Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 21

Narrative as a tool for the assessment of

linguistic and pragmatic impairments


Nicola Botting
University of Manchester

Abstract
Narrative ability is one of the most interesting and ecologically valid ways in
which to measure communicative competence both in normal populations and
in clinical groups, since narratives form the basis of many childhood speech
acts. Narrative may also prove to be a good tool for distinguishing clinical
groups who show overlapping symptoms but who are thought to experience
subtly different impairments. This article gives an overview of some of the
theoretical reasons for using narrative to assess both linguistic and pragmatic
impairments. As part of a preliminary investigation examining possible
similarities and differences across groups, ve children with severe pragmatic
language impairments (PLI) and ve with more typical speci c language
impairments (SLI) completed short picture based narratives using the Bus
Story and the Frog Story. These illustrative data are included throughout the
paper to highlight features of use to clinicians, particularly with respect to
differences in the narratives of children with PLI compared to their peers with
SLI. Furthermore, when compared to Tager-Flusbergs (1995) data from
children with autism, SLI narratives seem to be more similar to those of the
group with autism than did PLI narratives. Narrative ability was found to
relate directly to pragmatic skill but in different ways according to clinical
subgroup. Implications for both theory and practice are discussed.

Introduction
Narrative ability is one of the most interesting and ecologically valid ways in
which to measure communicative competence both in normal populations and
in clinical groups. It is also one of the most abundant sources of information
Address for correspondence: Nicola Botting, Centre for the Study of Language Impairments, Human
Communication and Deafness, School of Education, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M13 9PL,
UK. E-mail: nicola.botting@man.ac.uk
# Arnold 2002

10.1191=0265659002ct224oa
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

since narratives form the basis of many childhood speech acts. Spontaneous
speech samples have been used widely in speech and language therapy
practice, but completely unstructured samples are time consuming and are
often only used in descriptive ways by therapists. Narrative appears to be used
less often as a tool. One exception to this is The Bus Story (Renfrew, 1991)
narrative assessment. This story is regularly employed in primary language
units in the UK, and has strong relationships with future language and literacy
performance (Stothard et al., 1998) as well as genetic trends (Hohnen and
Stevenson, 1999). However, other forms of narrative may prove to be more
useful after the infant years, especially for children who have been regularly
tested using the Bus Story.
In addition to this, a group of children with pragmatic language impairments (PLI), who prove dif cult to assess using formal language tests (Botting
et al., 1998), seem to have increased in number within language units and
speechlanguage therapy services. These children are also dif cult to assess
using free conversational speech samples (e.g., Bishop and Adams, 1989; see
later discussion). Children with PLI are children often previously referred to in
the literature as having semantic-pragmatic disorder (Rapin and Allen, 1983;
Bishop and Rosenbloom, 1987; Bishop, 1989; Rapin, 1996). These children
have primary problems with the social use of language especially conversation
and some also have linguistic dif culties. However recent studies have
suggested these are not speci cally semantic in nature (Botting and ContiRamsden, 1999) and therefore this part of the terminology has been dropped
by some researchers (McTear and Conti-Ramsden, 1992; Bishop, 1998; ContiRamsden and Botting, 1999). It is still unclear, despite over a decade of
research in this area, whether these individuals are best described as having
language impairment, as part of the autistic spectrum, or neither (see Boucher,
1998; Botting, 1998;Brook and Bowler, 1992;Rapin and Allen, 1998;Gagnon et
al., 1997. Despite the belief of some that children with PLI are children with
an autistic spectrum disorder, a number of these individuals nd their way into
language units and by school year 2 (age 7), make up a signi cant proportion
(around 15%) of those receiving educational help for language impairments
(Conti-Ramsden and Botting, 1999; Conti-Ramsden et al., 1997; Botting and
Conti-Ramsden, 1999).

Rationale for considering narrative as a clinical tool


The present article addresses three main theoretical reasons for using oral
narrative as a clinical tool:
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

1) There is ample normative data showing clear developmental trends and


thus narrative can be used to compare populations with each other and over
time.
2) Narrative competence has also been shown to associate with literacy
ability.
3) Narrative involves a degree of pragmatic skill but is more formal
than conversation and is of interest in distinguishing different subgroups of children with communication dif culties, namely those with
primary linguistic dif culties (children with SLI), those with primary
pragmatic dif culties and relatively little linguistic de cit (children
with PLI) and children with both these areas of dif culty (children with
autism).

1) Narratives and typically developing children. Narratives have been


documented comprehensively in young normally developing children in
recent years (Bamberg, and Damrad-Frye, 1991; Bamberg 1987; Bamberg
and Marchman, 1990) and it is now established that quite young individuals
are skilled story tellers. Even pre-school children have been found to use
unscaffolded narratives and to show a good knowledge of story grammar given
favourable contexts (Peterson, 1990; Eaton et al., 1999). Clear developmental
patterns have also been shown in narrative style, in which very young children
show shorter story lengths (Leadholm and Miller, 1995); fewer different words
(Miller, 1991), less complex syntax (Gillam and Johnston, 1992); and a less
complex or incomplete story grammar or structure (Peterson and McCabe,
1983; Shapiro and Hudson, 1991). The Wisconsin Language Sample Analysis
cites three key indices of developmental progress in spontaneous language
samples: mean length of utterance; number of different words and total
number of words in a given time (Leadholm and Miller, 1995; Miller,
1991). The fact that narrative ability has good normative data available
means that it should be an ideal source of comparison for children in whom
language impairment is evident or suspected. It should be possible to directly
compare childrens skill at a number of different aspects of language such as
grammatical measures, uency, story structure and even pragmatics. Furthermore, narratives given by typically developing children are also thought to
correspond closely to skills involved in every-day conversation (Preece, 1987).
This base of normative data available for narrative means that clinicians can
refer performance in particular areas of language to that seen in typically
developing peers and can track the progress of individuals undergoing therapy
in comparison to normal developmental patterns.
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

2) Narrative and literacy skills. Oral narrative and literacy abilities are
closely interwoven skills. The latter develops later than the rst with written
narratives being estimated at around 60% the length of their oral counterparts
(Gillam and Johnston, 1992). Other recent studies also emphasize the
association between oral language and literacy skills and have pointed to
interesting mechanisms for the poor literacy levels often reported in children
with SLI (e.g., Conti-Ramsden et al., 2001). Kaderavek and Sulzby (2000) for
example, report that oral narratives using a familiar storybook as a prompt (a
task they describe as emergent story reading) given by typically developing
pre-school children were more complex and showed more of the devices used
in written language than the narratives given by peers with language impairment. Thus, a failure to gain early emergent reading skills may limit children
with language impairment. Cain and Oakhill (1996) found that reading
comprehension skill was also linked to story telling abilities in a group of
normally developing children with poor or strong comprehension skills. In
particular, those with poor comprehension skills also produced poor narratives
with less story structure. These authors conclude that poor story knowledge
(i.e., knowledge about how a complete story might look) is more likely a cause
than a result of poor reading comprehension. Oral narrative may also serve to
inform clinicians about the risk of literacy problems and give insights into the
ways in which therapy may aid development of this skill.

3) Using narrative to distinguish children with communication dif culties


from peers. Children with language impairments (speci c or as part of other
disorders) have been shown to produce poor narratives both when retelling
stories and when generating them (Merritt and Liles, 1987; Tager-Flusberg,
1995; Van der Lely, 1997; Liles et al., 1995). These individuals are sometimes
reported to produce narratives similar to those generated by younger normally
developing children, and this is often the reported impression of every-day
language.
Furthermore, there is a growing acceptance that language impairments are
not as short-term in nature as previously thought. Recent research has shown
that over 88% of children identi ed as having SLI at 7 years, still have
communication dif culties at age 11 (Conti-Ramsden et al., 2001). Many
formal tests of language used with this population are not designed to measure
the abilities of children past primary school age (11 years and older). In
addition, Bishop and Edmundsons (1987) study highlighted oral narratives of
the retelling type as a powerful predictor of longer-term language skill. As
children with SLI grow older, participate in more therapy and in turn develop
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

more strategies for coping with communication, different forms of narrative


task may also become useful in the assessment of language skill.
Finally, one of the main dif culties in assessing conversational and pragmatic language is that the pragmatically inappropriate events themselves are
both subtle and rare despite their traumatic effect on communication. Thus
studies seeking to identify speci c errors made by children with communication dif culties have proven to be complex (Bishop and Adams, 1989). In free
speech, it is notoriously dif cult to assess the roles of the researcher and child.
For example, the degree to which the adult scaffolds the conversation for the
child, and how much of the childs conversation is learned script are
important factors. An assessment tool is needed that is structured enough to
form the basis of a comparison between children, but which is naturalistic
enough to re ect conversational style and the errors within this. In addition the
task must be novel to the child and open to differing conditions (such as
retold or generated stories). Narratives of varying kinds may be a promising
paradigm in this sense.
Aims of the present article
The present article therefore has two aims. Firstly, to discuss the bene ts of
using narrative in a more precise and quantitative way and to encourage
practitioners to use this source of semi-structured language use to aid accurate
description of childrens dif culties, especially those with PLI. Secondly, to
highlight the possibility of using narrative to differentiate between various
similar and overlapping impairment patterns, namely those of speci c
language impairment (SLI), pragmatic language impairment (PLI) and autistic
spectrum disorders (ASD). This latter theme develops further the possible
relationships between narrative style and pragmatic competence. Data from
a pilot study will be presented throughout as an example of the ways in
which narrative may be examined. Details of this preliminary analysis are
described rst.

Details of illustrative data


Participants and measures describing group characteristics
This study comprised two clinical groups taken from participants involved
in a wider study reported elsewhere (Conti-Ramsden et al., 1997; Botting
et al., 1998) and who also took part in the study by Bishop (1998)
examining properties of the Childrens Communication Checklist (see below
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

for description). The groups were: ve children with the most common form
of SLI as found in language units (that is sentence level expressive and
receptive dif culties, see Conti-Ramsden et al., 1997) who had narrative data
available and Childrens Communication Checklist (CCC) scores above 132
(i.e., did not have pragmatic dif culties; see Bishop, 1998); and ve children
with relatively few expressive language dif culties but primary pragmatic
language impairments, all having CCC scores of 132 and below. These were
the only ve out of the 10 children from Botting and Conti-Ramsden (1999)
who were available to complete further narrative tasks. All childrens pragmatic impairment scores from the CCC were also used to explore the
relationship between narrative ability and pragmatic dif culty. This checklist
is completed by the childs teacher or therapist and consists of a number of
statements about communication and behaviour. The respondent must say
whether the behaviour de nitely applies, applies somewhat or does not
apply to that child. There are eight subscales, ve of which make up a scale of
pragmatic impairment. A score of 132 or less on this scale represents clinical
dif culty with social communication. The checklist is described more fully in
Botting and Conti-Ramsden (1999) and Bishop (1998).
The ages of the ve children with PLI (one of whom was female) ranged
from 7 years, 7 months to 8 years, 8 months. The ve comparison children
with SLI were matched for gender and age (range from 7 years, 7 months to
8 years, 1 month). In addition to the narrative stimuli which are described
shortly, children were assessed using a receptive language test (the Test for
Reception of Grammar, (TROG) Bishop, 1982); An expressive language
measure (ITPA Grammatic Closure subtest, Kirk et al., 1968); an expressive
vocabulary subtest (British Ability Scales (BAS) Naming Vocabulary, Elliot,
1983) and a non-verbal abilities test (Ravens Coloured Matrices; Raven,
1986). The BAS Single Word Reading subtest was also used. All children in
both groups had normal non-verbal intelligence scores as measured by Ravens
coloured progressive matrices (Raven, 1986) and borderline=low comprehension scores as measured by the Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop, 1982).
There were no signi cant differences between the groups on these measures
although there was a tendency for the children with PLI to have higher nonverbal scores. A signi cant difference was seen on word reading ability (Mann
Whitney exact P = 0.03) with the group with PLI showing superior performance. Table 1 shows a summary of the childrens characteristics including
some other measures detailed later, and (for all but one) values are given in
centiles for age where <15.9 is equivalent to <1 SD and 50 is the theoretical
population mean.

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool
Table 1

Non-verbal ability and language characteristics

Child Age=sex Ravens TROG BAS Naming BAS Single


ITPA Grammatic
centile centile vocabulary
word reading Closure Standard
centile
centile
score*
PLI1
PLI2
PLI3
PLI4
PLI5
SLI1
SLI2
SLI3
SLI4
SLI5

7;7=m
7;8=f
7;9=m
8;7=m
8;8=m
7;11=m
8;0=f
7;8=m
7;10=m
8;1=m

16
25
92.5
97.5
92.5
37.5
82.5
82.5
62.5
62.5

3
17.5
37.5
25
1
25
10
17.5
5
50

9
77
48
48
13
73
7
30
9
48

38
50
46
83
10
3
28
1
4
21

32
29
37
38
11
32
28
30
24
44

*Normative mean = 36, SD = 6.

Narrative measures
Retelling: The Bus Story (Renfrew, 1991). This is a standardized test used
frequently by speech-language therapists in the UK which consists of a story
book with pictures and no written words. Children are told the story orally by
the researcher whilst looking at each picture. The children are requested to
retell the story as close to the original as possible using the pictures as
prompts. The amount of original information included, the number of
subordinate clauses compared to mean for age and the mean sentence
length of the longest ve sentences compared to mean for age are recorded.
Generative: The Frog Story (Frog, where are you?; Mayer, 1969). This is
a 24-picture story book without words. In this study, the child was asked to
look through the pages of the book and then to tell the story spontaneously
from the beginning in whatever way he=she wants, using the pictures provided
as prompts. Berman and Slobin (1994) have reported a series of studies using
this assessment, and its episodic structure has been extensively examined in
normally developing childrens narratives (Bamberg, 1987; Bamberg and
Marchman, 1990; Bamberg and Damrad-Frye, 1991) and in clinical populations (Tager-Flusberg, 1995; Van der Lely, 1997).
Analysis
Length and narrative devices. Narratives were rst analysed for overall
linguistic differences. Variables for length using both number of words and
number of propositions (that is complete phrase structures with at least a noun
and verb present) were examined. The number of tense marking errors was
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

noted for each childs narrative as this is also a good indicator of linguistic
limitation. The stories were also analysed in terms of the number of narrative
devices used based on Bamberg and Damrad-Fryes (1991) ve category
system: Frames of Mind (referred to here as mentalizing terms), Negatives,
Causatives, Hedges (such as might, perhaps) and character speech (number
of actual words spoken by a character).
Through other narrative devices, story telling may be able to provide
insights into a childs understanding of others and their ability to use
appropriate mentalizing terms. Therefore, in our analysis two further categories were used: affective enhancers and socio-cognitive enhancers, following
Tager-Flusberg (1995). The rst of these refers to emotional terms such as felt
scared, lonely, angry; character speech; sound effects; audience hookers
such as suddenly; and emphatic markers such as really, really slowly.
Socio-cognitive enhancers refers to mentalizing terms such as thought,
remembered, knew; negatives such as the boy wasnt asleep; causal
statements; and inferences where the narrative gives information not directly
given in the pictures. Finally, the number of time references were also recorded
for this preliminary data.

Story structure. The overall story-telling style was assessed using TagerFlusbergs (1995) scheme. This assessed whether each child had included a
formal opening, orientation to characters=setting, explicit mention of the
theme (i.e., looking for the frog), a resolution and a formal ending. These
were also summed to give an overall story structure score for each child.
The analyses described thus far form the data base of the illustrative data. In
addition, data from Tager-Flusbergs (1995) study using the Frog Story is
discussed in relation to the SLI and PLI ndings. Her study involved children
with autism as well as a normally developing group and children with global
learning dif culties.

General considerations. Because of the small numbers of children in the


analysis, it is largely descriptive in nature. Deploying comparative statistics
would be unwise given the assumptions underlying statistical tests, which
require minimum numbers of participants. In the same way, no signi cance
values have been given for correlations reported, since the small number of
cases mean that even high correlations of around 0.8 do not reach statistical
signi cance. It is therefore more useful for the reader to decide the relative
importance of these values.
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

Results and discussion


Children with language impairments and their peers
So what differences might we see between language impaired children and
their typically developing peers, and which type of narrative is the most useful
for children over 7 years? Recall that for the 8-year-old children assessed as
part of this study, retelling narratives (Bus Story) and generated picture book
narratives (Frog Story) were used. Mean Bus Story Information scores were
found to be in the normal range for both the SLI and PLI groups (28th centile
versus 35th centile for age). In contrast, for both groups, mean number of
subordinate clauses was below the mean for age (2.6 below and 3.6 below
respectively) as was the mean sentence length (1.2 below and 1.8 below).
These examples suggest that basic analysis of information given in retold
narrative is not suf cient to detect language impairment at 8 years while a
more detailed analysis of clauses and length do reveal differing dif culties.
The Frog Story analysis also revealed interesting results. Here the children
with SLI had a mean story length of 221 words (SD = 62) which is somewhat
shorter than expected from a normally developing group (Tager Flusberg,
1995: mean = 284 words, SD = 80). The group with PLI in the present study,
however, created narratives of a length comparable with that of the normally
developing children (mean = 276 words, SD = 166) and this will be discussed
later. Tense marking errors were also evident in the group with PLI (20 errors
total) and those with SLI (28 errors total). Studies have shown in more formal
testing conditions that by 11 years normally developing children are virtually
error free with regard to this type of morphology (Simkin and Conti-Ramsden,
2001). Table 2 shows key linguistic results. Furthermore, in this group of
children with typical SLI, narrative tense marking errors were associated
strongly with formal language tests including grammar comprehension
(TROG; r = 0.9); grammar production (ITPA Grammatic closure;
r = 0.9); expressive vocabulary (BAS Naming skills; r = 0.9). Thus,
Table 2

Key linguistic group data (means)

Word
length
Bus

Word
length
Frog

Tense
errors
Bus

Tense
errors
Frog

Bus
story
info

Bus
story
sub.cl.

Sociocognitive
devices

Affective
devices

SLI

151

221

28

28th
centile

7.6

22

PLI

137

276

3.6

20

35th
centile

2.6
(from
pop.mean)
3.6
(from
pop.mean)

13.4

51

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

10

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

tense marking is another valuable area of assessment from narrative, and may
prove to be a useful alternative to formal testing, especially if the child is
regularly assessed over a long period of time, to avoid testretest issues. In
addition, some older children with SLI manage to recall correct answers in
testing situations, but given the task of generating narrative, are unable to
apply this knowledge. It is worth noting here that children with PLI were far
from competent in their linguistic knowledge in both types of narrative, but
that their tense marking errors were not related to language test results.

How did narratives relate to each other?


Interestingly Bus Story word lengths proved shorter for both groups than for
the Frog Story (151 versus 221 for SLI; 137 versus 276 for PLI) and the
difference was much larger for those with PLI. This is somewhat surprising
since one might suppose that a more rigid language style thought to typify
PLI would mean more dif culties with the generative condition. This nding
may also represent another difference between those with PLI and those with
autism, but no data exist to compare the two narratives in this population.
Tense marking errors were fewer for the Bus Story as expected, since it had
been modelled correctly. Both word length and tense marking showed
moderate correlations across story conditions (length r = 0.56; tense
r = 0.44).
In addition, for the SLI group, bus story information score was highly
correlated with a number of devices in the frog story: tense errors (r = 0.87),
use of sound effects (r = 0.8), use of emphatic devices (r = 0.7), use of
negatives (r = 0.7) and story orientation (r = 0.7). These results suggest that
sound effects, emphatics and negatives may be used as alternative devices for
children who cannot rely on working memory processes. The better children
recalled the Bus Story, the fewer linguistic errors and devices were present
during the Frog Story and a better story structure was also evident. To hold a
story structure in mind whilst producing the necessary linguistic structures
may require a higher degree of working memory capacity, compared with the
more immediate story telling effects created by devices such as emphatics
which presumably require little memory input. Many recent studies have
suggested that children with SLI have poorer working memory skills (EllisWeismer et al., 1999) and this may limit the ability to form a coherent story
narrative in both retelling and generative conditions. Instead, these children
may be able to make the story interesting and exciting by using these other
devices. Alternatively, these types of language may serve to buy the child
time while s/he processes the next steps of the story. For the group with PLI,
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

the light of other work. Recent theories regarding temporal processing in


children with PLI and evidence concerning their apparent lack of self imposed
structure in conversation might predict the opposite (Boucher, in press). It is
possible that an inherent lack of understanding about subtle types of turntaking and sequence mean that these often highly intelligent children are using

14

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

wider sample, and in what ways it relates to every-day conversation with these
children. It may be that children with pragmatic language impairment actually
pretend to be other characters whilst they are speaking to others. This
speculation would explain why roles are often confused and why these
children sometimes seem to assign themselves incorrectly in conversation
(for example talking to the headteacher as another teacher might). Given the
limited generative ability of children with autism, and the dif culty often
experienced with pretend play and role taking generally, it would intuitively
seem unlikely that this result would be found in a group with ASD. Indeed
Tager-Flusberg (1995) reported a mean use of character speech of 4.2 in her
group with autism much lower than the two groups described here.
Relationship of narrative to pragmatic ability. An interesting possible
feature of narrative for clinical use may be its relationship to pragmatic ability.
The analysis of this illustrative sample revealed that the two factors may not
only be related, but that this relationship may differ across clinical groups.
Table 4 shows raw data for pragmatic scores at age 8 for children with PLI,
alongside various narrative factors. Spearman rank correlations are shown,
which because of the small data set only reach statistical signi cance when
near perfect. Nevertheless, several features show a clear relationship with
pragmatic skill including mentalizing terms, story length and social-cognitive
enhancers. The less pragmatic impairment, the greater the use of such devices.
Surprisingly, this relationship is reversed when examining the group with
SLI. Here pragmatic score is inversely related to factors such as frames of
mind (correlations also shown in Table 4). Thus the better the pragmatic skill,
the fewer frames of mind are included. This may be due to a dual mechanism
effect between pragmatic skills and linguistic skills. That is, in children with
PLI it may be that linguistic devices are not used largely because of sociocognitive limitations despite suf cient linguistic skill, whilst for the children
with SLI mentalizing terms may not be present because of linguistic impairment, despite suf cient understanding of mind and of mentalizing terms.
These differences may prove important guiding features in the assessment and
description of children with communication dif culties. This suggestion is
supported by Tager-Flusbergs (1995) similar preliminary conclusion from
data showing that two autistic children with better narratives also passed false
belief tasks and by Capps et al., (2000) who also found an association between
theory of mind, social competence and narrative in children with autism. This
latter study also failed to nd the same associations in a group with general
developmental delay. Thus for children with PLI, poor socio-cognitive skill
may prove the limiting mechanism. For children with SLI, linguistic skill may
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Relationship between pragmatic score (CCC) and narrative devices

132
130
119
111
107

559
1
261
1
207
0
228
0
125
0
0.9*
0.87
0.6 0.22

66
98
11
23
18
0.6
0.8

8
7
3
1
2
0.90*
0.95*

2
2
0
0
0
0.87
0.58

*Signi cance at < 0.05. Inverse correlations in bold type.

1
2
3
4
5
Corr.
SLI r

4
3
0
0
0
0.89*
0.11

4
3
0
0
1
0.67
0.89*

3
0
0
0
0
0.71
0.35

35
13
7
7
5
0.95*
0.6

85
111
14
24
21
0.6
0.9*

5
3
4
5
3
0.32
0.74

Tense
Audience Emphatic SocialAffective
Story
marking hookers
markers cognitive enhancers structure
errors
enhancers
score

5
5
2
10
4
3
3
1
1
1
0.7
0.87
0.58 0.00

Child
CCC Words Hedges Character Frames Causatives Sound Time
with PLI
speech
Of
effects refs.
mind

Table 4

Narrative as a tool

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

15

16

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

be the central handicap to narrative and for those with autism, both mechanisms may be affected thus producing the poorest narrative style.
It is acknowledged that the numbers used in this illustrative analysis are too
small to draw rm conclusions, but the striking correlations in each clinical
group seem to suggest that the notion of a dissociation between pragmatic and
linguistic skill warrants further study. It is a widely held belief in the United
States, for example, that pragmatic impairment is very likely to be the result of
poor linguistic ability (Leonard, 1998). In addition there is much confusion
about the level of language impairment allowed within a diagnosis of
Aspergers disorder (Attwood, 1998). It may be that this latter problem can
be untangled by considering the different mechanisms behind communicative
dif culties. For example, a hypothetical ideal assessment of pragmatic
ability, considering the relationship with both linguistic and meta-cognitive
limitations, may be able to reveal that individuals with Aspergers Syndrome
have pragmatic language dif culties associated with social cognition. Conversely, peers with other types of language impairment might show dif culties
relating to linguistic knowledge. In theory, this would allow much more
satisfactory description and remediation of young children with a number of
different communication problems and also warrants deeper investigation.
In general, the illustrative and exploratory analysis used as an example here
seems to support previous work suggesting that children with PLI are not best
described as having speci c language impairment but also suggests that they
exhibit important differences in narrative style in relation to peers with autism
(Botting and Conti-Ramsden, 1999; Conti-Ramsden and Botting, 1999;
Bishop, 1998).

Concluding remarks
In work with clinical populations, the study of spontaneous communication in
the form of conversational analysis has not always proven as useful as one
might have hoped (e.g., Bishop and Adams, 1989). This is partly because it is
dif cult to control for the competent conversational partner (the researcher)
who often unwittingly eases the pragmatic burden for the child being studied.
Furthermore judgements about what constitutes an appropriate or inappropriate contribution are more problematic in naturalistic conversational settings.
On the other hand, in this article it is argued that the generation of stories from
pictures may provide an ideal mixture of structured but imaginative communication. Thus, one implication from this study is that narrative needs to be
used more regularly as an assessment tool in clinical practice. The Frog Story
used here would be a good choice for clinicians wishing to use it in this way
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

17

because of its widespread use in narrative, normative and cross-linguistic


research (e.g., Berman and Slobin, 1994). In addition, this study adds to recent
research suggesting that there is no clear divide between language impairment
and pervasive developmental disorder, and that children exist with pragmatic
impairments who are similar to, but not the same as, peers with autism. It may
therefore bene t practitioners and researchers to examine narrative in combination with pragmatic skills using tools such as the CCC.
Future practice and research need to work hand in hand to answer the
many persisting questions about pragmatic disabilities. Increased use of
pragmatic checklists such as the CCC would enable research to identify the
development of pragmatic impairment and to map out its association with both
narrative measures and more formal assessment tools. In turn, research needs
to identify clinically useful norms for differing types of narrative assessment,
especially forms that may be useful after the infant years and which may be
able to identify different clinical subgroups. Through these methods the
different possible mechanisms underlying communicative competence might
also be considered.

Acknowledgements
This article was undertaken as part ful lment of a post-doctoral Simon
Fellowship (part-time) awarded to the author by the University of Manchester.
Many thanks also to the Nuf eld Foundation whose generous funding also
made this research possible (grant DIR=28), to Gina Conti-Ramsden for
looking at earlier drafts of this article, and to the families and children who
participated in the study.

References
Attwood, T. 1998: Aspergers syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Bamberg, M. 1987: The acquisition of narratives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bamberg, M. and Damrad-Frye, R. 1991: On the ability to provide evaluative
comments: further explorations of childrens narrative competencies.
Journal of Child Language 18, 689710.
Bamberg, M. and Marchman, V. 1990: What holds a narrative together? The
linguistic encoding of episodic boundaries. Papers in Pragmatics 4, 58
121.
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

18

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

Berman, R. A. and Slobin, D. I. 1994: Relating events in narrative: a


crosslinguistic developmental study. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Bishop, D. V. M. 1998: Development of the Childrens Communication
Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry 39, 87993.
Bishop, D. V. M. 1982: Test for reception of grammar. Published by author at
Manchester University.
Bishop, D. V. M. and Adams, C. 1989: Conversational characteristics of
children with semantic-pragmatic disorder: II. What features lead to a
judgement of inappropriacy? British Journal of Disorders of Communication 24, 24163.
Bishop, D. V. M. 1989: Autism, Aspergers syndrome and semantic-pragmatic
disorder: where are the boundaries? British Journal of Disorders of
Communication 24, 10721.
Bishop, D. V. M. and Edmundson, A. 1987: Speci c language impairment as a
maturational lag: evidence from longitudinal date on language and motor
development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 29, 44259.
Bishop, D. V. M. and Rosenbloom, L. 1987: Classi cation of childhood
language disorders. In Yule, W. and Rutter, M., editors, Language
development and disorders. Clinics in developmental medicine, numbers
101102. London: MacKeith Press, 1641.
Botting, N. and Conti-Ramsden, G. 1999: Pragmatic language impairment
without autism: the children in question. Autism 3, 37196.
Botting, N. 1998: Semantic-pragmatic disorder as a distinct diagnostic entity:
Making sense of the boundaries (a comment on Boucher). International
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 33, 71108.
Botting, N., Crutchley, A. and Conti-Ramsden, G. 1998: Educational transitions of 7-year-old children with SLI in language units: a longitudinal
study. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders
33, 17797.
Boucher, J. (in press). Time parsing, normal language acquisition, and
language-related developmental disorders. In Perkins, M. and Howard,
S., editors, New directions in research into language development and
disorders. London: Plenum Press.
Boucher, J. 1998: SPD as a distinct diagnostic entity: Logical considerations
and directions for future research. International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders 33, 7181.

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

19

Brook, S. and Bowler, D. 1992: Autism by another name? Semantic and


pragmatic impairments in children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 22, 6181.
Cain, K. and Oakhill, J. 1996: The nature of the relationship between
comprehension skill and the ability to tell a story. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology 14, 187201.
Capps, L., Losh, M. and Thurber, C. 2000: The frog ate the bug and made his
mouth sad: narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology 28, 193204.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Simkin, Z. and Knox, E. 2001: Follow-up of
children attending infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age.
International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 36,
20719.
Conti-Ramsden, G. and Botting, N. 1999: Classi cation of children with
speci c language impairment: longitudinal considerations. Journal of
Speech, Language and Hearing Research 42, 1195204.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A. and Botting, N. 1997: The extent to which
psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with speci c
language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing
Research 40, 76577.
Dunn, M., Flax, J., Sliwinski, M. and Aram, D. 1996: The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria for identifying children with
speci c language impairment: an attempt to reconcile clinical and
research incongruence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39,
64354.
Eaton, J. H., Collis, G. M. and Lewis, V. A. 1999: Evaluative explanations in
childrens narratives of a video sequence without dialogue. Journal of
Child Language 26, 699720.
Elliot, C. D. 1983: British ability scales. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
Ellis-Weismer, S., Evans, J. and Hesketh, L. J. 1999: An examination of
verbal working memory capacity in children with speci c language
impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 42,
124960.
Gagnon, L., Mottron, L. and Joanette Y. 1997: Questioning the validity of the
semantic-pragmatic syndrome diagnosis. Autism 1, 3755.
Gillam, R. and Johnston, J. 1992: Spoken and written language relationships
in language=learning impaired and normally achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35, 130315.

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

20

Child Language Teaching and Therapy

Hohnen, B. and Stevenson, J. 1999: The genetic in uences on general


cognitive ability, language, phonological and reading abilities. Developmental Psychology 35, 590603.
Kaderavek, J. N. and Sulzby, E. 2000: Narrative production by children with
and without speci c language impairment: oral narratives and emergent
readings. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 43, 3449.
Kerbel, D. and Grunwell, P. 1998: A study of idiom comprehension in children
with semantic-pragmatic dif culties. Part II: Between group results and
discussion. International Journal of Language and Communication
Disorders 33, 2344.
Kirk, S. A., McCarthy, J. J., and Kirk, W. D. 1968: Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities. Illinois, USA: University of Illinois.
Leadholm, B. J. and Miller, J. 1995: Language sample analysis: The
Wisconsin guide. Wisconsin: Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction.
Leonard, L. 1998: Children with speci c language impairment. London: MIT
Press.
Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D. D. and Purcell, S. L. 1995: Measurement
of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. Journal
of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 41525.
Mayer, M. 1969: Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.
McTear, M. and Conti-Ramsden, G. 1992: Pragmatic disability in children.
London: Whurr.
Merritt, D. D. and Liles, B. Z. 1987: Story grammar ability in children with
and without language disorder: story generation, story retelling and story
comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30, 53952.
Miller, J. 1991: Quantifying productive language disorder. In Miller, J., editor,
Research on child language disorders: a decade of progress. Austin, TX:
Pro-ed, 21120.
Peterson, C. and McCabe, A. 1983: Developmental psycholinguistics: three
ways of looking at a childs narrative. New York: Plenum Press.
Peterson, C. 1990: The who, when and where of early narratives. Journal of
Child Language 17, 43355.
Preece, A. (1987) The range of narrative forms conversationally produced by
young children. Journal of Child Language 14, 35373.
Rapin, I. and Allen, D. 1998: The semantic-pragmatic de cit disorder:
classi cation issues. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 33, 71108.
Rapin, I. 1996: Developmental language disorders: a clinical update. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37, 64356.
Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Narrative as a tool

21

Rapin, I. and Allen, D. 1983: Developmental language disorders: Nosologic


considerations. In Kirk, U., editor, Neuropsychology of language, reading
and spelling. New York: Academic Press.
Raven, J. C. 1986: Coloured progressive matrices. London: HK Lewis.
Renfrew, C. 1991: The Bus Story: A test of continuous speech. Published by
author in Oxford.
Scott C. M. and Winsdor, J. 2000: General language performance measures in
spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-age
children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech Language
and Hearing Research 43, 32439.
Shapiro, L. R. and Hudson, J. A. 1991: Tell me a make-believe story:
coherence and cohesion in young childrens picture-elicited narratives.
Development Psychology 27, 96074.
Simkin, Z. and Conti-Ramsden, G. 2001: Non-word repletion and grammatical morphology: normative data for children in their nal year of
primary school. International Journal of Language and Communication
Disorders 36, 395404.
Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B. and
Kaplan, C. A. 1998: Language impaired preschoolers: a follow up into
adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 41,
40718.
Tager-Flusberg, H. 1995: Once upon a ribbit: stories narrated by autistic
children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13, 4559.
Taylor, R. 2000: Semantic-pragmatic disorder: How unitary is it? PhD thesis:
University of Shef eld.
Van der Lely, H. J. K. 1997: Narrative discourse in grammatical speci c
language impaired children: a modular de cit? Journal of Child
Language 24, 22156.

Downloaded from clt.sagepub.com at American University in Cairo on November 5, 2015

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi