Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 5

Epilepsy & Behavior 11 (2007) 20–24

www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Autism: The first firm finding = underconnectivity?


John R. Hughes *

Department of Neurology, University of Illinois Medical Center at Chicago, 912 South Wood Street, Chicago, IL 60612, USA

Received 14 December 2006; revised 13 March 2007; accepted 14 March 2007


Available online 24 May 2007

Abstract

In January 2005, J.R. Hughes and M. Melyn published an electroencephalographic study on autistic children and found 46% with
seizures and also a relatively high prevalence of 20% with epileptiform discharges but without any clinical seizures (Clin EEG Neurosci
2005;36:15–20). Because the discharges have always been viewed as focal events and the clinical seizures as requiring spread, the
conclusion from these data was that children with autism may have a deficiency of corticocortical fibers. Since that time many MRI
and functional MRI studies have been published confirming that one of the first findings in this devastating condition is underconnec-
tivity. Specific findings are the thinning of the corpus callosum and the reduced connectivity, especially with the frontal areas and also the
fusiform face area. Other studies involving positron emission tomography scans, magnetoencephalography, and perception have added
to the evidence of underconnectivity. One final point is the initial overgrowth of white matter in the first 2 years of life in autistic children,
followed later by arrested growth, resulting in aberrant connectivity; myelination of white matter will likely be significant in the etiology
of autism.
! 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Cerebral cortex; White matter; Magnetic resonance imaging; Functional magnetic resonance imaging; Fusiform face area; Corpus callosum;
Frontal area; Brain circuits; Myelination; Autism

1. Introduction on autism and autistic spectrum disorders that are consis-


tent with the original conclusions from the EEG study.
In January 2005, J.R. Hughes and M. Melyn [1] pub- The evidence is now clear that one major finding in autism
lished an EEG study on autism, reporting seizures in is the underconnectivity within the brain. This finding is
46%, as well as a relatively high prevalence of epileptiform consistent with the behavior of these children, who tend
discharges (20%) in others who did not have a history of to concentrate on some object, rather than on any person,
clinical seizures. Although 20% may not seem high, this but without any significant relationship to other sensory
value was significantly different from that of the control modalities, as may be expected from disconnected cerebral
group of matched children without autism. Because dis- circuits.
charges are usually regarded as an exquisitely focal finding
and the seizures require some spread from the focus, the
final conclusion from these data was that a clue to one 2. Theoretical considerations and methods
problem in autistic children may be a deficiency of cortico-
cortical fibers to account for this presumed lack of spread 2.1. Other theories
from a focus. After that study, 19 studies—mainly MRI
and functional MRI (fMRI) studies—have been published Some general conclusions relevant to autism are that
growth dysregulation [2] is likely involved and also that
mechanical factors from cortical folding influencing the
*
Fax: +1 312 996 4169. lamina [3] may also be implicated. A separate and different
E-mail address: jhughes@uic.edu view of this disorder is that females are better than males as

1525-5050/$ - see front matter ! 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.03.010
J.R. Hughes / Epilepsy & Behavior 11 (2007) 20–24 21

‘‘empathizers,’’ dealing with people, and males are better density is an index of neural connectivity and that
‘‘systemizers,’’ dealing with the rules governing the patients with autism exhibit less density in the genu,
behavior of things [4]. Autism is then viewed as an rostrum, and splenium of the corpus callosum, as a hypo-
‘‘extreme male brain’’ [5]. Another theory relates to plasia, rather than as an atrophy. In 2006, Vidal et al. [17]
‘‘mirror neurons,’’ action-coding cells that are activated reported a significant reduction in both the splenium and
both by passive observation of actions and by active execu- genu of the corpus callosum, and in the same year, Boger-
tion of the same movements [6]. These neurons are consid- Megiddo et al. [18] confirmed that the corpora callosa
ered to be reflected in mu waves (8–12 Hz) from central were disproportionately small, reflecting decreased inter-
scalp areas, known to appropriately attenuate in children hemispheric connectivity. Finally, Alexander et al. [19]
when either moving themselves or observing movement in also reported small corpus callosum volumes, in addition
others. The conclusion was that such an observation— to slow processing speeds. For example, the latter patients
execution matching system—exists even in the immature with small corpora callosa had significantly lower IQs
brain but that such a circuit in autism may be associated than controls.
with a ‘‘faulty mirror neuron’’ system.
3.1.2. Frontal area
2.2. Methods (fMRI) Another area of interest often mentioned in autistic chil-
dren is the frontal area. Reduced connectivity was indi-
The next eight studies that are mentioned deal with cated in the (fMRI) finding that bilateral inferior frontal
fMRI as a method to investigate autism, determining func- areas [20] were deficient in BOLD signal correlation with
tional neural connections. The first study used low-fre- the time series in the visual area. These findings were
quency oscillations that were temporally correlated in viewed as compatible with the hypothesis of ‘‘mirror neu-
functionally related brain regions to quantify the degree ron’’ defects mentioned previously. A general comment
of functional connectivity by detecting similarities in the was that a ‘‘dorsal stream connectivity’’ in autism was
oscillation, called ‘‘patterns with hierarchical clustering’’ not likely fully functional, based on BOLD signals during
[7]. The next study used a ‘‘time series to estimate fre- visuomotor coordination, reporting reduced connectivity
quency-dependent correlational matrices’’ [8]. The next with bilateral frontal areas. Another study demonstrated
study to determine functional connectivity used ‘‘concur- significant localized gray matter reductions within the
rent spontaneous activity and spatial independent compo- frontostriatal circuits and within the parietal networks.
nent analysis’’ [9]. Two other studies used the decrease in One other report on the frontal area, by Courchesne and
activity during various tasks [10], and one referred to a fre- Pierce [21], reviewed different methods. Evidence was
quent finding of decreased cerebral blood flow during a found that connectivity within the frontal lobe was disorga-
given task [11]. Other studies used ‘‘network modulation nized and inadequately selective, whereas connectivity
from rest to a given task’’ [12], measuring ‘‘absolute brain between the frontal cortex and other systems was poorly
state maps’’ from rest to a given activity [13] or ‘‘linear cor- synchronized and weakly responsive based on inferences
relational coefficients’’ [14] as a means of determining func- from stimulation data. One final conclusion was that the
tional connectivity. reduced long-distance corticocortical reciprocal activity
Thus, examining the complex multivariate correlational that appeared in their studies would impair frontal lobe
analyses in the rise and fall of the BOLD signal can help to function. Also reported were decreases in volume beyond
determine connectivity. the frontal area, namely, in ventral and superior temporal
gray matter, left internal capsule, fornices, and cerebellar
3. Decreased connectivity white matter [22]. The data were suggestive of abnormal
anatomy and connections of the limbic–striatal social brain
3.1. MRI studies system in autistics.

3.1.1. Corpus callosum 3.1.3. Other studies


The first group of studies discussed here used MRI to Other MRI studies have shown that minicolumnar
determine underconnectivity. One of the most common width and mean neuron and nucleolar cross sections are
findings in autism has been the decrease in the size of smaller in autistic children. The findings suggest a bias
the corpus callosum. As early as 1993, Courchesne et al. toward shorter connectivity fibers [23]. One other study
[15] reported the thinning of the corpus callosum as part demonstrated changes in cerebellar gray and white matter
of white matter volume loss, in addition to the loss in the volume [24], and a final MRI study showed a decrease in
parietal lobes. Also, the cortical volume loss in the same the gray matter in the right thalamus [25], although with
parietal lobes was found to extend into the adjacent supe- unexpected increases in the right fusiform gyrus, tempo-
rior frontal and occipital lobes. The cortical volume loss ral–occipital region, and left frontal pole. The latter
either could be secondary to disconnection or could result increases may be explained by an additional finding of
in underconnectivity or possibly be unrelated. Eleven increased total gray matter volume, which may reverse in
years later, Chung et al. [16] confirmed that white matter later years.
22 J.R. Hughes / Epilepsy & Behavior 11 (2007) 20–24

3.2. Functional MRI studies the latter studies may be a reflection of the short period
during which investigators have studied this problem, but
3.2.1. Pre-2006 investigations the emphasis on frontal areas seems to be clear. The pres-
It should be made clear that these studies do not directly ent section indicates that most of these studies were done
assess connectivity, and conclusions are drawn from indi- after 2005.
rect measurements. Most of these studies were published
in 2005 and 2006. Exceptions include the report from 3.3. Other studies
2003 [26] involving patients performing finger movements,
which demonstrated abnormal variability and scatter of One positron emission tomography (PET) study was
functional maps, suggesting early-onset disturbances in performed to demonstrate reduced functional connectivity
the development of cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways by showing less activation in the medial prefrontal, ante-
in autism. Two other exceptions to very recent publications rior, and superior temporal cortex and the connections to
were reported in 2004. One showed less activation in the extrastriate region [35]. A magnetoencephalography
Broca’s left inferior frontal gyrus, but especially less syn- (MEG) study revealed an abnormal left hemisphere
chronization between various cortical areas, related to gamma oscillation (40 Hz), ‘‘augmenting connectivity theo-
‘‘neurobiological foundations of underconnectivity in aut- ries of autism by demonstrating deficiencies in long-range
ism’’ [27]. The patients were tested during sentence compre- neural communication’’ [36]. In three studies involving per-
hension. The other study [28] showed reduced activation in ception, similar conclusions were drawn. In one study [37],
responses to the faces of strangers in the fusiform face area, increased amplitude of intrusive saccades and reduced
but also in the medial frontal lobe and the amygdala. The latency of target fixation led to the conclusion of faulty
general conclusion was that autistic children exhibit defects functional connectivity in corticocerebellar networks. In
in the systems that modulate the fusiform face area. How- another investigation [38], autistic children were found to
ever, it is impossible to distinguish whether the major issue be deficient in identifying complex texture-defined gratings,
is an abnormal area affecting other regions or a primary leading to the conclusion of atypical neural connectivity. In
disorder of connectivity. the third study [39], autistic children showed a lack of
‘‘attentional modulation,’’ especially for social stimuli,
3.2.2. 2006 investigations of frontal areas and the conclusion of poor connectivity between extrastri-
As in the MRI studies, the 2006 fMRI studies empha- ate and striate regions was drawn. One final study [40] con-
sized the frontal areas. Silk et al. [29] studied patients per- cluded that the developmental process was disrupted in
forming a mental rotation task and reported a dysfunction autistic children by showing ‘‘abnormal synthesis capacity’’
in frontostriatal networks in autism by showing less activa- with respect to brain serotonin. It is clear that the latter
tion in the lateral and medial frontal, dorsolateral prefron- studies were not directly measuring connectivity, but pro-
tal, and anterior cingulate cortex and also in the caudate duced data that were only consistent with underconnectivity.
nucleus. Turner et al. [30] studied patients during a visuo-
motor task and found that the circuits from BOLD signal 4. Emphasis on facial area
cross correlation with this task were less pronounced or
even absent in association with the orbitofrontal area and One of the major clinical findings in the autistic spec-
caudate nuclei, as well as the associative, oculomotor, trum is the lack of apparent recognition of faces, as these
and motor areas. These findings indicated an inefficiently children tend to concentrate on things and not people. A
organized functional connectivity between these regions. number of studies have provided evidence to account for
Similar results were reported by Just et al. [31], who studied this phenomenon. In 2003, Frith [41] reported that autis-
patients performing a Tower of London test to assess exec- tic children failed to activate the fusiform face area during
utive function. They reported lower synchronization face perception tests, also demonstrating weak activation
between the frontal and parietal areas, in addition to a of medial frontal cortex and the superior temporal gyrus.
smaller corpus callosum, indicating reduced intracortical The conclusion was that this finding was a marker for
connectivity or underconnectivity. Other reports, using abnormal connectivity, possibly from lack of neural prun-
patients processing sentences with different imagery con- ing. During the next year, Waiter et al. [25] reported an
tent, were that functional connectivity among cortical unexpected increase in gray matter volume in the right
regions, especially the language and spatial centers, is not fusiform gyrus, as well as in the right temporal–occipital,
well synchronized, again indicating underconnectivity left frontal, and medial frontal cortex. The conclusion was
[32]. Still another study [33] using a cognitive task reported that this finding may ‘‘reflect an abnormally functioning
loosely connected anterior–posterior circuits as an indica- social cognitive neural network.’’ Finally, in 2006 Bird
tion of cortical underconnectivity. A similar conclusion et al. [39] reported a lack of attentional modulation of
of ‘‘reduced corticocortical connectivity’’ was reached in face-selective regions, concluding abnormal connectivity.
another report [34] of patients performing a visuomotor Two other studies specifically involved fMRI. One was
task, even though the opposite was found for subcorti- a study that autistic children exhibit less fusiform activity
cal–cortical connectivity. The different areas implicated in in response to strangers than to familiar faces. In general,
J.R. Hughes / Epilepsy & Behavior 11 (2007) 20–24 23

they demonstrated a more limited network in response, autism is between the second and third years of life.
even to familiar faces, at the same time showing a Consistent with the latter relationship, data have shown
decrease in medial frontal lobe function. Their conclusion that the most significant decrease in (periventricular)
was that the dysfunction in the face area may reflect white matter during maturation occurs during the first 2
defects in systems that modulate the area, rather than a years [51], and patients with developmental delay have,
defect in the face area itself. The last study [42] reviewed in fact, exhibited a significant reduction in the content
the evidence that abnormal functional mechanisms are of myelinated white matter [48]. As mentioned previously,
involved in face recognition, resulting in ‘‘dysfunctional early brain overgrowth occurs, but later arrest is then
reciprocal cortico-subcortical connections.’’ observed [47]. The factors that may play a significant role
in this maturation include the myelin-forming oligoden-
5. Increase in white matter drocytes, and injury to their progenitors likely would con-
tribute to the pathogenesis of white matter development
With considerable evidence indicating a decrease in the [52]. One other factor that may be significant is hypogly-
networks involving various areas, it may be somewhat cemia, as the latter condition inhibits oligodendrocyte
surprising that an increase in white matter could be found. development and myelination, also triggering apoptotic
The earliest evidence came from Frith’s study [41] showing cell death in oligodendrocyte precursor cells [53]. This
an increase in total brain volume during the first few years reviewer is unaware of whether or not hypoglycemia is
as a marker of abnormal connectivity from lack of pruning. common in autism. These latter considerations are only
In the next year (2004), Courchesne [43] was more specific clues to uncovering the etiology of autism.
by pointing out that the overgrowth occurred during the In summary, the first firm finding in children with aut-
first 2 years of life, followed by abnormally slow or arrested ism or the autistic spectrum is underconnectivity within
growth in the next few years. This disruption was viewed as the brain. Both MRI and fMRI studies during the past 2
related to circuit formation, resulting in aberrant connec- years have shown thinning of the corpus callosum and
tivity. In the same year, Herbert et al. [44] spelled out that reduced connectivity, especially in the frontal and fusiform
the increased brain volume was an unexplained white mat- face areas, in addition to other circuits in the brain. The ini-
ter enlargement, involving more the later or longer myeli- tial overgrowth of the white matter in the first 2 years is
nating regions. These changes were viewed as affecting often followed by arrested growth, resulting in aberrant
primarily intrahemispheric and corticocortical connections. connectivity. The role of myelination of white matter is
In the next year, Herbert [45] confirmed that a growth likely to be significant. The goal for scientists is now to pro-
spurt shortly after birth was primarily from white matter vide further evidence that underconnectivity is indeed pres-
and that neuroinflammation was often present to explain ent with more definitive studies like postmortem
impaired complex processing and underconnectivity. In neuroanatomical analyses and to discover the reasons for
2006, Hendry et al. [46] reported that the increase in cere- the development of this underconnectivity in the hope of
bral white matter, especially in the frontal and parieto- eliminating the causes of this devastating disorder.
occipital areas, suggested abnormalities in white matter tis-
sue water content. Courchesne et al. [47] later discussed the References
early brain overgrowth and later arrest as a microstructural
maldevelopment resulting in short-distance overconnectiv- [1] Hughes JR, Melyn M. EEG and seizures in autistic children and
adolescents: further findings with therapeutic implications. Clin EEG
ity that was ineffective. In addition, the conclusion was a
Neurosci 2005;36:15–20.
‘‘failure of long distance cortico-cortical coupling and a [2] Akshoomoff N, Pierce K, Courchesne E. The neurobiological basis of
reduction in frontal-posterior reciprocal connectivity.’’ autism from a developmental perspective. Dev Psychopathol
2002;14(3):613–34.
6. Myelination [3] Hilgetag CC, Barbas H. Role of mechanical factors in the morphol-
ogy of the primate cerebral cortex. PloS Comput Biol 2006;2(3):22.
[4] Baron-Cohen S, Belmonte MK. Autism: a window onto the
As white matter myelination generally reflects the pro- development of the social and the analytic brain. Annu Rev Neurosci
gression of functional brain maturation [48], it is reason- 2005;28:109–26.
able to explore the process of myelination as a putative [5] Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in
etiology of autism. The time when this disorder usually the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science
reveals itself clinically is between 2 and 3 years of age 2005;310:819–23.
[6] Lepage JF, Theoret H. EEG evidence for the presence of an action
[43], and, therefore, it is this same time interval that observation–execution matching system in children. Eur J Neurosci
may lead us to discover the etiology of autism. The onset 2006;23:2505–10.
of myelination, as indicated by the expression of myelin [7] Cordes D, Haughton V, Carew JD, Arfanakis K, Maravilla K.
basic protein, is usually at 1 year of age, and the progres- Hierarchical clustering to measure connectivity in fMRI resting-state
data. Magn Reson Imaging 2002;20:305–17.
sion to ‘‘adultlike’’ staining occurs at nearly 2 years of age
[8] Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E. A
[49]. Other data would indicate that adult values are resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network
reached during the third year [50]. Thus, a crucial time with highly connected association cortical hubs. Neuroscience
for myelination and also for the clinical expression of 2006;26:63–672.
24 J.R. Hughes / Epilepsy & Behavior 11 (2007) 20–24

[9] Van de Ven VG, Formisano E, Prvulovic D, Roeder CH, Linden DE. [31] Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ.
Functional connectivity as revealed by spatial independent compo- Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism:
nent analysis of fMRI measurements during rest. Hum Brain Mapp evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task
2004;22:165–78. and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex
[10] Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connec- 2007;17:951–61.
tivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode [32] Kana RK, Teller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA.
hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;22:165–78. Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with
[11] Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Rao SM, Cox decreased functional connectivity. Brain 2006;129:2484–93.
RW. Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a [33] Cherkassky VL, Kana RK, Keller TA, Just MA. Functional
functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 1999;11:80–95. connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. NeuroRe-
[12] Jiang T, He Y, Zang Y, Weng X. Modulation of functional port 2006;17:1687–90.
connectivity during the resting state and the motor task. Hum Brain [34] Mizuno A, Villalobos ME, Davies MM, Dahl BC, Muller RA.
Mapp 2004;22:63–71. Partially enhanced thalamocortical functional connectivity in autism.
[13] Waites AB, Stanislavsky A, Abbott DF, Jackson GD. Effect of prior Brain Res 2006;1104:160–74.
cognitive state on resting state networks measured with functional [35] Castelli F, Frith C, Happe F, Frith U. Autism, Asperger syndrome
connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;24:59–68. and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to
[14] Bokde AL, Lopez-Bayo P, Meindl T, et al. Functional connectivity animated shapes. Brain 2002;125:1839–49.
of the fusiform gyrus during a face-matching task in subjects with [36] Wilson TW, Rojas DC, Reite ML, Teale PD, Rogers SJ. Children
mild cognitive impairment. Brain 2006;129:1113–24. and adolescents with autism exhibit reduced MEG steady-state
[15] Courchesne E, Press GA, Yeung-Courchesne R. Parietal lobe gamma responses. Biol Psychiatry 2007;in press.
abnormalities with MR in patients with infantile autism. Am J [37] Nowinski CV, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Takarae Y, Sweeney JA.
Roentgenol 1993;160:387–93. Oculomotor studies of cerebellar function in autism. Psychiatry Res
[16] Chung MK, Dalton KM, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ. Less white 2005;127:11–9.
matter concentration in autism: D voxel-based morphometry. Neu- [38] Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J. Enhanced and
roImage 2004;23:242–51. diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends
[17] Vidal CN, Nicolson R, DeVito TJ, et al. Mapping corpus callosum on stimulus complexity. Brain 2005;128:2430–41.
deficits in autism: an index of aberrant cortical connectivity. Biol [39] Bird G, Catmur C, Silani G, Frith C, Frith U. Attention does not
Psychiatry 2006;60:218–25. modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum
[18] Boger-Megiddo I, Shaw DW, Friedman SD, et al. Corpus callosum disorders. NeuroImage 2006;31:1614–24.
morphometrics in young children with autism spectrum disorder. J [40] Chugani DC, Muzik O, Behen M, et al. Developmental changes in
Autism Dev Disord 2006;36:733–9. brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and nonautistic children.
[19] Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of Ann Neurol 1999;45:287–95.
the corpus callosum in autism. NeuroImage 2007;34:61–73. [41] Frith C. What do imaging studies tell us about the neural basis of
[20] Villalobos ME, Mizuno A, Dahl BC, Kemmotsu N, Muller RA. autism? Novartis Found Symp 2003;251:149–66.
Reduced functional connectivity between V1 and inferior frontal [42] Brambilla P, Hardan AY, di Nemi SU, et al. The functional
cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. NeuroIm- neuroanatomy of autism. Funct Neurol 2004;19:9–17.
age 2005;25:916–25. [43] Courchesne E. Brain development in autism: early overgrowth
[21] Courchesne E, Pierce K. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be followed by premature arrest of growth. Ment Retard Dev Disabil
talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance Res Rev 2004;10:106–11.
disconnection. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2005;15:225–30. [44] Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Makris N, et al. Localization of white
[22] McAlonan GM, Cheung V, Cheung C, et al. Mapping the brain in matter volume increase in autism and developmental language
autism: a voxel-based MRI study of volumetric differences and disorder. Ann Neurol 2004;55:530–40.
intercorrelations in autism. Brain 2005;128:268–76. [45] Herbert MR. Large brains in autism: the challenge of pervasive
[23] Casanova MF, van Kooten IA, Switala AE, et al. Minicolumnar abnormality. Neuroscientist 2005;11:417–40.
abnormalities in autism. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 2006;112:287–303. [46] Hendry J, DeVito T, Gelman N, et al. White matter abnormalities in
[24] Kates WR, Burnette CP, Eliez S, et al. Neuroanatomic variation in autism detected through transverse relaxation time imaging. Neuro-
monozygotic twin pairs discordant for the narrow phenotype for Image 2006;29:1049–57.
autism. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:539–46. [47] Courchesne E, Redcay E, Morgan JT, Kennedy DP. Autism at the
[25] Waiter GD, Williams JH, Murray AD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Whiten beginning: microstructural and growth abnormalities underlying the
A. A voxel-based investigation of brain structure in male adolescents cognitive and behavioral phenotype of autism. Dev Psychopathol
with autistic spectrum disorder. NeuroImage 2004;22:619–25. 2005;17:577–97.
[26] Muller RA, Kleinhans N, Kemmotsu N, Pierce K, Courchesne E. [48] Pujol J, Lopez-Sala A, Sebastian-Galles N, et al. Delayed myelina-
Abnormal variability and distribution of functional maps in autism: tion in children with developmental delay detected by volumetric
an fMRI study of visuomotor learning. Am J Psychiatry MRI. NeuroImage 2004;22:897–903.
2003;160:1847–62. [49] Haynes RL, Borenstein NS, Desilva TM, et al. Axonal development
[27] Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical in the cerebral white matter of the human fetus and infant. J Comp
activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in Neurol 2005;484:156–67.
high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain [50] Schneider JF, Il’yasov KA, Hennig J, Martin E. Fast quantitative
2004;127:1811–21. diffusion-tensor imaging of cerebral white matter from the neonatal
[28] Pierce K, Haist F, Sedaghat F, Courchesne E. The brain response to period to adolescence. Neuroradiology 2004;46:258–66.
personally familiar faces in autism: finding of fusiform activity and [51] Zhang L, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Casey BJ, Heier LA, Ulug
beyond. Brain 2004;127:2703–16. AM. MR quantitation of volume and diffusion changes in the
[29] Silk TJ, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, et al. Visuospatial processing and developing brain. Am J Neuroradiol 2005;26:45–9.
the function of prefrontal-parietal networks in autism spectrum [52] Back SA. Perinatal white matter injury: the changing spectrum of
disorders: a functional MRI study. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1440–3. pathology and emerging insights into pathogenetic mechanisms.
[30] Turner KC, Frost L, Linsenbardt D, McIlroy JR, Muller RA. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2006;12:129–40.
Atypically diffuse functional connectivity between caudate nuclei and [53] Yan H, Rivkees SA. Hypoglycemia influences oligodendrocyte
cerebral cortex in autism. Behav Brain Funct 2006;2:34. development and myelin formation. NeuroReport 2006;17:55–9.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi