Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 15

European Sociological Review

VOLUME 23

NUMBER 2

2007

123137

123

DOI:10.1093/esr/jcl024, available online at www.esr.oxfordjournals.org


Online publication 13 December 2006

The Omnivore Thesis Revisited:


Voracious Cultural Consumers
Oriel Sullivan and Tally Katz-Gerro

Introduction
In this article we contribute to the literature on
cultural omnivorousness by introducing to the discussion a new but related dimension of leisure consumption. Since the concept of omnivorousness was first
defined by Peterson and Simkus (1992), research on it
has rested theoretically on the way cultural capital
involves an appreciation of a wide range of cultural
forms, including the fine arts, popular culture, and folk
culture. Accordingly, measures of omnivorousness have
been primarily based on the breadth of cultural tastes.
However, another important dimension of cultural
consumption relates to the frequency of participation
in relevant leisure activities. While there is a growing
literature on omnivorousness within the sociology of
culture, there has been no previous research that
attempts to theoretically locate the frequency of leisure
participation in relation to cultural omnivorousness.
The addition of the dimension of cultural participation

to that of cultural tastes expands the study of


omnivorousness as a phenomenon related not only
to the sociology of culture and consumption but also
to the sociology of work and time, since the money
and time commitments involved in actual leisure
participation (as opposed to reports of cultural tastes
or preferences) entail considerations of money and
time management. Thus we are able to combine two
strands of research, the first of which pertains to the
familiar fields of the sociology of culture and
consumption and highlights concepts such as culture,
tastes, and distinction (Bourdieu, 1984; Peterson,
2005). The second strand, less visible in the omnivorous literature to date, pertains to time and to
the changing pace of life and leisure in late modernity
(Linder, 1970; Garhammer, 1998; Gershuny, 2000).
We develop these arguments and present empirical analyses that identify and classify a measure of
the range and frequency of leisure participation
(voraciousness), in relation to the socio-economic

The Author 2006. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

We augment measures of cultural omnivorousness, based theoretically on the breadth


of cultural tastes, with a new but related dimension of voraciousness. This reflects
a quantitative dimension of leisure consumption based upon both the range and the
frequency of leisure participation. Voraciousness is theoretically interpreted in relation to
notions of cultural repertoires, to the changing pace of work and leisure in late modernity,
and to the insatiable quality of contemporary consumption. From British time use data,
voraciousness proved to share many relationships found in the analysis of omnivorousness,
for example, with educational qualifications and jobs social status. Moreover, these
relationships persisted over time irrespective of individuals time and money resources.
Since voraciousness is associated with high status individuals, and since it is not primarily
about the availability of time or money, we argue that it is a symbolic status marker
associated with notions such as being harried, keeping busy, multitasking, and embracing
a diverse cultural consumption pattern.

124

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

and cultural indicators that have been commonly


used in the analysis of omnivorousness. We show a
consistent pattern over time in these relationships, and
we situate our findings within the theoretical discussion of cultural repertoires and cultural boundaries.

The Omnivore Thesis

Revisiting Omnivorousness
The general goal of the research described here was to
problematize, revisit, and modify the contours of the
omnivore concept, through complementing it with a
measure based on both the range and the frequency of
cultural participation. Our reconsideration of omnivorousness stems from three sources. First, Bourdieus
(1984) emphasis on the differences in the way
individuals consume rather than on what they
consume led us to look more closely at the (voracious)
pattern of consumption in addition to the (omnivorous) contents of consumption. This emphasis stems
from a perspective that sees the expression of cultural
capital as embodied in consumer actions (Holt, 1997).
Holt asserts that research on the association between
cultural capital and social divisions should rely less
on cultural contents and more on consumption
practices. This is because the content of objectified
cultural capital has become a relatively weak exclusionary mechanism due to increasingly blurred cultural
hierarchies in contemporary societies.
Campbell (1987) had earlier linked the idea of the
insatiable consumer to the unique dynamic character
of modern-day consumption. High levels of individual consumption in contemporary developed societies
stem from the insatiability of consumers (the fact that
their wants appear never to be exhaustible). Hedonistic
consumers are unlimited in their appetites for intense
emotional experiences (Bell, 1979) and their focus of
desire is the experience the consumer anticipates of the
act of consumption (McCracken, 1990). If this desire
were provided not by leisure participation in itself
but by anticipation of a novel leisure activity, we
would expect modern consumerism to involve a high
turnover of goods and experiences, or, in our terms,
a voracious pattern of cultural participation. In a
different but related account of cultural behaviour,
Schulze (1992) offers a perspective in which cultural
repertoires are characterized by the aesthetics of
excitement (Spannungschema). For Schulze, it is selfrealization that drives the cultural preferences of the
affluent middle class, who engage in various highbrow,

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

In a series of influential articles, Peterson and his


colleagues (Peterson, 1992, 1997; Peterson and Simkus,
1992; Peterson and Kern, 1996) reformulated the link
between cultural capital and social boundaries. They
argued that members of the upper class in the US, who
formerly had been defined as cultural snobs in terms
of their preference for a limited range of highbrow
cultural tastes (Levine, 1998), are turning into cultural
omnivores; namely, an upper class that experiences
and appreciates a variety of cultural tastes: highbrow,
middlebrow, and lowbrow. The omnivore thesis stands
in contrast to Bourdieus formulation of homology
in cultural stratification, that upper class groups
show preference for highbrow culture while lower
class groups prefer lowbrow culture. The omnivore
thesis also differs from accounts of cultural consumption as an aspect of lifestyles that is fluid, flexible, and
transient, in the sense that consumption patterns are
individualized (Giddens, 1991), elective, do not reflect
well-established communities, and involve short-term
commitments (Slater, 1997).
The snob versus omnivore thesis caught the attention of a number of scholars who demonstrated its
applicability and its variations in diverse cultural fields
in different countries (e.g. Bryson, 1997; Van Rees
et al., 1999; Warde et al., 1999; Holbrook et al., 2002;
Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez, 2002; Emmison,
2003). These works characterized omnivorousness in
sundry ways based either on cultural tastes (particularly musical genres: Peterson and Simkus, 1992;
Peterson and Kern, 1996; Bryson, 1997; Emmison,
2003, and reading: Van Rees et al., 1999) or on cultural
behaviour (particularly leisure activities: Lopez Sintas
and Garcia Alvarez, 2002; Holbrook et al., 2002,
but also eating habits: Warde et al., 1999).
They then looked at the social bases of the
omnivorous type. In line with several major studies
that have long demonstrated a strong correlation
between economic class and patterns of cultural
consumption and lifestyle (Weber, 1974; Gans, 1974;
Bourdieu, 1984), several studies in recent years, while
acknowledging the importance of this relationship, also
argued that the complexity of the connections between
class location and cultural tastes should be further

probed (Bihagen and Katz-Gerro, 2000; Han, 2003;


Katz-Gerro, 2002, 2004, 2006). Specifically, research on
the omnivore type generally finds that younger age,
higher education, higher income, and higher occupational status are strongly associated with omnivorous
cultural preferences (Warde et al., 1999; Van Eijck,
2001; Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez, 2002; Holbrook
et al., 2002; Emmison, 2003).

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

or dining out (for examples see Warde et al., 1999;


Van Rees et al., 1999; Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez,
2002). However, some of these previous attempts to
measure omnivorousness based upon consumption
practices have tended to elide the original meaning
of omnivorousness (a broad range of cultural tastes)
with the type and frequency of participation in different
activities. In this article we insist upon preserving the
original meaning of omnivorousness, distinguishing
it from the measure of voraciousness, which is based
on the frequency of participation in specific cultural
activities.
Additionally, most empirical studies of omnivorousness have used standard survey research methods based
on questionnaire measures (Peterson, 2005). In our
study we employed time use datato the best of
our knowledge not used before in the analysis
of omnivorousnessto investigate the relationship of
status, time and participation in cultural activities.
The final issue relates to time pressure and the need
to take time into account when describing cultural
participation. A fast pace of life is a feature of modern
and post-modern societies (Toffler, 1970; Shaw, 1998).
Findings in the literature link economic development
with time congestion and reduced opportunity for
leisure on the one hand, and a shift of time use from
production to consumption on the other (Gershuny,
2000). An early perspective on the way individuals
deal with modern society that emphasized the rhythm
of time was offered by Simmel (1950; 1957). Simmel
emphasized the tempo and multiplicity of economic,
occupational, and social life in the urban setting,
in which individuals experience many contacts, short
contacts, and a quicker tempo. In a setting characterized by fluctuations and discontinuities, individuals
assert a personality by adopting habits, styles, and
fashion that make them stand out. Rosa (2003) claims
that to understand the nature and character of modernity and the logic of its structural and cultural
developments we should add a temporal perspective
to social analysis. He uses the term social acceleration
to describe a cardinal change in temporal patterns
whereby acceleration of the pace of life becomes the
overriding principle for individuals in late modernity.
Within accelerated society, speed becomes an imperative, and time compression and the intensification of
processes are necessary.
In his discussion of the harried leisure class Staffan
Linder was one of the first to identify the change that
has occurred over time in the association of work,
leisure and class (Linder, 1970; Sullivan and Gershuny,
2001). At the turn of the nineteenth century, when
Veblen (1994[1899]) was writing about conspicuous

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

popular, and folk cultural genres, providing a mix of


cultural experiences. Highbrow culture is, according to
Schulze, related to the experience of transcendence,
popular culture is related to the experience of fun and
pleasure, and folk culture is related to the experience
of incorporation and belonging. The three realms act
as organizing principles in the utilization of culture for
this class.
In many ways our depiction of a voracious cultural
consumer also resonates with Swidlers tool kit (1986)
or cultural repertoire (2001) metaphor. Swidler argues
that a persons cultural repertoire works like a tool kit,
playing a dominant role in structuring strategies for
action. It is a repertoire of habits, skills, and styles
from which people construct their understanding
of the world and how to conduct themselves in it.
Similarly, a voracious cultural consumer feels comfortable with switching and constantly making choices
between activities, which s/he engages in for brief
periods. In terms of orientation to time, this pattern
implies a polychronic (multi-tasking) timestyle
(Feldman and Hornik, 1981).1 A polychronic orientation to time, in the context of consumer behaviour,
means avoidance of immersion in one specific consumer activity and a preference for acting on a whim
or hopping back and forth from one activity to
another (Cotte et al., 2004).
The second issue in revisiting omnivorousness,
which is closely related to the theoretical discussion
above, is a consideration of measurement. In this
research we decided to employ time use data on leisure
participation; this was in response to research that has
drawn attention to the fact that omnivorousness is
composed of several different aspects, not all of which
have received the same expression in measurement
(Warde et al., 2000; Emmison, 2003; Peterson, 2005).
Van Eijck (2000) outlined three such aspects used for
measuring the omnivorous concept: musical tastes;
attendance at high arts events; and types of leisure
activity patterns. In their early studies Peterson and
Simkus (1992) and Peterson and Kern (1996) measured omnivorousness using tastes in musical genres,
and this has continued to be an important area
of research (see, for example, Bryson, 1996, 1997;
van Eijck, 2001). Subsequently, other scholars too used
cultural preferences to operationalize omnivorousness
(Bryson, 1997; Emmison, 2003). Yet, while several
authors have emphasized the importance of measuring
what people report doing rather than relying on
self-reported cultural tastes (Van Rees et al., 1999;
Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez, 2002), fewer studies
have chosen to look at actual behaviour in relation to
leisure activity participation, such as arts attendance

125

126

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

higher-class groups. This is designated by a cultural


change from honour attached to large amounts of
leisure time to honour attached to busyness at work
(Jacobs and Gerson, 2004; Gershuny, 2005). Could a
fast pace of out-of-home leisure participation also be
conceived of as a new marker of status distinction?
According to this hypothesis, the tasting of many
different out-of-home leisure activities with a fast
turnover would imply a kind of multi-cultural
capital (Bryson, 1997) of leisure. This argument is
consonant with other claims in the literature that some
high-status groups increasingly work more but also
consume more (Schor, 1992). This indicates in such
groups a fast tempo of life and a pattern of high
turnover of cultural experiences, combining a high
capacity to spend, little free time, and a consumption profile that aims at exhibiting a diverse cultural
repertoire.
The theoretical considerations discussed here suggest
the development of a complementary measure to
omnivorousness, which combines an assessment of
the range of activities (to reflect a diverse cultural
repertoire) with the frequency of participation
(to characterize a high turnover of activities). In this
article we introduce the concept of voraciousness as
a theoretical variation of omnivorousness. Time use
data are applied to provide a measure based on the
reported frequency of participation in different leisure
activities rather than on tastes. This measure reflects
a quantitative dimension of leisure consumption
based not only on the breadth of cultural tastes
(as in omnivorousness) but also on the frequency of
participation in different leisure activities. By combining this measure with the literature on time pressure
and the pace of leisure in modern societies we are
able to readdress the concept of omnivorousness with
a theoretical emphasis on how people consume rather
than on what they consume.

Method
The Data
Our data comprised the first wave of Home OnLine,
a panel study undertaken by the Institute for Social
and Economic Research at the University of Essex,
England, of adult individuals in households in Britain.2
The first wave was conducted between October and
December (inclusive) of 1998, and was selected according to a qualified form of randomization, which
ensures inclusion of geographically clustered areas
with representation of different social strata similar
to that of the population. Selection of households was

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

consumption among the rising professional classes, the


distribution of leisure time was markedly different
from today. As an example, we can compare the
perhaps apocryphal image of the bankers luncha
long, alcoholic midday indulgencewith the image of
the present-day investment banker or stockbroker,
frenetically busy on several telephones for the entire
day and much of the night. The current and growing
association between high (earned) income and time
scarcity has already been well documented (e.g. Jacobs
and Gerson, 1998; Gershuny, 2000; Sullivan and
Gershuny, 2004). Evidence of the working hours of
better-qualified and high-income earners suggests that
as qualification levels rise, so do hours of work
(Robinson and Godbey, 1997; Gershuny, 2000).
The connection between this literature and voraciousness is that one solution, which has been
suggested for the increasing scarcity of time among
certain groups, is an increase in intensity of activities,
at work and at leisure alike. This increase in intensity
involves more activities being done simultaneously,
but it may also involve shorter spells being spent
on each activity, so that the sequence becomes more
crowded, but also more fragmented, leading to ever
heavier feelings of time pressure (e.g. Shaw, 1998;
Bittman and Wajcman, 2000; Bittman, 2002; Mattingly
and Bianchi, 2003; Southerton, 2003; Katz-Gerro and
Sullivan, 2004). Individuals feel pressured to reduce
process time in their public and their private lives, and
they respond, among other things, by increasingly
compressing, fragmenting, and compartmentalizing
time (Southerton, 2003). For example, Hochschild
(1997) has argued that one of the corollaries of the
time bind for dual-career couples is that individuals
apply a pattern of workplace temporal organization to
their leisure time by fragmenting and re-sequencing it.
In a study that reformulates Bourdieus theory
of taste and applies it in an American context, Holt
(1997, 1998) also acknowledges the important link
among work, leisure and taste, and raises the question
of status. He argues that for individuals with high
cultural capital, the orientation to leisure mirrors their
approach to work. Individuals with high cultural
capital have careers characterized by an emphasis on
symbolic analysis (e.g. the necessity to synthesize and
manipulate information). Since the cultural skills of
symbolic analysis are required for occupational success,
they play a role in class reproduction and put demands
on busy individuals to acquire them through intense
involvement in leisure activities. In fact Linder (1970)
suggested that to be harried (to have too little time
and too much to do) might be a new form of status
distinction because of its growing association with

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

Sample Size and Response Rates


The original sample comprised 1000 households,
containing 2034 adult individuals. Of these, 1093
responded to the interview and completed the diary
and 668 responded to the interview only, a total
response rate of 87 percent. From this data set we
selected a subset of respondents aged between 16 and
65 who were identified in the interview as either the
head of household or the partner of the head of
household. This yielded a sample of 1317 individuals
and couples in households.4

The Variables
The first advantage of this data set for our purposes
was that it contained information on participation
in leisure activities both from questionnaire and
time use diary sources, making possible a choice
and a comparison of measures of voraciousness.
Questionnaire measures of activity participation are
the most familiar means of data-collection on leisure
participation in the area of cultural consumption,
although time use measures are becoming increasingly applied (e.g. Garhammer, 1998; Robinson and
Godbey, 1999). Therefore, it was important to be
able to compare the new diary source with the more
standard measures. It has been shown that diary
estimates of time spent in different activities, where
people record their participation in those activities
with at least some degree of contemporaneity (i.e., in
their diaries), differ from estimates based upon
responses to retrospective questions. It can safely be
assumed that diary estimates are in fact the more
accurate, since they do not involve the same problems

of retrospective recall or respondents estimations


of their usual behaviour (see Juster, 1985; Robinson,
1985; Kalfs, 1993). A further advantage of the particular diary used in the Home OnLine study is that
it was kept for a week rather than for a single day
(the more common data-collection procedure).
It therefore permits analysis of a wider and more
representative range of activities, encompassing those
done less frequently (e.g. once a week instead of
once a day). This is a valuable feature in the analysis
of leisure, particularly leisure activities outside the
home, since many such activities fall into this category.
Moreover, the data set contained a larger selection
of socio-economic and cultural variables, for use as
independent variables, than that available in other data
sets such as the standardized data files of the MTUS.
From these data we were thus able to construct and
compare different measures of voraciousness, based
both on time use diary data and on questionnaire
responses. Two questionnaire measures of voraciousness were derived from a question in the interview
schedule asking respondents how frequently they
engaged in particular leisure activities.5 The specific
out-of-home leisure activities selected for the measure
were: going to the cinema/concerts/the theatre; leisure
group participation; eating/drinking out; watching
sports; and doing sports/keeping fit/walking. Outof-home leisure activities in particular are chosen
because they express active consumer behaviours that
in general take both time and money to engage in, and
consequently provide a link to both socio-economic
and time resources. The measure was constructed
by summing the number of these activities done on
most days, at least once a week, and at least once
a month, yielding a scale of 0 (none)5 (all).
The second measure of voraciousness was based on
time use diary information, and was designed to be
as compatible as possible with the questionnaire
measures. In their diaries, respondents recorded their
participation in different leisure activities throughout
the week in quarter-hour slots. The diary measure
simply counted the number of different out-of-home
leisure activities done in the diary week, giving a range
from 0 (none) to 4 (all). The out-of-home leisure
activities selected for the diary measure were: going
to concerts/the cinema; walking; eating/drinking out;
and doing sports.6 Like the questionnaire measures
it was therefore a measure both of the range (since it
involves counting how many different activities are
participated in) and of the frequency (since in order
to be recorded during a specific week an activity has
to be done on average at least weekly) of participation
in different out-of-home leisure activities.

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

random within these areas, with an overrepresentation


of homes with personal computers. Counterweights
were included in the dataset to reproduce the expected
sample without overrepresentation of households
with computers. Two methods were used to collect
the data. Firstly, interviews were conducted with all
adult members (aged 16 or older) of the household.
In addition, interviewed respondents were provided
with a week-long diary in which they were asked to
record, from a list of activity categories, what they
did every quarter hour on each day of that week.
The activities recorded in the diary were based but
were not identical to) the standardized categories used
in the Multi-national Time Use Study (MTUS),3
a cross-national archive of time use diary studies
held at the University of Essex. Diaries were returned
by post; in return, respondents received a gift voucher.

127

128

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Independent Variables

Results
Comparison of Measures
The first step was to compare the time use and
questionnaire measures of voraciousness. There has
been some criticism in the literature of time use diaries
as data collection instruments in the area of free time
and leisure activities (see the Round Table Discussion
on non-response bias in time use surveys in Leisure &
Society, 1998). One of the most common arguments
is that, while time use diaries may be able to accurately
record activities which are done frequently and regularly, they may be unreliable when it comes to less
frequently performed activities. The use of a weeklong
diary, however, seems to go a long way in meeting
these objections. In Table 1, the first questionnaire
measure of voraciousness is based on the number of
activities reported from the survey as being done at
least once a week. With a weekly diary one would
expect on the average to record all these activities. The
second questionnaire measure also contains those
activities that are reported on the survey question
as being done at least once a month. On average
one-quarter of these activities will appear in a weekly
diary. Assuming that the diary gives an accurate record
of activity participation we would therefore expect
the diary estimate to have a higher distribution and
mean than the first questionnaire measure, but a lower

Table 1 Distributions and means of the time use diary and questionnaire measures of voraciousness
Number of different
out-of-home leisure
activities

Questionnaire
measure 1 N (%)

Weekly diary
measure N (%)

Questionnaire
measure 2 N (%)

None
One
Two
Three
Four
Five
N = 100%
Mean
Correlation coefficient
(Spearmans rho) with
weekly diary measure

135 (19.7)
304 (44.2)
186 (27.1)
60 (8.7)
2 (0.3)
1 (0.1)
688y
1.26

103 (15.0)
232 (33.7)
227 (32.9)
102 (14.8)
24 (3.5)

688y
1.58

57 (8.3)
179 (26.0)
247 (35.8)
150 (21.8)
49 (7.1)
7 (1.0)
688y
1.96

0.363

0.350

The analyses are based on a sample of 1317 respondents. However, the total N in this and all subsequent analyses in the article are a result of
the sample weighting which corrects for both the original over-sampling of households with a personal computer and for patterns of non-response.
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Indicators of human capital (as measured by highest


educational qualification), of economic capital
(as measured by the social status of the occupational
category), and of cultural capital (as measured by the
type of daily newspaper read), constituted our
independent variables. These three indicators represent
the most common determinants of cultural participation as indicated by Bourdieuvian theory and as found
in the empirical literature (Bourdieu, 1984; Katz-Gerro,
2002; Chan and Goldthorpe, 2006). Education is
regarded as representing cognitive competence, economic capital represents financial ability, and cultural
capital represents cultural disposition. While the three
are typically closely related and positively correlated,
each one operates through a different mechanism in
its influence on consumption patterns (Van Eijck
and Bargeman, 2004). We included the type of daily
newspaper read as an independent variable, because as
a measure of cultural capital we expected a correlation
between it and the extent of cultural participation.
Reading is considered part of cultured consumption
and is important for the pursuit of status (Zavisca,
2005), and newspaper reading is a low-cost readily
available form of reading. Since for most people reading a newspaper is a daily activity, and since the choice
of newspaper is quite one-dimensional in terms of
a cultural hierarchy, we considered it a reasonably
unequivocal indicator of cultural capital (Chan and
Goldthorpe, 2006). Sex and a family structure variable
based on a life-cycle set of categories combining

age with number of children were also used as control


variables in the multivariate analyses.

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

Who are the Voracious?


The next stage of the analysis was to see if the
relationship between our new measure of voraciousness and our measures of human, economic, and
cultural capital corresponded to the relationships
known from the literature on omnivorousness. For
example, in different studies the following groups were
found to exhibit omnivorous tastes: younger people
(Peterson and Kern, 1996); younger, highly educated,

service class (Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez, 2002);


highly educated, higher income, younger, white collar
occupation (Warde et al., 1999); highly educated
(Holbrook et al., 2002); highly educated, higher
occupational status, younger (Van Eijck, 2001).
Table 2 shows mean values for our measure of
voraciousness according to our various measures of
capital: educational level, social status of job, and type
of newspaper read.
In keeping with the above findings on omnivorousness, statistically strong relationships proved to exist
between these measures of human, economic, and
cultural capital and the number of different outof-home leisure activities participated in per week.
The means in the tables columns show that for all
three of our independent variables those who occupied
the higher categories participated in a greater average
number of different activities. All the relationships
are positive and monotonic in character (such that
the higher the level of capital the higher the number
of different activities participated in), and they are
all statistically significant.8
Having demonstrated the relationships between
voraciousness and the independent variables reflecting
different aspects of capital, we performed a multiple
analysis of variance on each of these relationships,
which held constant the effects of sex and of family
structure.9 These analyses were necessary to show that
the strong linear associations seen in Table 2 were not
attributable to the effects of these other important
structural variables. The results (not shown here,
available from the authors) demonstrate that all the
independent variables remained highly significant at
the P < 0.005 level or higher.10 The strong positive

Table 2 Number of different out-of-home leisure activities/week by various measures of capital


Social status of job

High management
Low management
Intermediate
Small employer
Low supervisor
Semi-routine
Routine
(Undefined)
Total


Mean (N)

1.79
1.83
1.77
1.56
1.49
1.39
1.35
(1.58)
1.64

(50)
(136)
(80)
(43)
(52)
(56)
(54)
(20)
(491)

Highest qualification

Mean (N)

Newspaper
read

Degree, nursing
A-levely, higher vocational
GCSEy, lower vocational
Failed GCSEy, youth training

1.89
1.63
1.59
1.47

Quality
Medium
Tabloid

(None)
Total

1.30 (167)
1.58 (696)

One-way analysis of variance statistically significant at P < 0.05.


One-way analysis of variance statistically significant at P < 0.001.
y
Key: A-level School matriculation exam.
GCSE General school certificate exam.
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).


(141)
(122)
(241)
(26)

(None)
Total

Mean (N)

2.00 (81)
1.66 (161)
1.38 (176)

(1.55) (278)
1.58 (696)

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

distribution and mean than the second questionnaire


measure. Since both the distribution and mean of
the diary measure sit squarely between those of the
two questionnaire measures, we can conclude that time
use diaries are indeed effective in measuring leisure
participation, even for activities done monthly.
In assessing the two measures we decided that
the time-use diary instrument (recorded over a week of
activities) may be the more accurate in recording the
true range of different weekly activities, since in the
diary activities were recorded simultaneously (or nearly
simultaneously) with their actual performance, while in
the questionnaire respondents were asked to assess the
frequency with which they participated in particular
activities. Comparison of these measures suggested
under-reporting by women to the survey question
about the number of different leisure activities, and
indeed, general under-reporting of activities in surveys
by comparison to time-use diary data has been
commented on before in the methodological literature
on time use diaries: see Dow and Juster 1985).
We therefore decided to present findings from the
diary measure only in the remaining analyses.7

129

130

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

(that people with higher levels of human and cultural


capital have more money) is almost certainly true; the
second (that they have more time) is more doubtful,
considering the increasingly reported association
between high levels of employment income and long
hours of work (e.g. Sullivan and Gershuny, 2004).
However, both propositions could be tested through
these data, to see whether the observed associations
between high levels of human and cultural capital and
voraciousness can be attributed to time or money.
We examined the relationship of our independent
measures with income (measured as usual net monthly
pay for the employed/self-employed) and with time
(measured as usual hours of work per week, for the
employed, and as total leisure time). In keeping with
previous findings (see for example Jacobs and Gerson,
1998; Robinson and Godbey, 1999; Gershuny, 2000;
Sullivan and Gershuny, 2004), these analyses support
the generally positive relationships expected between
higher levels of pay and higher levels of human,
economic, and cultural capital, and the negative
relationship expected between available time for leisure
and higher levels of capital (shown in Appendix 1).
On the whole people with higher levels of human,
economic, and cultural capital had more money but
less total leisure time than others.
But how were these facts about income and time
availability related to our measure of voraciousness?
To answer this question we needed to examine whether
the one-way associations between our measure of
voraciousness and the independent variables representing different aspects of capital remained significant in
a multiple analysis of covariance controlling not just
for sex and family structure (as above) but also for
covariates representing available time and income.
Table 3 shows the results.
The relationship of all three of our independent
variables (social status of job, level of highest qualification, and type of newspaper read) with voraciousness
is seen to have remained highly statistically significant
even when controlling for the effects of time and
income as covariates in the analysis. The only cases
where this was not so were for the models in which
net monthly pay was the covariate, and highest
qualification level and type of newspaper read were
the independent variables. In both these models
the relationship with voraciousness is in the expected
direction (i.e. higher levels of human, economic, and
cultural capital were associated with a higher mean
number of different out-of-home leisure activities
in a clear monotonic sequence), but does not reach
the accepted boundary of statistical significance.
However, in identical analyses that we ran with the

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

association found in the one-way relationships between


the number of different out-of-home leisure activities
and our measures of the level of human and economic
capital (highest qualification level, social status of job)
and of cultural capital (type of newspaper read)
persisted even when each analysis controlled for the
effects of family structure and of sex.
In addition, although the raw means show that men
participated in more different out-of-home leisure
activities than women, the effect of sex in the above
multiple analyses of variance was not statistically
significant. With regard to the family structure variable, adults living alone participate in the greatest
number of different out-of-home leisure activities.
Among those in couples, young couples (aged
under 36) without children participated in the highest
number of different out-of-home leisure activities
(i.e. were the most voracious) and those with children
aged under 12 in the household participated in the
lowest number. These results accord with findings on
leisure participation more generally, and also with the
findings from the omnivorous literature, in which it
is younger people without families who in general
display the widest range of cultural tastes.
The findings set out so far confirmed that we had a
measure of voraciousness that bore a strong similarity
to some of the behaviourally based measures of
omnivorousness used previously in the literature
(e.g. Lopez Sintas and Garcia Alvarez, 2002)
(as opposed to those focusing on expressed preferences
or tastes). However, this measure, we maintain, should
be conceptually distinguished from the meaning of
omnivorousness as it was originally conceived, namely
as a measure of the breadth of cultural tastes of
highbrow consumers. Nevertheless, the association we
found between high levels of status with voraciousness
accords with the same association found in the literature for measures of omnivorousness, including those
based on tastes.
To strengthen this connection, however, we need to
consider more closely what our new measure actually
means. There is a difference, after all, between enjoying
a wide selection of different music genres (the basis for
some measures of omnivorousness) and having time
to spend on several different out-of-home leisure
activities during the week. It could be hypothesized,
for example, that the extent of participation in outof-home leisure activities was primarily a function of
time or of money. The observed association between
high levels of human and cultural capital and
voraciousness may simply have been because such
people tend to have more money to spend on leisure,
or more time for it. The first of these propositions

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

131

Table 3 Multiple analysis of variance models showing effect of various variables on the number of different
out-of-home leisure activities per week

Independent variables

Covariate hours
worked per week
(employees only)

Covariate total
leisure time

P 0.004, beta 0.22

P 0.000, beta 0.20

P 0.000, beta0.20

1.78
1.86
1.62
1.28
P 0.001, beta 0.26
ns
ns
0.11
ns (beta 0.13)

1.81
1.77
1.57
1.30
P 0.002, beta0.20
ns
ns
0.08
P 0.033, beta0.14

1.83
1.74
1.55
1.33
ns
ns
P 0.000
0.12
P 0.000, beta0.20

1.82
1.64
1.55
1.48
P 0.008, beta0.23
ns
ns
0.07
NS (beta 0.12)

1.85
1.64
1.58
1.44
P 0.02, beta0.17
ns
ns
0.06
P 0.000, beta0.20

1.89
1.65
1.59
1.30
ns
ns
P 0.000
0.09
P 0.000, beta0.17

1.82
1.73
1.48
1.60
P 0.004, beta0.24
ns
ns
0.07

2.06
1.70
1.39
1.60
P 0.004, beta0.19
ns
ns
0.08

1.98
1.68
1.38
1.54
P 0.001, beta0.17
ns
P 0.000
0.09

Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).

questionnaire-derived dependent variable measure,


the direction of the predicted means was the same,
but, because of the larger sample numbers responding
to the questionnaire, these relationships were found
to be statistically significant, at P 0.015 and 0.000
respectively.
To summarize, our overall conclusion is that
individuals with high levels of human, economic, and
cultural capital have less time for leisure but still
engage in a greater range of out-of-home leisure
activities. We can assume that what characterizes these
groups in general is shorter periods of leisure, which
are also more diverse in terms of the range of different
out-of-home activities participated in. This conclusion
is supported by a further refinement of the analysis in

which the total amount of time spent on the same four


out-of-home leisure activities as were used to calculate
the dependent variable was entered as a covariate into
the same multiple analysis of variance. In this analysis,
which assessed the number of different out-of-home
leisure activities performed while holding constant the
total amount of time spent in these four activities, the
effects of social status of job, highest qualification level,
and type of newspaper read remained statistically
significant. The implication is that the higher levels of
diversity of participation in out-of-home leisure for
those with higher levels of human capital and cultural
taste are independent of the total amount of time
spent on these activities. A summary of these results
is shown in Table 4.

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Social Status of Job


Predicted means (adjusted):
Management
Intermediate
Small employer/low supervision
Semi/unskilled routine
Family Structure
Sex
Covariate
Model R2
Highest Qualification Level
Predicted means (adjusted):
Degree, nursing qualification
A-level, higher vocational
GCSE, lower vocational
None
Family Structure
Sex
Covariate
Model R2
c. Type of Newspaper Read
Predicted means (adjusted):
Quality
Medium
Tabloid
None
Family Structure
Sex
Covariate
Model R2

Covariate net
monthly income
(employees only)

132

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Table 4 Statistical significance levels for different


independent variables on the number of outof-home activities per week when holding constant
total time spent in those activities
Levels of statistical
significance
from multiple
analyses of variance
Statistically significant
at 0.000 in all
three analyses
0.014
0.001
0.004

Note: Analyses also include Family Structure and Sex as independent


variables.
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).

In addition to stating the theoretical difference


between voraciousness and omnivorousness, and
establishing that our new measure of voraciousness
shares many of the same relationships with aspects
of human, economic, and cultural capital reported
from the literature on omnivorousness, we would, in
principle, also need to show how omnivorousness and
voraciousness are themselves related. Unfortunately,
the Home OnLine data are not ideal for constructing
a measure of omnivorousness, as they contained no
direct information on cultural tastes. Indeed, largescale data including a combination of time-use data
with details about cultural tastes, or highly specific
information about cultural activities, are extremely
rare, if non-existent. However, in keeping with other
measures of omnivorousness found in the literature,
we were able to devise a measure of omnivorousness
according to a ranking of leisure activities as representing highbrow, middlebrow, and lowbrow cultural
tastes. This ranking was based both on the cultural
content of the activity as it is commonly contextualized
in the literature (as high, middle, or low culture) and
on information on rates of participation in particular
activities. The second criteria is increasingly used in
the literature, and is predicated on the tendency
for highbrow leisure activities to be practiced less
frequently in the population as a whole, and for
lowbrow leisure activities to be more popular, thus
avoiding researcher-defined classifications of cultural
content (e.g. Peterson and Simkus, 1992; Lopez Sintas
and Garcia Alvarez, 2005; Lopez Sintas and KatzGerro, 2005). Values on this measure ranged from

Cross-Time Comparisons
Two national-level British data-sets from the MTUS
archive: the 1975 BBC and the 1987 SCELI surveys11
(both of which included a week-long diary element)
permitted us to conduct a cross-time comparison to
see if our measure of voraciousness displayed the same
relationship with measures of human and economic
capital over time. As in any cross-time comparison
there were some questions of data compatibility to be
overcome. First, since the activity categories in the
harmonized MTUS data-sets are slightly different
from those used in the Home OnLine survey, the
voraciousness variable (number of different outof-home leisure activities participated in during the
week) was calculated slightly differently, on the basis
of a more detailed list of such activities. Secondly,
one of the problems of the MTUS archive is known
to be the relative lack of variables reflecting aspects
of human capital and social status, which confined us
to level of education, and a 3-category income variable
(for 1987 only). However, with these limitations in
mind, it was nevertheless possible to construct effectively the same dependent variable, and to test
its relationship with these aspects of human and
economic capital in multiple analyses of variance in
the same way as for the Home OnLine data.
To summarize the findings, we do see from the
earlier data some of the same effects as in the Home
OnLine data regarding the direction of the overall
relationship between voraciousness and measures of
human and economic capital.12 And in an analysis
of covariance equivalent to that shown in Table 3
(i.e. with education level, sex, and family status as
the independent variables, and time in paid work and
time in leisure as covariates in two separate models)
the only difference between the results from the Home
OnLine data and those from the earlier surveys
was the clear inverted J-shape of the relationship in
the earlier surveys between educational level

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Total time spent


in four out-of-home
leisure activities
(covariate)
Social Status of Job
Highest Qualification Level
Type of Newspaper Read

zero to three. We found a moderately low, statistically


significant correlation between this measure of omnivorousness and that of voraciousness (Spearmans
r 0.200, statistically significant at P < 0.01). That
there was a correlation was to be expected, since the
construction of the omnivorousness measure was based
partly on activity participation rates; but the overlap
was not extensive and there proved to be considerable independence between voraciousness and this
measure of omnivorousness. Further, direct comparison between these measures must for the meantime
wait for more suitable data to become available.

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

and voraciousness. Those with only secondary school


education appeared to participate in a greater number
of different activities per week than those with
education above secondary school level. This is in
contrast to the 1998 results, where there was an
unambiguous positive relationship between level of
education and voraciousness. While this may merely be
the outcome of data inconsistencies, it might also
represent a real change over time in the relationship
between voraciousness and human capital, reflecting,
for example, changes in the distribution of income,
leisure time, and leisure preferences across different
groups.

Our revisiting of the omnivore thesis in this article


took three directions. First, we introduced a methodological innovation into the omnivorousness literature
by exploiting time use data to measure actual behaviour. We believe that it is more accurate to measure
respondents behaviour directly rather than to rely on
responses to questionnaire items on usual behaviour
or preferences.
Second, by looking at activity patterns we emphasized the way individuals consume, rather than what
they consume. The aim here was to address the
temporal aspect of cultural omnivorousness, which,
we argued, is manifested both in a wide band of
cultural tastes and in a voracious frequency of
cultural participation. While omnivorousness is usually
regarded as relating to the range of cultural tastes,
voraciousness, as we define it, relates both to the range
and frequency of cultural practices. We found
voraciousness to share many of the relationships
commonly found in the analysis of omnivorousness
(for example, to educational qualifications, to measures
of job social status and to age, as well as to aspects
of cultural taste such as choice of daily newspaper),
and we argued that voraciousness and omnivorousness
represent two separate but related dimensions of
cultural consumption. We showed that the relationship
between voraciousness and measures of human and
economic capital, family structure, time in paid work,
and time in leisure generally persist over time
when we compared data from 1975, 1987, and 1998.
These findings and their interpretation confirmed
the validity of the conceptual stimuli that initially
led us to look for the division of omnivorousness into
different dimensions. We interpret the findings as
depicting a culturally active leisure-style, a tendency to
seek diverse experiences (Holt, 1997; Schulze, 1992),

an insatiable consumer behaviour (Campbell, 1987),


and a plentiful cultural tool kit (Swidler, 1986) among
higher status groups.
Third, we provided a theoretical link between these
dimensions of cultural omnivorousness and configurations of late modern cultural consumption, in which
status is continuously being reproduced. While we
were not able, from this data, to directly compare
voraciousness with omnivorousness as it was originally
measured according to cultural tastes (this would
require the rare combination of time-use data with
details about cultural tastes), it was clear from our
analyses that both omnivorousness and voraciousness
are associated with high status, in terms of higher
levels of education, job status, and cultural capital.
The activity patterns of high status individuals attested
that they participated in the greatest number of
different cultural activities, and we showed that this
could probably not be attributed simply to having
more time or money than others. We postulated a
pattern of high status individuals with an insatiable
appetite for multiple leisure activities notwithstanding
the time squeeze. Since voraciousness seems neither
to be about monetary access to leisure goods and
services nor about the overall availability of leisure
time, we argue that its significant feature is its role as
a status marker, a cultural boundary, and a sign of
social exclusion. Further, by adding time pressure and
pace of life to the discussion of cultural distinction,
we argue that voraciousness, like omnivorousness,
may combine an element of the status accruing to
time pressuredness with a connection to multi-cultural
capital (as identified by Bryson, 1996) through the
knowledge and performance of a range of time and
money-intensive (out-of-home) leisure activities.
We interpret these findings as suggesting a depiction
of status distinction associated with high status
individuals who tend to be harried or busy, to work
more and consume more, and to have a fast life
tempo while also embracing a diverse cultural
repertoire. The delineation of cultural boundaries is
modified here to depict not only an omnivorous
profile, but also a voracious pattern that may
characterize the dynamics of cultural exclusion.
As Veblen (1994 [1899]) pointed out, it is possible
that the lines of demarcation between social classes are
in general becoming more vague and temporary,
and that voraciousness is therefore actually related to
a globalized widening of shared tastes among
high status groups. Nevertheless, our theoretical
expectation would be that social emulation that
trickles down the social structure, resulting in
more blurred cultural boundaries between classes,

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Discussion

133

134

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Notes
1. Feldman and Hornik (1981) coined the term
timestyle to suggest that the choice of daily
activities constitutes the locus of the meaning of
time to individuals.
2. The original data collection was funded by British
Telecommunications plc.
3. For further details of the MTUS see Gershuny
et al., 2000.
4. We also derived a randomly selected sample of one
adult individual per household, and all analyses
were performed for both these samples. The results
for the individual sample were identical to those
for the sample of individual and couple households; given the greater numbers in the latter, we
present here only those results.
5. The response categories were: most days; at
least once a week; at least once a month;

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.
12.
13.

several times a year; once a year or less; never/


almost never.
The diarys leisure categories were not precisely
comparable with the questionnaires leisure
categories.
The unavoidable disadvantage of this decision was
slightly to reduce the effective sample size, since
not all the respondents returned a completed time
use diary.
When job status categories were combined into
a smaller number of categories to enlarge the cell
sizes the relationship became statistically significant at the P<0.005 level.
The measure of family structure was based on
life-cycle stages, including aspects of age and the
presence and age of dependent children in the
household. The categories are: living alone, aged
under 36; living with spouse, aged under 36 with
no dependent children; living with spouse, over
age 36 with no dependent children; living with
spouse, dependent children aged under 12 in the
house; living with spouse, dependent children
aged 12 or more in the house; other.
For the purposes of this analysis the categories of
job status were recoded into a 4-category variable:
management; Intermediate; small employers and
supervisors; semi-skilled and unskilled routine.
Further details about each may be obtained from
the MTUS website <http://iser.essex.ac.uk/mtus>.
Details of these analyses may be obtained from
the authors.
See Han (2003) for a discussion of the link
between social position and cultural disposition
in terms of tastes in music. Han argues that
omnivorousness is not only about a large number
of genres liked; it is more about switching
evaluation-distinction criteria.

References
Bell, D. (1979). The Cultural Contradictions of
Capitalism. London: Heinemann.
Bihagen, E. and Katz-Gerro, T. (2000). Culture
Consumption in Sweden: The Stability of Gender
Differences. Poetics, 27, 327349.
Bittman, M. (2002). Social Participation and Family
Welfare: The Money and Time Costs in Leisure in
Australia. Social Policy and Administration, 36,
408425.
Bittman, M. and Wajcman, J. (2000). The Rush Hour:
The Character of Leisure Time and Gender Equity.
Social Forces, 79(1), 165189.

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

always antecedes specialization and innovation adopted


by the upper classes, thereby reinforcing cultural
boundaries (see McCracken, 1990).
The conclusions presented above should be qualified
and further explored in future research on several
grounds. First, cross sectional data does not allow for
causal ordering, so the direction of the relationship
between human, economic, and cultural capital and
consumption cannot be assumed from these data
alone. Second, it is possible that there are anteceding
factors, not included in these analyses, which influence
both levels of achieved capital and types and levels of
cultural consumption. Third, future research could
develop more sensitive measures of active leisure-styles
and link them more systematically to measures both
of insatiability in the sphere of consumption and to
the acceleration of life tempo both over time and
across different countries.
Our analysis fleshes out a dimension of cultural
consumption that is not necessarily about participating
seriously in large numbers of activities for long periods
of time but about cultural tasting and switching
among, or differently combining, those activities.
Voraciousness may therefore not be about commitment to many activities but about commitment to not
leaving many activities untouched or unpractised.13
The voracious activity pattern may perhaps be one way
to reconcile the contradiction between the increasingly
overworked contention and the increase in leisure
time contention, if we maintain that discretionary
time can be devoted to work and to leisure alike by
means of voracious cultural participation.

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

Hochschild, A. (1997). The Time Bind. New York:


Metropolitan Books.
Holbrook, M. B., Weiss, M. J. and Habich, J. (2002).
Disentangling
Effacement,
Omnivore,
and
Distinction Effects on the Consumption of
Cultural Activities: An Illustration. Marketing
Letters, 13(4), 345.
Holt, D. (1997). Distinction in America? Recovering
Bourdieu. Poetics, 25(23), 93120.
Holt, D. (1998). Does Cultural Capital Structure
American Consumption? Journal of Consumer
Research, 25(1), 125.
Feldman, L. P. and Hornik, J. (1981). The Use of
Time: An Integrated Conceptual Model. Journal of
Consumer Research, 7, 407419.
Jacobs, J. A. and Gerson, K. (1998). Who Are the
Overworked Americans?. Review of Social Economy,
1(4), 442459.
Jacobs, J. A. and Gerson, K. (2004). The Time Divide:
Work, Family, and Gender Inequality. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Juster, F. T. (1985). The Validity and Quality of Time
Use Estimates Obtained from Recall Diaries. In
Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (Eds), Time, Goods,
and Well-Being. Michigan: Survey Research Centre:
University of MichiganChapter 4.
Kalfs, N. (1993). Hour by Hour: Effects of the Data
Collection Mode in Time Use Research. Amsterdam:
Nederlands Instituut voor Maatschappij en MarktOnderzoek.
Katz-Gerro,
T.
(2002).
Highbrow
Cultural
Consumption and Class Distinction in Italy,
Israel, West Germany, Sweden, and the United
States. Social Forces, 81(1), 207229.
Katz-Gerro, T. (2004). Cultural Consumption
Research: Review of Methodology, Theory, and
Consequence. International Review of Sociology,
14(1), 1129.
Katz-Gerro, T. (2006). Comparative Evidence of
Inequality in Cultural Preferences: Gender, Class,
and Family Status. Sociological Spectrum, 26, 6383.
Katz-Gerro, T. and Sullivan, O. (2004). Leisure, Tastes
and Gender in Britain: Changes from the 1960s
to the 1990s. Sociologie et Societes, XXXVI(1),
165186.
Levine, L. W. (1998). Highbrow/Lowbrow: The
Emergence of Cultural Hierarchy in America.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Linder, S. (1970). The Harried Leisure Class. New York:
Columbia University Press.
Lopez Sintas, J. and Garcia Alvarez, E. (2002).
Omnivores Show up Again: The Segmentation of

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A Social Critique on


the Judgment of Taste. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Bryson, B. (1996). Anything but Heavy Metal:
Symbolic Exclusion and Musical Dislikes.
American Sociological Review, 61, 884899.
Bryson, B. (1997). What about the Universe? Musical
Dislikes and Group-Based Identity Construction
among Americans with Low Levels of Education.
Poetics, 25(23), 141156.
Campbell, C. (1987). The Romantic Ethic and the Spirit
of Modern Consumerism. Oxford: Blackwell.
Chan, T. W. and Goldthorpe, J. (2006). Social Status
and Newspaper Readership. American Journal of
Sociology, Forthcoming.
Cotte, J., Ratneshwar, S. and Mick, D.G. (2004).
The Times of Their Lives: Phenomenological
and Metaphorical Characteristics of Consumer
Timestyles. Journal of Consumer Research, 31,
333345.
Dow, G. K. and Juster, F. T. (1985). Goods, Time, and
Well-being: The Joint Dependence Problem. In
Juster, F. T and Stafford, P. (Eds), Time, Goods,
and Well-Being. Michigan: University of Michigan
Survey Research Centre, Chapter 16.
Emmison, M. (2003). Social Class and Cultural
Mobility: Reconfiguring the Cultural Omnivore
Thesis. Journal of Sociology, 39(3), 211230.
Erickson, B. H. (1996). Culture, Class, and Connections.
American Journal of Sociology, 102, 217251.
Gans, H. J. (1974). Popular Culture and High Culture:
An Analysis and Evaluation of Taste. New York:
Basic Books.
Garhammer, M. (1998). Time Pressure in Modern
Germany. Loisir et Societe, 21(2), 327352.
Gershuny, J. (2000). Changing Times: Work and Leisure
in Postindustrial Society. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Gershuny, J. (2005). Busyness as the Badge of Honour
for the New Superordinate Working Class. Social
Research, 72(2), 287314.
Gershuny, J., Fisher, K., Gauthier, A., Jones, S. and
Baert, P. (2000). A Longitudinal, Multinational
Collection of TimeUse Datathe MTUS. In
Gershuny, J. (Ed.), Changing Times: Work and
Leisure in Postindustrial Society. Oxford: Oxford
University Press. Appendix 2.
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and Self Identity: Self
and Society in the Late Modern Age. Cambridge:
Polity Press.
Han, S.-K. (2003). Unraveling the Brow: What and
How of Choice in Musical Preference. Sociological
Perspectives, 46(4), 435459.

135

136

SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Shaw, J. (1998). Feeling a List Coming On: Gender


and the Pace of Life. Time and Society, 7(2),
383396.
Simmel, G. (1950). The Metropolis and Mental Life.
In Wolff, K. (Ed.), The Sociology of George Simmel.
Glencoe, Illinois: Free Press, pp. 409424.
Simmel, G. (1957). Fashion. American Journal of
Sociology, 62(6), 541558.
Slater, D. (1997). Consumer Culture and Modernity.
Cambridge: Polity Press.
Southerton, D. (2003). Squeezing Time: Allocating
Practices, Coordinating Networks and Scheduling
Society. Time and Society, 12(1), 525.
Sullivan, O. and Gershuny, J. (2001). Cross-National
Changes in Time Use: Some Sociological (Hi)
Stories Re-Examined. British Journal of Sociology,
52(4), 331347.
Sullivan, O. and Gershuny, J. (2004). Inconspicuous
Consumption: Work-Rich, Time-Poor in the
Liberal Market Economy. Journal of Consumer
Culture, 4(1), 79100.
Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in Action: Symbols
and Strategies. American Sociological Review, 51,
273286.
Swidler, A. (2001). Talk of Love: How Culture Matters,
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Toffler, A. (1970). Future Shock. London: The Bodley.
Van Eijck, K. (2000). Richard A. Peterson and the
Culture of Consumption. Poetics, 28, 207224.
Van Eijck, K. (2001). Social Differentiation in
Musical Taste Patterns. Social Forces, 79,
11631184.
Van Eijck, K. and Bargeman, B. (2004). The Changing
Impact of Social Background on Lifestyle:
Culturalization instead of Individualization?
Poetics, 32(6), 439461.
Van Rees, K., Vermunt, J. and Verboord, M. (1999).
Cultural Classifications under Discussion: Latent
Class Analysis of Highbrow and Lowbrow Reading.
Poetics, 26, 349365.
Veblen, T. (1994[1899]). The Theory of the Leisure
Class. New York: Penguin.
Warde, A., Martens, L. and Olsen, W. (1999).
Consumption and the Problem of Variety:
Culture Omnivorousness, Social Distinction and
Dining Out. Sociology, 33, 105127.
Weber, M. (1974). The Theory of Social and Economic
Organization. In Parsons, T. and Henderson, A. M.
(Eds and trans). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Zavisca, J. (2005). The Status of Cultural Omnivorism:
A Case Study of Reading in Russia. Social Forces,
84(2), 12331255.

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Cultural Consumers in Spanish Social Space.


European Sociological Review, 18(3), 353368.
Lopez Sintas, J. and Garcia Alvarez, E. (2005). Four
Characters on the Stage Playing Three Games:
Performing Arts Consumption in Spain. Journal of
Business Research, 58(10), 14461455.
Lopez Sintas, J. and Katz-Gerro, T. (2005). From
Exclusive to Inclusive Elitists and Further: Twinty
Years of Omnivorousness and Cultural Diversity
in Arts Participation in the USA. Poetics, 33,
299319.
McCracken, G. (1990). Culture and Consumption.
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Mattingly, M. and Bianchi, S. (2003). Gender
Differences in the Quantity and Quality of Free
Time: The US Experience. Social Forces, 81(3),
9991030.
Peterson, R. A. (1992). Understanding Audience
Segmentation: From Elite and Mass to Omnivore
and Univore. Poetics, 21, 243258.
Peterson, R. A. (1997). The Rise and Fall of
Highbrow Snobbery as a Status Marker. Poetics,
25, 7592.
Peterson, R. A. (2005). Problems in Comparative
Research: The Example of Omnivorousness.
Poetics, 33, 257282.
Peterson, R. A. and Simkus, A. (1992). How Musical
Tastes Mark Occupational Status Groups. In
Lamont, M. and Fournier, M. (Eds), Cultivating
Differences: Symbolic Boundaries and the Making of
Inequality. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago
Press, pp. 152186.
Peterson, R. A. and Kern, R.M. (1996). Changing
Highbrow Taste: From Snob to Omnivore.
American Sociological Review, 61, 900907.
Robinson, J. P. (1985). The Validity and Reliability of
Diaries Versus Alternative Time Use Measures. In
Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (Eds), Time, Goods,
and Well-Being. Michigan: Survey Research Centre:
University of Michigan.
Robinson, J. P. and Godbey, G. (1999). Time for Life: the
Surprising Ways Americans Use Their Time.
Philadelphia: Penn State University Press (2nd edition).
Rosa, H. (2003). Social Acceleration: Ethnical and
Political Consequences of a Desynchronized HighSpeed Society. Constellations, 10(1), 333.
Round Table Discussion. (1998). Non-Response Bias in
Time Use Surveys. Leisure and Society, 21(2),
548582.
Schor, J. (1992). The Overworked American. New York:
Basic Books.
Schulze,
G.
(1992).
Die
Erlebnisgesellschaft:
Kultursoziologie der Gegenwart. Frankfurt: Campus.

THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS

Authors Addresses
Oriel Sullivan (to whom correspondence should be
addressed), Department of Behavioural Sciences,
Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva 84105, Israel.
Email: sullivan@bgu.ac.il

137

Tally Katz-Gerro, Department of Sociology and


Anthropology, University of Haifa, Haifa 31905,
Israel. Email: tkatz@soc.haifa.ac.il

Manuscript received: January 2006

Appendix 1 Net pay, hours of work and leisure time by measures of human, economic, and cultural capital
Usual work
hours/week (N)

Total leisure
time/week (N)

1,314 (81)
1,241 (60)
842 (104)
842 (57)
302yy
0.000

40.9 (114)
39.9 (93)
36.4 (175)
34.9 (91)
472y
0.014

33.2 (139)
33.7 (119)
35.0 (239)
41.2 (190)
688
0.000

1,327 (115)
817 (54)
1,189 (50)
666 (72)
291
0.000

41.7 (181)
32.4 (79)
43.1 (93)
30.9 (102)
454
0.000

31.5 (184)
33.7 (79)
33.0 (95)
34.2 (109)
467
0.312

1,591 (41)
841 (65)
900 (83)
1,079 (113)
302
0.000

44.4 (61)
36.1 (103)
36.7 (114)
37.4 (193)
472
0.005

38.7 (79)
38.7 (158)
36.6 (174)
33.6 (275)
687
0.002

Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).


y
Employees only.
yy
Employees only (lower N due to non-response on income questions).

Downloaded from http://esr.oxfordjournals.org/ at Oakland University on September 29, 2014

Educational level:
Degree, nursing qualification
A-level, higher vocational
GCSE, lower vocational
None
N
Statistical significance from one-way
analysis of variance
Social status of job:
Management
Intermediate
Self-employed, low supervisors
Semi-skilled, unskilled routine
N
Statistical significance from one-way
analysis of variance
Type of newspaper read:
Quality
Medium
Tabloid
None
N
Statistical significance from one-way
analysis of variance

Net usual monthly


pay (pounds) (N)

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi