Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

Vocabulary

size, TOEIC
scores, and
testwiseness
Masaya Kanzaki
Kanda Institute of Foreign
Languages

Reference data:
Kanzaki, M. (2010). Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness. In A. M. Stoke (Ed.), JALT2009 Conference Proceedings. Tokyo: JALT.

The relationship between vocabulary and English proficiency tests was investigated among 31 learners.
They were given the Vocabulary Levels Test (120 questions) and a TOEIC practice test (200 questions),
and their scores were compared. The correlation between the two tests was .64. However, when the
subjects were divided into two groups based on the number of times they had taken the TOEIC, the
figure for the expert group (n = 15, exam taken = 8 times or more) was .76 and that for the novice
group (n = 16, exam taken = 6 times or fewer) was .49. This paper examines the difference between
the two groups in relation to testwiseness, a respondents capacity to use knowledge about a test itself,
in spite of what the test is supposed to measure, in order to gain a high score.
31120TOEIC200
20.64TOEIC2
1580.7616
60.492

huge amount of English vocabulary, EFL learners have to learn a lot of new
words when acquiring proficiency in the language. The development of a learners
overall English ability often correlates with growth of vocabulary knowledge, and vocabulary size is reflected in a learners English proficiency test performance. Nation and Meara
(2002) observed that there is a relatively close relationship between how many words you
know, as measured on the standard vocabulary tests, and how well you perform on reading
tests, listening tests and other formal tests of your English ability (p. 50).
iven the

Several studies have explored this close relationship. Qian (1999) compared the Vocabulary
Levels Test scores with the TOEFL reading comprehension section scores of 74 ESL students in
Canada and found that the two tests correlated well at .78. Similarly, Beglar and Hunt (1999)
compared the 2000 Word Level Test scores with the TOEFL scores of 496 EFL students in Japan,
and the correlation between the two tests was .71.
These results agree with what is often observed in the classroom. When students cannot
answer practice test questions correctly, it is often a lack of vocabulary hindering their success.

JALT2009 Conference Proceedings


ONLINE

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

740

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

any ability, knowledge, or skills to improve a TOEIC score that


do not include English ability, which the exam is designed to
measure. This includes how familiar test takers are with the test
format and question types, how well they manage time, and
how well they can guess answers, all of which improve with
test taking experience. To investigate the effect of testwiseness,
vocabulary and TOEIC scores were examined in relation to the
numbers of times the participants had taken the TOEIC, on
the assumption that those who have taken the test many times
have higher levels of testwiseness than those with less test taking
experience.

This is particularly noticeable among those preparing for the


Test of English for International Communication (TOEIC). The
purpose of this study is to investigate how closely vocabulary
size and TOEIC scores are related.

In addition, this study interpreted the results in relation to


testwiseness, which Millman, Bishop, and Ebel (1965) defined as:

A subjects capacity to utilize the characteristics and formats


of the test and/or the test taking situation to receive a high
score. Test-wiseness is logically independent of the examinees
knowledge of the subject matter for which the items are supposedly measures. (p. 707)

Since this study investigated testwiseness in regard to the


TOEIC, the term used in this paper refers to the capacity to use

Table 1. TOEIC score distribution


TOEIC Score

Under 500

500545

550595

600645

650695

700745

750795

800845

850895

900990

Note: Based on highest scores. One of the participants had never taken the TOEIC.

Table 2. Numbers of TOEIC tests participants had taken previously


TOEIC taken

10

11

12

13

14

15

16+

Table 3. Age distribution


Age group

2029

3039

4049

5059

6069

70+

12

Note: Three of the participants did not disclose their ages.

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

741

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

Method
A vocabulary test and a TOEIC practice test were given to 31
participants, and the scores of the two tests were compared.

1.

business

3.

horse

Participants

4.

Participants were 31 adult learners who took part in a four-day


intensive TOEIC preparatory course at a private English school
in Tokyo in August 2007. Of the participants, 20 were male, 11
were female, and all of them were Japanese speakers. The group
was widely varied in terms of TOEIC scores (Table 1), numbers
of TOEIC tests taken (Table 2), and age (Table 3).

Materials
As a measure of vocabulary size, four levels of the Vocabulary
Levels Test were used (30 questions per level, 120 questions in
total). As a substitute for the actual TOEIC, 200 questions from a
practice test book were used.

Vocabulary Levels Test


In this study, Schmitts (2000) version of the Vocabulary Levels
Test Version 1 was used, which is included in Paul Nations
vocabulary resource booklet available on his website.

The Vocabulary Levels Test consists of five levels, namely the


2000, 3000, 5000, 10000, and academic word levels. The levels
are based on frequency counts, except the academic word level,
which is based on Coxheads (2000) Academic Word List. In this
study, the 10000 level was excluded because the words tested in
that level were considered too difficult for the participants.

Each level of the test has 10 clusters of 6 words with 3 definitions, which makes a total of 30 questions per level. An example
of a cluster is shown below.

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

2.

5.
6.

clock

_____ part of a house

pencil

_____ something used for writing

shoe

_____ animal with four legs

wall

Test takers are instructed to match the words on the left with
the definitions on the right. Schmitt, Schmitt, and Clapham
(2001) reported the Cronbachs alpha reliability values for the
2000, 3000, 5000, and academic word levels of Version 1 as .920,
.929, .927, and .958, respectively. Therefore, this vocabulary test
is reliable.

TOEIC practice test


Questions from TOEIC Shin Koshiki Mondaishu Vol. 2 (2006) [the
New Official Practice Tests for the TOEIC Vol. 2] were used
in this study. As the Educational Testing Service, the creator
of the actual TOEIC, provided the materials, the two practice
tests in the book are very close to the actual TOEIC in terms
of vocabulary, question types, topic types, length, level of
difficulty, and formatting of questions in the test book.
The TOEIC consists of 200 multiple-choice questions, half
of which are in the listening section with the other half in the
reading section. In the actual TOEIC, raw scores (0200) are
converted to scaled scores (10990). In this study, only raw
scores are used for calculations and analyses.
The TOEIC has seven parts; the first four parts are in the
listening section and the last three parts are in the reading
section. Details of each part are shown in Table 4.

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

742

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

Table 4. Seven parts of TOEIC


Section

Listening

Reading

Part

Task

# of Qs

For each question with photo, listen


to four sentences and choose the one
that best describes the image.

10

Listen to a question or statement


followed by three responses and
choose the most appropriate
response.

30

Listen to a conversation and answer


three questions.

30

Listen to a short talk and answer


three questions.

30

Choose a word or phrase to fill in a


blank in a sentence.

40

Choose a word or phrase to fill in


three blanks in a passage.

12

Read a passage or a set of two


passages and answer two to five
questions.

48

Procedures
Two practice tests with a total of 400 questions in the practice
test book were used during the four-day intensive course.
To provide the same number of questions on each day of the
course, the two tests were divided into four half tests and the
participants answered 100 questions each day (i.e., the first
half of Test One on the first day, the second half of Test One on
the second day, the first half of Test Two on the third day, and
the second half of Test Two on the fourth day). In addition, the

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

participants were given the Vocabulary Levels Test; the 2000


and 3000 levels were administered on the second day, and the
5000 and academic word levels were administered on the third
day. Only those who attended both the second and the third
days were included in this study. The results of the practice
tests administered on the first and fourth days (i.e., the first half
of Test One and the second half of Test Two) were excluded
from this study because some of the participants who attended
both the second and the third days missed either the first or the
fourth day. The data collected on the second and the third days
were compiled and descriptive statistics and Pearson product
moment correlation coefficients were computed.

Results
Descriptive statistics
Table 5 shows the descriptive statistics for the Vocabulary Levels
Test. The mean of the academic word level is between those of
the 2000 and 3000 levels, which agrees with the results reported
by Schmitt et al. (2001).

Table 5. Descriptive statistics for Vocabulary


Levels Test (N = 31)
Level

2000

3000

5000

30

30

30

30

27.58

24.42

21.29

25.48

98.77

Range

16

17

20

20

67

SD

3.29

4.24

5.20

Academic

3.71

Total
120

14.57

Note: Total = all four levels combined

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

743

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

The Kuder-Richardson 21 reliability values for the listening


section, the reading section, and the whole of the TOEIC
practice test are .87, .87, and .92, respectively. The entire test
shows high reliability, and the listening and reading sections
show moderately high reliability. The descriptive statistics for
these three sections are shown in Table 6.

Table 7. Correlations between TOEIC practice


and vocabulary tests (N = 31)
Level

Listening

Reading

TOEIC-Total

2000

.28

.72

.55

3000

.36

.71

.61

Table 6. Descriptive statistics for TOEIC practice


test (N = 31)

5000

.42

.67

.61

Academic

.27

.62

.49

Section

VLT-Total

.39

.76

.64

Listening
100

Reading
100

Total
200

70.39

74.71

Range

49

48

83

SD

12.25

11.51

21.17

.87

.87

.92

3.97

3.77

5.51

K-R21
SEM

145.10

Note: Total = entire practice test

Correlation
Table 7 shows the correlation coefficients between the two tests.
The reading section of the practice test correlated well with the
vocabulary test at .72 with the 2000 level, .71 with the 3000 level,
.67 with the 5000 level, .62 with the academic word level, and
.76 with the total of the four levels. The figures for the listening
section, however, were much lower at .28, .36, .42, .27, and .39,
respectively. The correlation coefficient between the totals of the
two tests was .64, which is statistically significant (p < .01).

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

Note: TOEIC-Total = total of TOEIC practice test. VLT-Total =


total of Vocabulary Levels Test.

Dividing participants into two groups


To investigate the effect of testwiseness, the participants
were divided into two groups based on how many times
they had previously taken the TOEIC. This was done with
the assumption that the level of testwiseness increases with
test taking experience. As shown in Table 2, 16 out of the 31
participants had taken the test 6 times or fewer and the other
15 had taken it 8 times or more. The data was divided into two
categories, namely the novice group (TOEIC taken = 6 times or
fewer) and the expert group (TOEIC taken = 8 times or more),
and recalculated.

Table 8 shows a comparison of the vocabulary test results


of the two groups. The means of the novice group are slightly
higher than those of the expert group, except the 5000 level. The
difference in the total scores is 1.25 out of 120.

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

744

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

Table 8. Comparison of vocabulary test results between two groups


Level

2000

3000

5000

Academic

Total

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

27.75

27.40

25.00

23.80

20.94

21.69

25.69

25.27

99.38

98.13

Range

12

16

13

17

18

20

13

19

43

67

12.91

16.13

SD

2.73

3.79

3.95

4.45

5.24

5.13

3.23

4.14

Note: Novice = novice group (n = 16). Expert = expert group (n = 15).

Table 9. Comparison of TOEIC practice test results between two groups


Section

Listening

Reading

Total

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

Novice

Expert

68.88

72.00

75.56

73.80

144.44

Range

45

44

37

46

70

83

SD

12.45

11.82

10.73

12.23

20.07

22.27

145.80

Table 10. Comparison of correlations of two tests between two groups


Level

Listening
Novice

Reading
Expert

Novice

TOEIC-Total
Expert

Novice

Expert

2000

.00

.54

.48

.88

.25

.77

3000

.23

.53

.55

.84

.44

.74

5000

.37

.47

.64

.72

.57

.65

Academic

.01

.53

.50

.71

.27

.67

VLT-Total

.22

.56

.65

.85

.49

.76

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

745

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

Table 9 shows a comparison of the TOEIC practice test results


of the two groups. Compared with the novice group, the expert
group performed better in the listening section but worse in the
reading section. The average total score of the expert group is
1.36 higher than that of the novice group.

Table 11. Cumulative coverage for listening and


reading sections of TOEIC practice test

Table 10 shows a comparison of the correlation coefficients


of the two tests between the two groups. The correlations are
distinctively higher among the expert group than the novice
group with all the 15 pairs compared.

Discussion
Possible reasons for weak correlations
As shown in Table 7, the reading section of the practice test
correlated well with the vocabulary test while the correlation between the listening section and the vocabulary test was
weak. One possible reason for this is the Vocabulary Level Test
measures vocabulary knowledge only in written form. Since
the listening section requires test takers to recognize words in
spoken form, an aspect of vocabulary knowledge not measured
in the Vocabulary Levels Test is relevant in the listening section
of the TOEIC.

Another possibility is that vocabulary size is less important


in the listening section than in the reading section because
the vocabulary load is lighter in the listening section. The
vocabulary profile of the TOEIC practice test used in this study
indicates that the coverage of high frequency words is higher in
the listening section than in the reading section. Table 11 shows
the cumulative coverage of the first five frequency levels of 1000
word bands.

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

Word list

Listening

Reading

1000

86.56

74.40

2000

94.07

84.93

3000

95.89

88.63

4000

96.80

91.36

5000

97.14

92.60

Note: Figures are percentages. For vocabulary analysis, RANGE


(Heatley, Nation, & Coxhead, 2002) was used with British
National Corpus base lists.
In addition, processing time to use conscious knowledge
about vocabulary (Beglar & Hunt, 1999, p. 149) may have
influenced the correlation. Listening allows less processing time
than reading, and therefore the participants may not have had
enough time to fully utilize their vocabulary knowledge in the
listening section.

Differences between novice and expert groups


Table 8 shows that the average vocabulary test score of the
novice group is 1.25 higher than that of the expert group, and
Table 9 shows that the average TOEIC practice test score of
the expert group is 1.36 higher than that of the novice group.
Despite the lower vocabulary test scores, the expert group had
higher TOEIC practice test scores than the novice group, which
can be attributed to the higher level of testwiseness the expert
group attained through test taking experience.

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

746

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

The most notable difference is in the correlations between the


vocabulary and TOEIC practice tests. As shown in Table 10, the
correlations between the two tests are significantly higher among
the expert group than the novice group. The high correlations
among the expert group suggest that vocabulary size was an
important factor affecting their TOEIC scores, whereas the low
correlations among the novice group suggest that vocabulary size
was not a determining factor for TOEIC scores among the group.
This can be explained in relation to the levels of testwiseness
among each group: the level of testwiseness was even among the
expert group and varied widely among the novice group.
There are three possible reasons for the varied levels of
testwiseness among the novice group. First, testwiseness increases
not only through taking actual tests, but also through taking
mock tests. This study only looked at how many times the
participants had taken the TOEIC and did not consider how
much they had studied in preparation for the test. Those who
have done many practice tests are likely to be more testwise
than those who have done only a few, even when the number
of the TOEIC tests taken is the same. Second, testwiseness gained
through experience taking other tests can be applied when taking the TOEIC. This study only looked at test taking experience
of the TOEIC; therefore, it is possible that those who had gained
a high degree of testwiseness from other tests were included
in the novice group. Last, the rate of progress in regard to
testwiseness may vary. Testwiseness is a type of ability; therefore,
the rate at which people acquire it may differ from person to
person. For example, some learners may need to take six tests
to gain a certain degree of testwiseness while others only need to
take three tests to reach the same level.
Similar varieties likely existed among the expert group. In
other words, some had taken more practice tests than others,
some had more test taking experience from having taken other
tests, and some were more adept at acquiring testwiseness.

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

Nevertheless, the level of testwiseness appears to be even despite


the differences mentioned above. This suggests that there is a
ceiling to how much learners can improve their testwiseness;
those in the expert group had the same level of testwiseness because they had reached this testwiseness ceiling by the time they
had taken the TOEIC eight times.

Testwiseness threshold
The testwiseness ceiling is a threshold for fair score comparison.
Until learners have come to this ceiling, their TOEIC scores
cannot be compared fairly. This is because those with low levels
of testwiseness have an unfair disadvantage; their scores are
likely to be lower than those with higher levels of testwiseness,
even when English proficiency levels are the same. In other
words, when learners have reached the testwiseness threshold,
their TOEIC score begins to work as an indicator of their English
proficiency.

Determining how many times learners need to take the


TOEIC to reach the testwiseness threshold is not a simple task.
Level of testwiseness is affected not only by how many times the
TOEIC has been taken, but by the factors mentioned above, such
as how much test preparation has been done, how much test
taking experience through other tests has been acquired, and
how capable the test taker is at gaining testwiseness. The results
of this study show that those who have taken the TOEIC eight
times or more have reached the threshold. However, this study
does not take the other factors into consideration, and therefore
it is still open to speculation whether taking the TOEIC eight
times alone is enough to reach the threshold.

Conclusion
This study revealed that vocabulary size and TOEIC scores
correlate to a moderate degree, as the correlation coefficient

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

747

Kanzaki Vocabulary size, TOEIC scores, and testwiseness

between the Vocabulary Levels Test and the TOEIC practice


test was .64. The reading section of the practice test correlated
well with the vocabulary test at .76, and the listening section
correlated poorly at .39.

However, higher correlations were obtained among those


who had taken the TOEIC eight times or more. This suggests
that they had reached a uniformly high level of testwiseness
through test taking experience. The findings in this study also
suggest that levels of testwiseness vary widely among those who
do not have much test taking experience. There seems to be a
testwiseness threshold after which learners can achieve TOEIC
scores that accurately reflect their English proficiency. Those
who have not reached that threshold are likely to get lower
scores than their more testwise peers even when they have the
same level of English proficiency.
The implication is that TOEIC scores of those who have
not reached the testwiseness threshold cannot be compared
fairly because the difference in testwiseness may distort the
relationship between their TOEIC scores and English ability. To
compare learners English ability using the TOEIC, the effect of
testwiseness should thus be considered.

Bio data

Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34(2),


213-238.

Heatley, A., Nation, I. S. P., & Coxhead, A. (2002). RANGE (version 32)
[Computer program]. Retrieved January 12, 2007, from http://www.
vuw.ac.nz/lals/staff/Paul_Nation

Institute for International Business Communication (2006). TOEIC Shin


Koshiki Mondaishu Vol. 2 [The new official practice tests for the TOEIC
Vol. 2]. Tokyo: IIBC.
Millman, J., Bishop, C. H., & Ebel, R. (1965). An analysis of test-wiseness. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 23(3), 707-726.

Nation, I. S. P. (2005) Vocabulary resource booklet [Collection of files].


Retrieved February 14, 2007 from http://www.victoria.ac.nz/lals/
staff/paul-nation.aspx.

Nation, I. S. P., & Meara, P. (2002). Vocabulary. In N. Schmitt (Ed.), An


introduction to applied linguistics (pp. 35-54). London: Hodder Arnold.

Qian, D. D. (1999). Assessing the roles of depth and breadth of vocabulary knowledge in reading comprehension. Canadian Modern Language
Review, 56(2), 282-307.

Schmitt, N. (2000). Vocabulary in language teaching. Cambridge University


Press.
Schmitt, N., Schmitt, D., & Clapham, C. (2001). Developing and exploring the behaviour of two new versions of the vocabulary levels test.
Language Testing, 18(1), 5588.

Masaya Kanzaki recently completed his Masters degree in


TESOL at Temple University and is currently teaching at Kanda
Institute of Foreign Languages. His research interests include
vocabulary acquisition and language testing. <msyknzki@
poplar.ocn.ne.jp>

References
Beglar, D., & Hunt, A. (1999). Revising and validating the 2000 word
level and university word level vocabulary tests. Language Testing,
16(2), 131-162.

JALT2009 CONFERENCE
PROCEEDINGS

ONLINE

PREVIOUS PAGE

NEXT PAGE

FULL SCREEN

748

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi