Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 14

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution: Impact of Motivation Level and Extension

Typicality
Author(s): Sharon Ng
Source: Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 47, No. 1 (Feb., 2010), pp. 186-198
Published by: American Marketing Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/20618964
Accessed: 21-07-2016 18:55 UTC
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/20618964?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marketing Research

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

SHARON NG*
This research examines cross-cultural differences in brand dilution
effects and the moderating role of motivation and extension typicality.
Drawing from recent findings that indicate that culture affects the way peo
ple treat conflicting information, this research predicts that Easterners and
Westerners react differently to failures by a brand extension. In contrast
to previous findings that have suggested that failure in a typical extension
leads to less brand dilution for Westerners when they are highly motivated
(than when they are less motivated), this study argues that Easterners
exhibit greater brand dilution when they are less motivated (than when
they are highly motivated). The opposite pattern of results should emerge
when the extension is atypical. Three studies provide support for these
predictions and the underlying processes.
Keywords: brand dilution, culture, self-construal, motivation, extension
typicality

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution:


Impact of Motivation Level and

Extension Typicality
The introduction of brand extensions or new products
using an established brand name is one strategy by which
firms leverage a brand's goodwill (Aaker and Keller 1990;
Boush and Loken 1991). This strategy enables firms to
introduce new products at much lower risks and costs.
The proliferation of brand extensions in the marketplace
testifies to the value firms place on this strategy. How
ever, using a brand extension strategy is not without draw
backs. Although successful brand extensions help enhance
the equity of a brand, the converse is true as well: Should
an extended product fail, the negative affect or associations
it generates may filter back to the parent brand, thereby
"diluting" its equity (Loken and John 1993; Milberg, Park,
and McCarthy 1997). Thus, firms need to be cautious when
making brand extension decisions and understand how var
ious factors, such as culture, may affect consumers' reac
tions to extension success and failure.

the effectiveness of branding strategies. The number of


U.S. companies that have expanded into foreign markets
has increased exponentially, and the successful penetra
tion of overseas markets requires a good understanding
of the impact of culture on people's purchasing behavior.
A fundamental issue that marketing managers face is how

Both practitioners and researchers acknowledge the


importance of understanding the impact of culture on

resolve any inconsistency. They are more likely to focus on


just one piece of the information to the exclusion of another

*Sharon Ng is Associate Professor of Marketing, Nanyang Business


School, Nanyang Technological University (e-mail: angsl@ntu.edu.sg).
The author acknowledges financial support from Nanyang Business
School and thanks the two anonymous JMR reviewers for their helpful
comments. The author also thanks Akshay Rao and Michael J. Houston
for their insightful comments on a previous draft of this article. Special
thanks to Barbara Loken for her advice and mentorship during the early
part of this research. Ziv Carmon served as associate editor for this article.

consumers in different cultures react to various branding

strategies. In this research, I examine one aspect of this

issue by investigating how culture affects people's reactions


to brand extension failures.
Recent findings in cross-cultural literature show that peo

ple across cultures do not process information similarly.


Specifically, Easterners (e.g., Chinese people) are more tol
erant of inconsistency. When faced with two pieces of con
flicting information, they prefer to seek a middle position
by taking both pieces of information into account. In con

trast, Westerners (e.g., Americans) feel a greater need to

piece in such situations (Nisbett et al. 2001). This find


ing suggests a difference in the way Easterners and West
erners evaluate brand extension failure information. Why?

Most brands that are extended in the marketplace possess


at least a moderately positive brand image, so when an
extension fails, the negative information conflicts with the
brand's positive image. If Easterners and Westerners differ
in the way they process conflicting information, they also
should differ in their perceptions of brand dilution effects.

This claim is consistent with previous findings that the

? 2010, American Marketing Association Journal of Marketing Research

ISSN: 0022-2437 (print), 1547-7193 (electronic) 186 Vol XLyn (Febmary 201Q)> lg6_19g
This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 187


impact of an extension failure on brand equity depends on
other factors, such as the consistency of the extended prod
uct with the parent brand, motivation levels, information
accessibility and diagnosticity, and firms' branding strate

gies (Ahluwalia and Gurhan-Canli 2000; Gurhan-Canli and


Maheswaran 1998; John, Loken, and Joiner 1998; Loken
and John 1993; Milberg, Park, and McCarthy 1997). Build
ing on these findings, I propose that culture is another
important variable to consider.
Thus, the objective of this research is to address two basic
issues that are critical to understanding how firms should
brand their products in foreign markets. First, does culture
affect how consumers react to brand failure information?
I examine this issue in the context of bicultural consumers.

Specifically, I assess how these bicultural people respond


when different cultural orientations or processing styles
temporarily become more accessible. Second, do motiva
tion level and extension typicality moderate the effect? By
showing how the effect of culture varies according to peo
ple's motivation level and type of extended products, I high
light the complexity associated with understanding when
and how culture affects consumers' judgments. Answers to
these questions also can help marketing managers make bet
ter branding decisions in foreign markets.
The rest of this article proceeds as follows: First, I briefly

discuss relevant literature that provides the basis for the


hypotheses. Second, I report on the three studies conducted
to test these hypotheses. Third, I conclude with a discussion
of the implications of the findings and avenues for further

research.

Loken, and Joiner (1998) find that flagship products are rel
atively immune to negative feedback from extended prod

ucts. Milberg, Park, and McCarthy (1997) also show that


firms strategically try to mitigate any negative feedback
effects from product extensions through subbranding.

A second group of studies focuses on identifying vari


ables that moderate brand dilution effects. For example,
Loken and John (1993) examine how perceptions of "gen
tleness" and "quality" for Johnson & Johnson may depend
on the typicality of the brand extension (i.e., extent to which
the extension appears inconsistent with the parent brand).

They find that dilution is less likely when extension typ


icality is salient and consumers perceive the extension as
atypical of the family brand. Ahluwalia and Gurhan-Canli
(2000) find that enhancement and dilution effects are great
est when extension information is highly accessible (i.e., top
of the mind).

Thus, substantial research has investigated the factors


that affect brand dilution. However, most existing research

pertains to Western cultures. Do extension failures have


similar effects in Eastern cultures? Existing research is
silent on this issue. Drawing from recent findings in cross
cultural literature, I propose that people in different cultures
react differently to extension failures, depending on their
motivation level and the typicality of the extended product.

Impact of Motivation and Extension Typicality


Existing literature suggests that a key factor that affects
brand dilution effects is motivation level. Motivation affects

a person's willingness to process information (Eagly and

THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
Extant Literature on Brand Feedback Effects
To renew consumers' interest in their products and keep
pace with changing consumer tastes, firms must introduce
new products in the marketplace. However, the manner in
which new products should be introduced remains a critical

question. Should new products represent a separate brand


name or the same family brand? Each strategy has pros
and cons. Introducing new products under the family brand

name helps reduce promotion costs and encourages accep


tance of new products, especially if consumers are already
familiar with the brand and transfer their existing brand

beliefs to the new products (Keller 2003). However, infor


mation does not flow in only one direction. Should the

Chaiken 1993; Petty and Wegener 1999). Dual process

theories such as the elaboration likelihood model and the

heuristic-systematic model postulate that motivation may

affect the quality and quantity of information processed


(Petty and Wegener 1999). When motivation is low, people

engage in less resource-demanding processes. They pro

cess less information and scrutinize the same information

less carefully (Gurhan-Canli and Maheswaran 1998; Petty


and Wegener 1999). They rely only on a subset of infor
mation to make their judgments. However, when motiva
tion increases, people engage? in relatively more extensive
and effortful processing. They are more likely to scrutinize
all information presented and show increased consideration

of diagnostic information to arrive at a reasoned attitude


(Aaker and Sengupta 2000; Gurhan-Canli and Maheswaran
1998; Petty, Cacioppo, and Schumann 1983; Petty and

new product fail, negative beliefs generated from the exten


sion failure may filter back to the parent brand, thereby

Wegener 1999).

diluting its brand equity (John, Loken, and Joiner 1998;


Loken and John 1993). Because firms must protect their

less effortful processing and rely only on a subset of infor


mation when they experience low motivation, there is a lack
of agreement about what information people focus on when
they receive conflicting information. One stream of litera
ture (e.g., consistency bias, negativity effects, predecisional
distortion) argues that when motivation is low, people dis
count any inconsistent information and rely on their prior
attitude or beliefs in their judgment. For example, consis
tency bias research argues that with low motivation, people
are more likely to base their evaluations on prior brand atti
tudes and discount any inconsistent information. Research

brand equity, understanding when and to what extent brand

dilution occurs is important both managerially and the


oretically. As testimony to the importance of this issue,
many studies have attempted to address these questions
(e.g., Ahluwalia and Gurhan-Canli 2000; Gurhan-Canli and
Maheswaran 1998; John, Loken, and Joiner 1998; Loken
and John 1993; Milberg, Park, and McCarthy 1997).

Prior studies consist of two categories. One group of

studies focuses primarily on identifying situations in which


firms are insulated from dilution effects. For example, John,

Although general consensus states that people engage in

in predecisional distortion also shows that people tend to

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

188 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010


distort new information to make it consistent with their

existing preferences (Russo, Meloy, and Medvec 1998).


According to this perspective, in the event of an exten
sion failure, greater dilution occurs when motivation is high

than when motivation is low because, when motivation is


low, consumers rely more on their prior attitude toward
the brand (which is generally positive, because brands that
possess positive equity are more likely to engage in brand
extensions). However, when motivation increases, the con
sumer pays greater attention to all information presented
and gives greater weight to information that is more diag
nostic. Prior research on the negativity effect has shown that

people consider negative information (e.g., extension fail


ure) more diagnostic than positive information (Ahluwalia
2002), so greater dilution may occur with high motivation.
However, another group of studies argues for the reverse
effects. A person may be less likely to process all the infor
mation presented when he or she has low motivation, but in
that case, the person also chooses to rely on the information
he or she perceives as more diagnostic (Chaiken, Liberman,

and Eagly 1989). For example, Maheswaran and Chaiken


(1991) find that with low motivation, when the valence
of attribute information is inconsistent with that of the
heuristic cues, people tend to rely on the more diagnostic
attribute information to make their judgments. Aaker and
Sengupta (2000) also find that when there is an incongruity
between the source (e.g., endorser) and attribute informa
tion, the more diagnostic attribute information influences
consumers' judgments more.
According to this research stream, consumers should pay
greater attention to the extension failure information (which

is more diagnostic) when they have low motivation. How


ever, when people invest more effort to process all the
information available to them with high motivation, the
inclusion of other pieces of information (including positive
information, such as prior brand attitude) in the evaluation
might mitigate the negative effect of the extension failure.

In this situation, greater dilution is expected in the low


motivation condition.

This perspective is not to suggest that people do not


focus on diagnostic information when they have high moti

vation, nor that positive and negative information con


tribute equally to people's attitudes. Rather, a person who
takes into account both positive and negative information

should exhibit more favorable attitudes than another per


son who only takes into account negative information. With
high motivation, the more diagnostic negative information
should exert a greater impact, but the mere consideration of
the positive information also should exert some influence

on attitudes.

Thus, the two perspectives make different predictions


about the impact of motivation on brand dilution. Although

they appear contradictory, they may be equally valid,


albeit in different conditions. Studies by Gurhan-Canli and

Maheswaran (1998) show that depending on the typical


ity of the extension, higher motivation leads to greater or
lesser dilution. Specifically, the failure of an extension in

a product category that appears typical or similar to the


brand's existing products leads to greater dilution in the
low motivation condition than in the high motivation con

dition. Failure in a typical extension is a piece of diag


nostic information that people cannot ignore. Because they

consider only a subset of information when they experi


ence low motivation, people likely focus on this negative
information. However, when motivation increases, includ
ing other information reduces the impact of the extension
failure information. However, when an extension is atypi
cal, failure in the extension is not diagnostic of the brand's
quality, and in the low motivation condition, consumers
subtype this extension, such that it has minimal influence
on the overall brand schema. In the high motivation con
dition, consumers consider all pieces of information, and
extension failure information might exert some influence
on perceptions, even if it is not highly diagnostic informa
tion. In such cases, greater dilution should occur in the high
motivation condition than in the low motivation condition.

Thus, the impact of motivation on brand dilution effects


depends on the perceived typicality of the extension.
Hla: For a typical extension, when Westerners perceive an
extended product's performance on an attribute as below
expectations, they evaluate the parent brand lower on that

attribute when their motivation is low than when their

motivation is high.
Hlb: For an atypical extension, when Westerners perceive an
extended product's performance on an attribute as below
expectations, they evaluate the parent brand lower on that

attribute when their motivation is high than when their


motivation is low.

Brand Dilution Effects in Eastern Culture


Despite some differences, most prior studies concur that
in the face of conflicting information, people focus only on
selected information when they have low motivation. When

their motivation is high, people take into account more


information and pay greater attention to information that is

more diagnostic. However, is such information processing


universal? The answer is yes and no. Research shows that
similar to Westerners, Easterners engage in more effortful
processing of information when their motivation is high.

Easterners scrutinize the information provided in greater


detail and pay greater attention to information that is
more diagnostic (Aaker and Maheswaran 1997; Aaker and
Sengupta 2000). However, unlike Westerners, when their
motivation is low, Easterners do not necessarily focus only
on selected information in making a judgment. Research in
marketing and psychology shows that even with low moti

vation, Easterners have a greater tolerance for ambiguity


that leads them to incorporate information from conflicting
perspectives in their judgments.1
For example, Aaker and Sengupta (2000) show that when

motivation is low, Westerners focus on selected pieces


of information, whereas Easterners consider the multi
ple pieces of information provided. In their studies, they
also show that in low motivation conditions, when partici
pants from an Eastern origin receive conflicting information

about an attribute and an endorser, they are more likely


]An exception is Aaker and Maheswaran (1997), who find that in low
motivation conditions, Easterners behave like Westerners and focus only
on the diagnostic information. However, Aaker and Sengupta (2000) argue
that this finding applies only to the situation in which one of the cues is
highly diagnostic.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 189


to take into account both pieces of information, whereas

motivation should lead to less favorable attitudes toward

Westerners focus primarily on the attribute. They argue that


this distinction is because Easterners are more tolerant of

and the positive information in low motivation conditions).

the parent brand, versus considering both the negative

ambiguity and less compelled to resolve the incongruity.


This argument is consistent with prior research in cross
cultural literature that shows that Easterners deal with con

However, when the extension is atypical, more elaborate

tradictions in a dialectic or compromise manner (Peng


and Nisbett 1999). They process information holistically,

parent brand. Thus, they focus more on their prior attitude

attending to and assigning some causality to context (Choi

and Nisbett 2000). To Easterners, the world is constantly


changing, and it is important to understand the interrela
tion among events (Nisbett et al. 2001). An ever-changing

world also implies constant contradiction. This empha


sis on change and interrelation among events leads to a
more dialectic way of thinking, which "involves reconcil
ing, transcending, or even accepting apparent contradic
tions" and the search for the "Middle Way" (Nisbett et al.

2001, p. 294; see also Peng and Nisbett 1999). Unlike


Westerners, who are chronically more likely to reject one of
the propositions and focus on the piece of information they

consider more plausible or diagnostic, Easterners believe


that they can find truth in each perspective and should seek

a balance between extreme views.

In support of this argument, in psychology literature,

processing with high motivation should lead Easterners to

realize that the extension failure is not diagnostic of the

and perceive less dilution when their motivation is high


than when their motivation is low (i.e., focusing on the
more diagnostic positive information with high motivation

should lead Easterners to adopt more favorable attitudes


toward the parent brand, versus considering both negative
and positive information in low motivation conditions).2
H2a: For a typical extension, when Easterners perceive an
extended product's performance on an attribute as below
expectations, they evaluate the parent brand lower on that

attribute when their motivation is high than when their


motivation is low.

H2b: For an atypical extension, when Easterners perceive an


extended product's performance on an attribute as below
expectations, they evaluate the parent brand lower on that

attribute when their motivation is low than when their

motivation is high.

Peng and Nisbett (1999) reveal that participants who read


about two contradictory studies demonstrated more moder

ate attitudes than those who read only one of them. This
finding suggests that participants who read about the two
contradictory studies took into account both perspectives,
as reflected in their attitude. Evidence for greater toler
ance of inconsistency in Eastern cultures also emerges from
other domains. For example, Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi (1999)
show that unlike Americans, who are more likely to experi
ence only one form of emotions, both negative and positive
emotions can coexist for Chinese people. Cousins (1989)
also shows that Easterners consider both dispositional traits
and contextual cues when making a judgment; Westerners
take into account only the dispositional traits. Thus, sig
nificant evidence shows that unlike Westerners, who zoom
in on a particular aspect, Easterners are chronically more
likely to take information from varied perspectives into
account.

According to these findings, because extension failure


by a positively viewed brand may be construed as two
pieces of contradictory information (i.e., the positive image
of the brand versus the negative information about the new
product), Easterners should react differently to an extension
failure than Westerners. Specifically, Easterners chronically
take into account more pieces of information, so they are
more likely to incorporate both negative extension failure
information and positive brand attitude in their judgments
when they have low motivation. However, when motivation
increases, closer scrutiny should reveal that some informa

tion is more diagnostic than others, with greater weight


placed on this information (Aaker and Sengupta 2000).
Whether this weight leads to greater or lesser degrees of
dilution depends on the typicality of the extension. When

the extension is typical, more elaborate processing leads


Easterners to focus on the more diagnostic extension failure
information, prompting greater dilution when motivation is

high compared with when motivation is low (i.e., focus


ing on the more diagnostic negative information with high

Brand Enhancement Effects

Both Hj and H2 indicate cross-cultural differences in


brand dilution effects. What happens when an extension is
successful? In the event of extension success, the positive
information is consistent with the positive brand informa

tion. Because there should be no difference in the way

Easterners and Westerners evaluate congruent information,

a similar pattern of results is expected in both cultures.


Consistent with previous research that has shown that
greater elaboration leads to more polarized and stronger
attitudes, greater brand enhancement is also expected when
people are highly motivated than when they are not as moti
vated, across cultures.
H3: Both Easterners and Westerners exhibit a greater degree
of brand enhancement when their motivation is high than
when their motivation is low.

To test these hypotheses, I conduct three studies. Study 1


examines the impact of culture on the feedback effect of

negative information on a typical extension by making


either Eastern or Western cultural orientations more acces

sible through priming. Study 2 builds on the findings in


Study 1 by priming participants' processing styles more
directly and examining the moderating role of extension
typicality. Study 3 further examines what happens in the
event of brand extension success.

2It is argued that only people who take into account both existing
(positive) brand information and (negative) extension failure information

exhibit a different attitude from those who consider only one piece of
information. It is not argued that existing brand information and extension

failure information contribute equally to people's attitudes. The way the


two pieces of information combine may differ across contexts and people;
thus, no predictions are made about the relative impact of each piece of
information.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

190 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010

STUDY I

Method
Design. The objective of Study 1 is to examine whether
people across cultures exhibit different brand dilution
effects. In particular, it focuses on how extension failure
on a key parent brand attribute affects consumers' subse
quent parent brand beliefs about that attribute.3 To test the
hypotheses, a 2 (cultural prime: U.S. culture versus Chinese
culture) x 2 (motivation: low versus high) between-subjects

design was used. Typicality was held constant across con


ditions, and all participants reviewed information about a

moderately typical extension (Hla and H2a). All the factors


were manipulated, and participants were randomly assigned

to each condition.

Culture was manipulated by increasing the accessibil


ity of specific cultural orientations (i.e., Eastern culture
or Western culture) to participants through priming. This
methodology is consistent with recent findings that multi
ple cultural orientations exist within a single person (e.g.,

bicultural people; see Hong et al. 2000; Lau-Gesk 2003),


such that in any given context, one orientation may dom

inate the other. However, a less dominant cultural orien

tation may become temporarily more accessible through a


variety of priming techniques. In contrast to cross-national
designs traditionally used in prior literature, this approach
enables researchers to control for a host of extraneous vari

ables that may lead to potential confounding (e.g., Aaker


and Lee 2000; Chen, Ng, and Rao 2005; Hong et al. 2000).
Consistent with this paradigm, for the following studies,
I recruited bicultural respondents and made their Eastern
or Western cultural orientation temporarily more accessible
through visual primes.

Participants. One hundred five Chinese Singaporean stu


dents from a Singaporean university participated in this
study for partial fulfillment of course credit. Most previous
research has focused on the difference between Eastern cul

ture (epitomized by Chinese culture) and Western culture


(epitomized by U.S. culture), so for these studies, partici
pants viewed either a U.S. prime or a Chinese prime. Singa
porean students are a good sample for such studies because
most Singaporeans retain some knowledge of their ethnic
culture (e.g., Chinese) and yet are also exposed to Western
influences (e.g., products, films). Thus, they embody both
traditional Eastern culture and Western culture, to which
they have been exposed since birth. Moreover, because
English is the first language in the country, no problems
are associated with having to translate the questionnaire.
Approximately 26 students were in each cell, and a control
group of participants who were not exposed to the exten
sion information provided the baseline condition as a check
for dilution.
3In general, prior literature has operationalized brand dilution as either

dilution in the general affect toward the brand (e.g., Keller and Aaker
1992) or dilution in a person's perception of specific attribute beliefs (e.g.,

Loken and John 1993). The second operationalization is adopted because,


when a company uses an existing brand to introduce a new product, it
usually hopes to transfer some prevailing, positive association from the
parent brand to the new product. These associations define the brand and
often are the brand's key features that distinguish it from competitors

(Loken and John 1993).

Procedure and stimuli. Participants completed the online


study in front of a computer. As primes of each cultural
orientation, participants viewed collages comprising either

U.S. cultural icons or Chinese cultural icons. Hong and

colleagues (2000) have applied a similar priming tech

nique successfully in marketing studies (Chen, Ng, and Rao

2005). On the cover page of the study, participants read


that they would be asked to complete two unrelated studies.

The first study aimed to assess their knowledge of impor


tant icons, so they were to examine the icons carefully.
On the next page, participants saw either a U.S. collage
or a Chinese collage. The U.S. collage contained pictures
of U.S. cultural icons (e.g., Superman, Statute of Liberty,

U.S. flag, bald eagle). The Chinese collage included pic


tures of Chinese cultural icons (e.g., Confucius, Great Wall

of China, calligraphy, Chinese opera) (Chen, Ng, and Rao


2005). After viewing the collage, participants recalled the
icons they had just seen, which helped reinforce the prime.
Next, the respondents proceeded to the second study.4

The first page of the second study included the moti


vation manipulation. Participants read that Sony would be
introducing a new type of personal digital assistant (PDA)
that allows users to play games (with controls similar to
Game Boy) in either Singapore (high motivation condition)
or Asia (low motivation condition). Sony is a familiar brand
in Singapore (7 = "highly familiar," M = 6.5), and a pretest
showed that, in general, people are favorable toward it (7 =
"very favorable," M = 6.6). Because for this study extension
typicality remained constant, the extension product needed
to be perceived as moderately typical of Sony's existing
products. Prior research also has shown that culture or pro
cessing styles may affect people's perceptions of extension
similarity (Monga and John 2005; Ng and Houston 2005).
Easterners or people who process information holistically
can find relationships among products better and therefore
view products as more similar than Westerners or people
who process information analytically. Therefore, the experi
mental product must be one that both groups view similarly
in terms of typicality. A PDA fits this bill quite well. First,
Sony currently has products in the PDA category, and the

gaming capabilities described in relation to the extended


product are similar to the game consoles currently sold by
Sony. A pretest shows that PDAs with gaming capabilities

were typical of Sony's products, according to participants


primed with both Chinese and U.S. collages (7 = "typical,"
MUSi = 5.64, MChinese = 5.35). Second, because the influence
of positive brand information relative to negative extension
information is the key variable of interest, a product for
which the brand name is an important attribute was also
needed. Pretests show that brand name was important in
PDA purchases (7 = "important," M = 5.8).
The instructions indicated that the experimenters were
interested in participants' opinion of a new product. To
aid in their evaluation, they would receive a recent Con
sumer Reports review of the new product. The review
provided information about product performance on four
4 A suspicion check conducted at the end of the experiment asked partic
ipants to write down what they believed were the objectives of each study.

Two independent judges coded these responses with regard to whether the

participants linked the two studies together. Almost all the participants
believed the story and did not link the two studies.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 191


the motivation manipulation was successful. Only the main

attributes: design, battery life, audio quality, and price. The


four attributes were chosen because, to test for the dilution

effect of motivation was significant (F(l, 101) = 21.66,

of specific beliefs, the attributes must be associated strongly

p < .01). Participants in the high motivation condition (M =

with Sony and important for consumers' PDA purchases.


From actual reviews of PDAs published in magazines, in

4.44, SE = .16) were more motivated than those in the low


motivation condition (M = 3.42, SE= .16). No other main

newspapers, and on the Web, ten PDA attributes were con


sidered important. In a pretest, a group of 20 students rated
the extent to which they associated Sony strongly with the
attributes, as well as the importance of the attributes for
PDA purchases. The attribute they associated most strongly

effects and interaction effects were significant (p > .1).

more in line with Eastern (Western) culture, they should

with Sony and considered important for PDAs was audio


quality, which served as the focal attribute. For the nega

Ng, and Rao 2005). A full-factorial ANOVA performed

tive evaluation of the new product on the focal attribute,

on the politicians listed showed a significant effect of cul

the review rated the new PDA poorly on audio quality.


A separate pretest with 30 participants also showed that

the Chinese prime condition listed significantly more East

this information was moderately inconsistent with Sony's


image (7 = "inconsistent," M = 5.3; SD = 1.23). In addition,
to mask the focal attribute, the review contained ratings
of three other attributes, namely, design, battery life, and

price. To make the review sound believable, the new PDA


received negative ratings in terms of design but moderately
positive ratings on battery life and price. If the new product
were rated inconsistently on all four attributes, the scenario
would become too unbelievable; it is rare for a new product
to receive poor ratings on all dimensions. After reading the

review, participants rated Sony and the new PDA on the


four attributes, the perceived typicality of the new PDA,
and the consistency of the information provided, relative to

Sony's image.
To assess whether the negative information had any
impact on actual product choice, participants indicated
which brand of MP3 player (Sony or Creative Zen) they
would like to receive, should their names be drawn in a
lucky drawing promoted as part of the experiment. An MP3
player provides the choice option because it is another prod

uct for which audio quality is of paramount importance.


If the negative information provided affects people's per

ceptions of Sony's audio quality, they should be less will


ing to choose the Sony MP3 player. A Creative Zen MP3
player is the alternative MP3 player because it is popular
in Singapore, and most people have a moderately favorable

evaluation of the device (7 = "favorable," M = 5.92). The


pretest showed no significant difference in attitudes between

the two MP2 players (MSony = 5.66; F(l, 24) = .69, p > .1);

the participants also viewed the two brands as equivalent in


audio quality (MCreative = 5.69, MSony = 5.75; F(l, 24) = .37,

p>.\).

Finally, participants indicated any thoughts they had

about the review and completed some manipulation checks


and demographic measures. Participants in the control con

Two measures checked whether the priming manipu


lation was successful. First, participants named the first

three politicians that came to mind; if participants thought

be likely to list more Eastern (Western) politicians (Chen,

tural priming (F(l, 101) = 28.27, p < .01). Participants in


ern politicians (M= 1.78, SE = .10) than those in the U.S.
prime condition (M = 1.22, SE = .10). Participants in the
U.S. prime condition also listed significantly more Western

politicians (M = 2.12, SE = .11) than those in the Chinese


prime condition (M = .88, SE= .11). No other main effects
or interaction effects were significant (p > A).
Second, participants indicated their attitude toward a list

of four values frequently associated with Chinese culture


(e.g., taking care of aged parents, greeting teachers) and
four values frequently associated with the Western culture
(e.g., being unique, independence).5 Both scales demon
strated good psychometric properties (Chinese scale: a =
.78; Western scale: a = .83). Participants in the Chinese
prime condition agreed more with the Chinese cultural val
ues (M = 5.07, SE = .10) than with Western cultural values
(M = 4.31, SE=.10; F(l, 101) = 30.32,p<.01). In contrast,
those in the U.S. prime condition agreed more with the list
of Western cultural values (M = 5.62, SE = .17) than with
the Chinese cultural values (M = 4.56, SE = .17; F(l, 101) =
19.84, p < .01). Thus, the results from both manipulation
checks converged, in support of the cultural manipulation.
Third, the analysis of participants' ratings of the con
sistency of the information provided with Sony's image
showed that the audio quality information appeared mod
erately inconsistent with Sony's image (7 = "inconsistent,"
M = 5.3, SD = 1.66). Comparisons across the four condi
tions revealed no significant differences in the perceptions
of consistency (p > .1). There also was no difference in the
perceptions of extension fit between the new PDA and Sony
across conditions (p > .1).
Analysis. An ANOVA with cultural prime and motiva
tion as the between-subjects variables centered on the focal
attribute of audio quality. The main effects of cultural
prime and motivation were not significant (F(l, 101) =

.80, p > .1; F(l, 101) = .03, p > .1, respectively). How

dition were not exposed to the manipulations; they only

ever, consistent with expectations, a significant two-way

indicated their evaluations of Sony on the four attributes


and their overall attitude toward Sony.

interaction emerged between cultural prime and motivation

(F(l, 101) = 9.83,/?< .01) (see Figure l).6 Contrasts showed


that participants in the Chinese prime condition rated Sony

much lower in audio quality when their motivation was


high (M = 3.71, SE = .19) than when their motivation was

Results
Manipulation checks. For the motivation manipulation
check, participants responded to a two-item, seven-point
scale about how interested and involved they were when
completing the questionnaire (with 7 = "highly involved,"
"interested"). A full-factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA)
performed on the mean of the two-item scale shows that

5These cultural values reflect those typically used to assess self


construal (i.e., independent versus interdependent self) and cultural orien
tation (i.e., individualism versus collectivism).
6 Similar analyses of the three filler attributes did not yield any signifi

cant results {p> .1).

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

192 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010


Figure 1

IMPACT OF CULTURAL PRIMES AND MOTIVATION ON BRAND

EVALUATION

focused primarily on the information in the review. The


two judges agreed 94% of the time and resolved any dif

5
H Low motivation

4.5

ferences through discussion. Analysis of the thought data


showed a significant difference across the four conditions

H High motivation

(X2 = 7.46, p < .01). More participants in the U.S. prime

>? 4

low motivation condition indicated that they focused pri

CO
3

O 3.5

marily on the information from the review (n = 21) than

those in the U.S. prime-high motivation condition (n = 9).

' 3B

<

CO

participants' thought data for the extent to which they tried


to reconcile the information in the review with their prior
knowledge about Sony. Participants fell into two categories:
those who tried to reconcile the information and those who

Participants in the U.S. prime-high motivation condition


instead showed a greater desire to reconcile the information
in the review with their existing brand beliefs. The reverse
was true in the Chinese prime conditions: More participants
in the Chinese prime-high motivation condition indicated
that they focused primarily on the information from the

2.5

(/) 2

*? 1.5
CO

review (n = 17) than those (n = 9) in the low motivation

ce 1

condition. Thus, the pattern of results supported the study

predictions.

5H
U.S. Prime

Chinese Prime

low (M = 4.32, SE = .18; F(l, 103) = 5.44, p < .05). Par

ticipants in the U.S. prime condition revealed lower rat


ings of Sony's audio quality when their motivation was low

(M = 3.55, SE = .19) than when their motivation was high


(M = 4.14, SE = .19; F(l, 103) = 4.35, p < .05), consistent
with this study's predictions.

Although the two-way interaction was noteworthy, it


provided no insight about whether dilution occurred. The
manipulation might have increased the attitudes of all par
ticipants, with a greater increase in one condition than in
another. A more stringent test for dilution effects would
determine whether people in the experimental conditions
rated Sony lower in audio quality than those in the con
trol condition, in which the participants did not see any
priming or motivation manipulation. The comparison of
participants' beliefs about Sony's audio quality in all four
experimental conditions with those in the control group
(M = 4.73, SE = .25) showed significant dilution in the
Chinese prime-high motivation condition (MAcontrol group =

1.02, SE = .32; p < .05, confidence intervals [CIs] = .24


and 1.80) and in the U.S. prime-low motivation condi

tion (MAcontrol group = 1.18, SE = .32; p < .01, CIs = .40 and
1.96). However, the attitudes of those in the Chinese prime
low motivation and U.S. prime-high motivation conditions
were not significantly lower than those of the control group
mean (both p > .1). Thus, dilution occurred only in the Chi
nese prime-high motivation and U.S. prime-low motivation

conditions.7

Process evidence. To investigate whether the dilution


occurred as a result of the differential treatment of con
tradictory information, two independent judges coded the
7 An ANOVA of the overall brand evaluations assessed whether dilution
at the attribute level filters down to affect participants' evaluations of the
brand. No significant main effect or interaction effect was found (p > .1).

Impact on choice behavior. Finally, to examine whether


the dilution affected actual purchase behavior, participants'
choice of MP3 player was analyzed. A chi-square test of the
number of people who picked the Sony MP3 player versus

the Creative Zen MP3 player showed a significant differ


ence across conditions (x2 = 7.84, p < .01). More partici
pants in the U.S. prime-high motivation condition picked
the Sony MP3 player (n = 20) than those in the U.S. prime

low motivation condition (n = 11). However, more par


ticipants in the Chinese prime-low motivation condition
picked the Sony MP3 player (n = 22) than those in the
Chinese prime-high motivation condition (n = 9). There
fore, dilution filtered down to affect participants' subse
quent product choices.

Discussion
The results from this study provide support for the
proposition that Easterners and Westerners differ in the
way they process extension failure information and that this

effect is moderated by their level of motivation. Specifi


cally, for Western-primed participants, dilution is greatest
when motivation is low, but for Eastern-primed participants,
dilution is greatest when motivation is high. The findings
from the thought data also offer evidence with regard to
the process explanation; that is, the differential way East
erners and Westerners treat conflicting information leads
to cross-cultural differences in brand dilution effects. This
result is intriguing, especially for Eastern-primed partici
pants, because it contrasts with the general assumption that

people take all pieces of information into account only


when their motivation is high.
Although the Study 1 findings support Hla and Hlb, this
study suffers from several shortcomings. First, the investi
gation successfully primed the participants' cultural orien
tation, but it remains unclear which dimension of culture
drives these effects. The participants could differ on a mul
titude of cultural dimensions. Because the difference in pro
cessing styles is argued to affect the way people react to the
conflicting information, Study 2 addresses this limitation
by manipulating participants' processing styles directly.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 193


Second, there is a potential alternative explanation for the
Study 1 findings. Easterners exhibit a different pattern of
results, perhaps because they deal with conflicting informa
tion differently. However, it also is possible that Easterners
and Westerners deal with conflicting information similarly,
but a difference in the perceived diagnosticity of the infor

I, me), whereas those in the holistic condition circled pro


nouns that represented an interdependent self (e.g., we, us).
Because self-views are related to processing styles, by acti
vating different self-views, this manipulation should be able
to activate the related processing styles (K?hnen, Hannover,

parent brand may be more diagnostic, such that they rely on


parent brand equity only when they have low motivation.
When their motivation is higher, they take into account all
information and therefore exhibit greater dilution. Although
the thought data in Study 1 appear to support the initial

product that Nokia recently introduced. Nokia is the focal


brand in this study because a pretest showed that, in gen
eral, people have positive attitudes toward this brand (M =
5.96). It also is strongly associated with cell phones, and its
camera function induces positive perceptions of the excel
lent pictures it produces. To identify appropriate extension
products for this study, a pretest preceded the actual experi
ment. To examine how holistic and analytic processors view
failure in typical and atypical extensions, this study needed
products for which there is no difference in the typicality
perceptions across both groups of respondents.

mation drives the results. For Easterners, the role of the

explanation, more evidence could rule out the alternative


explanation. Therefore, in Study 2, extension typicality is

manipulated.
How can manipulation of typicality determine the accu
racy of the two explanations? The two process explana
tions make slightly different predictions in contexts that

include extension typicality. If Easterners consider more


information when their motivation is low, when the exten

sion is typical, they should exhibit greater dilution when


their motivation is high (H2a). When the extension is atyp
ical, greater dilution should occur when motivation is low
than when motivation is high (H2b). The alternative expla
nation makes the opposite prediction: If Easterners, similar
to Westerners, take into account only diagnostic informa
tion when they experience low motivation, and they differ
only in the type of information they perceive to be more

diagnostic (e.g., brand name), when they have informa

and Schubert 2001; Monga and John 2007).


Next, participants read a consumer report about a new

Of the 40 participants recruited for the pretest, half


received the holistic processing style manipulation, and the
other half considered the analytic processing style manip
ulation. After the manipulation, participants indicated the

extent to which they believed a list of 15 products were


similar to Nokia's existing products and that the prod
ucts fit Nokia's brand image. The pretest revealed prod
ucts perceived as both typical and atypical by both groups

of participants. An Internet phone that allows people to


engage in video conferencing online is the atypical exten
sion (7 = "highly typical," holistic processor = 3.97, analytic

tion that is highly diagnostic (e.g., failure of a very typi


cal extension), they should behave like Westerners and rely

processor = 4.26; no significant difference across condi

should exhibit greater dilution in low motivation (than in

as an image stabilizer, is the typical extension (holistic


processor = 6.43, analytic processor = 6.12; no significant

on this information as well. In this situation, Easterners


high motivation) conditions. In contrast, when the exten
sion is atypical, they should exhibit greater dilution in high
motivation (than in low motivation) conditions.

STUDY 2

Method
Study 2 aimed to fulfill two objectives: to examine
whether consumers behave differently when the extension

is typical (versus atypical) and to replicate the findings


from Study 1 with a direct manipulation of the participants'

processing styles.
Participants and design. One hundred nineteen students

from a Singaporean university received $5 each to partic


ipate in this study, which involved a 2 (processing style:
analytic versus holistic) x 2 (motivation: low versus high) x

2 (extension typicality: typical versus atypical) between


subjects design. The definition of typicality referred to
product category similarity. All factors were manipulated,
and participants were randomly assigned to each condition.

Seven conditions had 15 participants, and one condition


had 14 participants.
Procedure and stimuli. To activate their different pro
cessing styles, participants received a short description of
a trip to the city; they were told to circle all the pronouns
in the text (K?hnen, Hannover, and Schubert 2001; Monga
and John 2007). Participants in the analytic condition cir
cled pronouns that represented the independent self (e.g.,

tions). A cell phone with the latest camera functions, such

difference across conditions).

The product review offered participants information

about the extension on four attributes: exterior, user friend

liness, software, and camera picture quality. Picture qual


ity was the focal attribute; the other three attributes were
filler attributes. The product received negative ratings on
picture quality and user friendliness and neutral ratings for
the other two attributes. After reading the review, partic

ipants rated Nokia and the extended product on the list


of attributes, as well as the perceived typicality of the
new product. Finally, they completed a list of manipulation
checks and demographic items before being debriefed.

Results
Manipulation checks. To confirm the success of the pro
cessing style manipulation, participants responded to a list

of six questions that measured the extent to which they


believe things in life are related (e.g., "Everything in the

universe is somehow related to each other," "The whole is

greater than the sum of its parts"). This test is consistent


with the argument that holistic processors tend to pay more
attention to the field and perceive interrelationships among

things (Nisbett et al. 2001). The scale demonstrated good


reliability (a = .75), and the ANOVA of the mean of the
scale showed that the manipulation was successful. Partici
pants in the holistic processing condition were more likely
to believe that everything in the universe is highly related

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

194 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010


(M = 5.58, SE = .11) than those in the analytic processing
condition (M = 4.98, SE = .11;F(1, 111)= 13.94, p< .01).
The manipulation checks also showed that the motiva
tion manipulation was successful. Participants in the low

Figure 2

EFFECT OF EXTENSION TYPICALITY, MOTIVATION, AND


PROCESSING STYLES ON BRAND EVALUATIONS

motivation condition indicated significantly lower levels of

A: For a Typical Extension

motivation (M = 4.32, SE = .14) than those in the high


motivation condition (M = 4.94, SE= .15; F(l, 111) = 9.35,

p < .01). Finally, to check the typicality manipulation, par


ticipants' ratings of the typicality of the two extension prod
ucts were analyzed. The results show that the manipulation

was successful (F(l, 111) = 27.39, p < .01): Those in the


low typicality condition rated the extended product signif
icantly lower in terms of typicality than those in the high

typicality condition (Mlow = 4.19, SE = .16; Mhigh = 6.22,


SE = .16). There was no difference in the typicality percep
tions for the two processing style conditions.

Attribute evaluation. Similar to previous studies, a

2x2x2 ANOVA was run. The results show a significant


main effect of processing style (F(l, 111) = 4.42, p < .05)

5.5
m Low motivation

75
5
3

High motivation

O
S? 4.5 i
3

*-*

CL

CO
M

Z 3.5
O)
c
(0

ir

and a three-way interaction among processing style, moti

vation, and product typicality (F(l, 111) = 23.21, p < .01)


(see Figure 2). No other effects were significant. Further

2.5

way interactions in both the high typicality (F(l, 111) =


18.44, p < .01) and the low typicality (F(l, 111) = 5.03,
p < .05) conditions were significant. Specifically, when
typicality was high, participants in the analytic process
ing condition rated Nokia's picture quality significantly
less favorably when their motivation was low (M = 3.36,
SE = .26) than when their motivation was high (M = 4.50,
SE = .26; F(l, 111) = 11.06, p < .01). In contrast, partici
pants in the holistic processing condition rated Nokia sig
nificantly less favorably when their motivation was high
(M = 4.11, SE = .26) than when their motivation was low
(M = 5.00, SE = .26; F(l, 111) = 6.62, p < .05). The reverse
findings emerge when typicality was low. That is, in the
low typicality condition, participants in the analytic pro
cessing condition rated Nokia's picture quality less favor
ably when their motivation was high (M = 3.96, SE = .26)
than when their motivation was low (M = 4.69, SE = .26;
F(l, 111) = 4.42, p < .05). Participants in the holistic condi
tion exhibited no significant differences in their ratings of

Nokia's picture quality (Mlow = 4.53, SE = .26; Mhigh = 4.91,


SE = .26; p > . 1). However, the means are directionally con
sistent with the predictions. Overall, the pattern of results
is consistent with expectations.

Discussion
The findings from Study 2 support the hypotheses that
the typicality of an extension affects the degree of dilution
exhibited by Easterners and Westerners. The findings also
provide evidence contrary to the alternative explanation that
the results obtained in Study 1 were a function of Eastern
ers' reliance on parent brand equity in the low motivation

condition.

STUDY 3
Method

Analytic Holistic
Processing Style

probes into the three-way interaction revealed that the two

B: For an Atypical Extension

5.5
H Low motivation

& 5
75

High motivation

O
3

4.5

CO

To
o

"5 3-5

O)
c

CC 3
2.5

Analytic Holistic
Processing Style

information. Participants consider information about either


an extension success (congruent, positive attribute informa
tion) or an extension failure (incongruent, negative attribute

information) by Dell. In contrast to the extension fail


ure, information about a successful extension by a positive
brand should be consistent with parent brand beliefs, such

that no differences are expected in brand beliefs across


priming conditions (H3).
Participants and design. One hundred seventeen stu
dents from a Singaporean university participated in this
study for partial fulfillment of course credit. A 2 (cultural

prime: U.S. versus Chinese) x 2 (motivation: low versus


high) x 2 (brand extension: success versus failure) between

The objective of Study 3 is to examine what happens


when Easterners and Westerners receive nonconflicting

subjects design was run, with all factors manipulated and


participants randomly assigned to each condition. Three

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 195


conditions contained 14 participants, and five conditions
each had 15 participants. In addition, another 15 students
served as the control group. Extension typicality remained
constant across all the conditions.
Stimuli. In this study, Dell is the focal brand. The pretests

showed that, in general, people have favorable attitudes


toward Dell (M = 5.2). The product extension pertains to
digital cameras. Dell is not currently in the digital cam
era market, but this extension is believable because many
computer firms market digital cameras. Moreover, digi

The 30 participants in the pretest viewed the information


in the brand dilution condition as more incongruent with
Dell's image (M = 5.0, SE = .23) than the information in the

brand enhancement condition (M = 2.8, SE = .25; F(l, 29) =


3.56, p < .05). After reading the review, participants rated

Dell and the new camera on each attribute, the perceived


typicality of digital cameras, and the congruity of the infor

mation provided with Dell's image. Finally, they provided


their thoughts about the review and their responses to the
manipulation checks, as well as demographic information.

tal cameras increasingly represent technology or computer

related products. Both Chinese-primed and U.S.-primed


pretest participants perceived digital cameras as similar to

Dell's existing products (7 = "very similar," Mus = 5.7,


MChinese = 5.9).

Procedure. The procedure for this study is similar to


that described for Studies 1 and 2. Participants viewed the
priming collage before the motivation and branding manip

ulations. The study manipulation indicated that Dell was


planning to introduce a new product, a digital camera, to
the market. The focal attribute was picture quality, which
is an important attribute for digital cameras and relevant

to aspects of Dell's brand image. Three other attributes


(customization, user friendliness, and price) masked the

attribute of interest. Of the four attributes, two attributes

(picture quality and user friendliness) were manipulated to


create an enhancement or dilution condition.8 The other two
attributes were neutral, that is, neither positive nor nega
tive. To manipulate brand enhancement and brand dilution,
participants were told that the product performed either bet
ter or worse than other models in the market on the two

attributes. For example, in the brand enhancement condi


tion, the text indicated the following:
Dell Digito delivers excellent picture quality, richer
in color saturation, and clearer than other models
in the market. It combines the power of a 3x opti

cal zoom lens, a 3.2 megapixel CCD, and advanced


photo enhancing technique that makes the pictures

sharper and more vibrant relative to those taken by


other models.

The participants in the brand dilution condition instead


read the following:
Dell Digito delivers moderate picture quality. The
color saturation is not as rich and the picture is
not as clear as other models in the market. Even
though it is equipped with a 3x optical zoom lens, a

3.2 megapixel CCD and advanced photo enhancing

technique, the pictures are not as sharp and vibrant


as those taken by other models.

8The user-friendliness variable in the brand enhancement condition indi

cated, "Although Dell Digito comes with a range of automatic preset


shooting modes, it is not as user-friendly as the other models in the mar

ket. The complicated user interface means you would have to spend much
time learning how to use the camera. Not good for novice users or anyone

looking for a point-and-shoot camera with extra features." In the brand

dilution condition, it read, "Dell Digito also comes with a range of auto

Results
Manipulation and confounding check. The manipulation
checks showed that the motivation manipulation was suc
cessful. A full-factorial analysis indicated that only the
main effect of motivation was significant (Mlow = 3.08,
SE = .27; Mhigh = 4.13, SE = .28; F(l, 109) = 7.35, p < .01).

No other effects were significant (p > .1). An ANOVA


also showed that the priming manipulation was successful

(F(l, 109) = 24.08, p < .01). Participants in the U.S. prime


condition listed significantly more Western politicians (M =

2.28, SE = .14) than Eastern politicians (M = .72, SE = .14).


Those in the Chinese prime condition listed significantly
more Eastern politicians (M = 1.88, SE = .14) than West
ern politicians (M = 1.12, SE = .14). No other effects were
significant (p > .1). Tests of participants' ratings of whether
the extended product fits the brand's existing product cat
egories also indicated no significant difference across con

ditions (p> .1).


Results. As expected, a 2x2x2 ANOVA revealed a

significant main effect of brand extension (F(l, 108) =


23.95, p < .001). Participants in the enhancement condition

(M = 4.72, SE = .14) rated the parent brand significantly


higher on picture quality than those in the dilution condi

tion (M = 3.72, SE = .15). The results also showed a signif


icant three-way interaction for picture quality (F(l, 109) =

5.78, p < .05) (see Figure 3).9 All other main effects and
interaction effects were not significant (p > .1). To decipher
the three-way interaction, the interaction effects of brand
enhancement and brand dilution conditions were examined

separately.
Specifically, the two-way interaction between culture and
motivation was not significant in the brand enhancement

condition (F(l, 109) = .81, /?>.l). In both priming con

ditions, evaluations of Dell's picture quality were higher


in the high motivation condition (Mus = 4.81, SE = .27;
^Chinese = 5.04, SE = .26) than in the low motivation con
dition (Mu s = 4.64, SE = .26; MChinese = 4.40, SE = .26).
Compared with the control group (M = 4.31, SE = .20),
both groups of participants rated the parent brand signif

icantly higher in picture quality in the high motivation


condition, which indicates an enhancement effect (U.S.:
MAcontroi group = -51, SE = .31; one-tailed p < .05, CIs = -.12
and 1.13; Chinese: MAcontrol group = .74, SE = .31; p < .05,

CIs = .13 and 1.36). The enhancement effects were not sig
nificant in the low motivation conditions (both p > .1).

matic preset shooting modes that makes it more user-friendly than other
models in the market. The uncomplicated user interface means you won't
spend much time learning how to use the camera. Perfect for novice users
or anyone looking for a point-and-shoot camera with extra features."

9The corresponding interactions for the control attributes?customiza


tion, user friendliness, and price?were not significant (p > A).

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

196 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010


from the brand dilution condition did not occur in the

Figure 3

ASYMMETRIC EFFECT OF BRAND ENHANCEMENT AND


BRAND DILUTION
A: When Extension Succeeds (Brand Enhancement)
6
IH Low motivation

5.5 H

incongruent information.

5n

+
JiJ

?
O 4.5
?)
CO

try in brand dilution and enhancement effects across cul


tures. In addition, the lack of cross-cultural differences in
the brand enhancement (or information-congruent) condi
tion supports the argument that the mechanism driving the

cross-cultural difference in brand dilution effects is dif


ferences in the ways Easterners and Westerners evaluate

H High motivation

CO
3

brand enhancement condition (i.e., when information is


congruent), which signals a certain degree of asymme

?.

-i

4H

GENERAL DISCUSSION
A brand extension is an effective way for firms to lever
age their brand's equity (Keller 2003). Extant literature has

documented various benefits of engaging in brand exten

sions (e.g., Aaker and Keller 1990; Boush and Loken

1991), but there are both pros and cons of extending a

3.5

U.S. Prime

Chinese Prime

B: When Extension Fails (Brand Dilution)

High motivation

(0
3

J?

4H

o 3.5 H

O)

-E

CC

2.5

U.S. Prime

potential dilution of a brand's equity, should an extension


fail (Loken and John 1993). This study examines whether
culture influences brand dilution effects using investigations

with bicultural respondents, which document systematic

El Low motivation

> 4.5 H

brand to new categories. The peril of brand extension is the

Chinese Prime

Replicating the results from Studies 1 and 2, the two


way interaction between culture and motivation was sig
nificant in the brand dilution condition (F(l, 109) = 5.70,
p < .05). The planned contrasts showed that in the U.S.
prime condition, dilution was more extreme with low moti

vation (M = 3.51, SE = .27) than with high motivation


(M = 4.12, SE = .28; F(l, 112) = 3.05, p < .05). However,

in the Chinese prime condition, dilution was more extreme

with high motivation (M = 3.19, SE = .28) than with low


motivation (M = 4.07, SE = .28; F(l, 112) = 6.27, p < .05).
Consistent with the findings from Study 1, significant dilu

differences in the way people with a more Eastern orienta


tion, compared with those with a more Western orientation,
evaluate brand extension failure information. Specifically,
Study 1 shows that when a typical extension fails, Eastern
ers exhibit significantly greater dilution effects when their
motivation is high than when their motivation is low. This
effect may emerge because Easterners who are low in moti

vation are chronically more likely to integrate the nega


tive information into their existing brand schema. However,

when motivation is high, they scrutinize the information


in greater detail and place greater weight on the negative
information, which is more diagnostic. However, for West
erners, a failure in a typical extension causes those who are
low in motivation to focus only on the diagnostic negative
information, whereas those with high motivation pay atten
tion to all the information presented. This difference leads
to greater brand dilution in low motivation conditions.

Building on the results of Study 1, Study 2 replicates


the brand dilution results and shows a reverse pattern when
the extension is atypical. Study 3 further shows that in the

event of a brand extension success, both Easterners and


Westerners behave similarly. Altogether, the findings from
these three studies indicate that extension failure has differ
ential effects across cultures. Therefore, they offer signif
icant theoretical and managerial contributions to branding
and cross-cultural literature.

tion occurred only in the U.S. prime-low motivation con


dition (MAcontrol group = .79, SE = .35; p < .05, CIs = .10 and

Theoretical Implications

(MAcontrol group = 1.11, SE = .36; p < .01, CIs = .40 and 1.83).
The other two comparisons were not significant (both with
p > .1), in support of the hypotheses.

as extensions of prior research, the findings from the

1.49) and the Chinese prime-high motivation condition

Discussion

Theoretically, this study contributes on three fronts. First,

three studies provide further evidence that Easterners treat


conflicting information differently than Westerners, which

has important implications in a marketing context. Prior

The results from this study replicate the brand dilution

findings in Studies 1 and 2. The cross-over interaction

cross-cultural psychology literature provides some evidence


that Easterners treat conflicting information differently than

Westerners (e.g., Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi 1999; Peng and

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Cultural Orientation and Brand Dilution 197


Nisbett 1999), but it is unclear if such processing differ
ences affect how consumers process marketing informa

introductions, other branding strategies (e.g., subbranding,


individual branding) may be more beneficial.

tion. The only exception is research by Aaker and Sengupta


(2000), who examine this issue in a marketing context.
Second, examining cross-cultural differences in the way

regard to the brand enhancement results. Though not dis

people process conflicting information is a new addition


to brand dilution literature. Traditionally, brand dilution
literature has examined this issue from a categorization
perspective, with the assumption that new products that
may be grouped in the same category as existing products
would prompt favorable views but that the parent brand also
becomes more vulnerable to extension failure. This study's
findings bring a new perspective to this issue by showing
that independent of categorization, the way a person pro
cesses incongruent information is important. A person who

is chronically more likely to take into account all pieces

of information exhibits a different level of brand dilution

than one who focuses only on diagnostic information. By


manipulating extension typicality, the studies further show
that differences in cue diagnosticity across cultures can
not explain this effect. However, it is important to qual
ify that the relative strength of each cue (e.g., reputation,
attribute) is not manipulated, so further research should
examine what happens if consumers from different cultures
see cues of varying diagnosticity.
Third, this research builds on previous findings in brand
dilution literature by identifying an important variable that
moderates the extent to which brand dilution effects occur.
Significant research on the topic of brand dilution identifies
several notable factors that moderate brand dilution effects

(e.g., Ahluwalia and Gurhan-Canli 2000; Loken and John


1993). Implicitly, the same brand dilution processes and
findings should be applicable to other cultures. Yet recent
findings in cross-cultural literature show that this assump

tion is not necessarily true. Cross-cultural differences in


information processing occur in various domains, and it
seems naive to assume that the same brand dilution process
would be universal.

This recommendation is particularly significant with

cussed, the findings from Study 3 exhibit an asymme

try in the brand dilution and brand enhancement effects


across cultures. Unlike the results for brand dilution effects,
Easterners and Westerners demonstrate similar patterns for
brand enhancement. For Easterners, the brand dilution and
brand enhancement effects also differ in their magnitude.
Further analysis of the results in Study 3 reveals that though
both brand enhancement and brand dilution effects are sig
nificant among highly motivated Easterners, the difference
between the mean in the brand enhancement condition and
that for the control group is only .56, whereas the differ
ence between the mean in the brand dilution condition and
that for the control group is 1.18. This discrepancy seems
to suggest that in Eastern cultures, for high-involvement
products, introducing a new product under the family brand
will lead to some enhancement in brand equity if the prod
uct succeeds, but it will lead to relatively greater dilution

of brand equity if the new product fails. This observa


tion argues against brand extensions for high-involvement

products.
However, these recommendations only take into account

the impact of extension success and failure on the par


ent brand. This research does not examine the impact of

brand name on the way Eastern consumers evaluate brand

extensions. In making brand extension decisions, compa


nies should weigh the benefits of a new product introduc
tion with an existing family brand against the cost of the
potential brand dilution in the event of an extension failure.
To the extent that the benefits of introducing new products
under existing brands outweigh the potential risks of brand

dilution, it would be beneficial to engage in brand exten


sion. Further investigations could clarify when the bene

fits of brand extension would outweigh the risks of brand

dilution.

Other marketing implications. The evidence that East


erners and Westerners process conflicting information dif

Managerial Implications
Implications for brand management. These results have
important implications for brand managers operating in a

global world. As companies increasingly rely on foreign


markets for growth, managers need a better understand
ing of whether the branding strategies they use at home
can transfer to other cultures. Marketing strategies must

be localized to sell in foreign markets, but the more dif


ficult question is which marketing strategies should be
modified and how. This issue transcends industries. The

results herein provide some insights into these issues,


including the important implication that firms selling high

involvement products in Eastern markets (e.g., China)


should adopt a different branding strategy than those selling
low-involvement products. For example, among Easterners,

the brand dilution effect is stronger when the extension


is typical and the motivation level is high, so firms sell
ing high-involvement products should be judicious when
engaging in close brand extensions. They also should rec
ognize that if an extended product fails, the potential nega
tive feedback effect may be fairly strong. For risky product

ferently also has important implications for marketing


communications strategies. Though not examined in this
research, such processing differences may affect how con
sumers treat negative information about a positive brand.
For example, in the event of a public relations crisis (e.g.,

quality control failure by a favored brand leads to con


sumer harm), Easterners and Westerners should treat such
information differently. If the issue is severe, Easterners

who are highly motivated (e.g., if the issue is personally


relevant) may exhibit a more negative attitude toward the

company than those who are less motivated. In contrast,


Westerners who are highly motivated may exhibit a less
negative attitude toward the company than those who are
less motivated.

Limitations and Further Research


Several limitations of these studies suggest avenues for
further research. First, all the participants in the studies

came from a bicultural society. The use of a bicultural


sample allows for the random assignment of participants
and reduces the potential of confounds across cultures.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

198 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, FEBRUARY 2010


Although it was assumed that through priming partici

Hong, Yingyi, Michael W. Morris, Chi-Yue Chiu, and Ver?nica


Benet-Mart?nez (2000), "Multicultural Minds: A Dynamic Con
structivist Approach to Culture and Cognition," American Psy
chologist, 55 (7), 709-720.

pants tend to think in patterns that are more aligned with


one culture than another, the findings might not generalize

to monocultural societies. Additional research should test

whether people in monocultural societies behave similarly.

John, Deborah R., Barbara Loken, and Christopher Joiner (1998),


"The Negative Impact of Extensions: Can Flagship Products Be
Diluted?" Journal of Marketing, 62 (January), 19-32.

Second, the focus was primarily on brands with positive


evaluations. Most brands that engage in brand extensions
enjoy positive perceptions, but it would be worthwhile to

Keller, Kevin L. (2003), Strategic Brand Management: Building,


Measuring, and Managing Brand Equity, 2nd ed. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

examine how the results might change if the parent brand

were less than positively evaluated.

REFERENCES

-and David Aaker (1992), "The Effect of Sequential Intro

Aaker, David A. and Kevin L. Keller (1990), "Consumer Evalua


tions of Brand Extensions," Journal of Marketing, 54 (January),

27-41.

Aaker, Jennifer L. and Angela Y. Lee (2001), " 'F Seek Pleasures
and 'We' Avoid Pains: The Role of Self-Regulatory Goals in
Information Processing and Persuasion," Journal of Consumer
Research, 28 (June), 33-49.

-and Durairaj Maheswaran (1997), "The Effect of Cultural


Orientation on Persuasion," Journal of Consumer Research, 24

(December), 315-28.

- and Jaideep Sengupta (2000), "Additivity Versus Atten

uation: The Role of Culture in the Resolution of Information


Incongruity," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9 (2), 67-82.

Ahluwalia, Rohini (2002), "How Prevalent Is the Negativity Effect


in Consumer Environments?" Journal of Consumer Research,

29 (September), 270-79.

- and Zeynep Gurhan-Canli (2000), "The Effects of


Extensions on the Family Brand Name: An Accessibility

Diagnosticity Perspective," Journal of Consumer Research, 27


(December), 371-81.

Bagozzi, Richard, Nancy Wong, and Youjae Yi (1999), "The Role


of Culture and Gender in the Relationship Between Positive
and Negative Affect," Cognition and Emotion, 13 (6), 641-72.
Boush, David M. and Barbara Loken (1991), "A Process-Tracing
Study of Brand Extension Evaluation," Journal of Marketing
Research, 28 (February), 16-28.

Chaiken, Shelly, Akiva Liberman, and Alice H. Eagly (1989),


"Heuristic and Systematic Information Processing Within and
Beyond the Persuasion Context," in Unintended Thought, J.S.

Uleman and J.A. Bargh, eds. New York: Guilford Press,

212-52.

Chen, Haipeng, Sharon Ng, and Akshay Rao (2005), "Cultural


Difference in Consumer Impatience," Journal of Marketing
Research, 42 (August), 291-301.
Choi, Incheal and R.W Nisbett (2000), "Cultural Psychology of
Surprise: Holistic Theories and Recognition of Contradiction,"
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 890-905.
Cousins, S.D. (1989), "Culture and Self-Perception in Japan and
the United States," Journal of Personality and Social Psychol
ogy, 56 (1), 124-31.
-

Eagly, Alice H. and Shelly Chaiken (1993), The Psychology of


Attitudes. Orlando: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Gurhan-Canli, Zeynep and Durairaj Maheswaran (1998), "The


Effects of Extensions on Brand Name Dilution and Enhance

ment," Journal of Marketing Research, 35 (November),

464-73.

duction of Brand Extensions," Journal of Marketing Research,

29 (February), 35-50.
K?hnen, Ulrich, Bettina Hannover, and Benjamin Schubert
(2001), "The Semantic-Procedural Interface Model of the Self:
The Role of Self-Knowledge for Context-Dependent Versus
Context-Independent Modes of Thinking," Journal of Person
ality and Social Psychology, 80 (3), 397-409.

Lau-Gesk, Loraine G. (2003), "Activating Culture Through Per


suasion Appeals: An Examination of the Bicultural Consumer,"
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 301-315.

Loken, Barbara and Deborah Roedder John (1993), "Diluting


Brand Beliefs: When Do Brand Extensions Have a Negative
Impact?" Journal of Marketing, 57 (July), 71-84.

Maheswaran, Durairaj and Shelly Chaiken (1991), "Promoting


Systematic Processing in Low-Motivation Settings: Effect of
Incongruent Information on Processing and Judgment," Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (1), 13-25.

Milberg, Sandra J., C. Whan Park, and Michael S. McCarthy


(1997), "Managing Negative Feedback Effects Associated with
Brand Extensions: The Impact of Alternative Branding Strate
gies," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 6 (2), 119-^40.

Monga, Alokparna Basu and Deborah R. John (2004), "Con


sumer Response to Brand Extensions: Does Culture Matter?"
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 31, Barbara Kahn
and Frances Luce, eds. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer

Research, 216-22.

Ng, Sharon and Michael J. Houston (2005), "Exemplars or


Beliefs? The Impact of Self-View on the Nature and Rela
tive Influence of Brand Associations," Journal of Consumer
Research, 32 (4), 519-29.
Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Noren
zayan (2001), "Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic vs.
Analytical Cognition," Psychological Review, 108 (2), 291-310.
Peng, Kaiping and Richard E. Nisbett (1999), "Culture, Dialectics,
and Reasoning About Contradiction," American Psychologist,

54 (9), 741-54.
Petty, Richard, John T. Cacioppo, and David W. Schumann
(1983), "Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effec
tiveness: The Moderating Role of Involvement," Journal of
Consumer Research, 10 (September), 135-46.

and Duane T. Wegener (1999), "The Elaboration Likeli

hood Model: Current Status and Controversies," in Dual Pro


cess Theories in Social Psychology, Shelly Chaiken and Yaacov
Trope, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 73-96.

Russo, J. Edward, Margaret G. Meloy, and Victoria Husted Med


vec (1998), "Predecisional Distortion of Product Information,"
Journal of Marketing Research, 35 (November), 438-52.

This content downloaded from 193.226.34.227 on Thu, 21 Jul 2016 18:55:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi