Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 12

Brain Imaging and Behavior

DOI 10.1007/s11682-017-9678-y

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Local resting state functional connectivity in autism: site


and cohort variability and the effect of eye status
Sangeeta Nair 1 & R. Joanne Jao Keehn 1 & Michael M. Berkebile 1 & Jos Omar Maximo 1,2 &
Natalia Witkowska 1 & Ralph-Axel Mller 1,3

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

A b s t r a c t A u t i s m s p e c t r u m d i s o rd e r (A S D ) i s a However, there was gross consistency across all eyes-open


neurodevelopmental disorder with prominent impairments in samples of local overconnectivity (ASD > TD) in posterior,
sociocommunicative abilities, which have been linked to visual regions. There was also gross consistency of local
anomalous brain network organization. Despite ample evi- underconnectivity (ASD < TD) in cingulate gyrus, although
dence of atypical long-distance connectivity, the literature on exact loci varied between mid/posterior and anterior sections.
local connectivity remains small and divergent. We used While all eyes-open datasets showed the described gross sim-
resting-state functional MRI regional homogeneity (ReHo) ilarities, the pattern of group differences for participants
as a local connectivity measure in comparative analyses across scanned with eyes closed was different, with local
several well-matched low-motion subsamples from the overconnectivity in ASD in posterior cingulate gyrus, but
Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange and in-house data, with underconnectivity in some visual regions. Our findings sug-
a grand total of 147 ASD and 184 typically developing (TD) gest that fMRI local connectivity measures may be relatively
participants, ages 718 years. We tested for group differences susceptible to site and cohort variability and that some previ-
in each subsample, with additional focus on the difference ous inconsistencies in the ASD ReHo literature may be rec-
between eyes-open and eyes-closed resting states. Despite se- onciled by more careful consideration of eye status.
lection of highest quality data and tight demographic and mo-
tion matching between groups and across samples, few effects Keywords Autism spectrum disorder . Functional MRI .
in exactly identical loci (voxels) were found across samples. Local connectivity . Cingulate gyrus . Visual cortex . Default
mode network

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article


(doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9678-y) contains supplementary material, Introduction
which is available to authorized users.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a set of complex
* Ralph-Axel Mller neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by impaired so-
rmueller@sdsu.edu
cial communication and interaction, as well as repetitive be-
1
haviors and restricted interests. After decades of neuroimaging
Brain Development Imaging Laboratory, Department of Psychology,
San Diego State University, 6363 Alvarado Ct., Suite 200, San
research, biomarkers with high degrees of sensitivity and
Diego, CA 92120, USA specificity (brain features occurring in all or most individuals
2
Department of Psychology, University of Alabama,
with ASD, but not in those without ASD) remain elusive.
Birmingham, AL, USA Reasons include the neurofunctional complexity and hetero-
3
Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego State
geneity of the disorder, with ASD being an umbrella term for
University and University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA, an unknown number of neurodevelopmentally distinct vari-
USA ants (Geschwind and State 2015). An additional problem is
Brain Imaging and Behavior

the lack of established best practices in imaging analysis (e.g., Methods


Koldewyn et al. 2014; Nair et al. 2014).
In the past decade, some broad consensus has emerged Participants
viewing ASD as a disorder of brain network connectivity
(Mller 2007; Vissers et al. 2012; Wass 2011). However, this Data consisted of in-house anatomical and resting state function-
apparent consensus obscures a wide diversity of findings, al MRI (rs-fMRI) scans (labeled BSDSU^), as well as data from
stretching from predominant functional underconnectivity the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE), a grassroots
(Schipul et al. 2011) to mixed patterns reflecting network initiative dedicated to aggregating and sharing previously col-
specificity (Abbott et al. 2015) or age (Nomi and Uddin lected anatomical and rs-fMRI data sets from individuals with
2015) to predominant overconnectivity (Shih et al. 2011; ASD and age-matched TD controls (Di Martino et al. 2014).
Supekar et al. 2013). The vast majority of studies have inves- Six different low-motion datasets were analyzed: a Grand
tigated long distance connectivity, whereas the evidence on Total group including data from participants with eyes open
local connectivity remains limited. The technique of choice and closed during resting state scan from both ABIDE, NYU,
to test local connectivity in functional MRI has been regional and SDSU (Grand Total-EO + EC; N = 331; ASD = 147;
homogeneity (ReHo) a method based on signal correlations TD = 184); a Grand Total group including only data from par-
in small clusters of neighboring voxels that was initially de- ticipants with eyes open (EO) during resting state scan (Grand
veloped for cluster purification (Zang et al. 2004). Two early Total-EO; N = 257; ASD = 110; TD = 147); data from SDSU
ReHo studies of ASD (Paakki et al. 2010; Shukla et al. 2010) including only participants with eyes open during resting state
reported divergent results, presumably due to differences in scan (SDSU-EO; N = 53; ASD = 26; TD = 27); data from New
crucial methodological factors (e.g., resting state vs. York University (NYU) including only participants with eyes
task-activated data, treatment of head motion, size of local open (NYU-EO; N = 63; ASD = 25; TD = 38); data from
clusters tested). However, methodologically more rigorous ABIDE including only participants with eyes open and exclud-
recent studies have also produced partial inconsistencies. ing participants from SDSU and NYU groups (ABIDE-EO;
ReHo findings from the large-sample Autism Brain N = 141; ASD = 59; TD = 82); and data from ABIDE including
Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE) showed extensive local only participants with eyes closed (ABIDE-EC; N = 72;
overconnectivity in ASD in right frontal cortex (Di Martino ASD = 30; TD = 42). Within each subsample, groups were
et al. 2014), whereas studies in smaller samples indicated matched on age, nonverbal IQ, and head motion (root mean
local overconnectivity in posterior regions (Keown et al. square of displacement RMSD; see Table 1). Subsample
2013; Maximo et al. 2013) that was not seen at all in the (ABIDE-EO, ABIDE-EC, NYU, SDSU) by group analyses of
ABIDE study. A more recent study using a subset of variance (ANOVAs) showed marginal effects of subsample for
ABIDE data reported underconnectivity in posterior re- age (F(3323) = 2.341, p = 0.073) and nonverbal IQ
gions with additional age-related changes (Dajani and (F(3287) = 2.659, p = 0.049). While a strong effect of subsam-
Uddin 2015). However, this study applied relatively liberal ple for head motion (F(3323) = 11.420, p < 0.001) was found,
head movement criteria and included resting data from both the mean difference in RMSD was minimal at 0.016 (and did
eyes-open and eyes-closed state, which have been shown to not substantially affect the pattern of results; see BLimitations
be associated with substantial differences in ReHo (Liu and perspectives^ below and Supplementary Fig. 3).
et al. 2013). Additionally, there were no main effects of group on age
Given the inconsistencies in findings, we conducted a com- (F(1323) = 0.291, p = 0.590), nonverbal IQ (F(287) = 0.045,
parative ReHo study implementing several analysis pipelines p = 0.832), or head motion (F(1323) = 0.624, p = 0.430), as well
in a number of samples, including both ABIDE and in-house as no site by group interaction effects. Finally, there was no main
data. Although there are potential sources of variability that effect of subsample on ADOS scores (F(3106) = 1.907,
may affect multi-site reproducibility and reliability (e.g., dif- p = 0.133) in the ASD group.
ferences in pulse sequences, signal-to-noise ratios, smoothing; Diagnoses in the ASD group were based on the Autism
Friedman et al. 2008, 2006), several studies across a wide Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al. 2000)
range of typical and patient populations and in both resting at all sites, and additionally the Autism Diagnostic Interview-
state and task-based fMRI have indicated high inter-site reli- Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al. 1994) at some sites. However,
ability (Brown et al. 2011; Sutton et al. 2008; Turner et al. ADOS data were unavailable for a total of 37 participants (as
2013). The divergent findings from previous ReHo studies of stated in Table 1). Exclusionary criteria for both ASD and TD
ASD therefore call for further investigation. In the present groups varied based on acquisition site (See Supplementary
study, we examined whether inconsistencies could be resolved Table 1 for list of diagnostic and exclusionary criteria per
(i) through use of consistent analysis pipelines and (ii) through acquisition site). All participants and caregivers gave in-
strict separation of data acquired during eyes-open vs. eyes- formed consent in accordance with the responsible
closed conditions. Institutional Review Boards.
Brain Imaging and Behavior

Avg % time pts.


post-censoring
MRI data acquisition

All data were acquired from 3 Tesla scanners, but type of

93.8
94.2

95.5
95.1
92.3

94.3
92.6
92.7

92.9
93.8

93.8
92.9
scanner and image acquisition parameters varied across
ABIDE acquisition sites (see Supplementary Table 2 for sites
Mean (SD), Range

included in subsamples; acquisition protocols available at:


[29 scores missing]

[37 scores missing]


[29 scores missing]
13.62 (4.53), 724
12.44 (4.56), 622

12.27 (4.00), 324


12.56 (4.40), 224
11.26 (3.31), 619
11.88 (4.36), 321

[7 scores missing]
fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/abide/).
ADOS Total

Resting state imaging data for SDSU participants were ac-


quired on a GE 3T MR750 scanner with an eight-channel head
coil at the Center for Functional MRI at the University of
-
-

-
-

-
California, San Diego. High-resolution structural images were
0.050 (0.021), 0.0190.098 acquired with a standard FSPGR T1-weighted sequence (TR:
0.045 (0.012), 0.0240.067

0.051 (0.024), 0.0170.095


0.045 (0.012), 0.0260.073
0.049 (0.015), 0.0220.087
0.049 (0.014), 0.0240.104

0.052 (0.017), 0.0190.098


0.052 (0.018), 0.0160.119
0.059 (0.02), 0.0260.107
0.058 (0.016), 0.0290.09

0.048 (.016), 0.0190.098


0.048 (.016), 0.0170.119
t(139) = 0.032, p = 0.974

t(329) = 0.043, p = 0.965


t(255) = 0.086, p = 0.932
t(70) = 0.043, p = 0.966

t(61) = 0.052, p = 0.959

t(51) = 0.027, p = 0.979 11.08 ms; TE: 4.3 ms; flip angle: 45; FOV: 256 mm; matrix:
Mean (SD), Range;

256 256; 180 slices; 1mm3 resolution). Functional T2-


weighted images were obtained in a 6:10-min scan using a
t-test, p-value

single-shot gradient-recalled, echo-planar pulse sequence


RMSD

consisting of 185 whole-brain volumes (TR: 2000 ms; TE:


30 ms; slice thickness: 3.4 mm; flip angle: 90; FOV:
220 mm; matrix: 64 64). The first five time points were
t(293) = 0.105, p = 0.916
t(224) = 0.015, p = 0.988

discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects, leaving 180


t(61) = 0.055, p = 0.956
t(70) = 0.016, p = 0.988

t(51) = 0.017, p = 0.863


t(110) = 0.04, p = 0.968

106.04 (18.47), 53140


107.45 (13.21), 72137

107.76 (17.04), 72149

106.89 (17.19), 53136


102.32 (12.58), 76127

104.92 (15.13), 53136

104.51 (16.65), 53140


104.89 (17.27), 53149

104.70 (14.11), 67137


107.4 (14.87), 83129
102.43 (16.8), 64137

time points (6 min) for analysis.


Mean (SD), Range;

108 (16.86), 67135

Resting state imaging data for NYU participants were


t-test, p-value

acquired on a Siemens 3T MR2004A scanner with an eight-


channel head coil. High-resolution structural images were
acquired with a T1-weighted sequence (TR: 2530 ms; TE:
IQ

3.25 ms; flip angle: 7; FOV: 256 mm; matrix: 256 256; 128
slices; 1.3mm3 resolution). Functional T2-weighted images were
t(139) = 0.048, p = 0.962

t(329) = 0.037, p = 0.970


t(255) = 0.174, p = 0.862
12.08 (2.53), 8.5117.88
13.33 (2.55), 7.1517.17

13.46 (2.63), 7.1517.94


t(61) = 0.067, p = 0.947
t(70) = 0.031, p = 0.976

t(51) = 0.162, p = 0.872


12.13 (2.76), 6.4717.7

13.47 (2.64), 8.0117.9

obtained in a 6 min scan (TR: 2000 ms; TE: 15 ms; slice thick-
13.93 (2.43), 9.217.7
13.83 (2.26), 8.717.6
13.34 (2.4), 7.2617.5
13.7 (2.67), 8.3917.9
Mean (SD), Range;

13.67 (2.6), 817.94

13.3 (2.7), 6.517.9

ness: 4 mm; flip angle: 90; FOV: 240 mm; matrix: 240 240).
13.4 (2.6), 817.9
t-test, p-value*

Data preprocessing
Age

All resting state fMRI data were processed using Analysis of


Functional NeuroImages Software (AFNI; Cox 1996) and
16 Female
30 Female
12 Female

36 Female
14 Female

11 Female

FMRI software library (FSL; Smith et al. 2004). Functional


3 Female
3 Female
3 Female

5 Female
6 Female

6 Female

*Two-sample t-test comparing ASD and TD groups in each sample

images were motion, slice-time, and field map corrected.


Sex

Functional images were registered to the anatomical images


using FSLs FLIRT (Jenkinson et al. 2002), and images were
% eyes open

resampled to 3 mm isotropic voxels and standardized to the


MNI152 template atlas space via FSLs nonlinear registration
79.6
79.9
100
100

tool (FNIRT). FMRI time series were bandpass filtered


100
100

100

100
100
100

0
0

(0.008 < f < 0.08 Hz) to isolate spontaneous low-frequency


BOLD fluctuations using a second-order Butterworth filter.
147

147
184
110
26
30

25
59

27
42

38
82
N
Participant demographics

As differences in smoothness between single-subject datasets


would directly impact time series correlations between neigh-
Group

ASD
ASD

ASD
ASD

ASD

ASD
TD

TD

TD

TD
TD
TD

boring voxels, we consistently set the effective smoothness of


all datasets to a Gaussian FWHM of 6 mm, using AFNIs
Grand Total- EO + EC

3dBlurToFWHM. All nuisance regressors were band-pass fil-


t e r e d u s i n g t h e s e c o n d - o r d e r B u t te r w o r t h f i l t e r
Grand Total-EO

(0.008 < f < 0.08 Hz; Power et al. 2014; Satterthwaite et al.
ABIDE-EO

ABIDE-EC

SDSU-EO

2013). A total of 16 nuisance regressors were applied, includ-


NYU-EO
Table 1

Sample

ing six rigid-body motion parameters acquired from motion


correction and their first derivatives, as well as signal from
Brain Imaging and Behavior

cerebral white matter and lateral ventricles, and their deriva- Analyses were also performed both with and without global
tives. Individual participant masks were created to remove signal regression (GSR), given the ongoing debate highlighting
signal from cerebral white matter and lateral ventricles using both the virtues of GSR in denoising (Power et al. 2014) and its
FSLs FAST automated segmentation (Zhang et al. 2001). potential drawbacks, such as the creation of spurious anti-
Motion was quantified as framewise displacement (FD) correlations (Murphy et al. 2009), which may confound
between two consecutive time points (calculated based on six between-group comparisons (Abbott et al. 2015; Gotts et al.
dimensional rigid-body motion parameters). Any time point 2013). Findings from these additional analyses are presented
with FD > 0.2 mm as well as the following two time points in Supplementary Figs. 1 and 2 for completeness.
were censored, or Bscrubbed^ (Power et al. 2014). Participants
with fewer than 80% of time points remaining after censoring
were excluded from analyses. Average head motion, defined as Results
the root mean square of displacement (RMSD), did not signifi-
cantly differ between groups (ASD vs. TD). Analyses with GSR and ReHo standardization

Regional homogeneity Between-group effects for the analyses with GSR and ReHo
standardization for each subsample are presented in Fig. 1a
Regional homogeneity implements Kendalls coefficient of con- (for cluster listings see Supplementary Table 3). For the ReHo
cordance (KCC) and relies on rank order correlations between pipeline with GSR, group differences in the ABIDE-EO sub-
time series to assess the homogeneity of a given center voxel sample included local overconnectivity (ASD > TD) in right
and its neighboring voxels. ReHo can thus be considered a middle temporal gyrus, and across left calcarine and bilateral
measure of local connectivity (Anderson et al. 2014; Lopez- lingual gyri, and underconnectivity in bilateral thalamus, right
Larson et al. 2011). KCC within a cluster of voxels is equal to insula and inferior frontal gyrus, and left posterior cingulate
the parameter W, which ranges from 0 to 1, with higher values cortex and precuneus. The ABIDE-EC analysis yielded local
indicating greater homogeneity (Zang et al. 2004): overconnectivity in the ASD group across bilateral precuneus,
 2 left middle temporal gyrus, left superior frontal gyrus, and
Ri 2 n R right angular/middle temporal gyrus, contrasted by local
W underconnectivity across right paracentral and postcentral gy-
1 2 3 
K n n ri, right cuneus and lingual gyrus, right superior and middle
12
occipital gyri, left precentral and postcentral gyri. For the
In a previous study (Maximo et al. 2013), we tested the effect NYU subsample, group differences included local
of varying cluster sizes of 7, 19, and 27 voxels on ReHo in overconnectivity in left calcarine, lingual, and middle tempo-
comparisons between TD and ASD samples, and observed most ral gyri, and local underconnectivity across left superior me-
robust group differences for 27 voxels. For the present study, we dial and mid orbital gyri, left anterior cingulate cortex, right
therefore computed ReHo for a cluster size of 27 voxels. A gray- rolandic operculum, and right superior and middle frontal gy-
matter mask was used to avoid partial-volume effects. Voxel- ri. For the SDSU subsample, local overconnectivity in the
wise ReHo maps were obtained for each participant using ASD groups was detected in left calcarine cortex and cuneus,
AFNIs 3dReHo for a cluster size of 27 voxels. In each partic- right superior and middle temporal gyri, right temporal pole,
ipant, KCC for each voxel was then standardized to a ReHo z- and right amygdala, whereas local underconnectivity occurred
score by subtracting the mean KCC across all brain voxels and in bilateral paracentral as well as anterior and middle cingulate
dividing it by the standard deviation. Between-group differences cortices, right SMA, and left cerebellum. The Grand Total-EO
(ASD vs. TD) were examined with two-sample t-tests. Monte- subsample yielded local overconnectivity in calcarine cortex,
Carlo simulations were applied to correct for multiple compar- lingual gyrus and precuneus bilaterally, right superior tempo-
isons (Forman et al. 1995), using AFNIs 3dClustSim (version ral and supramarginal gyri, and bilateral middle temporal gy-
post May 2015), to obtain a corrected significance level of rus, contrasted by local underconnectivity across bilateral
p < .05 (using a voxelwise threshold of p < .01 uncorrected middle, anterior, and posterior cingulate cortices, bilateral
and a minimum cluster volume of 37 voxels for a corrected thalamus, left cerebellum, and rolandic operculum bilaterally.
threshold at p < .05). The Grand Total-EO + EC ReHo analysis showed local
While KCC standardization, as described above, is a com- overconnectivity in the ASD group across bilateral middle
mon default procedure in the ReHo literature because it reduces temporal gyrus, left calcarine cortex and lingual gyri, right
artifactual effects of individual variability (e.g., differences in cerebellum, left fusiform gyrus, left precuneus, and right
motion across participants), it renders the technique insensitive supramarginal gyrus. Local underconnectivity was detected
to potential global group differences in local connectivity. We across bilateral middle and anterior cingulate cortices, bilateral
therefore also ran parallel analyses without standardization. thalamus, and right rolandic operculum.
Brain Imaging and Behavior

Fig. 1 Surface renderings of regional homogeneity differences between results from pipeline with global signal regression and standardized
ASD and TD groups for each subsample (a), of differences between ReHo are shown (see Supplementary Figure 1 for findings from other
ABIDE eyes open and eyes closed cohorts shown separately for ASD pipelines). The two columns on the left depict the left hemisphere, the two
and TD groups (b), and of cluster overlap between subsamples (c), columns on the right the right hemisphere (p < .05, corrected)
showing only independent subsamples (excluding Grand Total). Only
Brain Imaging and Behavior

Note that Grand Total is included here because it represent- gyri for ABIDE-EC; overconnectivity in bilateral middle tem-
ed the largest available sample, although it should be under- poral gyrus and underconnectivity in thalamus for Grand
stood that this sample was inclusive of other subsamples and Total-EO; overconnectivity in visual regions and
the detected effects thus reflect a summary of those subsam- underconnectivity in thalamus for Grand Total-EO + EC).
ples rather than an independent finding. Analyses comparing ABIDE EO to EC in both ASD and TD
groups yielded similar patterns of local over- and
Analyses without GSR and standardization underconnectivity, albeit to a lesser extent. Nevertheless,
when sites contributing fewer than 10 participants were re-
Results for analyses without GSR and without ReHo stan- moved from multi-site subsamples, largely similar patterns
dardization were mostly similar to those from standardized of effects were retained (Supplementary Fig. 4).
ReHo with global signal regression pipeline, with some ex-
ceptions. For the Grand Total-EO + EC subsample, local
overconnectivity in left middle temporal gyri, left lingual gy- Discussion
rus, and left precuneus, and underconnectivity in bilateral thal-
amus was not seen in the non-GSR non-standardized analysis, Despite relatively large, high-quality subsamples, which were
and overconnectivity in right middle frontal gyrus and matched for head motion and available demographics,
underconnectivity in left rolandic operculum were detected between-group effects differed across datasets, suggesting that
solely in this analysis (see Supplementary Fig. 1a and ReHo may be highly sensitive to site and cohort variability.
Supplementary Table 4 for complete details). Results for anal- Differences in sample size across datasets could explain vari-
yses without GSR but with ReHo standardization were largely ations in overall extent of significant between-group effects,
similar to those without ReHo standardization and are present- which were in fact modest, but not the many differences in
ed in Supplementary Fig. 1b (for cluster listings, see regional patterns of such effects.
Supplementary Table 5). Tw o r e g i o n a l e f f e c t s b o t h r e f l e c t i n g l o c a l
overconnectivity in posterior, visual regions in ASD were
Eye status detected across all samples (except ABIDE-EC) for both the
GSR and the non-GSR pipelines, as well as Grand Total-EO
Two-sample t-tests directly comparing ABIDE eyes open and with additional site and motion covariates. However, the exact
eyes closed subsamples yielded extensive regions of signifi- location of these between-group effects varied between sam-
cantly greater local connectivity for EO compared to EC par- ples (Fig. 1c). Medial clusters peaked around the calcarine
ticipants in occipital regions extending into temporal and pa- fissure (V1) in most samples. Additional posterior clusters
rietal lobes, as well as in mostly ventral frontal and anterior detected in all samples were located in lateral temporo-
cingulate regions. Reduced local connectivity for the same parietal cortex (except for NYU, and for ABIDE-EC with
contrast was seen in mid-cingulate, paracentral, and (mostly added site and motion covariates). A further effect with rela-
medial) temporal regions. These patterns were mostly similar tively high consistency (detected in all samples except NYU)
in ASD and TD groups (see Fig. 1 and Supplementary was local underconnectivity in cingulate cortex. Again, exact
Table 3; for findings from non-GSR and non-standardized locations of this effect varied greatly across samples.
analyses, see Supplementary Fig. 2 and Supplementary Surprisingly, the robust local overconnectivity in right pre-
Tables 4 and 5). frontal cortex reported in the large-sample original ABIDE
study (Di Martino et al. 2014) could not be replicated. A
Analyses with added site and motion regressors few methodological differences may have played a role: Di
Martino and colleagues excluded sites with N < 6, but includ-
To examine effects driven by site-specific sample sizes and ed datasets with < 80% remaining time points after censoring.
motion, two-sample t-tests were calculated with site and head Censoring was only performed on time points of motion peaks
motion (RMSD) as nuisance covariates. These analyses in their study, whereas we additionally censored two subse-
showed similar patterns of effects (Supplementary Fig. 3), quent time points. More importantly, the much wider age
with the exception of ABIDE-EO, which had the lowest ratio range in Di Martino et al. (764 years vs. 718 years in ours)
of participants to number of sites. Specifically, local may account for differences in findings. Related to this, Dajani
overconnectivity (ASD > TD) was found only in right middle and Uddin (2015) reported local overconnectivity effects in
temporal gyrus. For the ABIDE-EC, Grand Total-EO, and right prefrontal cortex that were age-specific. However, their
Grand Total-EO + EC subsamples, results were consistent finding of a small right inferior frontal overconnectivity clus-
with primary analyses with the exception of small clusters of ter only in 711 year-old children with ASD, but neither in
local connectivity that did not survive correction (e.g., local adolescents nor in adults, could not account for the differences
overconnectivity in left middle temporal and superior frontal between our findings and those by Di Martino et al. (2014).
Brain Imaging and Behavior

Note that samples in Dajani and Uddin (2015) were exclusive- we also analyzed all data without any steps that would have
ly drawn from the NYU data in ABIDE, resulting in small resulted in such normalization (i.e., without standardization
subsamples of 20 participants per group in each age bin, and without GSR, which has also been shown to distort group
and that data from eyes open and eyes closed conditions were differences in some instances (Abbott et al. 2015; Gotts et al.
combined in these samples (see below). Itahashi et al. (2015) 2013). Although between-group effects were overall slightly
reported extensive local overconnectivity in right prefrontal less extensive, most regional effects were similar between
cortex in adults with ASD (ages 1950 years), consistent with standardized and non-standardized pipelines. In particular,
the location of findings in Di Martino et al. (2014). Since the even without standardization, there was a mix of local
finding by Itahashi et al. comes from a large and independent under- and overconnectivity effects, indeed with a slight pre-
sample (not included in ABIDE), it may provide a replication dominance of the former. This mixture of connectivity effects
of the original ABIDE findings however, with the potential was also shown in analyses including site and head motion as
caveat that these effects may be found only in adults. If so, this nuisance covariates and excluding sites that contributed fewer
caveat could in turn explain why no corresponding effect was than 10 participants. This corroborates that general local
detected in our study, which was limited to children and ado- overconnectivity in ASD at the coarse spatial scale of fMRI
lescents under the age of 18 years. ReHo does not exist.

General local overconnectivity in ASD is more myth than


fact Cingulate cortex is locally underconnected

Starting with Belmonte et al. (2004) and Just et al. (2004), the Findings of atypically reduced local ReHo in cingulate gyrus
idea of a general principle of atypically reduced long-distance in ASD were relatively consistent across samples, although
connectivity but increased local connectivity in ASD has sur- exact loci differed substantially. Underconnectivity in anterior
vived through repeated cross-citations in many reviews (e.g., cingulate cortex (ACC) was observed in Grand Total as well
Maximo et al. 2014; Minshew and Williams 2007; Vissers as SDSU and NYU eyes open samples (although not in the
et al. 2012; Wass 2011), based on surprisingly slim evidence. ABIDE-EO and EC cohorts). These findings are consistent
The hypothesis of general long-distance underconnectivity in with evidence of reduced gray matter in ACC from voxel-
ASD has been questioned elsewhere (Mller et al. 2011; Nair based morphometry, as reported in a voxelwise meta-
et al. 2014; Rudie and Dapretto 2013; Supekar et al. 2013). analysis (Yang et al. 2016), as well as cytoarchitectonic anom-
Functional MRI ReHo studies, while reporting many often alies in ACC of postmortem brains from children and adults
inconsistent regional findings, have in fact given a clear an- with ASD (Simms et al. 2009; Uppal et al. 2014).
swer to the second part of the claim: There is no general local Reduced distal functional connectivity between medial pre-
overconnectivity in ASD. Instead every single autism ReHo frontal cortex/ACC and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/
study published to date (Dajani and Uddin 2015; Di Martino precuneus is one of the best-replicated findings in ASD
et al. 2014; Itahashi et al. 2015; Jiang et al. 2015; Maximo (Abbott et al. 2015; Assaf et al. 2010; Burrows et al. 2016;
et al. 2013; Paakki et al. 2010; Shukla et al. 2010) has detected Doyle-Thomas et al. 2015; Monk et al. 2009; von dem Hagen
region-specific mixtures of atypically increased and reduced et al. 2013; Washington et al. 2013). In light of the findings by
local connectivity in ASD. Burrows et al. (2016), this underconnectivity can be
However, there are a few caveats to be considered. Aside interpreted as reduced interplay between self-referential and
from the coarse spatial scale at which ReHo fMRI assesses other-referential processing (thinking about oneself vs. think-
local connectivity (see below BLimitations and perspec- ing about other people), which may be considered a funda-
tives^), the ReHo pipeline implemented in almost all of the mental component of social cognition. Moreover, in middle
cited studies included a standardization step, which converts cingulate cortex (MCC), hypothesized to be crucial for cogni-
KCC into z-scores, normalizing effects to a distribution tive processes involved during social exchanges and perspec-
around zero in each participant. While this has advantages tive-taking, differential activity has been shown to be dimin-
with respect to differing data quality and the detection of re- ished in ASD (Lu et al. 2015). Our additional finding of
gionally specific effects, any global differences between a reduced local connectivity both in anterior and more posterior
clinical and a control population will be lost. It is therefore sections of the cingulate gyrus further suggests that reduced
theoretically possible that mixed between-group effects re- interplay between the two zones is accompanied by less coor-
ported in all of the above studies were artifacts of ReHo stan- dinated activity within each of them. Aside from other-refe-
dardization. Maximo et al. (2013), however, showed similar rential thought, PCC is considered to be important for arousal
regional patterns of mixed over- and underconnectivity in and awareness, balance between internal and external atten-
ASD even in the absence of standardization. Nonetheless, tion, and environmental change detection (Leech and Sharp
given the relatively small sample size in this earlier study, 2014).
Brain Imaging and Behavior

The ACC also plays a crucial role as a hub of the salience including some non-visual ones (Jao Keehn et al. 2016; Kana
network (SN), which provides a bridge between cerebral cor- et al. 2006; Shen et al. 2012).
tical networks and limbic and autonomic systems that are
crucial for homeostatic, emotional, and visceral functions Eye status has dramatic effect on ReHo patterns
(Dosenbach et al. 2007; Seeley et al. 2007). Furthermore,
the SN acts as a pivot in switching between task-positive Some previous resting state functional connectivity MRI (rs-
and task-negative states (Sridharan et al. 2008), thus being a fcMRI) studies of ASD have included EO and EC data in com-
prime modulator of cognitive state (Dosenbach et al. 2007; bination (e.g., Cerliani et al. 2015; Dajani and Uddin 2015; Di
Seeley et al. 2007). Disturbances of the SN have been found Martino et al. 2014; Itahashi et al. 2015). Our findings, however,
to be associated with behavioral impairment (Greicius 2008; suggest that eye status during rest may have dramatic effects on
Uddin et al. 2013). In ASD, decreased activation of SN re- local synchronization of activity across almost the entire brain.
gions has been observed during inhibition tasks (Agam et al. First, between-group effects in ABIDE-EC diverged greatly
2010; Kana et al. 2007) and in association with skin conduc- from the three EO samples, with bilateral local
tance response (Eilam-Stock et al. 2014), presumably indicat- underconnectivity in visual cortex close to V1 (similar to find-
ing atypical autonomic functioning. Interconnectivity within ings from Itahashi et al. (2015)), and extensive overconnectivity
the SN and with the amygdala has been found to be reduced in in bilateral precuneus and PCC for EC. Direct comparison be-
several studies of ASD (Abbott et al. 2015; Ebisch et al. 2011; tween EO and EC samples showed robust effects of eye status
Kana et al. 2007; von dem Hagen et al. 2013). on ReHo patterns, with much greater local functional connec-
tivity for EO in extensive posterior (visual) and some frontal
regions, contrasted by extensive clusters of greater ReHo for
Visual cortex is locally overconnected EC in mostly limbic and subcortical regions (mid-portions of
the cingulate gyrus, medial temporal lobe, thalamus). Similar
Local overconnectivity in visual cortex was one of the patterns were observed in both TD and ASD groups (Fig. 1b;
two broadly consistent findings across multiple datasets, Supplementary Fig. 3b). Although EO and EC conditions in-
including a Grand Total (EO + EC) with N = 331. cluded different cohorts (no participants scanned under both EO
Although as mentioned, previous ReHo studies have dif- and EC conditions were included), samples were well matched
fered, the finding is in agreement with some reports on and it is unlikely that the robust differences detected by us were
local connectivity in ASD (Keown et al. 2013; Maximo cohort effects. Several previous rs-fcMRI studies have directly
et al. 2013; Washington et al. 2013). The divergent find- compared EO and EC states. The regional patterns of our
ing in Di Martino et al. (2014) may be partly attributed to findings were largely consistent with those reported by Liu
the inclusion of eyes-closed data (see below). However, et al. (2013) and are probably related to similar patterns ob-
since we detected small local overconnectivity clusters in served for the amplitude of low frequency fluctuations (Liu
visual cortex even in the Grand Total sample that included et al. 2013; Wang et al. 2015). Additionally relevant to effects
EC data, other methodological differences mentioned ear- in visual cortex observed by us, Zou et al. (2009) found that
lier (e.g., the much wider age range up to 64 years in Di thalamic connectivity with visual regions was more strongly
Martino et al.) were probable additional factors. negative for EC than for EO.
Overconnectivity in posterior cortices with visual functions In a first approach, the effects of EO status on activity in
may relate to extensive evidence suggesting a potential spe- visual cortex may seem trivial, seemingly indicating that EC
cial status of vision in the uneven neuropsychological profile may be preferable, as it avoids visual stimulation that may
commonly seen in ASD. This includes islands of atypically confound intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) effects.
enhanced visual function (Simmons et al. 2009), as observed However, aside from low-pass filtering applied here (and
in visual search (Joseph et al. 2009; Kaldy et al. 2013) and in commonly in the iFC literature), which will minimize any
some studies on the embedded figures test (Eussen et al. 2016; effects of visual stimulus processing in higher frequency do-
Horlin et al. 2016; Keehn et al. 2009). Numerous studies of mains, additional considerations speak against such a simple
vision in ASD have suggested a preference for local process- conclusion. Empirical evidence shows that EO states, espe-
ing, possibly at the expense of global gestalt processing cially when eyes are fixated (on a cross), have greater test-
(reviewed in Dakin and Frith 2005), in agreement with the retest reliability for BOLD effect and cerebral blood flow
hypothesis of weak central coherence (Happe 1999). measurements than EC states (Patriat et al. 2013; Zou et al.
Another explanatory proposal has been generally enhanced 2015), suggesting that EO states are better controlled whereas
visual perception in ASD (Mottron et al. 2006). The special EC states are subject to undesirable variability. The most ob-
cognitive-behavioral status of vision in ASD is supported on vious example is the onset of drowsiness and sleep, which is
the neurobiological level by evidence of atypical participation difficult to monitor or prevent in EC conditions. Onset of sleep
of visual cortices across various tasks (Samson et al. 2012), may be associated with substantial changes in BOLD
Brain Imaging and Behavior

correlations (Spoormaker et al. 2010; Tagliazucchi et al. differences in findings may relate to cohort differences.
2012). Even beyond sleep, it is possible that mind wandering Admittedly, the included datasets were not ideally suited
may be more intense in EC than in EO conditions because the for the purposes of the present study. Unfortunately, no
visual link to the outside world that is typically maintained dataset exists that would even approach what would be
throughout the awake state is interrupted. Notably, we found optimal (i.e., very large samples of ASD and TD partici-
that ReHo was higher in the two main nodes of the default pants, each scanned multiple times on different scanners
mode network (PCC, medial prefrontal cortex) in EO than in and with different imaging protocols, both with eyes open
EC datasets, possibly indicating that an expected mental de- and with eyes closed). However, we carefully matched
fault state was maintained more reliably in the EO condition samples group-wise and across sites, covarying for poten-
(for comparison of dynamic changes within the default mode tially confounding factors, and applied strict quality
network between ASD and TD cohorts, see Falahpour et al. control criteria to minimize effects of cohort heterogeneity.
2016). In addition, acquisition of low-motion fMRI data in the
awake state was almost exclusively limited to high-
Limitations and perspectives functioning individuals with ASD across sites. The find-
ings presented here may thus not reflect local functional
As mentioned, fMRI ReHo can assess local connectivity connectivity at the lower end of the spectrum.
only at a relatively coarse spatial scale (9mm3 for each
27-voxel cluster). It can therefore not be directly inferred
what cytoarchitectonic patterns (and anomalies) described Conclusions
in postmortem studies of ASD (reviewed in Palmen et al.
2004) may be reflected in atypically increased or decreased Our study shows that local connectivity between-group effects
ReHo (cf. Schumann and Nordahl 2011). Specifically, it from rs-fMRI are sensitive to site and cohort differences, even
may be tempting to relate local BOLD correlations to hy- when data are selected for high quality and low motion and
potheses of minicolumnar anomalies (Casanova et al. 2006) when groups are tightly matched for demographics and motion.
and excitation/inhibition imbalance (Nelson and Valakh However, local overconnectivity in posterior (mostly visual)
2015; Rubenstein and Merzenich 2003). Reduced regions and underconnectivity in cingulate cortex were detected
intercolumnar inhibition (and thus greater neuronal cross- both in some smaller ASD samples acquired under controlled
excitation) might in principle be associated with greater conditions and large Grand Total samples with greater site var-
synchronization of local neuronal activity. However, iability, lending confidence to the finding. Some inconsistencies
supportive evidence of such a link from combined MR in overall patterns of findings and the surprising variability of
spectroscopy of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and exact loci of similar findings across samples may be in part
resting state fMRI has, to our knowledge, only come from explained by robust differences for data acquired in eyes open
two studies in motor cortex (Sampaio-Baptista et al. 2015; vs. eyes closed conditions. Our study generates two recommen-
Stagg et al. 2014), and great caution is warranted in the dations: First, careful attention needs to be paid to between-site
absence of more direct evidence. factors of variability in the use of large consortium datasets in
There were trade-offs between data quality (low motion) fcMRI studies, especially those examining local connectivity.
and loss in sample size. To maintain overall large sample Sample size alone will not directly translate into confidence in
sizes, sites contributing small numbers of datasets were in- findings. Second, mixing of data acquired under eyes open and
cluded in some analyses. Differences in sample size and sta- eyes closed conditions can confound findings because between-
tistical power across different analyses may have affected the group differences in some regions may have opposite polarity
findings, yet additional analyses covarying sample size and for the two conditions.
head motion showed that similar patterns of effects were
retained.
Compliance with ethical standards All procedures performed in stud-
Furthermore, since each site contributed different
ies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical
>cohorts of participants, site-specific differences could standards of the appropriate institutional research boards and with the
not be distinguished from those related to the known 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
heterogeneity of ASD. If idiopathic ASD, the population standards.
studied here, is understood as a clinical umbrella label for
Funding This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health
possibly hundreds of etiologically distinct rare disorders
R01 MH081023 (PI: RAM), K01 MH097972 (PI: Inna Fishman), and
(Geschwind and State 2015), complete convergence across IMSD R25GM058906.
cohorts may in fact not be expected. Sites differed in their
exact inclusionary and exclusionary criteria (Supplementary Conflict of interest All authors declare that they have no conflict of
Table 1), and despite our matching procedures, some interest related to the study presented here.
Brain Imaging and Behavior

References connectivity during rest in autism spectrum disorders. Annals of


Neurology, 77(5), 866876.
Ebisch, S. J., Gallese, V., Willems, R. M., Mantini, D., Groen, W. B.,
Abbott, A. E., Nair, A., Keown, C. L., Datko, M. C., Jahedi, A., Fishman, Romani, G. L., Buitelaar, J. K., & Bekkering, H. (2011). Altered
I., & Mller, R.-A. (2015). Patterns of atypical functional connec- intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula re-
tivity and behavioral links in autism differ between default, salience, gions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disor-
and executive networks. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 4034. der. Human Brain Mapping, 32(7), 10131028.
Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J., & Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced Eilam-Stock, T., Xu, P., Cao, M., Gu, X., Van Dam, N. T., Anagnostou, E.,
cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex Kolevzon, A., Soorya, L., Park, Y., Siller, M., He, Y., Hof, P. R., & Fan,
in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 52(1), 336347. J. (2014). Abnormal autonomic and associated brain activities during
Anderson, J. S., Zielinski, B. A., Nielsen, J. A., & Ferguson, M. A. rest in autism spectrum disorder. Brain, 137(Pt 1), 153171.
(2014). Complexity of low-frequency blood oxygen level- Eussen, M. L., Gool, A. R., Louwerse, A., Verhulst, F. C., & Greaves-
dependent fluctuations covaries with local connectivity. Human Lord, K. (2016). Superior disembedding performance in childhood
Brain Mapping, 35(4), 12731283. predicts adolescent severity of repetitive behaviors: a seven years
Assaf, M., Jagannathan, K., Calhoun, V. D., Miller, L., Stevens, M. C., follow-up of individuals with autism Spectrum disorder. Autism re-
Sahl, R., O'Boyle, J. G., Schultz, R. T., & Pearlson, G. D. (2010). search : official journal of the International Society for Autism
Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in Research, 9(2), 282291.
autism spectrum disorder patients. NeuroImage, 53(1), 247256. Falahpour, M., Thompson, W. K., Abbott, A. E., Jahedi, A., Mulvey, M.
Belmonte, M. K., Allen, G., Beckel-Mitchener, A., Boulanger, L. M., E., Datko, M., Liu, T. T., & Mller, R. A. (2016). Underconnected,
Carper, R. A., & Webb, S. J. (2004). Autism and abnormal devel- but not broken? Dynamic functional connectivity MRI shows
opment of brain connectivity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(42), underconnectivity in autism is linked to increased intra-individual
92289231. variability across time. Brain Connectivity, 6(5), 403414.
Brown, G. G., Mathalon, D. H., Stern, H., Ford, J., Mueller, B., Greve, D. Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A.,
N., et al. (2011). Multisite reliability of cognitive BOLD data. & Noll, D. C. (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation
NeuroImage, 54(3), 21632175. in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-
Burrows, C. A., Laird, A. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2016). Functional connectiv- size threshold. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 33(5), 636647.
ity of brain regions for self- and other-evaluation in children, adoles- Friedman, L., Glover, G. H., & The FBIRN Consortium. (2006).
cents and adults with autism. Developmental Science, 19, 564. Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter
Casanova, M. F., van Kooten, I. A., Switala, A. E., van Engeland, H., fMRI study: controlling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio
Heinsen, H., Steinbusch, H. W., Hof, P. R., Trippe, J., Stone, J., & (SFNR) differences. NeuroImage, 33(2), 471481.
Schmitz, C. (2006). Minicolumnar abnormalities in autism. Acta Friedman, L., Stern, H., Brown, G. G., Mathalon, D. H., Turner, J., Glover,
Neuropathol (Berl), 112(3), 287303. G. H., et al. (2008). Test-retest and between-site reliability in a multi-
Cerliani, L., Mennes, M., Thomas, R. M., Di Martino, A., Thioux, M., & center fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 29(8), 958972.
Keysers, C. (2015). Increased functional connectivity between sub- Geschwind, D. H., & State, M. W. (2015). Gene hunting in autism spec-
cortical and cortical resting-state networks in autism Spectrum dis- trum disorder: on the path to precision medicine. Lancet Neurology,
order. JAMA Psychiatry., 72, 767. 14, 1109.
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of Gotts, S. J., Saad, Z. S., Jo, H. J., Wallace, G. L., Cox, R. W., & Martin, A.
functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and (2013). The perils of global signal regression for group comparisons:
Biomedical Research, 29, 162173. a case study of autism Spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human
Dajani D. R., Uddin L. Q. (2015) Local brain connectivity across devel- Neuroscience, 7, 356.
opment in autism spectrum disorder: A cross-sectional investigation. Greicius, M. (2008). Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsy-
Autism research: official journal of the International Society for chiatric disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21(4), 424430.
Autism Research. 9:43. Happe, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in
Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216222.
Neuron, 48(3), 497507. Horlin, C., Black, M., Falkmer, M., & Falkmer, T. (2016). Proficiency of
Di Martino, A., Yan, C. G., Li, Q., Denio, E., Castellanos, F. X., Alaerts, individuals with autism spectrum disorder at disembedding figures: a
K., Anderson, J. S., Assaf, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., Dapretto, M., systematic review. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 19(1), 5463.
Deen, B., Delmonte, S., Dinstein, I., Ertl-Wagner, B., Fair, D. A., Itahashi, T., Yamada, T., Watanabe, H., Nakamura, M., Ohta, H., Kanai,
Gallagher, L., Kennedy, D. P., Keown, C. L., Keysers, C., Lainhart, C., Iwanami, A., Kato, N., & Hashimoto, R. (2015). Alterations of
J. E., Lord, C., Luna, B., Menon, V., Minshew, N. J., Monk, C. S., local spontaneous brain activity and connectivity in adults with
Mueller, S., Mller, R. A., Nebel, M. B., Nigg, J. T., O'Hearn, K., high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Molecular autism, 6, 30.
Pelphrey, K. A., Peltier, S. J., Rudie, J. D., Sunaert, S., Thioux, M., Jao Keehn R. J., Sanchez S. S., Stewart C. R., Zhao W., Grenesko-
Tyszka, J. M., Uddin, L. Q., Verhoeven, J. S., Wenderoth, N., Stevens E. L., Keehn B., Mller R.-A. (2016). Impaired downregu-
Wiggins, J. L., Mostofsky, S. H., & Milham, M. P. (2014). The lation of visual cortex during auditory processing is associated with
autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evalua- autism symptomatology in children and adolescents with autism
tion of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Molecular spectrum disorder. Autism research: official journal of the
Psychiatry, 19(6), 659667. International Society for Autism Research.
Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved
K., Dosenbach, R. A., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and mo-
Raichle, M. E., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2007). Distinct tion correction of brain images. Neuroimage, 17(2), 825841.
brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Jiang, L., Hou, X. H., Yang, N., Yang, Z., & Zuo, X. N. (2015).
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United Examination of local functional homogeneity in autism. BioMed
States of America, 104(26), 1107311078. Research International, 2015, 174371.
Doyle-Thomas K. A., Lee W., Foster N. E., Tryfon A., Ouimet T., Hyde Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S.
K. L., Evans A. C., Lewis J., Zwaigenbaum L., Anagnostou E., (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder?
NeuroDevNet A. S. D. I. G. (2015) Atypical functional brain Developmental Science, 12(6), 10831096.
Brain Imaging and Behavior

Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and
Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehen- Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 2743.
sion in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Mller, R.-A. (2007). The study of autism as a distributed disorder.
Brain, 127(Pt 8), 18111821. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research
Kaldy, Z., Giserman, I., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2013). The mecha- Reviews, 13, 8595.
nisms underlying the ASD advantage in visual search. Journal of Mller, R.-A., Shih, P., Keehn, B., Deyoe, J. R., Leyden, K. M., &
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 1513. Shukla, D. K. (2011). Underconnected, but how? A survey of func-
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. tional connectivity MRI studies in autism spectrum disorders.
(2006). Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 22332243.
decreased functional connectivity. Brain, 129(Pt 9), 24842493. Murphy, K., Birn, R. M., Handwerker, D. A., Jones, T. B., & Bandettini,
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2007). P. A. (2009). The impact of global signal regression on resting state
Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage,
and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biological Psychiatry, 44(3), 893905.
62(3), 198206. Nair, A., Keown, C. L., Datko, M., Shih, P., Keehn, B., & Mller, R. A.
Keehn, B., Brenner, L. A., Ramos, A. I., Lincoln, A. J., Marshall, S. P., & (2014). Impact of methodological variables on functional connec-
Mller, R.-A. (2009). Brief report: eye-movement patterns during an tivity findings in autism spectrum disorders. Human Brain
embedded figures test in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Mapping, 35(8), 40354048.
Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 383387. Nelson, S. B., & Valakh, V. (2015). Excitatory/inhibitory balance and
Keown, C. L., Shih, P., Nair, A., Peterson, N., & Mller, R.-A. (2013). circuit homeostasis in autism Spectrum disorders. Neuron, 87(4),
Local functional overconnectivity in posterior brain regions is asso- 684698.
ciated with symptom severity in autism spectrum disorders. Cell Nomi, J. S., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Developmental changes in large-scale
Reports, 5(3), 567572. network connectivity in autism. NeuroImage Clinical, 7, 732741.
Koldewyn, K., Yendiki, A., Weigelt, S., Gweon, H., Julian, J., Paakki, J. J., Rahko, J., Long, X., Moilanen, I., Tervonen, O., Nikkinen,
Richardson, H., Malloy, C., Saxe, R., Fischl, B., & Kanwisher, N. J., Starck, T., Remes, J., Hurtig, T., Haapsamo, H., Jussila, K.,
(2014). Differences in the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus but Kuusikko-Gauffin, S., Mattila, M. L., Zang, Y., & Kiviniemi, V.
no general disruption of white matter tracts in children with autism (2010). Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain
spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences activity in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1321, 169.
of the United States of America, 111(5), 19811986. Palmen, S. J., van Engeland, H., Hof, P. R., & Schmitz, C. (2004).
Leech, R., & Sharp, D. J. (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex Neuropathological findings in autism. Brain, 127(Pt 12), 25722583.
in cognition and disease. Brain, 137(Pt 1), 1232. Patriat, R., Molloy, E. K., Meier, T. B., Kirk, G. R., Nair, V. A.,
Meyerand, M. E., Prabhakaran, V., & Birn, R. M. (2013). The effect
Liu, D., Dong, Z., Zuo, X., Wang, J., & Zang, Y. (2013). Eyes-open/eyes-
of resting condition on resting-state fMRI reliability and consisten-
closed dataset sharing for reproducibility evaluation of resting state
cy: a comparison between resting with eyes open, closed, and fixat-
fMRI data analysis methods. Neuroinformatics, 11(4), 469476.
ed. NeuroImage, 78, 463473.
Lopez-Larson, M. P., Anderson, J. S., Ferguson, M. A., & Yurgelun-
Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., &
Todd, D. (2011). Local brain connectivity and associations with
Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize, and remove
gender and age. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2),
motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 84, 320341.
187197.
Rubenstein, J. L., & Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: in-
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic inter- creased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes,
view-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for care- Brain, and Behavior, 2(5), 255267.
givers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disor- Rudie, J. D., & Dapretto, M. (2013). Convergent evidence of brain
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685. overconnectivity in children with autism? Cell Reports, 5(3), 565566.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., Sampaio-Baptista, C., Filippini, N., Stagg, C. J., Near, J., Scholz, J., &
Dilavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation Johansen-Berg, H. (2015). Changes in functional connectivity and
schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication GABA levels with long-term motor learning. NeuroImage, 106, 1520.
deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism Samson, F., Mottron, L., Soulieres, I., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2012). Enhanced
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 05223. visual functioning in autism: an ALE meta-analysis. Human Brain
Lu, J. T., Kishida, K. T., De Asis-Cruz, J., Lohrenz, T., Treadwell- Mapping, 33(7), 15531581.
Deering, D., Beauchamp, M., & Montague, P. R. (2015). Single- Satterthwaite, T. D., Elliott, M. A., Gerraty, R. T., Ruparel, K., Loughead,
stimulus functional MRI produces a neural individual difference J., Calkins, M. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Hakonarson, H., Gur, R. C., Gur,
measure for autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Psychological R. E., & Wolf, D. H. (2013). An improved framework for confound
Science, 3(3), 422432. regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the prepro-
Maximo, J. O., Keown, C. L., Nair, A., & Mller, R.-A. (2013). cessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage,
Approaches to local connectivity in autism using resting state func- 64, 240256.
tional connectivity MRI. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 605. Schipul, S. E., Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2011). Inter-regional brain
Maximo, J. O., Cadena, E. J., & Kana, R. K. (2014). The implications of communication and its disturbance in autism. Frontiers in Systems
brain connectivity in the neuropsychology of autism. Neuroscience, 5, 10.
Neuropsychology Review, 24(1), 1631. Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2011). Bridging the gap between
Minshew, N. J., & Williams, D. L. (2007). The new neurobiology of MRI and postmortem research in autism. Brain Research, 1380,
autism: cortex, connectivity, and neuronal organization. Archives 175186.
of Neurology, 64(7), 945950. Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H.,
Monk, C. S., Peltier, S. J., Wiggins, J. L., Weng, S. J., Carrasco, M., Risi, Kenna, H., Reiss, A. L., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable
S., & Lord, C. (2009). Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connec- intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and execu-
tivity in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 47(2), 764772. tive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 23492356.
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Shen, M. D., Shih, P., Ottl, B., Keehn, B., Leyden, K. M., Gaffrey, M. S.,
Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight & Muller, R. A. (2012). Atypical lexicosemantic function of
Brain Imaging and Behavior

extrastriate cortex in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from func- Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., Lynch, C. J., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J.,
tional and effective connectivity. NeuroImage, 62(3), 17801791. Feinstein, C., Ryali, S., & Menon, V. (2013). Salience network-
Shih, P., Keehn, B., Oram, J. K., Leyden, K. M., Keown, C. L., & Mller, based classification and prediction of symptom severity in children
R.-A. (2011). Functional differentiation of posterior superior tempo- with autism. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(8), 111.
ral sulcus in autism: a functional connectivity magnetic resonance Uppal, N., Wicinski, B., Buxbaum, J. D., Heinsen, H., Schmitz, C., &
imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 70(3), 270277. Hof, P. R. (2014). Neuropathology of the anterior midcingulate cor-
Shukla, D. K., Keehn, B. M., & Mller, R.-A. (2010). Regional homo- tex in young children with autism. Journal of Neuropathology and
geneity of fMRI time series in autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Neurology, 73(9), 891902.
Neuroscience Letters, 476, 4651. Vissers, M. E., Cohen, M. X., & Geurts, H. M. (2012). Brain connectivity
Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & and high functioning autism: a promising path of research that needs
Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral
Research, 49(22), 27052739. links. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 604625.
Simms, M. L., Kemper, T. L., Timbie, C. M., Bauman, M. L., & Blatt, G. von dem Hagen, E. A., Stoyanova, R. S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Calder, A.
J. (2009). The anterior cingulate cortex in autism: heterogeneity of J. (2013). Reduced functional connectivity within and between 'so-
qualitative and quantitative cytoarchitectonic features suggests pos- cial' resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Social
sible subgroups. Acta Neuropathologica, 118(5), 673684. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(6), 694701.
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Wang, X. H., Li, L., Xu, T., & Ding, Z. (2015). Investigating the temporal
Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in func- patterns within and between intrinsic connectivity networks under
tional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. eyes-open and eyes-closed resting states: a dynamical functional
NeuroImage, 23(Suppl. 1), S208S219. connectivity study based on phase synchronization. PloS One,
Spoormaker, V. I., Schroter, M. S., Gleiser, P. M., Andrade, K. C., Dresler, 10(10), e0140300.
M., Wehrle, R., Samann, P. G., & Czisch, M. (2010). Development of a
Washington, S. D., Gordon, E. M., Brar, J., Warburton, S., Sawyer, A. T.,
large-scale functional brain network during human non-rapid eye
Wolfe, A., Mease-Ference, E. R., Girton, L., Hailu, A., Mbwana, J.,
movement sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(34), 1137911387.
Gaillard, W. D., Kalbfleisch, M. L., & Vanmeter, J. W. (2013).
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the
Dysmaturation of the default mode network in autism. Human
right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive
Brain Mapping, 35, 1284.
and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy
Wass, S. (2011). Distortions and disconnections: disrupted brain connec-
of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(34), 1256912574.
tivity in autism. Brain and Cognition, 75(1), 1828.
Stagg, C. J., Bachtiar, V., Amadi, U., Gudberg, C. A., Ilie, A. S., Sampaio-
Baptista, C., O'Shea, J., Woolrich, M., Smith, S. M., Filippini, N., Near, Yang, D. Y., Beam, D., Pelphrey, K. A., Abdullahi, S., & Jou, R. J.
J., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2014). Local GABA concentration is related (2016). Cortical morphological markers in children with autism: a
to network-level resting functional connectivity. eLife, 3, e01465. structural magnetic resonance imaging study of thickness, area, vol-
Supekar, K., Uddin, L. Q., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., Gaillard, W. D., ume, and gyrification. Molecular autism, 7, 11.
Kenworthy, L. E., Yerys, B. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Menon, V. (2013). Zang, Y., Jiang, T., Lu, Y., He, Y., & Tian, L. (2004). Regional homoge-
Brain hyperconnectivity in children with autism and its links to neity approach to fMRI data analysis. NeuroImage, 22(1), 394400.
social deficits. Cell Reports, 5(3), 738747. Zhang, Y., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR
Sutton, B. P., Goh, J., Hebrank, A., Welsh, R. C., Chee, M. W. L., & Park, images through a hidden Markov random field model and the
D. C. (2008). Investigation and validation of intersite fMRI studies expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Transactions on
using the same imaging hardware. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Medical Imaging, 20, 4557.
Imaging, 28(1), 2128. Zou, Q., Long, X., Zuo, X., Yan, C., Zhu, C., Yang, Y., Liu, D., He, Y., &
Tagliazucchi, E., von Wegner, F., Morzelewski, A., Borisov, S., Jahnke, Zang, Y. (2009). Functional connectivity between the thalamus and
K., & Laufs, H. (2012). Automatic sleep staging using fMRI func- visual cortex under eyes closed and eyes open conditions: a resting-
tional connectivity data. NeuroImage, 63(1), 6372. state fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 30663078.
Turner, J. A., Damaraju, E., Van Erp, T. G. M., Mathalon, D. H., Ford, J. Zou, Q., Miao, X., Liu, D., Wang, D. J., Zhuo, Y., & Gao, J. H. (2015).
M., Voyvodic, J., et al. (2013). A multi-site resting state fMRI study Reliability comparison of spontaneous brain activities between
on the amplitude of low frequency fluctuations in schizophrenia. BOLD and CBF contrasts in eyes-open and eyes-closed resting
Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7(7 AUG), 137. states. NeuroImage, 121, 91105.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi