Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 18

The relationship of Nivkh to Chukotko-Kamchatkan revisited

Michael Fortescue *
Institute of Scandinavian Studies and Linguistics, University of Copenhagen, Njalsgade 120, 2300 Copenhagen S, Denmark
1. Introduction
The current consensus as regards the interrelationship of the so-called Paleosiberian (or Paleoasiatic) languages is
that there is no such relationship, merely a collection of all the non-Altaic languages of eastern Siberia, as originally put
under the rubric Paleoaziatskij by Schrenck (1883). It was intended to cover Chukotko-Kamchatkan (i.e. Chukotian plus
Itelmen), Nivkh (Gilyak), Yukaghir and Yeniseian (later sometimes extended to Ainu and Eskimo-Aleut cf. Comrie,
1981:239). Nivkh in particular is regarded today as an isolate displaying a number of dialects but with little historical
depth between them (cf. Mattissen, 2003:4). The unity of the Chukotko-Kamchatkan family (CK) within Schrencks
grouping is on the other hand well established (cf. Fortescue, 2005), despite some lingering doubts as to the exact
relationship of Itelmen to the northern branch on the part of Russian specialists (cf. Maslova, 2001). With Yeniseic now
on its way to being rmly related to Na-Dene (cf. Vajda, 2010), and Yukaghirs probable relationship to Uralic and/or
Eskimo-Aleut still being the most likely hypothesis proposed (cf. Fortescue, 1998:44ff), it is time to take another look at
the two remaining groups, CK and Nivkh (itself once presumably part of a larger Amuric family
1
), and reconsider whether
there might still be a special relationship between them that could salvage at least part of Schrencks grouping as
genetically valid. Any progress in relating these distant languages one to the other or to completely different language
families must be based on the relationship between the individual reconstructed proto-languages concerned, not on
Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376
A R T I C L E I N F O
Article history:
Received 30 July 2010
Received in revised form 2 March 2011
Accepted 3 March 2011
Available online 8 April 2011
Keywords:
Paleosiberian
Chukotko-Kamchatkan
Nivkh
Genetic relationship
A B S T R A C T
With the availability today of reliable materials for comparing the languages that in the
past have been lumped together under the rubric Paleosiberian it has become possible to
reassess the genetic relationship or lack of it between the individual languages of this
traditional grouping. It will be demonstrated that the case for the genetic relationship of
two of the constituent groups, Chukotko-Kamchatkan and the isolated Amuric language
Nivkh (Gilyak), is actually quite strong, although the rest of the grouping must indeed be
abandoned as a genetic unit. A case is made for reconstructing a Chukokto-Kamchatkan-
Amuric proto-language associated with the Neolithic of the Lower Amur and adjacent
coasts of the Sea of Okhotsk. Emphasis is laid especially on the morphology and shared
typological features of these languages, but numerous lexical items based on systematic
sound correspondences are also introduced. A plausible archaeological framework
elucidating the evident closeness of the two linguistic entities is also sketched.
2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
* Tel.: +45 3532 3660; fax: +45 3532 8377.
E-mail address: fortesq@hum.ku.dk.
1
This term is due to Janhunen (1996:225), who suggests a Manchurian origin for the family on the Upper Amur during the Neolithic period. He further
tentatively proposes that the Kamchukotic homeland was on the coast of the Sea of Okhotsk immediately north of the Lower Amur region (Janhunen,
1996:237). I shall continue to refer to Nivkh as a family (of which it is the one surviving language), though its historical depth is quite shallow. The incursion
of Tungusic speakers fromboth south and west has undoubtedly conned the territory of the language to a much smaller area than it once covered, with the
ensuing demise or merging of earlier dialect and even language varieties.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Lingua
j our nal homepage: www. el sevi er . com/ l ocat e/ l i ngua
0024-3841/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2011.03.001
unanalysed contemporary lexical sources (which vary greatly in accuracy). A direct comparison of this sort is now
possible between Chukotko-Kamchatkan and Nivkh thanks to the reconstructions for the former in Fortescue (2005) plus
the comparative Nivkh les that I have since assembled, based on all the published lexical sources available to me
(Japanese as well as Russian) plus the comparative work of Hattori, Jakobson and Austerlitz, as well as the grammatical
descriptions of Krejnovic, Panlov, Gruzdeva and Mattissen. These are all included in the references below. Proto-Nivkh
forms can be set up according to the criterion that they should be reected in at least one northwestern dialect, i.e. the
mainland Amur dialect (A) plus that of the northwestern shore of Sakhalin opposite (NS), and at least one from the
eastern Sakhalin dialects, i.e. East Sakhalin (ES) and South Sakhalin (SS).
2
This results in some 1100 lexical sets plus 120
common afxes and postpositions.
This comparativebasis is relativelyshallow, arguablyonlyinvolvingdialects of a singlelanguage, andthe forms I reconstruct
for the proto-language are thus very close to the modern forms in the more conservative south-eastern dialects. Standard
comparative procedures can nevertheless be applied in order to reconstruct earlier stages of the language with Nivkh one at
least benets from the rich possibilities for internal reconstruction, a point stressed by Austerlitz (1982, 1984, 1990a).
A methodological paradox arises here, however: the level of Proto-Nivkh that I reconstruct from comparing contemporary
forms is shallower than that Austerlitz aims at, at least in some of his reconstructions, which must be assigned to various
pre-proto stages on my denition. My hypothetical Proto-CKA (Proto-Chukotko-Kamchatko-Amuric) may or may not lie
still deeper than any of these stages.
Various attempts to relate Nivkh to Paleosiberian and/or other languages have been made in the past, but none of
them has passed beyond listing a few dozen look-alikes (see Mattissen, 2003:4 for detailed references). The most serious
attempt was undertaken by Tailleur (1960), who limited his focus specically to Nivkh and Chukotko-Kamchatkan, but
this was premature, being based on unreliable data from individual languages, without any preliminary family-internal
reconstruction.
3
Later attempts to relate Nivkh to larger groupings such as hypothetical Nostratic were no more
convincing, culminating in the attempt by Mudrak and Nikolaev (1989) to relate it at one and the same time to
Chukotko-Kamchatkan and to Greenbergs controversial Almosan-Keresiouan. Although their brief list does cite
reconstructed proto-forms as far as possible it makes hardly any attempt to sort out detailed sound correspondences
between families. Of these earlier lists of look-alikes, many (if not most) must in fact be rejected now that Proto-CK can
be directly compared to Proto-Nivkh. It will be seen in what follows that a much stronger case can be made today for
relating Nivkh with CK than was possible for Tailleur, although the evidence will not be sufcient to declare
unequivocally that there is a direct genetic link between the two families comparable to the one that can be established
for the relationship between, say, Chukotian and Itelmen. Given that we are dealing here with a very distant relationship
it is too early to claim nal proof of genetic relationship what I hope to achieve, however, is a signicant increase in the
likelihood of such a relationship, bolstered by the relative perspicacity of the reconstructions presented. This will
principally involve the area of morphology. Although the lexical evidence is slender, it will be seen to involve basic
vocabulary, of the kind unlikely to have been borrowed between neighbouring languages. The list of potential
correlates between Nivkh and CK presented in Fortescue (1998) taken mostly from Tailleur can no longer be
interpreted as reecting simple borrowings. Many of them can in fact be dismissed as chance look-alikes, though, as we
shall see, a residue does remain, a residue that can be increased by more convincing parallels not noticed earlier. A
further justication for this new attempt is the light that the broader perspective sheds on some of the more puzzling
morphological and phonological details that have remained unexplained in both Nivkh and CK individually, such as the
historical source (and original semantic purport) of the Nivkh undergoer prex for unspecied object and the CK
inverse prex of similar shape, and of the areal nature of the remnant vowel harmony system of Nivkh and the strangely
skewed vowel harmony system of CK.
The earliest homelandof the northeasternPaleoasiatics (i.e. Chukotko-Kamchatkans) was, according to Levin(1963:283),
along the northern coasts of the Okhotsk Sea (where dwellings of the Nivkh and Kamchatkan type with winter roof entrances
havebeenexcavatedinconnectionwiththeOldKoryak cultureof thelatter part of therst millenniumBC).
4
TheNivkh, onthe
other hand, are believedtobe descendants of the oldest inhabitants of the territoryaroundthe mouthof the Amur whichthey
now occupy and further along the adjacent mainland coasts. The later Okhotsk Culture of the 5th to 13th century AD that
spread fromthe southern shores of Sakhalin to northern Hokkaido and up along the Kurile islands towards Kamchatka is also
considered to have been borne by the Nivkh (Levin, 1963:118). Vasilevskii (1969:153) specically relates the Old Koryak and
Okhotsk cultures and suggests their common origin. In the Conclusion I shall return to the archaeological evidence supporting
the idea of a common origin for contemporary Chukotko-Kamchatkan and Nivkh groups in the Neolithic of the Amur region,
2
The principal phonological differences between the Amur and the East Sakhalin dialects are: */a/ has reduced to // in many (but by no means all) initial
stemsyllables and /v/ and /w/ have merged as /v/ in A; nal /r/ (a sufx on nouns) has become /s/ and nal // (when representing an original sufx on both
nouns and verbs) has been lost in A. North Sakhalin is intermediate. South Sakhalin further differs fromES by maintaining a probably original distinction
between a lenis vs. fortis (or voiced vs. voiceless) initial consonant series as opposed to the unaspirated vs. aspirated one developed elsewhere along with
the introduction of a new, third voiced series as the result of nasal sandhi (I return to this below). The two series are conated (as fortes) in clusters (and
generally later in the word). For a precise account of the secondary origin of initial fricatives in Nivkh see Jakobson (1971:85ff).
3
As a single example of a dubious lexical comparison (one that Tailleur makes much of) consider the form lewlew-, which he gives as trick, deceive for
East Sakhalin, supposedly the same as Chukotian lewlew-et of that meaning. The nearest one can actually get to that formin the modern dictionaries is either
valt- lie, deceive or laulaud argue.
4
Compare the possible CK cognates of Nivkh tom(s) smoke-hole in the section on lexical material below.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1360
with the Tokareva culture of the shore of the Sea of Okhotsk of about 3500 BP as the missing link. In Fig. 1 the approximate
extent of these early cultures (in capitals) has been superimposed on the extent of the relevant languages in the region today.
Given the results of the present investigation, the general conclusion in Fortescue (1998) as to the relationship
between CK and the hypothetical Uralo-Siberian mesh needs to be readjusted somewhat.
5
I would no longer wish to
relate CK directly to that mesh, although I believe that some of the lexical evidence adduced for a link with it will hold up
in terms of borrowing/diffusion. The more important morphological and grammatical links of CK to other languages
would now seem, however, to point denitely towards the south, to Nivkh, a possibility that I left open in my
monograph.
One reason this relationship has not been more apparent in the past is the relative dearth of inectional morphology
in Nivkh (as opposed to its profusion in CK). The most reasonable starting point for a renewed line of attack on this matter
is to assume that Nivkh has been more worn down by contact with more numerous neighbours than has CK, which has
remained more isolated, spread over the sparsely populated tundra and adjacent icy shores to the north. Much of what
follows will be devoted to demonstrating how there are traces of an earlier, somewhat more complex morphology in
Nivkh that can be correlated with the relatively better preserved situation in CK. The sufxal morphology of the CK
languages has also been worn down (to be partially replaced by prexes), and these languages can be shown to have
innovated much of their inectional complexity since Proto-CK times, as reconstructed in Fortescue (2003). There are in
fact numerous typological traits (some quite unusual) shared by the two families that suggest genesis from a common
stock.
What are these common traits? First, as regards word-order, Proto-CK almost certainly displayed SOV order (although
it is fairly free in modern Chukotian, these languages display the verb-auxiliary, adjective-noun and possessor-noun
order expected of this type), as does Nivkh. Unsurprisingly, the morphology in both families is primarily sufxing CK
prexes are demonstrably more recent, corresponding to the expected pre-verbal position of subject, object and
adverbial constituents (cf. Fortescue, 2003:85). There are also postpositions/enclitics in both families (bound in Nivkh
and now mainly sufxal in CK). More signicantly, both show a considerable degree of polysynthesis in general and
(arguably) of incorporation in particular, both verbal and nominal (i.e. of nominal adjuncts as well as verbal objects),
although not all descriptions of Nivkh (e.g. Panlov, 1962:25ff) have agreed on the incorporation analysis. The specic
dependent-head variety of incorporation that typies Nivkh according to Mattissen (2003:104ff) can be seen also to
characterize CK as regards the dominant order of incorporated elements, namely dependent elements (objects, attributes
or adverbial converbs) before heads (verbs or nominal phrasal heads).
6
Although Georg and Volodin (1999:229) cite the
lack of incorporation in contemporary (western) Itelmen as a sign of the lack of unity of that family, its loss can be related
to the relative newness and transparent productivity of the fully-blown phenomenon in Chukotian and/or the
abrasion of Itelmen under Russian inuence (the surviving western language has been much inuenced by both Russian
and Koryak/Alutor).

Eskimo
Chukchi
Kolyma
Even
OLD KORYAK
TOKAREVA
Evenki
Koryak
Kerek
Alutor
Itelmen
Amur
Nivkh (A + NS)

Sakhalin
Nivkh (ES
+ SS)
Ainu
Hokkaido
Kamchatka
Kuriles
OKHOTSK
Sea of
Okhotsk
Fig. 1. Languages and cultures of the Sea of Okhotsk region.
5
This was proposed to cover Eskimo-Aleut, Yukaghir and Uralic as well as (much more tentatively) CK itself.
6
Summing up the structural parallelisms between polysynthesis in Chukchi and other northern languages and Nivkh dependent-head synthesis,
Mattissen (2003:288) states: Especially between Chukchi and Nivkh, differences seem to be of degree rather than kind.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1361
Other potentially diagnostic traits include the lack of distinction between grammatical case forms (nominative and
accusative also genitive) in both families, although Chukotian (not Itelmen) has developed ergative case marking and both
display locative case systems on nominals
7
; the lack of a special adjective class (there are quality/quantity verbs rather);
auxiliary verbs; dual number (only residual in Nivkh and perhaps not original in CK); indenite object/antipassive-like
prexes on transitive verbs; uctuating nasal (and other consonantal) endings on nominals that may reect earlier noun
classes (the so-called singulative markers of CK); and a distinctive root retraction (or high/low) form of vowel harmony
(with only traces left in Nivkh and Itelmen, but virtually intact in Chukotian). It should be pointed out that this cluster of
traits constitutes a quite different prole from those of Eskimo-Aleut, Yukaghir or Yeniseian. As regards other phonological
traits, both families display a rather similar and not particularly complex array of phonological units (see below),
although Nivkh appears to have undergone considerably more syncope than CK (with the egregious exception of Itelmen),
resulting in more complex consonant clusters and the contraction of what once must have been bisyllabic stems into
monosyllables (internal dialect differences and also distinct prosodic patterns in Nivkh help clarify this cf. Austerlitz,
1990a:21).
As for the major divergences between the two families that require explanation, these include ergativity in CK (a
secondary phenomenon, as discussed in Fortescue, 1997, 2003) but not in Nivkh; a complex system of numeral classiers in
Nivkh but not in CK; evidential/epistemic mood marking in Nivkh but not in CK; exclusive vs. inclusive 1st person in Nivkh
but not in CK; a reexive vs. non-reexive distinction in possessive markers in Nivkh but not in CK; reduplication of stems in
both families, but used for different purposes
8
; distinctive sandhi-like alternations at word as well as morpheme boundaries
in Nivkh (phonologically determined but utilized for morphosyntactic purposes)
9
; circumxes in CK; and a complex system
of verbal paradigms for mood/tense/aspect in CK (including remnants of an inverse system) but not in Nivkh.
10
All of these
will be touched upon in what follows.
2. The common sound system
The phonological system of the hypothetical proto-language let us call it Proto-Chukotko-Kamchatko-Amuric (or,
somewhat less unwieldy, Proto-CKA) can be set up here as an initial approximation at least. Internal reconstruction for
Nivkh (e.g. Austerlitz, 1990a) and comparative work on CK (Fortescue, 2005) converge on a symmetrical six vowel system
(plus schwa, outside of the vowel harmony system in CK see below) and a consonant system involving a velar/uvular
distinction and a single voiceless (or fortis) vs. voiced (or lenis) plosive series, with corresponding voiced fricatives, liquids
and nasals.
11
The combining of a single series of voiced rather than voiceless fricatives with a plosive system like this is
typologically rather unusual, but is denitely what is found in CK. The voiceless fricatives and initial fricatives in general
are of secondary origin in both families (cf. Fortescue, 2005:7ff for CK and Austerlitz, 1990a:20 for Nivkh). The distinction
between two open vowels /a/ and // (back/dominant vs. front/recessive) of Proto-CK is not found in Nivkh, and may
indirectly reect the decay of vowel harmony in that language, the source I reconstruct for CK front//, namely */L/,
becoming // or /a/ in that language. The different reexes here may reect position respectively within morphemes
originally of recessive/high vs. dominant/low harmony.
The two plosive series hypothesizedonthe basis of Nivkh must have collapsedinCK,
12
and most Nivkh dialects developeda
distinct voicedseries of plosives inconnectionwithnasal sandhi or nasal alternation (fromsequences of weak nasals cf.
Mattissen, 2003:29 plus plosives, the result remaining even if the nasal subsequently dropped). All dialects like Itelmen
within CK developed independent voiceless fricatives under certain phonotactic conditions (in Nivkh through the
fricativization of plosives including that of */t/ to /r/). Austerlitz (1990a:25) supports the idea of the secondary nature of
the three-way distinction in plosives of all dialects of Nivkh except South Sakhalin, but goes too far to my mind in suggesting
mainly on areal grounds that this might further be reduced to one.
I am also sceptical of his suggestion (Austerlitz, 1990a:20) that the velar vs. uvular distinction is secondary (at least as
regards my CKA level) there are just too many contrasting initial velar/uvular plus (same) vowel forms reconstructible in
7
Besides its locative case endings, Nivkh has an unmarked nominative case (like Itelmen and corresponding to the Chukotian absolutive), an
instrumental case and one specialized for standard of comparison, also a causee case -(a)X, called dative-accusative in Saveljeva and Taksami (1965)
and used in causative constructions with animate causee (perhaps originally a dative). Chukotian also has an instrumental case (used alongside the locative
to mark the ergative subject on nouns), two comitative cases, as well as an attributive and a referential case. In both families case endings can to some
extent be applied to bare verbal stems, e.g. in the Amur Nivkh supine and the CK innitive, both treated below.
8
For repeated, intense or reciprocal action with verbs in Nivkh and for the basic forms of certain nouns that do not take singulative sufxes in CK. There
are, however, some reduplicated nouns in Nivkh, like kff spider (and reduplication of nouns can also indicate plurality cf. Mattissen, 2003:8).
9
I use the word sandhi throughout this paper rather loosely it has been the subject of considerable debate whether the phenomenon is best
characterized by this termor not. Certainly it goes further than changes at morpheme boundaries between free words, as suggested by Panlovs use of the
term (see Mattissen, 2003:65ff for a discussion).
10
Most noticeable in this area is the almost total lack of person (subject) marking on verbs in Nivkh: it is only present in the distinct 1s and plural person
endings in converbs.
11
In the South Sakhalin dialect of Nivkh the distinction is between lenis and fortis, overt only in initial position before a vowel Austerlitz writes lenis /d/
vs. fortis /t/, etc. in that position (corresponding to /t/ vs. /t/ in other dialects), elsewhere neutralized to /t/, etc. The aspirated vs. plain distinction is also rare
outside of initial position in other dialects (a medial aspirated plosive may reect the rst phoneme in an originally independent word, as in postpositional
-tx on discussed below).
12
Compare Southern Wakashan, which has conated without trace the original voiced/voiceless plosive distinction preserved in Northern Wakashan
(Sapir and Swadesh, 1952).
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1362
what I call Proto-Nivkh, though the origin of uvulars may well have been due to anking vowels (as Austerlitz suggests) at a
still earlier stage (pre-Proto-CKA).
13
It is certainly true that initial uvulars may only appear before /a/ or /o/ in modern Nivkh
(Jakobson, 1971:84) and an initial velar has gone to the corresponding uvular before /a/ in many ES/SS forms that retain the
velar plus reduced // < */a/ in the Amur dialect, such as A kj sail, ES qaj (< *kaj). Original *qa-, by contrast, is kept in both
dialects, as in the stemof this shape meaning go downstream. This may also explain such medial correspondences as A cir
(< *cigar-?), ES cXar tree, wood, stick, where, as Austerlitz proposes (1994a:229), the uvular in ES and SS is due to the
following /a/ (plus a shift of stress in A and conation of /cX-/ with /cX-/ in ES) compare Chukotian cik()l() pole for
training reindeer with as a likely cognate, going directly with Nivkh tla shaft of spear, which Panlov (1962:197) relates to
cir plus -la, the numeral classier ending for poles.
Further differences among the contemporary languages can be rather simply (if speculatively) explained such as the
aspirated plosive series in most Nivkh (but not in CK), which probably just reects the original voiceless (or fortis) series. The
distinct palatal series of Nivkh appears also to have been original, collapsed in CK except for the distinct plosive /c/ (=*/t/),
but with some further residue (especially in Koryak) utilized for affective purposes. The origin of the glottal stop and
pharyngeals found in some CK (also the ejectives of Itelmen) are discussed in Fortescue (1998, 2003). In general, Occams
razor has been applied in providing a coherent picture of the probable original system. This can be laid out in the following
manner.
The specic sound correspondences between Nivkh and CK that lie behind this reconstruction (as reected in the lexical
and morphological material presented in this paper) can be summarized in Table 1. The complete phoneme inventory for
Proto-CKA is reconstructed in Table 2.
I assume that *//generally merged with */r/in Nivkh (as in Chukchi) note that/r/is treated as a voiced fricative as regards
sandhi alternations there. The lack of a sibilant is also notable in Table 2 in contemporary Nivkh /s/ and /z/ are the result of
the regular sandhi/transitivizing alternation of */t/ and */d/ (though in some cases medial /z/ appears to have developed
from */r/)
14
, and /s/ has developed from earlier /c/ (*/t/) in Chukchi and Alutor; /z/ or /s/ is generally from */j/ in Itelmen
(Fortescue, 2005:7ff). I cannot accept Austerlitz (1990a:19) very tentative, typologically based proposal that/h/might have
come froma proto-phoneme */s/. The reconstruction of /h/ in Table 2 requires further comment it is absent in CK although
common in Nivkh (initial only). I shall return to this matter in the section on lexical material. I do follow Austerlitz in
assuming that initial consonant clusters in Nivkh reect the loss of an intermediate unstressed vowel that was lost
(Austerlitz, 1990a:21), initial clusters being secondary, much as in the more conservative CK languages Chukchi and Koryak,
Table 1
Sound correspondences between Nivkh and CK.
CK Nivkh
p, t, c, k, q p, t, c, k, q
a
p, t, c, k, q
v, , , R v, r/z, , R
m, n, n, m, n, n,
w, j w, j
l, r l, r/z
h
i, u i, u
e, o e, o

a/
a a
a
This aspirated series could be written p, t, c, k, q, and the plain series belowit as b, d, d, g, G (reecting the conservative SS dialect). The voiced series of
the other dialects developed fromnasal sandhi, and the voiceless fricative series f, r, s, x, Xby ordinary (non-nasal) sandhi fromthe rst series (the result of
ordinary sandhi of the second series falls together with the voiced fricative series below).
Table 2
Reconstructed phoneme inventory for Proto-CKA.
i () u p t t k q
e L o b d d g G
a v R
m n n
w j h
l
r
13
Note that root retraction vowel harmony usually presupposes uvular or pharyngeal segments as a post-vocalic trigger that may or not then disappear
(cf. Bessell, 1992:167ff). The origins of the uvular/velar distinction and of root retraction vowel harmony are at all events probably related here.
14
As in Acuz-, ES cir- new, comparable with CKtur- new, and ES kuz- go out, comparable with CKtkur- go off. There is sporadic inter-dialect variation
between these two phonemes, as in the rst form cited here, also A hurur, ES huzus everywhere, A jaz-d, ES jar-d bite, but also A zozu-d, ES roru-d
extinguish and A vazud, ES varud sew strips of net.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1363
which still by and large disallow such clusters (and where medial clusters are limited to two consonants). There is also
conrmatory evidence for his reconstruction of initial C
pl
C
fr
V- sequences in Nivkh (where pl = plosive and fr = fricative) as
generally coming from *C
pl
VC
pl
V- in such correspondences as -tx on going with CK -tk(n) of that meaning. In fact this is
one of the examples he gives (reconstructed as *tky, where /y / is a stressed mid-high vowel corresponding to my schwa).
As regards the vowels, note that root retraction vowel harmony in CK operates between dominant /e/, /o/ and /a/ and
corresponding recessive /i/, /u/ and // such that if a word has one dominant vowel, all its vowels must be dominant too.
This works bi-directionally, as still marginally reected in the residual cases remaining in Nivkh, where a high series /i/, //
and /u/ alternate with a low /e/, /a/, /o/ series e.g. in allative e-rX to him fromi(f) (where the case sufx in its full formis
dominant -toX/-roX), and, less commonly, the epenthetic vowel following (some) reduplicated stems harmonizes with that
of the stem, as in perper-a-d sways vs. prpr-i-d lies about (Mattissen, 2003:79).
15
Notice that it is // that alternates with
/a/ in Nivkh, not // as in CK, and that in CK // is largely epenthetic and outside the vowel harmony system. The
reconstructed vowel systemin Table 2 presents a more symmetric picture for vowel harmony than in CKitself (where the //
alternating with /a/ is an extra front vowel
16
). The replacement of // by a centralized /L/ in Table 2 brings the alternation
pattern more in line with the simpler high vs. low system in Nivkh also with neighbouring Tungusic languages. These
display similar root retraction harmony systems in which soft (i.e. high) // varies with hard (i.e. low) /a/ (the latter in
fact a lowered schwa like /L/ in Even, which has maintained the purest system). In Tungusic this does not operate bi-
directionally as in CK but progressively alone, as in Mongolian (cf. Comrie, 1981:70f). This may well represent an areal
phenomenon.
17
There are some good potential cognates containing CK // corresponding to Nivkh //, notably r- mouth of river (and
related forms cited further on) that appear to go with CK r- owout, and tk edge of sleeping platform which goes with
CK -tn near or at edge of (also the Nivkh terminative case endings, discussed in the following section). Others show/a/
in Nivkh, which may simply be indicative of the early break down of the vowel harmony system in Nivkh at this point (the
alternation is very peripheral today).
18
Note that Amur // comes from */a/ in those forms where the Sakhalin dialects have
an /a/ (e.g. A tf, ES taf house cf. Jakobson, 1971:91), but r- is precisely not one of these, having schwa in all dialects.
Finally, the development of the sandhi-like phenomena at morpheme boundaries in Nivkh must be of later origin than the
hypothesized Proto-CKA stage. The processes involved all quite natural include the development of a voiced series of
plosives through nasal sandhi and the more general results of the morphophonemic principle whereby plosive plus plosive
sequences became dissimilated to fricative plus plosive or plosive plus fricative, with varying patterns according to the word
classes involved (Mattissen, 2003:44ff). Various morphophonemic processes have produced alternation in the shape of
morphemes within CK too, but these are more idiosyncratic, such as the alternation of initial /r-/ (Koryak /j-/) with medial
/-n-/ in Chukotian transitive verb stems (from *n-), and at all events are not reconstructible for the proto-language (cf.
Fortescue, 2005:10). Since there are no common morphophonological alternations at morpheme boundaries reconstructible
for Proto-CKA, there is no evidence that it had a comparable level of polysynthesis to that of modern Chukotian or even
Nivkh. In particular, it may have lacked incorporation as opposed to xed sequences of independent words altogether.
3. Nominal morphology
Let me proceed now with an investigation of the morphology of both families I shall return to a more exact
consideration of sound correspondences in connection with the lexical evidence presented in section 5. First I shall consider
nominal inection and derivation, and thereafter via pronominal and participial markers I shall move on to verbal
inection and derivation in a separate section.
The oldest spatial case markers in CK (reconstructible for the proto-language and found in numerous combinations and
functions in the modern languages) are general locative -k() and general lative (including allative) - (see Fortescue,
2005:426). Both elements would appear to have reasonable correlates in Nivkh. As regards the rst compare the locative/
ablative case marker -(u)x on nouns (also ES extension -uxe = Amur perlative -ue), and -k in ra-k where (and note hu-g/ES
hun-x there, CK n-k there, mi-k where). As regards * - (*-(a)?) compare sufx -a fairly close to (with locational
stems) and extension -ajo further from,
19
as in ES taz-a place out on the water somewhat removed from the shore/
village, taz-ajo place out on the water further removed from the shore (from tas (exact) place out on water near shore
Krejnovic, 1986:163). Gruzdeva (2008:185) refers to these as medial and distal respectively in the secondary deictic
system.
15
See further in Hattori (1962, 2) for traces of the system within polysyllabic stems.
16
Its development from */L/ in CK may have been triggered by the increasing functional burden of //, outside of the harmony system, whose allophones
in the environment of uvulars would have been difcult to distinguish from /L/.
17
An original bi-directional high vs. low system in Tungusic, shared with Nivkh, may have been inuenced by the Mongolian system to the west, which
also infused it with superimposed labial harmony.
18
An independent merger between // and /e/ has occurred in Chukchi, undermining its own harmony system. In those dialects of Koryak and Alutor
where there has been a merger it is between // and /a/ rather. In Itelmen, where vowel harmony broke down long ago except for the i/e and u/o alternations
in certain afxes, *// sometimes went to /a/ as opposed to /e/ by generalization of the dominant formof a morpheme, and original schwa could go to either
/i/, /e/ or /a/ (Fortescue, 2005:12).
19
With adverbial sufx -jo, which has comparative (as well as iterative and verbalizing) function (cf. Saveljeva and Taksami, 1970:524). It would seem
more likely that this would attach to an originally directional (or deictic) than to a static locative base. Note that -jo in Nivkh is a dominant vowel harmony
form alternating with recessive -ju (cf. Austerlitz, 1994b:259), as expected following -a, which also has a dominant vowel CK - is also dominant.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1364
The rst of these may appear added directly to verbal stems in both language families to produce non-nite/adverbial
forms, respectively CK innitive -k() and Nivkh converb ending -ke when V-ing also participial marker -k (especially
common in ES). Consider also the special comparative (standard) case marker -(a)k in Nivkh it is precisely the locative
case that is used for marking the standard of comparison in CK.
20
The second element (*-(a)) also appears to have verbal
function in both languages, namely CK - adverbial formant and Nivkh temporal converb endings -a(n) and -.
Another possibly common element is -r in such and such a location (with locational roots) in Nivkh, going with fossilized
dative -ri (with directional adverbs only) in CK, and Nivkh terminative/limitative (up to an edge) -tk/ES -taka
(regarded as a case ending in the standard grammars), going with CK -tn near or at edge of cf. Nivkh tk edge of
sleeping platform as a noun (Panlov, 1962:143). A similar item (somewhere between being a case ending and a
postposition) is Nivkh -tx on (no doubt originally an independent word cf. Mattissen, 2003:9f.), going with CK
derivational afx -tk(n) of the same meaning, already mentioned.
21
Of particular interest is the relationship of the CK ablative to the Nivkh case system: Tailleur (1960:117) directly links a
supposed ablative -nxe as in East Sakhalin tu-nxe from here (his hyphen) to the Koryak ablative -qo and (supposed)
Itelmen -nk (-enk is actually the locative). However, the Nivkh ending is in fact the locative/ablative -(u)x/-(u)xe already
mentioned (and found in all dialects) and added here to base tun- or tu-. The Koryak ablative reects Proto-CK ablative *-qo
(r), probably related to qor hither (added to the lative) and distinct from the Itelmen ablative -x?al, (on personal
pronouns -nx?al), which is probably the same as Alutor -qal, consisting of - plus qal(a) side, direction (see Fortescue,
2005:434for details). What is interestinghereis not somuchtheexact formbut the typologicallyoddassociationof theablative
and the locative in a single case ending in Nivkh (only the Amur dialect has a distinct locative -(u)in).
22
For the ablative is also
an innovation in CK and as can be seen from the above its meaning is curiously intertwined with that of the lative direction
towards meaning (and even seems to contain the old lative case ending). A clue: qor hither, this way, bring me X is used in
pointing and to here is in deictic contexts the same as from there.
As was noted above, neither family has a genitive case, but both have possessive pronouns. In CK these are formed from
the derivational possessive ending -in (productively applicable to nouns as well as pronouns),
23
corresponding to Nivkh
possessive pronoun marker -n (as a nominal root thing, perhaps reecting the common CKA source). Note also that 3s and
plural prexes/proclitics in possessive usage cause following nasal sandhi, as if an *- or -n has been lost (this is the essential
origin of this pattern of alternation in the dialects of Nivkh that have it). Most of the other case endings in both families are
secondary and/or relatable to independent stems, e.g. Nivkh instrumental -kir, related to verbal stem i-r-/-kir- use (as
suggested by Austerlitz, 1982:85), and allative -toX/roX/doX, which Austerlitz (1990a:28) relates to verb -tXop- touch, and
Chukotian allative -jt, based on verbal stem jt- go for plus lative - above. The CK comitative will be dealt with in
connection with the dual number below.
Let us turn to number marking on nominals. Both families have a dual/plural distinction, although it has been lost in
Chukchi and in Nivkh it is limited to 1st person pronouns. As I have argued in Fortescue (1997:373) the original plural (or
perhaps paucal) in CKmay have been-t(i), nowdual in Koryak (but not Chukchi or Itelmen), where the new plural is -wwi
< *-gvi from derivational sufx -iniv collection of.
24
Note that -wwi on personal names has a clear collective sense (X and
family/companions). The Itelmen plural is -n (=glottalized/n/).
25
In Nivkh the only trace of a plural -t is in the plural of
converbs (falling together with a different, 1s -t I shall return to below). The regular nominal plural is -kun(u)/-gun(u)/-un
(u), which may very well go with CK -iniv above (and hence have a parallel origin to the Koryak plural). Moreover, this is in
turn related to the comitative markers Amur du. -ke, pl. -ko (ES -kin, -kunu), which can be directly compared to CK comitative
prex k- fromadverbial knml together (and which combines with a participial or an instrumental sufx respectively to
form two related circumxes on nouns).
Beyondthe dual andplural inCK, one alsohas toconsider the singulative, anunusual feature of nouns inthis family: many
(perhaps most) must bemarkedbyaspecial sufx(or elsebyreduplicationof thestem) as beingsingulativeoriginallyperhaps
indicating distinct semantic classes (for instance items that typically occur in pairs or groups cf. Fortescue, 2005:433). These
are dropped(except inItelmen) whencaseendings are added. The most commonof these are -, -lnand-n(Itelmenalsohas
-c, and -lin and -mi, bothof which are especially usedfor paired objects). Nowa notable inter-dialectal aspect of Nivkhis that
many nouns in the East (and South) Sakhalin dialects end in a nasal (mainly -) which is lost in the mainland (Amur) dialect.
26
Could these be the remnants of singulative markers of the Chukotko-Kamchatkan kind? There may even be traces of the
semanticclass of objects towhichtheyoncereferred, for exampleinthefact that manyanimateobjects tendtoendwithsuchan
endinginthe morearchaiceasterndialects. The ending-n(alsoa nominalizer inall dialects) is commoninnouns referringtokin
20
Though Mattissen (2003:82) relates this to verbal stem (j)k- reach.
21
Compare also Nivkh -le/laxe next to, up against and CK -lku(n) between, among.
22
cf. Levinson (2003:101), where the conation of location and source (as opposed to goal) is not an attested possibility. Krejnovic (1979:302)
relates Amur -(u)in to the -n- in ES hunx there, etc. Note that there is a variant -uine (cf. Panlov, 1962:138), perhaps from *-uine, of which the rst part
could be the locative/ablative above.
23
Also ingredient in -kinthing associated with and a number of participial sufxes like -lin and -qin, where the -i- is probably a copular element.
The source of the prex may have been an independent word *nik meaning something (> CK *nik cf. Fortescue, 2005:187).
24
On verbs the plural is distinguished from the dual in Koryak by the addition of sufx -la- of several subjects/objects of an action (also found in Chukchi)
before the dual marker -t.
25
Probably deriving from CK Class 2 (animate) plural -()nti containing 3rd person demonstrative n- (which I shall return to below).
26
This dialect tends in general to showmore syncope than the more archaic eastern dialects according to Krejnovic as a result of the shifting forward of
word stress from the second to the rst syllable in many words in that dialect (Krejnovic, 1979:299).
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1365
or animals according toSaveljeva andTaksami (1970:528f.). Theyalso mentionthe ending-r foundina fewwords referring to
natural objects that occur in aggregates (Saveljeva and Taksami, 1970:523), which can be compared to CK-rri a group of -s.
As regards more clearly derivational (deverbal) sufxes, consider Nivkh -f place of -ing (the basic form is actually -v,
regularly devoiced in nal position, as in mif/miv- land from postposition mi- inside to which compare CK mi-k
where); this corresponds to CK -nv of the same meaning.
27
This is involved in purposive and/or supine forms of the
verb in both languages Nivkh has purposive -f-toX, with the allative case marker -toX following it (on a bare verbal stemthe
latter alone produces the so-called supine cf. Gruzdeva, 1998:51f), while in Chukotian the supine formant is -nv- with
the lative marker following -nv (Itelmen -no-ke). Also some important deverbal participial formants apparently common
to both families can be introduced here. In ES Nivkh there is an attributive participial form of verbs in -, which (like the
singulatives above) has been dropped in the mainland dialect. In the SS dialect this varies with -n, so perhaps it derives from
*-n and can thus be compared with CK -rn quality/action nominalizer. The 2/3s converb formant -r (plural -t) in
Nivkh
28
could in turn be correlated with the rst part of CK progressive/present participle -k(n), pl. -kt (I shall return to
this in the following section). The most important of these participial (or rather deverbal) forms in Nivkh is *-nt or *-nc (>
Amur -d, ES -nt/nd/d, SS -nt), which is not only a deverbal nominalizer but forms the indicative mood of verbs (same formfor
all persons), and furthermore verbalizes interrogative and demonstrative roots (see Mattissen, 2003:16 for examples).
29
It is
tempting to relate this to CK verbalizer (of nouns and adjectives) -()t, also a detransitivizer (e.g. in benefactive
constructions with incorporated object), and together with transitivizing prex *n- involved in transitivizing verbs.
However, there is also a Nivkh verbalizer -t which may be more directly related to the CK sufx, so there is no obvious
candidate for a direct CK correlate to Nivkh *-nt.
30
Finally, there is a sufx of permanent quality -la in Nivkh which is applied to quality verb stems (combinable with
following indicative *-nt). This can be compared with the Itelmen adjective formant -laX, perhaps going with Chukotian
participial -lRn one who s. Alternatively, it is possible that the original indicative in Nivkh was actually the archaic
narrative mood sufx -qana/-(ja)Rana, later ousted by the nominalizing*-nt sufx, and it is the former that goes with
Chukotian -lRn.
We can now turn to personal pronouns and in particular their bound (afxed and cliticized) forms, an area where Nivkh
andCKappear onthe surface to diverge considerably. First let us take the independent pronouns. The Nivkh1pl (inclusive) mer
(n)/ES mirn we (incl.) (where the -n marks plurality as innominal sufx -kun) is directlycomparable toCKmur(i) we (where
the -i is from plural *-ti via *-i).
31
On the other hand, there is no obvious candidate either to match with Nivkh 1s ni or with
1p exclusive nin (the plural of ni) in CK, where I is *km. There may be some evidence for a connection in the 1s form of the
converb -t mentioned above compare the CK 1s prex t- (if both reect some much reduced combination of a t-initial
demonstrative root plus a 1st person marker *m(V)).
32
Panlov (1962:236) suggests that 1s ni is identical with and the
sourceof numeral ni- one, whichhas areasonable cognate inCKnnntreatedinthe sectiononlexical material below. If this
is so, then the reverse order of derivation would seem more likely.
33
Better afxal matches with the Nivkh 1st person root
mer-/mir- are the CK 1s optative/imperative prex m-, pl. mn-, and the Itelmen transitive object sufxes 1s -mi, 1pl -mi
(< *-mit?). Whatever the case, there would seem to be somewhat less of a problem as regards the 2nd person Nivkh
ci (pl. cin), corresponding to CK 2pl. turi you (parallel to muri) and 2s k/kt (on a demonstrative base k- like the
1s form above). Note that the prexed form is c-, alternating in the SS dialect with d- before a dental consonant (the prexed
1 s form is n- alternating with n- Mattissen, 2003:55). This may have inuenced the 1 s form in Nivkh (so n(i)- could
originally have been from *m(i)r- just as c(i)- could have been from *t(i)r-). There is no prexed subject form on verbs
paralleling the 1s form in CK (1s marking is idiosyncratic in another way in Nivkh, namely as regards the converb sufx
mentioned above).
As regards the 3rd person, it is tempting to relate Nivkh 3s if or at least the corresponding prexed undergoer form
i- (j- before vowel) to CKn he, she. The principal evidence for this lies in the fact that i- as a possessor or emphatic/denite
27
Compare winv track and atnv wound, mentioned in the section on lexical material below.
28
ES regularly 2s/3s -r, 1s/all persons pl -t (or -n in the future or imperative combined presumably with intentional sufx -in); Amur -r/-t. The plural
person form-t may reect the plural -t mentioned above in connection with nominal plural marking. I shall deal with the 1s formof this shape under verbal
morphology below.
29
Jakobson (1971:100) analyses it as a thematic verbal element -n- (cf. the attributive form of verbs in --) followed by a nominalizer -c (=-s in the
Amur dialect, but -r in ES and SS).
30
However, Krejnovic analyses the indicative/nominalizer as semantically someone/something that is /that V-s (Krejnovic, 1979:307f.), as if the nal
-t/d of *-nt were an indenite pronoun. This could reect ES nunt/nud (indenite) what, with the same ending as in demonstrative tud, Amur td this. He
states that this form expresses more uncertainty than runt/rud, which is the form going with the Amur equivalent sid (and note also Amur n-d thing, do,
mentioned above in connection with possessives). So there is in fact a possible link to CK q(ut) what (the last part is ut this both will be found in the
section on lexical material below), as in Chukchi renut what, something, and/or CK nik(ut)something. Note extended formnudlu someone/something
(Amur sidlu). Nivkh *-nt is at all events a very general kind of nominalizer (also on transitive verbs, as in ES ind food from in- eat), and is in the right
position vis-a` -vis the verbal stem to reect an earlier nominal construction in Nivkh.
31
The 1st person dual form, in A megi (ES me), is from *men-gin or *me-gin according to Panlov (1962:238), where -gin is the dual comitative. His
suggestion that the rst part is mi/me two seems unnecessary.
32
As I have previously suggested, the 1s and 2s pronouns in CK may actually have come from still earlier forms *t()km and *t()kt, with regular loss of
initial /t/ in this position (Fortescue, 1997:374). Compare also Chukchi kur- < Proto-CK*tkur- go off.
33
Thus nin one (person) (compare the impersonal/regal one in English). Also worth noting is the use of Chukotian antipassive prex in- someone/
something as a 1s undergoer prex in inverse transitive verb inections (Tailleur, 1960: 143 directly compares this to the Nivkh 1st person marker). Cross-
linguistically, 1st person is the highest candidate for subjecthood, and its function as object/undergoer is therefore prone to being somehow marked.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1366
undergoer causes nasal sandhi in following plosives.
34
The corresponding ES plural forms in(un)/irn they could then be
related to CK ()i they (< *n plus plural -i). The -f of if and the -v or -m of Amuric plural im/iv (also im) are more
problematic, but they may well contain the place of -ing sufx -f (<*-nv mentioned earlier), as Austerlitz (1990a:108)
suggested, the - of the plural form representing nominal plural -kun/un. ES 3pl in(un) suggests the direct addition of
nominal plural sufx -un to hypothetical *n-. It seems best to postpone further treatment of the source of the undergoer
prex itself (probably contained in if) until the following section on verbal morphology, where I shall relate it in turn to CK
*n <CKA *hn. I shall in fact return there to the full array of afxed person markers of CK, but note for nowthat CK lacks the
prexed possessive forms found on nouns in Nivkh (for 1st and 2nd person; the plural forms are proclitic cf. Mattissen,
2003:61). Instead it has the independent possessive form of the independent pronouns before their head nouns (at least in
the same position before the head noun as the Nivkh possessive morphemes).
Finally, to address the matter of the apparent lack of numeral classiers in CK (which proliferate in Nivkh), consider the
possibility that the original distinction in Nivkh was between animate and inanimate noun classes and that the former was
marked by the sufx -n on nominals that Saveljeva and Taksami (1970:528f.) note as particularly common on names of
human kin and animals. Perhaps this is the same -n as the numeral classier for human beings, as in ni-n/ne-n one (human
being)
35
, but observe also the classier for animals -(a)n in both dialects. This can be compared with the -n that marks Class
2 nouns in CK (for individualized persons and animals in Chukchi and Koryak) it has been related by Zhukova (1974) to the
demonstrative n that which, however, is not limited to animates. In fact it might have been the same as the singulative
sufx of that form in its original class-indicating function. Note that in Itelmen the Class 2 ending is kept before plural -t
(producing generalized plural -n, as mentioned), and indeed before case endings as well. The same is true of ES Nivkh
nominal sufx - (not dropped before endings, singular or plural). The development of a full-blownnumeral classier system
in Nivkh could have been due to areal inuence (as in Japanese, Korean and Ainu even Tungusic languages have some
collective classiers). The other classes are marked by replacing the -n by sufxes transparently related to nouns in most
cases (e.g. -m for boats, from mu).
4. Verbal morphology
Before introducing some potentially common afxal elements relevant to the verbal morphology of both families, it is
necessary to make a brief excursion into the manner in which the complex person/number/mood/aspect paradigms of CK
both intransitive and transitive were built up from a simpler basis. I have presented a diachronic scenario for this in
Fortescue (1997), where I focused on the possible areal inuence of neighbouring Eskimo in producing the ergative clause
type in the Chukotian branch of the family. The common pre-CK starting point I proposed (before any transitive paradigms
developed) was the intransitive aorist (or perfective) paradigmreconstructible as belowin Table 3 fromthe modern forms. It
contains a perfective sufx *-Radded to the verbal stem (V) and still visible in the 1st/2nd/3rd singular and 3rd plural
Chukotian forms before the corresponding pronominal markers (and prior to rather radical assimilation/attrition of the
sufxes in Chukotian).
Contrasting with the aorist already at this stage there may have been a common CK progressive (or imperfect) paradigm,
represented by the present paradigm of Chukchi. This is reached in the Chukotian branch by removing the prexes and
replacing the sufxes by an ending (probably of participial origin) *-kn
36
, but in Itelmen by inserting a present tense sufx
-z/s- before the sufxes of the aorist. Actually the latter may reect the initial *-- in the Chukotian form, and either a
reanalysis occurred in Itelmen or the *-kn in Chukotian represents the original 3s aorist form*-Rn >It. -()en (compare
the 3pl form -kt, with additional plural -t > It. glottalized -n). In either case the second element could have been a later
addition, inuenced by the aorist. There is also an imperative/optative paradigm with similar endings but different prexes,
namely 1s m- (It. m-), 1pl mn- (It. mn-), 2s/pl q- (It. q-), 3s/pl n- (It. xan-). Georg and Volodin (1999:229) have argued that
Table 3
Chukotko-Kamchatkan verbal inections.
Chukotian
1s t-V-R-k 1pl mt-V-mk
2s V-R- 2pl V-tk
3s V-R-n 3pl V-R-t
Itelmen
1s t-V-k(icen) 1pl n-V-k(icen)
2s V-c 2pl V-sx
3s V-()en 3pl V-()e?n
34
Also independent pronoun if causes nasal alternation with some following endings (-doo, -da <-too, -ta). Its relationship to the ES and SS form ja is
problematic the latter may have a different origin involving interrogative/relative root ja-, as suggested by Austerlitz (1959a), or i/j- as above followed by
(Amur) a who (cf. Otaina and Nedjalkov, 2007:1742; Panlov, 1962:240 suggests ha that as the second part rather). Also Jakobson (1971:88) sees
prepositive i/j- here. Note also respectful 3rd person forms av (pl. avun), Amur f. These suggest the -v ending in the (ES formof) ethnonymNiv, lit.
inhabitant of (somewhere called) Ni, apparently containing -v- from pi-/-vi- be (in/at) and participial/attributive - (Krejnovic, 1986:163).
35
nen in ES and SS, which suggests that the ending was originally *-n.
36
The same in all persons, except 3pl -kt and 2pl -knitk (see below for the latter).
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1367
all the verbal prexes in Itelmen are borrowings from Chukotian, which I regard as unlikely (cf. Fortescue, 2003:53 for
arguments), but this has no great bearing onthe comparison withNivkh, since it is clear, as mentioned, that the CKprexes are
inanycaselater thanthesufxes andpost-datethehypothetical Proto-CKAstage. Thecorrespondingtransitiveparadigms were
built upessentiallybyaddinga 3rdpersonpronominal sufxtothe intransitive forms inbothbranches, extendedto1st and2nd
person objects by integrating two different inverse constructions (involving originally antipassive prex in- or general
inverse prex n- originally a non-specic 3rd person subject marker). For details see Fortescue (1997). The parting of the
way between the two branches occurred according to this model when the ergative construction subsequently developed in
Chukotian, superimposed on the otherwise thoroughly nominative-accusative morphology of the family.
The basis from which the comparison between CK and Nivkh verbal morphology should be made then is prior to the
building up of transitive paradigms in CK. The essential repertoire at the pre-CK stage would have consisted of a basic two-
way distinction between aorist (or perfective) and present (or progressive), the former characterized by sufx -R added
to the verbal stem compare ES Nivkh completive -Rar-/-Xar and resultative -Rare-/-Xare (the equivalent of -t- and
-ta- respectively on the mainland).
37
The sufx would have been followed by subject person/number markers. Note that
the 3s ending in CK was (and is still) -()n, which simply represents the demonstrative n that/he/she/it. The present/
progressive in -kn, on the other hand, apparently a participial form inected only for number, may be bi-partite, as
discussed in the previous section, with the rst consonant comparable to Itelmen present tense afx -z- and the Nivkh
converb -r (likewise inected only for number, apart fromthe 1s formmentioned above).
38
Perhaps its original function was
one of temporal subordination, like the Nivkh converb. As the aorist person endings decayed already in pre-CK, prexes were
added for 1st person (1s t-, 1pl. mt-, the latter presumably with plural -t). The present/progressive further distinguished
2pl subject by addition of the person marker -tk following a linking or copular -i-, which may once have occurred also with
other person subjects, but could be an innovation in Chukotian.
39
In CK a whole series of binary pairs of mood/tense forms emerged, respectively perfective and imperfective, as further
paradigms werebuilt up, for instancethefuture(involvingcircumx- - want to, future cf. verbal stemjin()- desire
for the prex, comparable to Nivkh (j)an- want) and the conditional (an extension of the optative
40
). Note that there are
numerous modal and focal sufxes in Nivkh but these are not organized into inectional paradigms like in CK(they can follow
general indicative, converb or imperative sufxes, with the usual minimal person and number distinctions). CKlanguages also
display numerous independent modal particles that overlap semantically withthe scalar and focal sufxes/enclitics of Nivkh
and include evidential/epistemic items. Although mood, aspect and tense do not formtight paradigms as in CK, there is a basic
future/non-future distinction in Nivkh (future marked by a sufx) and aspect is also distinguished by sufx (including the
completive sufx above). Moreover, it displays numerous converb sufxes besides -r which could in theory have developed
from independent particles. It is reasonable to suppose, in conclusion, that Proto-CKA had simple intransitive paradigms
marked by some kind of modal or aspectual sufx combined with person/number subject sufxes, like in (pre-)Proto-CK (as
reconstructedin Fortescue, 2003:59) but that these become so attenuatedin time in Nivkhas to be replaced almost entirely by
independent pronouns (which further became prexed/proclitic to the verb when used as object markers).
Another central category of the verbal morphology of bothfamilies is valency-increasing (and decreasing) by afxation. In
CK this is a matter of causative/transitivizing prexes or circumxes plus an antipassive prex, in Nivkh of initial
consonant alternation or a single (non-productive) transitivizing sufx plus a productive causativizing sufx. Neither family
has a passive voice apart from an innovatory paradigm in Itelmen although the CK inverse constructions mentioned
above are very ancient (they are shared by Chukotian and Itelmen). Can these things be compared? Perhaps surprisingly, yes.
Let us start with the situation in Nivkh. Most intransitive verbs start with a plosive and transitive ones with a fricative
(though sonorant-initial ones can be either) there are many pairs of verb forms associated by this alternation. A priori one
might expect this to correlate with the use of the ubiquitous transitivizing prex *n- in CK (Itelmen n-, <pre-CK *tn-
41
).
In fact there are just a handful of cases where there is additional nasalization present in the resulting Nivkh transitives, as in
e-mXaRu- make s.o. younger from paRa- (be) young, and e-mXaqu- shorten s.th. from pXaq- (be) short, which could
reect the nal nasal segment of *tn- compare also the difference between ES e-nra- aimat s.th. and Amur cognate e-zra-.
However, all of these contain the (indenite) 3rd person undergoer prex i-/e-
42
(j- before vowel). This morpheme is
required in all cases where fricativization of an initial plosive cannot apply (apart from before single fricatives, nasals and
liquids, when nothing further happens), i.e. before consonant clusters and vowels.
43
37
For the change of */a/ to/ / in A and /Ra/ in ES here compare the discussion of cir/cXar tree in the section on sound correspondences above.
38
According to Krejnovic (1979:321) the -r/t converb endings represent relics of person markers.
39
The remaining 1pl sufx -mk may have been inuenced by 2pl -tk (dissimilation from*-tt?), but the k in both could reect the old dual. As regards the
linking -i-, this is probably the same as copula verb i(t)-, which is comparable to ES Nivkh -i-, a verbal ordinal formant after numeral classiers (Krejnovic,
1979:305).
40
Marked by a prex (following person prexes) R-, which, being of adverbial origin, may be compared to the Nivkh conditional converb in -qa/Ra.
41
Note that initial fricatives in Chukotian generally derive from corresponding plosives, which are mostly preserved in Itelmen (Fortescue, 2005:7). The
Itelmen prex n- may reect assimilation after loss of the medial schwa in *tn- compare the other formof the transitivizing prex (appearing in certain
combinations) in-, with // regularly <*/t/ next to a sonorant (the /i/ may be intrusive). The CK prex itself probably comes from an independent auxiliary
verb nt- have (as) (<*tnt-), whose function is transitivizing (as opposed to intransitive auxiliary it- be) cf. Fortescue (2005:71).
42
The choice of allomorph is determined by vowel harmony with the stem one of the fewvestiges of that phenomenon in Nivkh. Exactly the same initial
vowel alternation is found in the CK antipassive morpheme in-/ena- discussed below (under virtually the same conditions i.e. a recessive vs. a
dominant stem vowel respectively).
43
Actually fricativization of the rst plosive of a cluster can occur, but the prex is still required.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1368
Now, although this prex is generally described as indicating a 3rd person undergoer (or object), it is often glossable as
indenite (do to) something/someone and although its presence is not itself determined by deniteness it is precisely not
present when a denite object precedes the verb and is integrated with it. As Otaina and Nedjalkov (2007:1718) put it: if
there is no explicit direct object, a 3sg object like he/she/it is generally implied unless it is used in a citation form. In other
words, the prex is neutral as to deniteness, only implying a denite object in a suitable context where one is understood.
Also Jakobson (1971:86) glossed this prepositive pronoun as someone or something. This is at least in part comparable to
what the CK antipassive prex in-/ena- does to transitive stems, namely render the implicit object indenite. It may in
fact be relatable to Nivkh ena- other, stranger (cited under the lexical correspondences below), which would be a reasonable
source for both an antipassive and an inverse marker.
44
In that case this morpheme might have more than one formand
function in Nivkh (respectively as stem and as prex), and the CK inverse might in fact nd its source still reected in
Nivkh, namely in the common antipassive construction.
45
For a specic 3s object or undergoer there is also a different i- prex in Nivkh, often called emphatic (as in Mattissen,
2003:57f.) but perhaps better denite, one that causes following nasal rather than fricativizing alternations,
46
just like
the 3s pronominal possessor discussed in the previous section in connection with CK pronominal n, so possibly from *in-.
Actually I would suggest that the ultimate source is CKA*hn, related to Nivkh demonstrative root h-/ES hu-, and it is such a
formthat can be compared directly with the CK 3rd person root n-. I shall return to this relationship at the end of the section
on lexical correspondences in connection with h-initial morphemes in Nivkh in general.
The oldest layer of transitive verbs in Nivkh could still in theory reect a pre-Nivkh transitivizing morpheme *tn-, as in
pre-CK, but this may have been obscured by the later diffusion of *inL- (>CK *in-), coming to apply to already transitivized
stems (an initial plosive would automatically become fricativized following the nal vowel of *inL- according to the general
rules of sandhi in Nivkh). There is some evidence that this might well have been the case, namely in the relic transitivizing
sufx -u- (as in nok-u- make s.th. narrow from nok- (be) narrow) this corresponds nicely to the sufxed part of the CK
transitivizing circumx n- -v. The prexed part (corresponding to initial fricativization in the more productive process
described above) could well reect the loss of *n-, as can be seen with transitive verbs of this kind starting with plosives, e.
g. faz-u- undress from intransitive paz- and Xav-u- heat up from qav-.
47
This -u- can also verbalize nouns, which CK
-v- also can. Recall that prexes are generally later phenomena than sufxes in CK, and thus the sufxed part of verbal
circumxes like this can be taken to be older than the prex. The use of *inL- to mark an (indenite) undergoer in Nivkh
might then only have been necessary with stems whose initial consonant or cluster precisely could not be fricativized
(after the sufx was no longer productive). The modern 3s undergoer prex may thus represent the conation of two
different morphemes, denite and indenite respectively.
The modern Nivkh forms and processes mentioned so far are all non-productive to formcausative (as opposed to merely
transitive) verbs productively, the sufx-ku- is required (applicable to transitive as well as intransitive verbs). Krejnovic
(1979:314) suggests aconnectiontothecomitativeafx-ku- here,
48
but perhaps it maybedirectlycomparedtothecommonCK
sufx -tku-, which is essentially a frequentative, but may also indicate protracted or intense activity and is used as a general
antipassive (of potential object) it means act or do with on nominal bases. Finally, the reciprocal prex u-/w- may be
compared to CK uviki body, self, which functions as a reexive pronoun and may well go with Nivkh vic, ES ut body.
49
5. Lexical material
Rather than simply presenting a list of lexical look-alikes in the manner of Tailleur (a hazardous approach at best), let me
start by singling out a handful of particularly good candidate cognates among basic items of vocabulary. These relate to the
common hunting/shing way of life that goes back to Neolithic times in the region. Consider then the following group of
forms, which contains one of the derivational morphemes introduced in the section on nominal morphology:
Nivkh (Amur) tf,
50
ES, SS taf house (stemtav-, ending in sufx -f/v place where) can be compared with Proto-CKtvanv
place (with the same sufx -nv place of on stemtva- be, live, sit note also Chukotian (t)varat people, fromthe same
44
Thus he does/you do something to me would be expressed in CK as he does/you do something to someone else, with *in-/ena- as undergoer (the 1s
object being higher on the animacy/person hierarchy than the subject). Why the dominant form of this hypothetical common morpheme should have
become lexicalized in Nivkh requires explanation but note that in the correlated Chukotian formena-ra-lRn neighbour it is due to the dominant vowel of
following stem-ra- house; perhaps Nivkh *tav of that meaning (as in Amur enadv- another house) was also dominant, as the reconstructed vowel
suggests (ena- is, however, a xed form today).
45
Perhaps better called generalized undergoer construction, since the term antipassive has become rather too closely associated with ergative
languages. Also non-ergative Itelmen, Yukaghir and Na-Dene languages, among others, have similar morphemes (Fortescue, 1998:61).
46
This causes a following plain (non-aspirated) plosive to be replaced by the corresponding voiced plosive inthe Amur dialect and is generally triggered by
a nal nasal that may or may not itself have dropped (it is generally retained in the ES and SS dialects).
47
Note also si- (fricativized from ci-) put, which can be compared with CK il- of that meaning listed in the section on lexical material below.
48
Used before a following converb ending -r it forms adverbials and may contain initial -, the attributive form of verbs in ES.
49
If not Itelmen reciprocal prex lu-. Otaina and Nedjalkov (2007:1742) tentatively suggest *un- as the source of the Nivkh prex u- (where *-n would
represent the plural), since it causes nasal assimilation of the same sort as 3s i- above. Compare, however, West Sakhalin ut-azi- be of the same height
(as), corresponding to Amur u-zi- with reciprocal u- (Otaina and Nedjalkov, 2007:1728). Note that u- is also a reciprocal prex in Ainu (just as i- is also a
3rd person undergoer prex) this could represent early borrowing from Nivkh during Okhotsk Culture times in northern Hokkaido.
50
Also raf (model) mortuary house (which may reect an original indenite possessor form *i-taf according to Jakobson (1971:91). But note also CK ra-
house (and r- enter, raan entrance), as Tailleur suggested Jakobson (1971:99) sees a possible verbal stem ra-/ta- install here, also reected (with
reexive prex) in pr- arrive.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1369
stem plus -rt set (of)).
51
So the original house word in both families could have been *dvanv, parallel with Chukotian
wi-nv track, mentioned earlier, the stemof which goes perhaps with Nivkh wi- go. Also Nivkh wo village (actually more a
gathering place for the winter, away from the summer shing sites Austerlitz, 1990b) has a possible CK cognate. As
Austerlitz suggests, this word (beginning, unusually for a noun, with /w/) probably contains the reciprocal prex w-/u-
before a hypothetical stem o- meaning gather, as also reected in verb w-op-u- of that meaning. It looks as if a nal
nominalizing element has been eroded, so why not -f/v place again? It is at all events attractive to relate this to CK
omak- together, transitive -umk-v- gather (and Chukchi umek group), so Nivkh wo <*w-umk-()nv, via assimilated
*wup-nv?
52
Now Nivkh (and Itelmen) summer houses are typically situated near the mouths of large rivers (Black, 1973:6). Consider
the following group of potential cognates: Nivkh eri, ES and SS i river, CKr- owout and It. i water.
53
What could be more
natural than that a river (*Lr-i) be regarded as water (*i) that ows out (*Lr-)? There is also the word for sea itself,
ke-rq (where (he)rq is side, direction cf. Austerlitz, 1959b:220), to which can be compared Chukchi e-curmn shore
(<*ke-curmn, where the second element means edge).
54
Further derivatives of the *Lr- root in Nivkh are: r, ES r mouth
of river and rk, ES rkr shore, bank, which can be compared to CK r-rn and r-i of precisely the same meanings
(recall what was said in the section on sound correspondences about reexes of */L/in Nivkh). The sufxes in CK are
respectively the nominalizer -rn treated under nominal morphology and i below. Probable cognates for
downstream and upstream will be found in Table 4 below.
Another promising lexical area is that of sh more specically salmon, with which the rivers of the whole area teem.
Thus compare: Nivkh co sh and CK wcu salmon, q(w)cu humpback salmon, It. cuv(aj) salmon (chinook). All of
these could contain an original stem *wLcu (i.e. *wLtu). This assumes syncope and/or assimilation in both Nivkh and
Itelmen (which would explain the ejective in the latter cf. Fortescue, 2003:67).
55
As regards inland hunting as opposed to shing, compare Nivkh -/ES aan- hunt (the ES form contains a- land
animal), a(i)- go for, chase after with. Chukotian -rt catch (It. es- hunt), and ta- go for
56
, perhaps all from root
*ra-. A further basic word, relating to the spoils of the hunt, is Nivkh in-/ni- eat (the i- is the 3rd person undergoer
prex), comparable with CK nu- eat,
57
perhaps both from *nu-, with assimilation of /u/ to /i/ in Nivkh.
In Table 4 belowI list most of the reasonably good i.e. more or less regular correspondences that have turned up so far
in the hope that they will form the basis for a more far-reaching search in the future. The Proto-Chukotian/Proto-Chukotko-
Kamchatkan forms are from Fortescue (2005), and the (Proto-)Nivkh ones are the oldest forms to be gleaned from the
modern dictionary and word-list sources (primarily Saveljeva and Taksami, 1965, 1970, Nakagawa et al., 1993, and Tangiku
et al., 2008). Basically this means ES if there is any discrepancy from corresponding Amur forms.
58
Lack of a gloss under
Proto-Chukotian indicates virtual identity to the gloss under Nivkh. The older Itelmen forms (in particular those from the
extinct eastern and southern languages, E. It. and S. It.) are not always reliable as to form. An apostrophe after a
consonant indicates an ejective in Itelmen, not an aspirated/fortis one as in Nivkh (except for /d/ and /n/, which are palatal in
Nivkh, as is /t/ in reconstructed Proto-CKA forms).
Let me now sum up the principal evidence here for the sound correspondences given in Table 1 in the light of the
hypothesized sound systemof the proto-language presented in Table 2. It would appear, if these correlations hold, that there
must have been considerable contraction/syncope in Nivkh, involving the weakening and disappearance of intervocalic
consonants (especially voiced fricatives and liquids), also the reduction of clusters involving sonorants (nasals, liquids and
semi-vowels). The basic shape of nominal stems in the proto-language seems to have been (C)VC(C)V(C), and of verbal stems
(C)V(C)-, with no long vowels and no clusters of more than two consonants (the latter medial only). The vowel
correspondences are fairly regular, with allowance made for instances of labial and dental assimilation (in particular of
schwa to /u/ and /i/ respectively).
First as regards the plosives. Note that the difference between voiced and unvoiced (or lenis and fortis) is overwhelmingly
conned to initial position in Nivkh, and so probably also in Proto-CKA. For the labials (collapsed to a single series in CK, as all
plosives) see: *pana, *pki(r)-, *pt-, *bl-, *blm-, *hp-, *ba-, and *bla- above. For the dentals see: *dvanv, *dR-, *diwl-,
*tlv-, *pt-, *tem-, *duv, *du-, *dod(o), *til-, *tek(a)-, *tl, *ta(la)-, *tL(q)-, *dLqL, *d(ak)- and *dvi-. For the
palatals see: *wLtu, *mtRal, *t(n)ti(l)-, *dLdi-, *tevar-, *dm(i), *mLtv-, *t-, and *tev-. For the velars see: *pki(r),
51
cf. Itelmen cognates a- sit, a-nom seat (-nom is the Itelmen reex of *-nv) and compare in turn Nivkh tiv-/riv- sit on and tifc chair (with another
nominalizer).
52
Compare more recent productive form wopuf meeting, containing precisely these three elements.
53
The latter form and its relationship to Chukotian presented somewhat of a mystery in Fortescue (2005:398).
54
Perhaps also Itelmen qiX sea, (with assimilation to the nal uvular /X/?) earlier forms attested for Itelmen are kix, kejaga and kaijan (see Fortescue,
2005:387).
55
Other kinds of salmon one might catch in the area are Nivkh wel summer salmon and weck kind of Siberian salmon, comparable to It. fec/vilc of that
meaning, and/or fackc loach, golets (the -c is a singulative sufx), also Nivkh va()s kind of salmon, sig and Chukotian wit(wit) small kind of salmon,
golets.
56
Also perhaps rnik animal and r-oR- give birth (animal) for the second element compare Nivkh qo-/eR- give birth to (an ablauting verb where
the initial e- is the form of the undergoer prex required by vowel harmony), as in *qola in the table below.
57
Which is transitive and seems to contain the transitivizing prex *n- discussed under verbal morphology (as reected e.g. in Koryak ju- < *u-), so
perhaps < *t(n)nu-.
58
Proto- is in quotations here, reecting the shallow nature of the comparative base, as discussed in the Introduction. ES forms without attested
equivalent in A (or vice versa) are marked as such. An oblique separating forms otherwise indicates morphophonological variants.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1370
Table 4
Further potential Nivkh-CK cognates.
(Proto-)Nivkh Proto-Chukotian/CK Proto-CKA
rf wound
a
atnv *atnv?
al-r berry
b
l-u- gather berries *Ll-?
alv-erq behind
c
lv- other (It. elveze- go back)
d
*LlvL-?
ES azmc man (male)
e
rm() leader, strong *LrmL-?
pil- big It. pl- big (Ch. pl- completely) *bl-?
pla(q) leaf It. pla(l) *bla-?
polm- blind plm- dark from rain/snow, blind *blm-?
(he)rq() side, direction q edge *dLqL?
rk-/-tk-/ES rak- carve, cut - dig, scratch *d(ak)-?
rru-/-tru- untie, release r- *dr-?
taX epidemic, cold, inuenza tRl- ill *dR-?
tvi- end -tvi- become *dvi-?
irl-/-tl- pull, drag iwl- (It. timp-) take (across), haul *diwl-?
tot (upper) arm E.It. soto/soto arm
f
*dod(o)?
t-/ES tu- this ut this
g
*du-?
tuv/ruv elder brother, tum friend (It. tumx *duv?
blood relative sibling of same age)
SS dicm-/ES zicv- tread/A zit- kick tti- tread (Kerek caci-tRu-kick) *dLdi-?
com() raft timi *dm(i)?
ena- other, stranger ena-ra-lRn neighbour
h
*ena-?
km- scurry back and forth (of insects) kame(cat)- move around (and kmk beetle) *gLm-?
kit-/ES kitn- run away ntv-
i
*gnt-?
kur- God, the world
j
k(R)ukl creator, Raven, It. kutx legendary creator of Kamchatka *gRu-?
qa- go downriver qalelle() downriver, downhill
k
*Ga(l)-?
qalRala-
l
bright qe-light *Ge-?
hawa- open mouth wide
m
awa- open (e.g. mouth) *hawa()-?
hup- tie p- be attached, penetrate *hp-?
hir stomach of animal ji(ji) gut *hi-?
hilx tongue (and jelel-/ jil(jil) tongue *hil?
helel- lick)
him cedar thicket im- dense, frequent *him-?
ur island ilir(i) *ilur(i)?
ki/xi- up kwji go up
n
*kLwji-?
klmr navel kil(kil) *kl-?
im-/-kim- give
o
kmit- take *(L)kmi(t)-?
kzm whitethorn
p
kilam, E.It. kerem whitethorn berry *krm?
ks-/ES kjru- happy krvi- *kr(vi)-?
kr- be hungry t(Rt)- *kt-?
le(le) very li really *li?
lir
q
wolf l()Ri(n) (It. Xine) *lRi()?
if heart
r
li(li) *li-?
ma(g)- strong
s
mj- big *mLj-?
moc/ES mc breast (womans) macve chest
t
*mLtv-?
ml- tow along shore mu(lRt)- wander, go off far
u
*mu(l)-?
ml wooden gurine in It. mila-cX baby, CK ml- ne, *ml-?
commemoration of the deceased small
mla ear (ES; and m-hear
v
) vilu(ln) ear (and valom- hear) *mlu-?
moq- break into pieces mq- small *mq-?
mezla()
w
rowan mic(Ral)/mc(Ral) (It. mce-) *mtRal?
nana recently (naf now) It. nen, S.It. dani now *na(n)-?
nui in front, rst E.It. duk- in front
x
*nu-?
nonq
y
young (animal) (u)nn child, baby *nun(L)?
(SS baby)
altr/af side (of body) all on both sides *al-?
ar skin (animal) (SS nl(n) skin, hide *Ll-?
alr skin)
otter nt
z
*Lt?
oi penis oj(n) tail *oj()?
pan gaiters, leg covering
aa
pana (boots of) leg skin of *pana?
reindeer
px- return pkir- come *pki(r)-?
pt(u)- split -pt piece of *pt-?
qa (noun)/Xa- (verb) name It. Xela()/Xaln *qela-?
qau(k) no, not have
ab
qm/It. qa?m no, not *qa(m)?
qav- hot (Xav-u- heat qevja- (let off) steam, scald *qjv(a)-?
up) self (It. qejve-/qevu- boil)
oRla child
ac
Rola boy *qola?
za-/-ta- beat tala- *ta(la)-?
ta-/ra- where, which q what (qu why, *tL(q)-?
ad
(and si-/NS su-/ru-/ tRr how many, It. saq
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1371
Table 4 (Continued)
(Proto-)Nivkh Proto-Chukotian/CK Proto-CKA
ES ru-/nu- what)
ae
what kind)
te-
af
go up on land It. tekej- get up *tek(a)-?
tom(s) smokehole in old It. temes roof (tem- cover), *tem-?
style house Kamen Koryak tome- stop up
smoke-hole
ag
t(lf) far tel(), It. tal-
ah
*til-?
ruv-/-tuv- burn tlv- *tlv-?
r door l- crawl into (sleeping compart-ment)(E.It. solo-nac lower door)
ai
*t(l)-?
si-/-ci- put
aj
il- put (down) *t(n)ti(l)-?
cev- spear, pierce cvi- cut, chop *tev-?
cevcev- wet It. cufcuf rain *tev(tev)?
cavr- grey cevaro *tevar-?
co- melt tl- (It. co-)
ak
*t-?
cuz-/ES cir- new tur- new *tur-?
u- burn (intr.) uji- make re *u(ji)-?
uii- disappear, not be uj (there is) no *uj(L)?
any
vic, ES ut body uviki *uvi-?
vukvuku- get dark
al
vulq- dark *vul-?
wal- cut, chop (and wa wala(-) knife, carve *wa(la)-?
sword, sabre)
avlx lip
am
wmlk(t) *wLmlkL-?
panx soup pa() (drink) soup
an
*ba-?
nin/ES nen() one (person)
ao
nnn *n-?
t()k father tl(n), It. isx *tl?
a
Containing -f (CK-nv) place of; also reduplicated rvrv- in ES as a verb. Note that initial /t/ becomes /r/ after a vowel in certaincircumstances in Nivkh
(/r/ as all fricatives is rare as the initial segment of non-derived nouns cf. Jakobson, 1971:93).
b
The -r (A -s) in Nivkh is a nominalizer (Saveljeva and Taksami, 1970:532), and the -u in CK is acquire, consume.
c
(h)erq is side, direction.
d
Note also CK javal(a) back, behind and ajval- lee(side) (metathesis?).
e
also perhaps Amur ar, ES ara male (the latter with a animal); but note also s/z master, owner (and the CK // Nivkh // correspondence
discussed in the section on sound correspondences).
f
For the s-/s in It. (via //) compare under*t(q) further down in this table this is not regular, but note that /s/ is the usual reex of */t/ next to another
voiceless consonant.
g
Perhaps with metathesis (and inuence from un (that) over there?) cf. lative -. Note also Sedanka W. It tin, E. It tyj this.
h
-ra- is house and -lRn one who has/lives in . Compare also Alutor ina-ra-k in a neighbouring house with Amur Nivkh ena-dv-uin in another house
(-dv- is house).
i
With sufx -v; note that Nivkh medial /nt/ is sometimes reduced to /t/, as in nominalizer -nt/t/d (cf. Gruzdeva, 1998:11).
j
Originally in Nivkh mythology there were four spirit masters (of the sea, the sky, the re and the mountains) referred to by this term (cf. Shternberg,
1999:158,165). The CK form is a personal name, not the common noun for raven.
k
Cf. perhaps CK qal(a) area around or beside, also lative ending -.
l
This presupposes an intermediate stage *qlla in Nivkh. Saveljeva and Taksami (1965) also have qalala-/kalRala- there appears to be metathesis of a
kind in the second form; the -la- is a sufx indicating a permanent property (cf. Mattissen, 2003:18), which may in turn have affected the *//in the rst
syllable.
m
And Amur havahavad have breathing difculty (with mouth open); cf. also perhaps havaf lung (only North Sakhalin has attested/w/here, so there is
some doubt about whether /w/ or /v/ is original).
n
Note also ke- upstream, going perhaps with CK krol(a) on top, upstream (Chukchi roca) for the ending compare CK knlo high.
o
The second form is with prex and metathesis compare old South Itelmen form emgatyz I take. For the seemingly contradictory semantics of give
and take compare the situation in Indo-European discussed by Buck (1988:748) under give in terms of a common meaning stretch out the hand.
p
But also kelm raspberry (bush).
q
The -r (A -s) is a sufx typical of animates acc. Taksami (1983:286); the -n in CK is a singulative sufx.
r
cf. -f from *-nv place of-ing, so perhaps from a verbal root *l(i)i-.
s
Also man very.
t
And E. It. ki-msevi-in big breasted; the Chukotian may have dominant /a/ due to original dominant singulative -ln, still found in the Koryak.
u
And Ch. muul?n (reindeer) caravan.
v
SS mu-/mo-.
w
West Sakhalin medlan acc. Glehn (in Schrenck, 18921900).
x
West It/n/corresponds to East (and sometimes South) Itelmen /d/.
y
The following uvular perhaps causing the change of */u/ to /o/ (there are parallels in the modern language); but note also neneq a little.
z
Reduplication of *t.
aa
Austerlitz (1984:40) relates this to Nivkh stem pa-/fa- put on shoes, socks, footgear.
ab
Takahashi (1942) has SS kavrnt/kavr (for qavrnt/qavr) from *qarv- or *qarm- by metathesis?.
ac
A oRla, ES eRla (the - is the sufx lost on the mainland discussed above); cf. qo-/eR- give birth to.
ad
*t(q)- appears to be weakened in most forms to *q- in Chukotian (jaq in Koryak, taq in Alutor, saq in It.) and (in some forms) to ra- in Nivkh.
Jakobson (1971:92) sees the prepositive indenite pronoun i-/j- as lying behind this ta- > ra- alternation (so perhaps also behind A si- what?).
ae
Also ES tamdid how. The what forms are problematical, but note that /s/ alternates with palatal/c/in Nivkh.; the forms with /u/ could be inuenced
by demonstrative root tu- this, here. The nu- form in ES is described by Krejnovic (1979:306) as specically completely indenite and thus may contain
n- thing cf. E. It nike/nakc what, something.
af
The form given by Gruzdeva (2008:187) for ES compare A tea- run up on sandbank (boat).
ag
Chukotian top- cover, close - compare also Nivkh to(rf) semi-subterranean house.
ah
The Chukotian contains dominant allative sufx -() (and the Nivkh contains spatial sufx -f).
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1372
*kLwji-, *kt-, *kl-, *gLm-, *gRu-, *-tk(n), *kr(vi)-, *(L)kmi(t)-, and *dk-. And for the uvulars: *qjv(a)-, *Ge-, *Ga(l)-,
*tL(q)-, *qela-, and *qa(m).
As regards the corresponding voiced fricatives, compare: *dvanv-, *dvi-, *qev-, *vul-, *v-, *tev-, *tevar-, *LlvL-,
*dvi-, *uvi-, *dr-, *Ge-, *qm, *lRi(), *li, *mu(l)-, *tl, *l(r), *qola, and *t-. And the corresponding
nasals: *matv, *mtRal, *mlu-, *mu(l)-, *mq-, *mLj-, *mLtv-, *ml-, *tem-, *dm(i), *him-, *nun(L), *ena-, *nu-,
*nu-, *na(n)-, *n, *al-, *ra-, *Lt, *oj(), *du-, *Ll-, *qal-, and *li-. And the semi-vowels and liquids (including *
/r/): *wLtu, *wi-, *wa(la)-, *hawa()-,*oj(),*uj(L), *lRi(), *mtRal, *diwl-, *ml-, *tel-, *vul-, *wa(la)-, *qal-, *LlvL-,
*bl-, *bla(), *hil, *tavar, *dr-, *krv(i)-, *Lr-, and *ilur(i).
There remains more to say about the problematical /h/. By far the most important root withthis initial segment in Nivkh is
demonstrative h-/ES hu- that and related proverb/auxiliary ha- do thus
59
, whichappear to go somehowwith CKin(in)
such (< *hin-?). Compare also Alutor ina over there, Itelmen xe(j)nin such and (em)xenin the same. The
reconstructed CK form is related to CK i now and the ending -in goes with demonstratives like an(in) that and un
over there compare the latter with ES hu- that, parallel with tu this going with CK ut from *du on Table 4. Itelmen
forms like xejnin show a new voiceless fricative deriving from // in initial position (after loss of initial vowel). In Nivkh
initial /x/ as opposed to /h/ is also innovative, deriving from /k/ under sandhi/transitivizing conditions, but this is
obviously not the source of /h/ here.
60
This leaves two possibilities (if the correlation is genuine): either the proto-form was
*hin-, the initial being preserved in Nivkh and lost in CK, or the/h/in Nivkh derives from *i- before a vowel with loss of /i/
and an idiosyncratic reex of //.
To decide which of these possibilities (if either) is correct we need to consider other examples of correspondences in CK
with Nivkh /h/ in Table 4 above, namely hup- tie, hawa- open mouth wide, hilx tongue, him cedar thicket, and hir
stomach. There is no obvious way the CK equivalents could come from *i- here (there are plenty of words beginning with
this sequence, though it is rare in Nivkh), so we are left with the rst possibility: CK probably lost (initial) /h/, a very common
phenomenon in the worlds languages (it is also dropped in Nivkh following prexes). Another possible h-initial transitive
verb in Nivkh corresponding to a CK vowel-initial one is hupu-/jupu- dip or plunge s.th. into compare Chukotian up-/jup-
push (in), stick in (also related ojp- stick through). As regards the tongue word (which always occurs reduplicated
jiljil or with a sufx in Chukchi), compare the Itelmen cognate cel (and other older forms in Fortescue, 2005:115), which
lacks the usual reex of initial */j/ (/s/ or /z/), so this could be secondary, due to the reduplicated formonce the initial */h/ was
lost. The stomach word hir stomach of animal (with sufx -r) corresponds to Chukotian ji(ji) gut, which is also
reduplicating but does have the regular reex of */j/ in initial position in Itelmen (zives/siis). This is problematical, but it
could reect original *hji or the like, just as tongue could be from *hjil (and note helel-/jelel- lick, suggestive of
reduplication also in Nivkh).
Let us return nowto hypothetical *hin-. When dealing with demonstrative/pronominal items like this which have high
frequency reexes in all the languages being compared, it is important to look at a whole range of related forms to see what is
going on. Thus Krejnovic (1979) has for Nivkh, besides the forms given, Amur ho()-/hoRo-, NS hpr-, and ES hmci-/hmra-
do thus. Further, Nivkh root h-/ES hu- actually appears as hd/hud
61
when functioning as an independent demonstrative,
and this should also not be seen in isolation, for there is also t(d)/ES tu-/tud this, which is comparable with CK ut this
under *du- above (contrasting with un (that) over there). Note also Nivkh hur-/ES hus- as the dummy object of certain
verbs like hur-tov- tie to and hur-tiv- sit somewhere (cf. Gruzdeva, 1998:29), also reduplicated hurur everywhere,
hurmiin, ES huzmi inside (from mi- inside), Amur huin, hug/hur, ES hunx/huz there, Amur tur/tuin/tug, ES tus/tunk/tur
here. The same locative base may be involved in verb stem hum-/ES hunv- be, live.
62
Moreover, there is Amur aehd, ES
ai
Secondary way out of the semi-subterranean house. It is the second element that means entrance (traditionally through the roof) in Itelmen. Note
Chukotian tll (< reduplicated *tltl) door, but also Nivkh r-/-t- push in or through. Otherwise Jakobson (1971:94), who suggests a possible earlier
form of the word *kr.
aj
si- is the regular fricative-initial form of this transitive stem (and ci- = *ti- the form occurring with suitable incorporated direct objects) whereas CK
il- contains transitivizing prex *t(n)- discussed in the section on verbal morphology.
ak
The Chukotian presupposes intermediate *tl- or the like. Note that the Nivkh formwith -- rather than -R as in Saveljeva and Taksami (1965) is from
Austerlitz for SS (1984:45).
al
But wlwl- dark, black. There seems to be entanglement between two stems here (both reduplicated).
am
ES also amavli.
an
And pa(t)- boil, cook; also CK p(pt)- boil, It. paxpa boiled sh.
ao
The nal -n may, as discussed above, represent an earlier animate class marker, whereas the -nn of the Chukotian could be fromln simply, only.
There is little else obviously in common between the numerals of Nivkh and CK, but nor is there between Aleut and Eskimo (just one and four), which are
known to be related. Possibly Nivkh ca-qr three and n-kr four (the forms used for various objects) could be related to CK ro(q) (It. coq) three and ra
(q) (It. caq) four the classier -qr in the Nivkh forms could have undergone metathesis after loss of the medial vowel. The Chukotian roots are actually
ro- and ra- (compare also Nivkh co-r and nu-r of animals, with an animate classier -r Panlov, 1962:202f.).
59
Note also -hagin, ES -zi any (one/thing), haan (Taksami, 1983 has hoRar) then (ES ha), with recognizable converb endings.
60
Note that verb stems in Nivkh that begin with /h/ quite regularly replace this by /j/ (the undergoer prex) when used transitively - except when the
undergoer is an incorporated noun (Mattissen, 2003:124f.).
61
Krejnovic (1979:305) has ES hnt, hunt that, with what looks like the same indicative/ nominalizer sufx -d/nt seen on verbs.
62
Though the sufx may be -m-/-nv-, which derives verbs from local roots. The -v- could reect pi- be if not jiv- have, be.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1373
aixnt that over there (distant) (with a deictic prex a-) Krejnovic (1979:305) has ES exnt, ehd, ahd that (a bit further off)
(=Amur ad), exnt (Amur ahd) (still further off), and aixnt (Amur aehd) that far away, all of which need to be compared
with *h-/hu. There is ample scope for explanation of the interrelated forms here (e.g. by way of metathesis, assimilation
and analogy), but this is a complicated matter and calls for detailed research, which I can only sketch here (see Panlov,
1962:240ff for a discussion of the relevant forms). The same applies to the interrogative forms listed in Table 4 (see Panlov,
1962:253ff).
One important outcome of this discussion of /h/ is corroboration of the suggestion that CK 3rd person demonstrative
n could be from *hn, going with Nivkh h-/hu-, as discussed briey in the section on verbal morphology. Note that the
Chukchi forms that go with CK in(in) are actually nin, not thus. In fact it is only Koryak and Alutor that have the
full reex inin (from or inuenced by i now). Koryak also has nnohe, she, it (< *n-ot, with an expressive
variant of ut there) alongside nRan thus, and Alutor has pronoun nno besides demonstrative n-in(a) that
(=Koryak nnin, It. nnu), which further appears to contain possessive ending *-in. So the forms to compare with Nivkh
h-/hu may well be CK nin and nut. This (reecting a common root *hn-) would better explain the forms like Nivkh
hu-g/ES hun-x, CK n-k there mentioned under nominal morphology in connection with the old CK locative case
marker.
6. Conclusion: the relationship between Chukotian, Itelmen and Nivkh
Given the lingering doubts among Russian scholars as to the exact nature of the relation between Itelmen and Chukotian
plus the cultural parallels between Kamchatka and the Amur/Sakhalin area unearthed by archaeologists (e.g. Vasilevskii,
1969:152), it is worth considering whether the evidence presented above might point in the direction of a more direct link
between Nivkh and Itelmen rather than between the former and Chukotko-Kamchatkan as a whole. One could easily
envisage, for example, an intrusive Chukotian superstrate above a common Nivkh-Itelmen basis on Kamchatka. The
traditional Itelmen and Nivkh had rather similar ways of life as sedentary shermen and hunters close to major waterways,
building similar winter and summer dwellings (the former with distinctive roof entrances), whereas Chukotians have
apparently lived for centuries in interior Chukotka as reindeer herders employing more mobile tent-like dwellings (for
further details on the relevant archaeological evidence see Fortescue, 1998:183ff).
What linguistic evidence might be adduced to support this? Actually, nothing very convincing. Certainly a number of
the Itelmen forms cited above (especially those from older sources, before Koryak inroads) have no obvious Chukotian
equivalent (e.g. Itelmen i water and East Sakhalin i river mentioned in connection with *Lr- in the previous section),
and there is a slightly better phonological t with the Itelmen cognate than the Chukotian one in cases such as Itelmen
isx, Chukotian tl(n), going with Nivkh tk father (ES tk) from assumed Proto-CKA *tl (or perhaps *tk if the
Chukotian actually contains a reex of singulative ending -ln). However, this is largely due to the rather supercial
effect of greater syncope in both Itelmen and Nivkh than in Chukotian, and in other respects Itelmen is phonologically
further removed from Nivkh than is Chukotian for example by displaying a variety of glottal elements, including
ejectives.
There is also the absence of ergativity in Itelmen and the possibility raised by Georg and Volodin (1999:229) that the
prexes of Itelmen are all borrowed from Chukotian. This would certainly make Proto-Itelmen look more like Proto-Nivkh
than Proto-Chukotian does. However, in connection with the rst of these points, recall the non-ergative alignment of the
verbal morphology of all CK languages and the arguably relative newness of ergativity in Chukotian. The second point is in
any case not particularly relevant (even if correct), since pre-Proto-CK probably also lacked prexes these arguably
represent a secondary development counterbalancing the abrasion of the old sufxes shared with Itelmen (Fortescue,
2003:85).
All in all, none of this would appear sufcient to undermine the status of Proto-CK as the unied basis to which Nivkh can
be directly compared. It should be added at this point that there is no evidence for massive borrowing having ever taken
place between Nivkh and either branch of CK (of the kind and degree seen in more recent times between Chukotian and
Itelmen), despite the probable proximity of the CK and Nivkh homelands along the shores of the Sea of Okhotsk, as the
archaeology of the region suggests.
A possible missing link between the Lower Amur and the area at present occupied by speakers of Chukotko-Kamchatkan
does, as I mentioned already in the Introduction, present itself in the formof the Tokareva Culture of the mid-northern Sea of
Okhotsk coast, dated by Kuzmin (2000:126) from about 3500 BP (refer back to Fig. 1). It is from this that the maritime Old
Koryak Culture is believed to have developed, eventually spreading to the western shore of Kamchatka (cf. Lebedintsev,
1998). This seems to represent a mixture of people and/or cultural inuences fromboth the interior (the Upper Kolyma) and
the Lower Amur region to the south. Certain tribes fromthe latter area began moving north along the Okhotsk coast starting
in the second millennium BC, a time when there would already have been Neolithic people moving down to the coast from
the interior (Lebedintsev, 1998:298). By the end of the rst millennium BC two closely related groups probably related
ethnically inhabited that mid-Okhotsk shore area, the Paleoasiatic ancestors of the foot Tungus and the Old Koryak
people themselves (Lebedintsev, 1998:301, 308).
63
It is thus possible that the language of the Tokareva people and all the
63
Actual Tungusic-speaking nomads (Evens and Evenki) only arrived in the area during the 15th to 17th century from the interior (Lebedintsev,
1998:313).
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1374
Chukotian languages that may have derived from it was not far removed from Proto-CKA, the language brought from the
Lower Amur, which would have prevailed over that of the presumably somewhat less developed people from the interior.
The early maritime adaptation these people developed allowed them to move on via the northern route to coastal
Kamchatka,
64
while in the south related groups from the Lower Amur (bearers of the Okhotsk Culture) moved out via
Sakhalin to northern Hokkaido and the Kuriles. Eventually the Old Koryak people may well have linguistically assimilated an
earlier inland population of Kamchatka (represented by the Neolithic Tarin or Tarya culture, the earliest signs of which are
dated to 5200 BP by Kuzmin, 2000:126). The Okhotsk Culture in turn was pushed back or absorbed by the Ainu coming up to
northern Hokkaido from further south under pressure from the Japanese.
The systematic evidence presented in this paper for a genetic relationship between Nivkh and CK as a whole (including
those of Tailleurs correlates that still look hopeful) must surely indicate an ancient commonality if they indicate anything
at all. That this commonality is not just due to the absorption of an Amuric substrate but springs from a single common
ancestor some 4000 years ago remains to be fully substantiated. I hope that I have at least reduced the likelihood of chance
being the principal factor behind the correspondences and similarities observed. Further lexical delving of the type I have
undertaken should increase the probability of a direct genetic relationship being involved.
References
Austerlitz, R., 1959a. Semantic components of pronoun systems: Gilyak. Word 15 (1), 102109.
Austerlitz, R., 1959b. Gilyak religious terminology in the light of linguistic analysis. The Transaction of the Asiatic Society of Japan, 3rd series 7, 207
223.
Austerlitz, R., 1982. Gilyak internal reconstruction, I: Seven etyma. In: Aronson, H.I., Darden, B.J. (Eds.), Papers from the 3rd Conference on the Non-Slavic
Languages of the USSR, Slavica = Folia Slavica, Columbus, vol. 5 (13), pp. 8188.
Austerlitz, R., 1984. Gilyak internal reconstruction, II: iron and questions related to metallurgy. In: Aronson, H.I., Darden, B.J. (Eds.), Papers from the 3rd
Conference on the Non-Slavic Languages of the USSR, Slavica = Folia Slavica, Columbus, vol. 7 (12), pp. 3848.
Austerlitz, R., 1990a. Typology in the service of internal reconstruction: Saxalin Nivx. In: Lehman, W.P. (Ed.), Language Typology 1987: Systematic Balance
in Language. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 1733.
Austerlitz, R., 1990b. Gilyak joX bride and wo village. Ural-Altaische Jahrbu cher 62, 105.
Austerlitz, R., 1994a. Gilyak internal reconstruction, III: ligneous matter. In: Aronson, H.I. (Ed.), Non-Slavic Languages of the USSR. Slavica, Columbus, pp.
229233.
Austerlitz, R., 1994b. Finnish and Gilyak sound symbolism the interplay between system and history. In: Hinton, L., Nichols, J., Ohala, J. (Eds.), Sound
Symbolism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 249260.
Bessell, N.J., 1992. Towards a phonetic and phonological typology of post-velar articulation. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia
Black, L., 1973. The Nivkh (Gilyak) of Sakhalin and the Lower Amur. Arctic Anthropology X, 1117.
Buck, C.D., 1988 (1949). A Dictionary of Selected Synonyms in the Principal Indo-European Languages. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London.
Comrie, B., 1981. Languages of the Soviet Union. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Fortescue, M., 1997. Eskimo inuence in the formation of the Chukotkan ergative clause. Studies in Language 21 (2), 369409.
Fortescue, M., 1998. Language Relations across Bering Strait: Reappraising the Archaeological and Linguistic Evidence. Cassell Academic, London.
Fortescue, M., 2003. Diachronic typology and the genealogical unity of Chukotko-Kamchatkan. Linguistic Typology 7, 5188.
Fortescue, M., 2005. Comparative Chukotko-Kamchatkan Dictionary. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin.
Georg, S., Volodin, A.P., 1999. Die Itelmenische Sprache: Grammatik und Texte. Harrassowitz, Wiesbaden.
Gruzdeva, E., 1998. Nivkh. Lincom Europa, Mu nchen.
Gruzdeva, E., 2008. Howto orientate oneself on Sakhalin: a guide to Nivkh locational terms. Evidence and Counter-evidence, Festschrift Frederick Kortlandt,
vol. 2, SSGL 33, Rodolpi, Amsterdam/New York, pp. 169188.
Hattori, T., 1962. Versuch einer Phonologie des Su dostgiljakischen. Journal of Hokkaido Gakugei University, Section 1-A 13 (1 and 2).
Jakobson, R., 1957 (1971). Notes on Gilyak. In: Studies Presented to Yuen Ren Chao on his Sixty-fth Birthday = Academia Sinica, Taiwan, Bulletin of the
Institute of History and Philology 29 (1), 255281. Reprint in 1971: Roman Jakobson, Selected Writings II. Mouton, The Hague, pp. 7297
Janhunen, J., 1996. Manchuria, an Ethnic History. Suomalais-Ugrilainen Seura, Me moires de la Socie te Finno-Ougrienne 222, Helsinki.
Krejnovic, E.A., 1979. Nivxskij jazyk. In: Solntsev, V.M. (Ed.), Jazyki Azii i Afriki, vol. 3. Glavnaja redaktsija vostocnoj literatury, Moskva, pp. 295
329.
Krejnovic, E.A., 1986. Ob imenax prostranstvennoj orientatsii v nivxskom jazyke. In: Skorik, P.Ja. (Ed.), Paleoaziatskie jazyki. Nauka, Leningrad, pp. 157
166.
Kuzmin, Y.V., 2000. Radiocarbon chronology of the Stone Age cultures on the Pacic coast of northeastern Siberia. Arctic Anthropology 37 (1), 120131.
Lebedintsev, A.L., 1998. Maritime cultures of the north coast of the Sea of Okhotsk. Arctic Anthropology 35 (1), 296320.
Levin, M.G., 1963. Ethnic Origins of the People of Northeastern Asia. University of Toronto Press, Toronto.
Levinson, S.C., 2003. Space in Language and Cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Maslova, E., 2001. Review of Georg, S., Volodin, A. P. (1999). Linguistic Typology 5, 105110.
Mattissen, J., 2003. Dependent-head Synthesis in Nivkh. John Benjamins, Amsterdam/Philadelphia.
Mudrak, O., Nikolaev, S., 1989. Gilyak and Chukchi-Kamchatkan as Almosan-Keresiouan languages: lexical evidence. In: Shevoroshkin, V. (Ed.), Explorations
in Language Macrofamilies. Universita tsverlag Brockmeyer, Bochum, pp. 6787.
Nakagawa, H., Sato, T., Saito, K., 1993. Saharin ni okeru Nivkhgo kisogoi no chiikisa. In: Murasaki, K. (Ed.), Saharin no Shosuminzoku (Ethnic Minorities in
Sakhalin). Kokusai Gakujutsu Kenkyu Seika Hokokusho. Hokudai (Hokkaido University) Press, Sapporo, pp. 209254.
Otaina, G.A., Nedjalkov, V.P., 2007. Reciprocal constructions in Nivkh (Gilyak). In: Nedjalkov, V. (Ed.), Reciprocal Constructions. John Benjamins,
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, pp. 17161747.
Panlov, V.Z., 1962. Grammatika nivxskogo jazyka, Vol. 1. Nauka, Moscow, Leningrad.
Sapir, E., Swadesh, M., 1952 (ms.). Wakashan comparative vocabulary. Boas Collection of the American Philosophical Society Library, Philadelphia.
Saveljeva, V.N., Taksami, C

.M., 1965. Russko-nivxskij slovar. Sovetskaja enciklopedija, Moskva.


Saveljeva, V.N., Taksami, C

.M., 1970. Nivxsko-russkij slovar. Sovetskaja enciklopedija, Moskva.


Schrenck (S

renk), L.von, 1883. Ob inorodtsax Amurskogo kraja, Vol. 1. Imp.-AN, St. Petersburg.
Schrenck (S

renk), L. von, 18921900. Reisen und Forschungen im Amur Lande in den Jahren 18541856, Anhang zum 3. Band, bearb. von W. Grube.
Akademie der Wissenschaften, St. Petersburg.
Shternberg, L.Ja., 1999. The social organization of the Gilyak. In: Grant, B. (Ed.), Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History, vol. 82.
64
In doing so they may have come into contact with Eskimo-speaking people who contributed to their developing maritime technology, well before
certain groups started moving permanently inland on Chukotka, where they adopted a reindeer-herding lifestyle learnt from Tungusic people.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1375
Tailleur, O.G., 1960. La place du Ghiliak parmi les langues pale osibe riennes. Lingua 9, 113147.
Takahashi, M., 1942. Karafuto Giriyakugo. Asahi Shimbunsha, Osaka.
Taksami, C

.M., 1983. Slovar nivxsko-russkij i russko-nivxskij. Prosvescenie, Leningrad.


Tangiku, I., Tanzina, N.Ya., Nitkuk, N.V., 2008. Basic Vocabulary of the Sakhalin Dialect of Nivkh Language (Nogliki Dialect). Institute of Languages and
Cultures of Asia and Africa, Tokyo University of Foreign Studies, Asian and African Lexicon No. 51
Vajda, E., 2010. Yeniseian, Na-Dene, and historical linguistics. Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska, New series, vol. 5 (12), pp. 100118.
Vasilevskii, R.S., 1969. The origin of the Ancient Koryak Culture on the northern Okhotsk coast. Arctic Anthropology VI (1), 150164.
Zhukova, A.N., 1974. Sufgirovannij artikel v korjakskom jazyke. Sklonenie v paleoaziatskix i samodijskix jazykax Tezis dokladov, Leningrad.
M. Fortescue / Lingua 121 (2011) 13591376 1376

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi