Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 16

Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT

Interference by process, not content, determines semantic


auditory distraction
John E. Marsh *, Robert W. Hughes, Dylan M. Jones
School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales CF10 3AT, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Distraction by irrelevant background sound of visually-based cognitive tasks illustrates the
Received 27 April 2007 vulnerability of attentional selectivity across modalities. Four experiments centred on
Revised 19 August 2008 auditory distraction during tests of memory for visually-presented semantic information.
Accepted 20 August 2008
Meaningful irrelevant speech disrupted the free recall of semantic category-exemplars
more than meaningless irrelevant sound (Experiment 1). This effect was exacerbated when
the irrelevant speech was semantically related to the to-be-remembered material (Exper-
Keywords:
iment 2). Importantly, however, these effects of meaningfulness and semantic relatedness
Auditory distraction
Semantic interference
were shown to arise only when instructions emphasized recall by category rather than by
Selective attention serial order (Experiments 3 and 4). The results favor a process-oriented, rather than a struc-
Interference-by-process tural, approach to the breakdown of attentional selectivity and forgetting: performance is
Semantic-category clustering impaired by the similarity of process brought to bear on the relevant and irrelevant mate-
rial, not the similarity in item content.
Ó 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction processes (such as inhibition; e.g., Houghton & Tipper,


1994) that are designed to resolve conflict during the
One of the most influential constructs in memory re- selection of candidates at retrieval (e.g., Anderson, 2003).
search is interference: the ease with which items are re- Set within this quintessentially attentional approach to
trieved from memory is dictated, at least in part, by other forgetting, the present article explores the nature of
stimuli or events that are similar in some way to the target phenomena relating to impaired retrieval from memory
(see, e.g., Anderson, 2003; Baddeley, 1986; McGeoch, due to distraction from irrelevant auditory events using
1942; Nairne, 1990; Nairne, 2002; Neath, 2000). The the structuralist, interference-by-similarity-of-content,
classical, structuralist, view has been that such interfer- approach as a theoretical counterpoint.
ence-by-similarity-of-content directly causes forgetting, One line of research in which a dynamic selective atten-
that is, forgetting is a passive side-effect of structural tion framework has been used to reconstrue putatively
changes that result from the storing of new, similar, events mnemonic phenomena is that concerned with the disrup-
in memory (Anderson, 1983; Cowan, 1999; McGeoch, tive effects of to-be-ignored sound on visual-verbal serial
1942; Mensink & Raaijmakers, 1988; Oberauer & Lange, recall whereby a list of around 6–8 verbal items (e.g., let-
2008; Oberauer, Lange, & Engle, 2004; Salamé & Baddeley, ters or digits) is to be recalled in strict serial order (the
1982). However, an alternative, more functional, view is irrelevant sound effect—hereafter ISE—e.g., Colle & Welsh,
that ‘forgetting’ (or the impairment of retrieval) reflects 1976; Jones, Madden, & Miles, 1992; Jones & Tremblay,
the legacy of dynamic and adaptive selective attention 2000; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982). The mere presence of
background sound depresses serial recall appreciably, the
weight of evidence favoring the view that the effect results
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 29 20 876788; fax: +44 029 20
874858. from interference-by-process, and is not a passive side-
E-mail address: marshje@cardiff.ac.uk (J.E. Marsh). effect of having similar items to remember and to ignore

0010-0277/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.003
24 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

(Hughes & Jones, 2005; Jones & Tremblay, 2000). Specifi- confirming the predictions of an early account based on
cally, this ‘classical’ ISE is thought to result from the oblig- the concept of a phonological store (Gathercole & Badde-
atory, preattentive, seriation (or ordering) of sound ley, 1993; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982). As a result of these
sequences producing competition for the deliberate pro- findings, the phonological store account of the ISE has been
cess of seriating the to-be-remembered items. Here we modified and expressed computationally such that irrele-
examine whether the principle of interference-by-process vant speech disrupts a representation of order within the
can be extended to a setting in which the focal memory passive store rather than interfering with item representa-
task involves not serial processing but semantic retrieval tions (Norris, Baddeley, & Page, 2004; Page & Norris, 2003).
strategies: Does the concurrence of similar semantic pro- However, problematic for any account that views irrele-
cessing (rather than serial processing) applied to relevant vant speech as disrupting the phonological store is recent
and irrelevant material now dictate the form and degree evidence showing that rehearsal is a precondition for its
of distraction? What little evidence there is seems to sug- expression (Jones, Macken, & Nicholls, 2004).
gest that the structural accounts seem perfectly adequate Third, the interference-by-content approach fails to
in this context, that is, disruption from irrelevant sound acknowledge adequately the critical importance of the nat-
in semantic memory seems amenable to a classical, and ure of focal task processing, the impairment of recall being
arguably simpler, ‘interference-by-content’, explanation chiefly determined by the co-existence of similar to-be-re-
(Beaman, 2004; Neely & LeCompte, 1999) rendering the called and to-be-ignored items within a store. That is, they
‘interference-by-process’ account rather paradigm-bound cannot account for why the ISE is only found if the focal
to serial short-term memory. The goals of the present ser- task necessitates or tends to encourage a seriation process
ies were to revisit the empirical signature of auditory dis- (e.g., serial rehearsal) and why the mere presence of simi-
traction in the context of episodic short-term memory lar content between the memory material and the sound is
tasks that tap semantic memory processes (particularly gi- not sufficient (or necessary) for the effect (Beaman & Jones,
ven the paucity of studies on the issue) to establish the de- 1997; Farley, Neath, Allbritton, & Surprenant, 2007;
gree to which it is distinct from that found in serial recall, Henson, Hartley, Burgess, Hitch, & Flude, 2003; Hughes,
and to examine thereafter how such distinct phenomena Vachon, & Jones, 2007; Perham, Banbury, & Jones, 2007).
might be reconciled with a dynamic process-oriented ap- Whilst the preoccupation of the interference-by-con-
proach to interference. tent approach is with item identity, on the interference-
by-process account, the key determinant of the disruption
1.1. Irrelevant sound effect in serial recall in serial recall is the extent to which both the irrelevant
sound and the focal memory task share similar seriation
The debate between the structuralist and process-based (or ordering) processes (Jones, 1993; Jones & Tremblay,
standpoints can be observed in microcosm in a body of re- 2000). A key observation underpinning this account is
search showing that the presence of irrelevant, to-be-ig- the changing-state effect (e.g., Jones et al., 1992) whereby
nored, sound markedly increases forgetting in a (usually a sound sequence—regardless of whether it comprises
visually-presented) serial recall task (e.g., Colle & Welsh, speech or non-speech—that exhibits abrupt changes in
1976; Jones et al., 1992; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982). The acoustic properties (e.g., ‘‘k v h q. . .”, or a sequence of tones
conventional viewpoint, that forgetting can occur as a di- changing in frequency) is invariably more disruptive than a
rect and passive consequence of the structural similarity continuous or repeating stimulus (e.g., ‘‘k k k k. . .”, or a re-
between to-be-remembered and irrelevant episodes or peated tone). On the interference-by-process account it is
stimuli (e.g., McGeoch, 1942), is evident in several theoret- assumed that the preattentive perception of acoustic
ical accounts of the ISE that view it as a mere consequence changes between segmentable elements in the sound
of auditory stimuli gaining access to the same representa- yields cues as to the order of those elements as a by-prod-
tional space as the to-be-remembered items (e.g., phono- uct of primitive, acoustic-based, perceptual organization
logical store, Burgess & Hitch, 1992; Gathercole & processes (cf. Bregman, 1990). These irrelevant order cues
Baddeley, 1993; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982; primary mem- compete for—and hence impair—the deliberate seriation
ory, Neath, 2000). Although these accounts differ in their process (serial rehearsal) supporting ordered recall of the
detail of how interference arises, the important point for to-be-remembered items (Hughes & Jones, 2005; Jones,
present purposes is that they are all examples of an inter- 1993). In support of this view, the ability to encode the or-
ference-by-content approach: recall is impaired as a result der of stimuli in an attended changing-state auditory se-
of the similarity in identity (i.e., content) between to-be- quence predicts the degree to which that sequence is
remembered and to-be-ignored items. disruptive when presented as irrelevant sound during seri-
Several strands of evidence converge to weaken the al recall (Macken, Phelps, & Jones, in press).
interference-by-content approach. First, non-speech In sum, results based on research using the serial recall
sounds such as tones—which bear little or no resemblance paradigm favor a dynamic process-based approach (Jones
to the to-be-remembered items—produce disruption simi- & Tremblay, 2000). However, the phenomenon of interfer-
lar in degree and kind to that from irrelevant speech (e.g., ence-by-process seems highly specific to a particular pro-
Jones & Macken, 1993; Neath & Surprenant, 2001). Second, cess (seriation) and little evidence is available with
the magnitude of disruption is unrelated to the degree of respect to whether such conflict occurs between other
phonological similarity between to-be-remembered and types of processes. In the present study, therefore, we ad-
to-be-ignored items (Jones & Macken, 1995; LeCompte & dressed whether the phenomenon extends to auditory dis-
Shaibe, 1997; but see Hughes & Jones, 2005) thereby dis- traction in the context of a focal task that is likely to be
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 25

dominated by semantic-based, rather than seriation, pro- amenable to, and indeed be better explained by, an inter-
cesses. This is a particularly pertinent issue in light of the ference-by-process analysis (cf. Marsh, Hughes, & Jones,
fact that the little evidence there is on semantic auditory submitted for publication, 2008). The starting point for this
distraction effects suggests that the interference-by-con- analysis is that, compared to serial recall, the semantic
tent approach offers a perfectly adequate explanation. richness of the items in category-exemplar recall, as well
as the longer list-length, demotes the likelihood of a seria-
1.2. Semantic auditory distraction: interference-by-content? tion strategy and instead promotes the use of semantic-
based organization processes. It is this shift in the nature
On the interference-by-process account of auditory dis- of the dominant process/strategy used to support perfor-
traction in serial recall, it is the processing of the precate- mance in the focal task, not simply the semantic richness
gorical, acoustic, attributes of the sound that is key to the of the to-be-recalled items, that renders such tasks vulner-
disruption (e.g., Jones & Macken, 1993). Consistent with able to competition from the processing of the semantic
this view, neither the lexical-semantic content of the irrel- content of the irrelevant sound. Thus, in contrast to the
evant sound (when speech is used) nor the similarity in interference-by-content approach, it is not the mere co-
terms of semantic content between the speech and the activation or co-registration of relevant and irrelevant
to-be-remembered list has any bearing on the magnitude semantic representations that impairs performance.
of disruption (Buchner, Irmen, & Erdfelder, 1996; Jones, Rather, it is the integrity of dynamic, semantic-based, orga-
Miles, & Page, 1990; LeCompte, Neely, & Wilson, 1997; nizational processes engaged in support of retrieval that is
Surprenant, Neath, & Bireta, 2007; but see Buchner, Roth- compromised by the semantic processing of the irrelevant
ermund, Wentura, & Mehl, 2004). Such findings are easily sound. In this way, semantic auditory distraction may be
explicable on the intereference-by-process account: The regarded as an extension of the general case of interfer-
key process supporting serial recall is an articulatory- ence-by-process: It may reflect a difficulty in selecting
based seriation process, not a semantically-based one amongst two sets of semantic representations both of
(e.g., Jones et al., 2004). Thus, the lexical-semantic attri- which represent plausible candidates for populating the
butes of irrelevant speech would not be expected to con- semantic-organizational skill used in the focal task, just
flict with the focal seriation process. However, the as with two sets of serial representations in the classical
absence of such lexical-semantic effects in the context of irrelevant sound effect.
serial recall can also be accommodated within the interfer- In the studies that follow, we scrutinize further the nat-
ence-by-content approach. In the typical serial recall task, ure of semantic auditory distraction and attempt to clarify
the to-be-recalled items (e.g., digits, letters) are relatively the extent to which semantic-organizational processing in
impoverished in terms of semantic content. Thus, the rep- the focal task—as opposed to the mere semantic richness of
resentations of to-be-recalled items—devoid of rich seman- the to-be-recalled items—is responsible for disruption by
tic content—may not be susceptible to degradation or the lexical-semantic attributes of irrelevant speech. We
retrieval-confusion as a result of activated semantic repre- use a setting in which a relatively long list of 32 exemplars
sentations of the irrelevant speech items. (e.g., ‘‘strawberry”, ‘‘pigeon”, etc.) drawn from a smaller set
In line with the interference-by-content approach, the of 4 semantic categories (e.g., ‘‘Fruit”, ‘‘Birds”, etc.) are pre-
results of a small number of studies suggest that when sented for recall. It is well established that under such con-
the items are semantically rich, recall is indeed impaired ditions participants tend at test to cluster the randomly
by the semantic attributes of the irrelevant sound presented exemplars according to their category at a great-
(Beaman, 2004; Jones et al., 1990; Martin, Wogalter, & For- er-than-chance level even without instruction to do so
lano, 1988; Neely & LeCompte, 1999; Oswald, Tremblay, & (Bousfield, 1953; Smith, Jones, & Broadbent, 1981). This
Jones, 2000). For example, in a category-exemplar recall semantic category-clustering (henceforth termed ‘‘seman-
task, in which a list of, say, 16 semantically-rich items tic-categorization”) implies secondary organization where-
(nouns) taken from a single semantic category are by participants bring to bear pre-existing conceptual
presented for free recall, the semantic similarity between relationships or semantic associations to guide encoding
the relevant and irrelevant items impairs performance and retrieval of episodic information which is distinct from
(Beaman, 2004; Neely & LeCompte, 1999). The free recall primary organization whereby the organization corre-
of relatively low-dominance category-exemplars (e.g., sponds to the serial order of the list (Tulving, 1968).
‘‘avocado”) is disrupted (as reflected in omission errors) In another study (Marsh et al., submitted for publica-
more by related, high-dominance, irrelevant category- tion, 2008), we have used lists of words drawn from a sin-
items (that are not included in the to-be-remembered list; gle semantic category to investigate the role that source-
e.g., ‘‘apple”) than by high-dominance, categorically-unre- monitoring processes play in governing the false recall of
lated, irrelevant items (e.g., ‘‘hammer”). Such results seem items presented as irrelevant sound. Single category lists
to be readily explained within an interference-by-content being ‘blocked’ by semantic category were ideally suited
approach: The semantic representations of the to-be-re- to this purpose because this method of list presentation in-
called items may be degraded or otherwise made less creases false recall (Brainerd, Payne, Wright, & Reyna,
accessible as a function of their semantic similarity to the 2003). The research reported here, though conceptually
irrelevant items (e.g., Anderson, 1983; Oberauer & Lange, similar, follows a distinct empirical line. In the following
2008; Oberauer et al., 2004; Rundus, 1973). experiments, we are not interested in false recall (indeed
However, our central contention in this paper is that random presentation of exemplars drawn from several
these semantic auditory distraction effects may also be semantic categories attenuates false recall), but rather in
26 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

how meaningful irrelevant sound may impair the recruit- narrative with that of the same narrative read in a lan-
ment of semantic category knowledge as an organizing guage that the participants neither spoke nor compre-
principle. Recall of semantically categorizable lists, as com- hended (Welsh). Given that the meaningfulness of speech
pared with lists of words drawn from single semantic cat- does not influence the ISE in the context of serial recall
egories, yields measures of semantic processing in the (e.g., Jones et al., 1990), an effect of meaningfulness in
form of the semantic organization of responses and the the present setting would imply that semantic-based tasks
probability of producing each category at test (Burns & are peculiarly susceptible to impairment by the lexical-
Brown, 2000). The purpose of the present study was thus semantic attributes of irrelevant speech. We also examined
to examine whether the meaningfulness of irrelevant whether any such impairment could be identified with a
speech interferes with semantic organization during free reduction in the efficacy of a semantic-organization strat-
recall of categorizable word-lists. egy, as indexed by a diminution in the propensity to cluster
Assuming quite distinct, semantic-based, processing in by category (Smith et al., 1981). Furthermore, in order to
the category-exemplar recall task as compared with the assess whether meaningless irrelevant speech was capable
seriation-based processing in serial recall, what predic- of producing disruption that is attributable to its changing-
tions does the interference-by-process approach make? state properties—as is the case in the context of serial re-
The first is that the empirical signature of auditory distrac- call—we also included a pink noise condition which con-
tion in semantic task settings will be qualitatively distinct veys no changing-state information: Aperiodic broadband
from that found in serial recall: the semantic properties of noise such as pink noise fails to disrupt serial recall perfor-
the sound should be endowed with disruptive power but mance (Ellermeier & Zimmer, 1997) but sometimes dis-
the acoustic, changing-state, properties of the sound rupts tasks that involve semantic processing (see Smith &
should prove relatively impotent. Whilst this first predic- Jones, 1992).
tion also flows from the interference-by-content approach,
the second is unique to the interference-by-process ap- 2.1. Method
proach: the impairment should be a product not only of
the processing of the lexical-semantic attributes of the 2.1.1. Participants
sound but also of the deployment of semantic-based pro- Thirty-six Cardiff University students took part in re-
cesses as a means of supporting retrieval in the focal task. turn for course credit. Each participant reported normal
That is, the mere presence of semantic content within the or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing and
to-be-recalled and irrelevant material should not be suffi- was a monolingual native English speaker.
cient to produce disruption. Thus, if the task does not
necessitate or encourage the use of semantic processes 2.1.2. Apparatus and materials
but instead seriation processes, the semantic attributes of 2.1.2.1. To-be-remembered material. Eight instances were
the sound should not have disruptive potency even when chosen from each of 64 categories in the Yoon et al.
the to-be-recalled items are rich in semantic content. The (2004) norms in order to construct 16 lists of 32 words,
same prediction holds for between-sequence semantic each list drawn from 4 semantic categories. Categories
similarity effects: the greater disruption found with great- chosen had minimal category-exemplar overlap, and
er semantic similarity between to-be-recalled and irrele- exemplars and categories were not repeated between or
vant items is not a passive by-product of their greater within lists in order to reduce the influence of proactive
overlap within some semantic-psychological space (e.g., interference (e.g., Shuell, 1968). The exemplars chosen
Oberauer et al., 2004). Rather, such similarity exacerbates were sampled outside of the 10 most frequently produced
the difficulty of coupling the correct (i.e., task-relevant) instances so as diminish the likelihood that items could be
set of semantic representations to the semantic-organiza- recalled by simple free association or guessing (e.g., Shuell,
tion processes being engaged to support retrieval. 1969).
The present series of experiments begins by seeking to Categories were randomly assigned to each list but with
establish whether or not the action of irrelevant sound in the constraint that associated categories (e.g., ‘‘Musical
the context of semantically-driven episodic tasks—using a Instruments” and ‘‘Type of Music”) did not appear to-
category-exemplar task—is indeed distinct from that in gether. Category-exemplars within each list were arranged
the standard serial recall setting. Later experiments in pseudo-randomly, so that no two members of the same
the series investigate the nature of between-sequence category were presented adjacently and that each category
semantic similarity effects in this setting and explore the was represented equally in each quarter of the list.
key hypothesis—based on the interference-by-process
framework—that when the same type of lists are subject 2.1.2.2. Irrelevant sound. The meaningful speech was Eng-
to primary organization (seriation) their recall should be- lish narrative taken from a horticultural essay, recorded
come immune to semantic auditory distraction. in a female voice and sampled with a 16-bit resolution,
at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz using Sound Forge 5 soft-
ware (Sonic Inc., Madison, WI; 2000). The meaningless
2. Experiment 1 speech was a Welsh translation of this narrative. The
speech in each of the irrelevant sound conditions was
The first experiment examined whether category- played to participants at 65–70 dB(A) via stereo head-
exemplar recall is disrupted more by meaningful than by phones that were worn throughout the experiment.
meaningless speech: we contrasted the effect of an English Third-octave pink noise was generated using a C program
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 27

that generates digital waveforms (the program is available variance (ANOVA) with Sound Condition as the within-par-
from: http://www.moshier.net/pink.html). The irrelevant ticipant variable. Other types of response (e.g., intrusions)
sounds were presented throughout the study and test were so low as to defy statistical analysis.
phases of the task. Table 1 shows the results of the various recall measures
in the four sound conditions. Section A shows the mean
2.1.3. Design scores for the overall probability of correctly recalling
The experiment had a within-participants design with exemplars in each condition. These indicate generally that
one factor: ‘Sound Condition’ which had four levels: Eng- performance was numerically better in quiet than in both
lish speech, Welsh speech, pink noise, and quiet. The 16 the pink noise and Welsh speech conditions (performance
to-be-remembered lists were randomized but presented in these latter two conditions appeared comparable),
in a fixed order for each participant. The sound conditions which, in turn, produced better performance than the Eng-
were randomized as follows: The 16 lists were divided into lish speech condition. An ANOVA confirmed a main effect
four blocks. In each block the four lists were randomly as- of Sound Condition on the overall probability of recalling
signed to one of the four speech conditions. To control for category-exemplars, F(3, 105) = 15.50, MSE = .006,
potential order effects, the order of the sound conditions p < .001, with post hoc tests (Fisher’s PLSD) revealing sig-
within each block was counterbalanced across nificant differences between quiet and pink noise
participants. (p < .01), quiet and Welsh speech (p < .005), quiet and Eng-
lish speech (p < .001), pink noise and English speech
2.1.4. Procedure (p < .001), and between Welsh and English speech
Participants were seated at a viewing distance of (p < .001). The same pattern of means was also evident
approximately 60 cm from a PC monitor on which cate- when considering the probability of recalling exemplars
gory-exemplars were displayed in a central position. Lists within each category recalled (Section B of Table 1): Perfor-
of category-exemplars appeared in lower case black 72- mance in quiet was better than in all the sound conditions
point Times New Roman font one word at a time against a and performance was better in the pink noise and Welsh
white background. Each word appeared for 2 s with an in- speech conditions than in the English speech condition.
ter-stimulus interval of 1 s. Retrieval was immediate with An ANOVA revealed a main effect of Sound Condition on
the end of the list being notified by the appearance of a the probability of recalling exemplars within each category
red ‘RECALL’ cue. recalled, F(3, 105) = 18.31, MSE = .004, p < .001, with post
Participants were tested in small groups of six partici- hoc tests (Fisher’s PLSD) revealing the same significant dif-
pants in a sound-attenuated room. Participants were ferences as for the analysis of the overall probability of cor-
seated in individual cubicles each equipped with a Sam- rectly recalled items. However, the pattern of results was
sung Syncmaster 171S PC and display. Participants were in- different for the probability of recalling each category (Sec-
formed that they would be presented with sixteen 32- tion C of Table 1). Here, the means for performance in Eng-
word lists, and that each list would be presented one word lish speech were lower than those for the quiet, pink noise
at a time on the computer monitor from which they were and Welsh speech conditions. An ANOVA revealed a main
asked to recall as many words as possible and write the effect of Sound Condition on the probability of recalling
words they remembered down in the order which they re-
called them on recall sheets when a ‘RECALL’ cue appeared
on the screen. Recall sheets contained 17 columns of 32
rows each. One practice trial was presented before the Table 1
experimental trials. Participants were not explicitly told Mean recall and clustering measures as a function of irrelevant sound
condition in Experiment 1. Standard errors of the means are presented in
that the lists were categorizable. Participants were in- parenthesis.
formed that they would have 2 min to retrieve as much
as they could of the list and that after this time a tone (A) Overall probability of recalling category-exemplars
Quiet .51 (0.019)
would sound to signal the beginning of the next list (some Pink noise .46 (0.016)
5 s later). Participants were instructed to ignore any sound Welsh speech .45 (0.021)
that they heard through the headphones and were told English speech .39 (0.022)
that they would not be tested on its content at any point (B) Probability of recalling exemplars from within each category recalled
in the experiment. The experiment lasted approximately Quiet .53 (0.016)
50 min. Pink noise .48 (0.014)
Welsh speech .48 (0.019)
English speech .42 (0.018)
2.2. Results and discussion
(C) Probability of recalling each category
Quiet .96 (0.011)
2.2.1. Recall measures Pink noise .96 (0.009)
Recall measures came in three forms: the overall mean Welsh speech .94 (0.012)
probability of correctly recalling category-exemplars English speech .90 (0.019)
across all categories, the mean probability of recalling (D) Mean Z scores
exemplars from within each category recalled, and the Quiet 3.17 (0.25)
probability of recalling each category (based on recalling Pink noise 3.04 (0.25)
Welsh speech 2.88 (0.25)
at least one word from a category). The data from each re- English speech 2.45 (0.24)
call measure were analyzed using a one-way analysis of
28 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

each category, F(3, 105) = 5.18, MSE = .006, p < .005. How- rather than one attributable to acoustic variability which
ever, in contrast to the other recall measures, post hoc test- produces the classical irrelevant sound effect in serial re-
ing (Fisher’s PLSD) revealed significant differences only call. Further evidence to buttress this conclusion was found
between the English speech condition and all other condi- in a supplementary experiment—one not reported in full
tions (p < .05, all comparisons). here for economy of exposition—which involved a stan-
dard visually-presented serial recall task (for a typical pro-
2.2.2. Clustering measure cedure, see Hughes & Jones, 2005) with the irrelevant
Whilst there are several potential ways of measuring sound stimuli used in Experiment 1. In this experiment—
semantic-categorization (for a review, see Murphy, 1979), in which the participants were 40 non-Welsh speakers
we restricted our analyses to the Z score (Frankel & Cole, sampled from the same population as in Experiment 1—a
1971). Z scores range from negative to positive values the within-participant design was used. This supplementary
maximum of which is determined by the number of cate- experiment revealed an irrelevant sound effect, F(3,
gories and category-exemplars represented by the to-be- 117) = 17.76, MSE = .033, p < .001—that is, an effect of
remembered list. The Z score is higher for outputted se- sound compared to quiet—but no effect of meaning thus
quences that demonstrate use of the semantic structure replicating Jones et al. (1990) with the particular materials
inherent in the to-be-remembered list. For example, the used in Experiment 1. The experiment also revealed no ef-
outputted sequence: ‘‘brass, lead, giraffe, pig, cherry, lem- fect of pink noise thus replicating Ellermeier and Zimmer
on, pliers, chisel”, that demonstrates strong use of seman- (1997). The mean probability of correct recall, marked with
tic organization (as is evident from the number of adjacent the strict serial recall criterion and collapsed across serial
repetitions of same-category items) will receive a higher position, did not differ between the Welsh speech condi-
score than a more randomly organized sequence: ‘‘brass, tion, M = .60; SE = .03, and English speech condition,
lead, cherry, pliers, lemon, chisel, giraffe, pig”, in which ex- M = .61; SE = .03 (p > .7), but both these means were signif-
actly the same number of categories and category-exem- icantly lower than those in the quiet condition, M = .73,
plars are recalled but in which fewer same-category SE = .02, and the pink noise condition, M = .74, SE = .02
adjacent repetitions are produced. Severely fragmented re- (p < .001 for both comparisons). This result replicates prior
call patterns such as ‘‘brass, pig, cherry, pliers, giraffe, lem- research showing that the meaningfulness of irrelevant
on, chisel, lead” will receive negative Z scores because they speech does not have a disruptive effect in the serial recall
represent below-chance clustering (in fact none of the re- setting but also shows that the Welsh speech used in
sponses in this example are clustered by semantic-cate- Experiment 1 was indeed capable of producing an ISE in
gory as indicated by the absence of any same-category the serial recall context. Its failure to produce any more
adjacent repetitions). disruption than pink noise in Experiment 1 suggests that
Z scores were calculated with all repeat and intrusion it was impotent in terms of producing a classical ISE in
errors removed. Section D of Table 1 shows the mean clus- the context of category-exemplar recall.
tering measure for each sound condition. The Z score The effect of meaningfulness in Experiment 1 ap-
means are lower in all the sound conditions than in the pears to be explicable as a conflict of semantic process-
quiet condition, and lower in the English speech condition ing that emerges when the primary task promotes a
compared to the three other conditions. An ANOVA con- degree of such processing. The best indication of an
firmed a main effect of sound on Z scores, F(3, impairment of semantic processing produced by the
105) = 4.22, MSE = .84, p < .001. Follow-up post hoc tests meaningful irrelevant speech is that meaningful speech
(Fisher’s PLSD) revealed that the only significant differ- reduces the use of secondary organization, that is, the
ences were between the English speech condition and each semantic-categorization of the to-be-recalled material,
of the other sound conditions (p < .05). Thus, English and also reduces the probability with which a category
speech reduced the level of semantic-categorization as is recalled. Both these recall measures are thought to
measured by the Z score. reflect semantic or ‘‘relational” processing (Burns &
The results of the recall measures, and the Z score clus- Brown, 2000; Hunt & McDaniel, 1993). The reduction
tering measure, suggest that there is an impairment to in category recall reflects, possibly, a failure to establish
semantic free recall tasks that is attributable to the mean- adequately at encoding, or use at retrieval, higher-order
ingfulness of irrelevant speech, given that semantic con- semantic encodings that can be used as a retrieval plan
tent would, arguably, have been the most salient for enabling inter-category transitions (e.g., Bower,
difference between the Welsh and English speech condi- Clark, Lesgold, & Winzenz, 1969). One of these seman-
tions. There was, however, some degree of disruption pro- tic encoding strategies might involve forging some kind
duced by pink noise and Welsh speech. This is not easily of semantic association between category-exemplars
explained if the free recall task engaged a purely seman- (e.g., ‘‘pigeon”, ‘‘chisel”) or categories (e.g., ‘‘Birds”,
tic-based retrieval strategy. The pink noise and Welsh ‘‘Tools”) where pre-experimentally there is none (cf.
speech, being devoid of semantic content, should not have Wingfield, Lindfield, & Kahana, 1998). There is also
produced disruption. However, because there were no sta- the possibility that the meaningful speech impairs gen-
tistically significant differences between the pink noise eration of category-names or exemplars (e.g., for use as
and Welsh speech conditions it is reasonable to conclude retrieval cues) during encoding or retrieval: It is well
that they both exerted the same general effect of sound accepted that tasks that involve semantic memory are
that is quite often reported in the context of tasks involving underpinned by generation processes (Gronlund & Shif-
semantic memory (Martin et al., 1988; Smith et al., 1981) frin, 1986).
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 29

3. Experiment 2 matching the irrelevant category-items (e.g., Anderson,


1983; Oberauer et al., 2004; Rundus, 1973). Thus, in order
In Experiment 2, the process-based account of semantic to adjudicate between the interference-by-process and
auditory distraction is once again scrutinized, this time not interference-by-content accounts we adopted the ap-
by manipulating the mere meaningfulness of the speech proach of presenting just eight irrelevant items randomly
but rather its semantic similarity to the to-be-recalled throughout the presentation and recall periods that, in
exemplars (see also Marsh et al., submitted for publication, some conditions, were semantically related to the to-be-
2008). The interference-by-process and interference-by- recalled items belonging to just one of the four categories
content accounts both assume that greater disruption will represented on the to-be-remembered list.
be found when irrelevant and to-be-remembered cate-
gory-exemplars are drawn from the same semantic cate- 3.1. Method
gory but the two approaches differ with regard to the
causal mechanism underlying such a between-sequence 3.1.1. Participants
semantic similarity effect. Thirty participants from Cardiff University took part in
The interference-by-process approach supposes that return for course credit. Each participant reported normal
between-sequence semantic similarity will impair recall or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing and
performance not because of the mere similarity in content was a native English speaker.
between the to-be-recalled and irrelevant category-exem-
plars but because it disrupts the strategy or process 3.1.2. Apparatus and materials
underpinning the semantic focal task. In this way the inter- Sixty categories were chosen from the Van Overschelde,
ference-by-process approach is similar to the strategy-dis- Rawson, and Dunlosky (2004) norms. From each of these
ruption interpretation which holds currency in other categories sixteen exemplars were chosen. The 8 highest-
domains of forgetting research (e.g., Basden & Basden, dominant exemplars were used for the irrelevant exem-
1995; Basden, Basden, & Stephens, 2002). In the context plars, whilst the to-be-remembered exemplars were cho-
of semantic auditory distraction the interference-by-pro- sen from the 11th to 18th ranked positions. The 60
cess account holds that the irrelevant information pro- categories were sorted into 12 pools of 5 categories be-
duces disruption because it suggests an organization of tween which there was no obvious semantic relation. For
semantic information that is incongruent with the partici- the related trials, the category presented as sound matched
pant’s encoding or retrieval strategy (cf. Greitzer, 1976). one of the 4 categories represented in the to-be-remem-
For example, suppose that a given participant’s initial re- bered list. For the unrelated trials, the sound consisted of
trieval plan involves recall of category A first, then D, then the category in each pool that was not represented on
C, then B. If irrelevant sound related to category B coincides the to-be-remembered list. The 8 irrelevant items for the
with the point in the participant’s plan when A exemplars unrelated and related conditions were randomly presented
are to be retrieved, the sound will specify information that throughout the study and test phases of each trial at a rate
cues a category (category B) that, if responded to, would of two per second, thus each irrelevant item was presented
impair the orderly execution of the plan. Irrelevant exem- 54 times throughout a given trial. The presence of any gi-
plars that are drawn from a category represented on the ven category as part of the to-be-remembered list and re-
list (e.g., ‘‘sparrow” and ‘‘robin” for the category ‘‘Birds”) lated and unrelated sound was counterbalanced between
are particularly likely to provide a misleading cue in this participants. This procedure resulted in the construction
way than irrelevant exemplars not represented on the list of 12 categorized lists comprising 32 exemplars, 4 to be
because they convey information that is ‘‘congruent-yet- used for each of the related, unrelated and quiet
incongruent” with the task demand; ‘‘robin” is a ‘‘Bird” conditions.
and hence context-appropriate but was not presented on
the to-be-recalled list and thus is response-inappropriate. 3.1.3. Design
A unique prediction of this process-based view that be- A repeated-measures design was used with a single fac-
tween-sequence semantic similarity disrupts the partici- tor—Sound Condition—which had three levels: (1) Speech
pant’s capacity to adhere to their original retrieval plan is categorically-unrelated to the to-be-remembered material;
that the irrelevant exemplars need only be semantically re- (2) categorically-related speech; and (3) quiet. The proce-
lated to a subset (e.g., one out of four) of the categories rep- dure was the same as in Experiment 1.
resented on the to-be-remembered list. For example, it
would matter not whether there are irrelevant items that 3.2. Results and discussion
are semantically related to category A to disrupt recall of
category A exemplars: so long as there are irrelevant items 3.2.1. Recall measures
on that trial that are semantically similar to at least one of Table 2 provides the results of the various recall mea-
the other subsets of exemplars (say, those comprising cat- sures in the three sound conditions (again, intrusion and
egory B) this may still corrupt the overall integrity of the repetition rates were too low for statistical analysis). Sec-
retrieval plan. In contrast, whilst the interference-by-con- tion A of Table 2 shows the mean scores for the overall
tent approach predicts that greater disruption will be pro- probability of correctly recalling category-exemplars in
duced with a greater similarity between to-be-recalled and each condition. The means show generally that perfor-
irrelevant exemplars this effect should be confined solely, mance was better in quiet than unrelated speech, which
or largely to the set of to-be-recalled category-items in turn was better than related speech. An ANOVA con-
30 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

Table 2 were .45 (SE = 0.016) for the non-matching categories and
Mean recall and clustering measures as a function of irrelevant sound .48 (SE = 0.023) for the matching category which was not
condition in Experiment 2. Standard errors of the means are presented in
parenthesis.
a significant difference, t(29) = 1.72, p > .05. Thus, the dis-
ruption was not confined to the retrieval of to-be-remem-
(A) Overall probability of recalling category-exemplars bered category-exemplars that matched the category
Quiet .53 (0.016)
Unrelated speech .44 (0.019)
represented by the irrelevant exemplars.
Related speech .40 (0.019)
(B) Probability of recalling exemplars from within each category recalled
3.2.2. Clustering measure
Quiet .54 (0.015) Section D of Table 2 shows the results of the mean clus-
Unrelated speech .50 (0.019) tering measure for each sound condition. The Z score
Related speech .45 (0.016) means are lower in all the sound conditions than in the
(C) Probability of recalling each category quiet condition, and lower in the related-speech condition
Quiet .99 (0.005) compared to the unrelated speech condition. An ANOVA
Unrelated speech .89 (0.018)
confirmed a main effect of Sound Condition on Z scores,
Related speech .88 (0.017)
F(2, 58) = 13.64, MSE = .996, p < .001. Follow-up post hoc
(D) Mean Z scores
tests (Fisher’s PLSD) revealed significant differences be-
Quiet 3.74 (0.27)
Unrelated speech 2.70 (0.27) tween quiet and unrelated speech (p < .001), and between
Related speech 2.48 (0.26) unrelated and related speech (p < .001). Thus the semantic
relationship between the to-be-recalled and irrelevant
items impairs semantic-categorization above and beyond
firmed a main effect of Sound Condition on the overall the effect of mere meaningfulness.
probability of recalling category-exemplars, F(2, Whilst replicating the key features of Experiment 1,
58) = 65.06, MSE = .002, p < .001, with post hoc tests (Fish- Experiment 2 adds to the few studies that have demon-
er’s PLSD) revealing significant differences between quiet strated between-sequence semantic similarity effects in
and unrelated speech (p < .001) and between unrelated semantic free recall tasks (Beaman, 2004; Marsh et al., sub-
and related speech (p < .001). The same pattern of means mitted for publication, 2008; Neely & LeCompte, 1999).
was also evident for probability of recalling exemplars Moreover, the between-sequence semantic similarity ef-
within each category recalled (Section B of Table 2): Perfor- fect obtained in Experiment 2 appears to fit better within
mance in quiet was better than for unrelated speech and a process-oriented rather than an interference-by-content
performance in unrelated speech condition was better approach. The finding that appears particularly supportive
than for related speech. An ANOVA revealed a main effect of the interference-by-process account but at the same
of Sound Condition on the probability of recalling exem- time problematic for the interference-by-content view is
plar within each category recalled, F(2, 58) = 22.61, the lack of category-specific impairment within the be-
MSE = .002, p < .001, with post hoc tests (Fisher’s PLSD) tween-sequence semantic similarity effect. That is, the dis-
revealing the same significant differences as for the analy- ruption produced by irrelevant exemplars that matched
sis of the overall probability of correctly recalling category- one of the to-be-remembered categories was not confined
exemplars (ps < .05). However, the pattern of results was to that matching category (for category-specific impair-
different for the probability of recalling each category (Sec- ment in a different context, see Mueller & Watkins,
tion C of Table 2). Here, the means for performance in the 1977). If disruption is produced as a passive side-effect of
sound conditions were lower than in the quiet condition the structural similarity of irrelevant and to-be-remem-
but there appeared to be no difference between the unre- bered items within a representational space (e.g.,
lated and related sound conditions. An ANOVA revealed a Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Oberauer & Lange, 2008;
main effect of Sound Condition on the probability of recall- Oberauer et al., 2004) or changes in the nature of the hypo-
ing each category, F(2, 58) = 19.12, MSE = .006, p < .001. thetical associative strengths between to-be-recalled items
However, in contrast to the two other measures, post hoc and retrieval cues within memory (Anderson, 1983; Run-
testing (Fisher’s PLSD) revealed significant differences only dus, 1973), then category-specific impairment would be
between the quiet and unrelated speech and related expected. This finding appears consistent, however, with
speech conditions (both p < .001). Thus, despite having an the notion of process or strategy-based disruption. The dis-
effect on the other two recall measures, semantic related- ruption, for example, could be attributed to the impair-
ness had no effect on the probability of producing each ment of a retrieval strategy rather than a clash between
category. the content of the to-be-recalled and irrelevant items
To examine the prediction of the interference-by-pro- (Basden & Basden, 1995; Basden et al., 2002). It seems rea-
cess account—that the additional disruption produced by sonable to suggest that the related irrelevant items can im-
related speech will not be specific to the category that pair the construction, or use of, a retrieval plan (e.g., Bower
matches the irrelevant exemplars but will be generalized et al., 1969) as sometimes the category the sound items
to all categories on the to-be-recalled list—we averaged activate will be in conflict with the desired (unrelated)
the probability of recalling category-exemplars within list-category being encoded or retrieved. Impairment in
each category recalled across the three categories that recalling exemplars from the categories that do not match
did not match the irrelevant exemplars and compared the irrelevant speech could be a result of the related irrel-
these with the mean probability of correctly recalling evant information suggesting an output order that is
exemplars from the category that did. The resulting means incongruent with the participant’s original retrieval plan
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 31

(or perhaps reflect the overhead of inhibitory processes de- vant speech, and thus lend support to the interference-
signed to minimize the ensuing conflict; see Section 6). by-process account. Moreover, the interference-by-process
We turn now to assess the interference-by-process ac- account would receive further support should the mean-
count further by testing more directly its tenet that the ingfulness of the irrelevant speech be endowed with dis-
semantic effects of irrelevant sound are jointly determined ruptive potency only when the to-be-recalled exemplars
by the nature of the focal task processes and the nature of are recalled by their meaning (e.g., by semantic-category)
the sound. Thus, the hypothesis tested in Experiments 3 rather than serial order.
and 4 is that the semantic effects of irrelevant sound
should only arise when (a) the irrelevant sound conveys 4.1. Method
semantic information and (b) the focal task involves
semantic processing. 4.1.1. Participants
Forty students from Cardiff University (none of whom
took part in Experiments 1 and 2) participated for course
4. Experiment 3 credit. Each reported normal hearing and normal or cor-
rected-to-normal vision and was a native English speaker.
Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that semantic audi- Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups:
tory distraction disrupts the semantic-categorization pro- semantic-categorization or seriation instructions.
cess. This represents an entirely distinct effect from that
found in the serial recall setting in which the acoustic, 4.1.2. Apparatus and materials
not semantic, properties of the sound disrupts the strategy These were similar to Experiment 1 with the following
of serial rehearsal (or seriation; Beaman & Jones, 1998). exception: Four exemplars were chosen, namely, the 11th
Experiment 3 sought further, arguably more direct, evi- to 14th most frequently produced responses for each of
dence that the two findings are distinct by making use of the 72 categories chosen from the Yoon et al. (2004)
a task instruction manipulation (Marsh et al., submitted norms. These were combined as in Experiment 1 to create
for publication, 2008; Weist & Crawford, 1973): By 18 lists of 16 words each, each list having 4 categories.
instructing one group of participants to recall in serial or-
der, and another to recall the same kind of list by category, 4.1.3. Design
we can assess the degree to which the dominant focal A mixed design was used with one between- and one
process modulates which attributes of the sound dictate within-participant factor. The between-participants factor
disruption. If the effects of the meaningfulness of irrele- was ‘Task Instruction’ of which there were two levels:
vant speech in Experiments 1 and 2 are indeed due to semantic-categorization and seriation. The within-partici-
semantic processing of the to-be-remembered material, pant factor was ‘Sound Condition’ as in Experiments 1
then we would expect semantic effects of irrelevant speech and 2.
to be found when participants are instructed to retrieve
the to-be-remembered material according to semantic 4.1.4. Procedure
category but not when encouraged to use, instead, a seria- The procedure was similar to Experiments 1 and 2 apart
tion process to recall the very same content by instructing from the following: Participants were informed that they
recall in serial order. In contrast, the interference-by-con- would be presented with a total of 18 lists of words that
tent accounts might be taken to predict an effect of the each contained 16 exemplars, 4 from each of 4 different
meaningfulness of irrelevant speech regardless of task semantic categories. The category-exemplars were drawn
instruction. There is good evidence that semantic activa- from positions 11–14 of each of 72 categories chosen from
tion of to-be-remembered items occurs regardless of task the Yoon et al. (2004) category norms. The irrelevant
instruction (e.g., Rouibah, Tiberghien, & Lupker, 1999), thus speech was English narrative. Meaningless speech was
the propensity for passive interference between semanti- the same narrative reversed using Sound Forge 5 software
cally similar representations should not differ according (Sonic Inc., Madison, WI; 2000). Participants given seman-
to the particular focal task strategy employed. In tic-categorization instructions were asked to try to
Experiment 3, we use shorter lists (16-exemplars) than in remember as many words as possible by semantically-cat-
Experiments 1 and 2 to facilitate the instructed use of a se- egorizing them and writing them down according to their
rial recall strategy. categories when the RECALL cue appeared: Participants
In this experiment meaningfulness was manipulated by were told to write down the exemplars as they came to
contrasting forward with reversed speech. Reversing mind, and to attempt to recall all the exemplars they could
speech removes phonetic properties that allow lexical remember from one category, exhausting that category,
access, and thus semantic processing (Sheffert, Pisoni, before moving on, and doing the same with the next cate-
Fellowes, & Remez, 2002) but preserves the overall spectral gory and so on. They were also told that if they could
features of forward speech. Given that the meaningfulness remember any individual words after the semantic-cate-
of speech (as indicated by the comparison between for- gory clusters they should write them at the end of the
ward and reversed speech) does not influence the classical clusters.
ISE in the context of serial recall (e.g., Jones et al., 1990), an Participants given seriation instructions were in-
effect of meaningfulness in the current experiment would structed to try and remember the words in their original
further support the prediction that focal semantic order of presentation and to recall each exemplar by
processing is peculiarly susceptible to meaningful irrele- assigning it to its original serial position when the RECALL
32 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

cue appeared. To maximise the level of exemplar recall, Table 3


participants in the seriation group were instructed that Mean recall measures as a function of irrelevant sound condition and task
instruction in Experiment 3. Standard errors of the means are presented in
they could leave gaps if necessary but were also told that parenthesis.
if they had a list-exemplar available to them for recall,
but could not remember the position, that they should Sound condition Categorization Seriation

guess the original position. Mean (SE) Mean (SE)


Recall sheets contained 18 columns of 16 rows each. (A) Overall probability of recalling category-exemplars
Participants given seriation instructions were given spe- Quiet .65 (0.027) .56 (0.027)
cially-prepared recall sheets with serial positions marked Reversed speech .60 (0.027) .48 (0.024)
Forward speech .45 (0.024) .48 (0.025)
on them, whilst the group instructed to categorize had
the same recall sheets but without the serial positions (B) Probability of recalling exemplars from within each category
Quiet .70 (0.020) .57 (0.027)
marked. Participants were explicitly instructed to ignore
Reversed speech .64 (0.017) .48 (0.024)
any sound that they might hear during the task. Sounds Forward speech .61 (0.023) .48 (0.025)
were presented throughout the presentation and test
(C) Probability of recalling each category
phases. Because the list length was halved for this experi- Quiet .94 (0.024) 1.0 (0.002)
ment the retrieval time allotted for each list was reduced Reversed speech .93 (0.030) .99 (0.006)
to 1 min. Forward speech .75 (0.025) .99 (0.006)

4.2. Results and discussion


F(1, 38) = 25.04, MSE = .02, p < .001, but no interaction be-
4.2.1. Recall measures tween these variables, F(2, 76) = 1.06, MSE = .004, p > .05.
The recall measures are distinguished as in Experiments Simple effects (LSD) revealed significant differences be-
1 and 2. For the seriation group, items were scored as cor- tween quiet and reversed speech (p < .05), and quiet and
rect regardless of whether they occupied their correct posi- forward speech (p < .001), and the difference between re-
tions in the output protocols. Section A of Table 3 shows versed and forward speech was significant (p < .005) for
the overall probability of correctly recalling category- the semantic-categorization group. In contrast, for the seri-
exemplars for both Task Instruction groups. Performance ation group the only significant differences were those be-
in quiet is clearly superior to performance in the speech tween the quiet and reversed conditions and between the
conditions for both groups. Of greater interest, and consis- quiet and forward speech conditions (p < .005). The simple
tent with the interference-by-process approach, perfor- effects analyses also revealed that recall performance for
mance with forward speech was poorer than that with the semantic-categorization group exceeded that of the
reversed speech for the semantic-categorization group seriation group in all three conditions (all p < .05).
but not the seriation group. A 3 (Sound Condition)  2 Section C of Table 3 shows the mean probability of cor-
(Task Instructions) ANOVA confirmed a main effect of rectly recalling each category. In general, category recall
Sound Condition on the overall probability of correctly was better in the seriation than semantic-categorization
recalling category-exemplars, F(2, 76) = 58.06, MSE = .004, group. For the semantic-categorization group, the proba-
p < .001. There was no main effect of Task Instruction, bility of recalling each category was lower in the forward
F(1, 38) = 3.8, MSE = .032, p = .059). However, there was a speech compared to reversed speech and quiet conditions.
significant interaction between Sound Condition and Task Interpretation of this particular subset of the results is
Instruction, F(2, 76) = 16.03, MSE = .004, p < .001, whereby complicated by the fact that the probability of recalling
the disruptive effects of meaningfulness arose when the re- each category was at ceiling in the seriation group. The rea-
trieval strategy was semantic-categorization but not when son for this is that recalling the first four presented exem-
it was seriation. Simple effects analyses (LSD) revealed sig- plars in their original presentation positions (as is required
nificant differences between quiet and the reversed and under seriation instructions) would guarantee category re-
forward speech conditions for semantic-categorization call for all categories represented on the to-be-recalled
and seriation instructions (ps < .05). Additionally, there lists. Despite this complication, the pattern of results for
was a significant difference between reversed and forward the semantic-categorization group appears to be consis-
speech for the semantic-categorization group only tent with Experiment 1: Meaningful speech, as compared
(p < .001). The simple effects analyses also revealed that re- with meaningless speech, disrupted the probability of cor-
call performance for the semantic-categorization group ex- rectly recalling each category. Confirming this, an ANOVA
ceeded that of the seriation group in the quiet condition restricted to the semantic-categorization group revealed
and the reversed speech condition (both p < .05). a main effect of sound on the probability of recalling each
The mean probability of recalling exemplars within category, F(2, 38) = 75.38, MSE = .003, p < .001, with post
each category recalled can be seen in Section B of Table hoc tests (Fisher’s PLSD) revealing significant differences
3. In general, this was greater for the semantic-categoriza- between quiet and forward speech and between reversed
tion than seriation instructed group, and smaller in the and forward speech (ps < .001).
speech conditions than the quiet condition. The means
were also higher in the reversed compared to forward 4.2.2. Clustering measure
speech condition. An ANOVA on these means revealed a Z scores were negative for the seriation group consis-
main effect of Sound Condition, F(2, 76) = 23.25, tent with the assumption that participants in this group
MSE = .004, p < .001, and a main effect of Task Instruction, would not have clustered the to-be-recalled exemplars
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 33

by semantic category. As such, only the scores for the cat- speaker. Participants were randomly assigned to one of
egorization group were included in the following analysis. the between-participants groups: semantic-categorization
The mean Z scores were lower in the reversed speech or seriation instructions.
(M = 2.86, SE = 0.19) and forward speech (M = 1.79,
SE = 0.16) conditions compared to the quiet condition 5.1.2. Apparatus and materials
(M = 3.14, SE = 0.17) and were lower in the forward com- These aspects of the method were similar to Experiment
pared to reversed speech conditions. An ANOVA revealed 2 with the following differences: The 4 highest-dominant
a main effect of sound on Z scores, F(2, 38) = 47.15, exemplars from each category were used for the irrelevant
MSE = .213, p < .001. Post hoc testing (Fisher’s PLSD) re- exemplars, whilst the to-be-remembered exemplars were
vealed significant differences between quiet and forward chosen from the 11th to 14th positions.
speech, and between reversed and forward speech (both
p < .001). Thus, like Experiments 1 and 2, the degree of 5.1.3. Design
semantic-organization was impaired by meaningful speech A mixed design was used with one between- and one
as compared with meaningless speech. within-participant factor. The between-participants factor
Experiment 3 revealed that semantic effects of irrele- was Task instruction as in Experiment 3. The within-partic-
vant speech—in terms of its meaningfulness—appear to ipants factor was, like Experiment 2, Sound Condition of
be process– rather than content-driven: The mere pres- which there were three levels: (1) Speech categorically-
ence of similar semantic content within the relevant and unrelated to the to-be-remembered material; (2) categori-
irrelevant material is insufficient for disruption; the cally-related speech; and (3) quiet.
semantic processing of irrelevant speech disrupts only
when dynamic semantic-organisation based processes are 5.1.4. Procedure
deployed as a recall strategy. Consistent with Experiments The procedure was the same as Experiment 3 with the
1 and 2, meaningful speech, as compared with meaningless exception of the following: Participants were informed
speech, disrupted the overall probability of recalling cate- that they would be presented with a total of 12 lists of
gory-exemplars and it also produced semantic interference words that each contained a total of 16 exemplars, 4 from
that was specific to the probability of correctly recalling each of 4 different semantic categories, and response
each category and the degree of semantic-categorization sheets contained 12 columns of 16 rows each. Like the
evident in the recall protocols. These results, therefore, foregoing experiments, sounds were presented throughout
provide further support for the interpretation of auditory the presentation and test phases of the tasks.
distraction as process-based (e.g., Jones & Tremblay,
2000) as opposed to structural or content-based (e.g., 5.2. Results and discussion
Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Neath, 2000).
5.2.1. Recall measures
The recall measures were the same as in Experiments
5. Experiment 4 1–3. Section A of Table 4 shows the overall probability of
correctly recalling category-exemplars. It is evident that
The results of the current series are thus far consistent performance in both speech conditions was poorer than
generally with the notion that the semantic processing re- quiet regardless of task instruction. Moreover, perfor-
quired by the primary task can be disrupted by semantic mance in related speech was poorer than performance in
processing of irrelevant speech. Extending the rationale unrelated speech for the semantic-categorization, but not
of Experiment 3, this suggests that between-sequence the seriation, group. A 3 (Sound Condition)  2 (Task
semantic similarity should also only be disruptive when
the primary task requires semantic processing, but not
when it relies on serial rehearsal. Experiment 4 sought
Table 4
additional support for the process-based interpretation of
Mean recall measures as a function of irrelevant sound condition and task
semantic auditory distraction by manipulating between- instruction in Experiment 4. Standard errors of the means are presented in
sequence semantic similarity (as in Experiment 2) and also parenthesis.
task instructions (as in Experiment 3). Finding that be-
Sound condition Categorization Seriation
tween-sequence semantic similarity impairs category-
Mean (SE) Mean (SE)
exemplar recall only under semantic-categorization
instructions and not under seriation instructions would (A) Overall probability of recalling category-exemplars
Quiet .62 (0.013) .53 (0.014)
serve to bolster the interference-by-process approach and
Unrelated speech .54 (0.019) .48 (0.018)
undermine further the interference-by-content approach. Related speech .51 (0.022) .48 (0.015)
(B) Probability of recalling exemplars from within each category recalled
5.1. Method Quiet .63 (0.013) .54 (0.017)
Unrelated speech .63 (0.014) .48 (0.018)
5.1.1. Participants Related speech .58 (0.018) .48 (0.015)
Sixty students from Cardiff University took part in (C) Probability of recalling each category
Experiment 4. None had taken part in Experiments 1–3. Quiet .98 (0.006) .99 (0.006)
Each participant reported normal hearing and normal or Unrelated speech .87 (0.016) .99 (0.004)
Related speech .86 (0.021) .99 (0.003)
corrected-to-normal vision and was a native English
34 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

Instruction) ANOVA confirmed a main effect of Irrelevant the category-exemplars within each category recalled
Sound Condition, F(2, 116) = 37.74, MSE = .003, p < .001, were .58 (SE = 0.021) for the non-matching categories and
and Task Instruction, F(1, 58) = 7.66, MSE = .02, p < .01, .57 (SE = 0.024) for the matching category. A paired t-test
and an interaction between these variables, F(2, revealed the impairment was not specific to the recall of
116) = 3.94, MSE = .003, p < .05. Simple effects analyses that category, t(29) = 0.44, p > .05. Thus, like Experiment
(LSD) revealed that, regardless of task instruction condi- 2, the disruption was not confined to the retrieval of to-
tion, there were significant differences between quiet and be-remembered category-exemplars that matched the cat-
unrelated speech (p < .001) and between quiet and related egory from which the irrelevant exemplars were drawn.
speech (p < .001). Additionally, there was a significant dif-
ference between the unrelated and related speech condi- 5.2.2. Clustering measure
tions but only for the semantic-categorization group The mean Z scores for the semantic-categorization
(p < .01). This analysis also revealed that recall perfor- group were lower in the unrelated speech (M = 2.61,
mance for the semantic-categorization group exceeded SE = 0.14) and related speech (M = 2.33, SE = 0.17) condi-
that of the seriation group in the quiet condition tions than in the quiet condition (M = 3.15, SE = 0.12) and
(p < .001), and the unrelated speech condition (p < .05). also appeared to be lower in the related compared to unre-
To summarize the results for the overall probability of lated speech condition. An ANOVA revealed a main effect
recalling category-exemplars, generally category-exem- of Sound Condition on Z scores, F(2, 58) = 19.15,
plars were better retrieved with semantic-categorization MSE = .274, p < .001. Post hoc testing (Fisher’s PLSD) re-
compared with seriation instructions, and disruptive ef- vealed significant differences between quiet and unrelated
fects of semantic similarity arose only when the retrieval speech (p < .001), quiet and related speech (p < .001), and
strategy required semantic-categorization. between unrelated and related speech (p < .05). Thus the
The mean probability of correctly recalling exemplars degree of semantic-categorization was impaired by both
within each category recalled is shown in Section B of Ta- unrelated and related speech but the impairment was
ble 4. In general, this was greater for the semantic-catego- greater in the related condition.
rization than seriation instructed group, and was smaller in The results thus confirm that between-sequence
the speech conditions than in the quiet condition. More- semantic similarity effects occur only when semantic pro-
over, the means in the related speech condition were lower cessing is part of the retrieval strategy (see also Marsh,
than those in the unrelated speech condition only for the Hughes, & Jones, 2006, 2008). Such a finding harmonizes
semantic-categorization group. An ANOVA on these data with the notion that such effects are better explained in
revealed a main effect of Sound Condition, F(2, 116) = terms of a process-oriented approach than by a content-
15.37, MSE = .003, p < .001, and also a main effect of Task based approach to auditory distraction.
Instruction, F(1, 58) = 33.11, MSE = .016, p < .001, and an
interaction between these variables, F(2, 116) = 3.6,
MSE = .003, p < .05. Simple effects analyses (LSD) revealed 6. General discussion
significant differences between quiet and unrelated speech
(p < .001), and quiet and related speech (p < .001) but not The results of the current series can be summarized as
between unrelated and related speech (p > .05) for the seri- follows: Experiment 1 demonstrated that the meaningful-
ation group. For the semantic-categorization group, there ness of irrelevant speech produces greater disruption to
were significant differences between quiet and related the free recall of categorizable word lists than meaningless
speech (p = .001), and between unrelated and related speech or pink noise. This experiment also revealed that
speech conditions (p < .005). the pattern of semantic interference as compared with
Section C of Table 4 shows the mean probability of the disruption produced by noise (pink noise and meaning-
recalling each category. In general, category recall was bet- less speech) shows a unique characteristic: it affects the
ter for seriation than semantic-categorization. For the recall of categories as well as the degree of semantic-cate-
semantic-categorization groups there was a loss of catego- gorization demonstrated at test. Experiment 2 revealed an
ries in the speech conditions which was evident for both effect of between-sequence semantic similarity whereby
the unrelated and related speech conditions. For the same the semantic relatedness between the to-be-recalled and
reason as in Experiment 3, we again restricted our analysis irrelevant items produces additional disruption to the
to the semantic-categorization group. An ANOVA revealed overall probability of recalling exemplars, the probability
a main effect of Sound Condition on the probability of of recalling exemplars within each category and the degree
recalling each category, F(2, 58) = 25.81, MSE = .005, of semantic-categorization observed although it has no ef-
p < .001, with post hoc tests (Fisher’s PLSD) revealing sig- fect on the probability of recalling each category. Experi-
nificant differences between quiet and unrelated speech, ments 3 and 4 revealed that effects of meaningfulness
and quiet and related speech (p < .001) but not between and between-sequence semantic similarity are found only
unrelated and related speech conditions (p > .05). when semantic-categorization is adopted by the partici-
As for Experiment 2, we addressed the question of pant and not when serial order is used as an organizing
whether the additional disruption produced by related strategy.
speech in the categorization group was attributable to an The results of all the experiments appear to be well ac-
impairment specific to the recall of the category that counted for within a process-oriented approach to disrup-
matches the irrelevant exemplars in that condition. The tion from task-extraneous material (Hughes & Jones, 2005;
resulting means for the probability of successfully recalling Jones & Tremblay, 2000; Neumann, 1996). The interfer-
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 35

ence-by-process view holds that in the case of the classical The process-oriented approach also seems to provide a
ISE, the processing of serial order in the sound is in conflict better interpretation of the results reported here than
with the processing of serial order in the primary task; the attentional resource-based accounts of disruption from
interference may be construed as the residual cost of irrelevant sound (Cowan, 1995; Neath, 2000; see also
mechanisms (perhaps inhibition; see Hughes & Jones, Lange, 2005). For example, the data seem particularly at
2003) designed to resolve a ‘competition-for-action’ con- odds with an attentional capture approach (Cowan, 1995;
ferred by the order cues generated from irrelevant and rel- Elliott, 2002; see also Lange, 2005). On this account, the
evant sources of information. This approach explains why classical ISE is the result of acoustic changes-in-state from
neither the meaningfulness of irrelevant speech nor be- one irrelevant item to the next causing an orienting re-
tween-sequence semantic similarity plays a role in the dis- sponse away (or capturing attention) from the focal task.
ruption when serial recall is instructed (Experiments 3 and At first glance, the account might be readily extended to
4; see also Buchner et al., 1996 and Jones et al., 1990): In deal with the kind of semantic auditory distraction effects
this case it is the information that the irrelevant sound observed here by appeal to the notion that between-se-
yields about serial order, not its meaning, that is broadly quence semantic similarity would also give rise to orient-
compatible with the action (or process) of serial rehearsal. ing responses via the priming of semantic features of the
The interference-by-process approach also explains why sound based on the semanticity of the to-be-recalled items
the meaning of speech becomes disruptive to the perfor- (cf. Cowan, 1995). However, as acknowledged by Saults
mance of free recall tasks only when semantic-categoriza- and Cowan (2007), recent evidence indicates that the
tion is an obvious or instructed strategy (Experiments 1-4). changing-state effect and attentional capture effects are
When the primary task involves dynamic semantic encod- functionally distinct (Hughes, Vachon, & Jones, 2005;
ing and retrieval processes—unlike the case with serial re- Hughes et al., 2007). For example, attentional capture ef-
call—the irrelevant semantic information extracted from fects are evident in short-term memory tasks that are as-
the speech produces competition for these processes. sumed not to engage a seriation process (missing-item
Impairment can thus be understood in terms of a relative task, cf. Beaman & Jones, 1997; Buschke, 1963) whereas
difficulty in selecting the correct source of semantic infor- the changing-state effect is contingent on seriation in the
mation as they both compete for the category of action focal task (Hughes et al., 2007). In relation to the present
being called for in the semantic recall task. That be- data, there is no reason to suppose that semantic features
tween-sequence semantic similarity produces more inter- of irrelevant sound should be primed—and hence endowed
ference than mere meaningfulness (Experiment 2 and 4) with attentional capturing power—any less by the to-be-
is particularly supportive of a process-oriented account: recalled items under seriation instructions than under
The irrelevant speech in this case specifies highly con- semantic-categorization instructions. Based on the notion
text-compatible, but ultimately response-inappropriate, that the priming of semantic features is underpinned by
information in the context of the semantic recall task. automatic semantic priming (e.g., Neely, 1976), attentional
One potentially problematic finding for the process-ori- capture due to the meaningfulness of the sound or due to
ented approach is that meaningless irrelevant speech between-sequence semantic similarity would be expected
disrupted recall of categorizable lists when semantic-cate- even when serial recall is emphasized because automatic
gorization was either spontaneously adopted (Experiments semantic priming occurs ‘full blown’ regardless of the focal
1 and 2) or instructed (Experiments 3 and 4). However, task (see Neely & Kahan, 2001). Thus, that semantic audi-
that pink noise (which does not convey acoustic variation) tory distraction was only produced under semantic-cate-
in Experiment 1 produced comparable disruption to mean- gorization instructions (Experiments 3 and 4) provides
ingless (Welsh) speech suggests that the effect of meaning- further evidence against this approach. The key difficulty
less irrelevant speech in this paradigm is not the typical for attentional resource-based accounts therefore is that
effect—attributable to acoustic variability—that disrupts they are too general; they cannot readily explain why the
serial recall. It seems, therefore, that this effect is a nature of auditory distraction is dictated by the prevailing
general effect of noise that often impairs performance on mental activity, whilst this is an axiomatic tenet of the
tasks that call upon semantic processing (see Smith & interference-by-process approach.
Jones, 1992). One challenge that flows from the view that the disrup-
That the disruption observed in the context of semantic tion reported in the present experiments is produced by a
tasks is determined by organizational processes that are conflict between the semantic processing of the sound and
brought to bear to meet the demands of the instructed re- semantic processing in the focal task is to identify more
trieval strategy (Experiments 3 and 4) suggests it is the precisely the nature of that focal semantic processing. This
process, rather than content, that dictates the degree and is because it is likely that a number of diverse semantic
type of disruption from irrelevant speech. This view of processes contribute to performance on the semantic-cat-
the impairment produced by irrelevant auditory stimuli egorization task, any of which could be potentially dis-
is consistent with a functionalist approach to memory gen- rupted by the meaning of irrelevant speech. For example,
erally which advocates that the goals of the individual and semantic processing is required in the task for at least
the retrieval environment (instructions, cues, task de- the following: (a) identifying the categorical structure of
mands) play a critical role in remembering and forgetting the list (Murphy, 1979); (b) reorganizing list exemplars
(Toth & Hunt, 1999), and according to which attempts to to encode and rehearse same-category-exemplars together
delineate the ‘‘structure(s)” of memory is an ill-rewarded (Weist, 1972); (c) coupling semantic retrieval and
endeavour. rehearsal processes; same-category-exemplars (e.g., ‘‘dog”,
36 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

‘‘horse”) may be automatically (e.g., by spreading activa- and limited capacity is turned on its head: A human per-
tion; Collins & Loftus, 1975), or deliberately, retrieved former’s limited capacity (in an empirical sense) reflects
and rehearsed together upon presentation of a related cat- the achievement of selective attention mechanisms de-
egory-exemplar (‘‘bear”) after intervening unrelated cate- signed to ensure that only task-relevant information as-
gory-exemplars (Weist & Crawford, 1973); and (d) cued sumes the control of goal-directed action, an
search of long-term memory (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, achievement that gives the illusion of a limited capacity
1981; Rundus, 1973), which involves generating list, or in the sense of a hypothetical property of the mind. The
candidate list, category names and exemplars for search qualitatively different impairments that are produced by
of, and retrieval from, long-term lexical-semantic memory different aspects of irrelevant auditory stimuli (e.g., acous-
(Gronlund & Shiffrin, 1986). Although there is already tic or semantic) in different settings (e.g., serial recall ver-
some evidence that the latter generative process is suscep- sus category-exemplar recall) are the manifestation of
tible to semantic-based disruption (Marsh, Hughes, & selective attention mechanisms operating to avoid ‘cross
Jones, 2006, 2008), a more fine-grained analysis of which talk’ from information appropriate to actions or skills in-
other semantic processes are impaired is needed. volved in, but inappropriate to the specific demands of,
The disruption produced by irrelevant auditory stimuli performing a given task. Two streams of information do
seems much better captured by a process-oriented ap- not come into conflict for what they contain but for how
proach (e.g., Hughes & Jones, 2005; Jones & Tremblay, they are processed.
2000) than either a structural (or content-based) approach
(e.g., Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Neath, 2000) or an
Acknowledgement
attentional resource-based approach (e.g., Cowan, 1995;
Lange, 2005; Neath, 2000). Latterly the interference-by-
The research reported in this article received financial
process view has been considered in terms of the selec-
support from an Economic and Social Research Council
tion-for-action approach to attentional selectivity (cf.
(UK) grant awarded to Dylan Jones, William Macken and
Allport, 1993; Hughes & Jones, 2005; Neumann, 1996). This
Robert Hughes (RES-000-22-1526). We thank Steve
approach holds that the planning and execution of a con-
Moshier for allowing us to use his C program to generate
textually-appropriate act—that demanded by the primary
pink noise for Experiment 1.
task—is compromised to the extent that irrelevant infor-
mation is compatible with the act at a general level but
incompatible with it at the level of the particular response
References
required.
On the selection-for-action account, interference from Allport, D. A. (1993). Attention and control: Have we been asking the
the irrelevant information reflects an overhead of the ac- wrong questions? A critical review of 25 years. In D. E. Meyer & S.
tion of mechanisms that prevent it from actually assum- Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV (pp. 183–218).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
ing the control of action. Thus, the nature and extent of Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA:
interference is a joint product of the character of the pri- Harvard University Press.
mary task and the nature of the potentially-distracting Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control
and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language,
information: When both irrelevant and relevant events 49, 415–445.
constitute plausible candidates for the skill deployed to Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
perform the focal task a selection-for-action problem is Basden, D. R., & Basden, B. H. (1995). Some tests of the strategy disruption
interpretation of part-set cuing inhibition. Journal of
generated. For example, the classical ISE with serial recall Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21,
may be conceived as a residual cost of preventing the 1656–1669.
competing irrelevant source of order information from Basden, D. R., Basden, B. H., & Stephens, J. P. (2002). Part-set cuing of order
information in recall tests. Journal of Memory and Language, 47,
assuming control of the skill of serial rehearsal deployed
517–529.
to retain the to-be-remembered sequence (Hughes & Beaman, C. P. (2004). The irrelevant sound effect revisited: What role for
Jones, 2005). working memory capacity? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 1106–1118.
We would argue that the strength of the process-ori-
Beaman, C. P., & Jones, D. M. (1997). Role of serial order in the irrelevant
ented approach, embedded in the selection-for-action speech effect: Tests of the changing-state hypothesis. Journal of
view, derives from its questioning of the more generally re- Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23,
ceived wisdom that attentional selectivity is imposed by 459–471.
Beaman, C. P., & Jones, D. M. (1998). Irrelevant sound disrupts order
some shortfall of the cognitive system as is traditionally information in free as in serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
held by structuralist approaches (e.g., a limitation on pro- Psychology, 51A, 615–636.
cessing or a limited attentional resource or set of re- Bousfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of
randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49,
sources; Cowan, 1995; Neath, 2000). Instead, the 229–240.
selection-for-action view is that selection (for action) is Bower, G. H., Clark, M. C., Lesgold, A. M., & Winzenz, D. (1969).
the problem to be solved (not the solution) and limitations Hierarchical retrieval schemes in recall of categorized word lists.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 323–343.
in performance (including susceptibility to disruption from Brainerd, C. J., Payne, D. G., Wright, R., & Reyna, V. F. (2003). Phantom
irrelevant information) are the consequence of adaptive recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 445–467.
mechanisms designed to resolve that selection problem Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organisation
of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(for extensive discussions, see Allport, 1993; Neumann,
Buchner, A., Irmen, L., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). On the irrelevance of
1996; see also Anderson, 2003). Thus, on the selection- semantic information in the ‘irrelevant speech’ effect. Quarterly
for-action approach, the relationship between selectivity Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 765–779.
J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38 37

Buchner, A., Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & Mehl, B. (2004). Valence of Jones, D. M., Madden, C., & Miles, C. (1992). Privileged access by
distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial irrelevant speech to short-term memory: The role of changing-
recall. Memory and Cognition, 32, 722–731. state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 645–
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1992). Toward a network model of the 669.
articulatory loop. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 429–460. Jones, D. M., Miles, C., & Page, J. (1990). Disruption of proof-reading by
Burns, D. J., & Brown, C. A. (2000). The category access measure of irrelevant speech: Effects of attention, arousal or memory? Applied
relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Cognitive Psychology, 4, 89–108.
Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1057–1062. Jones, D. M., & Tremblay, S. (2000). Interference in memory by process or
Buschke, H. (1963). Relative retention in immediate memory determined content? A reply to Neath (2000). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7,
by the missing scan method. Nature, 200, 1129–1130. 550–558.
Colle, H. A., & Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Lange, E. B. (2005). Disruption of attention by irrelevant stimuli in serial
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 17–32. recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 513–531.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of LeCompte, D. C., Neely, C. B., & Wilson, J. R. (1997). Irrelevant speech and
semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. irrelevant tones: The relative importance of speech to the irrelevant
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: speech effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
Oxford University Press. and Cognition, 23, 472–483.
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In LeCompte, D. C., & Shaibe, D. M. (1997). On the irrelevance of
A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of phonological similarity to the irrelevant speech effect. Quarterly
active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cambridge: Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 100–118.
Cambridge University Press. Macken, W. J., Phelps, F., & Jones, D. M. (in press). What causes auditory
Ellermeier, W., & Zimmer, K. (1997). Individual differences in the distraction? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review.
susceptibility to the ‘irrelevant speech effect’. Journal of the Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2006). Auditory distraction at
Acoustical Society of America, 102, 2191–2199. the interface of selective attention and semantic memory. Paper
Elliott, E. M. (2002). The irrelevant speech effect in children: Theoretical presented at the 4th annual Auditory Perception Cognition and Action
implications of developmental change. Memory and Cognition, 30, meeting, Houston, Texas.
478–487. Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2008). Auditory distraction in
Farley, L. A., Neath, I., Allbritton, D. W., & Surprenant, A. M. (2007). semantic memory: A process-oriented approach. Journal of Memory
Irrelevant speech effects and sequence learning. Memory and and Language, 58, 682–700.
Cognition, 35, 156–165. Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (submitted for publication).
Frankel, F., & Cole, M. (1971). Measures of category clustering in free Auditory distraction by meaningful speech at the intersection of
recall. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 39–44. selective attention and long-term semantic memory.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Martin, R. C., Wogalter, M. S., & Forlano, J. G. (1988). Reading
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. comprehension in the presence of unattended speech and music.
Greitzer, F. L. (1976). Intra-category rehearsal in list learning. Journal of Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 382–398.
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 641–654. McGeoch, J. A. (1942). The psychology of human learning. New York:
Gronlund, S. D., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1986). Retrieval strategies in recall of Longmans, Green.
natural categories and categorized lists. Journal of Experimental Mensink, J. G., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model of interference and
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 550–561. forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434–455.
Henson, R. N. A., Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Hitch, G. J., & Flude, B. (2003). Mueller, C. W., & Watkins, M. J. (1977). Inhibition from part-set cuing: A
Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order cue-overload interpretation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing signal hypothesis. Behavior, 16, 699–709.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1307–1334. Murphy, M. D. (1979). Measurement of category clustering in free recall.
Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in In C. R. Puff (Ed.), Memory organization and structure (pp. 51–83). San
selective attention. In A. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory Diego, CA: Academic Press.
mechanisms in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–111). San Nairne, J. S. (1990). A feature model of immediate memory. Memory and
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Cognition, 18, 251–269.
Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2003). A negative order-repetition priming Nairne, J. S. (2002). Remembering over the short-term: The case
effect: Inhibition of order in unattended sequences? Journal of against the standard model. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 53–81.
199–218. Neath, I. (2000). Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory.
Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2005). The impact of order incongruence Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 403–423.
between a task-irrelevant auditory sequence and a task-relevant Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2001). The irrelevant sound effect is not
visual sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception always the same as the irrelevant speech effect. In H. L. Roediger, III, J.
and Performance, 31, 316–327. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of
Hughes, R. W., Vachon, F., & Jones, D. M. (2005). Auditory attentional remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 247–265).
capture during serial recall: Violations at encoding of an algorithm- Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
based neural model? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Neely, J. H. (1976). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory:
Memory and Cognition, 31, 736–749. Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes. Memory and
Hughes, R. W., Vachon, F., & Jones, D. M. (2007). Disruption of short-term Cognition, 4, 648–654.
memory by changing and deviant sounds: Support for a duplex- Neely, J. H., & Kahan, T. A. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic: A
mechanism account of auditory distraction. Journal of Experimental critical evaluation. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M.
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1050–1061. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert
Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and G. Crowder. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 421–445. Neely, C. B., & LeCompte, D. C. (1999). The importance of semantic
Jones, D. M. (1993). Objects, streams and threads of auditory attention. In similarity to the irrelevant speech effect. Memory and Cognition, 27,
A. D. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness 37–44.
and control (pp. 167–198). Oxford: Clarendon Press. Neumann, O. (1996). Theories of attention. In O. Neumann & A. F. Sanders
Jones, D. M., & Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an (Eds.). Handbook of perception and action (Vol. 3, pp. 389–446).
irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in London: Academic Press.
working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Norris, D. G., Baddeley, A. D., & Page, M. P. A. (2004). Retroactive effects of
Memory and Cognition, 19, 369–381. irrelevant speech on serial recall from short-term memory. Journal of
Jones, D. M., & Macken, W. J. (1995). Phonological similarity in the Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30,
irrelevant speech effect: Within- or between-stream similarity. 1093–1105.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Oberauer, K., & Lange, E. B. (2008). Interference in working memory:
21, 103–115. Distinguishing similarity-based confusion, feature overwriting, and
Jones, D. M., Macken, W. J., & Nicholls, A. P. (2004). The phonological store feature migration. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 730–745.
of working memory: Is it phonological and is it a store? Journal of Oberauer, K., Lange, E. B., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Working memory capacity
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, and resistance to interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 51,
656–674. 80–96.
38 J.E. Marsh et al. / Cognition 110 (2009) 23–38

Oswald, C. J. P., Tremblay, S., & Jones, D. M. (2000). Disruption of Smith, A. P., & Jones, D. M. (1992). Noise and performance. In Handbook of
comprehension by the meaning of irrelevant sound. Memory, 8, human performance. In A. P. Smith & D. M. Jones (Eds.). The physical
345–350. environment (Vol. 1, pp. 1–28). London: Academic Press.
Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. G. (2003). The irrelevant sound effect: What Smith, A. P., Jones, D. M., & Broadbent, D. E. (1981). The effects of noise on
needs modelling, and a tentative model. Quarterly Journal of recall of categorized lists. British Journal of Psychology, 72,
Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1289–1300. 299–316.
Perham, N., Banbury, S. P., & Jones, D. M. (2007). Reduction in auditory Surprenant, A. M., Neath, I., & Bireta, T. J. (2007). Changing-state and the
distraction by retrieval strategy. Memory, 15, 465–473. irrelevant sound effect. Canadian Acoustics, 35, 86–87.
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative Toth, J. P., & Hunt, R. R. (1999). Not one versus many, but zero versus any:
memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93–134. Structure and function in the context of the multiple memory systems
Rouibah, A., Tiberghien, G., & Lupker, S. J. (1999). Phonological and debate. In J. K. Foster & M. Jelicic (Eds.), Memory: Systems, process, or
semantic priming: Evidence for task-independent effects. Memory and function? (pp 232–272). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cognition, 27, 422–437. Tulving, E. (1968). Theoretical issues in free recall. In T. R. Dixon & D. L.
Rundus, D. (1973). Negative effects of using list items as retrieval cues. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory (pp. 2–36).
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 43–50. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category
unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and
memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 150–164. Montague (1969) norms. Journal of Memory and Language, 50,
Saults, J. S., & Cowan, N. (2007). A central capacity limit to the 289–335.
simultaneous storage of visual and auditory arrays in working Weist, R. M. (1972). The role of rehearsal: Recopy or reconstruct? Journal
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 663–684. of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 440–450.
Sheffert, S. M., Pisoni, D. B., Fellowes, J. M., & Remez, R. E. (2002). Learning Weist, R. M., & Crawford, C. (1973). Sequential versus organized rehearsal.
to recognize talkers from natural, sinewave, and reversed speech Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 237–241.
samples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Wingfield, A., Lindfield, K. C., & Kahana, M. J. (1998). Adult age differences
Performance, 28, 1447–1469. in the temporal characteristics of category free recall. Psychology and
Shuell, T. J. (1968). Retroactive inhibition in free-recall learning of Aging, 13, 256–266.
categorized lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, Yoon, C., Feinberg, F., Hu, P., Gutchess, A. H., Hedden, T., Chen, H., et al
797–805. (2004). Category norms as a function of culture and age: Comparisons
Shuell, T. J. (1969). Clustering and organization in free recall. Psychological of item responses to 105 categories by American and Chinese adults.
Bulletin, 72, 353–374. Psychology and Aging, 19, 379–393.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi