Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

University of Massachusetts - Amherst

ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst
International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track 2011 ICHRIE Conference

Jul 30th, 9:45 AM - 10:45 AM

The Influence of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive Affect, Negative Affect and Delightedness
Joong-won Lee
California State University - Northridge, miplain@gmail.com

Linda Ralston
University of Utah, linda.ralston@health.utah.edu

Gary Ellis
Texas A & M University - College Station, gellis@ag.tamu.edu

JoungKoo Park
Dongguk University, jkpark85@dongguk.ac.kr

Joong-won Lee, Linda Ralston, Gary Ellis, and JoungKoo Park, "The Influence of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive Affect, Negative Affect and Delightedness" ( July 30, 2011). International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track. Paper 9. http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9

This Empirical Refereed Paper is brought to you for free and open access by the Hospitality & Tourism Management at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact scholarworks@library.umass.edu.

The Influence of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive Affect, Negative Affect and Delightedness

Lee et al.: Effect of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive affect, Negative affect, and Delightedness

Joong-won Lee, Ph.D. Dept. of Recreation and Tourism Management California State University, Northridge Northridge, CA USA Linda S. Ralston, Ph.D. Dept. of Parks, Recreation, and Tourism University of Utah Salt Lake City, UT USA Gary D. Ellis, Ph.D. Dept. of Recreation, Park, and Tourism Sciences Texas A & M University College Station, Texas USA Joung-Koo Park, Ph.D. Dept. of Tourism Management Dongguk University Gyeongju, KyungBook, South Korea

Final Paper Submitted to the International CHRIE 2011 Conference April 26, 2011

Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011

The Influence of International Nationality CHRIE and Service Quality on Positive Affect, Negative Affect and Conference-Refereed Track, Event 9 [2011] Delightedness ABSTRACT This study examined the interaction effect of nationality and dimensions of service quality on positive affect, negative affect and delightedness. Participants (n=216) viewed eight video clips of staged service encounters. Video clips ranged from 5-8 minutes in duration. Based on an orthogonal design, each video depicted a unique combination of levels of five service quality dimensions: reliability, responsiveness, empathy, tangibles, and assurance (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1988). Following each clip, participants were asked to complete self-report measures of positive affect, negative affect and delightedness. Data were analyzed using linear mixed model procedures. The presence or absence of each service quality dimension in the model was indicated with dummy vectors. Results indicate that service experience of guests is substantially affected by the five service quality dimensions; those dimensions did interact with culture/nationality. The interaction between nationality and reliability, assurance, and empathy was significant for all three outcome variables (delightedness, positive affect, and negative affect). In the analysis of positivity of affect, the interaction of tangibles and nationality was also significant. For negative affect, significant interactions were observed for four of the five service quality factors: reliability, responsiveness, assurance, and empathy. For delightedness, significant interactions were observed for reliability, assurance, and empathy. This study suggests that service providers might optimize guest experiences by focusing on preparation of staff to meet empathy, reliability, and assurance needs of guests, in addition to the other service quality dimensions. Key words: Service Quality, Nationality, Experimental Design, Positive affect, Negative affect, and Delightedness INTRODUCTION The hospitality and tourism industry depends heavily on the quality of affective guest experiences (emotional states) that result from service encounters (e.g., Stauss and Mang,1999: Williams and Buswell 2003). Thus, customer emotion (pleasure/positive) is one of the significant antecedents of customer loyalty and intention to purchase (Barsky, & Nash, 2002; Han & Back, 2008; Jang, & Namkung, 2009; Ryu, & Jang, 2007). This guest experience provides a positive effect on the customer purchases. Moroni-Hall observed that People buy based on emotion, and people rebuy based on an emotional connection (Gunter, 2006). According to Ellis and Rossman (2008), it is clear that the extent to which consumers value purchases they make is http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 2

determined not only the utility is acquired their purchases, but, often more Lee et al.: Effect ofby Nationality andthat Service Quality onthrough Positive affect, Negative affect, and Delightedness importantly, for the emotional and motivational states that are associated with the purchase process and with ownership experiences(p.2). As a result, a significant body of literature has been developed to describe service quality strategies that may ensure that guest experiences are positive. Among the more notable of these strategies are derived from the SERVQUAL (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1988) and SERVPERF (Cronin and Taylor 1994) perspectives. These models underscore the importance of five dimensions of service quality: reliability, responsiveness, assurance, tangibles, and empathy. When services are provided correctly (reliability), timely (responsiveness), with a sense of competence and commitment (assurance), with obvious concern for the individual guest (empathy), and in an attractive, orderly, and functional setting, guests are expected to exhibit pleasure (positive affect) and a sense of satisfaction with the service encounter. In the absence of such service performances, negative affect is expected, along with low positive affect and low satisfaction. The service quality factors that are assumed to elicit these immediate guest experiences may, however, function very differently, depending on the personal values and expectations of guests from different cultures (Ladhari, Bressolles, & Zins, in press; Ueltschy, Laroche, Zhang, Cho, & Yingwei,.2009). Many previous research studies (Donthu and Yoo, 1998; Furrer, Liu, & Sudharshan, 2000; Imire, Durden, & Cadogan, 2000; Kettinger, Lee, & Lee, 1995; Malhotra, Ulgado, Agarwal, Shainesh, & Wu, 2005; Matilla, 1999; Shih, 2006; Winsted, 1997) have found specific service quality dimensions to be of particular importance to people of different cultures (Reynolds and Smith, 2010). As such, it is reasonable to assume that culture may interact with service quality dimensions in terms of its effect on immediate affective experiences of guests. An
Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011 3

important question for service quality in the hospitality and tourism International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track, Eventindustry, 9 [2011] then, is identification of service quality performance elements that are particularly sensitive to people from different cultures. With such information, hosts may structure encounters that produce optimal experiences for guests with diverse cultural backgrounds. In one study of the effect of culture on service quality judgments, Shih (2006) found a significant effect of culture on guest reports of service quality in a Taiwanese restaurant. That design, however, failed to take into account the confounding effect of the habit of Western travelers/guests to provide gratuities to service providers. This study extended previous research (Shih, 2006 ; Lee, 2008) on service quality and culture by using an experimental design and controlling for nuisance variables that have not been previously controlled in correlational investigations of the relationships among service quality, culture, and guest experiences with international guests perspective. Specifically, this study examined the effect of culture and dimensions of service quality on positive affect, negative affect and delightedness of tourists/guests following a service encounter. METHOD Data were collected on-site at universities from students, faculty, and staff in three respective countries: China (n =74), Korea (n=99), and the United States (n=43). The university affiliated population was considered to be acceptable (but not ideal) for the purpose of this study. Substantial cultural differences exist across these three groups. University affiliated populations (students, faculty, and staff) are a group of frequent travelers and guests of hotels. It is important to note, however, that this population is not representative of broader populations of travelers. Direct generalization from this sample to international business and vacation travelers is, of course, not scientifically justifiable. The absence of scientific sampling and differences in age,
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 4

current income, family composition, travel motives, and a hostaffect, of other render Leepersonal et al.: Effect of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive affect, Negative and variables Delightedness such generalizations to be tenuous at best. The sample does, though, vary by the key concept of interest (nationality), and it does include individuals who have the proclivity to travel. The average number of days of staying at hotel in the past year for this sample was 10.79. The average age was 26.21 years old (range from 18 to 54 years old for US citizen, from 19 to 53 years old for Korean, from 20 to 50 years old for Chinese). The sample consisted of 87 males and 126 females. Outcome measures included five-item measures of positivity of affect and negativity of affect (Watson and Clark 1994), along with a single item measure of delightedness. Examples of positivity of affect items included happy, friendly, and pleased. The alpha reliability coefficient for the positivity of affect scale was .94. Five items were also used to assess negativity of affect (Watson and Clark 1994). Examples of these items included upset, hostile, and distressed. Cronbachs alpha for this scale was .91. Based generally on Kanos model (Kano, Serku, Takahashi, and Tsuji 1984), a single-item delightedness scale was created by using a graphic of a temperature thermometer. Kanos model of guest satisfaction asserts that product features that deliver unanticipated value elicit delight; a state of high satisfaction. These features have been described as Exciting Quality features in the Six Sigma literature (e.g., Pyzdek, 2003). An example of an Exciting Quality feature would be the unexpected addition of a free breakfast or an upgraded, contour pillow following purchase of a hotel room. Delightedness resulted from unexpected/surprising features of an offering that add value to the experience (Oliver, Rust, & Varki, 1997; Oliver, 2010). The delight of the customer has been identified as essential to survival in a fierce market environment (Whittaker, 1991; Oliver et al., 1997). Delight is considered as resulting from this high level of surprisingly
Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011 5

positive disconfirmation (Rust, & Oliver, 2000, p. 87). Oliver al. (1997) International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track, et Event 9 [2011]cite another definition of delight from Schlossberg (1993): Customer delight...is a strong, positive, emotional reaction to a product or service (p. 314). This identification of delightedness as an emotion is consistent with prior research linking delight to customers emotional state (Oliver et al., 1997). Five descriptors of delightedness levels were positioned at different mercury levels on the thermometer: fully delighted, satisfied, indifferent, dissatisfied, and disgusted. Scores on the single-item measure could range from 0 to 10. Lee, Ellis, and Ralstons study (in press) supported the validity of inferences that may be made from scores produced by the single-item measure. Each participant viewed eight video clips of staged service encounters and reported her or his experiences (affect measures and delightedness) following viewing of each clip. Video clips ranged from 5-8 minutes in duration and were recoded in English. To clarify participants understanding of the content of video, researchers and interpreters provided vocabularies that are relevant to check-in and check-out procedures before watching the each video. In addition, while viewing the first video clip, interpreters briefly explained the story line. Based on an orthogonal array (See Table 1), each video depicted a unique combination of levels of five service quality dimensions: reliability, responsiveness, empathy, tangibles, and assurance (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1988). In a given video clip, for example, the service encounter portrayed might reflect high reliability, low responsiveness, high empathy, low assurance, and high (positive) tangibles. All video clips included the same actors, the same service encounter, and the same setting, but the script and set were modified to manipulate the service quality dimensions. The video clips were professionally produced. Actors in the video were thee volunteers from the
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 6

United After videoand clip, respondents were asked to Negative imagineaffect, that they were in the role LeeStates. et al.: Effect ofeach Nationality Service Quality on Positive affect, and Delightedness of the guest depicted in the scene and then complete the questionnaire containing the positive and negative affect items and the single item measure of delightedness. The English version of the questionnaire was translated into Korean and Chinese. A minimum of three bilingual people for each language examined the accuracy, clarity and naturalness in translation for the questionnaire. Table 1. Orthogonal array of customer service performance represented in video clips
Video Clip 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Tangibles High High Low Low High Low High Low Reliability High High Low High Low High Low Low Responsiveness Low High High Low Low High High Low Assurance Low High Low High Low Low High High Empathy Low High Low Low High High Low High

Data were analyzed using linear mixed model procedures. The linear mixed model approach can provide many benefits compared to repeated measures. According to de Bruin, Smits, Rikers, and Schmidt (2008), the benefit of linear mixed model that Regular repeated measures analyses of variance typically cannot cope with missing data, and only take into account participants with complete data,... Also, these analyses [linear mixed model] estimate group effects,These models include random regression effects that account for the influence of participants on repeated measurements,(p.481).

Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011

The orthogonal design of thisCHRIE study Conference-Refereed included five factors (i.e., tangibles, International Track, Event 9 [2011] assurance, empathy, reliability, and responsiveness) and interactions between nationality and each of the five service quality factors. A full model (all main effects and interactions) would involve 11 separate hypothesis tests. Replication across three outcome variables (positivity of affect, negativity of affect, and delightedness) would yield 33 hypothesis tests. At the .05 level of significance, the experiment-wise error rate would be .82. An approach is required that balances the need for some degree of control of experimentwise error with the need to retain sufficient power to detect treatment effects given the limited resources available to complete the investigation. The approach taken was a compromise between the need for control of experiment-wise alpha and the need to avoid Type II error (i.e., failure to reject a false null). The approach was to consider the sets of five tests of interaction effects for each outcome variable (positivity and negativity of affect and delightedness) to represent a separate family of comparisons. This is consistent with Maxwell and Delaneys (2004, p. 291) position that families of comparisons represent conceptually distinct questions. By testing each hypothesis within each family at the .05 level of significance, the family-wise error rate was controlled to .226 (i.e., 1-(1-.05)5). It is notable that the experiment-wise alpha was .537, given this approach. RESULTS Results are presented in Table 2. Significant service quality- by-nationality interaction effects were identified in all three models. For delightedness, significant interactions were with reliability, assurance, and empathy. For positive affect, significant interactions between nationality and tangibles, reliability, assurance, and empathy were found. For negative affect, nationality interactions were significant for all service quality factors except tangibles.
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 8

2 R reduction error) values provide of the relative strength of PRE Lee et (proportional al.: Effect of Nationality andin Service Quality on Positive evidence affect, Negative affect, and Delightedness

these relationships. The empathy-by-nationality interaction effect is particularly notable. These values were .29, .35, and .38 for delightedness, positive affect, and negative affect, respectively. For delight and positive affect, all other R2PRE values were less than .05. For negative affect, R2PRE values were all .18 or higher. These results indicate that individuals representing the three nationalities had very different responses to empathy in the service encounters. Further, the R2PRE values indicate that, cross-nationality differences in responses to service encounters have much greater effect on negative affect than on delightedness and positive affect. Table 2. Result of linear mixed model for delightedness Interaction effect Delightedness F Tangibles * Nationality Reliability * Nationality Responsiveness * Nationality Assurance * Nationality Empathy * Nationality
*p < .05

Positive Affect F 5.817* 7.42* 2.27 15.26* 3.12* R2PRE .02 .04 .04 .02 .35

Negative Affect F 1.19 3.67* 3.98* 18.60* 37.68* R2PRE .19 .22 .18 .21 .38

R2PRE .01 .04 .04 .03 .29

2.46 3.25* 0.16 14.83* 4.16*

Interaction plots are presented in Figure 1. Review of those tables reveals the nature of crossnationality sensitivity to service quality factors. With respect to the interaction involving assurance, participants who were Chinese were less sensitive to assurance changes, as compared to participants from the United States and Korea.

Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011

International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Track, Event 9 [2011]

Figure 1. Mean comparisons by nationalities for dimensions of service quality on positive affect, negative affect, and delightedness
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 10

Lee et al.: Effect of Nationality and Service DISCUSSION Quality on Positive affect, Negative affect, and Delightedness

Results of this study (taking into account the confounding effect of the habit of Western travelers/guest to provide gratuities to service providers) are consistent with the previous study of Donthu and Yoo (1998) and partially consistent with the study of Mattila (1999). Donthu and Yoo (1998) observed that the assurance dimension is an important element for collectivist societies. Assurance was found to interact with culture/nationality for all three variables (positive affect, and negative affect, and delightedness) in this study. Mattila (1999) reported that Western customers place substantial importance on tangible cues. However, this study revealed that tangible was found to interact with nationality for only positive affect and not for negative affect and delightedness. This study is inconsistent with the studies of Furrer et al. (2000), Malhotra, et al. (2005), and Lee (2008). Furrer, et al. (2000) identified individuals described as Self-confidents, who were categorized as possessing small power distance, high individualism, medium femininity, low uncertainty avoidance and long term-orientation, did not put importance on the assurance. However, this study revealed that Chinese participants did not generate a different average score of positive affect, thus good or bad service did not influence assurance. Lee (2008) found no statistically significant service quality-by-culture interaction effect. Perhaps these inconsistent results are a function of different populations studied across these investigations. Another interesting result in the study is that Korean participants tend to score high on negative affect with all dimensions of service quality. Particularly, the responsiveness for Korean participants scored higher on negative affect than participants from the US and China. This result is not consistent with the study of Malhotra, et al. (2005). That study identified that elements of responsiveness and speed of service were considered as an important attribute by Western
Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011 11

individualist cultures.International The result CHRIE of this Conference-Refereed study might be related the 9 Korean Track,to Event [2011] cultures unique Pali-Pali syndrome ( a preference of speed) over service quality. Overall, this study observed that respondents from the three countries did react differently to manipulations of empathy. Further, it appears that ones nationality may have a more pronounced impact for negative affect than that of positive affect or delightedness. This observation requires additional research to better understand the role of culture and higher expectation of service quality. A limitation of this study was participant fatigue. This study required 50 minutes of video viewing and evaluation. Participants were guided to watch the video clips with eight scenarios that were similar in story lines with certain aspects of service quality manipulated. The similarity of the story line may have positively contributed to non-English speaking participants understanding of the video but also influenced the boredom and increased fatigue of the all viewers. We observed increased levels of fidgeting and heavy sighs after the fifth scenario. After completing this study, a few participants expressed concerns about the length of this study. Smith and Reynolds (2002) and Reynolds and Smith (2010) indicated that response bias can be a problem in cross-cultural studies. This study cannot be free from this issue. Response bias includes response sets and response styles. Response sets indicate the respondents desire to express a specific image of oneself by the way that they manipulate that image (Watkins & Cheung, 1995). Response style is a habit that provides impact on test participants responses. There is clear evidence that response styles differed among people from different cultures. For example, Japanese and Chinese students were more likely to respond at the middle point on the scale than U.S. and Canadian students. U.S. students respond at the extreme value more often than the Japanese, Chinese, or Canadian students (Chen, Lee, & Stevenson, 1995).
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 12

The interaction effect of empathy, andaffect, assurance nationality dimensions Lee et al.: Effect of Nationality and Service reliability, Quality on Positive Negativeby affect, and Delightedness on all outcome variables was particularly notable. These results suggest that in preparing frontline personnel in the hospitality industry, managers must not only train workers to maintain an attractive dcor, orderly procedures, well maintained environment, accuracy, and responsive service, but they must also attend to communication patterns that communicate assurance and empathy when they encounter international guests/travelers. Practices such as using guests names, sharing positive comments about their place of residence, and engaging guests by actively listening and responding to their experiences may be essential for optimizing affective experiences of our guests. The interactions of tangible and responsiveness by nationality suggest that those who are in the hospitality business might facilitate different needs of both international and domestic guests and then customize their service to enhance their guests experiences. Compared to other dimensions of service quality, the interaction effect of empathy and nationality is particularly salient on all three outcome variables. This result indicates that individuals representing three nationalities had very different responses to empathy in the service encounters. In addition, consistent with previous research, the empathy dimension generates significant differences between perceptions of good and bad service (Lee, 2008). Empathy has a notable impact on the positivity of guests experience, and the strength of that effect differs by nationality. Therefore, personnel in the hospitality industry need to pay particular attention to empathy. Finally, crossnationality differences in responses to service encounters have much greater effect on negative affect than on delightedness and positive affect. This result suggests that the failure of accommodating different needs from international and domestic guests will greatly affect guests negative experiences.
Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011 13

REFERENCES Track, Event 9 [2011] International CHRIE Conference-Refereed Barsky, J. D., & Nash, L. (2002). Evoking emotion: Affective keys to hotel loyalty. Cornell Hotel & Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 43(1), 39-46. Chen, C., Lee, S-Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Response style and cross-cultural comparison of rating scales among East Asian and North American students. Psychological Science, 6(3), 170-175. Cronin, J. J., and Taylor, S. A. (1994). SERVPERF versus SERVQUAL: Reconciling Performance-based and Perceptions-minus-expectations Measurement of Service Quality. Journal of Marketing, 58(1): 125-131. de Bruin, A.B. H., Smits, N., Rikers, R.M. J. P. & Schmidt, H.G. (2008). Deliberate practice predicts performance overtime in adolescent chess players and drop-outs: A linear mixed models analysis, British Journal of Psychology, 99, 473497 DOI:10.1348/000712608X295631 Donthu, N., and Yoo, B. (1998). Cultural Influences on Service Quality Expectation. Journal of Service Research, 1(2): 178-186. Ellis, G.D., & Rossman, J. R. (2008). Creating value for participants through experience staging: parks, recreation, and tourism in the experience industry, Journal of Park and Recreation Administration, 26 (4), 1-20. Furrer, O., Liu, B. S-C., & Sudharshan, D. (2000). The relationships between culture and service quality perceptions. Journal of Service Research, 2(4), 335-371. Gunter, H. (2006). Emotional connections build brands. Hotel and Motel Management, 221(6): 24-27. Han, H, & Back, K-J. (2008). Relationships among image congruence, consumption emotions, and customer loyalty in the lodging industry, Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research,32 (4), 467-490. Imrie, B. C., Durden, G., & Cadogan, J. W. (2000). Towards a conceptualization of service quality in the global market arena. Advances in International Marketing, 10 (Supplement 1), 143-162. Jang, S.C., & Namkung, Y. (2009). Perceived quality, emotions, and behavioral intentions: Application of an extended MehrabianRussell model to restaurants, Journal of Business Research, 62(4), 451460 Kano, N., Serku, N., Takahashi, F. and Tsuji, S. (1984). Attractive Quality and Must-be Quality. The Journal of the Japanese Society for Quality Control, 14(2): 39-48.
http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9 14

Kettinger, W.Effect J., Lee, C. C. & Lee, S. (1995). Global measures information service quality: A Lee et al.: of Nationality and Service Quality on Positive affect, of Negative affect, and Delightedness cross-national study. Decision Sciences, 26 (5), 569-588. Ladhari,R., Pons, F., Bressolles, G., & Zins, M. (in press). Culture and personal values: How they influence perceived service quality, Journal of Business Research, doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2010.11.017 Lee, J. W. (2008). The influence of culture and dimensions of service quality on positive affect, negative affect, and delightedness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah, Utah. Lee, J. W., Ellis, G.D., Ralston, L. S. (in press). Psychometric evaluation of a single-item, Kanobased measure of delightedness with service experiences, Tourism Analysis, Luke, D. A. (2004). Multilevel Modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Malhotra, N. K., Ulgado, F. M., Agarwal, J., Shainesh, G., & Wu, L. (2005), Dimensions of service quality in developed and developing economies: Multi-country cross-cultural comparisons, International Marketing Review, 22(3), 256-278. Mattila, A. S. (1999). The Role of Culture in the Service Evaluation Process. Journal of Service Research, 1(3): 250-261. Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction: A behavioral perspective on the consumer (2nd ed.). New York, NY :M.E. Sharpe. Oliver, R. L., Rust, R. T., & Varki, S. (1997). Customer delight: Foundations, findings, and managerial insight. Journal of Retailing, 73(3), 311-336. Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V. A. and Berry, L. L. (1988). SERVQUAL: A multiple-item scale for measuring consumer perceptions of service quality. Journal of Retailing, 64(4): 1240. Pyzdek, T. (2003). The Six Sigma Handbook. New York: McGraw-Hill. Raudenbush, S. W., and Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Reynolds, N., & Smith, A. (2010). Assessing the impact of response styles on cross-cultural service quality evaluation: A simplified approach to eliminating the problem. Journal of Service Research, 13(2), 230 243
Published by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst, 2011 15

Rust, R. T., & Oliver,International R. L. (2000). Should we delight theTrack, customer? Journal CHRIE Conference-Refereed Event 9 [2011] of the Academy of Marketing Science, 28(1), 86-94. Ryu, K., & Jang, S C. (2007). The effect of environmental perceptions on behavioral intentions through emotions: The case of upscale restaurants, Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 31(1), 56-72 Schlossberg, H. (1993). Departing exec says customer satisfaction is a continuous process. Marketing News, 27 (April 26), 8. Shih, C-H. (2006) Effects of Culture on Service Quality and Customer Satisfaction Ratings. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah Smith, A. M., & Reynolds, N. L. (2002). Measuring cross-cultural service quality. International Marketing Review, 19(5), 450- 487. Stauss, B., & Mang, P. (1999). ``Culture shocks'' in inter-cultural service encounters?, Journal of Service Marketing, 13(4/5), 329-346. Ueltschy L.C., Laroche, M., Zhang, M., Cho, H., Yingwei, R.(2009). Is there really an Asian connection? Professional service quality perceptions and customer satisfaction. Journal of Business Research,62(10), 972979 Watkins, D., & Cheung, S. (1995). Culture, gender and response bias: An analysis of responses to the self-description questionnaire. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(5), 490504. Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect ScheduleExpanded Form. (Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, 1994). Whittaker, B. (1991). The path to excellence. Canadian Business Review, 18(4), 19-21 William, C., and Buswell, J. (2003). Service Quality in Leisure and Tourism. Cambridge, MA: CABI Publishing. Winsted, K. F. (1997). The service experience in two cultures: A behavioral perspective. Journal of Retailing, 73 (3), 337-360.

http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/ICHRIE_2011/Saturday/9

16

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi