 (var.6. 12. ba-adba-ad in Susa MDP 57 no. and see above 12. na∞g ¡ ∞ na8-na8. It differs from one verb to the other. f ) VC-VC > VCCVC (loss of hiatus): ul4 ¡ ul-lu-ul “to hasten” ibid.4. bad-bad (”ulgi A 45 with var. i) C1VC2VC3 ¡ C1VC2-C1VC2VC3 (loss of the end of the second syllable in first base): zalag ¡ za-al-zu-le-g(a) = nuwwurum “illumi­ . 99 with fn. Sjöberg. tuku4 ¡ tu-ut-ke see above 12.2. kur9 ¡ ku4-ku4. e) Loss of consonant in second base: gen ¡ (*gen-gen) ge-en-gete [genge(n)-(e)d-e] “make firm” Innin “ag. 483 f. 200). 99 with fn. gá-∞gá instead of ∞gar-∞gar).2 b) Loss of final consonant: gar ¡ ∞gá-∞gá.4. of vowel in second base): bir-bir CT 15. B xv 7. (2) un∞ orthodox spelling: the scribe tried. c) Loss of final consonant in first base: bi-bi-z(é) < *biz-biz (Gudea Cyl. d) Assimilation of final consonant in first base to following initial consonant: bar6-bar6 ¡ UD (babbar). to render in syllabic script the actual pronunciation of the reduplicated form.80   12. ba(-ab)-ba-ar Proto-Ea 157 (MSL 14. We may generally distinguish between two ways of information: (1) standard orthography: repetition of the sign used to render the simple form or. sù-sù-lu-un = rußßunu “roaring” see CAD R 183 raßànu lex. 12. “u ba-ba-r(a-da) < BAR-BAR Gudea Cyl. to ge-en-ge-en).g.3. 51:37 (Hru“ka 1969. 38). la-la-ah < LAH4-LAH4.6. ul4-ul4). zu-lu-un ¡ zu-zu-lu-un.. 2 iv 20 (see / Falkenstein 1959 b. 22:8 / bi-ib-ri VS 2. h) C1VC2VC3 ¡ C1V-C1VC2VC3 (loss of second syllable in first base): ù-bi-GALAM-GALAMga-ga-la-am : gana utlellì “rise high (O Inanna)” TCL 6. g) C1VC2(V) ¡ C1VC1C2(V) (loss of consonant in first. see Å.1 a) Full reduplication: gi4-gi4. to the best of his ability. of a variant form (e. very rarely. A xxviii 11.

Forms of pattern (1) are rendered in Akkadian by both iprus and iparras. This distribution has led former grammarians to speak about (1) the “intransitive standard form” (“Normalform”) and (2a) “preterite” and (2b) “present-future” (see above.). The change of vowel in [zalzuleg(a)] is noteworthy and it is proba­ ble a phenomenon occurring much more frequently than may be guessed from standard spelling. see Å. but only much less frequently. 73 f. 1st 2nd 3rd 1st 2nd 3rd ba-zah-en (Ur III -èn) ba-zah-en ba-zah ba-zah-en-dè-en ba-zah-en-zé-en ba-zah-e“ (Ur III -é“) [ba-zah-en] “I run away” [ba-zah-en] [ba-zah] [ba-zah-enden] [ba-zah-enzen] [ba-zah-e“] .7. pl.7.1. Sjöberg. exhibiting absolutive person elements only. C As the Sumerian verb is essentially person-oriented it is appropriate to speak of its “conjugation” (or of “verbal inflection”). to ZALAG-ga) Innin “ag. Conjugation pattern 1: Intransitive (and passive) sg. For (2) there is one (2a) normally translated by iprus and one (2b) by iparras. pl. spelling only partly represents the full form because of a marked ten­ dency to neglect syllable-closing consonants in writing: Note the ubi­ quitous “u ba-ti for [“u banti] or the above-quoted OS ba-ug6-ge(-“) [ba-ug-e“]. In OS. Besides reduplication of the verbal base. 12. There are three patterns. sg. ZA 65 (1975) 194. 190 (with previous lit. 124 does not belong here according to Sjöberg. ibid. triplication and even quadruplication may be observed. (2) two pat­ terns for transitive verbs showing both ergative (“subject”) and abso­ lutive (“object”) person elements. p.  81 nate” (var. 12. Note: za-al-za-le-bi ibid. n.). 160. The three patterns may be reconstructed with relative certainty during and after Ur III. and to some degree still in Ur III. 235. pl. (1) one for intransitive (and passive) verbal forms. Attinger 1993. Cf. p. sg.

“I. pl. Was there originally only an opposition of “I” or “you” : “he. lugal-me-en “I am king”.7. This is easily explained by the fact that it has only one participant (absolute) while patterns 2a and 2b. Or this [e] may assimilate to a non-final vowel of the base: *ba“úm-mu-u“ [ba-“um-e“] “they were given” (example made up from corresponding active forms of pattern 2b). ba-ra-me “he certainly will not be” NG no..11. Assimilation of the vowel is not yet predictable for us: cf. 1st 2nd 3rd 1st 2nd 3rd ì-me-en ì-me-en ì-me(-àm) (OS -am6) ì-me-en-dè-en ì-me-en-zé-en ì-me-e“ (Ur III -é“) -me-en -me-en -àm (OS -am6) -me-en-dè-en -me-en-zé-en -me-e“ (Ur III -é“) [men] “I am” [men] [am] [menden] [menzen] [me“] In the majority of cases. ga-ba-húl-le-en-dè-en [ga-ba-hul-enden] “let us rejoice over it”. Conjugation pattern 1 is neutral as for tense or aspect (except when the base is extended by [e(d)]).g. ì-me-àm [i-me-a-am] “who is” NG no. The verb me The verb [me] “to be” and the copula [me] have conjugations which differ only in the 3rd sg. 6:2.. 18:34.. enden. ì-me-àm “he was” NG no. with their distinction of perfective (¢am†u based pattern) and imper­ fective (marû based pattern) are essentially characterized by the pres­ ence of two participants: absolutive and ergative. Note: The homophony—at least according to spelling—of 1st and 2nd person sg. 75:2. see 12. 70:9. pl. the king”. e“] may yield to the final vowel of the preceding base: ì-du-un [i-du(e)n] “I/you go”.1.82   The pattern may be extended by adding -e(d) to the base: [ba-zahed-en] “I will flee” in the oath ba-ra-ba-zah-e-dè-en “I swear I will not run away (another time)” (said by a captured runaway slave) (negative affirmative particle [bara]. not *[-hul-unden].. sg. enzen. 63:12. she. sg. [ed] here serves to express more precisely the temporal situation (future). 26:2. e. 12. For the free form cf.g. ì-me-“a-ke4-é“ [i-me-(e)“-a-(a)k-e“] .4). pl. it”? Phonetic pecularities: The [e] of [en. the bound form occurs. e.1. is striking. sg. where there is both a free and a bound form (copula).

. . followed by Falkenstein 1949. Indo-European. ¢am†u.7. pl. the suffixed ergative markers. A xxvi 15. and nam are normally used: za-a-kam [za(e)(a)k-am] “it is yours”. After the genitive particle [ak] and after [d] or [n].. It is unlikely that there was an original distribution of two forms: [am] after C and [m] after V because we find. Conjugation pattern 2a: Transitive Given its great similarity to pattern 1. because heteronymy in the cop­ ula conjugation is also found in other languages. -zu-um [zu-(a)m] “it is your (. 273–78. sg. more probable..e. For enclitic [am] derivation from *i-m(e) (or *a-m(e)). The free form is mainly used when [me] is understood as a “preterite” or “future” and when the form is nominalized by the particle [a].g. 72 ff. Attinger 1993. 312 f. probably after the hiatus originally present in [a’am] had disap­ peared. before pattern 2b. 1st 2nd 3rd 1st 2nd 3rd (person class only) ì-lá-en ì-lá-en ì-lá-e ì-lá-en-dè-en ì-lá-en-zé-en ì-lá-e-ne [i-laH-en] [i-laH-en] [i-laH-e] [i-laH-enden] [i-laH-enzen] [i-laH-ene] “I pay” The pattern offered here has been simplified because only one par­ ticipant has been noted. min-nam [min-am] “it is two”. The [a] of [am] may be superseded by a preceding vowel: -bi-im [bi-(a)m]” “it is its (. pl. . 12. sg. pl. perfective. . transliterated as am6. resulting in [âm]..)”. .. [am] was written by AN in OS.AN = àm arose. . the second participant. a reduced free form with prefixed particle [i] or [a] has been proposed: Poebel 1923. e.)”.2. A heteronymous form is. .)” Gudea Cyl. sg.  83 “pertaining to the fact that they are” = “because they are (. marû. 147. 84 f. has been dis­ regarded here (see below p. Thomsen 1984. From the frequent combination of the nominalizing particle [a] + copula [am]. the ‘object’ denoted by absolutive markers. e.). however. we describe pattern 2a. hé-àm.g. i. dam. the CVC cuneograms kam. imperfective. -a-am6 [a’am] the more recent (Ur III) spelling A.

or it may be assimilated to the non-final vowel of the base: -“úm-mu-un. there is the danger of a circular argument. is part of an autonomous marker of the conjugation pattern. only occur in verbs with a non-alternating base (cf. The system runs: sg. sg. with -te-∞ge26(GÁ) as marû base + [e]. enden. In verbs with differing ¢am†u : marû bases pattern 2a is linked to the marû base(s) (cf. also pl. -e“) by ergative (-e.) “to come/bring close (to something)”. How­ ever. -ene). place (something)”. 183 § 43. he gives”. 1st 2nd 3rd person 3rd non-person -(e)n-B(ase) -(e)n-B . with [e] super­ seded by the final vowel of the base. in case of è (¢am†u)/e(-d) (marû) “come/bring out”. -ge4-ge4 [gege]. in asserting that [e] is not part of the person elements. Here.)/te(-∞g) (m. some authors have posited. ba-tar-ra-a“ [ba-tar-e“] “they were split” Lugale 72. [e] and pl. or should be con­ sidered a separate “marû” marker so that -en of ì-lá-en [i-laH-en] would have to be segmentized as -e-n.n-B . sg. enzen. -è-dè [ed­ e] can only represent the marû base + additional. ad hoc. pattern 2a replaces absolutive (-Ø. and Jacobsen 1988. The same holds for the verb te/ti (¢. as with pattern 1. Phonetic details: As in pattern 1.4). [e] and 3rd pl. Put another way round. It is probable that 3rd sg. -“úm-mu [“um-en]. [ene] have the same ori­ gin as the nominal ergative particles sg. -“úm-mune [“um-enden]. we would have to ask whether in 3rd sg.1). joined Yoshikawa 1974. above 12. the marû base to be [te∞ga]. Such a “marû” [e] would. [“um-e] “I/you give. [e]. sg. 180. assimilation cannot be predicted. -“úm-mu-un-dè-en. [ene]. then.4. [“um-ene]. sg. 18. or whether [gege] would have been self-sufficient to indicate 3rd p. the [e] of [en. e. [gege] would represent *gege + e. ene] may yield to the final vowel of the preceding base: ì-∞gá-∞gá-ne [i-∞ga∞ga-(e)ne] “they set. for the 3rd p. autonomous. Since the great majority of—reduplicated or unreduplicated—marû bases end in a vowel.84   Pattern 2a is formally identical with pattern 1 in the 1st and 2nd persons. if graphically unrealized—absolutive elements for the person or non-person class ‘objects’. Note: Krecher 1995. Verbal forms of conjugation pattern 2a normally express—or at least imply.b-B . 12. Discussion has arisen about whether the [e] of [en] etc. But.

-(e)n-. (4) kings’. ha-ra-ab-“úm-mu [ha-(e)ra-b-“um-e] “let him give it to you”.before the base often alternates with -e..g. -n-. 3rd person d 85 ? ? -ne-B En-líl-le im-ma-“i-in-gi4-gi4 [imma-“i-(e)n-gigi] “Enlil sends me back (against the rebel lands)” Römer 1969. 2nd. forms of the absolutive marker are still unknown. (5) it’s. 163 ff. (6) he’s (has). however. ITT 1.. 86. -(e)n. more references for 2nd sg. distinguished between determinate action implying an object (e.(Attinger 1985. [For Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pat­ tern 2a see p. The fact that the absolutive markers for the 1st. If they existed they may have been replaced over time by periphras­ tic expressions by means of the free forms of the personal pronoun (see 9. “he fights”).. i. and 3rd sg. more references for 1st sg.7. never been an obstacle to practical under­ standing.1100:16.g.e. may have been homophonous (they are so at least in our Latin transliteration). and their restitution depends entirely on our (subjective) understanding of the context. since Michalowski 1980. It is still open to research whether Sumerian. as is shown by English multifunctional [s] in (1) wings. 1st pl. 150–52) where only conjugation . in the transitive verb. (3) king’s.1).) to you”. Final note to 12.  pl. the opinion has been voiced that Sumerian was a language with “split ergativ­ ity” (accepted by Attinger 1993. 2nd pl.2. 167–75. The coexistence of homophonous mor­ phemes has. person class. “me” in Attinger 1993. Ha-ba(-n)-zi-zi [ha-ba-(e)n-zizi] “May (the personal god) raise you here” (= PN) Limet 1968.: We have described conjugation pattern 2a in terms of ergativity although. “you” see Attinger 1985. Note: Until—and partly including—Ur III the consonants representing the abso­ lutive elements are disregarded in spelling (because of their position at the end of a closed syllable). “he fights an enemy”) and indeterminate action not implying an object (e. passim). can hardly have contributed much to clarity. 163–67. 298:109 (see Attinger 1985. The 1st and 2nd pl. 166). 311 f. ha-mu-ra-ne-“úm-mu [ha-mu-(e)ra-ne-“um-e] “let him give them (pers. -(e)n-.] In literary texts of OB date. (2) sings.

1st pl. -n-B-en -n-B-en -n-B-e -n-B-e -n-B-enden -n-B-enzen -n-B-ene 3rd sg. -(e)n-B-en -(e)n-B-en -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-enzen -(e)n-B-ene -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-e -(e)n-B-enden -(e)n-B-ene 3rd sg. 3rd sg. 2nd pl. p. 2nd sg. p. 2nd pl. .   -(e)n-B-en (?) -(e)n-B-en (?) -(e)n-B-e (?) -(e)n-B-e (?) ? ? -(e)n-B-ene (?) Crossed-out boxes would imply reflexive forms (I-myself. non-p.86 Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pattern 2a absolutive 1st sg. -b-B-en -b-B-en -b-B-e -b-B-e -b-B-enden -b-B-enzen -b-B-ene 1st pl. Boxes with a question mark refer to forms where the absolutive element cannot be safely stated or reconstructed.) which the Sumerian verbal system does not express. etc. 3rd pl. 2nd sg. p. 3rd sg. non-p. p. ergative 1st sg. ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? 3rd pl.

217 § 139a.7. 2: “Vielleicht ist das Personenzeichen der 1. thus suggesting iden­ tity with the 2nd sg. 1st 2nd 3rd p.“to you” as [er-a/e] instead of [e-r-a/e]. sg. 175] of a mark zero”. Moreover. The question will be discussed in more detail in 12. Note to 1st sg. 144. but this may be a late analogy formed on the identity of the absolutive/ergative markers [en] of 1st and 2nd sg. 12. in such cases as á mu-e-da-a-á∞g [mu-e+da-e-a∞g] “you (moved the arm with me =) ordered me” one would have expected [*r] to be indicated before the vowel of á∞g. -e.and -b. while pattern 2a as well as the imperative and cohortative would have to be considered as functioning on a nominative-accusative level. sg. sg. Before Krecher. sg.had been explained as a Hiatustilger (the [r] being borrowed from the dative case particle [ra]). pl. immediately precede the verbal base (in its ¢am†u variant if such exists). ps.  87 pattern 2b (¢am†u) would be termed ergative. -n. Extensive discussion by Attinger 1993. Conjugation pattern 2b: Transitive sg. proposed identifying the erga­ tive marker as [e(r)] because he analysed the dative and directive elements -e-ra.3.: The ergative marker can hardly have been zero. Krecher’s argument clearly is circular. -(V-)lá -e-lá -n-lá -b-lá -lá-enden (see note) -e-lá-enzen (see note) -n-lá-e“ In pattern 2b the ergative markers for sg.: Krecher 1985.4.7. but its original vocalic quality can no longer be ascertained. 159 fn.and -e-re. Note to 2nd sg. pl. [e]. ps. He is followed by Attinger 1993. Note Falkenstein 1949.appear identical with the absolutive markers of pattern 2a. sg. -r. . preferred “Poebel’s first suggestion [1923. Jacobsen 1988. mit dem der 2. and 3rd pl. 1st 2nd 3rd p. pl. without a definite result. 3rd p. 217–20 and passim. 3rd non-p. 198. Some rare OB verbal forms have [e] before the base. Now since *[er] only occurs between vowels and would otherwise have to be recon­ structed as *[e(r)].identisch”.

-B-en -B-en -B-Ø -B-Ø -B-enden -B-enzen -B-e“(?) These elements are identical with the absolutive markers of conjuga­ tion pattern 1.: A form -e-ene-B was offered by Thomsen 1984. .) and [en] (abs. [(e)ne] to sg. the Enlil­ bàni (of Isin) ‘key text’ HSM 1384 has in line 11 nu-mu-¢eÜ-“[úmm]u-un-zé-en [nu-mu-e-“um-e-nzen] “you did not give (it)” (Edzard 1976. 160 fn. pl. = niqtil ) or Chinese wo-men “we” = “I” + pl.)”. There was no pluralis maiestatis (“we” for “I”). pl. Note: For exceptions cf. etc. cannot be excluded. sg. . Since one ‘slot’ can only receive either an ergative or an absolutive marker. because a form *[i-n-tud-e“-en] would require to fill one ‘slot’ with both [e“] (erg. It is probably just by chance that very few texts record the speech of more than one person. marker as (sg. has not yet been demonstrated.. Falkenstein 1949.) [(V)] + . sg. formation by analogy with 2nd pl.) [e] + . We hesitate to posit the ergative 1st pl.) beat me (abs. 165). It looks like a ‘late’ suppletive form. However. sg. would such a phrase have to be expressed differently? The same applies to forms such as *“we beat you (pl. . 3rd pl. 1959. However. 1st 2nd 3rd p.)” cannot be transformed into “they beat me”. 1st 2nd 3rd p. Attinger 1993.2). The form -lá-enden is rarely attested in OB. -enzen. niqtlu (1st sg. . 1st sg. 43). Thomsen 1984. 3rd non-p. 2. 22. pl. 40. Maghrebi (and Maltese) Arabic 1st pl. (sg. 148 (but with caution in fn. 148. 176. Note to 2nd pl.88   Note to 1st pl. cf. (cf. . Therefore. 160. postulated by Poebel 1923. -enden because cross-linguistically “we” is rarely expressed as “I” + pl. and in such cases it is ergative that prevails: in-túddè-en [i-n-tud-en] “he (erg. [e].: A form -*me-lá. in cases where both might be wanted a selection had to be made. above 5. mechanically appending pl. Letters and depositions of witnesses always happen to be in the sg. The absolutive markers of conjugation pattern 2b are placed after the verbal base in suffix rank 2 (after [ed]): sg. “they beat us”.). 173 n.

p. ? ? ? ? – – –   89 Crossed-out boxes would imply reflexive forms (I-myself etc. ergative 1st sg. 3rd sg. 1st pl. 3rd sg. – – – – – – – 3rd sg. Boxes with a question mark refer to forms where the absolutive marker cannot be securely reconstructed. non-p. p. p. 2nd sg. so that one must have been selected at the expense of the other. 2nd pl. 2nd sg. – – – – – – – 1st pl. non-p. 3rd pl. 2nd pl.) which the pattern does not serve. -V-B-enzen -e-B-enden -n-B-enden -b-B-enden – – -n-B-enzen -b-B-enzen – – 3rd pl. .Table of absolutive-ergative combinations in conjugation pattern 2b absolutive 1st sg. -V-B-en -e-B-en -n-B-en -b-B-en – – -n-B-en -b-B-en – – 3rd sg. Boxes with a dash refer to forms where one ‘slot’ would have to be filled by two elements at the same time. p.

. In the latter case only part of the system is ergative (with ergative and absolutive cases and ergative behaviour of the verbal inflexion) whereas another part of the system has a nominative-accusative construction. On “split ergativity” cf. and Attinger 1993.s ì-nú-dè. a-re-e“ dug4-ge-e“ “they praised” Ur-Ninurta B 28. . or the noun may go on exhibiting ergative and absolutive case while only the verbal system suffers the “split”. The nominal system is definitely ergative. Dixon 1994. W. 80 (var.90   12.s mu-un-e11-[. Was Sumerian a language with “split ergativity”? The question has been raised whether Sumerian was a fully-fledged ergative language (with both the nominal and the verbal inflections displaying definite ergative characteristics) or whether it belonged— at least from a given period onward—to the type of language defined by “split ergativity”. èn-“è nú-dè-en [nud-en] “how long (O Gilgame“ . 12.5. e11-dè-en) “today I am about to descend to the (Foreign Country =) Netherworld” Inana’s Descent 32. 150–52. 70–110: “Types of split system”. . 2a and 2b. .7.7. Prefixless finite verbal forms Old Babylonian literary texts occasionally offer prefixless verbal forms which still have to be considered finite because their suffixes evi­ dently represent ergative or absolutive elements of conjugation pat­ tern 1. A 76. R.4. Thomsen 1984.]. Examples known as of 1965 have been collected and commented on by Römer 1965. define Sumerian as a language with “split ergativity”. 220–23. “Split ergativity” may affect both the nominal a n d the verbal syntax.) will you (lie =) sleep?’’ Gilg. and only during the OB period some scribes lost the notion of when to apply a “subject case” in -e and when not—an evolution that happened in the final stage of the development of the Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area. . The motivation for these forms is still unknown nor do we know how far they were productive. Huw. u4-da kur-“è àm-e11-dè-en-na (var. ì-nú-na). Michalowski 1980 passim. har-ra-an lú-du-ù-bi nu-gi4-gi4-dè “à-zu a-gim túm-mu-un “how did your heart (bring =) move you to (take) the road whose trav­ eller will not return?” Inana’s Descent 84. 51. M.

Either way. We do not have to re-name them “nominative” and “accusative” while the nominal subject of the phrase. [n] indicates the ergative. are claimed to show non-ergative. .  91 For the verb. is in the ergative case.3). both ergative [-e] and absolutive [n] are noted in their individual “slots”.11. Here. marû). the “slot” for absolutive [ b] before the base. However. (b) ses-gal-e dumu-é-dub-ba-a in-túd-dè [i-n-tud-e] “the school overseer will hit the pupil”. nominative-accusative. is implied in the imper­ ative itself. This explanation may be viewed as more plausible than the over­ all proposal to “split” conjugation patterns 2a (marû) and 2b (¢am†u.13) and cohortative (see 12. before the base [tud]. plus imperative and cohortative) into individual patterns. Here the ergative of the person who is commanded to speak. a “slot” has become free for absolutive [ b] “it”. the imperative (see 12. Do we have to see conjugation pattern 2a (marû) as evidence for “split ergativity” at all? Compare two examples: (a) ses-gal-e dumu-é-dub-ba-a in-túd [i-n-tud] “the (big brother =) school overseer hit the pupil”. Here again. a “slot” has become open for absolutive [ b] “it”. construction is equally claimed by the authors quoted for conjugation pattern 2a (transitive. conjugation pattern 2b (Black’s “standard construc­ tion”. because the cohorta­ tive particle [ga] automatically implies a 1st person subject. nominative-accusative. In forms like in-na-an-du11 [i-na-n-dug] “he (erg. see 12. there is no “slot” for the absolutive which would equally be [n]. A non-ergative. the question of “split ergativity” does not seem to be of any importance in Sumerian.) said (it) to him” “it” has to be understood. Here. The same holds true for the cohortative ga-na-ab-dug4 [ga-na-bdug] “let me tell it to him/her”. being already filled by ergative [n]. ses-gal-e. both using the ¢am†u base. du11-ga-na-ab [dug-a-na-b] “say it to him/her”. one nominative-accusative and the other ergative-absolutive. construction.3) is ergative.

[mda]. See 12. -na. with regard to the frequent non-notation of syllable-closing conso­ nants. separation. For each dimensional indicator there exists a variant with prefixed [m(u)]. 434 for derivation of the Akkadian ventive from common-Semitic affirmative (energetic) [an]. where pl. There is no need. we sometimes cannot know whether -da. 17.. Many dimensional indicators can be segmentized into a pronom­ inal head and a case element. at the beginning of the Sumero-Akkadian areal interaction. Following the gram­ matical terminology of Akkadian. (-)ba. or -(e)ne-da [(e)ne-da] “with them”. 400 f. it should be borne in mind that the Akkadian ventive marker is (historically) [aC] (before -k. and similarly for -“i-.was meant for [nda]. Rather.. p.: (-)V-m-ma-. Their main and original—but no longer exclusive—function is to indicate arrest and movement. to be kept separate from the [m] of the ventive final form -am. has. -ta-. -“um. 270–80 with lit. Note: The [m] of the Akkadian dative pronouns -kum. Foxvog 1974 and 1975. -n. -nim. company and related notions.8. proposed “Sumerian ventive” for the m-forms.: (-)mu-na-. The dimensional indi­ cators are arranged in a string within which the individual elements have their fixed and inalterable rank.4.8.92   12. e. with fn. If one wonders about the coincidence of both languages using an [m] element for ventive. in Akkadian. esp.g. D  The Sumerian verb is characterized by a series of up to six prefixed particles. person-class [n] and the (dative-)locative element [a].3 for the question whether dimensional indicators could also occur without reference to a person. [bda].4–9) to suffixed nouns in the same phrase.g. -“ ).2.. see Attinger 1993. [na] “for. Note to -da-: Due to the uncertainties of Sumerian spelling. consist­ ing of 3rd sg. to construe ad hoc assimilation rules like “am-kum” > akkum etc. . etc. the ventive (as opposed to affirmative) function of Akkadian [aC/am] may have been a re-interpretation of this morpheme under Sumerian influence. These indicators frequently correspond to case particles 4–9 (see 5. most probably. [(e)ne] is combined with comitative [da]. and it is only [am] before -m or in word-final position. See Pedersén 1989. 270. etc. e. or [da] alone. to him/her”. direction.

In many cases where [i] is shown. In view of our general reluctance to propose a clear phonemic distinction between Sumerian [e] and [i]. 2nd sg. because 1st person would automatically require ventive. . person mea (58) mini (62) mmini (59) – – – mueda (38) (39) munea (33) muneda (40) (53) *muri-. Boxes left blank reflect forms that the system did not allow.). mma non-person 1st pl. . 3rd sg. ba non-person 2nd pl. person 3rd sg. 1–62) below. 1 sg. . 3rd pl. 2 nd rd Comitative eda nda bda (6) (7) (8) (9) (e)neda (10) Ablative (11) nta bta ra (12) (13) (14) (15) Terminative (e)“i n“i b“i (16) (17) (18) (19) (e)ne“i (20) Directive (e)ri (21) ni (22) bi (23) (24) (e)ne (25) Locative 2 – – ni (26) – – sg. The grids have a horizontal division by cases and a vertical division by person. person (e)nea (5) Dimensional indicators: ventive Dative-loc.(60) -enzen mune“i (54) mune (61) Note: Variants are not indicated. or pl. 2nd pl. see the individual paragraphs (ns. Grid 1 (non-ventive) has no entries for the 1st (sg. but have been numbered for the sake of completeness.  93 It seems practical to arrange the dimensional indicators in numbered grids and to refer to the numbers for reference. 3rd pl. [e] may be preferable. we have left NI as ni and RI as ri instead of né and re. We will present a grid each for “non-ventive” and “ventive” indicators. Dimensional indicators non-ventive Dative-loc. person st Comitative muda (34) Ablative (41) (42) (43) mta (44) mmara (45) (46) (47) Terminative mu“i Directive (55) (56) (57) Locative 2 – – – ma (27) (48) mu mura (28) muna (29) (30) (31) (32) mueda (35) munda (36) mda (37) mue“i (49) muri mun“i (50) muni m“i (51) mmi mmeri (52) me 3rd sg. era na (1) (2) (3) (4) 3 sg.

2. na-e-a [na-b-e-a] “what you will say to him/her (is this)”..see (29).1.g. e-na(-n)-“úm “he/she gave (it) to him/her”. but non-ventive e-na-. OS texts from ”uruppag (Fàra) and Abu Íalàbì¢ as well as Sumerograms used in Ebla lexical and administrative texts use both BA (ba) and GÁ (ba4) as verbal prefixes in a distribution still imper­ fectly understood.8. For mu-na. Note: It is interesting to note.see (28) Note: [era] has mostly been analysed as [e] + [a] with [r] as a Hiatustilger. 77–82 (monolingual and bilingual lexical entries ba-B) and 83–87 (monol.has no connection with a locative element /a/”. [na]: “to him/her” is the most easily predictable dimensional indicator when the phrase contains a noun in the dative (with case particle -ra). 287. that the verbal-base [’e] turned into [e]. Non-ventive indicators (1–26) 12.2.(as well as bí-) as “conjugation prefixes”. Note: See d’Agostino 1990. e.7. For mu-ra. and Thomsen 1984. 176 ff.and ma-ra.3 [ba] : [ba] is treated here on the assumption that it is com­ posed of [b] and [a].“to him/her” (12. 12. entries ba4-B) with no overlap (with the seeming exception of ba-DU and ba4-DU where we should possibly reckon on two different readings of DU). 32 II i 15. diachronically.3.8. 12. lex. They do not seem to be mere graphic variants: “u ba4-ti “he received” has no correspondence “u *ba-ti and ge“ ba­ ∞ tuku “he heard” is not paralleled by ∞ge“ *ba4-tuku.8. OS let­ ter opening formula.1. Doubts have been raised about this interpretation. ù-na-a-du11 [u-na-e-dug] “(after you said to him/her =) please say to him/her” (polite imperative.94   12. StPohl 10.11. the latter no longer causing a hiatus between absolutive [b] and the base.2). Note: Spelling does not allow us to decide whether [era] was preceded by the indicator [e.. See the discussion above.9). ì-na.8. [era. in accordance with the general pattern of non­ ventive and ventive dimensional indicators. and bil. by M. treats ba. Civil apud Postgate 1974. note to 2nd sg. (V)ra]: e-ra-an-du11 “he told (it) to you” Inanna/Enki. see 12. 20 fn. 11: “(while) the prefix ba.2) rather favours a positive answer. 12. i] (12. written na-ab-bé-a from Ur III on.1.8. ga-ra-ab-“úm “let me give it to you”. ki a-ra-du11-ga : a“ar aqbûkum “where I told you” MSL 5. See also Krebernik 1998. 196:12.1. .1.1.

ba-na-ge(-n) “(someone/something) was confirmed/awarded to him” NG III p.4: A dative “to you (pl. Edzard 1976.  95 At present. being disconnected from dimensional notation. . where non-ventive [ba] is in clear opposition with ventive [mu]. however. Apart from indicating locative proper. in principle.. The origin of this usage is not entirely clear.5 [(e)nea]: hé-ne-ab-“úm-mu [he-(e)ne-a-b-“um-e] “let him give it (= the rent for the hired boat) to them (= PN1 and PN2)” TCS 1 no. ki-a-na∞g-e ha-ba-gub “let (the stela) stand at the libation place” Gudea Stat. 12. ba-na. 12. e.8. 169 f. has no counterpart in ventive [mma] (30). picking up something here and taking it there. 135:7. tried to derive it from ocurrences such as ba-an-“úm [ba-n“um] “he (gave him to =) ordered him to (do something)” where without ergative [n] a neutral form ba-“úm “he was given to (. the diachronic evidence cannot be said to support a hypothesis that the two spellings go back to two different morphemes. not linked to a specific goal (person. . Note the frequent variants in Ur III year formula: mu-un-hul “he destroyed” and ba-hul “(a city) was destroyed”. no.. [ba] may also. cf. Note the contrast between ba(-n)-de6 “he carried (it) away” and mu(-n)-túm “he brought (it) here”. e. [bax] and [bay]. the ventive function of [mu] is general.. For general discussion see Attinger 1993. Note: In the second example.)” is not attested as a dimensional indicator. non-person). with verbs of motion.is attested when [ ba] denotes passive. Note: The compound verb “u ti offers a typical example of the frozen use of a directive indicator.1. first attested in Ur III.g. 204 and 280–84. 114. denote moving away. Passive [ba]. locative [ba] and dative [na] are found in complementary distribution. A special function of [ ba]. “u ba(-n)-ti “he (brought the hand to it =) received (it)”. B vii 55.g. Although.1. 365:6 (Ur III).8. . is to denote pas­ sive voice. More research on the origin of passive [ba] is. very desir­ able.)” would have arisen.

*gíndi“-e. 309 r. Frayne. Note: bala. indicator [i/e].1. . with non-person class [b-e] serving in lieu of person class 3rd pl.8.9: I cannot offer an example for “with you” (pl.8. 3 kor of barley . 12. Note: Before [ni]. and so did Sollberger 1961. . i.10 [(e)neda]: e-ne-da-tu“ “he is residing with them” Nik. 12. di in-da-tuku-àm “it is that he had a lawsuit with him” PBS 13.6. 12. non-ventive. see (26) for Ukg.see (35). so I affirm. -e-da. 20. But 1:gín:e. It is impossible to decide whether or not [(e)neda] was preceded by the neutral. 12.. DP 621 iii 1 e-PI-∞gál “it is with them”.1. . 1–4. 32:3.1.“with them” in OS ªGirsu. kù-babbar 1:gín:e hé-eb-da-sa10 [he-bda(-n)-sa] “when he ruled as a king. 39:233. .cf. people will spread (with you =) during your reign” SKIZ 211:55. 12.. D. -ka-ni is virtually in the comitative.1. Poebel 1931. 34:1. preferred [(e)ne-da]. though with a question mark. resum­ ing the question in AfO 18 (1957/58) 94 (d) f.see (36). for more examples.in 12. mu-ù-da. ba-ra-ba-a-da-gi4-gi4-dè(-n) [bara-ba-eda-gigi-(e)d-en] “I will not. and see Poebel 1931. .). Falkenstein. one shekel of silver bought. . . 454 ff.8. can only be the subject (ergative) of the sentence.8. 545. each” Sin-kà“id 8:13–22 (and see 10:13–20. etc. [da] may be assimilated to -dì(TI)-. For -b-da.1. suggested that -PI. cost one shekel of silver”. 16 fnn. RIME 4. 208:14. 4. translated “3 gur of barley . Cf.. I swear it.8 [bda]: bala-nam-lugal-la-ka-ni 3 “e:gur-ta . come back (with =) against you (with a claim)” Genava NS 8 (1960) 306 no.). .8. [(e)ne].8. .6 [eda]: ù∞g e-da-lu ù∞g e-da-da∞gal “people will multiply.1. For mu-e-da-. 16–19. see RIME 4.might have been an allo­ graph for -be(da).7 [nda]: in-da-∞gál-àm “(PN’s tablet) is (with =) at the dispo­ sition (of PN2)” NG no.e. For mu-un-da.96   The dative “to them” is more frequently replaced by the direc­ tive [(e)ne] (25)..

NG I 84 f. Falkenstein 1949. see (47).as [bta] has not been maintained since Postgate 1974. see Limet 1968. however.14: No example available. 12. [b]). Note: J. it varies with simple ba-zal “had ended” and ba-ra-zal (for [ra] see below).1. [ra]. 12. “Excursus: the verb sa10 and the noun (níg-)sám”.or -mta.or -ra. explain the phonetic situation. . 63–66) incomplete spelling conceals from us whether -ta-. In the frequent OS PN E-ta-è (Struve 1984.1.1. Attinger 1993. Falkenstein 1939. NG III 152 f.8. 97 12.1. . Falkenstein.” Ent.have been inserted into the frozen ba-zal.e.15: No non-ventive example available. 438 who. A vii 3. to . N. 53: 8' (cf. á-sàg .8.(44) is intended. id.(p. . 17 f. Falkenstein’s pro­ posal to explain ba-ta.8. . This observation does not. an exception may be found in the Ur III PN In-ta-è-a “Having come out from her” (i. é-ta ha-ba-ra-è “the asakku (demon) definitely left the house”.. however.  12. Steinkeller 1989. whether it refer to the real mother of the child or to the protective deity).8.13 [bta]. Note: -(b)ta. iti-ta u4-n-kam ba-ta-zal “the nth of the month had ended” is a frequent note at the end of Ur III administrative documents. prefers to see -nta.8.12 [nta]: Although ablative is generally incompatible with person class. ní-te-ne-ne ba-ra-an-sa10-a“ [ba-ra-n-sa-(e)“] “They sold themselves” TMH NF 1/2.1. Postgate suggests in-ta “from the stalk” (i“innu) (referring to the ancestors). and the—pleonastic?—form ba-ta-ra-zal. 180–94. [ra]: íb-ta-ni(-n)-è “he let (the boundary ditch) go out from . showed that the two morphemes are in contemporary distribution: [ta] follows a consonant and [ra] a vowel. -bta.16 [(e)“i]: “u ba-a-“i-íb-TI [“u ba-e“i-b-te∞g-e] “he will receive it from you” Gudea Cyl.. for [t] and [r] (at least in our Latin transliteration) do not form a pair. 256–58.as a vari­ ant of -bta. 12. [ta] has a variant [ra] which looks out of place in a beautifully reg­ ular system. 5.). 79 ad 237).8.1. from the mother’s womb. . . 28 ii 3. therefore. 153–62. cf. being incompatible with a consonantal pronominal element ([n]. 12.11: No example available. 215 fn. .

comparable to [ba] (3) with its secondary function of a passive indicator. The complex of directive and locative 2.8.base Moreover. 234–47 and. Both person and non-person class 3rd sg. Note: in.1.base -ni -Vn X .has been taken over mechanically from the much more fre­ quent sg. cf. B ix 18.base Ø . 1) saw to it that he (subj. 41:6.17 [n“i]: in-“i(-n)-sa10 “(purchaser) bought (object) from (seller)” Falkenstein. The action of buying is made “in the direction” of the seller. person class [ni] (22) and non-person class [bi] (23) also developed a secondary function: they may denote a causative where [ni] or [bi] are turned—at least in our view—into a secondary ‘subject’. It has been exhaustively dealt with by Wilcke 1988.1. 1–49. 2) ate (it)” = “he made him eat (it)”. igi na-“i-bar-re [na-b“i-bar-e] “let him not look at it” Gudea Stat.8. 12.98   12. Attinger 1993. 12.X . 27:6.(see 17). 215–53. in-“i. 3rd sg. NG III 153 b.of in-ne. is the most difficult in the system of directional indicators.8. 12. We owe to Wilcke the decisive breakthrough.19: No example available to me.21–26.1.20 [(e)ne“i]: in-ne-“i-sa10 [i-(e)ne“i-n-sa] “(purchaser) bought (object) from (sellers)” UET 3.1. [ni] (22) and [bi] (23) as well as locative 2 [ni] (26) have variants [n] and [b] which might easily be confused with the absolutive or ergative elements [n] and [b]. If we acknowledge the existence of a Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area (see 17) we cannot help seeing a correlation between Sumerian [ni].Ø . Here.1.18 [b“i]: igi-zi ba-“i-bar [ba-b“i-n-bar] “he looked friendlily at it”.base Vb . [“]. [“a]. by Zólyomi 1999 (as well as B. closest to the absolu- tive/ergative elements or directly before the base. the parallel with [”] in the causative formation of the Akkadian verb leaps to the eye: u“àkil“u “he (subj. Jagersma apud Zólyomi).8.8. [bi] and the Akkadian causative markers [“u]. These variants occur only in a position immediately before the base: bi . . most recently. 12.

AfO 23 (1974) 84–87. the most frequent opener of Sumerian verbal forms. however.see (57). stood before a syllable with a vowel close to the opening . p.[BI] and bí. ∞gar-ma-ni-ib: “u“kinam “have (it) set to me”. see C. the Sumerian of the causative sections . In Pre-Sargonic Girsu. OBGT VI 19 ∞gar-ma-ab: “uknam “set (it) to me”. and V2 if there is Vb.[NE] which Kramer 1936. See.1.22 [ni]: (PN1-e PN2-ra object) al ì-ni(-n)-du[g4] “(PN1) demanded (object) [to =] from PN2” NRVN 1. 247:3. for Krecher’s interpretation of [er] as an indissoluble unit. Black 1991. 88). explained by way of regressive “vowel harmony”: V1 occurs if the following syllable contains Va. 12. Wilcke. generally.21 [(e)ri]: dUtu-eri(URU)-è “the Sungod has risen next to you” (PN). 78.8. is complicated and awkward. [ni] (26). MSL 4. is secondary in the system and a deviant of the directive set (21–25). for ventive mu-ni. nam ge4-rí-íb-tarar nam-dùg gú-mu-rí-íb-tarar “let me make a firm promise for you. e. We can only say that the Babylonian grammarians seem to have tried to force Sumerian into a straight jacket by devising un-idiomatic forms which would correspond to those of the complex and productive causative sys­ tem possessed by their own language”. following Poebel 1931. see. ∞gá-e ga-ri-ib-zu-zu “let me make it all known to you” TMH NF 3. 1:10. 79 ff. there was an orthographic distribution of bé.. Black.  99 Note: The bilingual grammatical lists abound in ‘equations’ between a Sumerian verbal form with -ni.1. bé-.. See the discussion below.or bi. above [era] (1). according to Poebel. 12.and an Akkadian ”-form. Note: [eri] may be analyzed as e+r-i/e with -r. .8. let me make a benevolent firm promise on your behalf ” ”ulgi D 384 f. 12. Note: In OS. or whether it is original to the system and there was per­ haps homography (not necessarily homophony) between [nia] (22) and [nib] (26).as a Hiatustilger. next to [mu] and [ba]. and ibid. Cf. that “All in all.23 [bi]: [bi] is. 27–35. . 34. Note: The contrast of non-ventive [eri] and ventive [muri] implies growing intensity. and passim. I cannot offer non-ventive examples for person class [ni] in causative function.g. admits. see also Sollberger 1952. túg ì-ni-in-dul “he draped a garment over her” ZA 55 (1962) 70. It is uncertain whether the directional indicator for locative 2.1.8. URU = eri occurs as one of the extremely rare bisyllabic syl­ labograms. (Akkadian gloss lu-uq !-bi-ki ). 25:13 f. (Klein 1981.

153:4 (Zólyomi.29. 1–2. See 5. “u-na ì-ni(-n)-ge4 “he delivered (object) (into her hand =) to her” AWL 178 v 4–5 (see Zólyomi 1999.4. Note: Here again.. eri-ni Ni∞ginki-“è ka-Ni∞ginki-ke4 má bí(-n)-ús “(Gudea. . For the shortened form [b] preceded by [i] or [a] note: ki“ib-PN2 . nam tar governs the directive. A iv 4. xxiii 22 f. . OS ªGirsu spelling usage should. 249:74). Still. Note: This example is especially instructive because it shows that ergative (Gudea) and directive (harbour) may co-occur as two cases marked by [e]. Cf.9. Eriduki-gin7 ki-sikil-la bí(-n)-§rú “(Gudea) built (the temple) on a place as pure as Eridu” (or: “pure as if (in) Eridu) Gudea Stat. 18 r. . probably be seen as an isolated phenomenon which realized in writing peculiarities that were perhaps more widely present in the spoken language. ì-íb-∞gál “there is (. 12. It goes without saying that we often risk a circular argument based on our Latin transliteration. on the field)” CT 7. . along the lines described by Poebel and Kramer. if we applied such a phenomenon not to the language as a whole. and bí. as in (21). bé-ra. more or less. -a(k) íb-ra [i-b(i)-ra] “(instead of PN’s seal) PN2’s seal has been (struck =) rolled (on the document)” ITT 2/1. “u-a bí(-n)-ge4 “he (returned into the hand =) delivered (objects)” AWL 81 v 3 f.100   grade of [a]. on his way) to her city N. but only to a very restricted sector. but not into.). 231:26 f. B iv 8–9. 2 (Zólyomi 1999. ibid.” Gudea Cyl. Note: The directive expresses motion close to. 3470 r. bí-ri. something. (let the boat come close =) moored the boat at the harbour of N. “à-kù-ge bí(-n)-pà “(the lord Ningirsu called =) chose (Gudea) unto his pure mind” Gudea Cyl. there is some chance that the distributional pattern worked. 16. Be that it as it may. Cf.before a syllable with a vowel close to the opening grade of [i]: e. Note: The directive is used to denote immediate contact of the seal with the sur­ face of the tablet. An-né Ki-en-gi-ra nam bí-in-tarar “An made a firm promise to Sumer” ”ulgi F 30 (see Zólyomi 1999. although “vowel harmony” is ubiquitous in languages of the agglutinative type. therefore.g. ì-in-∞gál “there is (in the basket)” UET 3. with locative 2). See also below. it would completely upset our ideas about Sumerian phonetics. 227:15). . A.

The per­ son caused to act (“akkanakkum) has to stand in a dimensional case.” has no causative implication at all and cannot serve as an argument. While the sg. instead of -na-. 12. -na-. the ergative sub­ ject of the one who feeds is implied in the verbal form. . . The second ‘subject’. 28/29 ii 41. be resumed by the infixed particle of the dative. it is in the directive.1. it is loca­ tive : directive.)”. Parallel to person class [ni].8.(2) is used. suggested that ì-íb-∞gál means “is found on (the surface of ) something” and ì-in-∞gál “is found in it”. thus explaining the contrastive use of directive [b(i)] and locative 2 [n(i)]. 9. but rejects Edzard’s expla­ nation. 12. The dative “agin-ra cannot.. 35. offers a clear locative : dative correspondence.23a: The imperative “úm-me-eb “give it to us” has most prob­ ably to be interpreted as a ventive form. . 72 f. gú-na e-na(-n)-∞gar where dative -na. quotes. cannot stand in a second absolutive case (in Akkadian: double accusative). the one being fed.1. but the author would still maintain his position. Note: More frequent in the sg.25 [(e)ne]: gú-ne-ne-a e-ne(-n)-∞gar [i-ene-e-n-∞gar] “he (put (it) on their neck =) charged (it) to their account”. 12.)” Ent. Note: Zólyomi’s example (1999.is used. For reasons still unknown to us *[i-ene-a-n-∞gar] has been replaced by [i-ene-e-n-∞gar].8. 318:6) É-an-na-túm-ra lú ti mu-ni(-n)-ra “some­ one (struck =) sent an arrow against E. however. 4/5 xi 3.  101 Note: Wilcke 1988. Zólyomi 1999. ab-§rú-a “(pedestals) which had been erected (at . directive -ni.24: No examples available to the author for “to you (pl. preferably in the dative if it is a person: lugal-e (ergative) “agin-ra (dative) eri (absolutive). because a sen­ tence *lugal-e “agin-ra eri mu-na-an-§rú would mean “the king built a city for the general”. non-person class [bi] may assume the function of expressing causative: ud5-dè ní∞g-àr-ra bí-íb-gu7-en “I will (make eat =) feed the goats with groats” Nanna’s Journey 271. Edzard 1976b. attempted an explanation of the Sumerian causative construction: The Akkadian sentence “arrum “akkanakkam àlam u“èpi“ “the king had the general build a city” cannot be ren­ dered in Sumerian with two absolutive (“accusative”) cases.1.)” Ukg. 219 with fn. ní∞g-àr-ra “groats” is in the absolutive (‘object’). .8. see (59). ab-ús-sa [a-b(i)-us-a] “(house) which is bordering on (. . in the pl.

have to be kept strictly apart. An autonomous locative element [ni] had already been noted by Jacobsen. (2) ba-n. mi-ni.(-n. Note: Comitative [da] (8) has become dì(TI) by regressive assimilation to [ni]. had defined [ni]. tumu“en-min-nam igi-ba “embi ba-ni(-n)-∞gar “he put kohl on the eyes of two pigeons” Ean. Gragg 1973. This poly­ valence of spelling does not facilitate the modern interpreter’s task. having fallen”.102   12.1.and mu-ni. 206 f. ba-ú“ “he died”.may stand for (1) ba-Ø-. OS ba. 1 xviii 2–3. forms a closed syllable. (also 1959a. 21 f. and Postgate 1974. 28–29 ii 1–3. reduced to ba-n(i)Ø/-. 73 ff. “ub-ba-bi “its being placed. We prefer to write “locative 2” in order not to have to rename the locative dimensional indicators [ba] (3) and [(m)ma] (30). ì-in-∞gál “it is contained therein” Wilcke 1988. 34:1. 90–96 (“locatif ”). In this case.g. 48). 97 bottom. Sumerian has no general aversion against bab(V)-. However. again Jacobsen 1965. has extensive material for locative [ni].. only mu-bi “this year” or gub-ba-bi. 24 f. reduced [n].26 [ni]: Falkenstein 1949. 40 f. e. ba-an-ku4 “it has been brought in there” Wilcke 1988.being the absolutive or ergative element) and (3) ba-niØ-. mub(V)-. . 27 n.8. each time allegedly caused by dissimilation of the second of two labi­ als (*ba-bi-. Also. see for more discussion (62). so that they definitely have to be kept apart. MSL 4 (1956) 39* (“allocative”). Note: [ban]. as a positional variant of [bi] in ba-ni. and see most definitively Wilcke 1988. Falkenstein was criticized by Gragg 1973. in OS spelling. the syllable-closing consonant is most often omitted in writing.or mu-ni-. *mu-bi-). and Postgate showed that [ni] in mi-ni. e(-n)-da(-n)-dug4-ga-a [e-n-da-n-dug-a-a] “u nu-dì-ni(-b)-bala-e [nu (-b)-da-ni-b-bala-e] “he (Erikagina) will not (turn the hand over (-a) =) change what Ningirsu had (spoken with =) agreed upon with (Erikagina)” Ukg. Sollberger 1952. reduced from [bani]. At any rate.(not = im-ma-ni-!) cannot be identical with the directive element. e-bi íd-nun-ta gú-eden-na-“è íb-ta-ni(-n)-è “(Enmetena) made his ditch-and-dyke come up from the ‘High Canal’ to the ‘Border-ofthe-Steppe’” Ent.(< *bi-ni-) and im-mini. 69–90. 95 (“neuter allative-illative”). cf.. Therefore. the directive dimensional indicator and locative 2 may follow each other and do not fall into the same “slot”.

may have been synchronic options. 12. 169 f.27 [ma]: Dimensional indicators for the 1st p. Note: The verb ba.8. the 1st person “to me/us”. (and pl. For whereas in the 2nd or 3rd person there may always have been a choice between “to you/him here” and “to you/him there”.28 [muera]: gá-e mu-zu-“è ∞gi6 an-bar7-ba gi“gag-ti mu-e-ra∞ TAR.and (ventive) mu-: . both for [muera].8.29 [muna]: mu-na. OS RTC 19 has been used to demonstrate the contrast of (non-ventive) e. etc.2. sumu. sg. (the ribs are being cut for you =) I get upset because of you” Father and Son 121.  12. in spite of Akkadian qâ“u “to give as a present”.TAR “(as for) me: (for your name =) on your behalf.is one of the most frequent sequences among linked dimensional indicators.8.8. cf..) are to the best of our knowledge exclusively ventive. in dedications: a mu-na(-“è)(-n)-ru “he dedicated (object) to (DN) (for the life of PN)”. 12. always implies motion towards the person (or object). or in dialogues: gù mu-na-dé-e “he says to him”. ma-an-∞gál “he let (it) be there for me”. This is mainly due to its ubi­ quitous occurrence in building inscriptions: mu-na(-n)-§rú “he built for (DN)”.was most probably conditioned by 1st person [ma] “to me”.v. is primar­ ily “to allocate (something to someone)”. The regressive vowel assimilation in ma-ra.2. 12. ha-mu-na-ab-“úm-mu [ha-mu-na-b-“um-e] “let him give it to him” Ur III.2. “u-zi ma-ra-a-∞ ar “I (put a faithful hand to you =) did faithfully g obey you” Gudea Cyl. and the idea of a gift is secondary. see Sollberger 1966 glossary p. Akkadian Enlil-iqì“am).2. Note: mu-ra.and ma-ra-. d En-líl-le-ma-an-ba “Enlil attributed to me (the child)” (PN. “with me/us”. B ii 20. Ventive indicators (27–62) 103 In the following series of ventive dimensional indicators the trans­ lations do their best to render the nuance given to the verbal forms by these indicators.: 640 s. day and night. nam-ti ha-mu-ra-sù(-d) “may life be long for you”. Since its publication. ma-a-dug4 [ma-e-dug] “you told me”.

Note: The pronominal element of the indicator.2. wife of the ensi of Adab. all actions and motions referring to the visiting person. Is [me] self-sufficient. e-na(-n)-“úm “(receiver of gifts) gave (gifts in return) to her (= vis­ iting person)” r. Nin-∞gi“kimti. or is it the result of haplological ellipsis: *[mume] > [me]? See (38) for [mueda]. Limet 1968. And contrast “u e-na(-n)-taka “(receiver of gifts) conveyed (gifts in return) to her (= visiting person)” r.30 [mma]: An zà-gal-la mu-na(-n)-tu“. iii 5. 12. A xiii 4–5.2. For more Gudea examples see Falkenstein 1949. rather than for morpho-syntactic subtlety. ii 3. mu(-n)-da-∞gen-na “(PN) who had come with her (on her way to person visited)” iii 6 (seen from the side of the visiting person).8. he made Enlil sit there next to An. 28/29 iii 37. avoiding repetition. probably resulting from hypothetical *[mumeda]. wife of ensi Lugal-anda of Laga“. Ummaki e-ma-zi(-g) [e-mma-zig] “Umma rose against it” Ukg. Note: The document was drawn up at the residence of Bara-namtara. 270:53. (and) he seated Ninmah at Enlil(’s side)” Gudea Cyl.31 [mea]: Alongside the frequent Ur III PN (in Umma) d”áramu(-n)-túm “”ara brought (the child)” (e. 5. ii 5 (seen from the side of the person visited).2). [me].g.104   “u mu-na(-n)-taka “(visiting lady) conveyed (gifts) to her (= person visited)” iii 2. dEn-líl-ra. 6 iv 10'–11'. and DN-me-túm (see 59). . of the possessive particle (cf. Íl-e nam-énsi Ummaki-a “u e-ma(-n)-ti [e-mma-n-ti] “Il seized for himself the ensi-ship at Umma” Ent. dNin-mah mu-ni(-n)-ús “(Gudea) seated An for him (= Ningirsu) at the (big side =) seat of honour. The change from [muna] to [immani] to [muni] was probably made for reasons of style. dumu-ù ama-ni-ra KA-§rú-a nu-ma-na-dug4 [nu-mma-na-n-dug] “no child would (say a ‘stuck-in’ word on something to =) disobey its mother” Gudea Cyl. 12. there are variants of the name: d”ára-me-a-túm “”ara brought us (the child)” Johnes/Snyder 1961. 202.8. An-ra dEn-líl im-mani(-n)-ús [-mma-ni-n-us]. Note: The seating at the banquet was Ningirsu-An-Enlil-Ninmah. All actions and motions in her direction are noted as ventives.. 528). mu-na(-n)-“úm “(visiting lady) gave (gifts) to her (= person visited)” iv 3. are noted as neutral non-ventives. e(-n)-da-∞gen “(PN) had come with her (on her way to person vis­ ited)” r. B xix 18–21. is also found as the 1st pl.

8. ad mu-un-di1-ni-íb-ge4-ge4 “(the king) takes council with her” Three Men of Adab 17.2. above (26) Ukg.2. 46. d Utu im-da-húl “the Sungod rejoiced over (the brick)” Gudea Cyl. 31. A v 1.38 [mueda](?): a nu-mu-e-da-∞gál “there was no water (with =) for us” Three Men of Adab 6.2. cf. 12.34 [muda]: ha-mu-da-gub “let (Nan“e) stand by me” Gudea Cyl.2. see (61).36 [munda]: á mu(-n)-da(-n)-á∞g “he (passed the arm with =) ordered him” Gudea Cyl. Note: [e] of [eda] assimilated to preceding [u].39: No example available. 2 ii 44. A viii 22.2. 12. eri-ni ki-Lagaski-e si11-lí-a u4 mu(-n)-dì-ni-íb-zal-e [mu-nda-ni-bzal-e] “his city (and) the land of Lagas spend the day here with him in rejoicing” Note: [a] of [da] assimilated to following [i] of [ni].8. 12.40 [muneda]: No example available. 105 12.32: No example available.8. Note: In hé-em-“i-húl “let him rejoice over it” the object of joy is noted in the terminative (51). see Alster 1991–93. A i 25.)” as a rhetorical form used by the story-teller? me-e-de-en-ha-ze “he will seize him (= Dumuzi) from us (= the demons)” VS 2. 12.8.2.8.35 [mueda]: á-“è mu-e-da-a-á∞g [mu-eda-V-á∞g] “I (passed (it) to the arm with you =) ordered you” “à-bi nu-mu-ù-da-zu [nu-mu-e-da-V-zu] “I did not learn (with =) from you (about) (its heart =) the matter” Gudea Cyl. 12. A xv 11.8. 12. Note: Is [mueda] a dissimilated form of *[mumeda]? Or is it literally “with you (sg.8.8.  12.2.2.37 [mda]: ad im-dab5-ge4-ge4 [i-mda-b-gege] “(Nisaba) was consulting with (the tablet) for herself (ventive)” Gudea Cyl. see Alster 1991–93.33 [munea]: No example available. . 32. 12.8.2. 34:1. A xix 19.

è(-d) 3. So.12.3 for the question whether other dimensional indicators could occur only with gen­ eral reference to the verb’s semantic meaning and with no (even suppressed) pronominal element. 4. This is hardly a free variant. see ZA 74 (1984) 46. In cases without dative (or directive) of the per­ son. locative + ablative.2.e. NG I 68 f.8.would be taken as the directive indicator [era] (1).to another.41–43).refers to her own.8.8. fn.2. See below 12. b) im-ma-ra-an-dú-ud [i-mmara-n-dud] “she gave birth from (her) own (vulva)” Enki-Ninh. etc. 129:13. 51:15 mu-ne-ra-è “(two witnesses) came forth (against two other persons)” only seemingly contains [(e)nera] “from (among) them”.2. This becomes clear from NG no. “to come forth”.v. C iv 6 has the non-ventive form íb-ta-ab-èè-a. discussion on possible [nta] in 12.2. i. 12. 205:53.2. ibta. see.2.41–43: No examples available. 127:16 mu-na-ra-ni-è-e“ “they came forth (as witnesses) against him (-na-)”. his alternative (a) was “(X) est né(e) pour toi” where -ra. Note: The parallel version of Stat.8. . 12.8. imta.8. è(-d) “to come forth” is construed with im-ta. environment. See Falkenstein. Note: Ablative is generally incompatible with 1st and 2nd persons as well as with 3rd person person-class.(see NG III 105) s.45–46: No examples available (see note on 12.or íb-ta. 12. B viii 7. and 61). The ablative is used here either to refer to the group of persons from which the witnesses rose or it has become “frozen” to denote the special meaning of è(-d).48 [mu“i]: mu-“i-in-“e “he favoured me”. see also no. 12.8. 25. “à-bi-ta nu-ù-ma-ra(-n)-è-a [nu-mmara-n-e-a] “(he swore) he had not removed (anything) from (the storehouse)” NG no. Note: An alternative interpretation would be [mma + ra]. but has to do with the fact that in Statue C the temple is Inana’s Eana and not Nin∞girsu’s own Eninnû. 12.2..44 [mta]. 253. [(e)ne] is in fact virtually a dative. [mmara]: a) [mta]: lú E-ninnû-ta im-ta-ab-è-è-a “he who would remove (the statues) here from the Eninnû” Gudea Stat.47: No example available. Note: [ra] probably never has a preceding pronominal element. NG no.1.106   12.8. Note: We follow Attinger’s translation proposal (b). replaced by the directive (cf. however.5.8.

14. tumu“en-min-nam sa∞g-ba eren ì-mi-du8 [i-mmi-n-du] “he stuck ‘cedar’ (twigs) at the heads of two pigeons (of his)” Ean.8. A x 5.8.30).8.8.50 [mun“i]: ki mu-un-“i-KIªG.54 *[mu-ne-“i]: No example available.8. 38. 12. 12. not to the person or object named. etc.’)” Note: mu-“è mu-na(-n)-“e21 means “he gave (the statue) as a name (‘. If it were an ergative and if 2nd sg. 2:56. D v 7.2.KIªG “he looks for (someone)” Lugalbanda I 270 f. 12. A iv 3.8. 12. ..51 [m“i]: hé-em-“i-húl “let him rejoice over it”. 12. . Note: [Ki-engere] is taken here as a vocative. 12. 2:36. were not intended. . 28/29 ii 4–5. 12. Note: Only a short excerpt of the vast and complicated morphological evidence for the verb nam tar could be offered.8.8.2.2.2. xviii 4.1.55 [mu]: mu-“è mu-“e21(SA4) [mu-V-n-“e] “he gave me as a name (‘. -na. [mmeri]: a) [mmi]: e-ba na-§rú-a e-me-sar-sar [e-mmi-n-sarsar] “at the dyke here (on this side) he inscribed stelae” Ent. 1 obv.1.8.’)” Gudea Cyl.2.8. mu-ni-ús see above (12. O) Sumer” Gudea Cyl.  107 12.refers to the deity to whom the statue is dedicated.2. one would expect rather *hé-em-“i-∞gál (51).23.53: No example available. 41 (”ulgi).2.21).8. xxi 16.58 [mmi]. see above (12.2.2. na-ám-zé-eb du10-mu-ni-ib-tarar “let me make a firm promise for him” (Emesal) TLB 2.2. see also 12.52: No example available. .49 [mue“i]: dEn-líl-le igi-zi mu-e-“i-in-bar [mu-e“i-n-bar] “Enlil looked at you full of joy” Iddin-Dagan Hymn: SKIZ 209:5. 12. etc. let their eyes be directed to (you. . .’) for his/her (= the deity’s) sake” Gudea Stat. 12. Here.2. B xxii 20.8. Ki-en-ge-re6 kur-kur igi-bi ha-mu-“i-∞gál “all the countries.57 [muni]: mu-“è mu-ni-“e21 “he gave him as a name (‘.56 [muri]: nam ge4-rí-íb-tarar nam-dùg gú-mu-rí-íb-tarar ”ulgi D 384 f. .8.

. Jacobsen 1965 (= 1970). 15.62 [mini]. no fixed form is used when “crossing.BAL [i-mmeri-BALBAL] “he crossed seven mountain ranges” Enm. see Edzard 1991. e. Traditionally.2. 12. . 32. there­ fore.8. [immeri] is hardly imaginable without the model of [immara] (see 12.2. . borders on Heaven” Gudea Cyl. P iii 7.). 12. Note: [me] is probably a (free?) variant of [mea]. apparently.2.2. lú GN-ta ì-im-e-re-“a [i-m(mi)-ere-(e)“-a] “(persons) who had come here from GN” UET 3. .2.31).2. Note: See above (12. ∞ge“-gán-na ib-ta(-n)-bala “he made (someone) step over the wooden rod” (cf. 265 analysed it as containing “(b)ri ‘beyond it’.8. B i 6. A 61 ff. . for which see above (12.108   é .61 [mune]: di∞gir-gal-gal-Lagaski-ke4-ne(-r) é-a-ne-ne mu-ne-§rú [mu-(e)ne-e-n-§ru] “he built here their houses for the great gods of Lagas” Gudea Stat. like a moun­ tain range” Gudea Cyl. 1633:11.g. I iii 6. an-né im-ús “the House . 8–53). 207 f. and see Wilcke 1988. 187 f. sa∞g an-“è mi-ni-íb-íl “the House (raised the head =) proudly looked up to Heaven . bé-ri-bala. [immeri] just a “rhyming” form of [immara]? Note: In OB Sumerian. Note: Many more examples for ì-im-B in Wilcke 1988. Edzard 1970. 171. b) [immeri]: hur-sa∞g-umun7-e im-me-ri-BAL. For more Gudea examples see Falkenstein 1949.8. .8.2. When the hero crosses mountain-ranges.8. [mmini]: a) [mini]: é-e hur-sa∞g-gin7 . 12.2. for which an alternative interpretation as [mma+ra]. Lugalbanda I 252.8. Note: The form is difficult to explain. one would expect [bta] (13) or [mta] (44). Was.31) note on whether [me] is self-sufficient or the result of haplological ellipsis. Hu.b).44. in-TE-bala (ibid. A xxi 23.60: No example available. was suggested (see 12.8. .. locative + ablative. and cf.44 end). Note: [mmi] here is reduced to [m(mi)] immediately before the base. . 75. ‘over it’” (= “Neuter Superlative”). “úm-me-eb [“um-me-b] “give it to us” Inana’s Descent 278. pass­ ing over (something)” is to be expressed. 15. 19–32.59 [me]: dNanna-me(-n)-túm “the Moongod brought us (the child)” Limet 1968..8. . OB texts offer a variety of forms: in-ti-bala/bal-lam Gilg.

Wilcke 1988... It could not be defined as. . is difficult to define. It has chal­ lenged Sumerologists for more than a century.8. i.8.) DUGUD-gin7 an-“à-ge im-mi-ni-íb-DIRI. resumed the Forschungsgeschichte. A xxi 20. -da­ always stand for [nda]. stemming from. 1–2.g.“toward” indicating that the verb implied the general idea of having something with it. spelled e.“from”. [mda] or could [da] alone be intended? So far the only possible exception seems to be ablative [ra] (see 12. [bda]. ] Prefixed [e] or [i].DIRIne “they let (the House) soar into the midst of Heaven as if it were a cloud” Gudea Cyl. Vanstiphout 1985.. [e]/[i].2. stayed”. e. until the beginning of the 2nd millennium B. “non-ventive” in a general way because it may . there­ after.C.8. e-∞gál “it is/was present”. but with no (even suppressed) pronominal element. .1. ì-til “he lived.g. moving towards a goal? The question is extremely hard to answer because of our well-known difficulties interpreting Sumerian syllabic spelling where.or ì-.e.7. i.. e.from Ur III onward. P  [. Were there -da. It is advisable to start from forms where [e]/[ì] precedes the ver­ bal base as the only element and where no suffixes occur. or -“i.9. 12. see.1): e-∞gen “he went”. e. is left without an oppositional mark.44) which is never found with a person element. indication—or implication—of a pronominal participant. Note: Our transliteration NI = i is confirmed by allograph i. 2–4.“with”. syllable-closing consonants could be neglected in writing: did.3. The function of [e]/[i] may be defined here as an element moving the verbal base out of its neutral (lexical) mode into a finite verbal mode. Dimensional indicators without reference to a person? The question must be asked whether in principle a dimensional indi­ cator could occur with general reference to the verb’s semantic mean­ ing. forms of the intransitive conjugation pattern 1 (see 12. 3rd sg. 12. 12.. If a basic function once existed it may well have vanished over the long period during Sumerian was spoken and written.13. But [ra] is an extraneous element anyhow in the system of the dimensional indicators. *[bra] or *[mra].  109 b) [mmini]: (é . seen this way.g. -ta.e..

Poebel begged the question. before a simple base. it may then also have been used when absolutive.1. ergative and dimen­ sional indicators preceded the base: in-sar [i-n-sar] “he wrote”. See above. ì-mi. 58 f. Because of e-ur4 (instead of *i-ur4) “he sheared”. 185–206 and others. (-)b.30.(as well as a-ab-. ì-rí-. ì-im. e-“è. Krecher 1985 proposed to reduce—or even partially eliminate—the existence of a Sumerian verbal prefix [e]/[i]. 162 f. 45 f. above [mma] 12. There is a distribution rule limiting the use of [e]/[i]: (1) It occurs with all non-ventive indicators except with [ba] (3).110   occur at least with a ventive directional indicator of the 3rd person non-person class. . e-da-. see below 12. lost its relevance since the discovery of the Sumerian “ventive” (above 12. each form deter­ mined by the quality of the vowel in the following syllable. e-ne-sor for -sur. cf.8. and passim) because he analysed im-ma.as original in-. in­ da-∞gál [i-nda-∞gal] “it is/was present with him”. In a number of cases. i]. there is no *[iba]. Römer 1982. He claimed that the vowel was mainly part of the pronominal element constituting or opening a verbal prefix (series). 1959. e.g. e-sar.8. e-ta-. 158–181.23 cf. Scholtz 1934. 65–69.23.versus ì-gu7(“kú”). etc. Poebel 1931 demonstrated that in the pre-Sargonic texts from Girsu (Lagas) e. to indicate “finalization” or “pronominalization” of the base. So.10) consti­ tuting a prefix series which could not possibly have been rendering merely a non-functional [i]. 2.. Poebel postulated e-or4. ì-in-.37. we may repeat that the prefix served. ib-.and ì. thus including an alleged prefix [e]/[i] as part of the pronominal prefix. which was essential to Falkenstein 1950..2. Yoshikawa 1979. has ı been disproved by Gragg 1973. etc. Even if we cannot recover the original function of [e]/[i]. see also his e-sor.to constitute a nasalized vowel (still main­ tained in Thomsen 1984. Krecher’s view was refuted by Wilcke 1988 who brought ample evidence of pre-OB spellings ì-íb-. termed “Konjugationspräfixe” by B. Landsberger apud R. 12.1. p.8.2. Falkenstein supposed ì.. he proposed to define pronom­ inal elements (-)n-.etc.8. on alleged “vowel harmony”. 92) of which [mu] is but part.show complementary distribution. This theory. The idea of two allegedly opposed elements [mu] and [e. involving a circular argument.8. By extension.as *˜-ba-. [mda] 12. and Postgate 1974. 12.

had no room for such notations as [alb].are found in com­ plementary morphemic distribution.both indicate the notion of state (not necessarily passive) or habitualness.3. mobility. he stresses that a. Attinger 1993. ergative. que donc al-B représente morphématiquement [a+B]”. [mda] (37). and [mmini] (62).g. both with extensive literature. *[alnda] > [anda]. the group next-but-one to the base. enim-bi al-til “the respective matter is in the state of having being settled” (ubiquitous in pre-Ur III and Ur III court documents). [mmeri] (58). 267–69 (a-) and 269 f. etc.. We. Note: As a matter of fact.e.8. [i]. dimensional indicator) ou d’un préfixe II (i. non-person class series [mma] (30).e. as against the notion of action. x y-“è ab-“i-∞gar “(x is set in relation to y =) x equals y”. For peculiarities of [ba] (3) which are still in need of further elu­ cidation see above 12..before the base must always go back to a-x-B and that “la seule fonction de -l-” (i. 12. al. ven­ tive indicators)”. Therefore this line of reasoning only really holds good for a Semitic language such as Akkadian where such clusters do not occur.  111 (2) It does not occur with ventive indicators except with the 3rd sg. P  [()] a..and al. (al-). [albra]. with the allomorphs [a] and [al] depending on Sumerian syllable structure: *[alb] > [ab]. it seems preferable to posit with Attinger just the one morpheme [a(l)].was until recently seen as unique among Sumerian verbal prefixes because of its alleged inability to combine with any other morpheme—apart from prospective [u]: *ù-al. Since an element [l] encountered exclusively in [al] would be hard to explain.1. [mta]. or becoming.> ù-ul-. Thus Attinger implicitly stated that a. 269) that “[al] semble être le pendant de [a] dans le cas où la base n’est pas précédé d’un préf. in [al]) “est d’indiquer que [a] est directement suivi de la base.10. . [mmi]. [bla]. clearly states (p. *[imuda] etc.and al. e. therefore. the group next to the base: absolutive.. [m“i] (51). a circular argument is involved: The Sumerian sys­ tem of syllable writing which we can only see through “Akkadian glasses”. NRVN I 202:3. discount the possible occurrence of such consonantal clusters in Sumerian syllable structure.e. [mmara] (44). There is no *[ima]. III (i.

ab-da-gub “for its interest PN. For the formula u4 an-§rú enim an-∞gál see Edzard 2003a. he is supposed to eat (long =) faraway food.3 12. 3rd p. Vet.) 12. The following chart is divided into a positive left and a negative right part.11–12. àm.(1st.5 NGA 12. NAI(2nd.(1st p. Prec.13.) 12.) 12.11.5 12.11.) BARA.) 12.11.Aff.) 12. he is supposed to drink faraway water.1 INGA 12. 125:8–9.112   a.) has (a credit) with him (= debtor)” SR 99 iv 12–15. 1 Aff. Enkidu and the Netherworld.) 12.*[a(l)-n(i)]: é me-lim5-bi kur-kur-ra a-dul5 “the awe of the House is spread over all the countries” Ent.*[a(l)-n-da]: an-da-tuku “(creditor. .2 NGA 12.13.2 12. 8 vi 2. 28–29 ii 39–41.4 BARA. Note: For more evidence. Ind. 3rd p.13. Aff. 12. he sits at the suburb” Gilgame“. his (water =) drink is sepa­ rate.) 12.*[a(l)-b(i)]: bára-§rú-a-di∞gir-ré-ne nam-nun-da-ki-∞gar-ra ab-§rú-a “the pedestals of the gods. (UET 6/1. BARA. 3rd p. Neg Coh. 58:6). 87–98.11. an-da.117 12.9 . ab-da.11.(2nd. see Edzard 2003a.6 Neg.11. . erg. (stands =) guarantees” TMH NF 1–2.*[a(l)-m(mi)]: en-na àm-∞gen igi-∞gu10-“è enim-bi a-bala-e na-baan-§rú “until he came here to report before me.13. ab.) (1st.13. 3rd p.11. 32:1–4. 3rd p. 89. 3rd p.[*a(l)-b-da]: (silver) má“-bi-“è dam-[a-ni] .11. Enkidu and the Netherworld 255 and passim.10 NGA Neg Ind.1 (1st p. Coh. an.11.3 (2nd. ú-[ni al]-bar a-ni al-bar ú-gíd al-gu7-e a-gíd al-na8-na8 eri-bar-ra-a al-tu“ “[his] (grass =) food is separate.) how does he fare?” Gilgame“. which had been erected there in Namnundaki∞gara” Ent.) 12.8 Prohib. . M    It seems advisable to treat these two kinds of indicators together because their functions partly interlink. NUNU(1st–3rd p. 2 ØØGAGAHÉHÉHÉNAIINAII(1st–3rd p.4 (1st.*[a(l)-n]: a-na-gin7 an-AG “(the one who had one son. [his] wife. . . he (= the addressee) should not be (stuck in =) kept” TCS 1 no.

11. [nu] is used indepen­ dent of conjugation pattern.9). . B v 4. . A iv 21.13.11 NGA INGA 113 Note to the chart: NAI and NAII are. Indicative Simple statements.2.11.1.) as a working material” Gudea Stat. 12.12. very probably. “à-ga-ni nu-mu-zu “I did not understand what he meant with respect to me” Gudea Cyl.11.is found in alan-e . 3rd p. There is a parallel. in the “indicative mood”. . NU”NU” Prosp. 12. and it may also be prefixed to nonfinite verbal forms such as participles or infinitives.may have been conditioned by the fourfold occurrence of suffixed -nu in preceding lines 50–52. It is an open question whether allomorph [la] has been influenced by the existence of Akkadian là “not”. ‘Archaizing’ nu-ba.1 12. nobody was supposed to use (sil­ ver . [nu] has the allomorph [la] before the dimen­ sional indicator [ba] (3): á! (DA)-bad-∞gu10 la-ba-ta-è “nobody escaped my outstretched arm” Gudea Cyl. . equally . of the same origin. are not specially marked.6 12.13. A iv 26. Negative indicative Its mark is [nu] (for allomorphs see below).12 12. Ù Aff. From NS onward. In OB.11. M  12. For allomorphs see below. B vii 53. .  Table (cont. 139 is an over-simplified presentation of the “Modal prefixes”. but have diverged. NAII to past events. the allomorph [la] before [ba] was extended to [li] before [bi]: za-e “u-∞gar-lugal-zu li-bí-in-gi4 “you(!) have not avenged your king” Ali Letters B 5:7. The table “Construction of Finite Forms” in Thomsen 1984.6 12.11. NAII as an affirmative may go back to a negative rhetorical question (see 12. . A i 28. ki∞g-∞gá nu-ba-∞gá-∞gá “for this statue .) 12. nu-ba.11. as compared to earlier ér nu-bí(-n)-dug4 “she uttered no laments thereon” Gudea Stat.instead of *la-ba.) ”I”IFrustr. NAI referring to future. “à-bi nu-zu “I did not understand its (heart =) meaning” Gudea Cyl. 3 (1st.

Note: nu-ga-àm [nu-(i)nga-am] contains the connecting indicator [inga] for which see 12. my wife!”. who being (was) she?” = “that woman.. however..1.3).2. [nu] is not used if a modal form is to be negated.1. B vii 50–52. Note: Michalowski 1989. a-ne-da-nu [ane-da-nu] : balu““u “it is (not with =) without him” MSL 13. A iv 23. (would there be) a prayer?” (PN) DP 113 iv 4. .9). In practice. in negative infinitive constructions even where they may have a modal connotation. 86: E 31 (Proto-Kagal).4).12. . Oh. Note: me-a-ni is an example of the “pronominal conjugation” (see 12. me-en-dè-nu [menden-nu] : ul nìnu “it is not us” MSL 4. .2. è in vetitive. HSS 3. Note: Similarly Babylonian Akkadian would not use ul except for a negated indicative in a main clause. munus-bi a-ba me-a-nu a-ba me-a-ni “that woman: who being (was she) not. 12.14.11. Suffixed -nu [nu] occurs suffixed to a noun as the negation of the copula [am] (see 12. suffixed -nu has often become a substitute for the syn­ tagma X-da nu-me-a “(not being with =) without X” (see 5. 32:3. assuming a verbal form nu-im-me.14. là is used in a dependent clause or in prohibitive. nu-B-(e)de/-(e)da (see 12. e. 114 iv 3.g.g.4.6).7. Nin-nu-nam-“itax “(There) being no Lady.11. final “in order not to . ù kù-nu za-gìn nu-ga-àm ù erida-nu nagga-nu sipar-nu “it is not sil­ ver nor is it lapislazuli. ay. 51:419 (OBGT).”.11. and it is neither copper nor tin nor bronze” Gudea Stat. whoever was she?” Gudea Cyl.6) or prohibitive (12. [nu] is used. OB grammatical and lexical texts show suffixed -nu with pronouns. 43 understood the passage as “he says not. in the relation between Sumerian nu-banda “foreman” and the Akkadian loanword laputtûm < *la-pant-.1. [nu] may be spelled nu-ù-. e.2): dam-∞gu10-nu dumu-∞gu10-nu é-∞gu10-nu im-me “he exclaims ‘my wife/my child/my house is no more’ ” LamSumUr 95–97. 23 ix 26.114   unexplained. as with veti­ tive (12. probably when followed by [i]: nu-ùme-en “I am not” NG no. 17 iii 10.

is not marked with a suffix (denoting person) after the base: ga-tìl(il) “(let me live =) ex-voto object”. See Attinger 1993.11.11. see 12.21. etc.11. Although combined with conjugation pattern 1 in the case of an intransitive verb. Note: Here the pl.alone was sufficient to mark the 1st sg. where we do not find the expected ga-na-ab-dug4 [ga-na-b-dug] as in Gudea Cyl. 12.) most probably has to be kept separate from the Ur III reference (TrDr. see above 12. do” behaves in a special way in OB: silim-ma ga-na-ab-bé-en [ga-na-b-e-(e)n] “let me say ‘hail’ to her” Iddin-Dagàn B 1–3. [nu] as a separate verbal base dub-sar “u nu-a nar míli (KA × LI) nu-a “a scribe having no hand (is like) a cantor having no voice” SP II 43.11. nu may also be considered a base of its own in the phrase a-baàm lugal a-ba-àm nu lugal “who is it (that was) king.1. ”ulgi D 384 f. x 2 see 12.is essentially linked to verbal forms with the ¢am†u base and. ga. u8 sila4 nú-a “a ewe having no lamb” TCL 2. It is denoted by prefixed ga-. Note: The lexical passage u8 sila4 nú-a: “a pu¢àdsa nì[lu] “a ewe whose lamb is resting” (see CAD L 42 la¢ru lex. e-ne-sù-ud ga!-da-e [ga-eda-e-(e)n] “(let me play with you =) let us have sex together” TMH NF 3. 222 f. however. Cohortative The cohortative mood occurs only in the 1st sg..  115 12.2. -enden is suffixed: ga-ba-húl-húl-le-en-dè-en [ga-ba-hulhul-enden] “let us mightily rejoice over it” ZA 45 (1939) 119. 476. base [e] may have been chosen because the intended act implied two (active) participants. Kinglist vii 1. and pl. 6. Apparently. 26:9) where -nu-a would not be an abbreviated spelling for *-nú-a. The cohortative is found in complementary distribution with the preca­ tive. 26:9. with allomorphs (Ur III) ge4-(rí-) and gú-(mu-). there is no ending -en after the base. nam gé4-rí-íb-tar ar etc..11. .2. 5621:1. The (irregular) verb dug4/e “to speak. 1 r. In the pl. For nu in OS Ean. in-nu [i-nu] has become the expression for “No!” by OB. ud5 má“ nu-a “a goat having no kid” TrDr. therefore. 25:20. who is it (that was) not king?” = “whoever was king?” Sum.3. ga. which supplies the 2nd and 3rd persons. A iii 22 f.8. (*NS).5.

.”.11. .(Ur III. hé.116   12.”. there are ‘mixed’ spellings HÉ-ab-. accord­ ing to Attinger 1993. 128–34. . . . being found in a wider range of phonetic con­ texts than is [ha]. HÉ-an. ∞ gi“kim-∞gu10 hé-sa6 “let my sign be favourable” Gudea Cyl.11.3). The negative cohortative is restricted by definition to the 1st per­ son and found with the marû verbal base and personal suffixes if the form is transitive. .occur at Girsu. ∞ gìri-bi ha-ma-∞gá-∞gá “may (Nan“e) show me the way there” Gudea Cyl. 1 xx 19. A ii 19. “you may . In Ur III letter-orders hé-na-ab-“úm-mu and ha-na-ab-“úm-mu “let him/her give it to him/her” (Sollberger 1966.11. ki-sur-ra-dNin-∞gír-su-ka-ke4 ba-ra-mu-bala-e(n) “I swear I will not transgress. It is found in complementary distribution with the cohortative (12.5. may you say a blessing thereat on my behalf ” Gudea Cyl. 838.”. 169 f. a multi-functional mor­ pheme which is also found in the vetitive (12. *HA-en-. ª Moreover.). . . but none of a type *HA-eb-. but. Negated cohortative Negative exhortation. sumu) the relative frequency of the two forms is 125:15. The distribution of [he] and [ha] is not entirely clear. s.11. . as well as (OB) hu.v. E4-nun-na bar-∞gu10-a “ùd hé-mi(-b)-sa4(-n)-za(-n) [he-mmi-b-sa(e)nzen] “O Enùna (pl. d’Agostino 1990. Precative forms are found with the marû form if the verb is tran­ sitive. 28:8. Note: For another allograph. etc.11.is “sans hésitation possible la forme de base”.6) and in the neg­ ative affirmative (12. Precative is marked by the prefix HÉ-. hi. Precative The precative mood is used with 2nd and 3rd person.8). “I will certainly not . § 191. A iii 18. B ii 6. . 12. . is expressed with the bi-syllabic prefix [bara]. as regards me. 293 with fn. local variation should be discounted. ”ulgi hymns). see Attinger 1993.(see below). where also reference is made to Ebla. 292 f.before mu-. graphically realized as hé­ or ha-.”. “he may . As both hé-naand ha-na.”. di ba-ra-a-da-ab-bé-en6 [bara-eda-b-e-(e)n] “I promise I will not go to court (with =) against you” NG no. “let him/her . “let me not . Nin∞girsu’s boundary” Ean.4.

The vetitive (negative precative) It is used with the 2nd and 3rd person. . en hé-a lugal hé-a “whether he be a lord or a king”. Affirmative 1 Like the precative (see 12.6.11.A-da-pe“-e [he-eda-b-pe“-e] “let the Land (stretch out the hand =) gain in abundance (with you =) under your rule” Gudea Cyl. . etc. B WISH-ing =) be it A. be it B”.8. A xi 9. (note that in TCS 1 no. . As against precative (occurring with 2nd and 3rd persons). affirmative 1 occurs in the 1st and 3rd persons. vetitive is marked by the prefixed particle [bara]. Edzard 1971.7. see 12. mí-ús-sá-zu mí-ús-sá-∞gu10 ba-ra-me “the son-in-law you (had in mind) should certainly not be(come) my son-in-law” NG no. 12. dub-ta hé-em-ta-∞gar “may his name . . Note: Since the copula is intransitive and has no opposed ¢am†u : marû forms. about what is being said. . B ix 16.7. . not 2nd person). e. 213–25.”. 213 f. it is difficult to decide whether this is a vetitive or a negative affirmative “will cer­ tainly not . which serves to remove doubt. It is found in complementary distribution with the neg­ ative cohortative (1st person.4). (“A WISH-ing. “Affirmative 1” is a catch-all term. 12. in con­ trast to in-nu “no” (cf.11. mu-ni . 12. cf. Note: Cf. Like the negative cohortative. be (set =) taken off the tablet” Gudea Stat. in blessing or curse formulae. .2).2.3) and the ¢am†u verbal base if the ver­ bal form is transitive.11. Note: See also Edzard 1971.11..11. 82:4 there is 3rd. It refers to something in the past and.11. hé-àm “let it be” became in effect an expression for “yes”.  117 kalam-e hé-∞gál-la “u HÉ.5) it is marked by the prefixed parti­ cle [he] and its allomorphs. “please do not”. The main function of this mood is to remove doubt.g. is found with conju­ gation pattern 2b (see 12. 12. therefore. 18:24 (oath). “honest!” in colloquial English. It is found with the marû verbal base if the verbal form is transitive. hé is used as an independent verbal base in the expression A hé-a B hé-a (= lù A lù B). on the side of the listener. “you/he should not”.

11. with the exception of subordinate clauses and of lines where the copula -me-en “I am” is used.11.8.. it is found with the ¢am†u verbal base and with conjugation pattern 2b (see 12. Enkidu and the Nether World 185.. ì-du10-ga bur-ra na-an-“e22-“e22-en “do not anoint (yourself ) there with sweet oil from a jar” ibid. . .”. Prohibitive The prohibitive mood is prefixed by the particle [na] graphically (and phonetically?) identical to the affirmative 2 particle [na]. 12. 12. túg-tán-tán-na na-an-mu4-mu4-un [na-ni-mumu-(e)n] “do not wear a clean garment there” Gilg. 219 f.”.11. nu-mu-un-su lú-á-tuku-ra ba-ra-na-an-∞gar “he did not—do not doubt it—(set =) pledge (as a security) a widow to a wealthy per­ son” Ur-Namma Code 115.7. did not let (my) hair stand on end because of it” ”ulgi A 70.7). As against the negative cohortative [bara] (see 12. Edzard 1971. l2.11. instead of “I verily . Affirmative 1 does not occur with the 2nd person. and as such it is the negation of the imperative.11.6). It occurs in the 1st and 3rd person. Negative affirmative The negative affirmative is marked by the prefixed particle [bara] which also serves the negative cohortative (12.11. are preceded by hé-. Edzard 1971. With a 3rd person form it equally implies strict interdiction. on the side of the listener. It might then be rendered by a pluralis maiestatis. . As it refers to some­ thing in the past.11.118   Affirmative 1 is extremely popular in royal statements about what the ruler did. the main function of nega­ tive affirmative is to rule out doubt. As with positive affirmative 1 (12. 187. performed.3) if the verbal form is transitive. “we . about what is being said.4) it does not occur with the 1st person (nor does the vetitive. .6). Directed at a 2nd person it expresses prohibition. ní ba-ra-ba-da-te su ba-ra-ba-da-zi “Yeah! I (the king) was not afraid of it.9. Note: In the royal hymn ”ulgi A nearly all statements of the king’s activities made in the 1st sg. . 218 f.4) and the vetitive (12. achieved.

(12. JSS 38 [1993] 1–13). . § (now. . Affirmative 2 Affirmative 2.2) in the case of transitive verbal forms. 3rd persons.8): assertion that something has not occurred. e. .11. 12. however) he (= Gudea) did build it” Gudea Stat. or that someone has not done something. (2) vetitive [bara] (2nd.may go back to a rhetorical negative question: “has it not . It serves to draw attention to the impor­ tance of something that was there or happened. 3rd persons. . also in colloquial English the negative tag question “. 3rd persons.g. Testen.7. prefixed by particle [na]. é ur5-gin7 dím-ma énsi-deli-e dNin-∞gír-su-ra nu-na(-n)-§rú na-mu(-n)rú “a house built that way no single ensi had (ever) built for Nin∞girsu.7. . has been amply treated by Falkenstein 1942. isn’t it?” meaning “it is really . 12. bil-mi-yorum “I do not know”. See also table on p. Turkish -mV.11. Edzard 1971. Affirmative 2 na. by all means?” turning into “it definitely has . It is often found at the beginning of tales: ∞gé“tu-ga-ni na-an-gub [na-n-gub] “You should know that (Inana) (set her ear =) decided to move (from the great Heaven to the great Earth)”.11. cf.  We summarize the negative moods: 119 (1) negative cohortative [bara] (1st person.11. Inana’s Descent 1. (4) prohibitive [na] (2nd. .and prohibitive na-—instead of chance homophony—a common origin is plausible.11. evin-mi “your house?”.3). Semitic là “not” and affirmative and/or optative elements containing la (see D. ques­ tion (and affirmation) are found in diverse languages: Latin ne and enclitic ques­ tion marker -ne.”. 112 f. 12.(negation) and -mV (question). B vii 4. . 220–21. Affirmative na. 12. 12.”.10. For that reason.9): negative imperative. it is usually found with the ¢am†u base and conjugation pattern 2b (see 12. interdiction (theoretically). Note: Phonetic assonance or even identity of elements denoting negation.11.9) because the latter uses the marû base and conjugation pattern 1 (see 12.4): strict instruc­ tion not to act or to repeat an action. if the ver­ bal form is transitive.6): interdiction. (3) negative affirmative [bara] (1st. . For affirmative 2 na. 181–223. . but is still mean­ ingful for what is to come. or rec­ ommendation not to act.can rarely be confused with prohibitive na..

1 r. Beside the extensive treatment of the morpheme by Falkenstein 1944. occur or have occurred. See Attinger 1993.GAL.11. das primär auf etwas Kommendes.before mu-. . The main function of [“a] is to reconfirm something that already had been stated or had occurred (Civil 1974: “Main event precedes”). with allomorph (OB) “u. . is found only in literary contexts.). cf. 297 with more lit. Attinger 1993. then extended as demain “tomorrow”. Note: Pleonastic extensions frequently occur in languages. as against affirmative 2 [na] which stresses the importance of some­ thing in the past. Vorauszusetzendes zurück.8) has a counterpart in na-nam “it definitely was. Oxford 1993. di-ku5 ka-a“ bar-re-da [z]a-a-da “a-mu-e-da-∞gál “Pronouncing deci­ sions rests (as it always has) with you (= Nergal)” SRT 12. and it is unknown whether it also occurred in colloquial Sumerian. starting out from *main (< mane).’” Ean. so”. French le lendemain “the next day”.11.:20. . e. cf. 44 and 48 fn. en-demain. still meaningful for the future.(var.11. 74 (= 1970. Note: A.11. see 12. 12. Affirmative 3 Affirmative 3 “a-. 73–118. the note of M. and still is. zu Erwartendes verweist” (Sumerian Grammar Discussion Group. (with more lit.11.. na-§rú-a mu-bi lú-a nu(-àm) mu-bi “i-e “the name of the statue— it is not a person’s (name)—its name (says =) reads (as we already know): ‘. 12.120   hé-àm “it is really so” (= “yes”.NUN correspondence ”A = NA see below. Civil apud Heimpel 1974. The term “frustrative” was proposed by Jacobsen 1965. 25. 222.12.11. [nu“] was thoroughly analysed by Römer 1976. l’endemain.weist mit Nachdruck— affirmativ—auf etwas schon Vorliegendes. nu-ú“. 371–78. 12. im Gegensatz zu na-. see Zgoll 2003). see Römer 1965. 294 f. Edzard 1971. “i-. It is a pleonastic formation of *na-àm [na-am] turned into [na-na-(a)m] and could be extended still further to (ur5) hé-na-nam-ma(-àm). Frustrative The prefixed particle nu-u“. For the UD. Ganter (Zgoll) contrasted [“a] and [na]: ““a..g. 91.and nu-“V-) expresses a hypothetical wish: something should be or have been. x 1–3. 249).5 end and 15.

(ì-bí-). 297. English “if you would (kindly) close the window” which equals “close the window. cannot be ruled out. most probably.in post-OB or in Emesal). 45:1439. . I did B”. . (2) [U] expresses a polite imperative: “after you did A (I would be grateful)” = “would you please do A”. “after I had done A. a. .before bí.. e. I will certainly not enter (there again)” Ur-Namma’s Death 213. C  12. 115 n.12.U” = lu-wu-um. i. 23 f. Note: Cf. as for me (= lamenting Inana). in its Akkadian column. [nu“] may perhaps be segmentized into negative [nu] + [”]. 51. -B sometimes comes close to a conditional clause. “if A. with me) his beau­ tiful features. 12. but the nature of the second element would remain quite uncertain. nor can some other unknown origin.(also u.  121 [nu“] is so far only found in literary and lexical contexts. Civil 1983. as well as apud Edzard 1984. A syntagma [U]. Prospective Prospective [U] has two main functions: (1) it serves to indicate that the verbal idea expressed by form A precedes (in time or argument) the verbal idea expressed by a subsequent form B.g. u4-ba ∞ge“ellag-∞gu10 é-na∞gar-ra-ka nu-u“-ma-da-∞gál-la-àm = ùma pukku ina bìt naggàri lù ezib “If only on that day my hoop(?) had stayed with me in the carpenter’s house” = “had the hoop(?) but stayed on that day in the carpenter’s house” Gilgame“. and ì. Krebernik 1983. precative [luw] and hypothetical [law] in substantivized form. ( l)a-wu-um represents. . a—rhetoric?—negation of affir­ mative 3. . n. Ebla NU. but *nu-“i-. then B”. and ni-i“. Cf.1.12. .before ba(a-ba-). The prospective cannot. 1.e. For rare spelling variants nu-ú“-. né-e“-.. proposed *nu-“è-.. sipa-∞gu10 hi-li-a-ni nu-u“-ma!(-da)-an-ku4-ku4 [nu“-ma/mada-n(i)kuku] ∞gá-e ba-ra-ku4-ku4-dè-en “Would that my shepherd (= the dead Ur-Namma) (could still) bring in unto me (var. [U] is written ù. 78. please”. no examples are known. Enkidu and the Netherworld 172 (= EG XII 1). at least. I went home”. -A.see Attinger 1993. be negated: *“after I did not see him.

Michalowski 1995. the author of a letter addresses the scribe by dug4-ga-na (-b) “tell it to him” in order to convey the message to the addressee.1. 1).. 295 f. 12. fit together a chariot.12. . polite imperatives: Civil 1960. . . with lit. A vi 15–19. and see Wilcke 1988. An Ur III (or earlier) collection of medical prescriptions nearly twenty times offers prospective verbal forms which should be con­ sidered as suggestions. 10 with fn. . .2). 144. Later on this quite direct formula was generally replaced by a ver­ bal form using the prospective: ù-na-dug4 [u-na-e-dug] “after you said to him/her” = “would you please tell him/her”. decorate (that chariot)” Gudea Cyl. .. A xi 24–25. esp.g. Heimpe1 1974.. one single person will (do work with a second person =) work as much as two” Gudea Cyl.DUN.e. Polite imperative There is a universal tendency to moderate the harsh form of a direct imperative: “pass me the salt”—“would you please pass me the salt”. . when the interest of the field will have been raised he (= the lessor) will set his rent/interest thereon” TCL 5. In OS. 2 “e gur a-“à ù-gíd. 1:6 [dug4]-ga-¢naÜ (= CIRPL Enz. úgu-ba má“ ì-íb-∞gá-∞gá “(when =) if he (= the tenant) (only) tilled (the amount of ) 2 kor of barley. harness to it a donkey stallion .2.1. A precedes B kalam-e zi-“à-∞gál ù-ma-“úm [u-mma-n-“um]. 39).ÙR ù-“i-lá [u-b“i-elaH] . “u ù-ma-ni-tag [u-mba-ni-e-tag] “would you please break the seal (on your storehouse). 229–233 with over 70 strings of prefixed particles starting with [U]. A typical example of polite imperatives are Nin∞girsu’s instructions given to Gudea to tell him how he should construct his (= Nin∞girsu’s) chariot and accessories: kì“ib ù-mi-kúr [u-mbi-e-kur] ge“ ù-ma-ta-∞gar [u-mba-(b)ta-e-∞gar] .122   Attinger 1993. 61:54 . lú-deli lú-min-da ki∞g mu-da-ak-ke4 “after he (= the Northwind) has given life to our Land. 11 no. . lay out the wood from there. ∞ ∞ge“ gigir ù-mu-silim [u-mu-e-silim] ANSE. e.12. 6170:13–15 (NG no. má“-a-“à-ga a-ba-ra-zi.. . 12. i. The formula must often have been pronounced [unêdug] in order to account for the Akkadian loanword form unetukku (see above 3.

12. . 12. is nasalized.12. throw a (twig of ) tamarisk into it” Falkenstein 1931. “non seulement . we too” OBGT I 410 f. . e4 . preceded by lines 202–204 ù. Connecting indicator [inga] The preposed particle [inga] occurs in its full form.12. bi. in-ga-me-en-danam—nìnuma “it is we. See above. (MSL 4. na-na-ga-.1) as well as after [nu“] (see 12. 90:28 f. table on p. [inga]. and see 66. [i]. In her theory. 58 ù-gaz “would you crush”. 80. ga-an-ga-. .g. also lines 412–418 where. The functions of [inga] have been summed up by Attinger 1993. e. for arguments militating ı against the existence of a nasalized element *[Û].12.12. .). ù-me-ni-dé..) verbal form in a series. “(et) de nouveau”.2. in prescriptions to perform a ritual. 112 f.2. in-ga-me-en-dè-en. “i-in-ga-. “et de plus”. . ibid. only notes an element [ga] instead of [inga]. inga-. mais encore”. [inga] stands out by its ability to com­ bine with at least five positive modal particles (GA-. [inga] was given much attention in the grammatical lists. in verb initial position (see 12. 137) list un-ga. both OBGT and NBGT. as indicating “et alors”. were treated sep­ arately from such strings as nu-un-ga-. A frozen form ù-/u-me-ni-B “would you please . hé-en-ga-—an example of the efforts of cuneiform scribes . 297 f..  123 ù-dé [u-e-de] “would you pour”. in-ga-. The NBGT 205–208 (MSL 4. As a connecting indicator. enga (in the well-known u-a-i(-e) sequence) = ù “and”. Thomsen 1984..” is found in post-OB incantations. . ∞ge““inig .7). un-ga etc. an-ga. Cf. as a rule. NU”-) as well as with negative NU-. “(et) aussi . [inga] is rendered by suffixed -ma in Akkadian. See 12.2–6). . ù-me-ni-“ub “would you pour water (in a jug). . 169–72. . 51). in-ga. each time.g.9. ”A-. and passim. . instead of giving it the value [ø].2. NAII-. so that [inga] would represent *[˜-ga].1 ff. only occurs with a second (or third .2.[nga] after a preceding vowel (see 12. bi-da = ù “and”. . e. 12. . HÉ-. “et par conséquence”. que”. preceding [ga]. and it is only found in first posi­ tion when it is followed by a second verbal form with [inga]..2. it is -Vn-ga. In such a case we would have to define [i] (see 12.9) as the modal indicator for the indicative (cf. The question arises whether [inga] might be segmentized into [i] + [nga].

lugal-∞gu10 za-gin7 a-ba an-ga-kal a-ba an-ga-a-da-sá “my lord. too. l2. to realize things” Gudea Cyl.11. gaba-bi “u e-ga-ma-ús [inga-mma-n-us] “(Umma) (came close to the breast of =) defied Laga“. let’s also return here” The Three Ox Drivers 13. .2. as for me.is a contraction of a-ba (i)n-ga-. 12. .1. .1. .. . (2) (i)nga is preceded by indicator [a(l)] (see 12. 156:7–11 (letter. This passage may be interpreted in two ways: (1) a-ba an-ga. . 198). [nu-nga] a-na gur-si-sá-ta.2. dNan“e me-zu me-na-me nu-un-ga-an-dasá “your ordinances are the greatest . Ur-dNin-∞gír-su.] mu-tar. (1) gur ki-su7-ta.12. it (also =) once more defied it” Ean. See also MSL 4.12. . unclear context).12. as for U.2. 145:395–398.2. eriki in-ga-àm eriki in-ga-àm “à-bi a-ba mu-zu “it is (such) a city. . A vii 10. come. gal mu-zu gal ì-ga-túm-mu “(Gudea) is wise (and) (also brings forth great (things) =) able. . it is (such) a city. . nu-ga-ma-tar “[. . who would also (= [anga]) be able to compete with you?” ”ulgi D (MBI 3 i 23–25). and. àm-á∞g[-∞gá]. 163:12–15 (new: MSL 5. For Gudea Stat. 150:37–40. . ù nam-érim. 1 ii 27–iii 22. 199:21. who is as powerful as you are. . (who knows its heart =) that no one can know its heart” Ke“ Hymn 59. nu-ga-á∞g [nu- nga-V-a∞g] “what(ever) may have been measured out from the reg­ ular kor. . he did not swear an (assertive) oath in my favour” ITT II 5758 r. 195:11. . in-ga- Laga“ki gaba-bi “u e-ma-ús .] has sworn. 12. any other ordinances could certainly not rival with you(rs)” Nan“e Hymn 251. B vii 50 nu-ga-àm “nor is it” see 12. I did not measure out (one) kor from the threshing floor” BIN 8.. [ga-nga] gan ga-ni-re7-en-dè-en gan ga-an-ga-àm-gi(!)-dè-en [ga-nga-mm(i)gigi(!)-(n)den] “come.. [. O Nan“e. . a-a-∞gu10 a-a-zu-gin7 in-ga-dím “my father is (made =) as good as your father” Sefati 1998.2. let’s go there.10). ù ∞gá-[e]. . 1'–4'.124   to define grammatical elements whose structure cannot be rendered with syllabic signs. me-zu mah-àm .3.

. an ki-ta baD-re6-dè sa∞g na-an-ga(-àm)-ma-an-“úm “the Lord.in line 1 is the affirmative naII. . 12. á imma-an-á∞g “then the very wise one . he in fact gave heed to separate heaven from earth” Creation of the Pickaxe 1 . . 159 ad line 74). . DP 95:4. 12.”. etc. na-an-gaba(!)-ti-[l]a-da [na-nga-ba-til-ed-a] “as long as he lives” NRVN 1. 12. . O House. É-an-na-túm-me gal na-ga-mu-zu “Eanatum in fact also knows this: . you used to be the place of joy for the Black-headed ones. .10). lit. . . [nanga] occurs in a frozen form in u4 na-an-ga-ma [na-nga(-i)me-a?] “whichever day it actually was” in the sense of “formerly” (see Wilcke 1969. i 31–32. . who even from his residence finds out about the (heart =) intentions of the gods .2. made appear here what is due. . .frequently found at the beginning of tales (see 12.2. . 18). [ “i-nga] u4-ba ∞gé“tu-diri . .5.11. Note: The actions described in lines 1 and 5 are connected by (i)nga . . ki “à-∞gu10 na-an-ga-ma-ab-bé-a “(I will loosen my sandals) at whichever place my heart tells me” Lugalbanda I 178 (this is the climax after a series of cohortative verbal forms introduced by ga-).2. and it is repeated in line 5. . . 236:4.4. he (= the adopted person) will (stand before them =) be at their service” ARN 7:8–11. and. 124. and . ki-tu“-a-ni-ta “à-di∞gir-re-e-ke4 “i-in-ga-zu-a . .2. . ì-ze-è∞g-bi-ta íb-ba sumu-ug-ga hé-en-ga-mu-e-da-TAB.. (i)nga.12. ù N. . see Struve 1984. [he-nga] é ki-hur-sa6-ga-ù∞g-sa∞g-∞gi6-ga ì-me-en-na-ke4-e“. etc. gave instruc­ tion” Inanna and Enki SLTNi.12. or N. .12. 5. na. en-na W. .” Ean.12. you should know ˆ (na-). r. 32:9–10 (Farber-Flügge 1973. . . PN. in bright fashion.8.  125 12. more­ over. . (the phrase each time introduces the magic ritual performed with pigeons).see below. 1 xxi 12–13. instead of its festivals (emesal for ezem) they (doubled with you =) gave in return to you both wrath and disaster” Lament of Ur 116–117. naM-ga-). na-an-ga-ti-la-a“ igi-ni-ne-“è ì-gub-bu(-d) as long as either W. will actually be alive.TAB-e-e“ “although. most probably because of rhyme. [na-nga] en-e ní∞g-du7-e pa na-an-ga-àm-mi-in-è (var. Note: For the ‘inverted’ sequence inga-na.6. “both . . Mu-ni-na-ga-me “Whichever be his/her (= the divinity’s) name” Old Sum..

. me-e “i-in-ga-m[èn-na]. II [“préfixe de conjugaison”]. “sec­ ondly”. [inga] might be isolated as “also” in GilgHuw. would that your overwhelming fierce awe were known. as one knows. . . [inga] should not find its fixed position in table on p. (see Falkenstein 1944.2. I 91 mìn-kam-ma-“è in-ga(-)nam-mu-na-ab-bé “a second time. in a comprehensive study of the particle [inga]. . ní-mah-a-za.8. still. M. also am . 297.” SBH 56:18 f.is spelled with ‘hiatus’ above. ama “i-in-ga-an-ù-dú “will someone bring forth somebody who would be as overwhelm­ ing as Ke“. Attinger’s position will have to be submitted to further research. . nu-u“-in-ga-zu-àm. . . Cohen 1975. . as we know (“i-). or will a mother bear somebody (who would be as over­ whelming as A“gi. 27 v 7. 12. -bida. In fact. . see Attinger 1993. 29. mais une sorte de “prédi­ cat conjonctif ” (§ 90. and 22 f. ressort clairement du fait qu’il peut être aussi bien précédé que suivi du préf. 149 f. I [“préformatif ”] ni un préf. . Kè“ki-gin7 rib-ba lú “i-in-ga(-an)-túm-mu. 12. . the warrior (of Ke“))?” Ke“ Hymn 18–19 (as well as at the end of each ‘House’ section). nu-u“in-ga-¢zu-àmÜ “My lord. etc. [(i)nga] appears inseparably embedded in the prefix string of the verb. me-e “i-in-ga-mèn-[na] “I who.2. nu-u“-in-ga.with var. . . . . . as one knows. ‘Irregular’ [inga-na] Attinger 1993.7.. are also my mother” VS 2. and who. According to Attinger who rather associates [inga] with -bi.12. [nu“-inga] lugal-∞gu10 ní-hu“-rib-ba-za. I assertif (na) (comp. inga-na(m)-mu-na-be2(-en) et na-an/nam-ga-)”. E. ù “and” (p. that your unsurpassed awe were known” Ninurta G 174–177 (and see 179. Note: Only a living informant would be able to say whether or not [inga-namu-na-b-e] and *[na-nga-mu-na-b-e] were freely exchangeable. . For bibl. 149]). 92). nu-u“-in-ga. . he definitely addresses him”.e [p. But in most of the other occurrences.in UET 6/2.126   nin9-∞gu10 ama-∞gu10 “i-ga-mi-in-na “my sister who. . 181. am . 297: “Que [(i)nga] n’est ni un préf. 183). 136:96. Note: The fivefold repetition of [inga] might be rendered by “firstly”.). but note Ur Lamentation 101 nu-“i-in-ga. .12. At any rate. 112 f. .

278 f. with an antecedent not provided by [inga]. “too”. . spelled i-ri. I The Sumerian imperative is regularly formed with the verbal ¢am†u (alternatively the reduplicated ¢am†u) base without any prefixed par­ ticles.12. because we are at a loss to define its function. See. §§ 676–678. and . [] A verb-connected element [iri]. otherwise prefixed to the base. i-rí. [he-nga] “also really/verily”. Summary of [inga].”. Therefore.9. as we already know”.12.2. . 12.) 12. 297. . [na-nga] “also in fact”. (“Ni la fonction ni même la caté­ gorie grammaticale de {iri} ne peuvent être déterminées”. B-V. 6. 298. .” (rhetorical question). Simple [inga] denotes “also”. .12a. indeed. [“i-nga] “also. We are unable so far to predict the imperative of a given Sumerian verb—as against many languages (Indo-European. follow either the base itself or the base+V. for discussion and bibl. Semitic.13. 2. i. we prefer to see V as a mark of imperative as such.e. [nu-nga] “not even”.  127 12. since Poebel 1923.. i] (see 12. X Y-gin7 [inga] means “X equals Y (nominal antecedent)”. Attinger 1993.9). account for the vocalic diversity. been interpreted as a reflex of the prefixed indicator [e. Attinger 1993. We have not entered it in table on p. . This explanation does not. 7. iri. [nu“-inga] “let/may also . After Attinger’s definition (1993.. 4. 3. 112 f. however. is so far attested only before [(i)nga]. There are basically two types: 1) the unextended imperative consisting of the base and an additional vowel whose quality is unpre­ dictable to us.2). one or more (up to four) particles. 296 f. the main functions of [inga] may be tentatively summed up as follows: 1. . 5. here. “instead of ”. The element V following the base has. see above 12. 2) the extended imperative. [inga] in consecutive verbal forms means “both . cf. and others)..

79). The first of the two forms may be seg­ mentized into [∞gar-mu-(e)n] with [(e)n] as the absolutive “me” of the transitive conjugation pattern 2b (see 12. 118. help m e ”. ér “é“-a “shed tears” Inana’s Descent 42.) may be safely excluded. . This fact has been claimed as an argument for Sumerian being a language with “split ergativity”. 6. “return” Gilg. but ∞ note gar-ra [∞gar-a] “ukun “set” OBGT VI 1 (MSL 4. ní-za gé“tu AG-ì “heed yourself ” Grain and Sheep” 162 (ASJ 9. Note: The verb “to open” is ∞gál taka. The OBGT in their ∞gar = “akànum paradigm. Unextended imperative (base +V) gi4-a “go back”.4.3).. because forms like *“put yourself (in my position)” would be reflexive and require ní-zu(-ne-ne) “your­ self ”. is noted by [n] (extremely rarely) or [b]. [∞gar]-mu-ub = “u“kinanni “cause me to be set (here)” ibid. A 110 za-e gá-e ∞ dah-ma-ab “(as for) you. as in the cohortative (see 12. ∞ ki-gub-za nú-ì “lie (down) at your resting place” Copper and Silver 33 (SRT 4:33). “add it to me” in GilgHuw. *“push me”. 51. ∞ 26). gál-lu) “open the house” Inana’s Descent 75 f. Nanna’s Journey 260–264. Note: “Help me” which in our language would imply a 1st person absolutive ‘object’. 80 OBGT VI 46. The 2nd (sg. occurs as dah-ma-ab. *“push us”. and pl. but see our alternative explana­ tion above in 12. The construction is with the dative. no context examples are available for the 1st (sg. “yourselves”. 59.g.128   The plural of the Sumerian imperative is formed by the addition of [nzen] after V or [zen] after C. lit. or the imperative still preserves a verbal form that was otherwise extended to gál taka. we are at a loss to explain the relation existing between the Sumerian and Akkadian forms. [(e)nzen] is obvious. e..e. or pl. In the extended imperative.11.1. As for the second exam­ ple.7. i.13. ∞ ∞ é ∞gál-ù (var. etc.) absolutive ‘object’. VI 51. A 117. Huw. offer two examples for an imperative + “me”: ∞gar-mu-un = “uknanni “set me (here)” MSL 4. 12.7. ∞ ge“ “udun gú-ba ∞gar-ì “set the yoke on their neck” Ur-Ninurta B r. either [∞gal-u] is an irregular imperative. the absolutive ‘object’ of the 3rd sg. So far.3). “u-ni-“è gar-ì “put into her hands” Message of Ludi∞gira 7. correlation with 2nd pl..

2. 172 f. As a rule. A 110. 266. . one to three of these elements occur (the plural marker not counted). ∞ gen-na-an-zé-en [∞gen-a-(e)nzen] “go” Inana’s Descent 227. du11-ga-na(-b) [dug-a-na-b] “say it to him (there)” CIRPL N 12 iii 1. gu4-ud-an-zé-en [gud-a-(e)n-zen] “jump.13. 12. 12.2. Does it ‘rhyme’ with [b] in the preceding imperatives? ní te-ba-ab té“ tuku-ba-ab “be devoted. 12. excep­ tionally as many as four (see 12..14.2. Note: ba. The plural of the unextended imperative is relatively rare.. “úm-me-eb “give it to us” Inanna’s Descent 245.  ∞gen-na “go” Inana’s Descent 70. dance” SRT 5:42.2. tuku-ba-an [tuku-ba-n] “marry her” NG no. 269.is here a frozen indicator. be reverent (in face of my words)” Father and Son 101.g.2. dimensional indicators.4).1.13. Huw. dah-ma-ab [dah-ma-b] “add it to me” Gilg. 44.7). Two elements má-a dab5-dab5-ba-ab [dabdab-ba-b] “take (all the cattle) on the boat” SR 87:5. zi-mu-ub-zé-en [zi-mu-b-zen] “wake me” Schooldays 16. who also dissociated the exclamation [gana] (see 15. cf. e. Extended imperative The extended imperative may contain such additional information as ventive. One element ∞gìri kur-“è nú-ba-an-zé-en [nu-ba-nzen] “(lay down the feet =) slide down (on your behinds) toward the Nether World” Inana’s Descent 227. ∞gá-nu (or ∞ge26-nu?) “come on” Lugalbanda I 135. referring to implied nam-dam-“è “for marriage”. 129 Note: The two imperatives of gen (¢am†u)/du (marû) “to go” have been gener­ ∞ ally discussed by Wilcke 1969.13. Note: [b] is difficult to explain here. or absolutive. a na∞g-mu-ub-zé-en [na∞g-mu-b-zen] “give me water to drink” Schooldays 13. 6:8.

9) are used. Note: Neither AHw.2. B-[eda]. (3) infinitives. there is no negated imperative in Sumerian. 106).2. Instead.8) or prohibitive [naI] (12.vv. vetitive [bara] (12.11. nor CAD K s. 12. B-[a] and B-[e(d)].130   12. the distribution of the ¢am†u or marû bases is relevant. and 12. .1. [sá du11]-¢ga-ba-naÜ-ni-íb [dug-a-ba-na-ni-b] = “utak ¢“idsumÜ “cause him1 to be brought close to him2” OBGT IX 40 (MSL 4.3 for the conjugation pat­ terns of the Sumerian verb. Cf. there is a ‘hybrid’ type (see 4) where particles of possession (see 5. B-B-[Ø]. ka“àdu (“utak“udu) has offered a convinc­ ing translation of these Akkadian imperatives. 2nd.7. B-[ada]. Three elements ur4-ur4-u4-mu-na-ab [ururu-mu-na-b] “collect it for him” SRT 12:60. 298 f. and (4) a con­ jugated participle. ∞gi“kim-a-ni [e-ne-ra] du11-mu-na-ab [dug-mu-na-b] “give [him] a sign (or: an identification) of his” ELA 496. or pl. It seems practical to call (2) participles. Attinger 1993. B-[ede]. As in many languages.4. 12. 12.3. with lit.13. or 3rd person.2. without or with copula. There are three types (see 1–3) of non-finite verbal forms. 105). Four elements ¢sá du11Ü-ga-àm-mu-¢na-ni-íbÜ [dug-a-mu-na-ni-b] = ¢“utak“iÜda““um “cause him1 to be brought in close contact with him2” OBGT IX 52 (MSL 4. N-   Non-finite verbal forms lack any of those prefixes or suffixes by which state or action can be linked to a ‘subject’ of 1st. (1) (2) (3) (4) B-[Ø]. Moreover. Both the Sumerian and the Akkadian forms look like learned scribal efforts—as do some other forms in the sá-du11(-g) = ka“àdu section of OBGT.13.14.7. ‘Conjugated’ participle or ‘pronominal conjugation’. In all except (1). é-di∞gir-re-e-ne nigin-na-ma-ni-ib [nigin-a-ma-ni-b] “go around the houses of the gods for my sake” Inana’s Descent 36. sing.2) occur.7.11. 12.

For more examples see Edzard 1972. sá-du11(-g) = sattukku “arrival” = “(regular) delivery” (cf. ki-tu“ “place sit” = “dwelling place”. ∞ge“-hur = gi“¢urru “wood carve” = “engraving. 188 ff.8 “Der kur-gul-gul-Typus (freie Redup­ likation)”. the section dInana za-a-kam “it is yours. B-[Ø].1. ki-nú “place lie down” = “bed”. with a pre-verbal element sá “be equal. 192: 140 f. design. Krecher 1978. 6. ù∞g-ge-en-ge-en “establishing the people”. . Note: bala has neither the suffix -a nor -e(d). kù nu-zu(-ù)-ne “(people) not knowing silver”.. the substantive may be seen as an object dependent on the verb. without any further affix. kù-dím “silver form” = “(who forms silver =) silver-smith”. 108. kur gul-gul “destroying the (foreign) countries”. 388). The loanwords in Akkadian.:12 “(Der mah-di-Typus und) der dub-sar-Typus”. 9 fn. Note: In Innin “agura. 12. 5 f. ground plan”. O Inana” (Sjöberg 1975. lines 115 ff.). ní∞g-kur-du11-du11 = ni5-in-kur-du-tu “constantly saying hostile things”. See Edzard 1972.). e. because ∞ge“ is not ‘governed’ by the verb hur.14. “deriding” (= epè“ namûtim “derision”) Inin “agura 159. dub-sar “tablet write” = “(who writes tablets =) scribe”.5) which occurs in the ¢am†u variant (if there is any). In our translation. ki-gub “place stand” = “position”. In some compounds of the type noun—B. “ár-“ár (= “utàbulum) “to interchange” (p. B-B-[Ø]: unextended bases 131 The unextended verbal base (single or reduplicated) occurs by itself as an abstract lexical unit and as such it is the form regularly used for entries in the lexical lists. comparable” (cf. The unextended base may be joined to a preceding substantive (in the absolutive case). Attinger 1993. the noun cannot be interpreted as an object depending on B: ki-bala “place transgressing” = “rebellious land”. This is not the dub-sar type. Note: Reduplicated B-B (base-base) has to be understood as the ‘free’ (non-marû) reduplication of the base (cf. This becomes clear from the spelling ki-bala-e sá-di “conquering the rebellious land” Gungunum 1:7. 642 f. The clearest example is supplied by du11-du11 from the ¢am†u base of the verb du11(-g)/e/di. tup“arru or kuttimmu. The unextended bases are cited here in the manner of entries in a lexical list.g.) contains verbs cited both as B(-e)-dè and as unex­ tended forms. show that the verb actually stands as the base by itself.  12.

Moreover.132   All these compounds appear to be ‘archaic’ formations which had already ceased to be productive in Old Sumerian.14. is not a formation of its own. 2–5. ka“ na∞g-∞gá [na∞g-a] “who has drunk beer” Lugalbanda B 24. §rú-dè [§ru-(e)d-e] “to build”. B-[a] Mes-lam-ta-è-a [e-a] “the one having come out of Meslam (sanc­ tuary)” (name of a god).1). proposed by Edzard 1972.g. but simply corresponds to B-[ed]. 19f. d . B in B-[ed] by the marû base. Note: The “mah-di-Typus”. [ed] has four allomorphs: (1) [ed] after a consonant (including H. e.7. and the first [e] of [ene]. B-[a]. cf. ba-ra-ba-zah-e-dè-en [bara-ba-zah-eden] “I swear I will not run away (again)”.1. e. the [e] of [ed] may be assim­ ilated to a preceding (first) vowel in the same way as the [e] of the verbal particles [en]. “úm-mu(-d) [“um-e(d)] “someone who will give”. without or with copula In view of the function which they assume in Sumerian syntax. B-[a] and B-[ed] may be called participles.14. (2) [(e)d] after a vowel and before a vowel.2). 3.. B in B-[a] is regularly represented by the ¢am†u base of the verb. [enzen]. Whereas [a] undergoes no variation.14. and contrasted there with the “dub-sar-Typus” (see above 12. 12.g. Both formations remained productive throughout.14. A xxvi 25. e.1 and 12.. 8 f.1. (4) [(e)(d)] ge4-ge4 [gege(e)(d)] “someone who will return”.g. (3) [e(d)] after a consonant in final position. “Der sá-du11(-g)-Typus”. Compare eme è-è(-d) [ed-e(d)] “(snake) dart­ ing (its) tongue in and out” Gudea Cyl.2. mes-Ane-pada construction 12.) and before a vowel.2 p. kù lá-a-bi-im [laH-a-bi-(a)m] “(NN) was the one weighed the silver” Forde Nebraska 63:13.7. B-[ed]. 12.1. An-gin7 dím-ma [dim-a] “made like An” (first line and title of Return of Ninurta to Nibru).1. nam tar-re-dè [tar-ed-e] “to make a binding promise”. e.. [e“]. [ed] marking the marû participle is most probably identical with the [ed] occurring in the extended conjugation patterns 1 and 2a (see 12. B-[a].2.. [enden]. Edzard 1972.2.g. See above “úm-mu [“um-e(d)].

that the directive case in no other instance can be shown to express the ‘ablative of agent’ (“done by someone”).1. therefore. 2 ii 6–7. Edzard 1972. see also pre-Sargonic Lú-pà (for Lú-pad-a) in Struve 1984. The mes-Ane-pada construction A special use of B-[a] is found in the syntagma labelled “mes-Anepada” construction by Falkenstein 1949. The originator of the gift. exam­ ples of intransitive participles by far outweigh transitive ones. dumu Eriduki-ge dú-da “child born by Eridu” Gudea Cyl. The participial verbal element of the construction.2. 135. see Limet 1968. . é-ninnû An-né ki ∞gar-ra “the Eninnû founded by An” Gudea Cyl. . -n-pà(-d) “An chose the young male”. Note: The closeness (and original identity?) of ergative and directive seems to become clear from the syntactical ambiguity of the construction which we may translate either “whom An chose” or “chosen (next to =) by An”. is expressed in the genitive. [∞ge“tu “um-a] is identical with dumu-ki-á∞g-∞gá [dumu kia∞g-a] “beloved child”.14. name of an Early Dynastic ruler of Ur. . It should be stressed. Edzard 1972. 255 ff. “úm-ma. 2. the verb in the B-[a] participle may in principle be either transitive or intransitive. seen statistically. However. 12–14. Enki. The full mes-Ane-pada construction may be reduced by the sup­ pression of one or two of its components: 1) Suppression of the absolutive: hur-sa∞g-e dú-da “born by the Mountain” Gudea frag. 486. is extended by the addition of an ‘object’ (∞gé“tu) in the absolutive case.) . passim. Mes-An-né-pà-da [mes an-e pad-a]. Thomsen 1984. ∞gé“tu-“úm-ma. 2) Suppression of the ergative: Lú-pà-da “person chosen (by .2. 8+3+5+4 iv' 3' (RIME 3/1. . It is a nominalized form of the phrase *An-e (erg. and the whole complex is in the ergative. where transformational rules are given. literally means “young male chosen by (the agency of ) An (ergative)”.  133 As shown above.)” Ur III PN. 103). 3) Suppression of absolutive and ergative: gé“tu “úm-ma in gé“tu∞ ∞ “úm-ma-dEn-ki-ka-ke4 “the one gifted with (ear =) wisdom of Enki” = “the one whom Enki gifted with wisdom” Ean. however.) mes (absol. 117. with fn. ninta-zi dUtu-ù [Utu-e] ní∞g-si-sá sag -e-e“ rig7-ga “the trustworthy ˆ man whom the Sun god presented with righteousness” ”ulgi D 5. 10–12. A ii 16. and 1950. is not “given wisdom”. 35 f. but someone gifted with wisdom although. . formally. A ix 11. 12.

simple or reduplicated (cf. dah-he-dam “to be added” YOS 4. B-[ede]. 214:42. B-[ed]. Sallaberger). A 661:3 an ergative Nam-tar-DU-e is noted (courtesy W. èn-bi tar-re-dam “to be (asked =) re-examined”.2. B-[ed] Nam-tar-re(-d) “(the god) is making a firm promise”. . 18:10. will each (have to) prove it”. ii 3. or whether we have to do with a different name.”.”. “is to . SR 65 iii 4. In lexical lists.2. B-[ada] The Sumerian verb has no infinitive form which serves for citation. ma“kim-di(-d) “functioning as a inspector” Ukg. d 12. Note: In Yang Zhi 1989 no. in spite of the pl. verbs are quoted with their base.14. Postgate).14. B-[ed+copula] 12.2.—(by them) it is to be established” (suggestion of J. N. There probably was no form *ge-né(-d)-me-é“ *[gen-ed-me-(e)“]. abbreviated PN. Or there is an anakoluthon: “A. but it sometimes is a matter of subjective interpretation how a given form should be classified. B-[ed+copula] En-líl húl-le(-d)-me-en “you are one who pleases Enlil” ”ulgi D 31. 135:2. The same person occurs in the ergative in the same document: Nam-tar-re-dè [namtar-ed-e] ibid. kù má“ ∞gá-∞gá-dam [∞ga∞ga-(e)d-am] “it is silver yielding interest” NG no.2. final note in Ur III accounts when the calculation made by the scribe still has to be checked.’s wife and M.134   12. ù M. 37:6.14. It is uncertain whether a secondary ergative formation of Namtare is implied: Nam-tar-ré-e. gà-la nu-dag-ge(-d) “not dilatory” I“mè-Dagàn A 33.14. B-[edam] not unfrequently has a modal connotation: “has to . 36–40. .1). ge-né-dam “A.’s wife and M.2. Edzard 1967. corresponding to the Akkadian pattern paràsum. subject.2. dam-A. . will (have to) prove it” NG no. su-su-dam “to be replaced” UET 3. ní∞g-nu-kúr-ru-dam “it is something that will not be altered” Ninurta C iii 9. B-[eda].3.’s wife and M. above 12.1. 12. Note: [gen-ed-am] is in the sing. . 4–5 vi 35. .14.2. Maybe usage of the conjunction ù “and” caused the form to be understood in a distributive way: “A.

took with him one mina of refined silver in order to buy (blind men =) garden workers” Nik. 293 i 1–5. . epè“am rè“ì“u ullâm “(he § definitely ordered me) to build the wall of Sippar (and) to raise its head” RIME 4.. however. . . although the proposed morphological analysis would not favour a parallel arangement of the respective forms. Enlil. B-[eda].g. The reading of -NE as -dè is substantiated by spellings -te (e.3. the subject implied in B-[ede] and the subject of the finite verb following B-[ede] are identical. . bàd-Zimbirki rú-ù-da sa∞g-bi íl-i-da = dùr Z. Identity of subject of . and B-[ada] are treated together here because of their similar syntactic behaviour. 28–29 A vi 9–20. then. . A morpho­ logical analysis B-ed-e is obvious. / The three Sumerian forms. . Moreover. and [ada] which are rendered by Akkadian infinitive constructions in (OB) bilingual texts. An dEn-líl dEn-ki dNin∞ mah-bi nam-bi ha-ba-an-tar-re-e“ “An.  135 There are. u4 “u-bala AG-dè ge“-hur ha-lam-e-dè . . forsake the (preordained) plans” Lament Sumer and Ur 1–55.g. . a-“àa“a5 tùmu-dè an-ta bala-e-da . 702:2). the subjects implied are all in the ergative. 190:120) or -de1 (e. In all these examples and others. 335 Hammu-ràpi 2:22–23 / 23–25. see Cyl. [eda]. 10 ì-nun : sìla ∞ge“gigir-e AG-dè “10 pounds of butter to (make =) use for (lubricating) (at) the chariot (PN received)” BIN 8. 12. formally.. Note: There are in this passage 51 more B-[ede] forms which depend on nambi ha-ba-an-tar-re-e“. A xx 23. literally mean “at being buying”. merchant. . . who would cross over here from up there in order to (carry away =) seize fields” Ent. l ma-na kù-luh-ha igi-nu-du8-a sa10-sa10-dè U. Enki.. dam-gàr. and Ninmah have definitely decreed . the . B i 11 (var. .g. e. NATN 1. sa10-sa10-dè [sasa-(e)d-e] “in order to buy” would. . -ke4 ba­ de6 “U. Sjöberg 1975. and there is no B-[eda] variant. B-[ede]. . lú-Ummaki-a . bases with suffixed [ede]. u6).14. at least in NS and OB. B-[ede] B is always represented by the marû base of the verb. constitute a marû participle B-[ed] with the addition of the directive particle [e]. . dEn-líl-le héha-lam-me “may Enlil destroy (any) person of Umma . This would. “at being a buyer”. d E4-nun-na ù di-dè im-ma-“u4-“u4-ge-é“ “the Enùna stopped here to gaze in awe” Gudea Cyl.. 320:7–9. that they would overturn the (appointed) time.1..

2..14. so that B-[eda] is clearly set off against B-[ede] (see 12. 307). or even B-[edam]. é-a-ni §rú-da ma-an-du11 “(Ningirsu) told me (to build =) I should build his house” Gudea Cyl. different manuscripts are occasion­ ally inconsistent in their use of B-[ede].14. . 21.136   B-[ede] and following verb does not—as a rule—occur with B-[eda] for which see 12. it cannot be analysed as *B-ad-a because we know of no morpheme *[ad].3. 43.14. A iv 20. B vi 16–23. B is always represented by the marû base of the verb.1). B-[eda] would. Note: The analysis of B-[ada] as B-a-da may challenge our analysis of B-[eda] as [B-ed-a] (12. In analogy to B-[ede]. 43. it would only be logical to define B-[ada] as the ¢am†u participle with the addition of a case particle. Edzard 1967. en dNin-∞gír-su-ra me-ni∞ da mu-na-da-dib-e “(Gudea) brings Ig-alim along with himself (and introduces him) to the lord Ningirsu . 25–27. Since we explain B-[ede] and B-[eda] as marû participle con­ structions.3. Edzard 1967. Krecher 1978. formally. . . Here.3. B in B-[ada] is always represented by the ¢am†u base of the verb. gidri-u4-sù-§rá “u ∞gá-∞gá-da . then. opened the way to this explanation. In all the examples (numbering 25) the subject implied in B-[eda] and the subject of the following verb are different. B-[eda] As with B-[ede]. . as with B-[ede]. B-[eda]. 12. In OB Sumerian literary texts.14. 307. see Edzard 1997. dIg-alim . 92–94. . comita­ tive [da] offers itself. that he (Ig-alim) hand over a sceptre for long days” Gudea Cyl. a morphological analysis B-ed-a is obvious.2). B-[ada] forms are always intransitive or passive and never have an ergative subject. .3. with locative [a]. constitute a marû participle B-[ed] with the addition of the locative particle [a]. B-[ada] Although B-[ada] occurs in contexts where it is clearly syntactically paralleled with B-[eda] [B-ed-a]. note Attinger’s reservations (1993. Note: Many parallel examples for B-[eda] in Gudea Cyl.14. so that the awareness of the distinction of the respective forms may have been lost. However. fn. 401 f.3. for a recent trans­ lation. and lead to propose *[B-ed-da] instead .3. 266. They were listed in Edzard 1972. but see the reservations made by Attinger 1993.2. B vi 11–x 17. 12. therefore. Thomsen 1984.

to whom Falkenstein refers treats none of the examples quoted below.13) suggesting -ne rather than -dè as had been assumed by most authors until recently. Note: F. Contrast (a) eri-ni . spelling -ni (see 12. 12. 12. introduced the term “pronominale Konjugation” for participles which are ‘conjugated’ by the addition of particles ex­ pressing possession. the best part of it. 84. settlements be founded” Gudea Cyl. with var. ku4-ra-∞gu10-ne ku4-ra-zu-ne ku4-ra-ni ku4-ra-bi not attested not attested ku4-ra-ne-ne [kur-a-∞gu-ne] “when I entered” [kur-a-zu-ne] [kur-a-(a)ni] [kur-a-bi] [kur-a-(a)nene] .4. 2nd pl. . who took the term to be a misnomer.2 ff. the close contact between B-[ede] and B-[eda] rather favours the interpretation given above. § Edzard 1972.14. B vi 14 with (b) eri rú-a-da ki-tu“ § ∞gar-ra-da “that cities be built. 3rd sg. The forms in question consist of (1) a ¢am†u or marû participle. p. in (a) “úm-mu-da there is a (hidden) ergative subject whereas (b) rú-a-da and ∞gar-ra-da have none. 1st pl.” Gudea Cyl. with many more examples for B-[ada]. Conjugated participle or “pronominal conjugation” Falkenstein 1949. . of an element -NE of unknown origin or function.. While we cannot exclude such an analysis. Wilcke 1990. be subject to inspection” Gudea Cyl. because “conjugation” should be restricted to the finite verb. 2nd sg. but still lacking a convincing morphological analysis. and esp. na-de6 “úm-mu-da [“um-ed-a] “that he might inspect his city . 3rd sg.4. RA 32 (1935) 108 f. 5. B-[a] or B-[ed]. . B xii 19. . Thureau-Dangin. plus (2) an additional [a] after the marû participle plus (3) a particle of possession (cf. The term has found wide acceptance in spite of the argument of Jacobsen 1988. 3rd pl. B xii 8–9.14.2). 496 with fn.  137 of [B-ed-a]. and (4) in the 1st and 2nd persons. non-p. 130 f. The paradigm reads: ¢am†u 1st sg. gú-eden-na eden-du10-ge na-de6 “úm-ma-da “that the Edge-of-theSteppe. Here.. 149. 25–29.14.

14. occurred. (and) when you will rise again. the people will also rise with you” Lugalbanda I 235 f. non-p.4. 2nd pl. (from =) on the royal dais” ”ulgi A 56.).14. and with marû participle of something about to happen. p. ¢am†u: nú-a-zu-ne ù∞g “i-mu-e-da-nú-dè zi-zi-da-zu-ne ù∞g “i-mu-e-da-zi-zi “when you (O Sungod) have (lain down =) gone to sleep. 604. A much-quoted contrastive example is ku4-ra-ni “when she had entered (her bedroom)” Gudea Cyl. mí du11-ga-zu-ni “when you praised” Cohen CLAM II 503:33.1. occur.14. v 4.138 marû 1st sg. occurring—each time in anticipation of a following finite verbal form or a noun with copula. 2nd sg. 1st pl.GUB-ta ní íl-la-∞gu10-ne “having presented myself.4. Note: The late version has mè!?-a du11-ga-zu-ne = [t]à¢aza ina lapàtika “when you (touch =) engage in the battle” (Akk. u4-da kur-“è gen-na-∞gu10-ne “when today I will have gone away ∞ to the Nether World” Inana’s Descent 33. 3rd sg. the people will also go to sleep with you. p. 3rd pl. 1st sg. or in course of happening. B v 12 versus ku4-ku4-da-ni “when (the warrior) was about to enter (or: was entering) (his house)” ibid.10 for du-ne-ne In general. cf. . 12. but see 12. 2nd sg. za-e gál-la-zu-ne “now that you have been (back) again” Lugalbanda ∞ I 227.   ku4-ku4-da-∞gu10-ne [kuku-(e)d-a-∞gu-ne] ku4-ku4-da-zu-ne [kuku-(e)d-a-zu-ne] ku4-ku4-da-ni [kuku-(e)d-a-(a)ni] ku4-ku4-da-bi [kuku-(e)d-a-bi] not attested not attested not attested. 12. 3rd sg. the forms with ¢am†u participle express the idea of something having happened.). piri∞g-gin7 KI-LUGAL.2.4. Attinger 1993. clad in awe. ¢am†u: nígin-na-∞gu10-ne “when I had turned around (heaven. turned around the earth)” Inana/Ebe¢ 25 (see 26 f.

. 12. [gú ídBu]ranuna-kù-ga-ka §rú-a-bi [ídBu]ranuna a na8-na8-da-bi “when (the tree) had been planted at [the bank] of the sparkling Euphrates. Enkidu and the Netherworld 28–29.14. marû: ku4-ku4-da-zu-ni “when you are about to enter” CT 42 no. . A 1:12.14.5. Le clergé d’Ur (1986) 282 f. me-lim5-nam-lugal-la mu4-mu4-da-zu-ne “when you robe yourself with the splendour of kingship” Samsu-iluna 50. see below 12. . of -ne) see 12. C. enkara . ku4-ra-ni “when she entered” (see above p. 12. 335:8.11..13.14. A iv 15–16. . A xix 17. te-∞ge26-e-da-∞gu10-ne “when I was approaching” Letter Coll. when it was drinking water (in =) from the Euphrates” Gilg. see RTC 330:7.)” TUT 213 r. a-ba me-a-nu a-ba me-a-ni “(who not being. Note: For the element -ni (var.4.6.4. Note: For discussion of the element -ne see 12. 3rd pl. 3rd sg.14. see D. kur-“à-ga du7-du7-da-∞gu10-ne “when I am goring at the innermost part of the foreign country” Inana/Ebe¢ 28. enormous as the earth” Gudea Cyl. AfO 23 [1970] 84 ff. zi-zi-da-∞gu10-ne “when I get up” Schooldays 18. marû: u4 zal-la-∞gu10-ne e-ne di-da-∞gu10-ne “when I spent the day. non-person class ¢am†u: sig4 é-“è sa∞g íl-la-bi “as (more and more) bricks raised (their) heads =) were piled up for the house” Gudea Cyl.4. 12. Wilcke. 2nd sg.).4. when I was playing around” = “when I spent the day playing around” TMH NF 3.3.14. 25:1 (cf. The verb “to come. 138).4. 3rd sg. who his/her being =) who was it at all?” Gudea Cyl.14. 12. 1st sg.4. person-class ¢am†u: an-gin7 ri-ba-ni ki-gin7 ri-ba-ni [rib-a-(a)ni] “(someone who) was enormous as the skies. zà-ga-na lá-a-ni “having tied the e. 7.: 7 (= Rìm-Sin D).14. . person class ¢am†u: gen-na-ne-ne “when they had gone (to take away .13. 3 ii 17.4. A iv 23.4.-weapon to (the king’s) side” Martu A 49.7.  139 12. go” in Ur III administrative (messenger) texts merits special attention because of its irregular behaviour.14.4. Charpin.

140

 

e-sír-ra dib-bé-da-zu-ne “when you walk along the street” Father and Son 31. a-“à ur4-ru-da-zu-ne “when you are working the field with the seeder-plough” Farmer’s Instructions 48. 12.14.4.8. 3rd sg. person class marû: um-mi-a . . . èn tar-re-da-ni “when the headmaster . . . was asking” Schooldays 28. ku4-ku4-da-ni “when he was about to enter” (see above p. 138). For marû du-ni (instead of *du-da-ni) see below 12.14.4.11. 12.14.4.9. 3rd sg. non-person class marû: e-bi bala-e-da-bi “when (Umma) is about to cross this dyke-and-canal” Ean. 1 r. v 38.
Note: This example, which would be the oldest available, is not unambiguous because OS spelling prevents us from knowing whether a further (unwritten) syllable/particle followed bala-e-da-bi.

na8-na8-da-bi see above 12.14.4.4. dal-le-e-da-bi [dal-ed-a-bi] “when (my sling-stones) are flying” ”ulgi B 37. 12.14.4.10. 3rd pl. person class marû: hur-sa∞g-umun7-kam-ma bala­ e-da-n[e!-ne . . .] “when they were crossing the seventh mountain range” Gilg. Huwawa A 62.
Note: This is the var. of ex. NiAA (see Edzard 1991, 188:62). The other ex.s have bala-e-da-bi, bala-e-d[a-n]i, bala-da-ni, bi-ri-bala.

12.14.4.11. Irregular behaviour of the verb gen/du ˆ The verb gen (¢am†u)/du (marû) “to come, to go” has a partially ∞ irregular behaviour with the conjugated participle. Whereas ∞gen-na∞gu10-ne (12.14.4.1) or ∞gen-na-ne-ne (12.14.4.5) are regular ¢am†u for­ mations, we find du-ni instead of *du-(e)da-ni and du-ne-ne instead of *du-(e)da-ne-ne. Ki-ma“ki-ta du-ni “when he was arriving from Kima“” HSS 4, 58:9 (see 12, r. 3 and 6). ”u“anaki-“è du-ni “when he was leaving for Susa” HSS 4, 58:3 (see 6, r. 9 and 12). du presents here an unextended marû participle du, i.e., without the addition of [ed]. Clear evidence for such a marû participle is offered by Enmerkar 157: lú-kin-gi4-a kur-“è du-úr [du-r(a)] “to the messenger travelling to the foreign country”. du-ne-ne “when they were leaving (for ”ima“gi)” HSS 4, 56 r. 10.

 

141

Note: There are spelling variants du-ni-ni ITT 3, 6332 r. 4; HLC 2, 58 85 r. 8; du-ni-ne RA 19, 43 XCII 7; in the latter form, the scribe probably added— mechanically—pl. -(e)ne to sg. du-ni.

For plenty more Ur III examples (as of 1955) see T. Fish, MCS 5, 13–26; also Edzard 1972, 17–19. 12.14.4.12. deli-g u10-ne etc. ˆ The formations B-a-∞gu10-ne etc. have an unexpected parallel in a nominal compound where number “one” occurs in the position of a verbal base: deli-ni “he/she alone”, e.g., Angin dima 197. deli-∞gu10-ne “I all alone” Lugalbanda II 328. deli-zu-ne “you all alone” Lugalbanda II 356. Most interestingly, the variant spelling -ne/-ni is shared by B-(d)agu10-ne/-ni and deli-∞gu10-ne/-ni. For discussion see 12.14.4.13. ∞ deli-zu-ne mah-me-en “(Suen, . . .) (of/in) your sole self, you are the greatest” ZA 63 (1973) 32 no. 5:20. deli-zu-ni mah-me-en “(Nuska, . . .) (of/in) your sole self you are the greatest” ZA 63, 17 no. 3a:11.
Note: Å. Sjöberg, not yet realizing the parallel between -ne and -ni, read dili-zu ì-mah-me-en.

delili-zu!-ni mah-me = e-di-“e-ka ßí-ra-ta VS 2, 89 obv.(?) 7' / 9'. /
Note: I owe these—and more—examples to the late H. Behrens.

12.14.4.13. Conjugated participles: unresolved questions Taken by themselves, 3rd sg. and pl. person class B-[a]-(a)ni, B-[a](a)nene as well as B-[ed]-ani, B-[ed]-anene would most easily be analysed as the ¢am†u or marû participles + possessive particles [ani] or [anene]. Such an analysis is not compatible, however, with 1st and 2nd sg. marû B-[ed]-a-∞gu10/-zu-ne or with 3rd sg. non-person class marû B[ed]-a-bi. Here, before the possessive particle an element [a] is inserted which would remind us, at first glance, of the supposed locative par­ ticle in B-[ed]-a (see 12.14.3.2). But addition of a possessive particle after a (dimensional) case particle would disagree with the supposed hierarchy of suffixed nominal morphemes in Sumerian which is: possession—number—case, e.g., ses-∞gu10-(e)ne-da “with my brothers” (see above, 8). Therefore, the element [a] in question might as well be of a different origin, with a different function. Or are B-[ed]-a-∞gu10­ ne, B-[ed]-ani, B-[ed]-a-bi, of different origin, converging to form a common paradigm, disregarding rules of suffix hierarchy?

142

 

The second problem is the suffix -ne (var. -ni) added after pos­ sessive particles -∞gu10, -zu. The former reading -dè instead of -ne has been disproved by the variant spelling -ni, attested both synchroni­ cally with -ne and diachronically as a later variant.
Note: C. J. Gadd, Iraq 22 (1960) 161 f., note to line 7, (“-ni must arise from reading NE (regular here) as ne instead of dè”). The variant was also noted by Aro 1961, 327 fn. 1 (“Auffälligerweise ni statt NE (dè) geschrieben”). It was dis­ cussed by Attinger 1993, 107 and 311, but without definite conclusion.

To sum up, the function of this [ne, ni] is still unknown, and we cannot explain why it occurs only in forms of the 1st and 2nd person. A completely analogous distribution of presence or absence of [ne/ni] is found in deli-∞gu10/zu-ne “on my/your own”, but deli-ni “on his/her own” (see 12.14.4.12). 12.15. C  By “compound verbs” Sumerian grammars traditionally understand frozen combinations of a noun (mostly a substantive, rarely an adjec­ tive) and a verbal base. The meaning of the compound is not a sim­ ple addition of the meanings of the individual elements, but a new one: A+B = C or A+B ≠ A+B. This very often becomes clear from the Akkadian translation, e.g., ki á∞g, equated with râmu “to love”, cannot be explained by combining the meanings “earth, ground” (ki) and “to measure out” (á∞g).
Note: A. Falkenstein, orally, suggested a gesture of reverence, making a gener­ ous move of the hand/arm towards the ground, as the origin of ki á∞g.

Postgate 1974, 35, proposed as “the most specific criterion for dis­ tinguishing a compound verb from a ‘simple’ one” the position of the nominal element immediately before the verbal complex. The nominal element may be extended by an adjective—or, occasionally, by a dependent genitive—but no independent word may enter between the nominal element and the verbal complex. Thus, ki mu-ra-á∞g-en “I love you” would not allow insertion of za-ra as in *ki za-ra mura-á∞g-en “It is you I love”. Civil 1976, 148 f., and Attinger 1993, 179, have been sceptical about the criterion A+B ≠ A+B, “difficilement utilisable dans le cas d’une langue morte aux catégories de pensée étrangère aux nôtres”. In fact, ∞gé“tu(-ga-ni) gub may be taken either literally as “to set (one’s) (ear =) mind” or, on a more developed meaning, “to intend/plan to do”.

 

143

Attinger 1993, 179, showed that there are two types of compound verbs: 1) Both the nominal and the verbal element have an origi­ nally independent status; 2) the verbal element serves as a verbal­ izer; the latter function is essentially restricted to du11(-g)/e/di “to say, do” and AG “to do, make”. While (1) may be said to be a closed class, limited by practical usage, (2) may be called an open class. So, e.g., the type nam-na∞gar AG “to (do carpentry =) act as a carpenter” could theoretically be extended to as many nam-x for­ mations as there are.
Note: In Akkadian corresponds . . . -ùta(m) epè“u(m) for which CAD E 201–225 offers over 50 examples.

Attinger 1993 listed as many as 213 examples for the verbalizer x(y) du11(-g)/e/di. Karahashi 2000, 2–10, has reviewed former discussions of the topic, between 1908 and 1993; add Postgate 1974, 35–40.
See, moreover, Thomsen 1984, 269–72; Attinger 1993, 178–82; Krecher 1993, 107–18.

12.15.1. Compound verbs: free formations Karahashi 2000 treated the subject with body-part terms as nomi­ nal elements, excluding formations with du11(-g)/e/di and AG (see 12.15.2). On pp. 72–108 she listed over 130 compound verbs. The main problem with the syntactical interpretation is the func­ tion of the nominal element: is it in the absolutive or may it also stand in a different case? Among Karahashi’s examples, there are 91 (out of 130, i.e., 70%) with a nominal element which (in our Latin transliteration) ends in a vowel: á “arm, side”, gaba “breast, chest”, gìri “foot”, gú “neck”, gù “voice”, igi “eye, front”, ki “place”, ∞ ní “self ”, su “body, flesh”, “u “hand”, zi “breath”, zú “tooth”. All these may, theoretically, be in the absolutive, or in the directive (with the [e] assimilated). So, “u ti/te(-∞g) “to receive, to take” could be interpreted either as “to make (an object) (absolutive) come close to the hand (directive)” or as “to make the hand (absolutive) come close (to an object)”. See below, p. 145f., for “u ti/te(-∞g). On the other hand, a nominal element ending in a consonant, e.g., dùg “knee”, ∞gé“tu(-g) “ear”, há“ “thigh”, sa∞g “head”, “à(-g) “heart, middle part”, is clearly recognizable as an absolutive if it stands by itself. “à(-g) hu∞g “to soothe the heart” can only have [“a∞gØ] as the nominal element, to the exclusion of *[“age] or *[“aga].

If the nominal element is in the absolutive. B vii 32–33. é-e gál ba-an-taka “he (loosened the . the “object” in the directive [(e)ri]. Note: For “u bar (< ba§r) see Krecher 1993. 113. Whatever the original meaning of ∞ge“ tag (touching wood in a ceremony?). Gordon. 5. Put the other way round. let me make a benevolent firm promise for you” ”ulgi D 384 f. the ordi­ nances of the Netherworld are absolutely perfect” Inana’s Descent 132. tukumbi “u mu-ri-bar-re(-n) “if I set you free” Prov. the “object” of “u bar “to set free” is in the directive [(e)ri]. I. It was also used for the abstract notion gál “to be. JCS 12 [1958] 46).8). this means that if a second “object” (or “subject”) is in fact in a dimensional case. zà(-g) “side” is in the absolutive and lugal-ni in the directive. and “u stands in the absolutive. ∞ nam ge4-rí-íb-tar nam-du10(-g) gú-mu-rí-íb-tar “let me make a firm promise for you. ∞gál (meaning unknown) is in the absolutive. Here. it may be taken as proof that the nominal element of the compound verb is in the abso­ lutive.55:3 (E. nam is in the absolutive. . to exist”. there cannot be a sec­ ond absolutive as a second “object” (or “subject” where the mean­ ing of the verb is intransitive). the “object” in the directive. . ge“ is in the absolutive. ninda [ninda-e] ∞ge“ ha-ba-ni-tag “I (”ulgi) indeed offered (bread =) food” ”ulgi A 55. etc.144   But a more important criterion for determing the syntactical char­ acter of the nominal element is the form taken by an “object” depend­ ing on a compound verb. . It will have to stand in a dimensional case. Inana. ∞ urdu-dè lugal-ni [lugal-(a)ni-e] zà(-g) mu-da-DU-àm [mu-n-da-n-DUam] “the slave (had it with him that he set the side next to his mas­ ter =) was allowed to go side by side with his master” Gudea Stat. Coll. This argument applies equally to dimensional indicators in the verbal chain (see 12. at the house =) he opened ∞ the house”. si-a dInana me-kur-ra-ke4 “u al-du7-du7 “be silent. Note: The nominal element ∞gál is—graphically—the same as ig “door”. whether as an independent part of speech or as an infixed particle.

Here. but an unmarked adverb cannot be excluded.. The “object” me-kur-ra(-k) is in the directive. are performed for me in ritual perfection” Gudea Cyl.g. it is uncertain. the obligation to repay a loan) has no coun­ terpart in Sumerian where “u ti/te(-∞g) is used in both senses. well be called adverbial. reliable. “u ti/te(-ˆ ) and other compound verbs and the special g behaviour of their “objects” “u ti/te(-∞g) “to receive.1. see Karahashi 2000.1.1. the addition of zi after igi adds a shade of meaning to the verbal expression and may. e. the “object” of “u ti/te(-∞g) is in the directive or locative.15. Be that as it may. my ‘festival of An’.  145 The original meaning of “u du7 is probably “to move the hand directly (towards a goal)” (with du7 “to make a direct movement (towards a goal)”). It is probably to be understood in the absolutive case. A i 3. Gù-dé-a gal mu-zu gal ì-ga-túm-mu “Gudea knows much. . A x 18. to take” is by far the most frequently attested compound verb. The subtle difference between OB Akkadian ma¢àru “to receive” (without any ensuing consequence) and leqû “to receive” (with ensu­ ing consequences. mean­ ingful way”. As a rule. therefore.15. whether such an extension was a productive feature of the system or whether it was restricted in use by phraseology. A vii 9–10. due to its ubiquitous usage in legal and administrative documents. Only an informant would still be able to tell whether igi-zi bar was tantamount to zi-dè-e“ igi bar “to look in a true. For examples from literary texts. however. Extended nominal element The nominal element of a compound verb is occasionally extended by an adjective. and he (also brings great =) is able.2. to accomplish great things” Gudea Cyl. 168–71. Beside igi bar “to look (at)” there is igi-zi bar: d En-líl-e en dNin-∞gír-su-“è igi-zi mu-“i-bar [mu-n-“i-n-bar] “Enlil had directed his (true =) meaningful gaze toward the lord Ningirsu” Gudea Cyl. 12. 12. me-gal-gal ezem-An-na-∞gu10 [ezem-An-a(k)-∞gu-e] “u-gal ma-du7-du7 “all the great performances. the nominal element is an adjective. too.

134 n. became more customary. and C. dNuska á-á∞g-∞gá-dEn-líl-lá “u ba-an-ti-a-ta “after . . Wilcke. or simply kiPN. “ù“uru in the case of si sá (Falkenstein’s position). but by Ur III times with ki-PN(-a(k))-ta. . (2) Or—more probably in view of the Sumero-Akkadian lin­ guistic area—it was due to the influence of the syntax of the underlying Akkadian verb. sa10/sa10-sa10. The “subject”. For an exhaustive treatment of the compound verb si sá see C. by Ur III also the notation of the person from whom something was received had undergone a change—at least in juridical and administrative contexts: Instead of PN-“è (see above. For this phenomenon two reasons may be adduced: (1) It was the result of an independent. cf.) “u ba-“i(-n)-ti “his master (accepted) from Gudea. see J. B iii 3–4. the influence of itti PN leqû is obvious. would be expected to be *[u“ub-kuge]. A ii 21–22. Krecher. . the nominal element and the verbal base have practically coalesced so as to behave like a simple transitive verb. Falkenstein 1950.. Gudea Cyl. A ii 21–22). . adjust. . PN-“è. This change of syntax has a clear parallel in the verb “to buy”. the “object” of “u ti/te(-∞g) is in the absolutive as if—contrarily to what one would expect—there actually were a second absolutive: . internal evolution of Sumerian morphosyntax. Occasionally.) siskur arax(DU)-zu-ni [arazu-(a)ni-e] Gù-dé-a-á“ en d Nin-∞gír-su-ke4 (erg.. In the above cases. Cyl. Nuska had received Enlil’s instructions” Enlil and Ninlil 2:44. kiPN(-a(k))-ta “from (the place of ) PN” or only ki-PN. 1 judged this as “der klarste Fall für die Beeinflussung der Konstruktion eines sumerischen Verbums durch die seines akkadischen Gegenstücks”. A A v 6. where the seller was originally noted with the termi­ native. A comparable—secondary—construction is also found with si sá “to direct. ibid.g. the lord Ningirsu accepted from him prayer and rite” Gudea Cyl. iv 1–2. or e“èru. Wilcke 2003. mostly in texts of OB tradition.146   and the person from whom something is received is originally in the terminative: lugal-a-ni (erg.”: gi“ ù-“ub-kù(-g) [s]i íb-sá “a (clean =) brand-new brick-mould was adjusted” Gudea Cyl. 505 r. in the passive construction. Here. The “object” is [a’a∞ga-Enlil-a(k)] instead of *[a’a∞ga-Enlil-ak-e]. Note: In the case of “u ti/te(-∞g). RIA 5 (1976–80) 496 r. etc. leqû in the case of “u ti/te(-∞g). e.

Krecher 1987. . . 2:16–19 (Anam of Uruk). C = STVC 17 v 2'. The underlying verb is “u gíd “to stretch out the hand (in either direction)” = “to offer. water which quenches fire has been directed there. before the string of prefixed verbal particles. e. Karahashi 2000. dumu-An-na An-x-la-ta té“ mu-u8-da-mah-di “daughter of An (= Bau).—stage of the evolution of the morpho-syntactic behaviour of compound verbs. A form *ki mu-un-∞gar “he laid the ground” has been transformed. .) 7 (Bau B). no. . Note: Does Ur III PN Íb-ta-ab-PA-è RA 8 (1911) 184 no. you alone are able to be as outstanding as An(?)” CT 36. é-∞gi6-pàr-en-na . 69.3. 40 (r. but the reason remains unknown. 4. -si-sá (see below). . the abode of the en (priestess)” RIME 4. mu-un-ki-∞gar “I(!) founded . Karahashi 2000. Moortgat.15. kù? a ní∞g-izi-te-na si mi-ni-in-si-sá ∞garza me-mah “u mi-ni-ib-“u-du7 “. Íb-ta-ab-PA-è has perhaps to be seen as a graphic variant. in general Wilcke 2003. 161 f. The nominal element is sometimes found twice with the verb si sá: “à-bi(-a) ur-sa∞g-ur-sa∞g-e-ne si mu-si-sá-e-ne “all the warriors go straightway into the interior (of the sanctuary)” Ke“ Hymn 60 (OB). Rollsiegel 3(1988) 554. the nominal element was simultaneously kept in place. si . ms. 1:10' already repre­ sent a case of incorporated pa è “to come forth radiantly”? In view of Íb-ta-abè and Íb-ta(-b)-è (see Limet 1968. 472 f. in such cases. the nominal element could be trans­ ferred to stand immediately before the verbal base. Complete incorporation of the nominal element In a final—Old Bab. ga-bi nu-mu-ra-“u-gíd-e “(the sheep) no (longer) offer their milk to you” LamUr I 362. So ms. . 431).g.  147 12.. . to receive” (cf.1. in finite and nonfinite forms. N = MBI 6 ii 48' has ga nu-mu-ra-AG-e.). . A post-OB example is hé-nir-∞gál “may he be authoritative” (PN) A. Cf. . the rites and sublime ordinances have been performed there in the most per­ fect way (for us) Flood i 44–45. redundantly. . .. Sometimes. .

etc. make a voice” = “to shout. serving as verbalizers. roar”. as Falkenstein did in his edition. however. PN1–5 enim PN6-ra in-na-an-e“-a [i-na-n-e-(e)“-a] “u-bala bí-in-AGé“ “PN1 . etc. it has been verbalized—or re-verbalized—by the addition of AG “to make”. make legal case” = “to judge”. PN5 (changed =) contradicted what they had said to PN6” NG no. -ize (English). noted that there is a group of compound verbs with the bases du11(-g)/e/di or AG essentially serving the function of turning a noun into a verb. ∞ ì“ du11(-g) “to do the penis” = “to have intercourse with a g woman”. We will only quote a few examples each for (a) evident verbal­ ization and (b) uncertain cases. 113:17–24. ma“kim du11(-g) “to do the bailiff ” = “to act as a bailiff ”. While verbalization applies to the majority of verbal compounds of the types x AG or x du11(-g)/e/di. . tu6 du11(-g) “to say an incantation” = “to conjure”. kardan “to do” (Persian). -iser (French). i 20–21.. 414–765 (alphabetical catalogue).15. i. It is realised. “free formations”. Note: No references are offered because all the evidence is amply available in Attinger 1993. morphologically.15. Note: Verbalization is a wide-spread phenomenon. A-nir du11(-g) “to say a lamentation” = “to lament”.1. sa-gaz lú mu-na [mu-n-a(k)] “someone committed a robbery here” NG no.15. For (a) note.148   12. .e.2. Semitic languages have not developed a productive way to verbalize nouns: here examples such as Akkadian ruggubu “to provide with a roof/an upper storey (rugbu)” or Arabic tal­ fana “to telephone” (extremely unpopular) are rather exceptions to the rule. Clear cases of verbalization á“ du11(-g)/e/di “to say a curse” = “to curse”. “u bala “to make the hand go across something” is a compound in itself. “to effect a change”. either (a) by a special ending attached to the noun or (b) by a special verb. [e4] pa5-bi “u-bala ba-ra-ak-ke4(-n) “I will not—I swear—change (the course) of (Girsu’s) canals and dykes” Ean. gù du11(-g) to say. A count for x AG has not yet been made. there certainly is a number of verbs which should rather be classified under 12. 121:3. our modern interpreta­ tion is often arbitrary. Attinger 1993 should be consulted for du11(-g)/e/di. yell.1. Compound verbs with a verbalizer Attinger 1993. Attinger 1993 collected 213 examples for x du11(-g)/e/di. in Indo-European. 12. 1 r. Sumerian would belong to category (b).. 179. Note: There is no need to restore mu-na<-AG>.2. In general. . -(is)ieren (German). di du11(-g) “to say. and for (b) etmek “to do” (Turkish).

but it is not certain. and see already I. 472 fn. Ean. is consistently SAG. 12. 1294) equated with the personal pronoun “he.DU]-ga-a suhu“-bi hara-ab-ge-ge-in “I (= Ninlil) will certainly consolidate for you the throne which Enki has already given you as a present” ”ulgi R 87.KA[B.2. in broken context). sá du11(-g)/e/di “to reach.DU Steible 1982 b 201: AnAgr. 2 vii 3–6. ge“ ˆ gu-za dEn-ki-k[e4] sa∞g ha-ba-ra-PA. as a present to Nin∞girsu” Ean.  149 12. 4 ii 2' (OS. Landsberger saw the origin in counting-out rhymes: “it is him. Note: While the spelling of the first. Gelb. Karahashi 2000. “to say ‘equal’” = “to measure up to something”.KAB = rig8 (see below. d En-ki-ke4 Mar-tu má“-an“e sa∞g-e-e“ mu-ni-rig7 “Enki gave the cat­ tle as a gift to the Amurru (nomad)” EWO 249.[SA]G. [sa(∞g)].[SA]G. Verbalization no longer recognizable to us al du11(-g)/e/di “to wish. her”. Note: Like with nam tar. Steible 1982 b 201). may be the appropriate etymology. [rig] offers several—diachronical—spelling variants. Difficult cases: artificial splits? sa∞g(-[e“]) [rig] “to give as a present” has been claimed as a pre-Old Akkadian loan from Akkadian “aràkum “to present” in its stative form “arik. Note: sa∞g rig7 here governs the dative (verbal infix [(e)ra]) instead of the directive—perhaps in parallel with the following [ha-(e)ra-b-gege-n]. 140 with fn. MAD 3 (1957) 284.3.KAB. 443 with fn. 93. with an artificial split of the Semitic root ”RK into [sa(∞g)] and [rig]. each one of the diri(g) type: PA. in fact. but this is uncertain. 2 vii 6). ‘nominal’. At least two arguments may be raised against a—direct—Akkadian origin of the verb in question: (1) Why was [sa∞g] chosen to represent the first syllable of alleged “arik. arrive” is derived from sá “être égal à” by Attinger 1993. instead . [sa]∞g r[i]g9(KAB. 641. a-ne/e-ne du11(-g)/e/di “to enjoy. KAB.2.15. she”. d Nin-∞gír-su-ra Lum-ma-gin7-du10 sa∞g-é“ mu-ni-rig8 “(Eannatum) gave (the canal) L. sa∞ (-[e“]) [rig] uses the directive for the (god or) per­ g son in whose favour the action is made. PA.DU = rig9 (see below. 1179. sa∞g [rig] as a split of *“arik would be quite an unusual type of borrowing. These spellings still remain unexplained. element. Summing up. to strive for” has a nominal element al with unknown etymology: see Attinger 1993. Cf. J. play” has a nominal element which Landsberger (see Attinger 1993.DU = rig7 in Ur III and OB.15.

86 f. above 4. gi“ is probably chosen not because of its literal meaning but because of its phonological proximity to ge“tú ‘ear’”. be put in the plural.(5))? For (2) one could refer to the parallel—post-OB—ní∞g-ba-a“ ba = ana qì“ti “aràku “to give as a present” (see CAD Q 156 qâ“u lex. Once such a change from verb to noun took place.TÚG. Sumerian nominalization most probably influenced Akkadian which stands out. by its great vari­ ety of infinitive constructions. ∞ -àm (see Karahashi 2000.: “in gi“-tuku. J.. the nominalizer. see her excursus on “gi“-tuku” ibid. sá? (2) Why was [sa∞g rig] most often construed with the addition of the adverbiative ending [e“(e)] (cf. 1961). For ∞gé“tu(-g) “ear” is regularly prolonged by -gV (e. 305. .16. It is not clear whether the spelling of the verbal element of [sa∞g(-e“) rig] was derived from the respective noun or vice versa. pp. the newly attained nominal complex behaves like a noun: it may be inflected for case. 26. Whatever the etymology of ∞ge“ tuku. 153 f.150   of. Thomsen 1984. The question has to be left to further research. There is hardly good reason to propose a “split” ∞gé“tu(-g) ¡ ge“ ∞ [tuG]. “Die sumerischen Entsprechungen und Vorbilder der akkadischen Infinitivkonstruktionen”. e. one would rather sug­ gest that this idiom was formed by analogy with [sa∞g-e“ rig]. Nominalization is important in Sumerian syntax because inflected complexes often stand for what we would interpret as subordinate clauses. esp. and such was not Karahashi’s intention either. gé“tu-ga-ni [∞ge“tug-ani] ∞ whereas tuku in ge“ tuku is.).2. possessive particles may be added. N     Sumerian finite verbal forms may be turned into nouns by the addi­ tion of a morpheme [a]. 84 f.PI and similar spellings) “ear” and the verb ∞ge“ tuku “to hear”. 323–32. Karahashi 2000. Die akkadischen Infinitivkonstruktionen (= StudOr. 241–50. 12. A noun rig7 “enclosed room(?)” was discussed by Krecher 1966. without exception.g. Some phonetic similarity has been noted between the noun gé“tu ∞ (-g) (GI”. it cannot be directly related to [∞ge“tug] “ear”. Aro. A nominalized complex cannot. But given the late attestation of [ni∞gba-“ ba]. p. prolonged by -a.). Attinger 1993. 86 f.g.. however. among other Semitic languages.

. ní∞g “thing”. ibid. what”. for similar state­ ments made under oath.. dam-“è ha-tuku bí-in-du11-ga PN1 PN2 nam-érim-àm “PN1 (and) PN2 swore that (PN3) declared: ‘I will take (PN4 .2) is quite improbable. The nominalized phrase is in the absolutive (or “casus pendens”). Note: Here. as a rule. lú . but with nominalized [bi-ndug] “he declared”. nam-érim-àm literally means “it is an evil (which should befall me [= the oath-taking party] in case of perjury)”. though we shall not provide examples for every case. Note: The phrase is to be understood as a vocative which is unmarked. because it is not explicitly expressed. as against the oath taken as a promise way that one would do or not do—or refrain from—something (“promissory oath”). B viii 42–43 (marû). time”. u4 “day. . morpho-syntactically.1. mu “year”. I will destroy in the foreign land” ”ulgi D 219. “thing. unmarked (-Ø) ní∞g Ki-en-gi-ra ba-a-gu(-ul)-la kur-ra ga-àm-mi-íb-gu-ul “what has been destroyed in Sumer. . lú “person”. . d E4-nun-na .14. . a-gin7 ba-e-ne-diri-ga “the Enùna . identity of particle [a] with the ending of the ¢am†u participle (§rú-a. and the nominal­ ized phrase is.16. will (let the hand pass over =) disregard his (= Gudea’s) judgment” Gudea Stat.) as a wife’” NG no. A caption 4–6 and passim in the Gudea Corpus (¢am†u). For this reason. in-§rú-a “who (had) built the Eninnû” Gudea Stat. line 335: ní∞g Ki-en-gi-ra ba-a-gu-ul-la kur-ra ì-mi-in-gu-ul. . It may be added to both ¢am†u and marû bases along with the respective conjugation pat­ terns. ní∞g. . by witnesses. usually indicated by a case particle following the nominalizer [a]. cf. .  151 The origin of nominalizer [a] is unknown. . e. . It seems most practical to arrange the material by the case in which the nominalized phrase occurs. Note: It is interesting to note that the quotation of direct speech is not made with the addition of the quotation particle [(e)“e]. 15:6–9. di-ku5-a-na “u ì-íb-bala-e-a “he who . nos.. . . see 12.g. 16:6–11. in apposition to. but often the antecedent has to be supplied. among whom you (= Inana) are so much superior” Ninme“ara 115. etc. cf. 12. 17:7–10. .. . or an adjec­ tive added to. lú É-ninnû . It is used for an oath concerning something that already happened (“assertory oath”). a head or antecedent on which it depends. The head of the phrase is ní∞g. under oath. The nominalized phrase has. . . etc. a deposition is confirmed.

I 2 i 7–ii 1. The nominalized phrase is in the locative. the phrase can only be defined—morpho-syntactically—as an apposition or an adjective joined to u4. lú E-an-na in-§rú-a-kam [i-n-§ru-a-(a)k-am] “it is the (personal) god of Gudea . . 63 ff. . resumed by the -bi of mu-lugal-bi. 99:44–46. . with the locative case particle deleted following a rule that in a sequence of parallel syntactical members only the last one will be marked by a case particle: u4(-da) . Therefore. The syntactical relation between the nominalized statement and the following nam-érim-àm is difficult to establish. enim-ama-ne-ne nu-ub-kúr-ne-a [nu-b-kur-(e)ne-a-a(k)] mu-lugal-bi inpà-dè-e“ “they invoked the royal name that they would not change the (word =) disposition of their mother” NG no. Note: In this and in many parallel cases it is not certain whether the nominalized phrase should really be understood as standing in the genitive. If we see the phrase as an apposition the head (u4) would virtually be in the locative. A xxvi 15. . . as a “casus pendens”. too.. 24:9'–12' (for = nanna see 13. the sequence an ki-a “in heaven (and) on earth” where the locative after an (*an-na = [an-a]) has been deleted: an/ /ki-a. 12.e. ì-me-é“-a-“è. .16. . i. ur-sa∞g-ug5-ga ì-me-“a-ke4-é“ [i-me-(e)“-a-(a)k-e“] “because they (= the aforementioned mythological beings) are warriors (once) slain (by Nin∞girsu)” Gudea Cyl. . -a-“è (see 12. See Falkenstein 1956.5). C i 2–5. Cf. or whether it stands in the absolutive.3). for the different formulations of “assertorischer Eid” and “promissorischer Eid”. Note: [ime“ak] is a free standing genitive to which the adverbiative ending [e“] has been added. . *mu . the one who built the Eana” Gudea Stat. The nominalized phrase is in the locative u4 dNin-∞gír-su-ke4 “à-ge ba-pà-da-a [ba-n-pad-a-a] “when Nin∞girsu had (called him in the heart =) chosen him” En. -a.16. One might say the nominalized phrase is in a casus pendens. . .16.152   PN1 PN2-na-an-na lú nu-ù-da-nú-a nam-érim-àm “PN1 swore that apart from PN2 nobody slept with me” NG no. We cannot give an answer to how [ime“ake“] was distinguished from a construction mu . The nominalized phrase is in the genitive di∞gir-Gù-dé-a . and there is no geni­ tival (regens-rectum) relation between the head (u4) of the phrase and the phrase itself.2.3. 12.

12. . . see Edzard 1997. Hammu-rapi RIME 4. En. bar “e-bi nu-da-sù-sù-da-ka [nu-n-da-n-su(d)sud-a-(a)k-a] “because he (= Ur-LUM-ma of Umma) no longer was willing to (lengthen.) u4-ba . . -a. .”. 2:20. .16. (3) . lit. The construction of the year date formulae exactly follows the pattern u4 . mu Lugal-Ba-gára é-a ba!-ku4 “the year (when) the Lord of Bagara entered (his) house” Gudea year 16. . 27. -a (see before). . “when . The head mu here behaves like a subjunction. on the other hand. the separate notation of nom­ inalizer [a] and locative case particle [a] often coalesced so that a single written [a] may stand for both. The nominalized phrase is in the loca­ tive and so is—virtually—the head mu. then . im-ti-a-ta “after he had come close to the Bagara (sanc­ tuary)” Gudea Cyl. .. . . (1) . . .. year name Gudea of Laga“ 3. mu bala∞g u“umgal-kalam-ma ba-dím-ma [ba-dim-a-a] “the year (when) the harp (called) ‘Dragon of the Land’ was fashioned”.g. e. . e. -a. “when . Moreover. . The nominalized phrase is in the ablative ba-dú-ud-dè-en-na-ta [ba-dud-en-a-ta] ninta-kal-ga-me-en “I am a mighty king since I was born” ”ulgi A 2. bar C. . 110: 3. . .  153 If. . the mu . . . (at that day =) then . . . 27. . I 20 i 9–iii 2) has its counterpart in Akkadian inu .. 335 no.g. inùmi“u . 28–29 A ii 27.g. esp. -a con­ struction was often reduced to a formula where [a] has been deleted after the verbal form. Note: The syntagma is bar-Noun-ak-a. see Edzard 1997. . . -a (etc. . The u4 . .4.” (e. A ii 7. (2) . extend =) restore that grain (with =) to him” Ent. . 2:1– 25/ /1–28. the sometimes very elaborate construction u4 . In Akkadian it has an exact parallel in the sequence inu/inùma . .”. which we have to under­ stand as “because of . . . But note still Hammu-rapi bíin-du11-ga-a RIME 4. there is no need to postulate the deletion of a case particle after u4. as against the u4 . By the time of the Ur III dynasty.. . “at the side of Noun” with the noun here represented by a verbal phrase + nominalizing [a]. . However. 335 no. p.. . . Ba-gára . -a construction. . see PSD B 109 f. . -a construction is extremely popular. we see the relation between head (u4) and phrase as a connection of substantive (u4) + adjective (nominalized phrase).

. 12a). Cf. di “u la-ba-an-ti-en-za-na [la-ba-n(i)-ti-enzen-a] enim-“è la-ba-dúrru-ne-en-za-na-ta [la-ba-durun-enzen-a-ta] “because you (pl. but with the case particle deleted after the head noun: mu[-“è] . “like . . 419–422.7.154 d   Nin-a-zu-ù . .16. See the discussion in 12. -a construction (see 12. . Sumerian mu . The nominalized phrase is in the equative lugal-ra . see 16. 24. see ibid. .5. 203:3–4.16. 12. -a.3).) did not accept the verdict (and) did not abide by the word” Enki-hegal/Enkitalu 191 (see Karahashi 2000. . . Lugal 390–392. The nominalized phrase is in the terminative mu ge“-nú-PN ba-an-zuh-a-“è “because he had stolen PN’s bed” NG ∞ no. 483. .16. 617. Note: We have to do with an extension of the u4 . 169 no. and cf.16. . 14:18–21. etc. . . . lines 19. -a-“è constructions correspond to Akkadian sub­ ordinate clauses with a““um “because”. PN3 married PN2” NG no. 12. .5 locative under u4 . . 21. . . 23–26. e∞ger ba-ùr-ra-ta “after (the flood) had swept thereover” SKL 40. Note: The comparative function of -gin7 “in the same way as .” here coalesced with a temporal function.6. both standing in the same case. . -a-“è. had pronounced many blessings thereon” ”ulgi X 126. “ùd-da mi-ni-in-pà-pà-da-ta “after Nin-azu . . Note: mu “name” and the following nominalized phrase may be defined—morpho-syntactically—as a head noun with an apposition. The locative supposed to follow the nominalized verbal form has been shifted after the possessive particle -bi.”. 12. mu PN-e mu-lugal pà-da du11-ga-na ba-ni(-n)-ge-na-“è PN2 PN3 baan-tuku “because PN1 had (confirmed on his saying =) admitted that (the royal name had been invoked =) an oath (invoking) the king had been sworn. . Nominalized phrase with a possessive particle u4 An-né kur-kur-ra sa∞g-ki ba-da-an-gíd-da-ba [ba-bda-n-gid-a-b(i)-a] “when An had frowned upon all the lands” LamSumUr 22.16. e4 mu-na-a-tu5-a-gin7 [mu-na-V-tu-a-gin] “as I (= Inana) bathed for the king” ”ulgi X 14–15.

. . . brought the Elamites . . Enlil. is “in its ‘what-he-did thereby’”.  d 155 En-líl lú-nam-tar-tar-re-dè a-na bí-in-AG-a-ba dEn-líl-le Elamki . Note: lú here functions as a ‘relative pronoun’. because lú “person” must be kept separate from (nam-)lú-u18-lu [(nam)lulu] “man(kind)”. [ana binAGaba] lit. . hardly “in order to decide the fate of mankind” (Michalowski’s translation). down against us from their highlands” LamSumUr 165 f. kur-ta im-ta-an-è “what Enlil who makes inescapable decisions actu­ ally did is that he.

This page intentionally left blank .

lú an-gin7 ri-ba ki-gin7 ri-ba-é“e “the person who. enormous as the earth” Gudea Cyl. pronoun. . 102. II 13 (ibid. T  [()“] Sumerian has a suffixed quotation particle corresponding to Akkadian -mi. both nom­ inal and nominal/verbal (general). [ri]). It is first found in the Gudea corpus. Note: Instead of é“e. or finite verbal form ([(e)“e]. 15). having said (-“i) that (U. 41. 29. as you said. The particles described here cannot even be said to follow a com­ mon morpho-syntactical behaviour pattern. We kept them apart from the (nominal) case particles in spite of Attinger 1993. . ∞ ge“-nú-∞gu10 gub-ba-ab-zé-en ga-ba-nú-e-“e “‘set up my bed. Ur-ni9-∞gar-ra Ab-ba-sa6-ga-a kù in-da-tuku-“i mu-lugal . because some of them may be affixed directly to a noun. [(e)“e] is a particle which may be added to both nouns and verbs (see 4. 26. . -“è was read until Thomsen 1984. and some to a nominalized verbal form ([∞ge“en]. A v 13 (see ibid. The matter is in need of more thorough treatment. [∞ge“en]. 260 “les postpositions ‘isolantes’” (Krecher’s term). I will repay it by . and OB and later texts (including the lexical evidence) have -e-“e. ga-ab-su bí-in-dug4 “A declared: ‘name of the king. [nanna]).1). . lacking a collective denominator. was as enormous as the skies. 147).1. has silver with him =) he owes U. .”).’. . . . while [“uba] stands apart in that it follows a noun standing in a dimensional case (but also may be the head of a copula). I want to sleep’ I said” Schooldays 15.TA (“mi. spelled é“e(”È). suffixed”) NBGT I 461 (MSL 4. -“i occurs in Ur III. see ibid. e-“e: mi-i KI.4) This heading is a catch-all for different kinds of particles. . and it was inter­ preted as the terminative case particle (“concerning . .2557:1–6. 13. 149). silver” TCL 2.CHAPTER THIRTEEN POST-NOMINAL AND/OR POST-VERBAL PARTICLES OTHER THAN CASE PARTICLES (5.

see above. igi-§rú-§rú-a na-an-gig-ga-∞ge“-“e-en ki-ad-da-∞gá mu-e-(var. “unu-man “were it we. define Akkadian man as a particle denoting the irrealis mood. since the Babylonian grammarians defined [∞ge“en] as a suffix (KI. It occurs with pronouns. who still thought of [e“e] as a “Potentialis. there can hardly be a connection with the adverbiative particle (see 11).a). (so) let me eat (it all). and nominalized verbal forms. e-ne-ne-∞ge“-en = nìnu-man. ∞ge“-en = ma-an (KI.6.2.und Irrealissuffix”. sg. . must be a misunderstanding. 52). 202.TA). sg. its function may only be described in a preliminary way. (so) let me store (food)” BWL 244 f. .e.). however. (following GAG § 152d) and CAD M/1.). me-en-dè-∞ge“-en. . ga-nam ga-ti-le∞ en-dè-en [ga-til-enden] ∞ge“-en ga-bí-íb-∞gar “well. as if it were a separate word. This. 147).158   The origin and etymology of [(e)“e] are unknown. me-en-zé-en-∞ge“-en.. 13. (or) well. because of you I am unable to sleep and rest at day or at night” Father and Son 119–121. we might live (Akkad. we might die (Akkad. nouns. another Sumerian expression rendering the idea of “without” is suffixed -[nanna]. 13. 113–130. attunu-man. this would also suit the Sumerian context of [∞ge“en]. you (pl. ì-)ki∞g-∞gáge“-“e-en ∞gá-e mu-zu-“è gi6-an-bar7(NE)-ba ù-du10 nu-mu-e-da-ku-ku∞ u[n] “. Both AHw. á mu-e-da-á∞g-∞ge“-“e-en á-á∞g-∞gá ma-ab-“úm-mu-un-e-“e “if I would tell you to do something you would say ‘(how come) you are giving me orders’” Father and Son 43 f. used as a suffix) NBGT I 460 (MSL 4. It is not yet possible to propose an etymology for [∞ e“en] ga-nam g ga-ug5-g(a)-en-dè-en [ga-ug-enden] ge“-en ga-an-gu7. T   [] In addition to the syntagma -X-da nu-me-a “not being with X” = “without X” (see 5.2.4. [g“] ([gi“]) ˆ ˆ The suffixed particle [∞ge“en] ([∞gi“en]) is rarely attested in context and.).3. therefore. they” OBGT I 460–462 (MSL 4. Note: ∞ge“-en is on both occasions written at the beginning of a new line.: 42–45. See Falkenstein 1952.TA) (i.

directly suffixed (see note below). [“uba] secondarily became a par­ ticle expressing the idea “apart from”. leave (behind). . Amar-Suena 9:3–8. .- / -  me-en-dè-na-an-na me-en-zé-en-na-an-na e-ne-ne-na-an-na = “a là n[iàti] “without us” = “a là [kunùti ] “without you” = “a là [“unùti] “without them” OBGT I 488–490 (MSL 4. [“uba] cannot even be defined as a suffixed particle. T  [“] “ub-ba [“uba] is originally either a ¢am†u participle of “ub “to throw. (where) from ancient days offerings (and) . however. . which had been neglected had not been there (and) (apart from) that house which had not been built (Amar-Suena did build . neglect”: “left neglected”.16. (see 12. because it may also stand as the head of a phrase containing a copula. thus not strictly fulfilling the features of the particles collected under 13. In the latter case. or an imperative: “leave (it)” (suggestion of J. “ub-ba would closely follow Akkadian ezib/ezub. dub-lá-mah u4-ul-lí-a-ta ki-“u-tag “uku-UD “ub-ba ì-me-a-na-an-na é-bi nu-§rú-àm “apart from the D. 53) 159 It is not yet possible to propose an etymology for the particle [nanna].)” RIME 3/2. Postgate). N. 53) Note: In none of the six occurrences does [“uba] function as an immediately suffixed particle. .4.) are set apart” “left apart from them” “it is we who are set apart” “it is you (pl. 254.) who are set apart” “it is of being left apart from them” “ub-ba-me-en-za-nam = ezi[b kunùtima] e-ne-ne-a “ub-ba-kam = ezi[b “unùtima] OBGT I 491–496 (MSL 4. .3). me-en-dè-a “ub-ba “ub-ba-me-en-zé-en e-ne-ne-a “ub-ba “ub-ba-me-en-da-nam = = = = ez[ib ezi[b ezi[b ezi[b niàti ] kunùti ] “unùti ] niàtima] “left apart from us” “you (pl.1). PN-na-an-na lú nu-ù-da-nú-a .2) or [nanna] (13. . It is only the Akkadian translations that treat [“uba] on the same level as [∞ge“en] (13. 13. but it would not be to bold to propose that the initial element [n(a)-] is related with the [n] of negative [nu] or prohibitive [na]. [“uba] is not. .

58) and OBGT XIV 4–5 (ibid. 7. . who preliminarily adopted Krecher’s term. 122. and 789–791 (MSL 4. 260. 260.4. as in some of the lexical entries quoted above governs a dimen­ sional case: [mu-Bau nin∞g(u)-ak-a-e(!)]. see Enmerkar 430 and Green 1978. of CAD E 429) PBS 1/2. 7. Krecher 1965 defined it as an “isolierende Postposition”. 13. Note: It is unlikely that the respective particle [ri] should be identical in mean­ ing and function with the deictic particle [ri]. sect. 128). Note: [“uba] here. 145. “à-zu im-mi-ib-du11-ga-ri/ /“à-zu mi-ni-ib-du11-ga-ta “after your heart spoke about it” Attinger 1993. mu dBa-ú nin-∞gá-ka-e(!) “ub-ba é-dNanna-kam ¢∞garzaÜ ki-gub-ba nu-tuku-a = a““um Bau bèltia ezub ina bìt Suen parßa u mazzàzam là i“ûma “that apart from that which relates to my mistress Bau. ten years had passed” GEN 40 = 83 = 127. For lexical occurrences of the latter see OBGT I 785–786. 135: 13–15 (see Falkenstein 1952.. I have no office (or) position in the house of the Moongod” (transl.e. showed that -ri may stand in variation with ablative -ta “after”. If that be the case. and see above. 145) examples favour an ablative interpretation of suffixed [ri]. an affixed particle highlighting the meaning of the verbal form. All of Krecher’s (1965) and Green’s (1978. more research is needed. 122). treated above. van Dijk 1953.4. At any rate. the choice of -ri rather than -ta may have been a matter of style rather than grammar. Attinger 1993.160   For more lexical evidence see CAD E 429 ezib (prep. T   [] -ri occurs after nominalized verbal forms ending in -a.5. mu-5-àm mu-10-àm ba-e-zal-la-ri “after five.) lex. i.

Ki“ . im-ug5-ga-ta “after Ki“ . Thomsen 1984. As for subjunctions.1. 16 and 83) are actually found in Akkadian contexts (Ebla and Abu Íalàbì¢): IAS no.C.1. too.1. 14. Sumerian ù can hardly be expected to occur more frequently. . Note: wa (or ù) is still much more frequent in the Ebla (24th cent. nor has Sumerian [u]. because what is expressed by a con­ junction in a non-agglutinative language such as Semitic or IndoEuropean is mostly represented by a nominalized verbal form plus a dimensional particle in Sumerian. because they are essentially atypical for Sumerian. and some of them are actually loans from Akkadian. Akkadian [u] has not so far been the subject of a detailed study. Sumerian has no original word for “and”. with [ta]. partly because it was replaced—for linking sentences—by [ma]. 6). The . however. Therefore. their number is restricted. conjunctions and. specially. . i. that the Ebla scribe used ù instead of the wa (PI) otherwise preferred for “and” in literary Ebla-Akkadian texts. C 14.e. 326/ /ARET 2 no.) variant of Old Akkadian. had been killed” (Curse of Akkade 2) where the nominalized verbal form [im-ug-a] is put in the ablative.CHAPTER FOURTEEN CONJUNCTIONS AND SUBJUNCTIONS Sumerian as an agglutinative language has few conjunctions or subjunctions. . B. It should be stressed. [u] Sumerian borrowed Semitic [wa] “and” in its Akkadian form ù [u]. in order to denote temporal distance. The allegedly oldest occurrences of ù in pre-Sargonic Sumerian (cf. subjunctions are treated without assigning them to strict grammatical categories. In the following. [u] occurs much less in Akkadian than it does in its cognate Semitic languages. In fact.. .

attest to ù as an Akkadian loan in pre-Sargonic Sumerian. ù may connect PN and a pronoun.1. becoming as Sumerian as. i. ù e∞ger-ab-ba-ne-ne ì-ba-e-ne “and they will also divide the estate of their father” NG no. ù “and. have used Ù as a Sumerogram which would. It would be difficult to explain. By Ur III times. Moreover. 1.6).2. at least indirectly. Arabic wa “and” became Turkish in the form of ve. [wrote] (it). 14. dub-∞gu10 ì-§rú ì-[sar] ì-til-ma mu-gub-ba-∞gu10 ma-an-gub-bu-u“ “I set up my tablet.. 182 s.2. u 1:2–4). The question needs more attention.2. the word would have become a subjunction secondarily. A. PN1 ù PN2 practically replaced former PN1 PN2-bi(-da) (see 4. however.e. Cf. S 14. more recently. and also” definitively gained currency in Sumerian by the time of Ur III.2. why in that case a variant ù-da should have occurred which is graphically more complex. [uda] [uda] is spelled ù-da or u4-da in Pre-Sargonic Laga“ inscriptions. 7:20. 14. NG no. apart from ù Akkadian connective -ma has also been borrowed.4. but it is not used in the enumeration of the names of judges or witnesses. Falkenstein.v. or two verbal forms (see Sollberger 1966 ibid. 7:17–18.2. WO 1/3 (1948) 176. and see Sollberger 1966.(7) and 5. . u4-da “at the day. The relation of sentence-connecting ù to the more traditional asyn­ detic construction is in need of further elucidation. and then they put before me (my standing lines =) the model lines (to be copied by me)” Schooldays 5–6.27).1. today”. finished (it).g.162   scribe may. [ma] By the OB period. two nouns. therefore. If the etymology of the subjunction were actually the locative of the word u4(-d) “day”. especially with regard to diachrony. ù frequently occurs connecting two personal names (e.

   163 [uda] introduces a clause with the verb in conjugation pattern 1 (intransitive) or 2a (transitive) (see 12. instead of tukum-bi. Beginning with the Code of Ur-Namma (Ur III). ha-mu-na-ra-pà-dè “if there is barley. it is usually written with four elements: ”U. clearly another Sumero-Akkadian areal feature. Note: The exact meaning of the sanction is unknown. 181). .TUR. 14. tukum na-∞gál. The whole complex may be interpreted as a conditional sentence construction consisting of a protasis (“if A”) and an apodosis (“then B”). especially so in EnmEns. é-ZAR-ta. Note: tukum alone may just be a scribal idiosyncrasy (note. both translated by us as pre­ sent tense.2. the unusual RA-. offers two OB occurrences of u4-da for “when”. u4-da) mu-“è-sa10-sa10 [mû“e-sasa-(e)n] kù “à-∞gá a-sa6-ga lá­ ma “if you (want to) sell it to me. let him find it for him from the . 2'–6'. let him give it to him. tukum. if there is not. exactly corresponding to the preterite (more rarely perfect) found with Akka­ dian “umma “if ”. tu-ku-um-bi ”U. 275 “umma lex. . Note: Thomsen 1984. ù-da (var. instead of *ha-. until when?” (note LamSumUr 451) which is formed after more frequent èn-“è (adi mati ) “until when?”. 4–5 xi 7–10/ /xi 38–xii 3. is found in tukum “e ì-∞gál. [tukumbi] [tukumbi] “if ” graphically is one of the most elaborate “dirig” com­ pounds in the spelling of Sumerian words. however. 26 where the verb following u4-da is in conjugation pattern 2b (preterite). The verb of the protasis is in con­ jugation pattern 1 (intransitive) or 2 b (transitive preterite).LÁ. 367 r.2. “if ”. too. a translation “today” would fit.TUR. 85. 43 vi 1–2. sect.” Sollberger 1966 no.GAR. RA-naan-“úm-mu. for both.7). tukum alone occurs in èn tukum-“è “how long still. Apart from final -bi.LÁ-bi = “u-um-mu Diri V 120 see CAD ”/3.GAR. whose ori­ gin has not yet been explained. in 3') because otherwise the corpus of Ur III letters regu­ larly has tukum-bi (see Sollberger 1966 p. tukum-bi regularly introduces conditional sentences. pay me silver (which is pleasant for my heart =) which I find appropriate” Ukg. u4-da ka-ka-na níg-érim ba-∞gá-∞gá ∞ge“gag ka-ka-na “è-gaz “if she (the seller) (puts evil in her mouth =) behaves dishonestly/treacherously a wooden peg will be driven into her” SR no. Such a clause is followed by a second clause equally with the verb in pattern 1 or 2a. .

thus virtually functioning as a preposition: en-na sukud-§rá-bi “(the walls) to their highest point” Curse Akk. (up to =) as many as could be brought” Ur-Namma A 87.2. see ibid. be followed by a finite. 311. In such a case. 14. [ena] en-na is parallel to Akkadian adi in its dual functions of (a) a subjunction and (b) a preposition. éren en-na ba-ug5-ga “soldiers (up to =) .4. Note: Susa var. . see above 12. . . 139. .2.12.164   tukum-bi lú-ù sag ∞ge“ bí-in-ra lú-bi ì-gaz-e-dam “if a person com­ ∞ mits a murder that person will have to be killed” Ur-Namma Code C 52–54 (§1). see Flückiger-Hawker 1999.3 end. [mu] mu “year” in Sumerian year formulae may. .16. how long?”. . verbal form. no. 125:8–9. There is no strict rule observable for the morphological behav­ iour of the verb dependent on the subjunction.2. (2) en-na + nominalized verbal form: gu4 . as many as have died” ibid. see Wilcke 2002. Since the etymology of [en] in en-na (or of [ena] as such) is unknown. 14. (1) en-na + finite verbal form: en-na àm-du igi-∞gu10-“è enim-bi a-bala-e “until he will have come here (and) the respective matter will have been brought to me” Sollberger 1966 no. . 227. 125. has in-na ba-¢“ubÜ-e. 68:4 (i-im-du).5. en-na na-an-ga-ti-la-a“ ARN 7:8–11. not nominalized. en-na ab-lah5-a “oxen .3. 113: 3–5. mu may be taken as a subjunction. see above 12. from Gudea of Laga“ onward. (3) en-na + nominalized verbal form with terminative case particle: PN1 ù PN2 en-na igi-∞gu10 -“è di in-da-an-du11-ga-a“ [i-n-da-n-duga-“(e)] “until PN1 will have had (his) lawsuit with PN2 in my pres­ ence” Sollberger 1966 no. it is difficult to describe the difference (if any) between [ena] “until” and en/èn-“è “until when. en-na may be used with a noun.

JNES 27 (1968) 229–242. Note: See (Th.   14. 229) “as if ” mainly introduces hypothetical comparisons. alam igi-zu dNin-∞gír-su-ka-kam “a statue. [d]ur9 “u àm-kar-kar-re. 36. i-gi4 -in-zu (and other variant spellings. W. Hallo (1993) 39. as Wilcke’s discussion of alleged “the eye noticed” makes clear (Wilcke 1968. Bauer. 170. (but) it is for his master” UET 6. 232. 238 f. W. Wilcke. [iginzu] 165 igi-zu. Note: The etymology of [iginzu] is still unknown. lugal-a-ni-“è-àm “the donkey stallion runs along as if it were for himself.). Jacobsen. Fs. see Wilcke 1968. C. as if it were one of Ningirsu” Gudea Stat. . 275. B vii 58–59.5. with additions added to offprint. Das modale Adverb I-GI4-IN-ZU im Sumerischen. and correct Edzard 1997. apud) Wilcke 1968.2. see J. Attinger 1993. i-gi4-in-zu ní-te-a-ni-“è.

This page intentionally left blank .

Note: [1] in exclamations (see also 15. See PSD A/1.2. ululation.. halali. Spanish olè. [] a-lu-lu “woe”. 145. etc. we will still briefly quote the most impor­ tant ones. 15. . [] a-la-la. see PSD A/1. 15. Hebrew hallelùyà.CHAPTER FIFTEEN EXCLAMATIONS “Exclamations” should be understood as a catch-all term for calls. whatever the etymology of the individual expression. exclamation of a positive character. 100. . . [] For a “woe”. Although exclamations are. they are wail- ing” GEN 155. many translations have to be taken as approximations only. . strictly speaking. “ouch”. a gú-∞gu10 . also used as a work cry.6) is a universal phenomenon. part of the lexicon rather than of grammar. Therefore. only Classical Greek élalã. The exact meaning of an exclamation being highly depen­ dent on its cultural context. or even such expressions as “yes” or “no”. it is difficult today to arrive at a strict definition (and delimitation from related terms). 107. see Krecher 1966. German hallo. a-nir im-∞gá-∞gá-ne “ouch. Cf. my neck.1. . The following list of transliterated terms is arranged alphabetically. . French hélas.3. olàlà. 15. interjections.

4.) prefixed by the modal precative indicator [he] (see 12.12. e-la-lu.2. 433. collected references to exclamations all based on the voiced lateral [1]. see PSD A/1. I will have to tell it to her” Gudea Cyl. e-el-lum.5).7. a-ù-àm-ma are exclamations partly of a lamenting. S. see 12. 54 (1985) 120 i 23–24. above. [. 15.e. i-lu-lam-ma. [] ga-na is an encouraging.4. often self-encouraging. see note to 15. [] al-li-li(-àm-ma).13). 15. Another (more emphatic?) assertion is na-nam.11. a-u3-a A. For hé-àm du11(-g)/e “to assent.8. Or. ga-na ga-na-ab-du11 “come on then.. . “let’s get moving”.168   15. 15. ] e-el-lu. i. to say ‘yes’” see Attinger 1993. [] a-ù-a. 199. i-li(-a). el-lu.11. the copula (3rd sg. . and cf. Krecher 1966. partly of a soothing character. 15. interjection of unclear meaning. a-ù-u8!-a. el-lú. 552 f. 15. [] hé-àm “let (it) be”. A iii 22 f. etc. exclamation.11. found in context with a-ù-àm-ma (see 15.5. Kramer. 15. with fn. The first syllable [ga] is most prob­ ably identical with the modal cohortative indicator [ga] (see 12.6. “come on now”. and p. N.10. 148. 153. with bibl. see PSD A/3. . and below. has become the Sumerian term for “yes” (Akkadian annu(m)). 128.3).5) and ù-li-li (15. etc.

304. the expression is found. be ashamed to oppose them” Enkita and Enki-hegal 102 (see Wilcke 1969.11.2) prefixed by indicator [i/e] (see 12. = Akkadian inimma. be respectful to them (= the words I recited to you). etc. only as a noun. lú-bi ì dUtu ì-e “that person will cry ‘woe. níg-érim i dUtu ∞gìri bí(-n)-ús “(Gudea) (set (his) foot on the neck of =) eliminated evil (and) complaint” Gudea Cyl.9).dUtu was eventually understood a genitival compound: lú-i-dUtuka [lu-i-Utu-(a)k-a(k)] “person of complaint” = “oppressor” as in UrNamma C 35 (together with ní-zuh “thief ”. 15. i. 15. inimmu (CAD I s. B xviii 11. where shall I sit down.). no. and see 295. see CAD I 148 inimma bil. No etymology available for the expres­ sion in either language. 82 fn. 15. the word he said has made me moan”). O Sungod’” Ukg. thereafter.). in-nu ní te-ba-ab in-nu té“ tuku-ba-ab “no. [()] me-li-e-a (pronounced as three or four syllables?) “alas”. 76). me-li-e-a me-a tu“-ù-dè-en me-a gub-bu-dè-en “woe is me. me-li-e-a may stand at the beginning or the end of a phrase.v. where shall I stand?” Ur Lament 294. i dUtu “complaint”. Strangely enough. [ U] i dUtu “woe. 6 ii 14'.9.10.11. sect. alas” (Akkadian “alas. [nu] would in this case have been treated as a verbal base. O Sungod” is found in contexts where someone desper­ ately turns for help to the Sun-god as the supreme god of justice. . passage has already to be understood as “that person will lay a complaint”. enim-du11-ga-“e-“a4-∞gu10 me-li-e-a: inimmu amat iqbû u“admimanni “the bitter word pronounced for me. It may not even be excluded that the above quoted Ukg. [] 169 in-nu “no” most probably may be explained as the negative indi­ cator [nu] (see 12.

e. 124). ù-a erim6-ma-∞gu10 “alas for my treasure house” VS 1.g. “Lullaby”).170   15. u5-a. such as. ù-u8-a. English woe. law).. Note: B. 25 ii 6 f. The different sounds which probably lie behind the different nota­ tion of the two [u] sounds as u5 and ù. u5-a a-ù-a “sleep baby”.. MSL 2 (1951) 29 f. beginning of the Sumerian lullaby (see Attinger 1993. tentatively suggested for u8 “ein langgedehntes offenes o” (as in English paw. Yiddish auvay. Note: The lexical context has to do with singing and performance. [] The great variety of spellings for [ua] “woe” has been collected by Krecher. [] munus-ù-li-li = zammirtu “ulili woman” (Akkadian “songstress”) Lu III ii 16 (MSL 12. German o weh. The onomatopoeia of [ua aua] in lullabies may easily be rendered by corresponding soothing sounds in other languages. Russian bayubayubayubáy or German eiapopeia. 15. ú-a. Landsberger.13.v. (Krecher 1966.12. cannot be reconstructed. it also occurs as a soothing sound in [ua aua]. 54). 114 f. .: ù-a. 49 s. etc. but the exact meaning of [ulili] cannot be reconstructed. 1966. etc.). While [ua] is essen­ tially a sound of lamentation (with its labial element comparable to Latin vae.

g. sweet”) or semi-phonetically (e. Only a few examples out of the very ample evidence for emesal will be offered here. rear” “good. It is.. The main difference between the standard form of Sumerian (eme-gi7(-r)) and emesal regards sounds and lexicon. see also Krecher 1967b. Emesal is spelled phonetically (e..g. eme-gi7(-r) a-ga dùg gaenim nir-∞gál di∞gir eme-sal a-ba zé-eb dae-ne-è∞g “e-er-ma-al dìm-me-er “back. The pronunciation is supported by the Akkadian loanword ummisallu (AHw. CAD E). Emesal is first attested in the early OB period. 124–36. The meaning of the term is not com­ pletely clear. well known that the modern counter-tenor or even altus is sung by sexually normal male persons. and syntax. s. Russian tonkij golos “thin (high-pitched) voice”. however.CHAPTER SIXTEEN EMESAL Emesal (eme-sal) is a sociolinguistic variety of Sumerian attested for the speech of women or goddesses and of the “cantor” (gala). zé-eb [zeb] = dùg “good. cf. with special attention to the phonetic evidence. vocabu­ lary. fine tongue” (sal = raqqu) referred to a highpitched voice. It is found in a oneto-one relation with eme-gi7(-r) in phonology. It is quite probable that also traditional eme-gi7(-r) spelling could be read in emesal when required by context. the whole material having been collected by Krecher 1967b and Schretter 1990. morphology. maybe “thin. 87 fn. sweet” (modal indicator: cohortative) “word” “person of authority” “deity” . Note: The fact that the “cantor” used a form of speech otherwise attested for female persons has caused much speculation: Was the gala a eunuch or an effeminate male? See the discussion in Schretter 1990. see also emesallu..v. dMu-ul-líl [Mullil] = Enlil where the second element líl is never given as *li-il). 1).

[lu] : [mulu]. individual eme­ sal words and name variants are also found in lexical lists (see Schretter 1990. 17–30). umun ir ta-àm ∞gá-ba-zé-em in-ga-da-te “person” “lady” “lord” “to bring” “what (is it)?” “he gave me” “he approached me” Apart from heteronymy (e. [g] : [d]... [g] : [z]. A special “Emesal Vocabulary” of post-OB date has been edited in MSL 4 (1956) 3–44.172 lú nin. emesal forms are not predictable for us. In spite of parallel occurrences of some of those correspondences. Krecher 1967b. It consists of three “Tablets” the first of which contains divine names and the second and third offer nouns. [∞g] : [m]. [u] : [e]). Apart from the usage of emesal in literary texts. ere“ en túm a-na-àm ma-an-“úm im-ma-da-te   mu-lu ga-“a-an. ga“an ù-mu-un.g. Schretter 1990 (see also the review of J. OLZ 87 [1992] 382–85). a few verbal bases and verbal forms as well as a very restricted number of pronouns and numerals. . Langenmayr 1992. The two verbal forms quoted last show that also the string of prefixed morphemes may have been affected by emesal variation.g. [g] : [b]. Black. [nin] : [ga“an]) there are consonantal and vocalic correspondences (e.

C. Seen in such a context. Many speakers of the two languages then lived in closest proximity. . borrowed by Sumerian as [u] (ù). The following diagram tries to demonstrate what happens when lan­ guages A and B enter such a “convergence area”. and—on the other hand—non-Sumerian.1. Consequently. The example illustrated is the com­ mon Semitic conjunction [wa]. 154).CHAPTER SEVENTEEN THE SUMERO-AKKADIAN LINGUISTIC AREA Sumerian and Akkadian coexisted in a region of about the size of modern Belgium during most of the 3rd millennium B. some scholars have maintained that Sumerian had already ceased to be a living spoken language as early as the time of the Third Dynasty of Ur. many features of both Sumerian and Akkadian may be easily explained. But what actually happened was the building up of a “linguistic area” or “convergence area” where “speakers can switch from one code to another with a minimum of additional learning” (Gumperz/ Wilson 1971. the typical grammatical categories of Sumerian. The final result of mutual contacts was—apart from extensive borrowing of vocabulary—a con­ siderable divergence of Akkadian from the traditional “Semitic” lan­ guage type. A here stands for Sumerian and B for Akkadian. traits in Sumerian. These have often been interpreted as faulty treatment of “classical” Sumerian by scribes who—allegedly—no longer would have been used to. “Semitic”. and during the first quarter of the 2nd millennium. 17. and aware of.

The subject-object-verb (SOV) word order is common to both Sumerian and Mesopotamian Akkadian as against more traditional Eblaite Akkadian where we often encounter VOS (cf.1). The same holds for the insertion of infinitive constructions which stand between S and V. Akkadian wa in its (pronunciation and) spelling ù has been borrowed into Sumerian where it is mainly used to connect nouns (see 14.2. often used as a “sentence opener” (Satzweiser.174 *Pre-Sumerian Sumerian (0 )| ù |   Semitic Akkadian ù (usage restricted as compared with other Semitic languages) | | wa (usage restricted. This.84). 23–42). the Akkadian verbal form of a PN is set in the ventive. “and also”. In Mesopotamian proper. the word order S(O)V prevails: Sin-iddinam “the Moongod gave me (the child)”. The older V(O)S order is still found—as an ‘archaism’—in Ur III and OB personal names. We will enumerate a number of other such features attesting to the Sumero-Akkadian “convergence area”. Edzard 1984.g. and as a con­ nector of phrases it has mainly been replaced by enclitic -ma. 39. this leading to veritable “Schachtelsatz” constructions (see Poebel 1947. . may be called a real areal feature. When. is still attested in its full set of functions in Ebla Akkadian.). 115 f.1. iddin-Sin “the Moongod gave (the child)”. see Richter 1970. most probably Akkadian word order was influenced by Sumerian. then.. however. not productive) Note: Common Semitic wa “and”. The relatively rare usage of ù in Sumerian then most probably had its reverse effect on Akkadian where ù is of limited application as compared to the usage of wa in the other Semitic languages. its usage is restricted. e. Sumerian itself lacks a connecting par­ ticle “and”. The SOV word order is also maintained in Akkadian when the subject is extended by the addition of a relative clause. Therefore. 17.

17. 20). The ventive : non-ventive dichotomy of the finite verbal system of Sumerian has left its impact on neighbouring Akkadian where a post-verbal morpheme [an]. -   17. iddin-am. [˙]. So if phoneme reduction in Akkadian is being considered as due to Sumerian substratum. [˙]. 17.—igi ∞gar “to set the eye” = pànam “akànum “to set the face” = “to prepare to (. etc. V 175 The formation of the Akkadian ventive.—a-“à è “to bring out =) rent a field” = eqlam “ùßûm (ditto). 433–36). and ['] can no longer be substantiated for Akkadian phonology. Pedersén 1989.—nam-X AG = X-ùtam epè“um “to exercise . [h]. Otherwise [an] maintained its identity except that the [n] was assimilated to a following consonant: iddin-aN-kum ¡ iddinakkum “he gave to you”. . p.)”.—∞gé“tu gub “to set the ear” = uznam “akànum (ditto) = “to be attentive”.4. as inherited from common Semitic. P  S  A     gú “ub “to throw the neck” = a¢am nadûm “to throw the arm” = “to be neglectful”. e. Note: Common Semitic [an] became [am] in Akkadian in word-final position only. permeating the whole ver­ bal system except for the non-finite forms.. The personal names Nanna-man“um “the Moongod gave (the child) to me” and Sin-iddinam (ditto) may be translated back and forth in word order (SV) and morpheme by morpheme.g. By Ur III times at the latest. escape the conclusion that Sumerian originally had an unvoiced laryngeal phoneme which we tentatively transliterated as [H] (see 3. loss of [H] on the Sumerian side would be said to follow a similar ten­ dency.1.2. on the other hand. L  S [H]   A []. inherited from common Semitic affirmative [an].3. was given a new function (cf. Nothing could be more eloquent for an areal situation. . Dead loss in Hilgert 2002.5. was due to Sumerian substratum. has not developed on its own. One cannot.  ['] It may be assumed that the reduction of the Akkadian consonantal phoneme inventory.

2) = qibima “say and .7. ..—“à-“è gíd “to draw to the heart” = “to consider carefully” = ana libbim “adàdum (ditto). or the [m] is assimilated to a following consonant: ßilu““u “in his shade”. Pedersén 1989. and the ending of the Akkadian terminative-adverbial [i“].4. ßabtàta “I am. mark of the terminative case particle (5.12.1. however. 17. in the second case. The application to substantives in Akkadian is most probably due to influence of the Sumerian copula (me. A   A      In Akkadian. Note: Application of the Sumerian copula has not. been completely reflected in Akkadian.g.1) which is mainly used with substantives: dub-sar-me-en “I am/you are a scribe”. -um (or more recent -u) is maintained in the con­ struct state: ßillum Sin “in the shade of the Moongod”. ki-á∞g-∞gá-∞gu10-me-en “you are my beloved” has no such Akkadian correspondence as *naràmtì-àti. So. 1988. seemingly identical with the nominative in -um. Arabic.1. or Ethiopic. 141) rests on the misinterpretation of the Akkadian locative-adverbial case in -um.176   the X-ship” = “to follow a profession. wardum ¡ warad.”.. 17. 430–33.— enim-ma tu“ “to (sit to the word =) obey” = ana awàti wa“àbum (ditto). Although there can be no doubt of the Semitic background of Akkadian [i“]. you are holding” may also be applied to substantives: belèku. So. napi“tum ¡ napi“t(i). . “you are king”. the conjugation pattern ßabtàku. trade”. Diakonoff 1965. e. 179. see also p. the two cases have to be strictly kept apart. Kienast 2001. e. the fact that the Akkadian case was maintained throughout the millennia may be due to the parallel existence of a case with comparable function(s) in Sumerian. where it is restricted to verbs. . C  There is a conspicuous phonetic similarity between Sumerian [(e)“(e)].g.8). see 12. Note: The theory of an original ergativity of Semitic (cf.6. “arràta “I am lord”.7. The two authors did not consider the important evidence of the construct state used for Akkadian nominative and locative-adverbial. The pattern as such has its counterpart in the “perfect” of.2. Hebrew. see 12. In the first case -um is elim­ inated or replaced by a Stützvokal: bèlum ¡ bèl.—ù-ma-a-du11 “would you please say” (polite imperative.

Note: Edzard’s article has a considerable number of misprints because the author had no opportunity to read and return proofs.1. 140 f.8. 112 f. a restricted number of adjectives (mostly indicating some kind of dimension) form a plural where the middle radical of the root is lengthened: rabûm “big” ¡ rabbûtum.7. etc. see Reiner 1966. following a substantive. and see 4.9. e. To quote but one example: There is a complete correspondence between Sumerian positive affirmative 1. lù iprus. occurring in the 1st and 3rd p. 121–141. chart on p. -ù 177 Sumerian and Akkadian both have a nominal device to express the abstract of title. Both in Sumerian and Akkadian. specially pp. For more details see Edzard 1973. and built on the preterite (iprus) conjugation pattern. .) than to the moods known from other Semitic languages. . 64. that it mainly occurs in the plural. arkum “long” ¡ arrakùtum..1.11.g. the respective pattern is fully productive.3. 17. conceding. indicate plural (5.10. Note: Both AHw.). equally occurring in the 1st and 3rd p.. T    In Akkadian. V  Akkadian has a set of verbal moods which it is easier to compare to the system of Sumerian moods (cf. One may easily compare this type of ‘internal’ plural with the reduplicated Sumerian adjectives which.7) and the Akkadian positive affirmative 1: lù aprus. -   17. however. 17. or adjective: nam-lugal = “arrùtum “all that has to do with a ruler” = “king-ship”. and CAD R offer an entry rabbû (sg. di∞gir-gal-gal-(e-ne) = ilù rabbûtum “the great gods”.b). profession. and built on the (¢am†u) conjugation patterns 1 or 2b (see 12. ße¢rum “small” ¡ ße¢¢erùtum.

This count yields 3. Í. ˇ. Note: PSD B was published in 1984 whereas PSD A (1992 ff. can only be dealt with here very briefly. are in no way eligible for a projection on the situation of Akkadian loanwords in Sumerian in general. E. L. 5 occur only in lexical lists. This count yields a little above 7% of Sumerian loanwords in the Akkadian vocabulary. The CAD volumes A. 13. no corresponding data may so far be offered for Akkadian loanwords in Sumerian because Sumerian lexicography is still in its infancy. Unfortunately. T.) is still incomplete. worth a whole book. D. Krebernik/M.7% of Akkadian loan­ words for Sumerian words beginning with the letter B. of these 13. they have never so far been challenged.178   17. S. Note: The above figures have been added from the author’s CAD reviews in ZA 53 (1959) to 90 (2000) (see M. K. I/J. P. It should also be kept in mind that (mutual) lexical borrowing may be—but is not necessarily—a symptom of an areal situation. We must repeat that the figure can in no way be regarded as representative for the vocabulary as a whole. 350 entries in PSD B. ZA 91 [2001] 13–20). Q .630 Akkadian lemmata. about 980 are Sumerian loan­ words (ca. H. B. Streck. In spite of the author’s own skepticism on the exactitude of these figures. ”. There are only about 13 words of Akkadian origin among ca. The following data of PSD B. and Z (with P. 320 of which are only attested in lexical lists).11. M. G. R. while not negligible. and U/W still missing) include ca. N. Of these. M      S  A This topic. .

but in connection with Akkadian. word for­ mation and nominal compounds. structural. have been included in the above chapters: e. such as.g. the vast complex of the non-finite verbal forms (12. Falkenstein’s monumental “Gudea” grammar (1949. Thirdly. the author may much too often have left the impression of Sumerian as one uniform block instead of a living being with a marked diachronic evolution.g. e. the system of the modal and connecting indicators (12.. There is no end of addenda—and corrigenda.g. I freely admit the highly subjective character of my own effort. according to the witty remark of the late I. It is not under the obligation of a more recent method—generative.4). On the other hand. or the nom­ inalisation of finite verbal forms (12. 12. At any rate. the stepchild of more recent grammars. is traditionally descrip­ tive. quite a few features that would traditionally figure in a “syntax” (the description of how parts of speech as well as phrases are “set together”.. or transformational. not to describe Sumerian as a completely isolated body. Finally. func­ tions and morpho-syntactical behaviour of the case particles (5. a historical grammar of Sumerian still has to be written. However. but also of the way by which parts of speech and phrases are morphologically marked for their functions).14). e. it lacks a section—or chapters—on syntax.. The author will not enumerate here topics that would have mer­ ited inclusion in a grammar of Sumerian. Diakonoff that there are as many kinds of Sumerian as there are authors of Sumerian grammars. as often as possible. the language of those inhabitants of Mesopotamia who were the immediate neighbours of the Sumerians for a whole millennium or even longer. the author has endeavoured. . in contrast to A. or literary style in general. in its essential aspects.12). M. morphological variation (both nominal and verbal) in different manuscripts of a given line of a lit­ erary composition.11. It is far from perfect as.CHAPTER EIGHTEEN SUMMARY—AND WHAT IS STILL MISSING? The present grammar. 1950).16).

This page intentionally left blank .

208–32. 122–27. Jacobsen). 45–61. id. Alster 1974: The Instructions of Suruppak. Civil 1974 see W. id. Cohen et al. in: (ed. id. id. SP 9–13). D.sag. Dixon 1994: Ergativity. M. W. 1990: Gilgame“ und Huwawa A. Black 1984: Sumerian Grammar in Babylonian Theory (= StudPohl SM 12). Bauer 1993: Ein sumerisches Sprichwort. R. id. id. B. W. 42. WO 8. La construction de du11/e/di “dire” (= OBO Sonderband). 1984: Zur Syntax der Ebla-Texte.und neusumerischen Texten. Maekawa) Fs. A. id. in: (ed. 21939. I. 2 1991. 1976a: (as 1971) ZA 66. ZA 75. A. . marû und freie Reduplikation beim sumerischen Verbum. F. Yoshikawa. 3. A Sumerian Proverb Collection (= Mesopotamia 2). 83–93. 1972: (as 1971) ZA 62. ZA 80. M. “ich habe dir gegeben”. Teil. JAOS 103. Th.. Deeter 1963: Armenisch und die kaukasischen Sprachen. J. 161–78. J. 22–36.BIBLIOGRAPHY Note: Bibliographical items that have been quoted above unabridged are not repeated here. Cohen 1975: ur. 13) 101–16. 1–34. 27–38. 7. 1971: ¢am†u. 165–203. 1973: Die Modi beim älteren akkadischen Verbum. 1995: Les préfixes absolutifs de la première et de la deuxième personne sin­ gulier dans les formes marû ergatives. A ”irnam“ubba of Ninurta. JCS 30. id. 159–77. Postgate 1974. van Dijk 1953: La sagesse suméro-accadienne. A. id. 1976c: “Du hast mir gegeben”. id. O. Or. Recherches sur les genres littéraires des textes sapientiaux. Attinger 1993: Eléments de linguistique sumérienne. WO 8. 1976: Lexicography. HSAO 1. 1. J. Edzard 1967: Das sumerische Verbalmorphem /ed/ in den alt. Diakonoff 1983: Towards a History of Mathematical Speculation. 39–41. Hallo. Heimpel 1974 and J. M. 8–53. E. 121–41. id. P. id. StudEb. E. Fronzaroli) Studies on the Language of Ebla (= QuadSem. P. in: (ed. Jacobsen (= AS 20) 123–57. W. ZA 61. 1976b: Zum sumerischen Eid. id. M. Lieberman) Fs. Cavigneaux 1978: L’essence divine. I. Bd. K. 1991–93: The Sumerian Folktale of the Three Ox-Drivers of Adab. St. J. d’Agostino 1990: Il sistema verbale sumerico nei testi lessicali di Ebla (= StudSem. 63–98. J.ur4. HdOr. 1–79. 26). 1970: Die bukànum-Formel der altbabylonischen Kaufverträge und ihre sumerische Entsprechung. id. M. W. 29–62. N. AS 20 (Fs. Deimel 1924: sumerische Grammatik (= Or. Abt. Über das sumerische Verbum sum. Aro 1961: Die akkadischen Infinitivkonstruktionen (= StudOr. JCS 43–45.me. NS 7). ZA 60. 2003: (in press) (on Sumerian adjectives) in: (ed. id. 177–85. M.“ár. id. 1980: Sumerisch 1 bis 10 in Ebla. Th.) Fs.

182



id. 1991: Gilgame“ und Huwawa A. II. Teil, ZA 81, 160–233.
id. 1995: The Sumerian Language, in: (ed. J. M. Sasson) CANE IV 2107–16.
id. 1997: Gudea and His Dynasty (= RIME 3/1).
id. 2003a: Zum sumerischen Verbalpräfix a(1), in: (ed. W. Sallaberger et al.) Fs.
C. Wilcke, 87–98. id. 2003b: Sumerisch 1 bis 120, in: (ed. Y. Sefati) Fs. J. Klein. id. 2003c: Wann ist Sumerisch als gesprochene Sprache ausgestorben, in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa. A. Falkenstein 1939: Untersuchungen zur sumerischen Grammatik 1. Der Plural des Kohortativs; 2. Das richtungsanzeigende Infix -ra-, ZA 45, 169–94. id. 1949–1950: Grammatik der Sprache Gudeas von Laga“. I. Schrift- und Formen­ lehre. II. Syntax (= AnOr. 28–29). id. 1952: Das Petentialis- und Irrealissufix -e-“e des Sumerischen, Indogerm. Forschungen 66, 113–30. id. 1956: NG I. id. 1959a: Das Sumerische (= HdOr. I/2/1–2/1). id. 1959b: Untersuchungen zur sumerischen Grammatik 5. Zum Akzent des Sumer­ ischen, ZA 53, 98–105. G. Farber-Flügge 1973: Der Mythos “Inanna und Enki” unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der me (= StudPohl 10). D. A. Foxvog 1974: apud Heimpel 1974. id. 1975: The Sumerian Ergative Construction, Or. 44, 395–425.
I. J. Gelb 21961: Old Akkadian Writing and Grammar (= MAD 3). Y. Gong 2000: Die Namen der Keilschriftzeichen (= AOAT 268). G. B. Gragg 1968: The Syntax of the Copula, in: (ed. J. W. M. Verhaar) The Verb ‘Be’ and its Synonyms 3 (= Foundations of Languages, Suppl. Series 8). id. 1973: Sumerian Dimensional Infixes (= AOAT 5). M. Green 1978: The Eridu Lament, JCS 30, 127–67, esp. pp. 145 f. for -ri. J. J. Gumperz/R. Wilson 1971: Convergence and Creolization. A Case for the Indo-Aryan/Dravidian Border in India, in: (ed. D. Hymes), Pidginization and Creolization, 151–67, esp. p. 154. W. Heimpel 1974: The Structure of the Sumerian Prefix Chain (ms.). M. Hilgert 2002: Akkadisch in der Ur III-Zeit (= IMGULA 5). B. Hru“ka 1969: Das spätbabylonische Lehrgedicht “Inannas Erhöhung”, ArOr. 37, 473–521. Th. Jacobsen 1956: Introduction to the Chicago Grammatical Texts, MSL 4, 1*–50*. id. 1965: About the Sumerian Verb, in: (ed. H.G. Güterbock/Th. Jacobsen) Fs. B. Landsberger (= AS 16) 71–101); reprinted in Jacobsen 1970, 245–70 + 430–66. id. 1970: (ed. W. L. Moran) Toward the Image of Tammuz and other Essays on Mesopotamian History and Religion (= HSS 21). id. 1988: The Sumerian Verbal Core, ZA 78, 161–220; the reader is warned for a number of regrettable misprints. B. Jagersma 1999: apud Zólyomi 1999. M. Jaques n.d. [1999]: Le vocabulaire des sentiments dans les textes sumériens (= thèse de doctorat, Université de Genève). R. Jestin 1943: Le verbe sumérien (I).
id. 1946: Le verbe sumérien (II).
id. 1951: Abrégé de grammaire sumérienne.
T. B. Jones/J. W. Snyder 1961: Sumerian Economic Texts from the Third Dynasty. I. T. Kaneva 1996: ”umerskij jazyk.
F. Karahashi 2000: Sumerian Compound Verbs with Body-part Terms (diss. Univ. of Chicago). D. Katz 1993: Gilgamesh and Akka.



183

B. Kienast 1980: Probleme der sumerischen Grammatik 4. Bemerkungen zu ¢am†u und marû im Sumerischen, ZA 70, 1–35. J. Klein 1981. Three ”ulgi Hymns. Sumerian Royal Hymns Glorifying King ”ulgi of Ur. id. 1993: The Suffix of Determination /a/, ASJ 15, 81–98. S. N. Kramer 1936: The Sumerian Prefix Forms be- and bi- in the Time of the Earlier Princes of Laga“ (= AS 8). M. Krebernik 1983: Zu Syllabar und Orthographie der lexikalischen Texte aus Ebla II, ZA 73, 1–47. id. 1998: Die Texte aus Fàra und Tell Abù Íalàbì¢, in: (ed. P. Attinger/M. Wäfler) Mespotamien. Späturuk-Zeit und Frühdynastische Zeit (= OBO 160/1), 237– 427. J. Krecher 1965: Zur sumerischen Grammatik 1. Isolierende Postpositionen, ZA 57, 12–29. id. 1966: Sumerische Kultlyrik. id. 1967a: Die pluralischen Verben für “gehen” und “stehen” im Sumerischen, WO 4, 1–11. id. 1967b: Zum Emesal-Dialekt des Sumerischen, HSAO 1, 87–110. id. 1978: Die Form und der Gebrauch der nominalen Verbalformen und die Determination im Sumerischen, Or. 47, 376–403. id. 1985: Die /m/-Präfixe des sumerischen Verbums, Or. 54, 133–81. id. 1987: Morphemeless Syntax in Sumerian as Seen on the Background of WordComposition in Chukchee, ASJ 9, 67–88. id. 1993: Über einige ‘zusammengesetze Verben’ im Sumerischen, in: (ed. A. F. Rainey et al.) Mem. R. Kutscher, 107–118. id. 1995: Die marû-Formen des sumerischen Verbums (= AOAT 240) 141–200. W. G. Lambert 1991: The Reference of ¢am†u and marû in Lexical Lists, ZA 81, 7–9. B. Landsberger 1944: Die Anfänge der Zivilisation in Mesopotamien (Ankara Üniv. Dil ve Tarih-Co<rafya Fak. Dergisi II/3, 431–37); see also (transl. and edited by M. de J. Ellis) The Beginnings of Civilisation in Mesopotamia (1974), Monographs of the Ancient Near East 1/2, 8–12. A. Langenmayr 1992: Sprachpsychologische Untersuchung zur sumerischen “Frauensprache”, ZA 82, 208–11. H. Limet 1968: L’anthroponymie sumérienne dans les documents de la 3e dynas­ tie d’Ur. P. Michalowski 1980: Sumerian as an Ergative Language, I, JCS 32, 86–103. id. 1898: The Lamentation over the Destruction of Sumer and Ur (= MesCiv. 1). id. 1993: Letters from Early Mesopotamia. O. Pedersén 1989: Some Morphological Aspects of Sumerian and Akkadian Linguistic Area, in: (ed. H. Behrens et al.) Fs. Å. W. Sjöberg, 429–38. A. Poebel 1923: Grundzüge der sumerischen Grammatik. id. 1931: The Sumerian Prefix Forms e- and i- in the Time of the Earlier Princes
of Laga“ (= AS 2). id. 1939: Studies in Akkadian Grammar (= AS 9). id. 1947: Miscellaneous Studies (= AS 14), esp. pp. 23–42: The “Schachtelsatz” Construction of the Narâm-Sîn Text RA XVI 157 f. J. N. Postgate 1974: Two Points of Grammar in Gudea, JCS 26, 16–54. M. A. Powell 1971: Sumerian Numeration and Metrology (= diss. Univ. of Minnesota). id. 1989: Maße und Gewichte, RIA VII (1987–90) 457–517. W. Richter 1970: Exegese als Literaturwissenschaft. Entwurf einer alttestamentlichen Literaturtheorie und Methodologie. E. Reiner 1966: A Linguistic Analysis of Akkadian.

184



W. H. Ph. Römer 1965: Sumerische “Königshymnen” der Isin-Zeit. id: 1969: Einige Beobachtungen zur Göttin Nini(n)sina . . ., in: (ed. W. Röllig) Fs.
W. von Soden (= AOAT 1). 279–305. id. 1976: Kleine Beiträge zur Grammatik des Sumerischen 1. Das modale gram­ matische Element nu-u“-, in: (ed. B. L. Eichler et al.) Fs. S. N. Kramer (= AOAT 25) 371–78. id. 1980: Das sumerische Kurzepos “Bilgame“ und Akka” (= AOAT 2009/1). id. 1982: Einführung in die Sumerologie (esp. pp. 27–97, “Einiges zur Sprache”). G. Rubio 1999: On the alleged ‘Pre-Sumerian’ substratum, JCS 51, 1–16. W. Sallaberger 1993: Der kultische Kalender der Ur III-Zeit (= UAVA 7/1 und 7/2). A. Salonen 1968: Agricultura Mesomotamica. R. Scholtz 1934: Die Struktur der sumerischen engeren Verbalpräfixe (Konjugations­ präfixe) speziell dargelegt an der I. und II. Form (E- und Mu-Konjugation) (= MVAG 39/2). M. Schretter 1990: Emesal-Studien. Y. Sefati 1998: Love Songs in Sumerian Literature. Critical Edition of the DumuziInanna Songs. A. Shaffer 1963: Sumerian Sources of Tablet XII of the Epic of Gilgame“ (= Ph.D. diss. Philadelphia). ¿. W. Sjöberg 1975: in-nin “à-gur4-ra. A Hymn to the Goddess Inanna by the enPriestess En¢eduanna, ZA 65, 161–253. E. Sollberger 1952: Le système verbal dans les inscriptions “royales” présargoniques de Laga“. id. 1961: Le syllabaire présargonique de Laga“, ZA 54, 1–50. id. 1966: The Business Administration Correspondence under the kings of Ur (= TCS 1). H. Steible 1982: Die altsumerischen Bau- und Weihinschriften I. II (= FAOS 5/ I–II). G. Steiner: Intransitiv-passivische und aktivische Verbalauffassung, ZDMG 126, 230–80. id. 1981: ›am†u und Marû als verbale Kategorien im Sumerischen und im Akkadischen, RA 75, 1–14. P. Steinkeller 1979: Notes on Sumerian Plural Verbs, Or. 48, 54–67. M. P. Streck 2000: Das amurritische Onomastikon der altbabylonischen Zeit I, Die Amurriter. Die onomastische Forschung. Orthographie und Phonologie. Normalmorphologie (= AOAT 271/1). V. V. Struve 1984: (ed. G. Kh. Kaplan) Onomastika rannedinasti‘eskogo Laga“a. M.-L. Thomsen 1984: The Sumerian Language. An Introduction to its History and Grammatical Structure (= Mesopotamia 10); 32001, 3rd unchanged and unex­ tended edition. H. Vanstiphout 1985: On the Verbal Prefix /i/ in Standard Sumerian, RA 79, 1–15. Aa. Westenholz 1991: The Phoneme /o/ in Akkadian, ZA 81, 10–19. C. Wilcke 1969: Das Lugalbandaepos. id. 1968: Das modale Adverb I-GI4-IN-ZU im Sumerischen, JNES 27, 229–42.
id. 1988: Anmerkungen zum ‘Konjugationspräfix’ /i/- und zur These vom “silbi­
schen Charakter der sumrischen Morpheme” anhand neusumerischer Verbalformen beginnend mit ì-, íb-, ì-im- und ì-in-, ZA 78, 1–48. id. 1990: Orthographie, Grammatik und literarische Form. Beobachtungen zu der Vaseninschrift Lugalzaggesis (SAKI 152–156), in: (ed. Tz. Abusch et al.) Fs. W. L. Moran, 455–504. id. 2002: Der Kodex Urnammu (CU): Versuch einer Rekonstruktion, in: (ed. Tz. Abusch) Mem. Th. Jacobsen, 291–333.



185

id. 2003: (in print) (the verb si sá), in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa. C. Woods 2003: (in print) (Sumerian demonstratives), in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa. Yang Zhi 1989: Sargonic Inscriptions from Adab (= The Institute for the History of Ancient Civilizations. Periodic Publications of Ancient Civilizations 1). M. Yoshikawa 1968a: On the Grammatical Function of -e- of the Sumerian Verbal Suffix -e-dè/-e-da(m), JNES 27, 251–61. id. 1968b: The Marû and ›am†u Aspects in the Sumerian Verbal System, Or. 37, 401–16. id. 1979: The Sumerian Verbal Prefixes mu-, i- and Topicality, Or. 48, 185–206. id. 1981: Plural Expressions in Sumerian Verbs, ASJ 3, 111–24. id. 1993a: Spatial Deictic System in Sumerian, ASJ 15, 185–92. id. 1993b: Studies in the Sumerian Verbal System (= ASJ SS 1), a reprint of 24 articles published between 1968 and 1993. A. Zgoll 2003: . . ., in: (ed. K. Maekawa) Fs. M. Yoshikawa. G. Zólyomi 1999: Directive infix and oblique object in Sumerian: An account on the history of their relationship, Or. 68, 215–53.

This page intentionally left blank

INDEX

[a] (exclamation) 167 a > i (e.g., dab5 > díb) diachronically 14 ablative(-instrumental) (case) 33 f., 41 f. absolutive (case) 33–35 achevé (verbal aspect) 73 adjectives 25, 47–48 adverbiative 26, 42 adverbs 26 f., 69 affirmative 1 (verbal mood) 117 f. affirmative 2 (verbal mood) 119 f. affirmative 3 (verbal mood) 120 a(l)- (prefixed verbal indicator) 111 f. [alala] (exclamation) 167 [allili] (exclamation) 167 [alulu] (exclamation) 167 Anatolia 4 “and” (lack of ) 41 areal situation (Sumero-Akkadian) 4 f., 173 ff. [aua] (exclamation) 167 b (variant of verbal indicator [bi]) 100 f. B = base B-[Ø] (verb) 130–132 [ba] (non-ventive directional indicator) 94 f. B-[a] 132 f., 137 f. B-[ada] (verb) 134 f., 136 f. [bara] (modal verbal indicator: vetitive) 117 [bara] (modal verbal indicator: negative affirmative) 118 base (verb) 71 and passim Basque (prefix chains) 72 B-B-[Ø] (verb) 130 f. [bda] (non-ventive directional indicator) 96 B-[ed] 130, 134 B-[eda] 134–136 B-[ede] 134–136 [bi] (demonstrative) 50 [bi] (non-ventive directional indicator) 98–101 -bi(-da) “and” 41

*[bra] 22 [b“i] (non-ventive directional indicator) 98 [bta] (non-ventive directional indicator) 97 cardinal numbers 61–66 case 33–45 Caucasus 4 causative 101 causative (Akkadian) 98 f. circumpositional syntagma 56 class (person, non-person) 1, 24 clusters (syllabic) 22 cognates of Sumerian (alleged) 2 f. cohortative (verbal mood) 115 f. comitative (case) 33 f., 40 f. compounds (nominal) 2 compound verbs 142–150 conjugated participle 137–142 conjugation 81 conjugation patterns 81–88 conjugation pattern 1 (intransitive and passive) 81–83 conjugation pattern 2a (transitive) 83–87 conjugation pattern 2b (transitive) 87–90 conjunctions 27, 161–162 conjunctions and subjunctions 161–165 connecting indicators (verb) 112, 121–127 consonants 7–8, 14–21 copula 55, 82–83 cuneograms, classification of 8–10 dative (case) 33 f., 40
dedli 31 f.
deli 62, 141 f.
demonstrative particles
49–51 demonstrative pronouns
57 determination (of nouns)
49–51 determinatives 9 f.
dili see deli
dimensional indicators (verb) 92–109 diphthongs 13 f.

175 -H 40 ¢ 20 [ha] (modal verbal indicator: precative) 116 f. ergativity 1 f. 132 f.. 167–170 exclusive 30 finite verb 71 focus 2 fractions 67 free genitive 38 f. 129 f. Hurrian 4 [i] (prefixed verbal indicator) 109–111 [ilu] (exclamation) 168 [ilulamma] (exclamation) 168 [(i)mmeri] (ventive verbal indicator) 108 imperative 127–130 extended i.. imperfective (verbal aspect) 74 inachevé (verbal aspect) 74 inalterable (class of verbs) 75 inclusive 30 indefinite pronoun (?) 59 indicative (verbal mood) 113 infinitive (Akkadian) 71 [(i)nga] (connective verbal indicator) 123–127 interrogative pronouns 57 f.. 35 f. [hu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator: affirmative 1) 117 f. 128 f.. 36–39 Georgian (stops) 16 Girsu/Laga“ 4 grids (for verbal bases) 71 f. [heam] (exclamation. [ena] (subjunction) 164 [enea] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 95 f. 168 heteronymy (verbal base) 71 hi-a 32 Hiatustilger (r) 94. “intransitive” 36 [inu] “no” (exclamation) 169 inverted genitive construction 31 e/i (difficulty to distinguish them) 14 [e] (prefixed verbal indicator) 109–111 -e (demonstrative?) 50 Ebla glosses 4 [ed] 74. ¢am†u 71 ff. 42 [e“i] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 97 exclamations 27. 50. ∞g 17 [ga] (modal verbal indicator: cohortative) 115 [gana] (exclamation) 168 Gatumdu 4 “-gé” 36 [ge] (modal verbal indicator: cohortative) 115 §gen/du (irregular behaviour in non-finite verb) 140 f. unextended i. [he] (modal verbal indicator: affirmative 1) 116 f.  genitive (case) 33 f. 44 ergative (case) 33 f. [ha] (modal verbal indicator: affirmative 1) 117 f.. [ene“i] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 98 equative (case) 33 f. 35 f. . “frozen” morpheme 2 frustrative (verbal mood) 120 f.. “yes”) 27. 99 [hu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator: precative) 116 f. 67 dug4/e/di 76 f..188 directive (case) 33 f. 90 f. polite i.. 74–78 [gu(mu)] (modal verbal indicator: cohortative) 115 H (Sumerian consonantal phoneme) 19 f. 82 [eda] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 96 [elala] (exclamation) 168 Elamite 3 [ellu] (exclamation) 168 eme-gi7(-r) 1 eme-Ki-en-gi-ra 1 Emesal 66 (numerals) 171 f. 43 f. [he] (modal verbal indicator: precative) 116 f. distribution 41 f. [era] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 94 [eri] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 99 [e“] (adverbiative) 26.. passim ¢am†u participle 47. 122 f.

. Iran 4 [i Utu] (exclamation) 169 ke4 36 KID 36 ll. locative 2) 98 [na] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 94 [na] (modal verbal indicator: affirmative 2) 119 f. 16. [mmi] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 f. borrowed from Akkadian) 162 marû 73 f.. [na] (modal verbal indicator: prohibitive) 118 f. lexical lists 2. linguistic area (Sumero-Akkadian) 4 f. [(e)neda] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 96 negative affirmative (verbal mood) 118 negative precative see vetitive negative verbal indicators 113–118 [(e)ne“i] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 98 [ni] (non-ventive dimensional indicator: locative 2) 98 f. l2 17 f.. 37 locative (case) 33 f. [mda] (ventive dimensional indicator) 105 [me] 1st p. 57 [(e)ne] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 101 [(e)nea] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 95 f. 39... [nda] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 96 [ne(n)] (demonstrative) 49 f. pronoun) 55 f. 26 linear script (Elamite) 3 linguistic affiliation (of Sumerian) 2 f. [mmini] (ventive dimensional indicator) 109 modal indicators (verb) 113–121 “monosyllabic myth” 4 [m“i] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [mta] (ventive dimensional indicator) 106 189 [mu] (= mu-V-) (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [mu] (“year”. 23 f. 25. as subjunction) 164 [muda] (ventive dimensional indicator) 105 [mueda] (ventive dimensional indicator) 105 [mueda(?)] (ventive dimensional indica­ tor) 105 [muera] (ventive dimensional indicator) 103 [mue“i] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [muna] (ventive dimensional indicator) 103 f. [me] (ventive dimensional indicator) 108 [mea] (ventive dimensional indicator) 104 measures (terminology) 67 [mel(i)ea] (exclamation) 169 mes-Ane-pada construction 133 [mini] (ventive dimensional indicator) 108 [mma] (ventive dimensional indicator) 104 [mmara] (ventive dimensional indicator) 106 [mmeri] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 f. loan-words (Sumerian in Akkadian) 7. [munda] (ventive dimensional indicator) 105 [mune] (ventive dimensional indicator) 108 [muni] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [mun“i] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [muri] (ventive dimensional indicator) 107 [mu“i] (ventive dimensional indicator) 106 [n] (variant of [ni]. [nam] (nominal prefix) 25 Nan“e 4 nasals (consonants) 16 f. 102 . 173–178 linguistic environment (of Sumerian) 3–4 liquids 17 f. 44 locative 2 see [ni] logograms 8 [ma] (ventive dimensional indicator) 103 -ma (conjunction.

polite imperative 122 f. 3) 120 prospective (connective verbal indicator) [(e)“i] (non-ventive dimensional 121–123 indicator) 97 Proto-Ea 8 . 13 sign names (Akkadian) 7. 48 r.190  “Proto-Elamite” script 3 quadruplication (of verbal base) 81 quotation particle 157 f. 14 ordinal numbers 66 f. subjunctions 27.. 162–165 phonetic indicators 8 subordinate temporal clause 42 f. syllabic spelling (in Akkadian. [ra] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 97. 61–66 o (in Sumerian) 7 sibilants 20 f. 3) 120 pronominal conjugation 137–142 [“e] (demonstrative) 51 pronouns 25 f. stops 8. sg. (number) 31 r. [“a] (modal verbal particle: affirmative prohibitive (verbal mood) 118 f. phonology 13–21 substantivation 25 vowels 13–14 see also nominalization consonants 14–21 substantives 24 plural bases (verb) 74. syllabaries 11 possessive pronouns 56 f. to post-nominal and/or post-verbal identify Sumerian) 7 particles other than case particles syllable-closing consonants (spelling of ) 157–160 11 Präsens-Futur 73 syllable structure 22 Präteritum 73 syllabograms 8 precative (verbal mood) 116 f. o (in Akkadian) 7. 31–33 numerals 26. 31.. 90 f. 61–67 [nu“] (modal verbal indicator: frustrative) 120 f. of verbal base 75 reflexive pronoun (?) 58 [(e)ri] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 99 [ni] (non-ventive dimensional indicator: 3rd p.. r. person class) 99 “no” 27. passive 95 ‘Sprachgruppe’ 3 perfective (verbal aspect) 74 standard construction (verb) 74 person 30 f. alleged) 4 Sumero-Akkadian linguistic area 4 f. 56 f. 79–81 pseudo r. of verbal base 75 f. 29–31. 169 nominalization (of finite verbal forms) 150–155 non-finite verb 71 non-finite verbal forms 130–142 non-ventive dimensional indicators 93 (grid) 94–102 [n“i] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 98 [nta] (non-ventive dimensional indicator) 97 [nu] (modal indicator: negative indicative. Sanskrit (stops) 16 secondary construction (verb) 74 sequence (of post-nominal particles) 53 sexagesimal system 26. 55–59 [“i] (modal verbal particle: affirmative pronouns (alleged) 58 f. specification (of nouns) 49–51 spelling of Sumerian 8–11 particles 23 spelling variants 7 parts of speech 23–27 “split ergativity” 2. negation of non-finite verbal forms) 113–115 “nuclear” writing 11 number 2. 24. 78 substratum (pre-Sumerian. 173–178 possession 24. of adjectives 25. 14–16 personal pronouns 55 f. 109 redundancy of spelled consonants 10. 35 reduplication 24 f. §r 18 f.

prospective 121–123 see also polite imperative [u] (conjunction) 161 f. 13 f. 148 f. [u] (verbal connective indicator.. [“u(mu)] (modal verbal particle: affirmative 3) 120 “u ti/te(-∞g) (behaviour of the “object”) 145 f. 168 87 . [ta] (non-ventive verbal indicator) Tepe Ya˙yà 3 terminative (case) 33 f. quantity 7 “without” 41 “word” 23 word order (S–O–V) 2 “yes” zero 27. 57 -ùtu (Akkadian) 25 109 191 ventive (Sumerian) 92 ventive (Akkadian) 92 ventive dimensional indicators 93 (grid) 103–109 verb 26. 13 f. 71–155 verbalizer (in compound verbs) 143.. vetitive (verbal mood) 117 vocative (case) 35 “vowel harmony” 99 f. phonemes 7 quality 7. ternary system (of numerals) 66 “transitive” 36 triplication (of verbal base) 81 [tukumbi] 163 f. [ua] (exclamation) 170 [uda] (subjunction) 162 f. [ulili] (exclamation) 170 ur5 (demonstrative) 55. 42 f. 110 vowels 7.

Grapow. Brunner. ISBN 90 04 06555 5 2. B. E. Lentz. A. E. Doerfer. Poppe. D. H. Spies. ISBN 90 04 00857 8 2/2. M. G.HANDBOOK OF ORIENTAL STUDIES (HANDBUCH DER ORIENTALISTIK) Section I: NEAR AND MIDDLE EAST Abt. Mit Beiträgen von E. Heissig. ocharisch. Röhrborn. E. Mit Beiträgen von K. verbesserte und erweiterte Au age. E. F. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1958). H. A. Schott. Rosenberger. R. B. Schott. Heichelheim. U. Baumstark. J. Ashtor. A. S. Mit Beiträgen von A. Bolens. I: DER NAHE UND MITTLERE OSTEN ISSN 0169-9423 Band 1. ISBN 90 04 00852 7 3. D. ISBN 90 04 04802 2 Band 7 Armenisch und Kaukasische Sprachen. von Gabain. 1988. Kees. Fahd. Regierung und Verwaltung des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. Friedrich. P. L. ISBN 90 04 00849 7 3. ISBN 90 04 00860 8 Band 6. Henning. G. Arié. Mit Beiträgen von N. Neumann. Müller. Sinor. Baer. G. 1968. Togan. Religionsgeschichte des alten Orients. HoVmann. Menges. E. Literatur. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1959). 1979. Nachdruck mit Berichtigungen und Ergänz-ungen. Gershevitch. S. A. 1982. Altkleinasiatische Sprachen [und Elamitisch]. Ägyptische Schrift und Sprache. A. 1971. Lewis. G. ISBN 90 04 00863 2 . Otten. J. Benzing. Höfner. 1973. Ägyptologie 1. ISBN 90 04 03777 2 2. Mit Beiträgen von B. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1955) mit Zusätzen und Berich­ T tigungen. 2. Baldick and Sh. R. Littmann. Turkologie. Hempel. Widen­ gren. Mit Zusätzen von H. A. W. O. Wirtschaftsgeschichte des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. Spuler. Pritsak. Solta. Mit Beiträgen von I. ISBN 90 04 00853 5 4/2. Bailey. Mit Beiträgen von A. Boyce. Dietrich. Kadkan¯. W. M. Mongolistik. H. Regierung und Verwaltung des Vorderen Orients in islamischer Zeit. Spiegel. ISBN 90 04 00862 4 Band 8. Geschichte der islamischen Länder 5/1. ISBN 90 04 00854 3 Band 3. Iranistik 1. O. E. B. Westendorf. M. W. S. R. Brockelmann. 1963. ISBN 90 04 06481 8 3. B. 1969. Battal-Tamays. J. W. Lopatin. Morenz. S. 1981. Otto. Reiner. A. R. Fück. W. Kammenhuber. Literatur. 1964. I. 1981. 2. C. Bosch Vilá. Morrison. J. Reprint with additions of the 1st (1963) ed. 1977. O. Keilschriftforschung und alte Geschichte Vorderasiens 1-2/2. Rodinson. Brunner. Mit Beiträgen von D. 1970. H. Sourdel und J. 1964. Dietrich. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1953-1954). B. O. ISBN 90 04 06497 4 Band 2. F. Spuler. Z. K. Kees. Mit Beiträgen von H. Menges. M. With Contributions by G. G. H. W. G. T. Mit Beiträgen von H. Geschichte des alten Ägypten. Brunner. Cooper. Linguistik. History of Persian Literature from the Beginning of the Islamic Period to the Present Day. ISBN 90 04 05915 6 5/2. Semitistik Semitistik. Reprint. 1967. Caferoglu. ISBN 90 04 00855 1 Band 4. H. Mit Beiträgen von G. Aalto. Spuler. Orientalische Geschichte von Kyros bis Mohammed. 1968. Rücker. Lambton. Fecht. ISBN 90 04 00859 4 3. H. V. Eissfeldt. V. 1964. Otto. ISBN 90 04 08550 5 6/1. Taeschner. L. H. A. Inglisian. J. K. C. Hansen. J. Altaistik 1. Religion 1/1. Schröder. Idris und K. W. Mit Beiträgen von A. Deeters. Altenmüller. Geschichte des alten Vorderasien. Helck. Temir. Mit Beiträgen von H. M. Schmökel. G. S. ISBN 90 04 03194 4 Band 5. 1966. A. Mit Beiträgen von J. Pritsak. S. Mit Beiträgen von W. H. ISBN 90 04 03017 4 2/1. Tungusologie. Krause. O. Morenz. Ehrenkreutz. K. 1979. Heubeck. Dresden. Posch. Fuchs. Morgen­ stierne. Otto. H.

B. {-s-l – {-l-y. A History of Zoroastrianism. ISBN 90 04 10489 5. Introduction—Sources— { – {-kh-r. I. Thielmann. The Writings of Central Sudanic Africa. Volume I.). ISBN 90 04 09604 3 Band 11 Endress. O. Arabic Literature of Africa. Die Natur. Alshut. ISBN 90 04 03783 7 Ergänzungsband 6 1. 1964. Unter Mitarbeit von J. Chrestomathie de papyrologie arabe. Borger. 1961. 1992. ISBN 90 04 00865 9 Ergänzungsband 2 1. Materials for a Dictionary of the Mediæval Translations from Greek into Arabic. 934-722 v. A Historical Chart of the Muslim World. ISBN 90 04 10216 7 Fascicle 4. S. Ch. S. Chehata. Marín. E. Hein. ISBN 90 04 09271 4 2. Under the Achaemenians. Korosc. R. Fascicle 1. Schmeink. M. 1992. 1966. 1970. Arnzen. Ila – inna. Gutas. Ibrahim. Spies. Cultural Anthropology of the Middle East. Hofheinz. & Gutas. 1: 1965-1987. R. 1995. ISBN 90 04 03423 4 Ergänzungsband 7 Gomaa. Kornrumpf. K. W. ISBN 90 04 06506 7 1/2/2/3. A. 1973. ISBN 90 04 09494 6 Fascicle 2. Mit Verbesserungen und Zusätzen. (eds. The Early Period. 1900. Radtke and K. Leipoldt. A History of the Druzes. Hunwick. ISBN 90 04 08847 4 1/2/2/2.-J. 1964. M. Ansay. Alshut. Spuler. Gutas. Schmeink. 1992. Band 12 Jayyusi. H. M. Chr. J. Hinz. 1994. Grohmann. Arberry. with the assistance of K. Compiled by John O. A. K. J.und Geheimwissenschaften im Islam. D. Die Medizin im Islam. ISBN 90 04 09450 4 Volume II. Editorial Consultant: Albrecht Hofheinz. with the assistance of K. 1972. Chief consultant to the editor. St. Ullmann. Compiled by G. M. Hein. ISBN 90 04 00864 0 Ergänzungsband 1 1. Arnzen. Fück. (ed. ISBN 90 04 09599 3 Band 13 Hunwick. Documents relatifs à la vie privée. Pohl. O. 2nd ed. R.). G. W. A. Rev. Religionsgeschichte des Orients in der Zeit der Weltreligionen. Lapanne-Joinville. Nachdruck der I Erstausgabe (1955) mit Zusätzen und Berichtigungen. The Legacy of Muslim Spain. 1970. S. and O’Fahey. Boyce. D. O’Fahey. Beck. Schramm. Endress & D. with the assistance of . G. A History of Zoroastrianism. Mit Beiträgen von A. W. ISBN 90 04 00870 5 2. L. and Grenet. M. 1989. ISBN 90 04 09437 7 Band 10 Strijp. Nachdruck der Erstausgabe (1961). Einleitung in die assyrischen Königsinschriften. sociale et administrative dans les premiers siècles islamiques. B. M. E.1/2/2/1. 1991. ISBN 90 04 03333 5 Ergänzungsband 8 Kornrumpf. Ullmann. G. S . Mayr und W. J. Till. Ch. with the assistance of Chr. Baz. Vol. A Greek and Arabic Lexicon.). S. J. with the . Endress. M. Pohl. ISBN 90 04 09893 3 Fascicle 3. 1982. E. ed. Y. Compiled by G. With a Contribution by R. Zoroastrianism under Macedonian and Roman Rule. Seidl. ISBN 90 04 03549 4 Ergänzungsband 9 Firro. Pohl. ¸ ISBN 90 04 00867 5 Ergänzungsband 5 1/1. Chr. J. Arabische Chronologie und Arabische Papyruskunde. St. Mit Beiträgen von E. ISBN 90 04 09551 9 Ergänzungsband 3 Orientalisches Recht. Gutas. 1972. Hein. 1994. {-kh-r – {-s-l. 1. Einleitung in die assyrischen Königsinschriften. ISBN 90 04 00869 1 1/2. Dietrich. Boyce. 1992. 1973. R. M. slamische Maße und Gewichte umgerechnet ins metrische System. Compiled by R. J. R. J. Arnzen. Compiled by G. Chr. R. Strothman. Compiled by R. A History of Zoroastrianism. St. V. . Hein & J. Adam. Arnzen. Mit Beiträgen von J. Samaran. M. with the assistance of M. ISBN 90 04 00866 7 2. Tyan. Roussier. C. S. Boyce. A Bibliography. I. Chr. (eds. The Writings of Eastern Sudanic Africa to c. 1993. (GALex ). Gutas & R. G. Vikør. A. M. R. von Gabain. Pritsch. Widengren. Endress & D. Endress & D. Osmanische Bibliographie mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der Türkei in Europa. Abu Salim. Khoury. F. Das zweite Jahrtausend vor Chr. 1997. 2.

English Summaries and Extensive Citations. Jagd. N. Canaanite in the Amarna Tablets. Hunwick with the assistance of Ousmane Kane. 1996. ISBN 90 04 10614 6 Band 29 Corriente. 1996. ISBN 90 04 10068 7 Band 21 Hoftijzer. S. 1994. 2: 1988-1992. M. ISBN Set (4 Volumes) 90 04 10503 4 Volume I. Mosak Moshavi and B. Kassim. H. ISBN 90 04 10523 9 Volume IV. Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Verbal System. (ed. ISBN 90 04 9820 8. Steiner. 1996. Etymologisches Wörterbuch der Kartwel-Sprachen. Rudiger Seesemann. Reese and Ewald Wagner. V.). Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Pronouns. Nouns. Band 22 Lagarde. L. H. R. 2: Ab-bildungen. Compiled by John O.S. ISBN 90 04 10444 5 Band 25 Rainey.L. Judaism in Late Antiquity. Index du Grand Commentaire de Fahr al-DÊn al-Ra z Ê. The Empire of the Mahdi. A Dictionary of Andalusi Arabic. and Jongeling. Orthography. W.C. Dictionary of the North-West Semitic Inscriptions. Bernard Salvaing. La civilisation phénicienne et punique. Spiel. 2003. ISBN 90 04 10051 2 Band 19 al-Zwaini. 1995. Bd. ISBN 90 04 10129 2 Band 17 Neusner. Bonner. The Writings of Western Sudanic Africa. A Bibliography of Islamic Law. 1994. Stefan Reichmuth and Muhammad Sani Umar. V. 1997. Morphosyntactic Analysis of the Particles and Adverbs. J.). ISBN 90 04 010745 2 Band 28 Sivan. Numerals. The Rise of the Fatimids. Porten. ISBN Set (2 Parts) 90 04 09821 6 Part One: { . 2 Parts. Bd. ISBN 90 04 10362 7 Band 23 Kinberg. ISBN 90 04 09817 8 Part Two: M . Hamidu Bobboyi. ISBN 90 04 10521 2 Volume II. ISBN 90 04 10056 3 Band 27 Strijp. 1997. Judaism in Late Antiquity. Corpus der bildlichen Quellen zu Leibesübungen. O’Fahey with the assistance of Hussein Ahmed. Cultural Anthropology of the Middle East. Manuel de recherche. M. (ed. K. References and Index of Texts Cited. ISBN 90 04 10494 1 Volume III. F. V. J. ISBN 90 04 10421 6 Band 24 Fähnrich. and Stolbova. und Sardshweladse. With Full Definitions. Jonathan Miran. (éd. Roman Loimeier. 1995. Compiled by R. ISBN 90 04 09799 6 Band 16 Neusner. Materials for a Reconstruction.T. ISBN 90 04 09974 3 (Set) Band 15 Haas. 1994. A. A Bibliography. 1994. and Peters. 1997. O. (eds. A Grammar of the Ugaritic Language. A Lexicon of al-Farra " ’s Terminology in his Qur"an Commentary. The Writings of the Muslim Peoples of Northeastern Africa. J. ISBN 90 04 10938 2 Volume IV.F. und Herb. A Linguistic Analysis of the Mixed Dialect used by Scribes from Canaan. A. 1994. V. ISBN 90 04 12444 6 Band 14 Decker. 1995. Phonology. Mohamed M. Lidwien Kapteijns.1: Text.). R. 1994. D. 2003. Part Two: Historical Syntheses. Tanz und verwandten Themen. ISBN 90 04 09799 6 Band 18 Orel. With appendices by R. ISBN 90 04 10522 0 Volume III. 1980-1993. Translated from the German by M. ISBN 90 04 10009 1 Band 20 Krings. Vol.). Bildatlas zum Sport im alten Ägypten. Geschichte der hethitischen Religion. 1996. E. ISBN 90 04 09846 1 . Scott S. 1995. Part One: The Literary and Archaeological Sources. Hamito-Semitic Etymological Dictionary. ISBN 90 04 10524 7 Band 26 Halm.assistance of Razaq Abubakre. Mark Sey and Ivor Wilks.

1998. J. 1998. H. 1999. L. H. The Mishnah. ISBN 90 04 12121 8 Band 49 Avery-Peck. and Sou´ek. H. 1999. D. ISBN Set (2 parts) 90 04 10809 2 Band 34 Klengel. A Grammar of the Bedouin Dialects of the Northern Sinai Littoral. G. The Kingdom of Kush. Corpus Inscriptionum Arabicarum Palaestinae (CIAP). T. 1 : Glossary 2001. Ancient Jewish Art and Archaeology in the Diaspora 1998. J.L. Vol.2. Etymological Dictionary of Egyptian. and Society in Eastern Arabia. M. W. Astral Sciences in Mesopotamia. Vol. A.1: B. J. G.J. B. 2000.F. ISBN 90 04 110836 Band 31 Török. J. ISBN Set (2 Bände) 90 04 10319 8 Band 37 Civil. C. R. Vol. ISBN 90 04 11284 7 Band 43 Daiber. 1999. ISBN 90 04 11858 6 (dl.H. 1. ISBN 90 04 09648 5 Volume II. ISBN 90 04 11492 0 Band 46 Neusner. 2) ISBN 90 04 11645 1 (set) Band 51 Holes. 2 Volumes. ISBN 90 04 10878 5 Band 36 Westendorf. ISBN 90 04 10988 9 Band 40 Neusner. Religious Perspectives 1999. G. Index of Names.J. Handbook of Ugaritic Studies. and Healey. 1999.E. III. ISBN 90 04 10201 9 Band 35 Hachlili. J. 1999. and Wyatt. ISBN Set (2 Volumes) 90 04 11347 9 Volume I. Onomasticon of the Hittite Pantheon. ISBN 90 04 11348 7 Band 44 Hunger.W. ISBN 90 04 10127 6 Band 45 Neusner. The Mishnah. 1999. Systematische Bibliographie der Hethitologie. ISBN 90 04 11510 2 Band 48 Takács. 2. Geschichte des hethitischen Reiches 1998. 1999. Bibliography of Philosophical Thought in Islam. The Old Syriac Inscriptions of Edessa and Osrhoene. 1999. and Porten. and Pingree. ISBN 90 04 010448 8 Band 32 Muraoka. Judaism in Late Antiquity. 1997. A. 1) ISBN 90 04 11859 4 (dl. ISBN 90 04 11186 7 Band 41 Neusner. 1997. 1999. 1999. M. ISBN 90 04 11538 2 Takács.E. Culture. W. (2 Volumes) 2000. J. ISBN 90 04 010745 2 Vol. ISBN 90 04 11282 0 Band 42 Drijvers. ISBN 90 04 11007 0 Band 38 Siegelová. Handbuch der altägyptischen Medizin. A Grammar of Egyptian Aramaic. A Dictionary of Samaritan Aramaic.1. Handbook of the Napatan-Meroitic Civilization. Social Perspectives 1999. ISBN 90 04 11491 2 Band 47 Khan. Mesopotamian Lexicography. van. Etymological Dictionary of Egyptian. Dialect. ISBN 90 04 10763 0 Band 52 Jong. 1999. Judaism in Late Antiquity. Bridging the Linguistic Gap between the Eastern and Western Arab World. 2000. and Neusner.Band 30 Sharon. A Grammar of Neo-Aramaic. ISBN 90 04 11868 3 . H. 1999. ISBN 90 04 10499 2 Band 33 Gessel. Terms and Topics.G. Alphabetical List of Publications 1999. R. Judaism in Late Antiquity IV. J. ISBN Set (3 Bände) 90 04 11205 7 Band 39 Watson. ISBN 90 04 11262 6 Band 50 Tal. V. N. III. de. 1: A. Vol. 2001. B.

J.. and Neusner. ISBN 90 04 13198 1 Band 71 Edzard.J. A. Judaism in Late Antiquity III. D. B. U. ISBN 90 04 11801 2 Band 62 Grossmann. Christliche Architektur in Ägypten.. 2001. A Comparative Lexical Study of Qur’§nic Arabic.. Band 56 Avery-Peck. Judaism in Late Antiquity III. and Chilton. A Phoenician-Punic Grammar. Studies in the Aramaic Legal Papyri from Elephantine. Ch. ISBN 90 04 12001 7 Band 57 Avery-Peck. 2001. World View. With an Introductory Essay. the World and God in the Stories of FarÊd al-DÊn #Aãã§r ISBN 90 04 12068 8 Band 70 Borg. 2001. edited and translated by Wilfred G.. 2001. Y.J. Sumerian Grammar. M. ISBN 90 04 12000 9.3. ISBN 90 04 12608 2 Band 72 Westbrook. A History of Ancient Near Eastern Law. A Glossary of Cypriot Maronite Arabic (Arabic-English). 2003. J. A. The Mishnah in Contemporary Perspective. A. H. ISBN 90 04 12128 5 Band 63 Weipert. J. 2003. Neusner. A. 2000.Band 53 Avery-Peck. G. Watson.R. A. The Ocean of the Soul. Classical Arabic Philology and Poetry.. The Special Problem of the Synagogue. ISBN 90 04 12100 5 Band 61 Zammit. Judaism in Late Antiquity V. ISBN 90 04 12003 3 Band 58 Gacek. Narrative illustration in Persian lithographed books.C.J. Where we stand: Issues and Debates in Ancient Judaism. 2002.J. The Luwians. 2003. Comparing Judaisms. ISBN 90 04 12868 9 Band 67 del Olmo Lete. The Judaism of Qumran: A Systemic Reading of the Dead Sea Scrolls. A. and Neusner. A History of the Animal World in the Ancient Near East. and Sanmartín. translated by John O’Kane with Editorial Assistance of Bernd Radtke. and Chilton. R. ISBN 90 04 12995 2 . ISBN 90 04 11771 7 Band 55 Avery-Peck.. J. B. 2001.2. R. Part I. ISBN 90 04 12061 0 Band 60 Marzolph.O. A Dictionary of the Ugaritic Language in the Alphabetic Tradition (2 vols) ISBN 9004128913 (set) Band 68 Melchert. 2003.4. The Arabic manuscript tradition. and Neusner. ISBN 90 09 12126 9 Band 65 Avery-Peck. ISBN 90 04 13009 8 Band 69 Ritter. J. B.J. Where we stand: Issues and Debates in Ancient Judaism. 2002. A Glossary of Technical Terms and Bibliography. Neusner. 2001. Man. J. The Judaism of Qumran: A Systemic Reading of the Dead Sea Scrolls.. ISBN 90 09 12515 9 Band 66 Muffs. P. Theory of Israel. A Bibliographical Handbook of Important Editions from 1960 to 2000.E. ISBN 90 04 11892 6 Band 54 Krahmalkov. 2002. A. R. Judaism in Late Antiquity V. 2002 ISBN 90 04 12342 3 Band 64 Collins. 2002. J. H.1.