Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 12

The Contribution of Emotional Intelligence to the Social and Academic Success of

Gifted Adolescents as Measured by the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale Adolescent Version

Section:
Social and Emotional Development
Emotional intelligence has been popularly portrayed as critical to human success and sometimes even more
important than IQ (e.g., D. Goleman, 1995), yet sparse evidence exists supporting such a claim. The purpose of this
study was to measure emotional intelligence, as conceptualized by J. D. Mayer and P. Salovey (1997), in order to
better understand its contribution to the success of gifted adolescents. An adolescent version of the unpublished
Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS-A) was used to measure emotional intelligence, while measures of
interpersonal relations, social stress, and grade point average were utilized to gauge success. Thirty-nine gifted
adolescents (mean age = 16.5) participated. The results of hierarchical multiple regression analyses revealed that
emotional intelligence did not significantly contribute to the social and academic success of these adolescents.
Recommendations for future research in this area are provided, including suggestions for designing studies that could
be more practical for educators and other helping professionals.
The concept of emotional intelligence became popularized by the publication of Daniel Goleman's (1995) best-selling
book on this construct and many subsequent magazine and newspaper articles (e.g., Henig, 1996; Peterson, 1997).
These publications captured the imagination of the media and the general population by claiming extraordinary
predictive validity for emotional intelligence. Goleman argued that general intelligence (IQ) typically predicts only
about 20% (r = .45) of the variance that determines various domains of life success, leaving about 80% to other
factors. Moreover, he professed his belief that many of these "other" factors are related to a construct that has been
termed emotional intelligence. Goleman also detailed his belief that emotional intelligence can sometimes be more
powerful than IQ - that it can contribute greatly to several important life outcomes including improved learning, less
aggression, better decision making, and many other characteristics that imply successful living. In fact, he claimed
that increasing emotional intelligence leads to "advantage in any domain in life" (p. 36).
Although Goleman's (1995) work has brought a great deal of attention to the construct of emotional intelligence, the
grandiosity of his claims has been questioned by the originators of the term "emotional intelligence" (Mayer & Salovey.
1997; Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2000). These researchers have addressed several potential problems.
First, Mayer et al. (2000) highlighted how Goleman's (1995) model includes several personality and motivational
characteristics (e.g., empathy, persistence) in addition to intelligence. They argued that referring to these multiple
variables as one entity - emotional intelligence - is both impractical and misleading. Second, they argued that
Goleman's arguments about how much variance emotional intelligence cart predict are misleading. Goleman
maintained that emotional intelligence can account for more variance than general intelligence (IQ), when studying
factors such as academic, occupational, and social success. If this were true, emotional intelligence would predict
these outcomes at about r = .45 or better, a finding that Mayer et al. suggested would be extraordinary.
Despite their concerns with Goleman's (1995) concept of emotional intelligence, Mayer et al. (2000) reaffirmed that
there is reason to be excited about emotional intelligence. However, they emphasized the need to view emotional
intelligence as a mental ability, not as a conglomeration of various cognitive, motivational, and personality
characteristics. Additionally, they have offered a more conservative estimate of the predictive ability of emotional
intelligence, suggesting that satisfactory new variables should add between 1% and 5% additional variance when
predicting outcomes related to success. Findings like this could be significant and meaningful, even though far less
extraordinary than Goleman's predictions for emotional intelligence.
Modern Models of Emotional Intelligence Defined
Several models of emotional intelligence have emerged in recent years. As indicated previously, in one model
Goleman (1995) includes a blend of several characteristics. Included are: (a) knowing one's emotions, (b) managing
emotions, (c) motivating oneself, (d) recognizing emotions in others, and (e) handling relationships. Goleman

suggested that a wide array of specific qualities such as impulse control, persistence, empathy, good moods, hope,
and optimism are subsumed within these broader components and are characteristic of emotionally intelligent
individuals. As a whole. Goleman conceived emotional intelligence to be "a master aptitude, a capacity that
profoundly affects all other abilities, either facilitating or interfering with them" (p. 80). This statement reflects his belief
that emotional intelligence is extremely powerful in how well people perform in life.
Similar to Goleman (1995), Bar-On (1997) includes a wide range of social and personality characteristics in his model
of emotional intelligence (e.g., intrapersonal skills, interpersonal skills, adaptability, stress-management, general
mood). Also like Goleman, Bar-On agreed that emotional intelligence has predictive ability, specifically suggesting that
it can help optimize academic potential and life success. In this second model of emotional intelligence, Bar-On went
one step beyond Goleman and developed instruments to measure these components, the BarOn EQ-i (Bar-On, 1997)
and the BarOn EQ-i-Youth Version (Bar-On & Parker, 2000).
In 1990, Salovey and Mayer published their first conceptualizations of emotional intelligence, which have since
evolved into a third model of emotional intelligence (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). Within this model attempts have been
made to avoid nonability factors (e.g., persistence, warmth) completely. Although these characteristics are clearly
important components of the human experience, Mayer and Salovey believe they are separate from emotional
intelligence. They suggest that nonability qualities be studied independently from emotional intelligence. Their recent
conceptualization of emotional intelligence is as follows:
Emotional intelligence involves the ability to perceive accurately, appraise, and express emotion; the ability to access
and/or generate feelings when they facilitate thought; the ability to understand emotion and emotional knowledge; and
the ability to regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth. (Mayer & Salovey, 1997, p. 10)
Within this definition, the included abilities have been arranged into four "branches," with the lower branches
consisting of psychological processes that are more basic in nature (e.g., the perception, appraisal, and expression of
emotion), and higher branches involving more complex abilities such as the understanding and reflective regulation of
emotions. Like Bar-On (1997), Mayer and Salovey developed scales reflecting their model of emotional intelligence the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 1997) and an adolescent version, the
MEIS-A (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 1996). Both were unpublished at the time of data collection for this study. The
instrumentation section of this article contains additional details on the Mayer and Salovey model and the branches it
uses.
Emotional Intelligence and Adolescent Success
Goleman (1995) and Bar-On (1997) have theorized that emotional intelligence is highly related to a variety of social,
behavioral, and academic benefits. In actuality, however, only a limited amount of empirical evidence exists indicating
that emotional intelligence contributes to any form of successful living.
The results of three recent studies have provided some indication of the predictive nature of emotional intelligence in
adolescents. Utilizing a group of 52 junior high school students from an urban setting, Rubin (1999) found that high
MEIS-A emotional intelligence scores were inversely related to aggression. Likewise, Trinidad and Johnson (2002)
found that higher emotional intelligence scores (also utilizing the MEIS-A) were related to lower admissions of
smoking and alcohol use in a group of high school students. In a third study that utilized the MEIS-A, with a small pilot
study sample of gifted adolescents (N = 11), Mayer (2001) suggested that emotional intelligence was seemingly
related to the ability to organize emotions that can occur in peer relationships. Whether emotional intelligence
predicted success per se in each of these studies is debatable. There is growing evidence, however, that emotional
intelligence is related to positive behavioral and social outcomes.
In one additional study completed during the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV)
validation process (Wechsler, 2003), the BarOn EQ-i - Youth Version (BarOn & Parker, 2000) was utilized along with
the WISC-IV and the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test - Second Edition (WIAT-II) in a clear attempt to predict a
more traditional form of success - academic achievement. Each instrument was administered to a group of 141
students from a variety of backgrounds, all between the ages of 11 and 17. When controlling for the WISC-IV Full
Scale IQ (FSIQ), a multiple regression analysis revealed that the BarOn Total EQ did not add significantly to
knowledge of WIAT-II total academic achievement. Certain BarOn subscales (Intrapersonal, Stress Management,
General Mood, Positive Impression) did appear to predict achievement beyond the WISC-IV-FSIQ. The authors
concluded that additional research would be needed to sort out this matter.

Emotional Characteristics of the Gifted


Although little is known specifically about the emotional intelligence of gifted and talented adolescents, much is known
about characteristics similar to emotional intelligence in gifted and talented adolescents. For example, gifted youth are
often found to have increased sensitivity and exceptional empathy (Davis & Rimm, 1998; Piechowski, 1997), and
Roeper (1982) stated that emotionally gifted people often have "the capacity to integrate emotions, intellect, and
creativity against enormous odds" (p. 24). These qualities sound strikingly similar to aspects of the Mayer and
Salovey (1997) model of emotional intelligence. It appears likely that many gifted young people are able to utilize this
integration of emotional and cognitive abilities in a manner that enables more successful interaction with the
environment.
In contrast, gifted and talented students can also be extraordinarily challenged by their emotional states. For example,
guilt, depression, feelings of inadequacy, and fearfulness (Davis & Rimm, 1998), as well as difficulty adjusting to new
environments and loneliness (Falk, Piechowski, & Lind, 1994) have all been identified as being common among
adolescents with high general cognitive ability (high IQ scores). Additionally, Lum (1988) found that despite adequate
social skills, many gifted children were at-risk for interpersonal problems; and Osborn (1996) suggested that
exceptionally gifted children are often at-risk for social isolation. Given findings like these, it is clear that not all gifted
adolescents have the same capacity to process their highly variable emotional experiences in an efficient and
effective manner.
Purpose
Despite superior general cognitive ability as measured by standardized tests, the population of gifted and talented
students can experience as much variation in their successes and failures as other adolescents (Hallahan &
Kaufmann, 1994; Rimm, 1996). Certainly personality and environmental factors play a part in this variability, but it is
hypothesized that knowledge of cognitive abilities other than general intelligence (IQ) may also help predict
successful outcomes for gifted and talented individuals. Given the mixed results within available studies of emotional
intelligence in adolescents, it is clear that additional studies are needed to better understand the predictive ability of
this construct. Larger studies that include gifted adolescent samples are particularly in need.
The purpose of this study was to measure emotional intelligence, as conceptualized by Mayer and Salovey (1997),
with an attempt to better understand how it can contribute to the success of gifted and talented adolescents.
Measures of interpersonal relations, social stress, and grade point average were utilized to gauge success. The
following hypotheses were formulated:
Hypothesis #1: In gifted and talented adolescents, knowledge of emotional intelligence will add significantly to the
explanation of social success (interpersonal relations and social stress) once general intelligence (IQ) has been
controlled statistically.
Hypothesis #2: In gifted and talented adolescents, knowledge of emotional intelligence will add significantly to the
explanation of academic achievement (grade point average) once general intelligence (IQ) has been controlled
statistically. Controlling for IQ was done to further address current questions about the predictive nature of emotional
intelligence above and beyond IQ.
Method
Participants
The total subject pool (N = 39) was comprised of a sample of adolescents enrolled in a Midwestern residential high
school designed for 11th and 12th grade gifted adolescents (composite IQ standard score M = 129). Although all
participants were considered students with high academic ability, the state test scores and grades of the students
varied significantly (GPA range for semester of study = 2.0 to 4.0; M = 3.27). According to school administrators, the
purpose of this school is to provide a physical, intellectual, and social environment in which students with exceptional
ability can thrive in an appropriately exceptional learning, community. Participants ranged in age from 15 to 18, with a
mean of 16 years 6 months. Fifty-nine percent of the participants were female and 41% were male. Eighty-seven
percent of the participants described themselves as White, 5% as African American, 3% as Asian American, 3% as
Hispanic American, and 3% as Other. Participants were also questioned about extracurricular strengths and activities.
Seventy-four percent rated themselves as being "very talented" in at least one of the following areas: play a musical
instrument, sing, visual arts, athletics, dance or ballet, theatre, or debate.
Instrumentation

Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale - Adolescent Version (MEIS-A).


The Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale - Adolescent Version (MEIS-A) was used to measure emotional
intelligence. This unpublished instrument was developed by Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (1996) and further described
by Caruso, Van Buren, Mayer, and Salovey (1998). The complete MEIS-A consists of eight separate tasks that are
similar to the adult version, the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 1997). Both
measures consist of subtests that correspond with the four branches of emotional intelligence as defined by Mayer
and Salovey (1997), and are forerunners to the recently published Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test
(Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002). For the current study, a short-form version of the MEIS-A was used. The four
subtests in this version (one from each branch of the Mayer and Salovey definition) yield a reliable measure of
adolescent emotional intelligence (Alpha reliability = .91) as indicated by the authors (D. R. Caruso, personal
communication, June 16, 1999). The MEIS-A short-form tasks and their descriptions are presented below under each
appropriate branch of the Mayer and Salovey model.
Branch one - Perceiving emotions. Branch one tasks measure one's ability to perceive and identify emotional content
in other people and other objects (faces, graphic designs, written stories). Mayer and Salovey (1997) argued that this
is an essential component of a test of emotional intelligence as "the developing person begins to evaluate emotion
wherever it might be expressed" (p. 12). The Faces task was utilized to measure this branch of adolescent emotional
intelligence. The participants were asked to look at eight color photographs of human faces and then indicate the
emotion in the face by rating their perception of various feelings (anger, sadness, happiness, disgust, fear, surprise).
Branch two - Assimilating emotions. Branch two tasks assess the participants' ability to bring emotion and cognitive
processes together. The Synesthesia task from the MEIS-A was utilized to measure this branch. The written
Synesthesia items required participants to think about six different future events that could make them feel a certain
way (e.g., happy, jealous). After imagining this event, and while feeling how it night feel, subjects were asked to
endorse 10 different scales ranging from one to five. (e.g., 1 = warm to 5 = cold, 1 = sharp to 5 = dull). In other words,
participants were required to process emotional and cognitive/perceptual material simultaneously, a concept that is
key to the Mayer and Salovey model of emotional intelligence.
Branch three - Understanding emotions. Within this branch, close analysis of emotions is required, much more so
than within the other branches. The Perspectives task was used to measure this ability. Each item includes a brief
written scenario involving a potentially emotional event. After reading the scenario, participants were asked to rate the
probability of several different feelings occurring within two main characters in each scenario.
Branch four - Managing emotions. Branch four involves the ability to manage emotions in oneself and others.
Although many people may be able to perceive, assimilate, and even fully understand emotions, proper regulation of
emotional experiences may prove more difficult. The Managing Emotions task was utilized to measure this skill. This
task includes six items that require participants to read a short paragraph about a situation in which they or others
might be involved (e.g., receiving a bad grade after studying). Participants were asked to rate how effective a given
response to the situation might be.
MEIS-A scoring and technical information. Although various scoring methods have been developed for the MEIS-A,
the consensus scoring method was used in the current study. The authors of the MEIS-A have concluded that the
consensus scoring option is the most reliable (Caruso, Van Buren, Mayer, & Salovey, 1998). Consensus scoring
involves determining the percentage of participants who endorsed each response, with the participants from each
individual study in question being used. For example, on any given test item, if 80% of the participants chose the third
of five possible responses, then any participant in the sample who chose that third response would receive a raw
score of .80 for that item. A total raw consensus score is calculated by totaling the raw scores from all lest items. Zscores can then be calculated for a standard score representation of emotional intelligence For a more detailed review
of all scoring methods, refer to Caruso et al.
Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC)
The Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC) (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) is a measure of emotional
difficulties, behavioral difficulties, and adaptive skills. The adolescent version-self-report (BASC-SRP-A) was utilized
for the current study. Of the many scales included in the BASC-SRP-A, only the Social Stress and Interpersonal
Relations scales were utilized for the current study. The Interpersonal Relations scale assesses participants' "success
at relating to others and the degree of enjoyment derived from this interaction" (Reynolds & Kamphaus, p. 61). The

Social Stress scale measures participants' "feelings of stress and tension in personal relationships; a feeling of being
excluded from social activities" (Reynolds & Kamphaus, p. 58).
Test of Cognitive Skills/Second Edition (TCS/2)
The Test of Cognitive Skills/Second Edition (TCS/2) (1993) is a measure of general intelligence. It is a groupadministered test that is part of the statewide tests of educational progress administered within schools in several
states. Each of the participants in this study completed the TCS/2 in their 10th grade year of high school.
The TCS/2 is a described as a reliable measure of "verbal, nonverbal, and memory abilities that can contribute greatly
to students' success in an educational program" (Test of Cognitive Skills - Second Edition, 1993, p. 1). It utilizes a
standardization sample of 7,300 students from 42 schools across the United States. The TCS/2 includes 80 test items
and four subtests: Sequences, Analogies, Memory, and Verbal Reasoning. The entire TCS/2 takes 45 minutes to
complete and yields an overall score called the Cognitive Skills Index (CSI). The CSI is a normalized standard score
(M = 100, SD = 16) that "has the same statistical properties as the traditional Intelligence Quotient (IQ)" (p. 28).
Procedures
Arrangements were made for students to choose from one of several group-administered test sessions to complete a
demographic sheet, the MEIS-A, and the BASC-SRP-A. All testing was completed within the school building of the
student participants. The participants were able to choose from several late afternoon test times in order to avoid
interference with classes or other extracurricular activities. All students involved were cooperative and completed all
testing within 60 minutes. The CSI scores from the Test of Cognitive Skills and student grades were collected
separately on different days from student academic files. Grade point average was calculated from grades received at
the end of the semester during which the current study occurred.
Data Analysis
Because "success" was measured in three different ways for this study, three separate multiple regression analyses
were conducted. Hierarchical regression analyses were used to determine the influence of emotional intelligence
above and beyond general intelligence (IQ). Each multiple regression procedure included the MEIS-A overall z-score
(emotional intelligence) and the TCS/2 Cognitive Skills Index (a standard score measure of general intelligence) as
predictor variables. Each procedure then utilized one of the three criterion variables of interest, each of which
reflected adolescent success in the social or academic realm: social stress (SS) as measured by the BASC (T-Score),
interpersonal relations (IR) as measured by the BASC (T-Score), or student grade point average (GPA, based on a
four-point scale). For statistical comparisons, an alpha level of .05 was used.
Results
Descriptive statistics for each variable are outlined in Table 1. The CSI mean standard score of 128.7 (96th percentile)
was consistent with what is commonly used as a rough measure of giftedness. This was expected and reflects the
high level of general cognitive ability among the participants in this study. The range of Grade Point Averages (2.0 to
4.0) was greater than might be expected and the mean (3.27) was lower than expected for a gifted population.
However, this variability is understandable given the high level of competition and the caliber of the instruction and
curriculum at this residential school for gifted students. The MEIS-A z-scores ranged from nearly one standard
deviation below the mean (-.87) to nearly one standard deviation above the mean (.88), suggesting that all of the
participants in the current study were within or near the average range of emotional intelligence as measured by the
MEIS-A.
A significant negative correlation (p <.01) was obtained between the BASC Social Stress scale and the BASC
Interpersonal Relations scale (see Table 2). This moderate correlation was expected, as it would be in any sample. As
social stress decreases, the quality of interpersonal relations increases, and vice versa. Additionally, a significant
positive correlation (p < .05) was observed between the CSI and GPA. This was also expected given that IQ and
academic achievement are almost always found to be highly correlated (Sattler, 1992).
Unexpectedly, a significant correlation was not observed between the CSI and the MEIS-A. Mayer and Caruso (1999)
suggested that moderate correlations are expected between all types of intelligences. In this case, there was virtually
no correlation between emotional intelligence, as measured by the MEIS-A, and general intelligence (IQ), as
measured by the CSI (see Table 2). Moreover, there was virtually no correlation between the MEIS-A and any of the
other variables.

In order to better understand how emotional intelligence contributes to interpersonal relations once IQ has been
controlled for, a multiple regression analysis was conducted. Table 3 includes data related to this first multiple
regression. The results revealed a multiple R of .020, indicating that about 2% of the variation in interpersonal
relations is accounted for by a linear composite of the CSI and MEIS-A predictor variables. This multiple R was not
found to be statistically significant. F( 2, 36) = .370. p = .693. Low correlations between the CSI, MEIS-A, and the
Interpersonal Relations scale were responsible for these insignificant multiple regression results. It was hypothesized
that knowledge of the MEIS-A would contribute significantly to predicting interpersonal relations once the CSI was
accounted for statistically. However, the squared semipartial correlation was .0086, indicating that 0% of interpersonal
relations was predicted from the MEIS-A once the CSI was removed from the equation.
A second multiple regression analysis was carried out using the BASC Social Stress Scale. It was hypothesized that
this would augment the study of social success within this sample, adding additional information to what was learned
when using the BASC Interpersonal Relations scale in the first multiple regression. Table 4 includes data related to
this second multiple regression. The results revealed a multiple R of .023, indicating that about 2% of the variation in
social stress is accounted for by a linear composite of the CSI and MEIS-A predictor variables. This multiple R was
not statistically significant, F( 2, 36) = .425, p = .657. Similar to the first multiple regression, low correlations between
the CSI, MEIS -A, and the Social Stress scale were responsible for these insignificant results. The squared
semipartial correlation was .0001, indicating that 0% of social stress was predicted from the MEIS-A once the CSI
was removed from the equation.
A third multiple regression analysis was used to better understand the influence of emotional intelligence on academic
achievement, with student grade point average being utilized as the criterion variable. Table 5 includes data related to
this multiple regression. The results revealed a multiple R of . 141, indicating that about 14% of the variation in GPA
is accounted for by a linear composite of the CSI and MEIS-A variables. This multiple R was not statistically
significant (F(2. 36) = 2.955. p = .065). The CSI alone shared 14% of the variance with GPA, which was statistically
significant (F( 1, 37) - 5.914, p = .020). After removing the CSI from the equation, however, the MEIS-A was found to
contribute insignificantly to the prediction of GPA. Indeed, with a squared semipartial correlation of .0037, the results
indicate that 0% of GPA is predicted from the MEIS-A after the CSI is statistically removed.
Discussion
The results suggest that the social and academic success of the gifted adolescent participants in this study were
essentially independent of the overall emotional intelligence level of these students. In light of these findings, the view
that emotional intelligence is a critical component of successful outcomes could not be supported within this study.
Goleman (1995) claimed that emotional intelligence is at least as important as IQ in predicting various forms of
success and in some cases more important. Even the conservative estimate that new ability variables should predict
between 1% and 5% additional variance beyond general intelligence (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2000) was not
observed in the current results. Consequently, the predictive power of emotional intelligence remains in question, at
least when studying gifted adolescents in a residential setting.
Several factors likely influenced the results. First, there was less variability than expected in the emotional intelligence
of this sample. Although some variability was observed, all the MEIS-A scores for this sample remained in the
average range normatively. Given the literature concerning the emotional and cognitive abilities of gifted adolescents
(e.g., Falk, Piechowski, & Lind, 1994; Roeper, 1982), it was expected that the emotional intelligence levels of the
gifted and talented participants in this study could vary appreciably. Instead, it appears possible that the residential
school setting influenced the results. Struggling students may have honed emotional problem solving skills over time
in a supportive environment that encourages exploration of emotional and social dilemmas. However, this conclusion
does not explain why none of the gifted students studied demonstrated a higher than average level of emotional
intelligence. With this issue in mind, the authors question the ability of the MEIS-A short form to measure the
emotional intelligence of gifted students in a valid manner. Suggestions for addressing these issues are proposed in
the next section.
Recommendations for Future Research
Despite the results of the current study, the findings of other similar studies (e.g., Maree & Ebersohn, 2002; Mayer,
2001; Rubin, 1999; Trinidad & Johnson, 2002) suggest that additional emotional intelligence research or related

studies may still be useful for understanding variations in the success of gifted adolescents. We suggest several
possibilities for future studies.
First, rather than study an overall level of emotional intelligence, various personality or temperamental traits that are
related to Goleman's (1995) model of emotional intelligence (e.g., persistence, optimism) could be more practical and
meaningful. Although not specifically part of the Mayer and Salovey (1997) model of emotional intelligence, and
probably better underscored as personality traits, characteristics like persistence and optimism do appear to require
the cognitive processing of emotional information. Researchers who take this approach could help identify the specific
constructs--whether they are considered cognitive abilities or otherwise--that contribute to successful outcomes. One
hypothesis, for example, might be that persistence contributes significantly to the social and academic success of
gifted adolescents once IQ is controlled for statistically.
A second suggestion for future research concerns the setting for studies involving gifted and talented adolescents. It
appears likely that future research would be more sensitive to the social and academic needs of the majority of gifted
adolescents if conducted in regular school settings. The needs of gifted students would likely be more highlighted in
regular school settings, where necessary supports and challenges are less likely to be consistent. The social and
academic experiences of the gifted adolescents in the current study were likely influenced in a positive way by their
residential school environment. Students in this setting may often feel more appropriately challenged as they interact
with other similar students. Moreover, they likely feel more socially accepted and more interested in social
relationships in this setting. Indeed, the majority of the participants in this study reported social stress and
interpersonal relations that were within normal limits, seemingly reflecting this support, and possibly reducing the
likelihood of significant findings.
Finally, future research that specifically addresses the emotional intelligence construct must involve improved
measurement of this variable. Fortunately, advancements have been made since the data was collected for the
current study. Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (2002) recently published an updated "ability-based" measure of their fourbranch model of emotional intelligence, the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Because
their goal has always involved the development of an ability-based instrument, Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso attempted
to exclude the personality components that have consistently confounded other models and test instruments. The four
branches of this measure continue to reflect factors ranging from the perception of emotion to the management of
emotion. The MSCEIT was developed for educational and research purposes and was intended for adolescents 17
and older. As another option, Bar-On and Parker (2000) have added a youth version emotional intelligence test
(BarOn EQ-i -YV) to the mix of measurement options (ages 7-18). These measurement advances are clearly an
improvement over the unpublished instrument used in the current study.
In summary, careful consideration of the research setting, the changing measurement options, and the model
connected to each measurement option will be essential in future studies involving emotional intelligence and gifted
students. When the goal is a more applicable understanding of the variables that contribute to success in gifted
adolescents, researchers should give consideration to analyzing more specific related constructs -- constructs that
may or may not be directly subsumed under a model of emotional intelligence. Similar to current concerns involving
the measurement of IQ in children and adolescents, the measurement of a total emotional intelligence score may be
impractical for those who work directly with students. The results of studies involving specific emotional processing
factors instead of one total emotional intelligence score will likely be more valuable for the helping professionals who
work directly with gifted and talented adolescents.
Table 1 Descriptive Statistics For All Variables Measured (N = 39)

Legend for Chart:


A
B
C
D

Variable
Minimum
Maximum
M

E - SD
A
1. CSI

109.00

145.00

128.69

10.39

2. MEIS-A

-0.87

0.88

0.01

0.46

3. IR

31.00

57.00

52.10

7.21

4. SS

38.00

68.00

50.05

9.47

5. GPA

2.00

4.00

3.28

0.56

6. AGE

191.00

221.00

202.87

6.87

Note: CSI = Cognitive Skills Index (Standard


Scores);
MEIS-A = Multifactor Emotional Intelligence
Scale - Adolescent
Version (z-Scores); IR = Interpersonal Relations
(T-Scores);
SS = Social Stress (T-Scores); GPA = Grade Point
Average
(Four-Point Scale); AGE = Age in months.
Table 2 Pearson Correlations Between All Variables Measured (N =39)

Legend for Chart:


A
B
C
D
E
F

Variable
1
2
3
4
5

1. CSI

--

2. MEIS-A

-.029

--

3. IR

-.107

-.090

4. SS
-5. GPA
116
--

.151
.371(*)

--

-.016
.046

-.453(**)

-.240

(*) p < .05 (2-tailed).


(**) p < .01 (2-tailed).
Note: CSI = Cognitive Skills Index; MEIS-A =
Multifactor
Emotional
Intelligence
Scale
Adolescent
Version;
IR = Interpersonal Relations; SS = Social
Stress;
GPA = Grade Point Average.
Table 3 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Interpersonal Relations In
Gifted Adolescents (N = 39)

Legend for Chart:


A
B
C
D
E

Variable
df
R
MS
F

Step 1
CSI

.012

22.780

.431 NS

MEIS-A

.020

19.906

.370 NS

Note: NS = Nonsignificant (p > .05)

Table 4 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Social Stress in Gifted
Adolescents (N = 39)

Legend for Chart:


A
B
C
D
E

Variable
df
R
MS
F

CSI

.023

78.223

.869 NS

MEIS-A

.023

39.330

.425 NS

Note: NS = Nonsignificant (p > .05)


Table 5 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Grade Point Average in Gifted
Adolescents (N = 39)

Legend for Chart:


A
B
C
D
E

Variable
df
R
MS
F

CSI

.138

1.663

5.914(*)

MEIS-A

.141

.851

2.955 NS

(*) p < .05 Note: NS = Nonsignificant (p > .05)

REFERENCES
Bar-On, R. (1997). The emotional quotient inventory (EQ-i): Technical manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health
Systems.
Bar-On, R., & Parker, J. D. (2000). The emotional quotient inventory: Youth version (EQ-i-YV). North Tonawanda. NY:
Multi-Health Systems.
Caruso, D. R., Van Buren, A., Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1998). The measurement of emotional intelligence in
adolescents. Unpublished manuscript.
Davis, G. A., & Rimm, S. B. (1998). Education ?f the gifted and talented (415 ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Falk, R. F., Piechowski, M. M., & Lind, S. (1994). Criteria for rating intensity of overexcitabilities (Manual). Unpublished
manuscript, Northland College in Ashland, WI.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Hallahan, D. P., & Kaufman, J. M. (Eds.). (1994). Exceptional children. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Henig, R. M. (1996, June). Are you smarter than you think? McCalls 123, 84-91.
Lum, M. S. (1988). Gifted population and adjustment: A literature review of giftedness on conduct, family adjustment,
emotional functioning, social functioning, and perceived competence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Biola
University, La Mirada, CA.
Maree, J. G., & Ebersohn, L. (2002). Emotional intelligence and achievement: Redefining giftedness? Gifted
Education International, 16(3) 261-273.
Mayer, J. D. (2001). Emotional intelligence and giftedness. Roeper Review, 23(3), 131-137.
Mayer, J. D., & Caruso, D. R. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence.
Intelligence, 27(4), 267-299.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional
development and emotional intelligence (pp. 3-31). New York: Basic Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (1996). Multifactor emotional intelligence scale - Adolescent version (MEISA). Unpublished instrument, University of New Hampshire.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (1997). Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS). Unpublished
instrument - Item and answer booklet, University of New Hampshire.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Models of emotional intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
Handbook of Human Intelligence (pp. 396-420). New York: Cambridge.
Mayer. J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). The Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) User's Manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
Osborn, J. (1996). Special education needs of gifted and talented children. Youth Mental Health Update, 8(4), 7-14.
Peterson, K. S. (1997, February 18). Signs of intelligence: Do new definitions of smart dilute meaning? USA Today, p.
D1.
Piechowski, M. M. (1997). Emotional giftedness: The measure of intrapersonal intelligence. In N. Colangelo & G. A.
Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (pp. 366-381) Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behavior Assessment System for Children, Circle Pines. MN: American
Guidance Services.
Rimm, S. B. (1996). Why bright kids get poor grades: And what you can do about it. New York. Crown
Roeper, A. (1982). How the gifted cope with their emotions. Roeper Review, 5,21-24.

Rubin, M. M. (1999). Emotional intelligence and its role in mitigating aggression: A correlational study of the
relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression in urban adolescents. Unpublished manuscript,
Immaculata College, Immaculata, PA.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 9(3), 185-211.
Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children. San Diego: Jerome M. Sattler.
Test of Cognitive Skills (2nd ed.). Manual. (1993). Los Angeles: CTB Macmillan/McGraw-Hill.
Trinidad, D. R., & Johnson, C. A. (2002). The association between emotional intelligence and early adolescent
tobacco and alcohol use. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(1), 95-105
Wechsler, D. (2003). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) - Technical and
Interpretive Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
~~~~~~~~
By Scott A. Woitaszewski and Matthew C. Aalsma
Scott Woitaszewski is currently an assistant professor in the Department of Counseling and School Psychology at the
University of Wisconsin - River Falls. He has taught courses in child and adolescent assessment, intervention
strategies, and school consultation in addition to directing student research and supervising school psychology
interns. His current research interests include the identification and application of resiliency factors in students with
disabilities.
Matthew Aalsma is currently an assistant professor of pediatrics and psychology at the Indiana University School of
Medicine. His research focuses on adolescent health behavior. Current research projects include assessing the role
of adolescent romantic relationships in how health behavior is expressed in adolescence as well as the development
of antisocial behavior in adolescence.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi