Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 54

IES

IES PRACTICE
PRACTICE GUIDE
GUIDE

Effective Literacy and


English Language Instruction
for English Learners
in the Elementary Grades

NCEE 2007-4011
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
The Institute of Education Sciences (IES) publishes practice guides in education
to bring the best available evidence and expertise to bear on the types of systemic
challenges that cannot currently be addressed by single interventions or programs.
Authors of practice guides seldom conduct the types of systematic literature searches
that are the backbone of a meta-analysis, though they take advantage of such work
when it is already published. Instead, they use their expertise to identify the most
important research with respect to their recommendations, augmented by a search
of recent publications to assure that the research citations are up-to-date.

One unique feature of IES-sponsored practice guides is that they are subjected to
rigorous external peer review through the same office that is responsible for inde-
pendent review of other IES publications. A critical task of the peer reviewers of a
practice guide is to determine whether the evidence cited in support of particular
recommendations is up-to-date and that studies of similar or better quality that
point in a different direction have not been ignored. Because practice guides depend
on the expertise of their authors and their group decisionmaking, the content of a
practice guide is not and should not be viewed as a set of recommendations that in
every case depends on and flows inevitably from scientific research.

The goal of this Practice Guide is to formulate specific and coherent evidence-based
recommendations for use by educators addressing a multifaceted challenge that
lacks developed or evaluated packaged approaches. The challenge is effective lit-
eracy instruction for English learners in the elementary grades. The Guide provides
practical and coherent information on critical topics related to literacy instruction
for English learners.
IES PRACTICE GUIDE

Effective Literacy and


English Language Instruction
for English Learners
in the Elementary Grades

Russell Gersten (Chair)


RG RESEARCH GROUP AND UNIVERSITY OF OREGON

Scott K. Baker
PACIFIC INSTITUTES FOR RESEARCH AND UNIVERSITY OF OREGON

Timothy Shanahan
UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS AT CHICAGO

Sylvia Linan-Thompson
THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT AUSTIN

Penny Collins
Robin Scarcella
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA AT IRVINE

NCEE 2007-4011
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
This report was prepared for the National Center for Education Evaluation and Re-
gional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences under Contract ED-02-CO-0022
by the What Works Clearinghouse, a project of a joint venture of the American In-
stitutes for Research and The Campbell Collaboration, and Contract ED-05-CO-0026
by Optimal Solutions Group, LLC.

Disclaimer
The opinions and positions expressed in this practice guide are the authors’ and do
not necessarily represent the opinions and positions of the Institute of Education
Sciences or the United States Department of Education. This practice guide should
be reviewed and applied according to the specific needs of the educators and edu-
cation agency using it and with full realization that it represents only one approach
that might be taken, based on the research that was available at the time of pub-
lication. This practice guide should be used as a tool to assist in decision-making
rather than as a “cookbook.” Any references within the document to specific educa-
tion products are illustrative and do not imply endorsement of these products to
the exclusion of other products that are not referenced.

U.S. Department of Education


Margaret Spellings
Secretary

Institute of Education Sciences


Grover J. Whitehurst
Director

National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance


Phoebe Cottingham
Commissioner

July 2007

This report is in the public domain. While permission to reprint this publication is
not necessary, the citation should be:

Gersten, R., Baker, S.K., Shanahan, T., Linan-Thompson, S., Collins, P., & Scarcella,
R. (2007). Effective Literacy and English Language Instruction for English Learners in
the Elementary Grades: A Practice Guide (NCEE 2007-4011). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sci-
ences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee.

This report is available on the IES web site at http://ies.ed.gov/ncee

Alternate Formats
On request, this publication can be made available in alternate formats, such as
Braille, large print, audio tape, or computer diskette. For more information, call the
Alternate Format Center at (202) 205-8113.
iii

Contents
Foreword from the Institute of Education Sciences    iv

Preface from the authors   vi

About the authors   ix

Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest   xi

Overview   1

Checklist for carrying out the recommendations   2

Recommendation 1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress   5

Recommendation 2. Provide intensive small-group reading interventions   10

Recommendation 3. Provide extensive and varied vocabulary instruction   13

Recommendation 4. Develop academic English   16

Recommendation 5. Schedule regular peer‑assisted learning opportunities   20

Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies   22


Recommendation 1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress   22

Recommendation 2. Provide intensive small-group reading interventions   23

Recommendation 3. Provide extensive and varied vocabulary instruction   24

Recommendation 4. Develop academic English   26

Recommendation 5. Schedule regular peer-assisted learning opportunities   27

Appendix 2. Levels of evidence for the recommendations in the practice guide   29

Notes   31

References   34
iv

Foreword from that do not involve randomization, and the


the Institute of bottom level from the opinions of respected
authorities. Levels of evidence can also be
Education Sciences constructed around the value of particular
types of studies for other goals, such as the
What is a practice guide? The health care reliability and validity of assessments.
professions have embraced a mechanism
for assembling and communicating evi- Practice guides can also be distinguished
dence-based advice to practitioners about from systematic reviews or meta-analyses,
care for specific clinical conditions. Vari- which use statistical methods to summarize
ously called practice guidelines, treatment the results of studies obtained from a rule-
protocols, critical pathways, best practice based search of the literature. Authors of
guides, or simply practice guides, these practice guides seldom conduct the types
documents are systematically developed of systematic literature searches that are
recommendations about the course of care the backbone of a meta-analysis, though
for frequently encountered problems, rang- they take advantage of such work when it
ing from physical conditions such as foot is already published. Instead, they use their
ulcers to psychosocial conditions such as expertise to identify the most important re-
adolescent development.1 search with respect to their recommenda-
tions, augmented by a search of recent pub-
Practice guides are similar to the products lications to assure that the research citations
of expert consensus panels in reflecting the are up-to-date. Further, the characterization
views of those serving on the panel and of the quality and direction of the evidence
the social decisions that come into play as underlying a recommendation in a practice
the positions of individual panel members guide relies less on a tight set of rules and
are forged into statements that all are will- statistical algorithms and more on the judg-
ing to endorse. However, practice guides ment of the authors than would be the case
are generated under three constraints that in a high-quality meta-analysis. Another
typically do not apply to consensus panels. distinction is that a practice guide, because
The first is that a practice guide consists of it aims for a comprehensive and coherent
a list of discrete recommendations that are approach, operates with more numerous
intended to be actionable. The second is and more contextualized statements of what
that those recommendations taken together works than does a typical meta-analysis.
are intended to be a coherent approach to
a multifaceted problem. The third, which Thus, practice guides sit somewhere be-
is most important, is that each recommen- tween consensus reports and meta-analyses
dation is explicitly connected to the level in the degree to which systematic processes
of evidence supporting it, with the level are used for locating relevant research and
represented by a grade (for example, high, characterizing its meaning. Practice guides
moderate, or low). are more like consensus panel reports than
meta-analyses in the breadth and com-
The levels of evidence, or grades, are usually plexity of the topics they address. Practice
constructed around the value of particular guides are different from both consensus
types of studies for drawing causal conclu- reports and meta-analyses in providing
sions about what works. Thus, one typically advice at the level of specific action steps
finds that the top level of evidence is drawn along a pathway that represents a more or
from a body of randomized controlled trials, less coherent and comprehensive approach
the middle level from well designed studies to a multifaceted problem.
Foreword v

The Institute of Education Sciences (IES) One unique feature of IES-sponsored prac-
publishes practice guides in education to tice guides is that they are subjected to
bring the best available evidence and exper- rigorous external peer review through the
tise to bear on the types of systemic chal- same office that is responsible for inde-
lenges that cannot currently be addressed pendent review of other IES publications.
by single interventions or programs. Al- A critical task of the peer reviewers of a
though IES has taken advantage of the his- practice guide is to determine whether
tory of practice guides in health care to pro- the evidence cited in support of particular
vide models of how to proceed in education, recommendations is up-to-date and that
education is different from health care in studies of similar or better quality that
ways that may require that practice guides point in a different direction have not been
in education have somewhat different de- ignored. Peer reviewers also are asked to
signs. Even within health care, where prac- evaluate whether the evidence grades as-
tice guides now number in the thousands, signed to particular recommendations by
there is no single template in use. Rather, the practice guide authors are appropriate.
one finds descriptions of general design A practice guide is revised as necessary to
features that permit substantial variation meet the concerns of external peer reviews
in the realization of practice guides across and gain the approval of the standards
subspecialties and panels of experts.2 Ac- and review staff at IES. The external peer
cordingly, the templates for IES practice review is carried out independent of the
guides may vary across practice guides and office and staff within IES that instigated
change over time and with experience. the practice guide.

The steps involved in producing an IES- Because practice guides depend on the
sponsored practice guide are, first, to se- expertise of their authors and their group
lect a topic, informed by formal surveys of decisionmaking, the content of a practice
practitioners and requests. Next is to recruit guide is not and should not be viewed as a
a panel chair who has a national reputa- set of recommendations that in every case
tion and up-to-date expertise in the topic. depends on and flows inevitably from sci-
Third, the chair, working with IES, selects a entific research. It is not only possible but
small number of panelists to coauthor the also likely that two teams of recognized
practice guide. These are people the chair experts working independently to produce
believes can work well together and have a practice guide on the same topic would
the requisite expertise to be a convincing generate products that differ in important
source of recommendations. IES recom- respects. Thus, consumers of practice
mends that at one least one of the panelists guides need to understand that they are,
be a practitioner with experience relevant to in effect, getting the advice of consultants.
the topic being addressed. The chair and the These consultants should, on average, pro-
panelists are provided a general template vide substantially better advice than an
for a practice guide along the lines of the in- individual school district might obtain on
formation provided here. The practice guide its own because the authors are national
panel works under a short deadline of six to authorities who have to achieve consensus
nine months to produce a draft document. among themselves, justify their recom-
It interacts with and receives feedback from mendations with supporting evidence, and
staff at IES during the development of the undergo rigorous independent peer review
practice guide, but its members understand of their product.
that they are the authors and thus respon-
sible for the final product. Institute of Education Sciences
vi

Preface from National Assessment of Educational Prog-


the authors ress data, to correlational studies and
longitudinal studies of patterns of typical
development. For questions about what
The goal of this Practice Guide is to formu- works best, high-quality experimental and
late specific and coherent evidence-based quasi-experimental studies, such as those
recommendations for use by educators meeting the criteria of the What Works
addressing a multifaceted challenge that Clearinghouse, have a privileged position
lacks developed or evaluated packaged (www.whatworks.ed.gov). In all cases we
approaches. The challenge is effective lit- pay particular attention to patterns of find-
eracy instruction for English learners in ings that are replicated across studies.
the elementary grades. At one level, the
target audience is a broad spectrum of Although we draw on evidence about the
school practitioners—administrators, cur- effectiveness of specific programs and
riculum specialists, coaches, staff develop- practices, we use this information to make
ment specialists, and teachers. At another broader points about improving practice.
level, a more specific objective is to reach In this document we have tried to take a
district-level administrators with a Practice finding from research or a practice recom-
Guide that will help them develop practice mended by experts and describe how the
and policy options for their schools. The use of this practice or recommendation
Guide includes specific recommendations might actually unfold in school settings.
for district administrators and indicates In other words we aim to provide sufficient
the quality of the evidence that supports detail so that a curriculum director would
these recommendations. have a clear sense of the steps necessary
to make use of the recommendation.
Our expectation is that a superintendent
or curriculum director could use this Prac- A unique feature of practice guides is
tice Guide to help make decisions about the explicit and clear delineation of the
policy involving literacy instruction for quality—as well as quantity—of evidence
English learners in the elementary grades. that supports each claim. To do this, we
For example, we include recommendations adapted a semistructured hierarchy sug-
on curriculum selection, sensible assess- gested by the Institute of Education Sci-
ments for monitoring progress, and rea- ences. This classification system uses
sonable expectations for student achieve- both the quality and quantity of available
ment and growth. The Guide provides evidence to help determine the strength
practical and coherent information on of the evidence base in which each rec-
critical topics related to literacy instruc- ommended practice is grounded. (This
tion for English learners. system appears in appendix 2.)

We, the authors, are a small group with Strong refers to consistent and generaliz-
expertise on various dimensions of this able evidence that an approach or prac-
topic. Several of us are also experts in tice causes better outcomes for English
research methodology. The range of evi- learners or that an assessment is reli-
dence we considered in developing this able and valid. Moderate refers either to
document is vast, ranging from expert evidence from studies that allow strong
analyses of curricula and programs, to causal conclusions but cannot be gener-
case studies of seemingly effective class- alized with assurance to the population
rooms and schools, to trends in the on which a recommendation is focused
Preface vii

(perhaps because the findings have not do not mean to suggest that it is the best
been sufficiently replicated) or to evidence study reviewed for the recommendation
from studies that are generalizable but or necessarily an exemplary study in any
have more causal ambiguity than offered way.
by experimental designs (such as statisti-
cal models of correlational data or group We have not addressed two main areas.
comparison designs where equivalence
of the groups at pretest is uncertain). For First, we did not address English learners
the assessments, moderate refers to high- in middle school and high school. Schools
quality studies from a small number of face very different issues in designing in-
samples that are not representative of the struction for students who enter school
whole population. Low refers to expert when they are young (and often have re-
opinion based on reasonable extrapola- ceived no education or minimal instruc-
tions from research and theory on other tion in another language or educational
topics and evidence from studies that do system) and those who enter in grades 6
not meet the standards for moderate or to 12 and often are making a transition to
strong evidence. another language and another educational
system. For that reason we chose to focus
In this English Learner Practice Guide we on only one of these populations, students
use effect sizes for describing the magni- in the elementary grades.
tude of impact of a program or practice
reported in a study. This metric is increas- Second, we did not address the language
ingly used in social science research to of instruction. Our goal is to provide guid-
provide a gauge of the magnitude of the ance for all English learners, whether
improvement in performance reported in a they are taught to read in their home lan-
research study. A common index of effect guage, in English (by far the most preva-
size is the mean difference between the lent method in the United States), or in
experimental and comparison conditions both languages simultaneously. The rec-
expressed in standard deviation units. In ommendations are relevant for students
accordance with the What Works Clearing- regardless of their language of reading
house criteria we describe an effect size of instruction. The best language to use for
+0.25 or higher as substantively important. initial reading instruction has been the
This is equivalent to raising performance subject of great debate and numerous re-
of a group of students at least 10 percen- views of the literature.
tile points on a valid test.
Some experts conclude that students are
For each recommendation we include an best served by having some reading in-
appendix that provides more technical in- struction in their native language,3 others
formation about the studies and our deci- that students should be taught to read si-
sions regarding level of evidence for the multaneously in both English and their na-
recommendation. To illustrate the types of tive language,4 still others that the results
studies reviewed we describe one study in are inconclusive.5 Many reviews have cited
considerable detail for each recommenda- serious methodological flaws in all the
tion. Our goal in doing this is to provide studies in terms of internal validity;6 oth-
interested readers with more detail about ers have not addressed the quality of the
the research designs, the intervention research design.7 Currently, schools op-
components, and how impact was mea- erate under an array of divergent policies
sured. By including a particular study, we set by the state and local school district.
viii Preface

In most cases school administrators have and Lana Edwards Santoro and Rebecca
little say on issues involving language of Newman-Gonchar of RG Research Group.
initial reading instruction, so we do not We also wish to acknowledge the excep-
take a position on this intricate issue for tional contribution of Elyse Hunt-Heinzen,
this Practice Guide. our research assistant on the project, and
we thank Charlene Gatewood of Optimal
We would like to thank the following in- Solutions and the anonymous reviewers
dividuals for their helpful feedback and for their contributions to the refinement
reviews of earlier versions of this Guide: of this report.
Catherine Snow and Nonie Lesaux of Har-
vard University; Maria Elena Arguelles, in- Dr. Russell Gersten
dependent consultant; Margaret McKeown Dr. Scott Baker
of University of Pittsburgh; Michael Coyne Dr. Timothy Shanahan
of University of Connecticut; Benjamin S. Dr. Sylvia Linan-Thompson
Clarke of University of Oregon and Jeanie Dr. Penny Collins
Smith of Pacific Institutes for Research; Dr. Robin Scarcella
ix

About the authors UIC Center for Literacy. He was president of


the International Reading Association until
Dr. Russell Gersten is executive director of May 2007. He was executive director of the
Instructional Research Group, a nonprofit Chicago Reading Initiative, a public school
educational research institute, as well as improvement project serving 437,000 chil-
professor emeritus in the College of Edu- dren, in 2001–02. He received the Albert J.
cation at the University of Oregon. He Harris Award for outstanding research on
currently serves as principal investigator reading disability from the International
for the What Works Clearinghouse on the Reading Association. Dr. Shanahan served
topic of instructional research on English on the White House Assembly on Reading
language learners. He is currently princi- and the National Reading Panel, a group
pal investigator of two large Institute of convened by the National Institute of Child
Education Sciences projects involving ran- Health and Human Development at the
domized trials in the areas of Reading First request of Congress to evaluate research
professional development and reading on successful methods of teaching read-
comprehension research. His main areas ing. He has written or edited six books,
of expertise are instructional research on including Multidisciplinary Perspectives on
English learners, mathematics instruc- Literacy, and more than 100 articles and
tion, reading comprehension research, research studies. Dr. Shanahan’s research
and evaluation methodology. In 2002 Dr. focuses on the relationship of reading and
Gersten received the Distinguished Spe- writing, school improvement, the assess-
cial Education Researcher Award from ment of reading ability, and family literacy.
the American Educational Research As- He chaired the National Literacy Panel on
sociation’s Special Education Research Language-Minority Children and Youth
Division. Dr. Gersten has more than 150 and the National Early Literacy Panel.
publications in scientific journals, such as
Review of Educational Research, American Dr. Sylvia Linan-Thompson is an associ-
Educational Research Journal, Reading Re- ate professor, Fellow in the Mollie V. Davis
search Quarterly, Educational Leadership, Professorship in Learning Disabilities at
and Exceptional Children. The University of Texas at Austin, and
director of the Vaughn Gross Center for
Dr. Scott Baker is the director of Pacific In- Reading and Language Arts. She is associ-
stitutes for Research in Eugene, Oregon. He ate director of the National Research and
specializes in early literacy measurement Development Center on English Language
and instruction in reading and mathemat- Learners, which is examining the effect of
ics. Dr. Baker is co-principal investigator on instructional practices that enhance vo-
two grants funded by the Institute of Edu- cabulary and comprehension for middle
cation Sciences, and he is the co­director school English learners in content areas.
of the Oregon Reading First Center. Dr. She has developed and examined reading
Baker’s scholarly contributions include interventions for struggling readers who
conceptual, qualitative, and quantitative are monolingual English speakers, English
publications on a range of topics related to learners, and bilingual students acquiring
students at risk for school difficulties and Spanish literacy.
students who are English learners.
Dr. Penny Collins (formerly Chiappe) is
Dr. Timothy Shanahan is professor of an assistant professor in the Department
urban education at the University of Illi- of Education at the University of Califor-
nois at Chicago (UIC) and director of the nia, Irvine. Her research examines the
x About the authors

development of reading skills for children Dr. Robin Scarcella is a professor in the
from linguistically diverse backgrounds School of Humanities at the University
and the early identification of children at of California, Irvine, where she also di-
risk for reading difficulties. She is involved rects the Program of Academic English/
in projects on effective instructional inter- ESL. She has taught English as a second
ventions to promote academic success for language in California’s elementary and
English learners in elementary, middle, secondary schools and colleges. She has
and secondary schools. Dr. Collins is on written many research articles, appear-
the editorial boards of Journal of Learn- ing in such journals as The TESOL Quar-
ing Disabilities and Educational Psychology. terly and Studies in Second Language Ac-
Her work has appeared in Applied Psycho- quisition, as well as in books. Her most
linguistics, Journal of Educational Psychol- recent volume, Accelerating Academic
ogy, Journal of Experimental Child Psychol- English, was published by the University
ogy, and Scientific Studies of Reading. of California.
xi

Disclosure of potential series is not referenced in the practice


conflicts of interest guide.

Dr. Baker has an author agreement with


Practice guide panels are composed of in- Cambium Learning to produce an instruc-
dividuals who are nationally recognized tional module for English learners. This
experts on the topics about which they are module is not written and is not referenced
rendering recommendations. IES expects in the practice guide.
that such experts will be involved profes-
sionally in a variety of matters that relate Dr. Linan-Thompson was one of the pri-
to their work as a panel. Panel members mary researchers on intervention studies
are asked to disclose their professional that used Proactive Reading curriculum,
involvements and to institute deliberative and she developed the ESL adaptations
processes that encourage critical examina- for the intervention. Linan-Thompson co-
tion the views of panel members as they authored the research reports that are de-
relate to the content of the practice guide. scribed in the Guide.
The potential influence of panel members’
professional engagements is further muted Dr. Shanahan receives royalties on vari-
by the requirement that they ground their ous curricula designed for elementary and
recommendations in evidence that is docu- middle school reading instruction, includ-
mented in the practice guide. In addition, ing Harcourt Achieve Elements of Reading
the practice guide is subjected to indepen- Fluency (Grades 1-3); Macmillan McGraw-Hill
dent external peer review prior to publica- Treasures (Grades K-6); and AGS Glove-Pear-
tion, with particular focus on whether the son AMP (Grades 6-8). None of these prod-
evidence related to the recommendations ucts, though widely used, are aimed spe-
in the practice guide has been has been cifically at the English learner instructional
appropriately presented. market (the focus of this practice guide).
Macmillan publishes a separate program
The professional engagements reported aimed at the English learner population.
by each panel members that appear most Shanahan is not involved in that program.
closely associated with the panel recom-
mendations are noted below. Dr. Scarcella provides on-going teacher
professional development services on aca-
Dr. Gersten, the panel chair, is a co-­author demic vocabulary through the University
of a forthcoming Houghton Mifflin K-6 of California Professional Development
reading series that includes material re- Institutes that are authorized by the Cali-
lated to English learners. The reading fornia State Board of Education.
1

Overview been sufficient research aimed at under-


standing how to improve the quality of
The National Assessment of Educational literacy instruction for English learners.
Progress (NAEP) has tracked the achieve- Only about a dozen studies reach the level
ment of Hispanic students since 1975. Al- of rigor necessary to determine that spe-
though many English learners are in the cific instructional practices or programs
Hispanic designation, English learners as do, in fact, produce significantly better
a group have only recently been disaggre- academic outcomes with English learners.
gated in the NAEP analyses. Recent analy- This work has been analyzed and reviewed
sis of long-term trends8 reveals that the by the What Works Clearinghouse (the
achievement gap between Hispanics and work of the Clearinghouse is integrated
Whites in reading has been significantly into our text when relevant; new studies
reduced over the past 30 years for 9-year- will be added periodically).
olds and 17-year-olds (although not for
13-year-olds).9 Despite the paucity of rigorous experimen-
tal research, we believe that the available
Despite apparent progress in the ear- evidence allows us to provide practical
lier grades, major problems persist. For recommendations about aspects of in-
instance, the 2005 achievement gap of struction on which research has cast the
35 points in reading between fourth- sharpest light. This research suggests—as
grade English learners and non-English opposed to demonstrates—the practices
learners was greater than the Black-White most likely to improve learning for Eng-
achievement gap.10 And the body of sci- lish learners.
entific research on effective instructional
strategies is limited for teaching English Over the years many terms have been used
learners.11 to refer to children who enter school using
a language other than English: limited Eng-
There have been some significant recent lish proficiency (LEP), English as a second
advances. Of particular note is the in- language (ESL), English for speakers of
crease in rigorous instructional research other languages (ESOL), second language
with English learners. Districts and states learners, language minority students,
have increasingly assessed progress of and so on. In this Practice Guide we use
English learners in academic areas and in “English learners” because we feel it is the
English language development. Several ex- most descriptive and accurate term for the
amples in the literature illustrate success largest number of children. This term says
stories among English learners—both for nothing about children’s language profi-
individual students and for schools. These ciency or how many other languages they
students, despite having to learn English may use—it simply recognizes that they
while mastering a typical school curricu- are learning English.
lum, have “beaten the odds” in academic
achievement.12 This Practice Guide provides five recom-
mendations, integrated into a coherent
How can we increase the chances that and comprehensive approach for improv-
more English learners will achieve these ing the reading achievement and English
successes? To answer, we must turn first language development of English learners
to research. Unfortunately, there has not in the elementary grades.
2 Overview

Recommendations 4. Ensure that the development of formal


or academic English is a key instruc-
1. Conduct formative assessments with tional goal for English learners, begin-
English learners using English lan- ning in the primary grades. Provide
guage measures of phonological pro- curricula and supplemental curricula
cessing, letter knowledge, and word to accompany core reading and math-
and text reading. Use these data to ematics series to support this goal.
identify English learners who require Accompany with relevant training and
additional instructional support and professional development (Level of
to monitor their reading progress over Evidence: Low).
time (Level of Evidence: Strong).
5. Ensure that teachers of English learn-
2. Provide focused, intensive small-group ers devote approximately 90 minutes
interventions for English learners de- a week to instructional activities in
termined to be at risk for reading prob- which pairs of students at different
lems. Although the amount of time in ability levels or different English lan-
small-group instruction and the inten- guage proficiencies work together on
sity of this instruction should reflect academic tasks in a structured fashion.
the degree of risk, determined by read- These activities should practice and
ing assessment data and other indica- extend material already taught (Level
tors, the interventions should include of Evidence: Strong).
the five core reading elements (phono-
logical awareness, phonics, reading flu- One major theme in our recommendations
ency, vocabulary, and comprehension). is the importance of intensive, interactive
Explicit, direct instruction should be English language development instruction
the primary means of instructional de- for all English learners. This instruction
livery (Level of Evidence: Strong). needs to focus on developing academic
language (the decontextualized language
3. Provide high-quality vocabulary in- of the schools, the language of academic
struction throughout the day. Teach discourse, of texts, and of formal argu-
essential content words in depth. In ment). This area, which researchers and
addition, use instructional time to ad- practitioners feel has been neglected, is
dress the meanings of common words, one of the key targets in this Guide.
phrases, and expressions not yet
learned (Level of Evidence: Strong).
3

Checklist for Recommendation 2.


carrying out the Provide intensive small-group
reading interventions
recommendations
Use an intervention program with stu-
Recommendation 1. dents who enter the first grade with weak
Screen for reading problems reading and prereading skills, or with older
and monitor progress el em ent a r y stu d ent s w i t h r e a din g
problems.
Districts should establish procedures
for—and provide training for—schools to Ensure that the program is implemented
screen English learners for reading prob- daily for at least 30 minutes in small, homo-
lems. The same measures and assessment geneous groups of three to six students.
approaches can be used with English learn-
ers and native English speakers. Provide training and ongoing support
for the teachers and interventionists (reading
Depending on resources, districts should coaches, Title I personnel, or paraeducators)
consider collecting progress monitoring data who provide the small-group instruction.
more than three times a year for English
learners at risk for reading problems. The Training for teachers and other school
severity of the problem should dictate how personnel who provide the small-group in-
often progress is monitored—weekly or bi- terventions should also focus on how to de-
weekly for students at high risk of reading liver instruction effectively, independent of
problems. the particular program emphasized. It is im-
portant that this training include the use of
Data from screening and progress moni- the specific program materials the teachers
toring assessments should be used to make will use during the school year. But the train-
decisions about the instructional support ing should also explicitly emphasize that
English learners need to learn to read. these instructional techniques can be used
in other programs and across other subject
Schools with performance benchmarks areas.
in reading in the early grades can use the
same standards for English learners and for Recommendation 3.
native English speakers to make adjust- Provide extensive and varied
ments in instruction when progress is not vocabulary instruction
sufficient. It is the opinion of the panel that
schools should not consider below-grade- Adopt an evidence-based approach to
level performance in reading as “normal” vocabulary instruction.
or something that will resolve itself when
oral language prof iciency in English Develop districtwide lists of essential
improves. words for vocabulary instruction. These
words should be drawn from the core read-
Provide training on how teachers are to ing program and from the textbooks used
use formative assessment data to guide in key content areas, such as science and
instruction. history.
4 Recommendations

Vocabulary instruction for English learn- Provide teachers with appropriate pro-
ers should also emphasize the acquisition of fessional development to help them learn
meanings of everyday words that native how to teach academic English.
speakers know and that are not necessarily
part of the academic curriculum. Consider asking teachers to devote a
specific block (or blocks) of time each day to
Recommendation 4. building English learners’ academic English.
Develop academic English
Recommendation 5.
Adopt a plan that focuses on ways and Schedule regular peer-assisted
means to help teachers understand that in- learning opportunities
struction to English learners must include
time devoted to development of academic Develop plans that encourage teachers
English. Daily academic English instruction to schedule about 90 minutes a week with
should also be integrated into the core activities in reading and language arts that
curriculum. entail students working in structured pair
activities.
Teach academic English in the earliest
grades. Also consider the use of partnering for
English language development instruction.
5

Recommendation 1. It is very important to assess phonological


Screen for reading processing, alphabet knowledge, phonics,
and word reading skills. These measures,
problems and whether administered at the middle or
monitor progress end of kindergarten (or at the beginning
of the first grade) have been shown to ac-
Conduct formative assessments with curately predict later reading performance
English learners using English language in all areas: word reading,16 oral reading
measures of phonological processing, fluency,17 and reading comprehension.18
letter knowledge, and word and text So, it is essential to administer some type
reading. Use these data to identify of screening to provide evidence-based be-
English learners who require additional ginning reading interventions to students
in the primary grades.
instructional support and to monitor
their reading progress over time.
In no way do these findings suggest that
oral language proficiency and comprehen-
How to carry out the sion are unimportant in the early grades.
recommendation These language abilities are critical for
long-term success in school.19 We expand
1. Districts should establish procedures for— on this point in Recommendation 4, by dis-
and provide training for—schools to screen cussing the importance of directly teach-
English learners for reading problems. The ing academic English. The assessment
same measures and assessment approaches findings point to effective ways to screen
can be used with English learners and native English learners for reading problems and
English speakers. to determine whether they are making suf-
ficient progress in foundational areas of
Research shows that early reading mea- early reading.
sures, administered in English, can be
used to screen English learners for read- 2. Depending on resources, districts should
ing problems. This finding is important consider collecting progress monitoring data
because until recently it was widely be- more than three times a year for English
lieved that an absence of oral proficiency learners at risk for reading problems. The
in English prevented English learners from severity of the problem should dictate how
learning to read in English,13 thus limiting often progress is monitored—weekly or bi-
the utility of early screening measures. weekly for students at high risk of reading
The common practice was to wait until problems.20
English learners reached a reasonable
level of oral English proficiency before 3. Data from screening and progress moni-
assessing them on measures of beginning toring assessments should be used to make
reading. In fact, oral language measures decisions about the instructional support
of syntax, listening comprehension, and English learners need to learn to read.
oral vocabulary do not predict who is
likely to struggle with learning to read.14 Data from formative assessments should
Yet research has consistently found that be used to modify (and intensify) the read-
early reading measures administered in ing and English language development (or
English are an excellent means for screen- ESL) instruction a child receives. These
ing English learners, even those who know interventions should be closely aligned
little English.15 with the core reading program. Possible
6 1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress

interventions are described in Recom- general sense of students’ early literacy


mendation 2. skills, but these scores should not be used
as an indication of how well students are
Caveat: Measures administered at the be- likely to respond to instruction.
ginning of kindergarten will tend to over-
identify students as “at risk.”21 A better 4. Schools with performance benchmarks in
indication of how students will respond reading in the early grades can use the same
to school instruction comes from perfor- standards for English learners and for native
mance scores from the middle and end English speakers to make adjustments in in-
of kindergarten. These scores should be struction when progress is insufficient. It is
used to identify students requiring seri- the opinion of the panel that schools should
ous instructional support. Scores from the not consider below-grade-level performance
beginning of kindergarten can provide a in reading as “normal” or something that will

Summary of evidence to support this are useful in both kindergarten and first
recommendation grade.25
• Measures of reading single words and
This recommendation is based on a large knowledge of basic phonics rules are
number of studies that used reading assess- useful in first grade.26 Toward the mid-
ment measures with English learners. Level dle and end of the first grade, and in the
of Evidence: Strong. next few grades, measures of reading
connected text accurately and fluently
Twenty-one studies demonstrated that three are useful.27
types of measures—phonological process-
ing, letter and alphabetic knowledge, and For students in grades 2 to 5. Three studies
reading of word lists or connected text—are have demonstrated that oral reading fluency
valid means of determining which English measures are valid screening measures for
learners are likely to benefit from typical English learners and are positively associated
classroom reading instruction and which with performance on comprehensive stan-
children will require extra support (see ap- dardized reading tests. Oral reading fluency
pendix 1 for details).22 The primary purpose is emerging as a valid indicator of reading
of these measures is to determine whether progress over time for English learners.28
interventions are necessary to increase the
rate of reading achievement. These mea- These criterion-related validity studies are
sures meet the standards of the American particularly important because another set
Psychological Association for valid screen- of studies has investigated whether English
ing instruments.23 learners can attain rates of reading growth
comparable with those of their monolingual
For students in kindergarten and grade 1. The peers. These studies have demonstrated that
early screening measures for kindergarten English learners can learn to read in English
and the first grade fit into three categories: at the same rate as their peers in the primary
grades (K–2).29 Much of this evidence comes
• Measures of phonological awareness— from research in Canada and from schools
such as segmenting the phonemes in a providing intensive and systematic instruc-
word, sound blending, and rhyming— tion for all children, supplementary instruc-
are useful in both kindergarten and first tion for those falling behind, and instruction
grade.24 in settings where growth in oral proficiency
• Measures of familiarity with the alphabet is supported by both peer and teacher-stu-
and the alphabetic principle, especially dent interactions. Evidence on reading inter-
measures of speed and accuracy in let- ventions for English learners in the United
ter naming and phonological recoding, States is the focus of Recommendation 2.
1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress 7

resolve itself when oral language proficiency note that the authors of this Guide did not
in English improves. conduct a comprehensive review of avail-
able assessments (such a large undertaking
Using the same standards for successful was beyond the scope of this project), and
reading performance with English learn- individual schools and districts should be
ers and native English speakers may mean careful when selecting assessments to use.
that a higher percentage of English learn- It is important to select assessments that
ers will require more intensive reading in- are reliable and valid.
struction to reach the benchmarks, but we
believe that this early emphasis on strong 5. Provide training on how teachers are to
reading instruction will be helpful in the use formative assessment data to guide
long run. Providing intensive early read- instruction.
ing instruction for English learners does
not imply they have a reading disability or The primary purpose of the formative
they are not able to learn to read as well assessment data is to determine which
as other students. It means that while they students are at risk (or not making suffi-
are learning a new language and learning cient progress) and to increase the inten-
to read in that language simultaneously, sity of reading instruction systematically
they face challenges other students do not for those students. We recommend that
face. The instruction they receive should school-based teams of teachers be trained
reflect the nature of this challenge. to examine formative assessment data to
identify which English learners are at risk
A score on a screening measure indicat- and to determine what instructional ad-
ing that an English learner may be at risk justments will increase reading progress.
for reading difficulties does not mean the These teams can be for one grade or across
child has a reading disability. Being at risk grades. We believe that the reading coach,
means that the English learner needs extra in schools that have one, should play a key
instructional support to learn to read. This role on these teams. Although principals
support might simply entail additional should also play an important leadership
time on English letter names and letter role, it may be difficult for them to attend
sounds. In other cases additional support all meetings or be extensively involved.
might entail intensive instruction in pho-
nological awareness or reading fluency. Possible roadblocks and solutions
Additional diagnostic assessments can
be administered to determine what areas 1. Some teachers believe that reading prob-
require instructional attention. lems may resolve themselves once English
learners develop proficiency in oral English.
Unless districts have considerable re- So, they are hesitant to refer these students
sources and expertise, they should not try for additional assistance or to provide in-
to develop the formative assessment mate- tensive instruction in foundational areas of
rials on their own. Several screening and beginning reading.
progress monitoring materials that have
been developed and tested with native- There is no evidence to support the position
English-speaking students are appropriate that early reading problems experienced
to use with English learners. Information by English learners will resolve themselves
about formative assessments can be found once oral language skills in English are
from a number of sources, including the established.30 Districts should develop
Web and commercial developers. Please and disseminate materials explaining that
8 1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress

using English oral language proficiency task, but this is different from knowing
is as accurate as flipping a coin to decide word meanings. For an assessment to be
which English learners are likely to have valid the examiner must clearly explain
difficulty learning how to read. the nature of the task and the child must
understand what she or he is being asked
To demonstrate that phonological, letter to do. If possible, adults who are fluent in
knowledge, and word reading measures the child’s native language can be hired
are effective screening measures, princi- and trained to administer assessments.
pals and reading coaches can look at data But good training is essential. When ap-
from their own schools and see the links propriate, the examiner can explain or
between scores on these measures in kin- clarify the task in the language the child
dergarten and the first grade and later understands best. For districts with many
scores on state reading assessments. native languages and few professional ed-
ucators fluent in each native language, it
2. Some teachers may feel that it is unfair to is possible to make CDs of instruction in
test a child in a language that she or he does the appropriate native languages.
not understand.
Make sure at least two or three practice
Although this is true in many areas, it is items are provided before formal admin-
not true for tasks involving phonological istration, when the task is modeled for the
processing, as long as the child under- child and corrective feedback is provided.
stands the nature of the task.31 If students This will give all children (especially Eng-
possess phonemic awareness of a word lish learners) the opportunity to under-
such as cake or fan, even without know- stand what the task requires of them. An
ing the meaning they should be able to tell important consideration for all assess-
the examiner the first, middle, and last ments is to follow the testing guidelines
sounds in the word. Phonological aware- and administration protocols provided
ness is an auditory skill that greatly helps with the assessment. It is acceptable to
students with reading development, and it provide practice examples or explanations
transfers across languages. That is, if stu- in the student’s native language outside
dents learn the structure of sounds in one the testing situation. During the testing,
language, this knowledge will help them however, it is essential that all assessment
identify individual sounds in a second lan- directions and protocols be followed. Re-
guage without being taught explicitly what member, the purpose of the assessment is
those individual sounds are. It is possible to determine whether children are phono-
to demonstrate this to teachers by having logically aware or know the letters of the
them pull apart the sounds in words from alphabet. It is not to determine how quickly
an unfamiliar language, such as Russian or or well children learn the formative assess-
Arabic. Reading coaches can demonstrate ment task when they are given explicit in-
that once a student knows how to identify struction in how to complete the task.
the beginning, ending, or middle sound of
a word, knowing the meaning of a word is 3. Some teachers may feel that native lan-
irrelevant in being able to reproduce the guage assessments are more valid than
sound. English language measures for this group
of students.
Teachers should be clear that, for pho-
nological processing tasks to be valid, Formative early reading assessments in
English learners have to understand the English are valid for English learners.32 If
1. Screen for reading problems and monitor progress 9

district and state policies permit testing a 5. In districts that have the same early read-
child in her or his native language, it is pos- ing goals and standards for English learners
sible to get a richer picture of her decod- and non-English learners, it is likely that the
ing skills or familiarity with the alphabet. current performance of many English learn-
But this is not necessary for phonological ers will be below these standards.
awareness because it easily transfers across
languages. Students who have this aware- Although the average performance of Eng-
ness in their native language will be able lish learners may be lower than that of
to demonstrate it on an English language non-English learners, there is no reason to
assessment as long as they understand the assume that English learners cannot make
task.33 In other words, even students who the reading progress necessary to reach
are limited in English will be able to dem- high standards of performance.34 This
onstrate knowledge of phonological aware- progress will require providing more in-
ness and decoding in English. tensive instruction than the district might
normally provide in both reading and lan-
4. Districts should anticipate that schools will guage development.
have a tendency to view data collection as
the terminal goal of conducting formative as- 6. Teachers may focus too much on what is
sessments, especially early in the process. tested—phonemic skills, decoding ability, and
oral reading fluency—and neglect instruction
It is important to remind school personnel in comprehension and vocabulary.
that data collection is just one step in the
process. The goal of collecting formative In monitoring student progress in phono-
assessment data is to identify students logical processing, phonics, and reading
who are not making adequate progress fluency, instruction in the development
and to increase the intensity of instruction of comprehension and higher order think-
for these students. In a system where the ing skills may be overlooked. But these
performance of all children is assessed skills should not be neglected. Instruc-
multiple times a year, it is easy to become tion in comprehension and higher order
consumed by ways of organizing, analyz- skills should receive attention in the ear-
ing, and presenting data and to lose sight liest phases of reading development. The
of the primary purpose of data collection: challenge for schools will be to maintain a
to determine which students need extra strong instructional focus on both higher
support and which do not. and lower order skills.
10

Recommendation 2. • Clear feedback from the teacher when


Provide intensive students make errors.
• Explicit instruction in all areas of read-
small-group reading ing, including explicit comprehension
interventions instruction and explicit vocabulary
instruction. Sufficient coverage of five
areas—phonological awareness, pho-
Provide focused, intensive small-group nics, reading fluency, vocabulary, and
interventions for English learners comprehension—should be a key cri-
determined to be at risk for reading terion in selecting an intervention pro-
problems. Although the amount of gram for use in the school district.36
time in small-group instruction and
the intensity of this instruction should 2. Ensure that the program is implemented
daily for at least 30 minutes in small, homo-
reflect the degree of risk, determined
geneous groups of three to six students.
by reading assessment data and
other indicators, the interventions
Students make gains in reading when they
should include the five core reading have daily instruction in small homoge-
elements (phonological awareness, neous groups based on reading skill and
phonics, reading fluency, vocabulary, receive explicit, clear, direct instruction.37
and comprehension). Explicit, direct So, there is no compelling reason why all
instruction should be the primary students in the group need to be English
means of instructional delivery. learners. In fact, we think there could be
advantages to groups that include na-
How to carry out the tive English speakers and English learn-
recommendation ers because native English speakers can
provide models of more advanced Eng-
1. Use an intervention program with students lish language usage. But to ensure that
who enter the first grade with weak reading students can accelerate their learning,
and prereading skills, or with older elemen- students who are making solid progress
tary students with reading problems.35 based on ongoing assessments should be
regrouped (for example, move students
Because there are many similarities be- making rapid progress to higher perform-
tween the three programs assessed here, ing groups).38
we conclude that other programs that fol-
low the same principles of direct and ex- 3. Provide training and ongoing support for
plicit instruction to teach core reading el- the teachers and interventionists (reading
ements in small groups are likely to have coaches, Title I personnel, or paraeducators)
the same beneficial effects. The major in- who provide the small-group instruction.39
structional principles that characterize the
three programs are: Each of the four research studies that
produced a positive impact on reading
• Multiple opportunities for students to achievement involved extensive training
respond to questions. of the teachers and interventionists. This
• Multiple opportunities for students to training is most effective when all person-
practice reading both words and sen- nel who work with English learners par-
tences, either in a small group or with ticipate together in the same professional
a peer. development activities.40
2. Provide Intensive Small-Group Reading Interventions 11

One key aspect of these interventions is ever their focus, as outlined in the teacher
pacing. It is particularly important that manuals and training materials.
the teachers and interventionists receive
training in how to teach these programs at 4. Training for teachers and other school
an appropriate pace. This critical aspect of personnel who provide the small-group
instruction is frequently overlooked. When interventions should also focus on how to
it is missing from instruction, it is easy for deliver instruction effectively, independent
children to become bored or to lose focus, of the particular program emphasized. It is
which can lead to behavior problems. important that this training include the use
of the specific program materials the teach-
T he t h r e e inter vent ion pr og r a m s ers will use during the school year. But the
­studied—and others like them—contain training should also explicitly emphasize
highly engaging activities of short du- that these instructional techniques can be
ration. The Panel believes that teachers used in other programs and across other
should implement the activities, what- subject areas.41

Summary of evidence to support this comprehension, and vocabulary. The What


recommendation Works Clearinghouse found that all three
curricula demonstrated potentially posi-
This recommendation is based on four high- tive effects on reading achievement. The
quality randomized controlled trials at vari- designation potentially positive refers to an
ous sites with different interventions that effect supported by at least one study but
share core characteristics in design and con- not enough studies to support the Clearing-
tent. Level of Evidence: Strong. house’s highest evaluation of positive.

In the past several years four high-quality An important finding was that in two of the
randomized controlled trials have been four studies the interventions demonstrated
conducted on reading interventions for lasting effects on reading performance. In
struggling English learners. These stud- investigating the longitudinal effects of En-
ies appear as Intervention Reports on the hanced Proactive Reading, positive achieve-
What Works Clearinghouse website.42 Ap- ment outcomes were maintained when stu-
pendix 1 provides technical details on the dents who received the intervention in the
methodology used in these studies, the key first grade were assessed at the end of the
findings, and statistical significance levels. second grade.46 Students in the first grade
These interventions used the following intervention group read at higher levels than
three programs: students in the control group one year after
the intervention ended. For the SRA program
• Enhanced Proactive Reading.43 the positive reading effect was maintained
• Read Well.44 two years after the intervention ended.47
• SRA Reading Mastery/SRA Corrective
Reading.45 The programs used in these studies had many
characteristics in common. They formed a
The participants in these research studies central aspect of daily reading instruction
were English learners in grades 1–5 with and took between 30 and 50 minutes to imple-
serious reading problems (reading at least ment per day. In each study program imple-
one year below grade level or scoring in the mentation involved intensive small-group
lowest quartile on standardized tests). Read- instruction following the principles of direct
ing achievement was assessed on a wide and explicit instruction in the core areas of
range of measures, including word reading, reading.
12 2. Provide Intensive Small-Group Reading Interventions

Examples of these techniques include in- opportunity for small-group instruction.


structional pacing, providing feedback Reducing fragmented instruction must in-
to students, including error corrections, volve the effective coordination of services
modeling, and providing wait time for for English learners, who frequently receive
student responses. For many teachers this additional services in multiple areas and
fast-paced interactive instruction will be from multiple funding sources.
unfamiliar, and coaching support in the
classroom will be critical for them to be 3. Students will miss valuable instructional
effective. This training and coaching in the time in other areas.
classroom should be provided by “master”
teachers with experience in the specific Although students will miss some instruc-
program. tion in other areas while they are receiving
additional small-group reading instruc-
Possible roadblocks and solutions tion, learning to read is critical to all other
learning demands. So, time spent ensuring
1. Teachers may be uncomfortable iden- that students acquire strong reading skills
tifying students for additional reading in- will pay off in the long run. Evidence for
struction if their English language skills are this claim can be found in the sustained
low.48 effects of intervention studies.49

English language proficiency is not a good 4. Arranging a building-level or grade-level


gauge of how well English learners can schedule that allows for additional small-
respond to additional reading instruction group instruction is a complex process.
(see Recommendation 1). In addition to Individual teachers may feel that they do
helping with the development of critical not have the time or resources to provide
reading skills, extra instructional time additional small-group instruction to these
devoted to vocabulary, reading compre- students.
hension, and listening comprehension will
help directly with the development of Eng- Different professionals can provide small-
lish language proficiency. group reading interventions, and schools
will have to consider the options seriously
2. Students already are pulled out of class for if barriers to time and scheduling are to
other services (such as speech, English lan- be overcome.50 The key is training and col-
guage development, or English as a second laboration among all personnel who pro-
language). Pulling students out for additional vide instruction to English learners. This
reading instruction makes their instructional requires a shared focus and commitment.
day too fragmented. The benefits of having a pullout program
for interventions are that students can
A fragmented instructional day is a legiti- be homogeneously grouped, receive ad-
mate concern (and not just for English learn- ditional time on task, and be regrouped
ers). But the Panel believes that reading de- regularly as needed to maximize learning
velopment is too important to withhold any opportunities.
13

Recommendation 3. instruction includes multiple exposures


Provide extensive and to target words over several days and
across reading, writing, and speaking op-
varied vocabulary portunities. A small but consistent body of
instruction intervention research suggests that Eng-
lish learners will benefit most from rich,
intensive vocabulary instruction that em-
Provide high-quality vocabulary phasizes “student-friendly” definitions,52
instruction throughout the day. Teach that engages students in the meaningful
essential content words in depth. In use of word meanings in reading, writing,
addition, use instructional time to speaking, and listening,53 and that pro-
address the meanings of common vides regular review.54 The goal of rich
words, phrases, and expressions not vocabulary instruction is for students to
develop an understanding of word mean-
yet learned.
ings to the point where they can use these
and related words in their communication
How to carry out the and as a basis for further learning.55
recommendation
The core reading program used in the
Vocabulary instruction is essential in classroom is a good place to begin choos-
teaching English learners to read. It is ing words for instruction and methods
rare that core reading programs include for teaching them. For English learners
adequate guidelines for vocabulary in- additional words need to be identified
struction for English learners. So, dis- for instructional attention, and teaching
tricts need to provide teachers with tools procedures need to be much richer and
that will help them support vocabulary more extensive than instruction usu-
development. ally recommended within core reading
programs.56
1. Adopt an evidence-based approach to vo-
cabulary instruction. Valuable for professional development,
teacher study groups and lesson study
The Panel believes that an evidence-based groups can get teachers engaged in plan-
approach should require that teachers ning effective vocabulary instruction.57
provide daily explicit vocabulary instruc- These study groups can be guided by avail-
tion. Evidence-based vocabulary instruc- able texts that provide evidence-based ap-
tion should be a strong part of reading proaches to vocabulary instruction. Activi-
instruction and an integral part of Eng- ties in these study groups should include a
lish language development. Vocabulary good number of hands-on activities, such
instruction should also be emphasized in as transforming textbook definitions into
all other parts of the curriculum, includ- “student-friendly” definitions, identifying
ing reading, writing, science, history, and crucial words in the texts students will
geography. read, and developing daily lesson plans for
intensive vocabulary instruction.58
Typically, the vocabulary instruction
supported by research studies is more 2. Develop districtwide lists of essential
thorough and explicit than that usually words for vocabulary instruction. These
provided in classrooms.51 Researchers con- words should be drawn from the core read-
verge in noting that effective vocabulary ing program and from the textbooks used
14 3. Provide Extensive and Varied Vocabulary Instruction

in key content areas, such as science and Words for instruction should be selected
history. carefully. Long lists of words cannot be
taught in depth because rich vocabulary
A major part of any vocabulary curricu- instruction is time intensive. Only a hand-
lum is specifying the words to be taught. ful of words should be taught in intensive
It is the Panel’s opinion that adopting a ways at any one time. Some authorities
districtwide core vocabulary list for Eng- recommend teaching only about eight to
lish learners will help focus instruction on ten words per week this way, while others
valuable words and reduce unnecessary suggest teaching two to three words per
duplication. A core vocabulary list does day (but always with lots of future review
not prevent teachers or students from and extension).60
adding to this list when problem words
arise in the classroom—in fact, some dis- Reading coaches, teacher teams, curricula
tricts even build in space for the addition specialists, and summer workshops for
of such words. teachers can generate vocabulary lists for
intensive instruction. A key is for teachers
The lists currently identified in core read- to have these lists as they teach reading,
ing programs are inadequate for this pur- social studies, and science units, so they
pose.59 They often fail to emphasize the know in advance which words to teach in
words most critical for understanding a depth. Study groups and grade-level teams
story or most useful for the child’s lan- can do this work.
guage development. For example, many
vocabulary lists stress decoding issues 3. Vocabulary instruction for English learn-
rather than meaning. Thus, to accomplish ers should also emphasize the acquisition
vocabulary instruction goals, districts of meanings of everyday words that native
must develop their own lists and provide speakers know and that are not necessarily
access to these lists for their teachers. part of the academic curriculum.61

Summary of evidence to support this reading comprehension.64 It suggests that


recommendation intense and explicit vocabulary instruction
enhances reading comprehension. Two other
This recommendation is based on three stud- studies support the impact of vocabulary in-
ies conducted specifically with English learn- struction on reading comprehension.65
ers. This recommendation is also indirectly
supported by a strong body of research con- Research shows that English learners need
ducted with native English speakers. Level to learn many words to catch up with their
of Evidence: Strong. native-English-speaking peers’ word knowl-
edge.66 Clearly, not all of the words they need
Three intervention research studies evaluated to learn to make up this gap can be taught
the effectiveness of explicit vocabulary instruc- through explicit vocabulary instruction. Our
tion for English learners.62 They converge in recommendation thus integrates procedures
showing that explicit and intensive vocabulary from studies on explicit vocabulary instruc-
instruction helps English learners understand tion with English learners,67 extensive re-
what they read (see appendix 1 for details). One search with native English speakers,68 and
study, appearing on the What Works Clearing- expert opinion in establishing a comprehen-
house website,63 is rated as demonstrating a sive framework of vocabulary instruction for
potentially positive effect on students’ English English learners.
3. Provide Extensive and Varied Vocabulary Instruction 15

The vocabulary gap between English learn- vehicle for work on vocabulary instruc-
ers and native English speakers is substan- tion, giving teachers a way to share their
tial because English learners do not know frustrations and jointly collaborate on so-
many of the simpler words or conversa- lutions. Study groups can also be a way
tional words that native English speakers to keep effective vocabulary instruction
acquire before they enter school or learn in the forefront of instructional priorities.
in school without explicit teaching. Many They are especially valuable when led by
of these words are crucial for understand- vocabulary experts, who can provide clear
ing text and other academic content. For suggestions about how teachers can con-
example, English learners may not know tinue to move forward to provide effective
such words as bank, take, sink, or can. instruction in the classroom.
Textbook publishers assume that students
know these words and do not include Coaching teachers in effective vocabulary
them as vocabulary targets. Nor do they instruction should have a strong in-class-
provide recommendations for how to ad- room component. There are routines in
dress teaching these words should teach- good vocabulary instruction that teachers
ers have students who do not know them. can learn. For some teachers, these rou-
English learners can acquire these words tines will be learned best through in-class-
easily if teachers provide them with brief room coaching, where coaches provide im-
instruction during lessons. This instruc- mediate feedback and demonstrations.
tion can emphasize the meanings of com-
mon phrases and expressions, not just 2. Some teachers may incorrectly assume
single words. that English learners know a concept and
the word for that concept in their primary
During reading instruction, teachers language—when, in fact, they do not. This is
can teach many of these common words particularly true for technical terms encoun-
­explicitly—in roughly the same way that tered in science, geography, and history. If
they teach content words, but much more students do not know the concept in their
quickly. They can teach many words as primary language, the Panel suggests teach-
they arise in the classroom, drawing at- ing the word directly in English.
tention to the potentially confusing words
and phrases. District practice should en- Caveat: For teachers to help English learn-
sure that these words are also taught ers develop vocabulary knowledge by
and reviewed during English language making connections to a student’s primary
development. language, teachers need some knowledge
of the primary language. If the linguistic
Possible roadblocks and solutions transfer involves a simple concept or a
one-to-one correspondence between the
1. Teaching vocabulary effectively is difficult. student’s primary language (each language
Many teachers will struggle learning how to has an identifiable word for the concept),
provide effective vocabulary instruction to teachers may be able to help students even
English learners.69 when these teachers know very little of the
primary language. But if the concepts are
Concerted professional development and difficult or there is no clear word for the
coaching will be necessary to ensure that concept in the student’s native language,
all teachers learn to provide effective vo- teachers will need more extensive knowl-
cabulary instruction to English learners. edge of the primary language to be able to
Teacher study groups can be an excellent help the student.
16

Recommendation 4. instructional time should focus on the


Develop academic explicit instruction of academic English.71
Recent correlational research supports
English this position.72

Ensure that the development of English learners do not need to master


formal or academic English is a key conversational oral English before they
instructional goal for English learners, are taught the features of academic Eng-
beginning in the primary grades. lish.73 In reading, knowledge of academic
Provide curricula and supplemental English helps students gain perspective
curricula to accompany core reading on what they read, understand relation-
ships, and follow logical lines of thought.
and mathematics series to support this
In writing, knowledge of academic English
goal. Accompany with relevant training
helps students develop topic sentences,
and professional development.
provide smooth transitions between ideas,
and edit their writing effectively. Reading,
How to carry out the discussing, and writing about texts needs
recommendation to be a central part of the English lan-
guage development instruction dispersed
1. Adopt a plan that focuses on ways and throughout the day.74
means to help teachers understand that in-
struction to English learners must include Many teachers may be unaware of the fea-
time devoted to development of academic tures of academic English75 and thus do
English. Daily academic English instruc- not instruct students in the features re-
tion should also be integrated into the core quired to succeed in school.76 The Panel
curriculum. feels that the best way to promote the de-
velopment of academic English is to use
Academic English is the language of the a curriculum with a scope and sequence
classroom, of academic disciplines (sci- aimed at building academic English. Un-
ence, history, literary analysis) of texts fortunately, the Panel knows of no exist-
and literature, and of extended, reasoned ing curricular materials that have solid
discourse. It is more abstract and decon- empirical support for this purpose. That
textualized than conversational English. is why it is important to select published
Those who are knowledgeable about ac- materials carefully and to devote consid-
ademic English know, for example, that erable thought and planning to how these
some words used in everyday conver- materials will be used effectively in the
sation, such as fault, power, or force, classroom.
take on special meanings when used in
science. It is also unfortunate that few resources
provide guidance to districts in teaching
Most scholars believe that instruction in academic English to English learners. Some
academic English—done early, consis- preliminary frameworks and guidelines—
tently, and simultaneously across con- developed by Feldman and Kinsella,77
tent areas—can make a difference in Eng- Girard,78 Dutro and Moran,79 Snow and
lish learners’ ability to understand the Fillmore,80 Diaz-Rico and Weed,81 and
core curriculum and that its importance ­Scarcella82—list topics to address when
increases as children enter the upper focusing on academic English, such as
grades.70 But even in the primary grades, adverbial forms, conditional sentences,
4. Develop Academic English 17

prepositions, words that express relation- of adjectives and adverbs.86 Students need
ships. But these are not designed for regu- practice in using these features in the con-
lar use by teachers in the classroom or as text of meaningful communication (both
an instructional manual. oral and written).87 They also must learn
to use language accurately in a range of
Teachers will need extensive professional situations—to tell stories, describe events,
development and support in using cur- define words and concepts, explain prob-
riculum materials effectively to teach aca- lems, retell actions, summarize content,
demic English.83 and question intentions.88

2. Teach academic English in the earliest Note: For students entering school, atten-
grades. tion in the first year of instruction must
also be devoted to informal, social lan-
Instruction focused on academic English guage. For example, newcomers (English
should not wait until students are able to learners who have recently arrived in the
read and write in English. Before English United States) benefit greatly from imme-
learners are reading, the development diate instruction in social language (Hi!
of age-appropriate academic English—­ What’s up?) and survival language (Help!
morphology, syntax, vocabulary—can be Fire!).89
accelerated orally through planned and
deliberate daily instruction.84 3. Provide teachers with appropriate profes-
sional development to help them learn how
Focused instruction in academic English to teach academic English.
can also build on students’ work with
text. For example, when English learners In the opinion of the Panel, professional
read expository text that includes aca- development needs to be ongoing and to
demic language, teachers should discuss entail a specific and manageable number
the text and the language in structured of key features and principles. Basic fea-
ways.85 Instruction should also focus on tures of English morphology, syntax, and
teaching English learners to use specific discourse need to be addressed carefully
features of academic language related to and gradually so as not to overwhelm
tense agreement, plurals, and proper use teachers.

Summary of evidence to support this and only indirectly address classroom in-
recommendation struction, we cannot conclude that the stud-
ies affirm the effectiveness of instruction of
Because there is little empirical research on academic English at this time. Level of Evi-
the topic and primarily just expert opinion, dence: Low (primarily expert opinion).
the level of evidence is low. Two studies re-
viewed by the What Works Clearinghouse90 Despite the paucity of experimental research,
demonstrate that focused interventions in the strong consensus of expert opinion92 is
two relatively narrow areas of academic that English learners require considerable ex-
English (quality of oral narrative and syntax) plicit and deliberate instruction to learn the
are potentially effective.91 That is, evidence features of the type of formal English used
suggests that they lead to better outcomes in the schools and in academic discourse.93
in highly specific areas of formal, academic This consensus applies to the importance of
English. But because the studies address teaching academic English from the earliest
very selective aspects of academic English grades.94
18 4. Develop Academic English

Professional development should also in- processing and retention. Third, during
clude extensive practical activities, such English language development time, the
as analyzing texts used by students for focus is clearly on language. When teach-
academic English instruction, determining ers try to merge English language develop-
features of language that students need to ment with academics, it becomes easy to
complete specific oral and written assign- lose track of the dual objectives and focus
ments, and designing “student-friendly” more on teaching reading or mathemat-
explanations. Professional development ics or science than on teaching academic
should also give teachers opportunities English. The obvious exception is writing
to practice teaching academic language instruction, a natural fit with teaching aca-
with feedback. demic English.

4. Consider asking teachers to devote a spe- It is easy to overlook academic English and
cific block (or blocks) of time each day to to allow teachers and students to commu-
building English learners’ academic English. nicate in informal English. For this reason,
it might be a good idea for administrators
Experts agree that English learners require to structure specific blocks of time each
time each day when the primary instruc- day to ensure its instruction. For example,
tional goal is developing academic English in kindergarten, the instruction of aca-
(as opposed to mastering the academic demic English can be routinely incorpo-
content).95 A recent observational research rated into the instruction of storytelling
study found that students’ growth in Eng- and vocabulary development at specific
lish language proficiency was much higher times each day. As Saunders, Foorman,
in classrooms where a separate block of and Carlson97 have shown, providing spe-
time was devoted to ESL or English lan- cific blocks of instruction in English lan-
guage development.96 So, in addition to guage development leads to gains in mea-
the better integration of teaching academic sures of oral language proficiency. In later
English in the context of academic content grades, specific blocks of time dedicated
such as reading or mathematics, the Panel to the development of academic English
also suggests that there be specific times can be scheduled, for example, in reading
during the day when the primary instruc- and writing instruction and in the instruc-
tional focus is on English language devel- tion of vocabulary in all subject matter.
opment and that some of the time be de- Scheduling regular blocks of time for the
voted to academic English. We are aware instruction of academic English should
that this recommendation extrapolates not only guarantee an increased focus
from only one study and that this study on academic English in the classroom. It
looked at all English language develop- should also make teachers more diligent
ment instruction, not only academic Eng- in structuring instructional activities that
lish instruction. So, this should be consid- require the use of academic English and in
ered as merely a recommendation based monitoring their students’ development of
on our opinion. academic English.

We believe that devoting specific blocks of Possible roadblocks and solutions


time to academic English has three distinct
advantages. First, it increases the time Eng- 1. Some educators may want to cushion their
lish learners have to learn the language. English learners, believing that academic
Second, instruction spaced throughout the English is too hard for them to develop or
day provides better opportunities for deep that the expectations are too demanding.
4. Develop Academic English 19

Many teaching approaches still advocate to all students, including English learners
giving English learners highly simplified, and native English speakers.
informal texts that are easy to read but not
challenging. The problem with regularly 3. Many teachers fail to link vocabulary in-
giving English learners a diet of familiar struction to instruction on proper language
reading material is that the academic texts usage.
of assessments and most content areas re-
main unfamiliar. Informal, narrative texts Even when English learners know word
tend to be familiar, but reading these texts meanings, they may be uncertain about
does not lead to proficiency in academic how to use new words appropriately. As
English. In academic writing crammed knowledge deepens, words have to be
with facts, the content is often unfamiliar used with the appropriate number (goose,
to English learners. geese), tense (is, are, was), and word form
(fun, funnier, funny). Systematic instruc-
The focus on developing academic English tion in usage and language conventions
can come after a challenging text has been needs to be a core feature of English lan-
read and discussed, so that the vocabulary guage development, and many of the
and meaning are clear. Then the teacher words used should be the same words
can come back to the story and focus on students are working with during their
the aspects of language that may be prob- reading lesson. Teachers should model
lematic for English learners (sentence con- appropriate syntax, word order, and tense
struction, word usage, prepositions) in the agreement and have students practice
familiar text. Language-focused activities these skills with new vocabulary words.
will have more meaning for English learn- Teachers should be careful and explicit
ers if they already have a general under- about pointing out or modeling appropri-
standing of the material in the text. ate use, as students use new vocabulary in
the context of sentences that should, over
2. There may not be enough time in the in- time, become more complex and gram-
structional day to provide English learners matically correct.
with sufficient instruction on the features of
academic English. Note that instruction in the proper usage of
words is very different from correction of
This problem is particularly relevant when any and all errors a student makes in word
English learners enter the upper grades usage. In the Panel’s view, error correction
with little knowledge of academic English, needs to be focused on the instructional
limited reading ability, and large educa- target of the lesson. If the instructional
tional gaps. Teachers need to be aware that focus of the vocabulary lesson is on word
many features of academic English can forms such as success, successful, and
and should be included during the block of succeed, teachers should correct errors in
time devoted to reading instruction. Virtu- word forms but ignore other errors. For in-
ally all students would benefit from activi- stance, in the learner’s sentence, “The boy
ties that teach them how to build complex is very succeed on mathematics,” teachers
sentences through sentence combining— should point out that the correct word is
and how to use words such as however and successful but should not focus on the in-
but to build an argument. Thus, a partial correct use of the word on. In restating
solution to the time problem is to include the sentence, the teacher might emphasize
daily academic English instruction as part correct usage by saying “Yes, the boy is
of the core reading instruction delivered very successful at mathematics.”
20

Recommendation 5. the early part of the school year, so that


Schedule regular teachers can practice immediately with
their own students. Training need not be
peer‑assisted learning lengthy and could be provided by read-
opportunities ing coaches. Coaches should also observe
teachers as they get started and help teach-
ers during the difficult early phases.
Ensure that teachers of English learners
devote approximately 90 minutes a 2. Also consider the use of partnering for Eng-
week to instructional activities in which lish language development instruction.101
pairs of students at different ability
levels or different English language The Panel members know that there was
proficiencies work together on no experimental research on this topic,
but we still consider this to be a promis-
academic tasks in a structured fashion.
ing practice, based on the documented
These activities should practice and
success of peer-assisted learning in other
extend material already taught.98
areas of language arts. During the part of
the day reserved for English language de-
How to carry out the velopment, for example, peers would work
recommendation together on reading connected text to each
other and then discussing the text in a
1. Develop plans that encourage teachers structured way. Students could read short
to schedule about 90 minutes a week with passages of text and then practice sum-
activities in reading and language arts that marizing the text for a few minutes, using
entail students working in structured pair specific summarization strategies. Or, after
activities. reading the text, they could answer ques-
tions, generate “gist” statements, or use
Kindergarteners can learn peer-assisted another comprehension procedure, such
learning techniques if the routines are rea- as “prediction relay,” thinking ahead in
sonably simple and taught in an explicit the text and predicting what might happen
fashion.99 Older elementary students can based on the story content to that point.
learn fairly sophisticated strategies for
providing peers with feedback on compre- Possible roadblocks and solutions
hension and vocabulary. Students can also
assist each other in learning or clarifying 1. Some teachers may feel that the added
the meanings of words in English.100 time required by English learners may take in-
structional time away from other students.
The Panel recommends that the focus of
the pair activities be tied to areas that A benefit of peer-assisted instruction is that
emerge as key targets from a district’s all students can participate. So, teachers
evaluation data. These could include oral do not have to plan additional activities for
reading fluency, vocabulary development, separate groups of students in the class.
syntax, and comprehension strategies. This partner work gives teachers a way to
structure learning opportunities that ad-
Districts should provide professional de- dress some of the unique learning needs of
velopment for teachers setting up peer- English learners. It also gives them a way to
assistance learning systems. Professional address the learning needs of other students
development should be scheduled during in the class. Students who have learning
5. Schedule Regular Peer-Assisted Learning Opportunities 21

disabilities or who are low performers, as respond to errors, the format can be used
well as average and above-average students, in a number of different content areas
will benefit from working with a partner in across grade levels. The use of peer-
a structured way if the activities are orga- ­assisted instruction across grade levels
nized and carried out appropriately. provides a consistent and familiar struc-
ture for practicing specific content.
Peer-assisted learning is not, however, a
substitute for teacher-led instruction. It 3. Teachers may be concerned that this takes
is an evidence-based approach intended time away from instruction.
to replace some of the independent seat-
work or round-robin reading that students Most teachers replace some of the inde-
do, for example, when the intention is to pendent seatwork or round-robin reading
provide practice and extended learning with peer-assisted learning. Again, peer-
opportunities for students. assisted learning is not a substitute for
instruction. It is an opportunity for Eng-
2. Teachers may be concerned about the time lish learners to practice and work with
it takes to teach students the routines. skills and concepts they are learning. It
allows students to receive feedback as
Once students have learned peer-assisted they practice.
instructional routines, such as how to

Summary of evidence to support this


as providing potentially positive effects on
recommendation
reading achievement.104 One of the two met
This recommendation is based on several high- the Clearinghouse evidence standards105
quality experiments and quasi experiments and the other met the standards with
with English learners. In addition, many peer- reservations.106
assisted studies also have been conducted with
Partner work is an opportunity for students
native-English-speaking students, and the re-
to practice and extend what the teacher has
sults have consistently supported the positive
taught during regular instruction. Partner
impact of peer tutoring on student learning
work is excellent for tasks in which correct
outcomes. Level of Evidence: Strong.
and incorrect responses can be clearly deter-
Three high-quality experiments and quasi ex- mined (word and text reading and phonologi-
periments have evaluated the effectiveness of cal awareness activities, such as identifying
English learners working in pairs in a struc- sounds in words).
tured fashion several times a week.102 These
However, evidence also demonstrates that
studies spanned virtually all of the elementary
partner activities can build skills for tasks
grade levels. All these studies demonstrated
in which correct and incorrect responses are
positive impacts on reading achievement for
harder to determine, such as reading compre-
students at various ability levels. Two addi-
hension and other tasks that require student
tional studies provide evidence of the posi-
explanations. In three of the five studies, stu-
tive impact of student activities in cooperative
dents worked in pairs to practice, consolidate,
groups of four to six students.103 Although less
and extend prereading, decoding, comprehen-
evidence supports cooperative groups than
sion, and spelling skills. In each of the studies
pairs of students working together, the guid-
student pairs, with different abilities in either
ance here is relevant for districts wanting to
reading or English language proficiency, were
implement some type of cooperative learning
provided with clear instructional activities
structure in their schools.
and taught procedures for working effectively
Of the five studies, two were reviewed by with peers. Teachers used guides that included
the What Works Clearinghouse and rated prompt cards and activities for students.
22

Appendix 1. Example of a criterion‑related


Technical information validity study

on the studies In a recent study by Geva and Yaghoub-


Zadeh (2006), second-grade English learn-
Recommendation 1. Screen ers (Cantonese, Punjabi, Tamil, and Portu-
for reading problems and guese) and native English speakers were
monitor progress assessed in English on cognitive and lin-
guistic measures (nonverbal intelligence,
rapid letter naming, phonological aware-
The Panel rated the level of evidence as ness, vocabulary, and syntactic knowl-
Strong. It considered 21 studies that ad- edge) and reading measures (pseudoword
dressed the criterion-related validity of reading, word recognition, and word and
assessment measures to screen English text reading fluency).
learners in reading and to monitor their
reading progress over time. The body Phonological awareness, rapid letter nam-
of research on early screening measures ing, and word recognition accounted for
meets the standards of the American Psy- the bulk of the variance on word and
chological Association for valid screen- text reading fluency. These measures ac-
ing instruments (American Educational counted for 60 percent and 58 percent of
Research Association, American Psycho- the variance on measures of fluency of
logical Association, & National Council on word and text reading, respectively, after
Measurement in Education, 1999). oral language measures (vocabulary and
syntactic knowledge) were entered into
Eighteen reviewed studies conducted the hierarchical regression models. The
screening and criterion assessments with pattern of relationships among the mea-
English learners at different points in sures was similar for the English learners
time on measures of phonological aware- and native English speakers. Oral language
ness, letter knowledge, and word and measures, although entered first into the
text reading. Although the number of regression models, accounted for just 11
studies in this category was large, we percent and 12 percent of the variance on
noted that in many of these studies the measures of word and text reading fluency,
samples of English learners were not respectively. In other studies the predic-
adequately representative of the popu- tive validity for oral language measures is
lation of English learners in the United even smaller for kindergarten and the first
States. So, we have some concern about grade. We thus assert that oral language
the generalizability. proficiency is a poor predictor of subse-
quent reading performance.
However, the fact that so many studies
have replicated these findings supports Studies that systematically
this recommendation. In addition, the set monitored student progress
of screening measures demonstrates mod- over time in grades 1 to 5
erate predictive validity for English learn-
ers from homes speaking a variety of lan- Four studies also investigated the regu-
guages: Spanish, Punjabi, Tamil, Mandarin, lar monitoring of student progress over
Cantonese, Farsi, Hmong, and Portuguese, time (Baker & Good, 1995; Dominguez
among others. de Ramirez & Shapiro, 2006; Leafstedt,
Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies 23

Richards, & Gerber, 2004; Wiley & Deno, that English learners can develop equiva-
2005), with three of four investigating the lent degrees of fluency in reading both
use of oral reading fluency. Two of these word lists and connected text by the sec-
focused specifically on the technical issues ond grade (Geva & Yaghoub-Zadeh, 2006;
of monitoring progress regularly. They in- Lesaux & Siegel, 2003). There is also some
dicated that oral reading fluency was sen- limited evidence that English learners
sitive to growth over periods as short as can develop equivalency with native Eng-
two weeks when used in the early grades lish speakers in reading comprehension
(Baker & Good, 1995) and when used (Chiappe, Glaeser, & Ferko, 2007; Lesaux,
with students up to grade 5 (Dominguez Lipka, & Siegel, 2006; Lesaux & Siegel,
de Ramirez & Shapiro, 2006). In two of 2003). We conclude that it is reasonable
the studies (Baker & Good, 2005; Wiley to expect that English learners can learn
& Deno, 2005) oral reading fluency pre- to read at rates similar to those of native
dicted the performance of English learn- speakers if they are provided with high-
ers on comprehensive reading tests such quality reading instruction.
as the SAT-10 and state-developed reading
assessments.
Recommendation 2.
Comparable expectations Provide intensive small-
for English learners group reading interventions
An interesting and important sidelight of
the validity studies is the corresponding The Panel rated the level of evidence as
set of descriptive statistics. Many of the Strong. We located four high-quality, ran-
studies demonstrate that English learn- domized controlled trials demonstrating
ers can perform at comparable levels of support for the practice of explicit, sys-
proficiency to native English speakers on tematic small-group instruction. Each of
measures assessing phonological aware- the studies met the standards of the What
ness, word reading, and reading connected Works Clearinghouse (WWC). Conducted at
text fluently. These studies have been various sites by different research groups,
conducted with English learners in the they targeted different interventions that
primary grades who receive their instruc- share core characteristics in design and
tion exclusively in the general education content.
classroom alongside their native-English-
­speaking peers. It is in these contexts For sample sizes, there were 91 first grad-
that they develop comparable word read- ers in one of the studies of Enhanced
ing, word attack, and spelling skills in Proactive Reading, 41 first graders in the
kindergarten through the second grade other, 33 students in grades 2–5 for Read
(Chiappe & Siegel, 1999; Chiappe, Siegel, Well, and 17 students in kindergarten
& Wade-Woolley, 2002; Lesaux & Siegel, through third grade for SRA Reading Mas-
2003; Limbos & Geva, 2001; Verhoeven, tery. All the students were English learn-
1990, 2000). ers. In three of the studies, all were stu-
dents reading at or below the first-grade
The comparable development of early level.
reading skills for English learners appears
to extend beyond accuracy in word rec- Effect sizes were consistently positive
ognition and spelling. There is evidence for reading but inconsistent for English
24 Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies

language development. Only the study The What Works Clearing­house concluded
of Enhanced Proactive Reading (Vaughn, that the effects for reading achievement
Mathes, et al., 2006) demonstrated a sta- were not statistically significant (largely
tistically significant effect in reading. Yet because of analysis at the classroom level,
all the studies demonstrated substantially which decreased power), but five of the
important effect sizes for reading: 0.89 seven effect sizes, as well as the average
and 0.25 for Enhanced Proactive Reading, effect size, were large enough to be sub-
0.76 for SRA Reading Mastery, and 0.25 for stantively important. These effects were
Read Well. average for overall reading achievement
(effect size = 0.27) and for specific mea-
Despite the different names and some dif- sures of letter-sound knowledge (0.26),
ferences in lesson content and sequenc- decoding (word attack, 0.42), reading flu-
ing, all three interventions have many ency (DIBELS passage 1, 0.32; DIBELS pas-
features in common: fast-paced, intensive, sage 2, 0.27), and word reading efficiency
highly interactive small-group instruction; (0.41). Impacts on letter-word identifica-
frequent review; frequent opportunities tion and passage comprehension were
for students to respond; heavy emphasis not considered important (0.13 and 0.06,
on systematic teaching of phonological respectively).
awareness and phonics principles; use of
decodable text; and emphasis on fluency In the second Enhanced Proactive Reading
as well as comprehension. study (Vaughn, Mathes, et al., 2006), which
met the WWC standards with reservations
Example of a study of intensive (because of randomization problems),
small-group reading intervention there was a statistically significant and
substantively important impact on reading
In one Enhanced Proactive Reading study overall (0.89), on decoding (word attack,
(Vaughn, Cirino, et al., 2006), 91 Eng- 1.53), and on comprehension (1.32).
lish learners below the 25th percentile in
English reading from four schools were Together, these two studies, plus the other
randomly assigned (at the student level) studies in this set, showed potentially pos-
to the intervention or comparison con- itive effects in reading achievement and
dition. The intervention involved daily no discernible effects in English language
small-group reading instruction focusing development.
on five areas: phonological awareness, let-
ter knowledge, word recognition, fluency,
and comprehension. There were 120 50- Recommendation 3.
minute lessons. Teachers modeled new Provide extensive and varied
content, and the lessons were fast paced. vocabulary instruction
Students’ responses were primarily cho-
ral, with some individual responses. Stu-
dents in the comparison group received The Panel rated the level of evidence as
the same core reading instruction as stu- Strong. We reviewed three studies that
dents in the intervention condition, and directly investigated the impact of vo-
many students also received supplemen- cabulary instruction with English learn-
tal instruction, although it was different ers. A randomized controlled trial (Carlo
from the supplemental instruction pro- et al., 2004) reviewed by the What Works
vided to English learners in the interven- Clearing­house and was found to meet
tion condition. the WWC evidentiary standards with
Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies 25

reservations (because of differential attri- (heterogeneous groups based on language).


tion). Perez (1981) also conducted a ran- On Thursdays students completed word
domized controlled trial, and Rousseau, association, synonym/antonym, and se-
Tam, and Ramnarain (1993) conducted mantic feature analysis activities. On Fri-
a single-subject study. All three studies days specific intervention activities varied,
showed improvements in reading com- but the central objective was to promote
prehension, and in the one study that as- general word analysis skills, rather than
sessed vocabulary specifically (Carlo et al., to focus specifically on learning the tar-
2004), the effect was positive. get words.

The Panel also considered that many stud- In the control classrooms, English learners
ies of vocabulary instruction for native received instruction normally included in
English speakers have found that explicit the school curriculum.
word meaning instruction improves read-
ing achievement (see Beck & McKeown, In the WWC analysis the intervention was
1991; Blachowicz & Fisher, 2000; Blacho- found to have a potentially positive impact
wicz, Fisher, Ogle, & Watts-Taffe, 2006; on both reading achievement and English
Mezynski, 1983; National Institute of Child language development. But because of the
Health and Human Development, 2000; small sample size (with the classroom as
Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986). We also reviewed the unit of analysis), the gains in these
intervention research conducted with Eng- domains were not statistically significant.
lish learners. The effect size in reading comprehen-
sion was 0.50, and the average effect size
Example of a vocabulary across five specific measures of English
intervention study language development was 0.43. Both ef-
fect sizes were considered substantively
In the study of the Vocabulary Improve- important.
ment Program (Carlo et al., 2004), 16 class-
rooms were randomly assigned to treat- Perez (1981) also found that a vocabulary
ment (n = 10) and control (n = 6) conditions. intervention had a positive impact on read-
These classrooms included 142 fifth-grade ing achievement with third-grade English
English learners and 112 English-only stu- learners. In a multiple baseline study,
dents. The intervention lasted 15 weeks. At Rousseau et al. (1993) found that discus-
the beginning of each week, 10 to 12 target sion of key words prior to text reading
words were introduced, and instruction in combination with teacher reading of
was provided four days per week for 30 to the text prior to students’ reading of the
45 minutes. Each fifth week was a review text on their own resulted in a positive
of the previous four weeks. impact on both oral reading and reading
comprehension.
On Mondays English learners previewed
a reading assignment in their native lan- Reading interventions and
guage. On Tuesdays intervention activities vocabulary development
began, with English learners reading the
assignment in English and defining the These three studies are the only direct
target vocabulary words in large-group tests of the impact of vocabulary instruc-
discussion with the teacher. On Wednes- tion on the reading development of Eng-
days the English learners completed cloze lish learners. But it is important that many
activities (fill in the blanks) in small groups complex interventions that have improved
26 Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies

the reading achievement of English learn- The two randomized controlled studies
ers also include explicit teaching of vocab- pertaining to academic English (Scientific
ulary. Various studies reviewed positively Learning Corporation, 2004; Uchikoshi,
by the What Works Clearinghouse make it 2005) are described in greater depth on the
clear that these more complex interven- What Works Clearinghouse website (www.
tions have been successful in increasing whatworks.ed.gov). Both were assessed as
English learners’ reading and language possessing high control for internal valid-
achievement, but these studies were not ity; they were rated as meets evidence stan-
designed to allow the specific effects of vo- dards without reservations.
cabulary teaching to be calculated. These
successful programs include Read Well In one randomized controlled trial (Uchiko-
(Denton, Anthony, Parker, & Hasbrouck, shi, 2005), 108 Spanish-speaking English
2004); Instructional Conversations (Saun- learners were assigned to watch either
ders, 1999; Saunders & Goldenberg, 1999); 54 half-hour episodes of Arthur (Arthur
Enhanced Proactive Reading (Vaughn, emphasizes stories with a plot, conflict,
Cirino, et al., 2006); and SRA Reading Mas- and resolution) or the same number of
tery (Gunn, Biglan, Smolkowski, & Ary, episodes of Reading Between the Lions (a
2000; Gunn, Smolkowski, Biglan, & Black, book-based program emphasizing pho-
2002). In all these programs, potentially nics and reading). Arthur had an overall
confusing or difficult words for English positive impact on measures of English
learners were drawn from reading texts language development (effect size = 0.29)
and given additional instructional atten- and specifically on overall quality of the
tion, often using procedures similar to students’ retelling a story (0.44); these ef-
those noted in the explicit vocabulary fects were not statistically significant. See
studies reviewed above. Dickinson and Tabors (2001) and Snow,
Tabors, Nicholson, and Kurland (1995) for
discussions of the role of narratives in
Recommendation 4. emerging literacy and the link of narra-
Develop academic English tives to the subsequent academic success
of monolingual children.

The Panel rated the level of evidence as The study of FastForWord (Scientific Learn-
Low. Two studies (Scientific Learning Cor- ing Corporation, 2004), a computer-based
poration, 2004; Uchikoshi, 2005) demon- program conducted with 81 English learn-
strate that focused interventions in two ers in kindergarten through the fifth grade,
relatively narrow areas of academic Eng- assessed three aspects of comprehension
lish (quality of oral narrative and syntax) of oral language that encompass three do-
are potentially effective. But because the mains: word classes and relations, gram-
studies address very selected aspects of matical morphemes, and elaborated sen-
academic English and only indirectly ad- tences. The effect size across these three
dress classroom instruction, we cannot areas was 0.88 (statistically significant).
conclude at this time that the studies af-
firm the effectiveness of instruction in aca- Example of a study of
demic English. Additional support is pro- academic English
vided by a recent classroom observational
study that correlates devotion of specific The correlational study by Saunders, Foor-
blocks of time to English language devel- man, and Carlson (2006) supports the
opment with enhanced outcomes. recommendation that student growth in
Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies 27

oral language is stronger in classes that academic English (see also Arreaga-Mayer
designate specific blocks of time for Eng- & ­Perdomo-Rivera, 1996).
lish language development. This observa-
tional study was conducted in 85 kinder-
garten classrooms in 11 school districts in Recommendation 5.
two states with large populations of Eng- Schedule regular peer-
lish learners. In 26 classrooms the entire assisted learning
school day was in English. In the remain-
opportunities
ing 59 classrooms teachers used Spanish
for most of the day but spent some time on
English language development instruction The Panel rated the level of evidence as
(also known as ESL or ESOL). The Woodcock Strong. Three studies of English learners
Language Proficiency Battery—Revised: addressed peer-assisted learning (Calhoon,
English and Spanish Forms (WLPB-R; Wood- Al Otaiba, Cihak, King, & Avalos, 2006; Mc-
cock, 1991; Woodcock & Muñoz-Sandoval, Master, Kung, Han, & Cao, in press; Saenz,
1993) was used to measure oral language Fuchs, & Fuchs, 2005) and two investigated
development; word reading skills were as- the use of cooperative groups (Calderón,
sessed with the word identification (�������
Identi- Hertz-Lazarowitz, & Slavin, 1998; Klingner
ficación de letras y palabras) subtest from & Vaughn, 1996).
the WLPB-R. Students were assessed at
the beginning and the end of the school Two studies were randomized controlled
year. trials, and two were high-quality quasi
experiments. The Saenz et al. study (ran-
Two findings are worth noting. First, domized controlled trial) met the WWC
whether academic instruction was in Eng- evidence standards without reservations.
lish or Spanish, classrooms with a fixed Calhoon et al. was also a randomized
block of time devoted to English language controlled trial. The Calderón et al. quasi
development had greater proportions of experiment met the WWC criteria with
time during the school day devoted to oral reservations. McMaster et al. was a meth-
language development. Students in these odologically acceptable quasi experiment.
classes made significantly greater growth Because a set of four studies across mul-
in both language and literacy outcomes tiple sites conducted by multiple research
than students in classes where English lan- teams reached consistent conclusions
guage development was infused through- about the positive academic impacts of
out the day. So, it seems important for structured work in heterogeneous teams
teachers to have a block of time each day of two or four, we consider the evidential
during which English language develop- basis strong.
ment is the primary focus.
The study by Klingner and Vaughn (1996)
Second, very little time was devoted to used a weaker design (with threats to in-
building academic English in any of the ternal validity). This study compared peer-
various programs. On average, only 4.5 assisted learning (using groups of two)
percent of the time was devoted to vo- with reciprocal teaching (using groups of
cabulary development and less than 2 four). Both interventions seemed promis-
percent of the time was spent on work on ing, and impacts were roughly equivalent
language structures, such as grammar and for the two. But because the design did
syntax. In other words, less than 10 per- not include a control group, the study can-
cent of the time was devoted to developing not make strong claims. It does, however,
28 Appendix 1. Technical information on the studies

provide additional evidence of the poten- students per group in low, average, and
tial effectiveness of structured peer-as- high achieving groups, for a total of 11
sisted learning. students per classroom. Peer-assisted in-
struction was conducted three times per
Nature of the impacts week in 35-minute sessions for 15 weeks.
on student learning Relatively strong readers were paired with
relatively weak readers for the tutoring
In the kindergarten (Saenz et al., 2005) and sessions, and pairs were rotated every
first-grade (Calhoon et al., 2006) studies, three to four weeks. Each student assumed
positive effects were found for peer-as- the role of tutor and tutee and engaged
sisted learning on letter-sound and word in three reading activities: partner read-
attack measures, phoneme awareness, ing with story retelling, summarizing text
and oral reading fluency. The effect sizes (paragraph shrinking), and making predic-
were substantively important. In grades tions (prediction relay). In these activities
3–6 the impact on reading comprehension the stronger reader was the tutee first, and
was significant. tutors were trained to respond with struc-
tured prompts when tutees were having
Example of a study on difficulty. Treatment fidelity was very high,
peer‑assisted learning above 90 percent in all areas.

The Saenz et al. (2005) study provides a In this study, there was a positive impact
good example of how peer-assisted learn- on reading comprehension, as measured by
ing works and how this research is fre- questions answered correctly. There was
quently conducted. Twelve classroom no interaction with learner type, and the
teachers were randomly assigned to peer effect sizes were 1.03 for English learners
tutoring and control conditions. Within with learning disabilities, and 0.86, 0.60,
each classroom four groups of English and 1.02, respectively for the low, average,
learners were identified: two English learn- and high achieving groups. These effect
ers with learning disabilities, and three sizes were substantively important.
29

Appendix 2. 2. Failure to provide pretest information


Levels of evidence for on a salient pretest variable—for quasi
experiments only. In this case we don’t
the recommendations know whether the effect is due to the
in the practice guide practice or to important initial differ-
ences between the experimental and
control groups.
We rely on the What Works Clearinghouse
(WWC) Evidence Standards to assess the 3. Differential attrition between interven-
quality of evidence supporting educational tion and control groups or extremely
programs and practices. The What Works high attrition (without an adequate at-
Clearinghouse addresses evidence for the tempt to account for this factor in data
causal validity of instructional programs analysis procedures).
and practices according to WWC Stan-
dards. Information about these standards Strong level of evidence
is available at www.whatworks.ed.gov/
reviewprocess/standards.html. In general, characterization of the evi-
dence for a recommendation as strong re-
The technical quality of each study is rated quires both studies with high internal va-
and placed in one of three categories: lidity (studies whose designs can support
causal conclusions) and studies with high
• Meets evidence standards —for random- external validity (studies that in total in-
ized controlled trials and regression clude enough of the range of participants
discontinuity studies that provide the and settings on which the recommenda-
strongest evidence of causal validity; tion is focused to support the conclusion
• Meets evidence standards with reser- that the results can be generalized to those
vations—for all quasi-experimental participants and settings). Evidence for
studies with no design flaws and ran- this Practice Guide is strong if:
domized controlled trials that have
problems with randomization, attri- • A systematic review of research gener-
tion, or disruption; and ally meets the standards of the What
• Does not meet evidence standards—for Works Clearinghouse and supports the
studies that do not provide strong evi- effectiveness of a program, practice, or
dence of causal validity. approach—and there is no contradic-
tory evidence of similar quality.
Criteria for assessing problems of attrition
and randomization are described in detail OR
in a set of Technical Working Papers. The
following are the main reasons for exclud- • Several well designed, randomized,
ing studies: controlled trials or well designed quasi
experiments generally meet the stan-
1. There is only one teacher per condition dards of the What Works Clearing-
or one school per condition. This cre- house and support the effectiveness of
ates a major problem in interpretation a program, practice, or approach—and
because it is uncertain whether one there is no contradictory evidence of
particular teacher or one particular similar quality.
school produces the effect, or whether
the effect is due to the practice. OR
30 Appendix 2. Levels of Evidence for the Recommendations in the Practice Guide

• One large, well designed, randomized, pretest and therefore do not meet the
controlled, multisite trial meets the standards of the What Works Clear-
standards of the What Works Clearing- inghouse but that consistently show
house and supports the effectiveness enhanced outcomes for participants
of a program, practice, or approach— experiencing a particular program,
and there is no contradictory evidence practice, or approach and have no
of similar quality. major flaws related to internal valid-
ity other than lack of demonstrated
• For assessments, evidence of reliabil- equivalence at pretest (such as only
ity and validity meets the standards in one teacher or one class per condi-
Standards for Educational and Psycho- tion, unequal amounts of instruc-
logical Testing.107 tional time, or highly biased outcome
measures).
Moderate level of evidence
OR
In general, characterization of the evi-
dence for a recommendation as moderate • Correlational research with strong
requires studies with high internal validity statistical controls for selection bias
but moderate external validity or studies and for discerning influence of endog-
with high external validity but moderate enous factors, and there is no contrary
internal validity. In other words moder- evidence.
ate evidence is derived from studies that
support strong causal conclusions but • For assessments, evidence of reliabil-
for which generalization is uncertain, or ity that meets the standards in Stan-
from studies that support the generality dards for Educational and Psychologi-
of a relationship but for which causality is cal Testing but provides evidence of
uncertain. Evidence for this Practice Guide validity from samples that are not ad-
is moderate if: equately representative of the popula-
tion on which the recommendation is
• Experiments or quasi experiments gen- focused.
erally meet the standards of the What
Works Clearinghouse and support the Low level of evidence
effectiveness of a program, practice,
or approach with small sample sizes In general, characterization of the evi-
or other conditions of implementation dence for a recommendation as low means
or analysis that limit generalizability— that the recommendation is based on ex-
and there is no contrary evidence. pert opinion derived from strong find-
ings or theories in related areas or expert
OR opinion buttressed by direct evidence that
does not rise to the moderate or strong
• Comparison group studies that do not level. Evidence is low if it does not meet
demonstrate equivalence of groups at the standards for moderate or high.
31

Notes Dominguez de Ramirez & Shapiro (2006); Geva


& Yaghoub-Zadeh (2006); Geva et al. (2000);
1.  Field & Lohr (1990). Lafrance & Gottardo (2005); Leafstedt, Richards,
& Gerber (2004); Lesaux & Siegel (2003); Limbos
2.  American Psychological Association (2002).
(2006); Limbos & Geva (2001); Manis, Lindsey,
3.  Greene (1997). & Bailey (2004); Quiroga, Lemos-Britton, Mosta-
fapour, Abbott, & Berninger (2002); Swanson,
4.  Slavin & Cheung (2005).
Sáez, & Gerber (2004); Verhoeven (1990, 2000);
5.  August & Hakuta (1997); Rossell & Baker Wang & Geva (2003); Wiley & Deno (2005).
(1996).
23.  American Educational Research Association,
6.  August & Hakuta (1997); Francis, Lesaux, & American Psychological Association, & National
August (2006). Council on Measurement in Education (1999).

7.  Greene (1997). 24.  Chiappe, Siegel, & Wade-­Woolley (2002); Geva
et al. (2000); Lafrance & Gottardo (2005); Lesaux
8.  See http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/ltt/
& Siegel, (2003); Limbos & Geva (2001); Manis et
results2004/sub_reading_race2.asp (retrieved
al. (2004).
October 9, 2006).
25.  Chiappe, Siegel, & Wade-­Woolley (2002); Geva
9.  See http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/
et al. (2000); Lesaux & Siegel (2003); Limbos & Geva
nrc/reading_math_2005/s0015.asp (retrieved
(2001); Manis et al. (2004); Swanson et al. (2004).
March 16, 2007).
26.  Limbos & Geva (2001); Swanson et al. (2004).
10.  See http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/
nrc/reading_math_2005/s0015.asp. 27.  Baker & Good (1995).

11.  August & Hakuta (1997); Shanahan & August 28.  Baker & Good (1995); Dominguez de Ramirez
(2006). & Shapiro (2006); Wiley & Deno (2005).

12.  Morrison Institute for Public Policy (2006). 29.  Chiappe & Siegel (1999); Chiappe, Siegel, &
Wade-­Woolley (2002); Lesaux & Siegel (2003); Lim-
13.  Fitzgerald (1995); Krashen (1985).
bos & Geva (2001).
14.  Bialystok & Herman (1999); Geva, Yaghoub-
30.  August & Hakuta (1997); August & Shanahan
­Zadeh, & Schuster (2000); Limbos & Geva (2001).
(2006); Geva et al. (2000).
15.  Chiappe & Siegel (1999); Chiappe, Siegel, &
31.  Cisero & Royer (1995); Gottardo (2002); Hsia
Wade-­Woolley (2002); Lesaux & Siegel (2003); Lim-
(1992); Mumtaz & Humphreys (2001).
bos & Geva, (2001).
32.  Chiappe, Siegel, & Wade-­Woolley (2002); Geva
16.  Chiappe, Siegel, & Wade-Wooley (2002); Geva
et al. (2000); Limbos (2006); Manis et al. (2004);
et al. (2000); Lesaux & Siegel (2003); Limbos &
Townsend, Lee, & Chiappe (2006).
Geva (2001); Manis et al. (2004); Swanson et al.
(2004). 33.  Cisero & Royer (1995); Gottardo (2002);
Quiroga et al. (2002).
17.  Geva & Yaghoub-Zadeh (2006); Lesaux & Sie-
gel (2003). 34.  Chiappe & Siegel (2006); Chiappe, Siegel,
& Wade-­Woolley (2002); Lesaux & Siegel (2003);
18.  Chiappe, Glaeser, & Ferko (2007); Lesaux,
Geva et al. (2000); Limbos & Geva (2001); Verho-
Lipka, & Siegel (2006); Lesaux & Siegel (2003).
even (1990, 2000).
19.  Miller, Heilmann, Nockerts, Iglesias, Fabiano, et
35.  Denton, Anthony, Parker, & Hasbrouck
al. (2006); Proctor, Carlo, August, & Snow (2005).
(2004); Gunn, Smolkowski, Biglan, & Black (2002);
20.  Baker & Good (1995); Dominguez de Ramirez Vaughn, Cirino, et al. (2006); Vaughn, Mathes, et
& Shapiro (2006). al. (2006).

21.  Baker (2006). 36.  August & Siegel (2006); Quiroga et al. (2002);
Shanahan & Beck (2006).
22.  Arab-Moghaddam & Sénéchal (2001); Baker
(2006); Baker, Gersten, Haager, & Dingle (2006); 37.  Denton et al. (2004); Gunn et al. (2002);
Baker & Good (1995); Chiappe, Siegel, & Gottardo Vaughn, Cirino, et al. (2006); Vaughn, Mathes,
(2002); Chiappe, Siegel, & Wade-­Woolley (2002); et al. (2006).
32 Notes

38.  Vaughn, Linan-Thompson, & Hickman-Davis 65.  Perez (1981); Rousseau et al. (1993).
(2003).
66.  Umbel, Pearson, Fernandez, & Oller (1992);
39.  In two of the four intervention studies, in- Verhallen & Schoonen (1993).
structional assistants were trained to provide the
67.  Carlo et al. (2004); Perez (1981); Rousseau et
instruction. Gunn et al. (2002); Vaughn, Cirino, et
al. (1993).
al. (2006); Vaughn, Mathes, et al. (2006); Cirino
et al. (2007). 68.  NICHD (2000).

40.  Haager & Windmueller (2001). 69.  Baker et al. (2006); Gersten et al. (2006).

41.  Vaughn, Cirino, et al. (2006); Vaughn, Mathes, 70.  August & Hakuta (1997); Bailey (2006); Fran-
et al. (2006). Gunn et al. (2002). cis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Genesee, Lindholm-
Leary, Saunders, & Christian (2006); Goldenberg
42.  For further information on the What Works
(2006); Scarcella (2003); Schleppegrell (2001,
Clearinghouse, visit www.whatworks.ed.gov.
2004); Snow & Fillmore (2000).
43.  Vaughn, Cirino, et al. (2006); Vaughn, Mathes,
71.  August & Hakuta (1997); Bailey (2006); Cal-
et al. (2006).
lahan (2005); Diaz-Rico & Weed (2002); Francis,
44.  Denton et al. (2004). Rivera, et al. (2006); Genesee et al. (2006); Gold-
enberg (2006); Meltzer & Haman (2005); Scar-
45.  Gunn et al. (2002).
cella (2003); Schleppegrell (2001, 2004); Snow &
46.  Cirino et al. (2007); Gunn et al. (2002). Fillmore (2000).

47.  Gunn et al. (2002). 72.  Proctor et al. (2005).

48.  Franklin (1986); Limbos & Geva (2001). 73.  Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006).

49.  Gunn et al. (2002); Cirino et al. (2007). 74.  August & Hakuta (1997); Callahan (2005)
Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Genesee et al.
50.  In the intervention studies, teachers and in-
(2006); Goldenberg (2006); Meltzer & Haman
structional assistants were trained to provide
(2005); Scarcella (2003); Snow & Fillmore
instruction.
(2000).
51.  National Institute of Child Health and Human
75.  Fillmore & Snow (2002).
Development (NICHD) (2000).
76.  Michaels & Cook-Gumperz (1979); Saunders
52.  Carlo et al. (2004); Perez (1981).
et al. (2006); Schleppegrell (2004).
53.  Carlo et al. (2004); Perez (1981); Rousseau,
77.  Feldman & Kinsella (2005).
Tam & Ramnarain (1993).
78.  Girard (2005).
54.  Carlo et al. (2004); Perez (1981).
79.  Dutro & Moran (2002).
55.  Gersten, Dimino, & Jayanthi (in press).
80.  Snow & Fillmore (2000).
56.  August, Carlo, Dressler, & Snow (2005); Bla-
chowicz, Fisher, Ogle, & Watts-Taffe (2006). 81.  Diaz-Rico & Weed (2002).

57.  Gersten, Dimino, Jayanthi, Kim, & Santoro 82.  Scarcella (2003).
(2006).
83.  August & Hakuta (1997); Francis, Rivera, et al.
58.  Gersten et al. (2006). (2006); Meltzer & Haman (2005); Scarcella (2003);
Snow & Fillmore (2000).
59.  Hiebert (2005).
84.  Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Saunders, Foor-
60.  Beck, Perfetti, & McKeown (1982); Biemiller
man, & Carlson (2006); Schleppegrell (2004); Fill-
(1999).
more (2004); Scarcella (2003).
61.  August et al. (2005).
85.  Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Gibbons
62.  Carlo et al. (2004); Perez (1981); Rousseau et (2002).
al. (1993).
86.  Goldenberg (2006).
63.  See www.whatworks.ed.gov.
87.  Celce-Murcia (2002); Fillmore & Snow
64.  Carlo et al. (2004). (2000).
Notes 33

88.  Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Fillmore & Snow is not a valid predictor of who needs extra sup-
(2000). port in learning to read in the early grades in no
way indicates that oral English language profi-
89.  Bailey (2006); Gibbons (2002); Schleppegrell
ciency is not important for the development of
(2004). Note that English learners who enter
reading in the long term. In fact, experts consis-
school in the primary grades without the abil-
tently consider building oral proficiency in the
ity to use English in such ways can learn grade-
features of academic English to be critical. In
appropriate academic English as well as their
Recommendation 1, we were addressing screen-
English-speaking peers if they are given access
ing measures for learning how to read (the act
to the same rigorous curriculum early and ap-
of reading and understanding the relatively
propriate instructional support and interven-
straightforward books suitable for students in
tions, delivered daily in blocks of time dedicated
the early grades).
to the development of academic language. When
students receive high-quality instruction in aca- 94.  Echevarria, Vogt, & Short (2004); Francis, Ri-
demic English early in their education, we see vera, et al. (2006).
gains in their test scores later.
95.  Francis, Rivera, et al. (2006); Gersten & Baker
90.  See www.whatworks.ed.gov. (2000); Fillmore & Snow (2000).
91.  Scientific Learning Corporation (2004); 96.  Saunders et al. (2006).
Uchikoshi (2005).
97.  Saunders et al. (2006).
92.  August & Hakuta (1997); August & Shanahan
98.  90 minutes is the median amount of time per
(2006); Bailey (2006); Callahan (2005); Francis,
week in the research.
Rivera et al. (2006); Gennesee et al. (2006); Gold-
enberg (2006); Meltzer & Haman (2005); ­Scarcella 99.  McMaster, Kung, Han, & Cao (in press).
(2003); Schleppegrell (2001, 2004); Snow & Fill-
100.  Calderón, Hertz-Lazavowitz, & Slavin (1998).
more (2000).
101.  Klingner & Vaughn (1996).
93.  At this stage, the reader may be a bit confused.
In Recommendation 1 (Formative Assessments to 102.  Calhoon, Al Otaiba, Cihak, King, & Avalos
Screen for Reading Problems and Monitor Prog- (2006); McMaster et al. (in press); Saenz, Fuchs,
ress), we noted that studies consistently find that & Fuchs (2005).
oral English language proficiency is a weak pre-
103.  Calderón et al. (1998); Klingner & Vaughn
dictor of how quickly a child will learn to read in
(1996).
English. Yet, in Recommendation 4 we argue for
the importance of intensive work on the develop- 104.  Calderón et al. (1998); Saenz et al. (2005).
ment of academic English, including oral language
105.  Saenz et al. (2005).
proficiency, beginning in kindergarten.
106.  Calderón et al. (1998).
A subtle but important distinction needs to
be made to explain the seeming contradiction. 107.  American Educational Research Association
The fact that oral English language proficiency et al. (1999).
34

References English to the test. New Haven, CT: Yale


University Press.
American Educational Research Associa- Baker, S. (2006, March). English language
tion, American Psychological Associa- learners and reading first: Some pre-
tion, & National Council on Measure- liminary evidence of effectiveness. Paper
ment in Education. (1999). Standards for presented at the CORE Literacy Leader-
educational and psychological testing. ship Summit, San Francisco, CA.
Washington, DC: AERA Publications. Baker, S., Gersten, R., Haager, D., & Dingle,
American Psychological Association. M. (2006). Teaching practice and the
(2002). Criteria for practice guideline reading growth of first-grade English
development and evaluation. American learners: Validation of an observation
Psychologist, 57, 1048–1051. instrument. Elementary School Journal,
Arab-Moghaddam, N., & Sénéchal, M. 107, 199–219.
(2001). Orthographic and phonological Baker, S. K., & Good, R. (1995). Curriculum-
processing skills in reading and spell- based measurement for English read-
ing in Persian/English bilinguals. Inter- ing with bilingual Hispanic students:
national Journal of Behavioral Develop- A validation study with second-grade
ment, 25, 140–147. students. School Psychology Review, 24,
Arreaga-Mayer, C., & Perdomo-Rivera, C. 561–578.
(1996). Ecobehavioral analysis of in- Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1991). Con-
struction for at-risk language-minority ditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R.
students. Elementary School Journal, Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P.
96, 245–258. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading
August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, research (Vol. 2, pp. 789–814). White
C. (2005). The critical role of vocabu- Plains, NY: Longman.
lary development for English language Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G.
learners. Learning Disabilities Research (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary
and Practice, 20, 50–57. instruction on lexical access and read-
August, D., & Hakuta, K. (1997). Improving ing comprehension. Journal of Educa-
schooling for language-minority chil- tional Psychology, 74, 506–521.
dren. Washington, DC: National Acad- Bialystok, E., & Herman, J. (1999). Does
emy Press. bilingualism matter for early literacy?
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Developing literacy in second-language 2, 35–44.
learners: Report of the National Literacy Biemiller, A. (1999). Language and read-
Panel on Language-Minority Children ing success. Newton Upper Falls, MA:
and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brookline Books.
August, D., & Siegel, L. (2006). Literacy Blachowicz, C. L. Z., & Fisher, P. J. L. (2000).
instruction for language-minority chil- Vocabulary instruction. In M. L. Kamil,
dren in special education settings. In P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr
D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), De- (Eds.), Handbook of reading research
veloping literacy in second-language (Vol. 3, pp. 503–523). White Plains, NY:
learners: Report of the National Literacy Longman.
Panel on Language-Minority Children Blachowicz, C. L. Z., Fisher, P. J. L., Ogle
and Youth (pp. 523–553). Mahwah, NJ: D., & Watts-Taffe, S. (2006). Vocabulary:
Erlbaum. Questions from the classroom. Reading
Bailey, A. (Ed.). (2006). The language de- Research Quarterly, 41, 524–539.
mands of school: Putting academic
References 35

Calderón, M., Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., & kindergartners from diverse linguistic


Slavin, R. (1998). Effects of bilingual co- backgrounds. Applied Psycholinguistics,
operative integrated reading and com- 23, 95–116.
position on students transitioning from Chiappe, P., Siegel, L., & Wade-Woolley, L.
Spanish to English reading. Elementary (2002). Linguistic diversity and the de-
School Journal, 99, 153–165. velopment of reading skills: A longitudi-
Calhoon, M. B., Al Otaiba, S., Cihak, D., nal study. Scientific Studies of Reading,
King, A., & Avalos, A. C. (2006). Effects 6, 369–400.
of a peer-mediated program on reading Cirino, P. T., Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson,
skill acquisition for two-way bilingual S., Cardenas-Hagan, E., Fletcher, J. M., &
first grade classrooms. Manuscript sub- Francis, D. J. (2007). One year follow-up
mitted for publication. outcomes of Spanish and English inter-
Callahan, R. (2005). Tracking and high ventions for English language learners
school English language learners: Limit- at-risk for reading problems. Manuscript
ing opportunity to learn. American Edu- submitted for publication.
cational Research Journal, 42, 305–328. Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The de-
Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., velopment and cross-language trans-
Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D., et fer of phonological awareness. Con-
al. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing temporary Educational Psychology, 20,
the vocabulary needs for English lan- 275–303.
guage learners in bilingual and main- Denton, C. A., Anthony, J. L., Parker, R., &
stream classrooms. Reading Research Hasbrouck, J. E. (2004). Effects of two
Quarterly, 39, 188–215. tutoring programs on the English read-
Celce-Murcia, M. (2002). On the use of ing development of Spanish-English bi-
selected grammatical features in ac- lingual students. The Elementary School
ademic writing. In M. C. Colombi & Journal, 104, 289–305.
M. J. Schleppegrell (Eds.), Developing Diaz-Rico, L. T., & Weed, K. Z. (2002). The
advanced literacy in first and second crosscultural, language, and academic
languages (pp. 143–158). Mahwah, NJ: development handbook: A complete K–12
Erlbaum. reference guide. Boston, MA: Allyn &
Chiappe, P., Glaeser, B., & Ferko, D. (2007). Bacon.
Speech perception, vocabulary and the Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. (Eds.). (2001).
development of reading skills in English Beginning literacy with language. Balti-
among Korean- and English-speaking more: Brookes.
children. Journal of Educational Psy- Dominguez de Ramirez, R., & Shapiro, E.
chology, 99, 154–166. S. (2006). Curriculum-based measure-
Chiappe, P., & Siegel, L. S. (1999). ������
Phono- ment and the evaluation of reading
logical awareness and reading acquisi- skills of Spanish-speaking English lan-
tion in English- and Punjabi-speaking guage learners in bilingual education
Canadian children. Journal of Educa- classrooms. School Psychology Review,
tional Psychology, 91, 20–28. 35, 356–369.
Chiappe, P., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). ��������
A longi- Dutro, S., & Moran, C. (2002), Rethinking
tudinal study of reading development English language instruction: An archi-
of Canadian children from diverse lin- tectural approach. In G. Garcia (Ed.),
guistic backgrounds. Elementary School English learners reading at the highest
Journal, 107, 135–152. level of English literacy. Newark, DE: In-
Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, ternational Reading Association.
A. (2002). Reading-related skills in
36 References

Echevarria, J., Vogt, M. A., & Short, D. J. http://www.centeroninstruction.org/


(2004). Making content comprehensible files/ELL1-Interventions.pdf.
for English learners: The SIOP model Franklin, E. A. (1986). Literacy instruc-
(2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. tion for LES children. Language Arts,
Feldman, K., & Kinsella, K. (2005). Cre- 63, 51–60.
ate an active participation classroom. Genesee, F., Lindholm-Leary, K., Saunders,
The CORE Reading Expert. Retrieved W., & Christian, D. (2006). Educating
from http://www.corelearn.com/pdfs/ English language learners. New York:
Newsletters/CORE%202005%20Spring Cambridge University Press.
%20Newsletter.pdf. Gersten, R., & Baker, S. (2000). What we
Field, M. J., & Lohr, K. N. (Eds.). (1990). Clini- know about effective instructional prac-
cal practice guidelines: Directions for a tices for English-language learners. Ex-
new program. Washington, DC: National ceptional Children, 66, 454–470.
Academy Press. Gersten, R., Dimino, J., Jayanthi, M., Kim,
Fillmore, L. W. (2004). The role of language J., & Santoro, L. (2007). Teacher study
in academic development. In Excerpts groups as a means to improve reading
from a presentation by Lily Wong Fillmore comprehension and vocabulary instruc-
at the Closing the Achievement Gap for tion for English learners: Results of ran-
EL Students conference. Santa Rosa: CA: domized controlled trials. Signal Hill,
Sonoma County Office of Education. Re- CA: Instructional Research Group.
treived from http://www.scoe.k12.ca.us/ Gersten, R., Dimino, J., & Jayanthi, M. (in
aiming_high/docs/AH_language.pdf. press). Development of a classroom ob-
Fillmore, L. W., & Snow, C. E. (2002). What servational system. In B. Taylor & J. Ys-
teachers need to know about language. seldyke (Eds.), Reading instruction for
In C. T. Adger, C. E. Snow, & D. Chris- English language learners: The Bond sym-
tian (Eds.), What teachers need to know posium. New York: Teachers College.
about language (pp. 7–54). Washington, Geva, E., & Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z. (2006). Read-
DC, and McHenry, IL: Center for Applied ing efficiency in native English-speak-
Linguistics and Delta Systems Co., Inc. ing and English-as-a-second-language
Fitzgerald, J. (1995). English-as-a-second- children: The role of oral proficiency
language learners’ cognitive reading and underlying cognitive-linguistic pro-
processes: A review of research in the cesses. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10,
United States. Review of Educational Re- 31–57.
search, 65, 145–190. Geva, E., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Schuster,
Francis, D., Lesaux, N., & August, D. (2006). B. (2000). Part IV: Reading and foreign
Language instruction. In D. August & T. language learning: Understanding indi-
Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in vidual differences in word recognition
second-language learners: Report of the skills of ESL children. Annals of Dys-
National Literacy Panel on Language-Mi- lexia, 50, 121–154.
nority Children and Youth (pp. 365–413). Gibbons, P. (2002). Scaffolding language,
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. scaffolding learning: Teaching sec-
Francis, D. J., Rivera, M., Lesaux, N., Kief- ond language learners in the main-
fer, M., & Rivera, H. (2006). Practical stream classroom. Portsmouth, NH:
guidelines for the education of English Heinemann.
language learners: Research-based rec- Girard, V. (2005). English learners and
ommendations for instruction and aca- the language arts. In V. Girard (Ed.),
demic interventions. Retrieved from Schools moving up: A WestEd initiative.
Retrieved November 8, 2006, from
References 37

http://www.schoolsmovingup.net/cs/ second language. Elementary School


wested/view/e/140. Journal, 96, 275–293.
Goldenberg, C. (2006). Improving achieve- Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypoth-
ment for English learners: Conclusions esis: Issues and implications. New York:
from 2 research reviews. Retrieved Longman.
from http://www.colorincolorado.org/ Lafrance, A., & Gottardo, A. (2005). A lon-
article/12918. gitudinal study of phonological pro-
Gottardo, A. (2002). The relationship be- cessing skills and reading in bilingual
tween language and reading skills in bi- children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26,
lingual Spanish-English speakers. Top- 559–578.
ics in Language Disorders, 22, 46–70. Leafstedt, J. M., Richards, C. R., & Gerber,
Greene, J. P. (1997, Spring). A meta-­analysis M. M. (2004). Effectiveness of explicit
of the Rossell and Baker review of bi- phonological-awareness instruction
lingual education research. Bilingual for at-risk English learners. Learning
Research Journal, 21. Retrieved April Disabilities: Research & Practice, 19,
24, 2007, from http://brj.asu.edu/ 252–261.
articlesv2/green.html. Lesaux, N., & Siegel, L. (2003). The develop-
Gunn, B., Biglan, A., Smolkowski, K., & Ary, ment of reading in children who speak
D. (2000). The efficacy of supplemen- English as a second language. Develop-
tal instruction in decoding skills for mental Psychology, 39, 1005–1019.
Hispanic and non-Hispanic students Lesaux, N. K., Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S.
in early elementary school. Journal of (2006). ��������������������������������
Investigating cognitive and lin-
Special Education, 34, 90–103. guistic abilities that influence the read-
Gunn, B., Smolkowski, K., Biglan, A., & ing comprehension skills of children
Black, C. (2002). Supplemental instruc- from diverse linguistic backgrounds.
tion in decoding skills for Hispanic and Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplin-
non-Hispanic students in early elemen- ary Journal, 19, 99–131.
tary school: A follow-up. The Journal of Limbos, M. (2006). Early identification
Special Education, 36, 69–79. of second language students at risk
Haager, D., & Windmueller, M. (2001). Early for reading disability. Dissertation Ab-
literacy intervention for English lan- stracts International, 66 (10-A), 3566A.
guage learners at-risk for learning dis- Limbos, M., & Geva, E. (2001). Accuracy
abilities: Student outcomes in an urban of teacher assessments of second-lan-
school. Learning Disability Quarterly, guage students at risk for reading dis-
24, 235–250. ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
Hiebert, E. H. (2005). State reform policies 34, 136–151.
and the task textbooks pose for first- Manis, F. R., Lindsey, K. A., & Bailey, C.
grade readers. Elementary School Jour- E. (2004). Development of reading in
nal, 105, 245–266. grades K–2 in Spanish-speaking English
Hsia, S. (1992). Developmental knowledge language learners. Learning Disabilities
of inter- and intraword boundries: Evi- Research & Practice, 19, 214–224.
dence from American and Mandarin McMaster, K. L., Kung, H., Han, I., & Cao, M.
Chinese speaking beginning readers. (in press). Peer-assisted learning strate-
Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 341–372. gies: A “tier 1” approach to promoting
Klingner, J. K., & Vaughn, S. (1996). Recip- responsiveness to beginning reading
rocal teaching of reading comprehen- instruction for English learners. Excep-
sion strategies for students with learn- tional Children.
ing disabilities who use English as a
38 References

Meltzer, J., & Haman, E. T. (2005). Meet- Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow,
ing the literacy development needs of C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking chil-
adolescent English language learners dren reading in English: Toward a model
through content-area learning, part two: of comprehension. Journal of Educa-
Focus on classroom teaching and learn- tional Psychology, 97, 246–256.
ing strategies. Retrieved from http:// Quiroga, T., Lemos-Britton, Z., Mostafapour,
www.alliance.brown.edu/pubs/adlit/ E., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2002).
adell_litdv2.pdf Phonological awareness and beginning
Mezynski, K. (1983). Issues concerning reading in Spanish-speaking ESL first
the acquisition of knowledge: Effects graders: Research into practice. Journal
of vocabulary training on reading com- of School Psychology, 40, 85–111.
prehension. Review of Educational Re- Rossell, C. H., & Baker, K. (1996). The edu-
search, 53, 263–279. cational effectiveness of bilingual edu-
Michaels, S., & Cook-Gumperz, J. (1979). A cation. Research in the Teaching of Eng-
study of sharing time with first grade lish, 30, 7–74.
students: Discourse narratives in the Rousseau, M. K., Tam, B. K. Y., & Ramna-
classroom. In Proceedings of the fifth rain, R. (1993). Increasing reading pro-
annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguis- ficiency of language-minority students
tics Society (pp. 647–680). Berkeley, CA: with speech and language impairments.
Berkeley Linguistics Society. Education and Treatment of Children,
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., 16, 254–271.
Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. Saenz, L. M., Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2005).
J. (2006). Oral language and reading in Peer-assisted learning strategies for
bilingual children. Learning Disabilities English language learners with learn-
Research & Practice, 21, 30–43. ing disabilities. Exceptional Children,
Morrison Institute for Public Policy. (2006). 71, 231–247.
Why some schools with Latino children Saunders, W. M., & Goldenberg, C. (1999).
beat the odds and others don’t. Tempe, Effects of instructional conversations
AZ: Author. and literature logs on limited- and fluent-
Mumtaz, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2001). The English-proficient students’ story com-
effects of bilingualism on learning to prehension and thematic understanding.
read English: Evidence from the contrast Elementary School Journal, 99, 277–301.
between Urdu-English bilingual and Saunders, W. M., Foorman, B. P., & Carl-
English monolingual children. Journal son, C. D. (2006). Do we need a separate
of Research in Reading, 24, 113–134. block of time for oral English language
National Institute of Child Health and development in programs for English
Human Development. (2000). Report of learners? The Elementary School Jour-
the National Reading Panel. Teaching nal, 107, 181–198.
children to read: An evidence-based as- Scarcella, R. (2003). Accelerating academic
sessment of the scientific research litera- English: A focus on the English learner.
ture on reading and its implications for Oakland, CA: Regents of the University
reading instruction (NIH Publication No. of California.
00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Govern- Schleppegrell, M. J. (2001). Linguistic fea-
ment Printing Office. tures of the language of schooling. Lin-
Perez, E. (1981). Oral language compe- guistics and Education, 12, 431–459.
tence improves reading skills of Mexi- Schleppegrell, M. J. (2004). The language
can American third graders. Reading of schooling: A functional linguistics per-
Teacher, 35, 24–27. spective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 39

Scientific Learning Corporation. (2004). Im- assessing Latino/a children in Spanish


proved language skills by children with for risk of reading disabilities. Paper
low reading performance who used presented at the meeting of the Soci-
FastForWord language: Maps for Learn- ety for the Scientific Study of Reading,
ing. MAPS for Learning, 3, 1–13. Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Shanahan, T., & August, D. (2006). Report Uchikoshi, Y. (2005). Narrative develop-
of the National Literacy Panel: Research ment in bilingual kindergarteners: Can
on teaching reading to English language Arthur help? Developmental Psychology,
learners. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 41, 464–478.
Shanahan, T., & Beck, I. L. (2006). Effective Umbel, V. M., Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, M.
literacy teaching for English-language C., & Oller, D. K. (1992). Measuring bilin-
learners. In D. August & T. Shanahan gual children’s receptive vocabularies.
(Eds.), Developing literacy in second-lan- Child Development, 63, 1012–1020.
guage learners: Report of the National Vaughn, S., Cirino, P. T., Linan-­Thompson,
Literacy Panel on Language-Minority S., Mathes, P. G., Carlson, C. D.,
Children and Youth (pp. 415–488). Mah- ­Cardenas-Hagan, E., et al. (2006). Ef-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. fectiveness of a Spanish intervention
Slavin, R., & Cheung, A. (2005). A synthe- and an English intervention for English
sis of research on language of reading language learners at risk for reading
instruction for English language learn- problems. American Educational Re-
ers. Review of Educational Research, 75, search Journal, 43, 449–487.
247–284. Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson, S., &
Snow, C. E., & Fillmore, L. W. (2000). Clear- ­Hickman-Davis, P. (2003). Response
inghouse on languages and linguistics. to treatment as a means for identify-
Retrieved from http://www.cal.org/ ing students with reading/learning
ericcll/teachers/teacher.pdf. disabilities. Exceptional Children, 69,
Snow, C. E., & Fillmore, L. W. (2000). What 391–410.
teachers need to know about language. Vaughn, S., Mathes, P., Linan-Thompson, S.,
Retrieved from http://www.cal.org/ Cirino, P., Carlson, C., Pollard-Durodola,
resources/teachers/teachers.pdf. S., et al. (2006). Effectiveness of an Eng-
Snow, C. E., Tabors, P. O., Nicholson, P. lish intervention for first-grade English
A., & Kurland, B. F. (1995). SHELL: Oral language learners at risk for reading
language and early literacy skills in problems. Elementary School Journal,
kindergarten and first-grade children. 107, 153–180.
Journal of Research in Childhood Edu- Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1993). Lexi-
cation, 10, 37–48. cal knowledge of monolingual and bi-
Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects lingual children. Applied Linguistics, 14,
of vocabulary instruction: A model- 344–363.
based meta-analysis. Review of Educa- Verhoeven, L. (1990). Acquisition of read-
tional Research, 56, 72–110. ing in a second language. Reading Re-
Swanson, H. L., Sáez, L., & Gerber, M. (2004). search Quarterly, 25, 90–114.
Do phonological and executive processes Verhoeven, L. T. (2000). Components in early
in English learners at risk for reading second language reading and spelling.
disabilities in grade 1 predict perfor- Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 313–330.
mance in grade 2? Learning Disabilities Wang, M., & Geva, E. (2003). Spelling acqui-
Research & Practice, 19, 225–238. sition of novel English phonemes in Chi-
Townsend, D., Lee, E., & Chiappe, P. (2006). nese children. Reading and Writing: An
English or Spanish? The efficacy of Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 325–348.
40 References

Wiley, H. I., & Deno, S. L. (2005). Oral read- Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Lan-
ing and maze measures as predictors of guage Proficiency Battery-Revised.
success for English learners on a state Itasca, IL: Riverside.
standards assessment. Remedial and Woodcock, R., & Muñoz-Sandoval, A.
Special Education, 26, 207–214. (1993). Woodcock-Muñoz Language Sur-
vey. Itasca, IL: Riverside.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi