Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 3

OPINION ARTICLE

published: 29 April 2014


HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00261

Conceptual mappings and neural reuse


Cristbal Pagn Cnovas 1* and Javier Valenzuela Manzanares 2
1
Institute for Culture and Society, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
2
English Department, University of Murcia, Murcia, Spain
*Correspondence: cpaganc@unav.es

Edited by:
Jose Angel Lombo, Pontifical University of the Holy Cross, Italy
Reviewed by:
Mark Bernard Turner, Case Western Reserve University, USA

Keywords: conceptual mappings, conceptual blending, neural theory of language, neural reuse, metaphor, SNARC effect, material anchors for conceptual
blends, mental timeline

TOO MUCH NEURAL REUSE, EVEN FOR ACTIONS , etc. According to CMT, concep- But the metaphorical brain seems quite
A METAPHORICAL BRAIN tual metaphors are static, ontological, fixed insufficient to account for the perva-
Conceptual mappings are correspon- sets of partial correspondances between siveness and complexity of neural reuse.
dances between conceptual domains two conceptual domains. Metaphor trans- In a recent BBS target article, Michael
(SPACE, TIME, FORCE, EMOTION, etc.) fers inferences from the source domain, Anderson (2010) shows that what is going
or between entities within the same con- more concrete or better structured, to the on in the brain dwarfs the predictions of
ceptual domain. Through mapping, we target domain, which is more abstract or embodiment or CMT-NTL. Statistics run
project inferences, elements, and rela- less delineated. A system of thousands of on thousands of fMRI studies indicate that
tions from one mental configuration to metaphorical mappings constitutes the even fairly small brain regions are typically
another. Sets of mappings can become main mechanism for abstract thought in reused in multiple tasks, with even higher
entrenched, creating powerful cognitive the human mind. reuse probabilities if a region is involved
habits. For example, across many cultures From the nineties, the semantic pos- in perception or action (Anderson et al.,
around the world, temporal relations are tulates of CMT have been used to 2010).
conceived by means of spatial relations, develop the Neural Theory of Language Neural reuse is ubiquitous and
in language (Saturday is almost here), (NTL) (Gallese and Lakoff, 2005; Feldman, dynamic, and many of its results cannot
artifacts (timelines, calendars, sundials), 2008; Lakoff, 2008). In NTL, concep- be explained as domain-structuring inher-
or gesture (Nez and Sweetser, 2006). tual metaphors are replaced by neural itance. Anderson examines, among others,
Some of the mappings for this template mappings, combinations of simple neural the following examples: the SNARC effect
are: duration is spatial extent, events are circuits that carry out conceptual map- (a mental number line with magnitudes
landmarks, or time is motion along a path. pings. The major function of all this neu- increasing from left to right), the corre-
From over 30 years of conceptual map- ral binding is the reuse of sensorimotor lation between finger representation and
pings research emerges a picture of the brain mechanisms for new roles in lan- numerical cognition, the interaction of
conceptual system as a set of mapping guage and reasoning. This is congenial word and gesture, or the phonological
habits. Instead of a static repository of with the grounding of abstract concepts loop in working memory. These cases
concepts, we have a dynamic network in perceptual experience (Barsalou, 1999, present no metaphorical projection, and
connecting mental structures. Mapping 2008), called embodiment in conceptual some of them involve more than two com-
is not exceptional: it is the norm. It mappings research (Johnson, 1987, 2008; ponents. Rather than direct transfer of
is through mapping that concepts are Lakoff, 1987; Lakoff and Johnson, 1999). information, a given system seems to be
formed, learned, and developed creatively. The overarching idea of CMT-NTL is reused for a non-primary purpose because
These ideas have boosted the interest in the a metaphorical mind-brain, based on it happens to have a function or structure
most remarkable manifestations of map- direct, binary transfer across domains. The that are appropriate for the particular cog-
ping in language and thought: metaphor, transfer is carried out through static cog- nitive task at hand. As Anderson claims, we
metonymy, analogy, counterfactuals, etc. nitive habits, which are implemented by need a broader theoretical framework, able
Metaphor has received far more atten- neural circuitry connecting pairs of brain to account for those individual phenom-
tion than all the other phenomena areas. For the brain, metaphor is once ena as well as for the general prevalence of
combined. Researchers in Conceptual more privileged over all other manifes- neural reuse.
Metaphor Theory (CMT) (Lakoff and tations of mapping, just like it was for
Johnson, 1980; Lakoff, 1993), have identi- the mind. A good example of the rapidly FROM TRANSFER TO EMERGENCE: THE
fied many sets of cross-domain mappings growing interest in the neuroscience of NETWORK MODEL OF CONCEPTUAL
underlying conventional metaphorical metaphor is the Frontiers issue about the INTEGRATION
expressions: TIME IS SPACE, LIFE IS A topic, currently being edited by Vicky T. Can conceptual mappings research pro-
JOURNEY, ANGER IS HEAT , EVENTS ARE Lai and Seana Coulson. vide such a framework? For one thing,

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 261 | 1


Cnovas and Manzanares Conceptual mappings and neural reuse

the CMT-NTL model is certainly not and neural reuse never stops. Through it, awareness of potential correspondances
the only one in the field. CMT and minds and cultures select the few integra- between numbers and spatial relations
the pervasiveness of mapping were the tions that are really useful, anchor them dates back to Babylon (Nez, 2009). It
point of departure of Gilles Fauconniers by means of symbolic procedures, and pass took thousands of years for the pattern
Mental Space Theory (Fauconnier, 1985, them on to the next generation. To become to find an appropriate niche, alongside a
1997), later developed by Fauconnier and productive habits, both generic templates representational format that would ensure
Mark Turner into Conceptual Integration of conceptual integration and patterns of its transmission.
Theory, or Blending Theory (BT) (Turner, neural reuse need to find an adequate The number line is what gets called
1996, 2014; Fauconnier and Turner, 2002). niche within the general system (about a material anchor for a conceptual blend
Beyond the common ground with the notion of neural niche, see: Anderson, (Hutchins, 2005). A perceptual structure is
CMT (Fauconnier and Lakoff, 2013), BT 2010, and the commentary by Atsushi Iriki used as an input in the integration process.
introduces significant innovations. Mental therein; Iriki and Sakura, 2008; Iriki and In the blend, perceptual relations are fused
spaces are not vast domains such as TIME Taoka, 2012). with conceptual relations. Now consider a
or SPACE, but small conceptual packets very similar case. Varied psycholinguistic
that flexibly combine entrenched struc- ONE EXAMPLE: OPPORTUNISTIC evidence shows that processing temporal
tures and contextual information, for local REUSE IN THE NUMBER LINE AND THE expressions causes the automatic activa-
purposes of thought and action. Mappings TIMELINE tion of a mental timeline, also running
are established through structural or func- In blending as in neural reuse, a given from left to right in cultures with that writ-
tional correspondances between input item, once identified as potentially useful, ing system (Torralbo et al., 2006; Weger
spaces in a generic mental space. The is integrated into the network under con- and Pratt, 2008; Santiago et al., 2010).
participation of more than two inputs is struction. If necessary and possible, the Blending theorists have revised the TIME
quite typical. Selectively projected to a item is adjusted for optimization in its IS SPACE metaphor, and shown that it is a
blended space or conceptual blend, ele- new function. If it works, the item is kept complex network that produces a motion
ments from the inputs interact, typically in the network, although it still remains scene with special rules and constraints,
producing emergent structure, which can- available for its older functions. Networks designed to facilitate the representation of
not be accounted for by direct trans- and their components are discarded and time: all observers are on the same spot,
fer between domains. Inferences can take entrenched in a dynamic, extremely agile all objects move along the same path, and
place in the blend, but also be projected process. What is going on here is not spatial relations can even be modified by
back to the inputs, which can be modified direct transfer of structure, but rather the the emotional attitude of the observer:
as the process unfolds. The mappings, the construction of a new whole with old Monday is almost here, but Friday is so
emergence of novel structure, the adjust- pieces. The novel properties are not bor- far away (Fauconnier and Turner, 2008).
ment of the inputs, and everything else rowed from the structures being reused, The straight line is particularly useful for
going on is guided by universal governing but result from their performance in a new anchoring this blended scene. The result is
principles and competing optimality prin- network. a graphic representation with novel prop-
ciples, by the functional requirements of Among other examples, Anderson erties, which allow us to see diachrony at
the particular network of mental spaces, (2010) illustrates this with the spatial- a glance, to divide it easily into periods, to
dictated by context and goals, and by the numerical association of response codes represent events as dots, etc.
creativity of the individual or group who (SNARC) effect, that is, a mental num- The timeline is a generic integration
are striving to make the most of it all. ber line in which numerals are arrayed template that blends at least four inputs:
The overarching picture that results from left to right, in order of increas- time and time measures, spatial extent,
from BT bears important differences with ing magnitude (Dehaene et al., 1993). As objects, and events (Coulson and Cnovas,
that of CMT-NTL. Advanced blending Anderson explains, there is no metaphoric 2013). Spatial shifting from left to right is
underlies all manifestations of mapping, mapping or perceptual grounding here: absent from all these components, but it
including metaphor, which is just one in sensorimotor experience, magnitude happens to facilitate the task immensely,
more surface product of this species- may increase with height (the MORE IS UP and thus it is imported to the blend, for
defining capacity for integrating disparate metaphor), but not laterally, and certainly local purposes. The pattern is not func-
mental components into new, meaning- not in the direction of writing. Numerals tional as a metaphor in language, where
ful wholes. The human brain is a bub- do not inherit the structure of the spa- past and future are not on the left or right,
ble chamber of mental spaces, constantly tial shifting mechanism: the left-to-right but it is extremely productive in gesture
building new integration networks, and line has been picked opportunistically, and (Casasanto and Jasmin, 2012), where the
a culture is an even larger bubble cham- integrated with numeral magnitude, sim- lateral axis is more easily available. Again,
ber (Fauconnier and Turner, 2002, p. ply because the resulting blend meets the the timeline has a long history of failed cul-
321322). Just like evolutionand neu- requirements of the task. We could add tural representations behind it (Rosenberg
ral reuse, blending is opportunistic: it to Andersons argument by pointing out and Grafton, 2010), but, once the blending
reuses whatever is functionally suitable, that the mental number line, as a sym- template found its niche, it is reused time
right there and then. As it happens with bolic device, needed considerable cultural and again, and can even be adapted for
the natural selection of living organisms, time to emerge: it was only invented in representing complex emotions and creat-
the process of trial and error in conceptual seventeenth-century Europe, although ing sophisticated poetic effects (Cnovas

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 261 | 2


Cnovas and Manzanares Conceptual mappings and neural reuse

and Jensen, 2013). Metaphor is useful, but Cnovas, C. P. (2011). The genesis of the arrows of Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of
not enough to understand the timeline: a love: diachronic conceptual integration in greek metaphor. in Metaphor and Thought, ed A. Ortony
mythology. Am. J. Philol. 132, 553579. doi: (Cambridge, CA: Cambridge University Press).
broader framework of reuse and integra-
10.1353/ajp.2011.0044 doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139173865.013
tion is needed. Cnovas, C. P., and Jensen, M. F. (2013). Anchoring Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor.
time-space mappings and their emotions: the in The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor
WHAT KIND OF MODEL WE NEED timeline blend in poetic metaphors. Lang. Lit. 22, and Thought, ed R. W Gibbs, Jr (Cambridge:
BT researchers have indeed identified 4559. doi: 10.1177/0963947012469751 Cambridge University Press), 1738. doi: 10.1017/
many recurrent patterns and theorized Casasanto, D., and Jasmin, K. (2012). The Hands CBO9780511816802.003
about them, but we still lack a gen- of Time: Temporal Gestures in English Speakers. Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live
Available online at: www.degruyter.com/view/ By. Chicago, IL: University Of Chicago Press.
eral framework for generic integration j/cog.2012.23.issue-4/cog-2012-0020/cog-2012-00 Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the
templates (some work along those lines: 20/cog-2012-0020.xml Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge
Fauconnier and Turner, 2002; Fauconnier, Coulson, S., and Cnovas, C. P. (2013). Understanding to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic
2009; Cnovas, 2010, 2011; Turner, 2014). timelines. J. Cogn. Semiotics 5, 198219. doi: Books.
A fruitful interaction with research on 10.1515/cogsem.2013.5.12.198 Mitchell, M. (2006). Complex systems: network
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The thinking. Artif. Intell. 170, 11941212. doi:
neural reuse can impose further con- mental representation of parity and number mag- 10.1016/j.artint.2006.10.002
straints and requisites than those observed nitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122, 371396. doi: Nez, R. (2009). Numbers and arithmetic: neither
in the semantic or semiotic analyses. A 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371 hardwired nor out there. Biol. Theory 4, 6883. doi:
model of conceptual mapping habits fully Fauconnier, G. (1985). Mental Spaces: Aspects of 10.1162/biot.2009.4.1.68
compatible with neural reuse may include Meaning Construction in Natural Language. Nez, R. E., and Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. behind them: convergent evidence from aymara
the following: Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and language and gesture in the crosslinguistic com-
Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University parison of spatial construals of time. Cogn. Sci. 30,
Network thinking (Mitchell, 2006) Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO978113914220 401450. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_62
rather than direct binary transfer. Fauconnier, G. (2009). Generalized integration net- Rosenberg, D., and Grafton, A. (2010). Cartographies
works. New Dir. Cogn. Linguist. 147160. of Time: A History of the Timeline. New York, NY:
Flexibility in the activation, selection,
Fauconnier, G., and Lakoff, G. (2013). On metaphor Princeton Architectural Press.
and integration of conceptual and neu- and blending. Cogn. Semiotics 5, 393399. Santiago, J., Romn, A., Ouellet, M., Rodrguez, N.,
ral patterns. Fauconnier, G., and Turner, M. (2002). The Way and Prez-Azor, P. (2010). In hindsight, life flows
Focus on emergence. We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Minds from left to right. Psychol. Res. 74, 5970. doi:
Emphasis on competing optimal- Hidden Complexities. New York, NY: Basic 10.1007/s00426-008-0220-0
Books. Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., and Lupiez, J. (2006).
ity principles, e.g., a left-to-right Fauconnier, G., and Turner, M. (2008). Rethinking Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spa-
straight line leaves aside many relevant metaphor, in The Cambridge Handbook tial frames of reference. Cogn. Sci. 30, 745757. doi:
aspects of time or magnitude, but its of Metaphor and Thought, ed R. W Gibbs 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67
functionality is privileged. (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press), Turner, M. (1996). The Literary Mind: The Origins
Detailed examination of how context 5766. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511816802.005 of Thought and Language. New York, NY: Oxford
Feldman, J. A. (2008). From Molecule to Metaphor: A University Press.
and goals, including cultural diachrony, Neural Theory of Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Turner, M. (2014). The Origin of Ideas: Blending,
shape the process of integration. Press. Creativity, and the Human Spark. New York, NY:
A model of entrenchment not based Gallese, V., and Lakoff, G. (2005). The brains con- Oxford University Press.
on ontological projection, but on the cepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in Weger, U. W., and Pratt, J. (2008). Time flies like an
idea of attaining a niche through conceptual knowledge. Cogn. Neuropsychol. arrow: space-time compatibility effects suggest the
22, 455479. doi: 10.1080/026432904420 use of a mental timeline. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15,
instance-based learning and context- 00310 426430. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.2.426
sensitive usage. Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for concep-
tual blends. J. Pragmatics 37, 15551577. doi: Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare
REFERENCES 10.1016/j.pragma.2004.06.008 that the research was conducted in the absence
Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: a funda- Iriki, A., and Sakura, O. (2008). The neuroscience of any commercial or financial relationships
mental organizational principle of the brain of primate intellectual evolution: natural selection that could be construed as a potential conflict of
michael. Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 169. doi: and passive and intentional niche construction. interest.
10.1017/S0140525X10000853 Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 363, 22292241.
Anderson, M. L., Brumbaugh, J., and Aysu Suben,
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.2274 Received: 27 March 2014; accepted: 09 April 2014;
A. (2010). Investigating functional cooperation Iriki, A., and Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, published online: 29 April 2014.
in the human brain using simple graph-theoretic Neural, Cognitive) niche construction: a scenario Citation: Cnovas CP and Manzanares JV (2014)
methods, in Computational Neuroscience, of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use Conceptual mappings and neural reuse. Front. Hum.
(Springer) 3142. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-88630- and language from the control of reaching actions. Neurosci. 8:261. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00261
5_2. Available online at: http://link.springer. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1023. doi: This article was submitted to the journal Frontiers in
com/chapter/10.1007/978-0-387-88630-5_2 10.1098/rstb.2011.0190 Human Neuroscience.
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Copyright 2014 Cnovas and Manzanares. This is
Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577660. doi: 10.1017/ Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. an open-access article distributed under the terms of the
S0140525X99002149 Chicago, IL: University Of Chicago Press. Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Johnson, M. (2008). The Meaning of the Body: use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is per-
Rev. Psychol. 59, 617645. doi: 10.1146/annurev. Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Reprint. mitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are
psych.59.103006.093639 Chicago, IL: University Of Chicago Press. credited and that the original publication in this journal
Cnovas, C. P. (2010). Erotic emissions in greek Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.
poetry: a generic integration network. Cogn. 1997th Edn. Chicago, IL: University Of Chicago No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which
Semiotics 6, 732. doi: 10.3726/81610_7 Press. does not comply with these terms.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 261 | 3

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi