Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

INVESTIGATING PRINCIPLES OF HUMAN BRAIN FUNCTION UNDERLYING WORKING MEMORY: WHAT INSIGHTS FROM SCHIZOPHRENIA?
G. D. HONEY* AND P. C. FLETCHER
University of Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, Brain Mapping Unit, Downing Site, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK

AbstractWorking memory dysfunction is a core component of schizophrenia, which likely contributes substantially to the pervasive and profound cognitive decits observed in patients with this illness. Developments in functional imaging have facilitated the investigation of the neural basis of these cognitive decits. A strong tradition within neuropsychology has been that circumscribed lesions provide observations which constrain theoretical models, and generate testable predictions on the basis of observed relationships between structural abnormalities and behavioral dysfunction. In this article, the extent to which the neuropsychological tradition can be applied to neuropsychiatry to advance understanding of the biological basis of working memory is addressed. Empirical studies in schizophrenia research are reviewed in relation to principles of normal brain function sub-serving working memory: the functional role of the lateral prefrontal cortex, physiological response capacity constraints, interregional functional integration, and compensatory adaptations. However, complex heterogeneous psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia cannot be considered akin to a pure lesion model, and there are considerable methodological challenges in interpreting disruptions of working memory in psychiatric conditions, resulting from clinical, treatment and performance related confounds. The increasing use of psychopharmacological models of disease in healthy human subjects is therefore considered as an attempt to address, or to some extent circumvent these issues. 2005 Published by Elsevier Ltd on behalf of IBRO. Key words: working memory, schizophrenia, psychiatry, fMRI, functional imaging.

Schizophrenia is a complex psychiatric disorder which encompasses a wide range of behavioral phenomena expressed to varying degrees in symptomatic patients. The positive symptoms of the disorder, including hallucinations (primarily in the auditory modality) and delusional ideation, are perhaps the most widely recognized characteristics of the illness, while negative symptoms, for example, social withdrawal and attened affect, are also
*Correspondence to: G. D. Honey, University of Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, Addenbrookes Hospital, Brain Mapping Unit, Box 255, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK. Tel: 44-01223-764673; fax: 4401223-764675. E-mail address: gh242@cam.ac.uk; URL: http://fs0.psychiatry.cam. ac.uk/gh242/ (G. D. Honey). Abbreviations: fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; NAA, N-acetylaspartate; PCP, phencyclidine; PET, positron emission tomography; TMS, transcranial magnetic stimulation.
0306-4522/06$30.00 0.00 2005 Published by Elsevier Ltd on behalf of IBRO. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.036

intrinsic features which contribute substantially to the debilitating nature of the disorder. In addition to these and other experiential disturbances, patients with schizophrenia also show decits across a broad range of neuropsychological domains. Impairment of cognitive function is increasingly recognized as a core feature of this illness (Green, 1996; Green and Nuechterlein, 1999), and has recently been identied by the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) Initiative to comprise primary decits involving working memory, attention/vigilance, verbal learning and memory, visual learning and memory, reasoning and problem solving, speed of processing, and social cognition (Green et al., 2004). Working memory, the ability to maintain and utilize information in short-term memory (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 1986), is a process which is central to everyday functioning, and contributes signicantly to other areas of cognition. The theoretical concept of working memory was developed in response to limitations of previous models of short-term memory, such as Atkinson and Shiffrins Two-Process Model (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968), to fully account for performance impairments in neuropsychological patients. The working memory model, proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974), identied a three component system: the phonological loop (comprising a limited capacity phonological store in which verbal information is stored temporarily and maintained by subvocal rehearsal); the visuospatial sketchpad (a parallel sub-system to the phonological store, specialized for non-verbal material); and the central executive, responsible for strategic co-ordination and execution of the slave systems. The original model was updated, to include an episodic buffer, which provides an interface between the sub-systems of working memory and long-term memory (Baddeley, 2000). Decits in working memory have been consistently reported in schizophrenic patients (Weinberger and Cermak, 1973; Park and Holzman, 1992; Fleming et al., 1995; Keefe et al., 1995; Morris et al., 1997; Park and McTigue, 1997; Spindler et al., 1997; Park et al., 1999), and also rst-degree asymptomatic relatives (Conklin et al., 2000). There is some evidence of disproportionate memory impairment in the context of other domains of cognitive dysfunction (Saykin et al., 1991, 1994), and prognostic implications of such decits in psychosocial rehabilitation programs (Green, 1996). Accordingly, decient working memory is key to a number of contemporary cognitive psychological models of schizophrenic symptoms (Goldman-Rakic, 1990a, 1994; Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992; Weinberger, 1993).

59

60

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

Given the centrality of working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia, the question arises as to whether the information ascertained from this disorder may provide further insights into normal brain function underlying working memory. The precedent for this approach is provided by the contribution neuropsychological investigations have made to the development of theoretical models throughout cognitive psychology. Indeed, the concept of working memory and precursive models, have beneted substantially from investigations of behavioral impairments in patients following neurological damage, sometimes referred to as natural memory experiments. These studies, typically based on individual case histories, are often particularly inuential in addressing subtle discriminations, which may prove intractable to standard experimental methodology: for example, observing intact subvocal rehearsal in patients with anarthria, demonstrating that the subvocalization process, necessary to maintain and refresh information in working memory, does not require explicit articulation (Baddeley and Wilson, 1985). Similarly, the twocomponent model of the phonological loop, involving both subvocal rehearsal and phonological storage, was substantiated on the basis of observations in patient P.V., who showed a specic sensitivity to phonological similarity effects for information presented in the auditory modality. Visually-presented information was insensitive to phonological similarity, word length or articulatory suppression. Vallar and Baddeley (1984) interpreted this as an indication of a specic impairment at the level of phonological storage (thereby indicating its process separability), causing the patient to strategically avoid use of the rehearsal mechanism for visual information, whereas the phonological store has automatic access to auditory information. These examples demonstrate how neuropsychology can be critical in developing and constraining theoretical models of cognitive function. This approach can also be extended to incorporate the neurobiological implementation of cognitive processes. This follows the classical tradition inaugurated by Brocas localization of speech production to the inferior frontal gyrus on the basis of the monosyllabic speech capacity of a patient following a lesion to this region identied postmortem, and similarly Wernickes observation that damage to the left posterior temporal cortex produces decits in speech comprehension. Progressing beyond single dissociations such as these, relating the role of a particular brain region with a specic cognitive process is most compellingly demonstrated by double dissociations, in which the loss of function X and preservation of function Y is observed in patient A, while the reverse pattern is evident in patient B). To what extent can these principles of the neuropsychological approach be applied to neuropsychiatry? Specically, can the behavioral decits in working memory observed in patients with schizophrenia inform current models of the neurobiological basis of working memory? The neuropsychological tradition of relating structural lesions to cognitive dysfunction is clearly limited in its application to schizophrenia, since the neuropathology in schizophrenia is diffuse, subtle and variable between pa-

tients and over the course of the illness. However, putative functional pathology associated with schizophrenia may prove revealing in relation to observed cognitive decits. In this article, the extent to which functional imaging studies of schizophrenia can be considered to have validated biological models of working memory in the normal human brain is reviewed. Implicitly, this asks the question of whether the principles of lesion-based neuropsychology are directly transferable to (dys)functional imaging? The focus on functional neuroimaging is warranted on the basis that this offers an unrivalled technique in cognitive neuroscience to investigate the biological basis of human cognitive function in vivo. For descriptive purposes, this review is organized in terms of principles of brain function which have emerged as important in the biological implementation of working memory. We rstly consider the functional role of the lateral prefrontal cortex: animal studies have clearly demonstrated the involvement of this region in working memory, however, the interpretation of its role in humans, as evidenced by functional neuroimaging is complicated by issues including reverse causality (the inuence of behavior on physiological response and vice versa), functional diaschisis (a primary abnormality in a remote brain region), and the limitations of the application of principles used as gold standards in neuropsychology to functional imaging. We further consider the response properties of the prefrontal cortex, and how capacity limitations may explain the relationship between working memory performance, and hypo- or hyper-activity in frontal regions in patients. The prefrontal cortex clearly does not function in isolation, and we review studies which have examined its connectivity with other structures, in both healthy subjects and patients. Finally, we consider how functional decits in regions including prefrontal cortex may be mitigated by compensatory recruitment of other brain areas, and the implications for behavioral assessments. There are a number of interpretative difculties in extrapolating from the disease state to the normal brain which are considered, and this is particularly relevant in the case of schizophrenia, in which the uncertain etiology/pathophysiology, and heterogeneity of the clinical prole and its treatment, seriously limit what can be generalized from this disorder to normative brain function subserving working memory. In conclusion, we suggest that an alternative approach involving the use of psychopharmacological models of disease may circumvent some of these issues. Functional neuroimaging in psychiatry as a tool in cognitive neuroscience While there is no implied anatomical localization of the subcomponents of Baddeleys model of working memory, basic and clinical neuroscience research has increasingly provided support for the dissociation of processes proposed, and has gone some way to formulating a functional topology. Functional neuroimaging, primarily using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has, with increasing sophistication in experimental design, explored the biolog-

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

61

ical basis of working memory in healthy human subjects (for review, see references (DEsposito et al., 2000; Owen, 2000; Fletcher and Henson, 2001; Wager and Smith, 2003)). However, a limitation of this approach is that imaging can reveal only the engagement of neural systems which are correlated with cognitive function. As a cognitive process is experimentally manipulated, PET and fMRI provide a remarkable capability to visualize the brain regions which show task-related responsivity. However, while the results of increasingly focused experimental paradigms can be persuasively argued to represent a causal relationship, ultimately this cannot be demonstrated satisfactorily using these techniques in isolation, and must be interpreted in the context of information from other methodological approaches which involve perturbed function, such as behavioral impairments in neuropsychological patients, or noninvasive experimental interventions, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). TMS allows experimental manipulation of regional neuronal activity by applying a brief, high-amplitude pulse of current which temporarily interferes with the activity of local neurons. The observation of impaired cognitive performance following local disruption of neural activity in a given region indicates that the functional integrity of a region is necessary for performance of a task. The application of this technique to working memory has been reported by several groups (Mottaghy et al., 2000; Mull and Seyal, 2001; Oliveri et al., 2001; Mottaghy et al., 2002; Nixon et al., 2004). Similarly, the integration of ndings from neuroimaging and those of neuropsychological impairment following discrete lesions facilitates identication of regions which are associated with, and necessary for particular functions, respectively (Fiez, 2001). The use of schizophrenia as an in vivo disruption of biological function can be considered in some ways parallel to the TMS and neuropsychological approach, in that behavioral decits are related to primary physiological disturbances. Indeed, schizophrenia may go beyond the classical lesion model, involving disruptions of inter-regional functional connections, rather than localized regional deficits. Thus schizophrenia offers the opportunity to investigate a systems-level disruption of biological function, and concomitant behavioral impairments may provide some indication of the nature of the processes perturbed by a functional lesion. Convergent evidence regarding structurefunction relationships may therefore be ascertained from complementary methodologies, incorporating functional imaging in healthy subjects and patients with schizophrenia, TMS and neuropsychological performance in patients with discrete lesions. It is now a decade since the rst fMRI studies were reported in patients with schizophrenia (Renshaw et al., 1994; Wenz et al., 1994), and a large number of these have incorporated working memory paradigms (Weinberger et al., 1996; Callicott et al., 1998, 2000, 2003a; Stevens et al., 1998; Honey et al., 1999, 2002a, 2003a; Manoach et al., 1999, 2000, 2001; Barch et al., 2001, 2002, 2003; Egan et al., 2001; Menon et al., 2001; Perlstein et al., 2001, 2003; Wykes et al., 2002; Quintana et al., 2003; Schlosser et al., 2003a,b; Walter et

al., 2003; Jacobsen et al., 2004; Kindermann et al., 2004; Mendrek et al., 2004, Thermenos et al., 2005), making a review of this body of literature, and its implications for understanding principles of normal brain function associated with working memory, timely. Principles of human brain function involved in working memory Functional role of the lateral prefrontal cortex. Extensive single-unit recording studies in non-human primates demonstrated that cells in and around the principal sulcus in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex exhibit delayrelated activity during delayed match to sample paradigms (Goldman-Rakic, 1987, 1990b). Furthermore, impeding lateral prefrontal activity, either via pharmacological blockade of dopamine D1 receptors (Sawaguchi and GoldmanRakic, 1991, 1994; Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995), or neurochemical/surgical lesions (Funahashi et al., 1993) of local tissue, impairs working memory. The functional involvement of the homologous region in humans in working memory has subsequently been conrmed by a large number of functional imaging studies using both PET and fMRI. However, this indicates that the prefrontal cortex is associated with working memory in humans, but it does not test the hypothesis that prefrontal activation is a requirement for working memory. Does psychiatric imaging therefore conrm the prediction from animal studies that pathological conditions which compromise the integrity of prefrontal functioning should be associated with working memory impairments? Goldman-Rakic (1990a, 1999) noted a parallel between the performance decits of schizophrenic patients and those of non-human primates following lesion of the principal sulcal region of the prefrontal cortex, indicating that a pathological functional lesion of the prefrontal cortex in humans may underlie working memory decits observed in these patients. Early imaging studies in schizophrenia, involving complex cognitive tasks engaging a range of executive processes, typically exhibited a reduced frontal response in patients with schizophrenia, termed hypofrontality (Buchsbaum et al., 1986; Weinberger and Berman, 1988; Paulman et al., 1990; Andreasen et al., 1992; Berman et al., 1993). More recently, with the growing consensus that working memory represents a core decit in schizophrenia, studies involving tasks designed to more specically isolate working memory processes also reported a hypofrontal response (Callicott et al., 1998; Carter et al., 1998; Stevens et al., 1998; Barch et al., 2001, 2002, 2003; Perlstein et al., 2003). A possible implication of these studies for normal brain function was that the lateral prefrontal cortex, which is consistently activated in healthy volunteers during working memory tasks, is a necessary requirement for working memory processing, and in psychiatric conditions in which prefrontal function is disrupted, a failure of working memory is observed. While these clinical studies largely involved medicated out-patients experiencing chronic illness, this is unlikely to represent an effect of disease chronicity or anti-psychotic treatment, since Barch et al. (2001) observed these effects in patients

62

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

who were scanned within 12 days of their rst contact with psychiatric services and had not been treated with neuroleptics prior to their involvement in the study. However, there is an interesting distinction here which exemplies the complexities of applying the neuropsychological approach to neuropsychiatry: Reverse causality. The observation of a cognitive decit following a structural lesion to region A which was absent prior to the lesion would naturally lead the neuropsychologist to infer that region A was in some way involved in the neural architecture underpinning the psychological function that was lost. Clearly, it would be untenable to imply causality in the opposite direction. However, in evaluating the loss of biological function, as opposed to structure, causality may indeed operate in either direction. It is possible that a physiological abnormality may lead to a cognitive decit, however it is equally possible that the behavioral decit, perhaps mediated indirectly via other factors (for example increased distractibility due to pervasive psychotic experiences), causes the subject to disengage from the task, and thereby fail to recruit task-related activity in associated brain regions (for further discussion, see (Price and Friston, 1999)). Frith et al. (1995) proposed that the impaired performance of schizophrenic patients on frontal lobe tasks may result in the use of alternative strategies which do not engage prefrontal cortex, and thus result in hypofrontality. Similarly, Ebmeier et al., suggested that The poorer performance of frontal activation tasks by patients with schizophrenia is probably sufcient explanation for the difference from controls, who perform such tasks well (Ebmeier et al., 1995). Manoach et al.(1999) suggested that faced with a task which is too difcult, patients may feel overwhelmed and disengage. The relationship between physiological activation and cognitive performance is complex. Several studies, which have attempted to characterize this further, are considered further in Physiological capacity constraints. Functional diaschisis. Both abnormal prefrontal response and cognitive impairment could potentially be explained by diaschisis: a primary abnormality in a remote brain region. Several regions, including the posterior parietal cortex and basal ganglia show delay-related activity in non-human primates and similarly, activation in functional imaging studies in humans. There is certainly evidence of abnormal activation of these structures in schizophrenia. It is possible therefore that decits in either of these could represent the primary abnormality. Conceivably, this may lead to secondary, downstream effects in other brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex. Further investigation of the connectivity between these regions will therefore be critical in interpreting the functional involvement of the prefrontal cortex in working memory impairment. Corroborative evidence that prefrontal dysfunction is critical to working memory impairment is potentially available if it is the case that (i) prefrontal abnormalities are also evident in other psychiatric disorders in which working memory impairment is present, and (ii) pathology did not overlap in other regions except prefrontal cortex. This overlap across disorders would tend to suggest that pre-

frontal dysfunction represents the central abnormality in working memory dysfunction. For example, there is some evidence of working memory impairment in patients with unipolar depression (Sweeney et al., 1998; Pelosi et al., 2000), however, few functional imaging studies have investigated the role of the prefrontal cortex during working memory performance in this patient population. Barch et al. (2003) compared patients with major depression to patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. In contrast to the hypofrontal response in schizophrenia, they did not observe a prefrontal decit in depressed subjects; however, working memory performance was disrupted in the schizophrenic, but not the depressed patients. A recent study reported by Hugdahl et al. (2004) found that despite similar performance decits in patients, schizophrenic subjects showed attenuated frontal response during a mental arithmetic task which was not evident in depressed subjects. While this task incorporated a working memory requirement, more process-specic tasks may be required to address the question of whether prefrontal function is disrupted during working memory performance in patients with non-schizophrenic psychiatric conditions, where a behavioral decit is also evident, and whether there is regionalspecicity of this overlap. While not conclusive, these studies could potentially be informative with regard to whether functional disruption in this region is necessary for working memory performance. Dissociation methodology. The use of double dissociations, which are prevalent in neuropsychological investigations and provide compelling indications of process separability and neuroanatomical localization, does not translate in a straightforward manner to functional imaging data. For example, Bechara et al. (1995) reported that a patient with selective bilateral damage to the amygdala failed to acquire conditioned autonomic responses to visual or auditory stimuli but was able to demonstrate explicit knowledge of the declarative facts about which stimuli were paired to the unconditioned stimulus. The opposite pattern was observed in a patient with selective bilateral damage to the hippocampus. Finally, both conditioning and explicit awareness were found to be disrupted in a patient with bilateral damage to both amygdala and hippocampal formation. This elegant study provides strong evidence for a double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge, and association with the integrity of the amygdala and hippocampus respectively. Could the principles underlying these ndings be extended to functional imaging? Process separability would be less convincing on the basis of functional observations, since an alternative explanation of these data could involve a single process which underlies the functional activation of both regions; if this process was engaged to a greater extent in one task, e.g. acquisition of conditioning, and produces both hippocampal activation and deactivation of amygdala, then the apparent double dissociation is misleading. Henson (2005) recently outlined how process separability is identiable in functional imaging data, if one extends Dunn and Kirsners (1988) principle of reversed association. The application to functional imaging data

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

63

corresponds to the observation of increased task-related activity in two brain regions, relative to an independent baseline condition, and simultaneously, that the two regions contrast negatively across two task conditions. This particular pattern of activity guards against the possibility that a double dissociation simply represents opposing effects of a shared single process, perhaps mediated via reciprocal connectivity between the two regions. However, while a reversed association imbues the observed association between structure and function with a greater degree of process specicity, it does not change in any way the nature of this relationship, i.e. it remains a (processspecic) correlative observation, which is not an indication of whether the pattern of activation is necessary or sufcient. As such, disease-related perturbation of activity observed in regions for which a reverse association can be demonstrated remains ambiguous with regard to its implications for normal brain function, as for single and double dissociations. For the reasons outlined above, the interpretation of functional abnormalities, such as hypofrontality in response to working memory tasks in schizophrenia, is confounded by alternative explanations which limit the feasibility of extending these observations to the normal human brain. Brain function, as opposed to structure, has the property of dynamic adaptation, and this causes the functional relationships between regions to be uid and context-dependent. Physiological abnormalities observed using functional imaging are therefore insufcient to identify regions which are necessary for specic cognitive processes. However, while the application of the principles of classical neuropsychology to modern functional imaging methods may be inappropriate, this is not to suggest that there is no insight to be gained into the normal brain based on the functional decits identied using neuroimaging in patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychological and imaging-based investigations are synergistic approaches (Rorden and Karnath, 2004), however, the nature of the insights offered by these two techniques differs considerably. Below, the possible insights from functional imaging are considered further. Physiological capacity constraints. Neuroimaging studies in healthy volunteers have demonstrated that the functional relationship between prefrontal response and cognitive demand is not a simple linear association. Prefrontal activation increases in line with task demands until the capacity limitation of working memory is reached, at which point, activation decreases (Callicott et al., 1999). The relationship between cognitive performance and cortical physiology is therefore best described by an inverted-U function, in contrast to response in other (though not all) brain regions, which plateau as capacity is reached (Callicott et al., 1999). The hypofrontal response observed in patients with schizophrenia, may therefore reect the normal attenuation of response following overload of working memory capacity, occurring at an abnormally reduced threshold due to pathology. Since patients with schizophrenia have reduced working memory capacity, studying this popula-

tion offers the opportunity to dissociate the factors of cognitive load and capacity, since capacity will be breached at a lower load threshold in patients compared with healthy volunteers. This is an important dissociation since increments in working memory load are often not purely quantitative increases, but may also induce qualitative differences between levels, such that further processes and adjunctive strategies are engaged as demand increases (Honey et al., 2000). The observation of similar principles of brain function underlying capacity limitation at low load in patients, compared with high load in controls would therefore provide robust support for the proposed relationship between physiological response and cognitive load, independent of the qualitative variations associated with experimental task manipulations. The suggestion that hypofrontality may reect reduced cognitive capacity, with normal (or increased) frontal response occurring within performance capacity was rst proposed by Fletcher et al. (1998) employing a word-recall task: they observed that patients showed normal frontal activation when the cognitive demands of the task were low, but failed to show the increased prefrontal activation to increased word list length observed in controls. As they noted, since list length was also associated with time, this nding may relate to a failure to engage long-term memory processes or the maintenance of information in working memory. The latter interpretation was supported by subsequent studies involving more specic working memory tasks. Using the Sternberg item recognition task (Sternberg, 1966) Manoach et al. (1999, 2000) reported increased frontal activation in patients performing a task which was within capacity, but for which behavioral indices demonstrated that patients found the task more difcult than controls. Interestingly, a negative correlation was observed between error rate and prefrontal activation in the patients, suggesting that activation increases with demand, until cognitive capacity is exceeded. As they noted, this leads to the intriguing speculation that further increases in load may have resulted in a hypofrontal response in the patients, whereas this increase may have remained within the performance capacity of the controls, and therefore resulted in increased activation in line with demand, thus reversing the between-group comparison observed at lower cognitive loads. In accordance with this, Perlstein et al. (2001) found that hypofrontality was observed in the patient group only at the highest load, the only level which differentiated groups on behavioral performance. The hypofrontal response variably reported in schizophrenia is thus not a static phenomenon, but dynamically related to task requirements and subjects capacity range to perform the task. Callicott et al. (2000) recruited patients based on prior screening of minimal prociency ( 90% accuracy on the n-back task) and demonstrated a hyperfrontal response to working memory demands as observed by Manoach et al. (1999, 2000). They also conrmed the correlation between accuracy and prefrontal response, and additionally showed that while this was observed for both dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in controls, the pattern was reversed, specically in

64

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

the dorsolateral region. Furthermore, this association between behavior and physiology was predicted by reduced N-acetylaspartate (NAA) concentrations in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: neuronal pathology (reduced NAA) was associated with inefcient dorsolateral activation. The relationship between cognitive performance and physiological response is clearly a complex association; the above summary is not conclusive, and does not account for all discrepancies in the literature (see Methodological challenges for further methodological challenges; for review of other relevant technical issues in assessing prefrontal function in schizophrenia, see Manoach (2003)). However, the proposed relationship between increasing prefrontal activation in response to increased cognitive load, and the disruption of prefrontal function once cognitive capacity is reached, is strengthened by the observation from schizophrenia studies, where the interpretation appears to hold, despite the fact that cognitive capacity is reduced, and therefore capacity limitations are reached at lower thresholds. Psychiatric studies therefore serve to test the hypothesized relationship between performance and physiology over a broader dose-response curve of cognitive ability. The contribution of studies in schizophrenia has therefore been to provide convergent evidence, critical to the development of a robust theory of working memory. Functional connectivity. The prefrontal cortex clearly plays a critical role in working memory, however a more complete description of its neurobiological basis requires consideration of a complex integration of information across a large scale neurocognitive network to support cognitive function. Physiological connectivity is inferred from neuroimaging data on the basis of the correlation observed between time-series from two distinct brain regions. This correlation may indicate a direct inter-regional causal relationship, or alternatively may be mediated by additional regions. In order to evaluate a quantitative assessment of the functional relationship between multiple brain regions, multivariate methods such as path analysis and structural equation modeling (McIntosh et al., 1994; Buchel and Friston, 1997; Bullmore et al., 2000) are increasingly applied to imaging data in order to test hypothesis-driven models of integrative function within anatomical constraints. These techniques have been termed functional and effective connectivity respectively (Friston et al., 1997). Theoretical models of schizophrenia increasingly identify functional dysconnectivity as a primary pathophysiological mechanism (Weinberger, 1993; Friston and Frith, 1995; Bullmore et al., 1997). Accordingly, a number of studies have reported functional abnormalities involving the afferent and efferent projections of the prefrontal cortex (Fletcher et al., 1999; Bunney and Bunney, 2000; MeyerLindenberg et al., 2001; Stephan et al., 2001; Ford et al., 2002; Lawrie et al., 2002; Kim et al., 2003; Schlosser et al., 2003a; Winterer et al., 2003). Schizophrenia, as a disorder of functional connectivity may therefore provide an appropriate test of connectionist models of the components of

working memory, involving a functional lesion, compared with discrete structural lesions typically the focus of neuropsychology. Both functional (Li et al., 2004) and effective (Honey et al., 2002c) connectivity analyses of working memory have been reported in healthy volunteers. Honey et al. (2002c) reported that increasing working memory load was associated with increased connectivity between frontal and parietal regions, and also increased inter-hemispheric communication between dorsolateral frontal regions. The proposed dysconnectivity and associated failure of working memory performance in schizophrenia provides an ideal opportunity to test the hypothesis of whether the strength of fronto-frontal and fronto-parietal connections is indeed required to meet increasing working memory demands. In support of this, Meyer-Lindenberg et al. (2001) found that while a pattern of connectivity involving lateral prefrontal, cingulate and parietal regions was observed in controls, a pattern incorporating inferotemporal, parahippocampal and cerebellar connectivity was observed for the patient group. Similarly, Kim et al. (2003) found that prefrontal activation correlated signicantly with bilateral parietal regions in controls, but not in patients. Schlosser et al. (2003a) compared patients treated with either typical or atypical anti-psychotics and healthy volunteers. They found that both patient groups showed reduced inter-hemispheric communication between dorsolateral frontal regions, and that this was most severe in the typically-treated group, and replicated these ndings in drug-free patients (Schlosser et al., 2003b). Taken together, these studies provide convergent evidence that fronto-parietal and interhemispheric frontal connectivity is central to working memory function. These studies also reported impaired working memory performance in patients; it will be important for future studies to determine whether increased functional connectivity is observed at working memory loads within the performance capacity of the patients. Compensatory adaptation. Functional reorganization following a structural lesion resulting from a cerebrovascular infarction is commonly observed in cerebral ischemic patients. In functional disorders such as schizophrenia, there is some evidence that similar compensatory adaptations may also occur in response to compromised function in a given brain region. The observations of alternative patterns of activity in patients, in the context of intact behavioral performance, have been reported by several groups. This may be an indication of the greater sensitivity of functional imaging to neurocognitive dysfunction in comparison to routine neuropsychological assessments, whereby gross measures, such as task accuracy and reaction time may present as unimpaired, but conceal more subtle physiological abnormalities, leading to physiological compensation in other brain regions, or cognitive compensation, such as adapting alternative strategies. In relation to normal brain function, such observations could potentially provide information that the functional recruitment of particular brain regions may not be necessary for working memory, and can be replaced or at least supplemented,

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

65

without loss of function. Additionally, aberrant activity at lower performance requirements may provide insight into the cause of behavioral disruption as task demands increase. Manoach et al. (2000) found that patients with schizophrenia showed increased activation of basal ganglia and thalamus during performance of a working memory task, which was not observed in controls. This observation was independent of whether or not performance was equated across groups (by comparing high load in controls and low load in patients). The authors suggest that studies in patients with Parkinsons disease indicate that fronto-striatal circuitry is critical in supporting working memory function (Owen, 1997) and subcortical activation is frequently observed under conditions involving increasing working memory load (Barch et al., 1997; Goldberg et al., 1998; Callicott et al., 1999; Rypma et al., 1999). They therefore speculate that the activation of basal ganglia and thalamus in the patient group may reect a failure to automate aspects of task performance, and tune patterns of activity to optimize performance. Increased recruitment of the basal ganglia is compatible with the ndings of Schlosser et al. (2003a), who reported that patients treated with either typical or atypical anti-psychotics showed increased connectivity between thalamus and prefrontal cortex. The implication of these studies is that basal ganglia projections to frontal cortex via the thalamus are involved in the normal circuitry of the early phases underlying working memory, and that other regions, such as prefrontal and parietal cortices, supplant subcortical activity as performance improves. These predictions were subsequently supported in a recent study demonstrating that increasing practice on a working memory task in healthy volunteers was associated with diminishing activation of putamen, thalamus and anterior frontal gyrus (Landau et al., 2004). On the basis of prior associations between fronto-striatal circuitry and the formation of arbitrary visuomotor associations and abstract rules (Murray et al., 2000), the authors suggest that the attenuation of fronto-striatal recruitment may reect subjects reducing need to focus on task rules, as subjects became more practiced (Landau et al., 2004). The persistence of such activation in patients with schizophrenia may therefore reect a compensatory response to maintain task requirements during performance. This aberrant activation of basal ganglia may also lead to inefcient recruitment of the frontal cortex: Callicott et al. (2003b) found that in patients with performance levels similar to low-performing controls, a predominantly hyperfrontal response was observed, but regions of hypofrontality were also evident. Studies of working memory in patients with schizophrenia have therefore provided an indication of the transition from inefcient, novice task performance to automation and expertise, characterized by a development from subcortical to cortical mediation of function. Methodological challenges Observations based on physiological abnormalities associated with working memory decits in schizophrenia have

certainly provided useful information which allow hypotheses regarding specialization and integration of function to be tested in vivo by observing the effects of physiological abnormalities in the brain. However, one should be cautious in extrapolating from the disease state to the normal brain, and indeed this is particularly the case for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is a disorder of unknown etiology/ pathophysiology. Schizophrenia cannot be considered as a pure lesion model. In contrast to the discrete localized structural lesions explored in neuropsychological investigations, gross structural abnormalities are not typical of schizophrenia. Any observed relationship between a cognitive decit and presumed neurophysiological functional abnormality must be considered tentatively since the pathophysiological basis of schizophrenia remains undetermined. Schizophrenia is therefore a complex illness with no known etiology, no biological markers, treatment which is ineffective or only partially effective in a large number of cases, and its expression varies widely across individuals. These are critical questions which must be addressed before one can condently draw conclusions from an observed abnormality in schizophrenia in relation to normal human brain function (Honey et al., 2002b). Heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia incorporates heterogeneous psychotic phenomena, none of which are pathognomonic of the illness. Patients differ widely along numerous parameters, including clinical presentation, prognosis, insight, cognitive dysfunction, neurological impairment, institutionalization, susceptibility to pharmacological side-effects and demographic history. Accordingly, clinicians confront each individual case with a variety of principal and concomitant management options, which will often vary over the course of an individuals progression through the illness, and particularly between patients. Given this complex scenario of heterogeneity of both illness characterization and treatment, research efforts are forced to confront numerous confounds and myriad possible confound interactions. Consequently, research which groups subjects simply on the basis of diagnostic categories may be misleading, incorporating subjects which may have few, if any symptoms in common. However, in order to examine the neurobiological basis of working memory dysfunction, a case-control design has typically been adopted, involving the comparison of patient groups with healthy volunteers. Given the heterogeneity of psychiatric disorders, and variability over the course of the illness, this design may not be optimal: If a diverse group of disorders is pooled together in studies of biological correlates, important ndings may be lost because fundamental differences have been averaged out. Only a broad spread of variance is left behind as a clue to suggest the possible heterogeneity of schizophrenia. This variance is perhaps one of the most consistent observations in research on schizophrenia (Andreasen, 1987). This point is supported by the observation by Manoach et al. (2000) examining individual responses to a working

66

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

memory task: they found that increased spatial heterogeneity of response in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was evident in patients with schizophrenia compared with controls. The source of this heterogeneity is unknown, however, as the authors point out, this observation raises the possibility that group-averaging may underestimate frontal response to working memory tasks in schizophrenia. While this could not explain a hyperfrontal response, it may provide an explanation for hypofrontality, as frequently reported. Strauss et al. (1974) suggested that, Group comparison studies, using summarizing statistics, the normal science in schizophrenia research, may at times be misleading if not inappropriate. Such group summary data are crucial to PET studies and almost ubiquitously used in fMRI studies of working memory. Shallice et al. (1991) suggested: inferences about the impaired functioning of particular regions of the brain in schizophrenics on the basis of the performance of groups of patients on individual neuropsychological tests should only be made tentatively. The implication of the heterogeneity of prefrontal response to working memory tasks in schizophrenia is that the expression of certain symptoms may be of particular relevance to working memory dysfunction, and that this will potentially impact on the cortical response to working memory demands. Accordingly, McGrath et al. (2001) found that working memory impairment corresponded with the severity of negative symptoms and thought disorder; Menon et al. (2001) found that negative symptoms and thought disorder were inversely correlated with activation in the frontal operculum and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex respectively. Honey et al. (2003a) found that a positive sub-syndrome was associated with a reduced fronto-temporal response to working memory, and negative symptoms were associated with increased response in medial and lateral premotor regions. Further investigations of associations between psychiatric symptoms, cognitive performance and physiological response may yield important information about the implications of working memory (dys)function, which would inaccessible by other means, and may serve to inform current models of working memory. Treatment-related confounds. Patients with schizophrenia are treated with a variety of anti-psychotics. The mechanism of action of these therapeutic agents is currently unknown. To a varying degree, all anti-psychotic medications have a considerable inuence on the dopaminergic system (Strange, 2001; Kapur and Mamo, 2003), which is critically involved in working memory performance (Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1991, 1994; Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995). There is some evidence that atypical anti-psychotics (newer compounds which tend to have reduced propensity to cause parkinsonian symptoms in humans and animal models as observed with typical anti-psychotics (Kerwin, 1994)) have some improvement in working memory and executive function (Green et al., 1997; Meltzer and McGurk, 1999; Harvey et al., 2003). However, the impact of anti-psychotics on the cerebral response to working memory has frequently been over-

looked in interpreting cortical responses in schizophrenic patients. Honey et al. (1999) investigated the effect of the atypical antipsychotic, risperidone, on prefrontal function. Atypical antipsychotics were predicted to enhance prefrontal function during performance of a working memory task, hypothetically via increased dopaminergic drive to the prefrontal cortex. This was predicted on the basis that patients with schizophrenia perform poorly on tests of working memory, and typically exhibit a hypofrontal response to such tasks (see Functional role of the lateral prefrontal cortex), which can be reversed by administration of dopamine agonists (Daniel et al., 1989). Furthermore, atypical antipsychotics have been shown to increase prefrontal dopaminergic activity in animal models (Hertel et al., 1996). The substitution of typical antipsychotics for risperidone was associated with increased activation of prefrontal cortex, compared with patients maintained on typical antipsychotics (Honey et al., 1999). There is also evidence that anti-psychotic treatment affects the functional connections of the prefrontal cortex (Stephan et al., 2001; Nahas et al., 2003). The interpretation of physiological decits observed during the performance of working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia is therefore complicated by the effects of both the illness and its treatment. It is possible to circumvent this issue by studying patients nave to drug exposure, or following a drug-washout period. Such studies (Barch et al., 2001; Stephan et al., 2001; Schlosser et al., 2003b), which are particularly difcult to perform and relatively infrequently reported, therefore carry considerable inuence, as highlighted elsewhere in this review. Psychopharmacological models Overview. Exposure to psychedelic substances in non-psychotic individuals produces profound perceptual, sensorimotor and cognitive disturbances. These effects vary considerably across drug categories, such as psychomotor stimulants (e.g. cocaine and amphetamine), psychotomimetic indoleamines (e.g. lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD)) and dissociative anesthetics (e.g. phencyclidine (PCP) and ketamine). The experiences produced by some of these compounds bear impressive similarity [to] . . . certain primary symptoms of the schizophrenic process (Luby et al., 1959). The use of psychotomimetic compounds to induce a transitory state of psychopathology in healthy controls has therefore increasingly been employed as a human in vivo model of psychosis, to reproduce the features of schizophrenia in healthy subjects. In accordance with the view that working memory dysfunction is a key aspect of schizophrenia, these compounds also perturb working memory performance, and thereby provide an opportunity to explore neurotransmitter mechanisms involved in working memory disruption. Combining psychopharmacological models of disease with functional imaging therefore provides a powerful approach to exploring the biological basis of working memory (dys)function. While this approach introduces additional considerations, such as the effect of the drug on the dependent imaging measure, e.g. the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) contrast (Honey and Bullmore, 2004; Shah

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71

67

and Marsden, 2004), the primary advantage of this technique is that specic neurotransmitters can be experimentally augmented/inhibited in order to test hypotheses regarding their involvement in working memory and schizophrenia. This approach circumvents many of the interpretative problems outlined earlier, associated with observing cognitive decits in patients with schizophrenia, by avoiding confounding factors such as chronicity of illness/treatment. In contrast to the static performance decits in patient groups, dose administration can be graduated to establish a dose-response curve, up to and beyond cognitive capacity constraints. Furthermore, subjects serve as their own controls in double-blind, placebo-controlled, repeated measures designs, to some extent mitigating issues of group heterogeneity associated with between-group comparisons. The use of low-dose administration also offers the possibility of observing cognitive/ physiological effects of the drug which are below the threshold at which psychotic phenomenon are observed, thus facilitating a dissociation of these effects. For example, the presence of a hallucination may be sufciently attentionally distracting to indirectly impair working memory performance, despite no specic effect on working memory processing per se. Eliciting cognitive disturbances at levels of drug exposure below that at which psychotic symptoms are evident thereby allows a more direct interpretation of the psychopharmacological manipulation. Before this approach can serve to clarify observations made on the basis of performance of schizophrenic patients, the validity of psychopharmacological models remains to be fully explored and validated at the cognitive and physiological level. However, psychopharmacological modulation of working memory has already proved a valuable research tool in increasing understanding of how the normal brain implements working memory processes, and also in validating current models of working memory. Dopamine. The dominant hypothesis of the pathophysiology of schizophrenia has centered around a disturbance of dopaminergic neurotransmission. The most compelling evidence for this is based on two observations: (i) the efcacy of anti-psychotic drugs in treating positive symptoms of schizophrenia is highly correlated with their afnity for post-synaptic dopamine (D2) receptors (Carlsson and Lindqvist, 1963; Matthysse, 1973; Creese et al., 1976; Seeman et al., 1976), and (ii) psychostimulants which serve to increase dopaminergic transmission cause psychotic symptoms. Amphetamine, which increases dopamine release and blocks re-uptake has been used in humans to demonstrate the inverted-U-shaped association between dopaminergic tone and prefrontal response to working memory tasks, previously observed using electrophysiological recordings of single unit activity recorded from prefrontal neurons in nonhuman primates (Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Mattay et al. (2000) found that dextroamphetamine improved working memory performance only in subjects with low capacity prior to drug; subjects with high capacity showed performance impairment and a greater increase in prefrontal

activation. This study demonstrates that dopamine optimizes efciency of prefrontal activity in low-capacity individuals, and reduces efciency in high-capacity individuals, by increasing dopamine beyond optimum levels. Mattay et al.(2003) also demonstrated that working memory performance under amphetamine is inuenced by a common polymorphism [val(158)-met] in the catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene, important for metabolism of synaptically released dopamine in the prefrontal cortex: individuals with the val/val genotype showed improved working memory performance and reduced prefrontal activity under amphetamine compared with placebo, whereas performance was disrupted by amphetamine in individuals with the met/met genotype, and increased prefrontal activity was observed (Mattay et al., 2003). Glutamate. The proposed involvement of glutamate in schizophrenia developed from the observation that dissociative anesthetics such as PCP and ketamine reproduce both positive and negative symptoms of psychosis in healthy volunteers (Krystal et al., 1994), as well as exacerbating existing symptoms in patients (Lahti et al., 1995), linked to its afnity to the NMDA receptor (Javitt and Zukin, 1991). Ketamine may therefore provide a more comprehensive model of psychosis than amphetamine, which elicits only the positive symptoms of psychosis. Ketamine also produces disruption of cognitive function similar to that observed in schizophrenia, including impairments in working memory (Adler et al., 1998; Honey et al., 2003c, 2004). Honey et al. (2003b) demonstrated that impairment of working memory results specically from a disruption of manipulating the contents of working memory, whereas the requirement to simply maintain items online was not affected. This dichotomy serves to support the theoretical dissociation between rehearsal processes required to maintain information in storage, and executive processes such as updating, manipulating and monitoring. In this study, analogous measures of visuo-spatial working memory were not measurably impaired, tentatively providing some indication of domain-specicity (Honey et al., 2003b). This pattern of ndings provided some support for the proposed anatomical dissociation of function between the dorsolateral region of the prefrontal cortex, typically engaged in association with the executive component of working memory tasks, compared with the association between the ventrolateral region and maintenance processes (DEsposito et al., 1999; Fletcher and Henson, 2001). In a subsequent study, it was further shown that ketamine augments fronto-parietal activation in response to manipulation compared with maintenance processes (Honey et al., 2004). These ndings suggest that manipulation and maintenance processes, envisaged as separate component processes within current theoretical models of working memory, are both anatomically and pharmacologically dissociable.

CONCLUSIONS
The extrapolation of observations from studies in schizophrenia to contribute to understanding of the processes

68

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71


Berman KF, Doran AR, Pickar D, Weinberger DR (1993) Is the mechanism of prefrontal hypofunction in depression the same as in schizophrenia? Regional cerebral blood ow during cognitive activation. Br J Psychiatry 162:183192. Broca P (1861) Nouvelle observation daphmie produite par une lsion de la moiti postrieure des deuxime et troisime circonvolution frontales gauches. Bulletin de la Socit Anatomique 36:398 407. Buchel C, Friston KJ (1997) Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling and fMRI. Cereb Cortex 7:768 778. Buchsbaum MS, Wu J, DeLisi LE, Holcomb H, Kessler R, Johnson J, King AC, Hazlett E, Langston K, Post RM (1986) Frontal cortex and basal ganglia metabolic rates assessed by positron emission tomography with [18F]2-deoxyglucose in affective illness. J Affect Disord 10:137152. Bullmore E, Horwitz B, Honey G, Brammer M, Williams S, Sharma T (2000) How good is good enough in path analysis of fMRI data? Neuroimage 11:289 301. Bullmore ET, Frangou S, Murray RM (1997) The dysplastic net hypothesis: an integration of developmental and dysconnectivity theories of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 28:143156. Bunney WE, Bunney BG (2000) Evidence for a compromised dorsolateral prefrontal cortical parallel circuit in schizophrenia. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 31:138 146. Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJ, Duyn J, Coppola R, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (2000) Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex 10:1078 1092. Callicott JH, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Bone AD, Verchinksi B, Weinberger DR (2003a) Abnormal fMRI response of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitively intact siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160:709 719. Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Verchinksi BA, Marenco S, Egan MF, Weinberger DR (2003b) Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: More than up or down. Am J Psychiatry 160:2209 2215. Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Finn K, Coppola R, Frank JA, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (1999) Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 9:20 26. Callicott JH, Ramsey NF, Tallent K, Bertolino A, Knable MB, Coppola R, Goldberg T, van Gelderen P, Mattay VS, Frank JA, Moonen CT, Weinberger DR (1998) Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain mapping in psychiatry: methodological issues illustrated in a study of working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 18:186 196. Carlsson A, Lindqvist M (1963) Effect of chlorpromazine or haloperidol on formation of 3 methoxytyramine and normetanephrine in mouse brain. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol (Copenh) 20:140 144. Carter CS, Perlstein W, Ganguli R, Brar J, Mintun M, Cohen JD (1998) Functional hypofrontality and working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 155:12851287. Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D (1992) Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol Rev 99:4577. Conklin H, Curtis C, Katsanis J, Lacono W (2000) Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their rst-degree relatives: evidence from the digit span task. Am J Psychiatry 157:275277. Creese I, Burt D, Snyder S (1976) Dopamine receptor binding predicts clinical and pharmacological potencies of anti-schizophrenic drugs. Science 192:481 483. Daniel DG, Berman KF, Weinberger DR (1989) The effect of apomorphine on regional cerebral blood ow in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1:377384.

underlying working memory in the normal brain must be made with some caution, and with appropriate caveats given the blurred taxonomic boundaries of the illness, and variability of the effects of the disease and its treatment on brain function. To date, this contribution is limited to providing convergent evidence for observations made in healthy volunteers. This is a modest, though important contribution. As our understanding increases of the neurobiological mechanisms which give rise to the cognitive decits in schizophrenia, or the sub-syndromes which comprise it, we may see this information serving to generate testable hypotheses regarding normal brain function underlying working memory. Alternative approaches which index psychosis indirectly may provide an effective solution to some of these issues. These approaches include the study of susceptible populations, such as non-affected familial relatives of psychotic patients, or as we have explored in this review, the use of psychopharmacological models of the illness. The application of these techniques is likely to facilitate greater understanding of both schizophrenia and the biological basis of working memory (dys) function.

REFERENCES
Adler CM, Goldberg TE, Malhotra AK, Pickar D, Breier A (1998) Effects of ketamine on thought disorder, working memory, and semantic memory in healthy volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 43: 811 816. Andreasen NC (1987) The diagnosis of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 13:9 22. Andreasen NC, Rezai K, Alliger R, Swayze IV, Flaum M, Kirchner P, Cohen G, OLearny D (1992) Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-nave patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:943958. Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM (1968) Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In: The psychology of learning and motivation (Spence KW, Spence JT, eds), pp 89 195. New York: Academic Press. Baddeley A (1986) Working memory. Oxford: Claredon Press. Baddeley A (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4:417 423. Baddeley A, Hitch G (1974) Working memory. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 8 (Bower G, ed), pp 4790. New York: Academic Press. Baddeley AD, Wilson B (1985) Phonological coding and short-term memory in patients without speech. J Mem Lang 24:490 502. Barch DM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Forman SD, Noll DC, Cohen JD (1997) Dissociating working memory from task difculty in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 35:13731380. Barch DM, Carter CS, Braver TS, Sabb FW, MacDonald A 3rd, Noll DC, Cohen JD (2001) Selective decits in prefrontal cortex function in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:280 288. Barch DM, Csernansky JG, Conturo T, Snyder AZ (2002) Working and long-term memory decits in schizophrenia: is there a common prefrontal mechanism? J Abnorm Psychol 111:478 494. Barch DM, Sheline YI, Csernansky JG, Snyder AZ (2003) Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: specicity to schizophrenia compared with major depression. Biol Psychiatry 53: 376 384. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Adolphs R, Rockland C, Damasio AR (1995) Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science 269:11151118.

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71


DEsposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J (1999) Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Cogn 41:66 86. DEsposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B (2000) Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res 133:311. Dunn JC, Kirsner K (1988) Discovering functionally independent mental processes: the principle of reversed association. Psychol Rev 95:91101. Ebmeier KP, Lawrie SM, Blackwood DH, Johnstone EC, Goodwin GM (1995) Hypofrontality revisited: a high resolution single photon emission computed tomography study in schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 58:452 456. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, Goldman D, Weinberger DR (2001) Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:6917 6922. Fiez JA (2001) Bridging the gap between neuroimaging and neuropsychology: using working memory as a case-study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 23:19 31. Fleming K, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Weinberger DR (1995) Verbal working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: use of a BrownPeterson paradigm. Psychiatry Res 56:155161. Fletcher P, McKenna PJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Dolan RJ (1999) Abnormal cingulate modulation of fronto-temporal connectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 9:337342. Fletcher PC, Henson RN (2001) Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain 124:849 881. Fletcher PC, McKenna PJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ (1998) Brain activations in schizophrenia during a graded memory task studied with functional neuroimaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:10011008. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Whiteld S, Faustman WO, Roth WT (2002) Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 51:485 492. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ (1997) Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 6:218 229. Friston KJ, Frith CD (1995) Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci 3:89 97. Frith CD, Friston KJ, Herold S, Silbersweig D, Fletcher P, Cahill C, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Liddle PF (1995) Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during the performance of a verbal uency task. Br J Psychiatry 167:343349. Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS (1993) Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic scotomas. J Neurosci 13:1479 1497. Goldberg TE, Berman KF, Fleming K, Ostrem J, Van Horn JD, Esposito G, Mattay VS, Gold JM, Weinberger DR (1998) Uncoupling cognitive workload and prefrontal cortical physiology: a PET rCBF study. Neuroimage 7:296 303. Goldman-Rakic P (1987) Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behaviour by representational memory. In: Handbook of physiology: the nervous system, Vol. 5 (Plum F, ed), pp 373417. Bethesda: American Physiological Society. Goldman-Rakic P (1990a) Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: the relevance of working memory. In: Psychopathology and the brain (Carroll B, Bartrett J, eds), pp 123. New York: Raven Press. Goldman-Rakic PS (1990b) Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. Prog Brain Res 85:325335; discussion 335336. Goldman-Rakic PS (1994) Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6:348 357. Goldman-Rakic PS (1999) The physiological approach: functional architecture of working memory and disordered cognition in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 46:650 661.

69

Green MF (1996) What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive decits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 153:321330. Green MF, Marshall BD Jr, Wirshing WC, Ames D, Marder SR, McGurk S, Kern RS, Mintz J (1997) Does risperidone improve verbal working memory in treatment-resistant schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 154:799 804. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH (1999) Should schizophrenia be treated as a neurocognitive disorder? Schizophr Bull 25:309 319. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, Barch DM, Cohen J, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese F, Goldberg TE, Heaton RK, Keefe RS, Kern RS, Kraemer H, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Zalcman S, Marder SR (2004) Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biol Psychiatry 56:301307. Harvey PD, Green MF, McGurk SR, Meltzer HY (2003) Changes in cognitive functioning with risperidone and olanzapine treatment: a large-scale, double-blind, randomized study. Psychopharmacology 18:18 Henson R (2005) What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist. Q J Exp Psychol 58:193233. Hertel P, Nomikos GG, Iurlo M, Svensson TH (1996) Risperidone: regional effects in vivo on release and metabolism of dopamine and serotonin in the rat brain. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 124: 74 86. Honey G, Bullmore E (2004) Human pharmacological MRI. Trends Pharmacol Sci 25:366 374. Honey GD, Bullmore ET, Sharma T (2000) Prolonged reaction time to a verbal working memory task predicts increased power of posterior parietal cortical activation. Neuroimage 12:495503. Honey GD, Bullmore ET, Sharma T (2002a) De-coupling of cognitive performance and cerebral functional response during working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 53:4556. Honey GD, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET (2002b) Functional brain mapping of psychopathology. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72: 432 439. Honey GD, Fu CH, Kim J, Brammer MJ, Croudace TJ, Suckling J, Pich EM, Williams SC, Bullmore ET (2002c) Effects of verbal working memory load on corticocortical connectivity modeled by path analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Neuroimage 17:573582. Honey GD, Bullmore ET, Soni W, Varatheesan M, Williams SC, Sharma T (1999) Differences in frontal cortical activation by a working memory task after substitution of risperidone for typical antipsychotic drugs in patients with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:1343213437. Honey GD, Sharma T, Suckling J, Giampietro V, Soni W, Williams SC, Bullmore ET (2003a) The functional neuroanatomy of schizophrenic subsyndromes. Psychol Med 33:10071018. Honey GD, Suckling J, Zelaya F, Long C, Routledge C, Jackson S, Ng V, Fletcher PC, Williams SC, Brown J, Bullmore ET (2003b) Dopaminergic drug effects on physiological connectivity in a human cortico-striato-thalamic system. Brain 126:17671781. Honey RA, Turner DC, Honey GD, Sharar SR, Kumaran D, PomarolClotet E, McKenna P, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Fletcher PC (2003c) Subdissociative dose ketamine produces a decit in manipulation but not maintenance of the contents of working memory. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:30. Honey RA, Honey GD, OLoughlin C, Sharar SR, Kumaran D, Bullmore ET, Menon DK, Donovan T, Lupson VC, Bisbrown-Chippendale R, Fletcher PC (2004) Acute ketamine administration alters the brain responses to executive demands in a verbal working memory task: an FMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 12031214. Hugdahl K, Rund BR, Lund A, Asbjornsen A, Egeland J, Ersland L, Landro NI, Roness A, Stordal KI, Sundet K, Thomsen T (2004) Brain activation measured with fMRI during a mental arithmetic task in schizophrenia and major depression. Am J Psychiatry 161:286 293.

70

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71


the brain response to amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:6186 6191. Matthysse S (1973) Antipsychotic drug actions: a clue to the neuropathology of schizophrenia? Fed Proc 32:200 205. McGrath J, Chapple B, Wright M (2001) Working memory in schizophrenia and mania: correlation with symptoms during the acute and subacute phases. Acta Psychiatr Scand 103:181188. McIntosh AR, Grady CL, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV, Rapoport SI, Horwitz B (1994) Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET. J Neurosci 14:655 666. Meltzer HY, McGurk SR (1999) The effects of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 25:233255. Mendrek A, Laurens KR, Kiehl KA, Ngan ET, Stip E, Liddle PF (2004) Changes in distributed neural circuitry function in patients with rst-episode schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 185:205214. Menon V, Anagnoson RT, Mathalon DH, Glover GH, Pfefferbaum A (2001) Functional neuroanatomy of auditory working memory in schizophrenia: relation to positive and negative symptoms. Neuroimage 13:433 446. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, Holt JL, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, Berman KF (2001) Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 158:1809 1817. Morris SK, Granholm E, Sarkin AJ, Jeste DV (1997) Effects of schizophrenia and aging on pupillographic measures of working memory. Schizophr Res 27:119 128. Mottaghy FM, Doring T, Muller-Gartner HW, Topper R, Krause BJ (2002) Bilateral parieto-frontal network for verbal working memory: an interference approach using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Eur J Neurosci 16:16271632. Mottaghy FM, Krause BJ, Kemna LJ, Topper R, Tellmann L, Beu M, Pascual-Leone A, Muller-Gartner HW (2000) Modulation of the neuronal circuitry subserving working memory in healthy human subjects by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurosci Lett 280:167170. Mull BR, Seyal M (2001) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of left prefrontal cortex impairs working memory. Clin Neurophysiol 112:16721675. Murray EA, Bussey TJ, Wise SP (2000) Role of prefrontal cortex in a network for arbitrary visuomotor mapping. Exp Brain Res 133: 114 129. Nahas Z, George MS, Horner MD, Markowitz JS, Li X, Lorberbaum JP, Owens SD, McGurk S, DeVane L, Risch SC (2003) Augmenting atypical antipsychotics with a cognitive enhancer (donepezil) improves regional brain activity in schizophrenia patients: a pilot double-blind placebo controlled BOLD fMRI study. Neurocase 9:274 282. Nixon P, Lazarova J, Hodinott-Hill I, Gough P, Passingham R (2004) The inferior frontal gyrus and phonological processing: an investigation using rTMS. J Cogn Neurosci 16:289 300. Oliveri M, Turriziani P, Carlesimo GA, Koch G, Tomaiuolo F, Panella M, Caltagirone C (2001) Parieto-frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex 11:606 618. Owen AM (1997) The functional organization of working memory processes within human lateral frontal cortex: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Eur J Neurosci 9:1329 1339. Owen AM (2000) The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Exp Brain Res 133:33 43. Park S, Holzman PS (1992) Schizophrenics show spatial working memory decits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:975982. Park S, McTigue K (1997) Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res 26:213220. Park S, Puschel J, Sauter BH, Rentsch M, Hell D (1999) Spatial working memory decits and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia: a 4-month follow-up study. Biol Psychiatry 46:392 400.

Jacobsen LK, DSouza DC, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Skudlarski P, Krystal JH (2004) Nicotine effects on brain function and functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 55:850 858. Javitt DC, Zukin SR (1991) Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 148:13011308. Kapur S, Mamo D (2003) Half a century of antipsychotics and still a central role for dopamine D2 receptors. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 27:10811090. Keefe RS, Roitman SE, Harvey PD, Blum CS, DuPre RL, Prieto DM, Davidson M, Davis KL (1995) A pen-and-paper human analogue of a monkey prefrontal cortex activation task: spatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 17:2533. Kerwin RW (1994) The new atypical antipsychotics. A lack of extrapyramidal side-effects and new routes in schizophrenia research. Br J Psychiatry 164:141148. Kim JJ, Kwon JS, Park HJ, Youn T, Kang Do H, Kim MS, Lee DS, Lee MC (2003) Functional disconnection between the prefrontal and parietal cortices during working memory processing in schizophrenia: a [15 (O)]H2O PET study. Am J Psychiatry 160:919923. Kindermann SS, Brown GG, Zorrilla LE, Olsen RK, Jeste DV (2004) Spatial working memory among middle-aged and older patients with schizophrenia and volunteers using fMRI. Schizophr Res 68:203216. Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner JD, Heninger GR, Bowers MB Jr, Charney DS (1994) Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:199 214. Lahti AC, Koffel B, LaPorte D, Tamminga CA (1995) Subanesthetic doses of ketamine stimulate psychosis in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 13:9 19. Landau SM, Schumacher EH, Garavan H, Druzgal TJ, DEsposito M (2004) A functional MRI study of the inuence of practice on component processes of working memory. Neuroimage 22: 211221. Lawrie SM, Buechel C, Whalley HC, Frith CD, Friston KJ, Johnstone EC (2002) Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucinations. Biol Psychiatry 51:1008 1011. Li ZH, Sun XW, Wang ZX, Zhang XC, Zhang Da R, He S, Hu XP (2004) Behavioral and functional MRI study of attention shift in human verbal working memory. Neuroimage 21:181191. Luby ED, Cohen BD, Rosenbaum G, Gottlieb JS, Kelley R (1959) Study of a new schizophrenicomimetic drug: serenyl. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 71:363369. Manoach DS (2003) Prefrontal cortex dysfunction during working memory performance in schizophrenia: reconciling discrepant ndings. Schizophr Res 60:285298. Manoach DS, Gollub RL, Benson ES, Searl MM, Goff DC, Halpern E, Saper CB, Rauch SL (2000) Schizophrenic subjects show aberrant fMRI activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during working memory performance. Biol Psychiatry 48:99 109. Manoach DS, Halpern EF, Kramer TS, Chang Y, Goff DC, Rauch SL, Kennedy DN, Gollub RL (2001) Test-retest reliability of a functional MRI working memory paradigm in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Am J Psychiatry 158:955958. Manoach D-S, Press D-Z, Thangaraj V, Searl M-M, Goff D-C, Halpern E, Saper C-B, Warach S (1999) Schizophrenic subjects activate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task, as measured by fMRI. Biol Psychiatry 45:1128 1137. Mattay VS, Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Heaton I, Frank JA, Coppola R, Berman KF, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (2000) Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive performance and cortical activation. Neuroimage 12:268 275. Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Egan MF, Kolachana B, Callicott JH, Weinberger DR (2003) Catechol Omethyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in

G. D. Honey and P. C. Fletcher / Neuroscience 139 (2006) 59 71


Paulman RG, Devous MD Sr., Gregory RR, Herman JH, Jennings L, Bonte FJ, Nasrallah HA, Raese JD (1990) Hypofrontality and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: dynamic single-photon tomography and neuropsychological assessment of schizophrenic brain function. Biol Psychiatry 27:377399. Pelosi L, Slade T, Blumhardt LD, Sharma VK (2000) Working memory dysfunction in major depression: An event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiol 111:15311543. Perlstein WM, Carter CS, Noll DC, Cohen JD (2001) Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 158:11051113. Perlstein WM, Dixit NK, Carter CS, Noll DC, Cohen JD (2003) Prefrontal cortex dysfunction mediates decits in working memory and prepotent responding in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 53:2538. Price CJ, Friston KJ (1999) Scanning patients with tasks they can perform. Hum Brain Mapp 8:102108. Quintana J, Wong T, Ortiz-Portillo E, Kovalik E, Davidson T, Marder SR, Mazziotta JC (2003) Prefrontal-posterior parietal networks in schizophrenia: primary dysfunctions and secondary compensations. Biol Psychiatry 53:1224. Renshaw PF, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Cohen BM (1994) Greater hemodynamic response to photic stimulation in schizophrenic patients: an echo planar MRI study. Am J Psychiatry 151:14931495. Rorden C, Karnath HO (2004) Opinion: Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nat Rev Neurosci 5:812 819. Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1999) Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage 9:216 226. Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS (1991) D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: involvement in working memory. Science 251: 947950. Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS (1994) The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol 71:515528. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Mozley PD, Mozley LH, Resnick SM, Kester DB, Staniak P (1991) Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48:618 624. Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, Kester DB, Mozley LH, Staniak P, Gur RC (1994) Neuropsychological decits in neuroleptic naive patients with rst-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:124 131. Schlosser R, Gesierich T, Kaufmann B, Vucurevic G, Hunsche S, Gawehn J, Stoeter P (2003a) Altered effective connectivity during working memory performance in schizophrenia: a study with fMRI and structural equation modeling. Neuroimage 19:751763. Schlosser R, Gesierich T, Kaufmann B, Vucurevic G, Stoeter P (2003b) Altered effective connectivity in drug free schizophrenic patients. Neuroreport 14:22332237. Seeman P, Lee T, Chau-Wong M, Wong K (1976) Antipsychotic drug doses and neuroleptic/dopamine receptors. Nature 261:717719. Shah YB, Marsden CA (2004) The application of functional magnetic resonance imaging to neuropharmacology. Curr Opin Pharmacol 4:517521. Shallice T, Burgess PW, Frith CD (1991) Can the neuropsychological case-study approach be applied to schizophrenia? Psychol Med 21:661 673. Spindler KA, Sullivan EV, Menon V, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A (1997) Decits in multiple systems of working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 27:110.

71

Stephan KE, Magnotta VA, White T, Arndt S, Flaum M, OLeary DS, Andreasen NC (2001) Effects of olanzapine on cerebellar functional connectivity in schizophrenia measured by fMRI during a simple motor task. Psychol Med 31:10651078. Sternberg S (1966) High-speed scanning in human memory. Science 153:652 654. Stevens AA, Goldman-Rakic PS, Gore JC, Fulbright RK, Wexler BE (1998) Cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia during auditory word and tone working memory demonstrated by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:10971103. Strange PG (2001) Antipsychotic drugs: importance of dopamine receptors for mechanisms of therapeutic actions and side effects. Pharmacol Rev 53:119 133. Strauss JS, Carpenter WT Jr, Bartko JJ (1974) The diagnosis and understanding of schizophrenia. Part III. Speculations on the processes that underlie schizophrenic symptoms and signs. Schizophr Bull 11:61 69. Sweeney JA, Strojwas MH, Mann JJ, Thase ME (1998) Prefrontal and cerebellar abnormalities in major depression: evidence from oculomotor studies. Biol Psychiatry 43:584 594. Thermenos HW, Goldstein JM, Buka SL, Poldrack RA, Koch JK, Tsuang MT, Seidman LJ (2005) The effect of working memory performance on functional MRI in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 74:179 194. Vallar G, Baddeley AD (1984) Fractionation of working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a short-term store, phonlogical processing, and sentence comprehension: A neuropsychological case study. Cogn Neuropsychol 1:121141. Wager TD, Smith EE (2003) Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3:255274. Walter H, Wunderlich AP, Blankenhorn M, Schafer S, Tomczak R, Spitzer M, Gron G (2003) No hypofrontality, but absence of prefrontal lateralization comparing verbal and spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 61:175184. Weinberger DR (1993) A connectionist approach to the prefrontal cortex. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 5:241253. Weinberger DR, Berman KF (1988) Speculation on the meaning of cerebral metabolic hypofrontality in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 14:157168. Weinberger DR, Mattay V, Callicott J, Kotrla K, Santha A, van Gelderen P, Duyn J, Moonen C, Frank J (1996) fMRI applications in schizophrenia research. Neuroimage 4:S118 S126. Weinberger E, Cermak LS (1973) Short-term retention in acute and chronic paranoid schizophrenics. J Abnorm Psychol 82:220 225. Wenz F, Schad LR, Knopp MV, Baudendistel KT, Flomer F, Schroder J, van Kaick G (1994) Functional magnetic resonance imaging at 1.5 T: activation pattern in schizophrenic patients receiving neuroleptic medication. Magn Reson Imaging 12:975982. Wernicke K (1874) Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslau, M. Crohn und Weigert. Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS (1995) Modulation of memory elds by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 376: 572575. Winterer G, Coppola R, Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (2003) Functional and effective frontotemporal connectivity and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 54:11811192. Wykes T, Brammer M, Mellers J, Bray P, Reeder C, Williams C, Corner J (2002) Effects on the brain of a psychological treatment: cognitive remediation therapy: functional magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 181:144 152.

(Accepted 27 May 2005) (Available online 15 December 2005)

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi