Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401

DOI 10.1007/s11199-016-0606-1

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Parenthood as a Moral Imperative? Moral Outrage


and the Stigmatization of Voluntarily Childfree
Women and Men
Leslie Ashburn-Nardo 1

Published online: 11 March 2016


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract Nationally representative data indicate that adults findings offer the first known empirical evidence of percep-
in the United States are increasingly delaying the decision to tions of parenthood as a moral imperative.
have children or are forgoing parenthood entirely. Although
some empirical research has examined the social conse- Keywords Parenthood . Gender . Childfree . Moral outrage .
quences of adults decision to be childfree, few studies have Stigma
identified explanatory mechanisms for the stigma this popu-
lation experiences. Based on the logic of backlash theory and
research on retributive justice, the present research examined A 2013 Time magazine cover featured the headline, BThe
moral outrage as a mechanism through which voluntarily childfree life: When having it all means not having children^
childfree targets are perceived less favorably than are targets (Sandler and Witteman 2013). Indeed, data from the Centers
with children for violating the prescribed social role of parent- for Disease Control document that the birthrate in the United
hood. In a between-subjects experiment, 197 undergraduates States steadily declined between 2007 and 2013 (Martin et al.
(147 women, 49 men, 1 participant with missing gender data) 2015), and a 2010 Pew Research Report estimates that 1 in 5
from a large U.S. Midwestern urban university were randomly women in the United States will remain childfree (Livingston
assigned to evaluate a male or female married target who had and Cohn 2010). The declining birthrate is not limited to the
chosen to have zero or two children. Participants completed United States, but is also occurring in Europe, China, and
measures of the targets perceived psychological fulfillment Japan (CBS/AP 2014). This growing demographic of
and their affective reactions to the target. Consistent with ear- childfree adults provided the impetus for revisiting the litera-
lier studies, voluntarily childfree targets were perceived as ture regarding stigmatization of voluntarily childfree women
significantly less psychologically fulfilled than targets with and men.
two children. Extending past research, voluntarily childfree In the present research, I used experimental methodology
targets elicited significantly greater moral outrage than did to investigate negative perceptions of people who choose not
targets with two children. My findings were not qualified by to have children. Specifically, my study examined whether
targets gender. Moral outrage mediated the effect of target targets who had chosen not to have children, compared with
parenthood status on perceived fulfillment. Collectively, these targets who had children, would be seen as less psychologi-
cally fulfilled. Unlike some previous studies that have exam-
ined such perceptions (e.g., Mueller and Yoder 1997), the
present study also included target gender as a potential mod-
erator of the relationship between target parenthood status and
* Leslie Ashburn-Nardo perceived fulfillment. Including target gender as a variable
lashburn@iupui.edu allowed for the possibility that penalties for disinterest in hav-
ing children might be greater for women than for men. Most
1
Department of Psychology, Indiana University Purdue University
importantly, using the lens of backlash theory (Rudman and
Indianapolis, 402 N. Blackford St., LD 124, Fairchild 2004) and the model of retributive justice (Darley
Indianapolis, IN 46202-3275, USA and Pittman 2003), my study identified an important
394 Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401

mechanism responsible for the stigmatization of voluntarily unfulfilled or maladjusted. However, remarkably absent from
childfree people: moral outrage. this literature is much theorizing regarding why such stigma-
tization of voluntarily childfree people persists in the United
States, even in the face of steadily declining birthrates.
Perceptions of People Without Children

Although by no means a voluminous literature, the study of Parenthood as a Social Prescription


perceptions of people who are without children of their own
whether by choice or by circumstanceis not new. Within the Although choosing to become a parent may be becoming
social psychological literature, the earliest studies appear to be somewhat less typical in the United States, extant findings
from the 1970s and 1980s, perhaps a timely reaction to the suggest that, in the eyes of most perceivers, choosing parent-
U.S. Supreme Courts 1973 landmark decision on reproduc- hood is still significantly more desirable than being voluntar-
tive rights (Roe v. Wade 1973). These studies, as will be de- ily childfree (Kopper and Smith 2001; Koropeckyj-Cox et al.
scribed later, were descriptive in nature and utilized both qual- 2007). In the stereotyping literature, what is typical can be
itative and quantitative approaches. Resultsobtained with distinguished from what is desirable (Burgess and Borgida
samples of male and female U.S. college students unless oth- 1999). Whereas descriptive stereotypes include behaviors
erwise notedconsistently indicated less favorable percep- seen as typical of a social group, prescriptive and proscriptive
tions of non-parents than of parents, with the least positive stereotypes imply an expectation or obligation. In other
ratings often of voluntarily childfree people. Specifically, rel- words, prescribed behaviors are behaviors that people ought
ative to those who had chosen to become parents, targets who to do and proscribed behaviors are behaviors that people
had chosen not to have children were viewed as less fulfilled ought not to do.
and more poorly adjusted psychologically (Jamison et al. In an effort to identify descriptive, prescriptive, and
1979). Collectively, this literature demonstrates stigmatization proscriptive gender stereotypes, Prentice and Carranza (2002)
of this childfree demographic. Perhaps the only targets stig- examined the perceived typicality and desirability of various
matized more than those who were voluntarily childfree were traits and characteristics for women and men among U.S. male
women who were pregnant and unhappy about it (Shields and and female college students. Findings revealed that women
Cooper 1983). were prescribed to be warm and kind, and men were prescribed
Researchers revisited these questions in subsequent years to have good business sense; women were proscribed from
in samples of U.S. college students and working adults, and being rebellious and men from being emotional (Prentice and
findings changed very little. Specifically, in the 1990s, Carranza 2002). The findings of Prentice and Carranza also
Mueller and Yoder provided evidence that voluntarily indicated that perceptions of what is typical for women and
childfree women not only were evaluated less favorably than men differed very little from perceptions of what is desirable
mothers by both women and men (Mueller and Yoder 1997), for women and men. This consistency likely underscores the
but also reported experiencing more stigma and pressure power of gender socialization. That is, whether through direct
about their parenthood decision than did mothers whose fam- experience or through observation, girls and boys learn to en-
ilies were normative in size (Mueller and Yoder 1999). For gage in behaviors that are socially rewarded and to avoid be-
example, in the latter study, voluntarily childfree women re- haviors that are socially punished (Bandura 1977).
ported being frequently asked personally invasive questions Most germane to the present research, Prentice and
such as BWhy wouldnt you want kids?^ (Mueller and Yoder Carranza (2002) found that having an interest in children
1999, p. 912). Likewise, research published in the 2000s re- was prescribed as highly desirable for both women and men.
ported that voluntarily childfree people were more negatively This prescription was somewhat more relaxed for men than
evaluated by women and men than people who were infertile for women, but the mean desirability of Binterest in children^
or for whom there was no explanation for their childlessness was above the scale midpoint for both genders as well as for
(Kopper and Smith 2001). Such penalties were attenuated in a people in general. As with many of the other gender stereo-
later study of male and female college students when the types in their research, Prentice and Carranza found that inter-
childfree status was perceived as temporary rather than as est in children was both a descriptive and prescriptive stereo-
permanent (Koropeckyj-Cox et al. 2007). type; in other words, it was seen as both a typical interest as
Taken together, the aforementioned studies demonstrate well as an obligatory one.
that across three decades and across various methods and Aside from any biological motives to reproduce, U.S. chil-
measures, reactions to people who choose to be childfree, dren are indeed socialized by their parents to want to become
relative to those who choose to have children, have remained parents. Classic tests of social learning theory demonstrate
consistently negative. Voluntarily childfree people elicited less that children imitate the warm, nurturing behaviors of models
favorable evaluations and were perceived as psychologically with whom they identify (Bandura and Huston 1961). Thus, to
Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401 395

the extent that children identify with their parents, they ob- communality and has been conducted with U.S. samples.
serve and learn parenting behavior. Consider, for example, the The findings of Prentice and Carranza (2002) indicate that
popularity of playing the imaginative game Bhouse^ with ba- women are stereotypically associated with warmth and kind-
by dolls. One qualitative study found that U.S. parents encour- ness (communality) and men with ambition and self-reliance
aged such nurturing play for children of both genders, even (agency). Consistent with backlash theory, highly agentic
among boys Bto prepare sons for fatherhood^ (Kane 2006, p. and therefore counter-stereotypicfemale job applicants are
160). Indeed, national survey data indicate that parental so- penalized in terms of perceived warmth and social compe-
cialization has a strong influence on childrens parenthood tence, which in turn results in decreased likelihood of being
decisions (Starrels and Holm 2000). hired (Rudman and Glick 1999). Similarly, female leaders
Peers also play an important role in shaping parenthood who exhibit dominance are rated less likable and less hireable
decisions among U.S. adolescents. For example, one nation- than women who conform to gender prescriptions of kindness
ally representative longitudinal survey found that knowing and warmth (Rudman et al. 2012).
peers who had a child significantly predicted teens own child- Women not only are penalized for being what they are
bearing at least in the short term; over time, the influence of proscribed not to be (e.g., agentic), but also face backlash
peer parenthood decreased (Balbo and Barban 2014). These when they fail to meet prescribed expectations. For example,
findings suggest that peers having children established a given that women are expected to be communal, they are
norm of acceptability regarding parenthood. similarly expected to engage in organizational citizenship be-
Collectively, these studies help explain the process by havioraltruistic workplace behavior that is not part of ones
which having an interest in children became both a descriptive job description. Women who fail to engage in such behavior
and prescriptive stereotype for women and men. Through par- are disliked (Heilman and Chen 2005) and have lower salaries
ents (Starrels and Holm 2000) and peers (Balbo and Barban and fewer promotions (Allen 2006). Furthermore, backlash is
2014), people learn that parenthood is both typical and expect- not limited to women who violate gender-role expectations.
ed. Although these studies were conducted with U.S. samples, Men who exhibit modesty about their accomplishments,
the gender stereotyping literature in general provides compel- thereby purposely downplaying prescribed leadership ability,
ling evidence to suggest that these norms likely hold across are penalized in terms of likability and hireability (Moss-
cultures. Multi-nation studies demonstrate remarkable consis- Racusin et al. 2010). In short, women and men are subject to
tency in gender stereotypes and expectations across a variety social and economic backlash for violating expectations based
of cultures (Williams and Best 1990). Indeed, studies conduct- on culturally shared prescriptive gender stereotypes.
ed in Turkey (opur and Koropeckyj-Cox 2010) and India In the present research, violating the prescribed interest-in-
(Riessman 2000) demonstrate similar stigmatization of people children stereotype by being voluntarily childfree is expected
without children as observed in the United States. to evoke similar backlash. Specifically, voluntarily childfree
targets should be perceived as less psychologically fulfilled
than are targets who have chosen to be parents, thereby ful-
Backlash: Reactions to Norm Violators filling their prescribed roles (Jamison et al. 1979; Shields and
Cooper 1983).
What happens when people violate strongly held norms and
expectations such as those regarding parenthood and interest
in children? Backlash theory (Rudman and Fairchild 2004) Beyond Backlash: Moral Outrage
suggests there are potentially serious consequences. Rudman
and Fairchild (2004) proposed backlash theory to explain the Outside the gender stereotyping literature, research on moral
maintenance of cultural stereotypes. They argued that people transgressions suggests especially harsh penalties for people
who violate social role expectations based on widely shared who intentionally commit a wrongdoing. In their model of
cultural stereotypes are subject to perceivers backlash, such retributive justice, Darley and Pittman (2003) propose that
as social and economic sanctions and sabotage. This backlash perpetrators who are perceived as intentionally (rather than
is justified in the minds of perceivers because the targets are accidentally or negligently) committing a wrong or inflicting
thought to have brought it upon themselves by not fulfilling harm evoke moral outrage in perceivers and, in turn, motivate
their expected roles. In short, backlash theory suggests that retribution or punishment. Moral outrage includes feelings of
cultural stereotypes often serve as injunctive norms, specify- anger, contempt, and disgust (Rozin et al. 1999). Darley and
ing which behaviors most others will approve or disapprove Pittman reviewed evidence across a variety of studies
(Cialdini et al. 1990) and justifying punishment of people who supporting its role in retributive justice.
violate these norms. Intentional wrongdoings need not involve a specific victim,
Much of the extant empirical research in support of back- but can be harms to the Bfabric of society,^ that is, threats to
lash theory has involved gender stereotypes of agency and cultural worldviews and violations of societal norms (Darley
396 Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401

and Pittman 2003, p. 330). Consistent with that reasoning, Based on the logic of backlash theory (Rudman and
Okimoto and Brescoll (2010) found among online respon- Fairchild 2004), Hypothesis 1 predicts that voluntarily
dents in the United States that female politicians who actively childfree targets will be perceived as significantly less psycho-
sought powersomething women, according to gender role logically fulfilled than targets who have two children; that is,
prescriptions, ought not to doelicited moral outrage, which they will experience backlash for having violated a cultural
in turn predicted their lower likelihood of receiving votes. In prescription. Hypothesis 2 is based on the logic of the model
other words, these women were viewed as more than atypical of retributive justice (Darley and Pittman 2003) and indicates
or unfavorable; perceivers were actually outraged, angered, a predicted main effect of target parenthood status on moral
and even disgusted by them. Furthermore, these emotional outrage; that is, participants should report significantly greater
reactions mediated the relationship between the communality moral outrage in response to voluntarily childfree targets than
violation and perceivers penalization of the women via their in response to targets with children. Finally, consistent with
voting behavior. These findings suggest that the womens Okimoto and Brescoll (2010), moral outrage should serve as a
power-seeking behavior was seen as morally wrong; the wom- mechanism through which childfree targets are penalized.
en had disturbed the natural order of things by violating their Thus, Hypothesis 3 predicts that moral outrage will mediate
prescribed role. Okimoto and Brescoll provided the first ex- the relationship between targets parenthood status and per-
tension of backlash theory to include moral emotions in re- ceptions of psychological fulfillment.
sponse to behavior that countered culturally accepted gender Given that interest in children is somewhat more intensely
stereotypes. prescribed for women than for men (Prentice and Carranza
Given that Okimoto and Brescoll (2010) obtained evidence 2002), it is possible that moral outrage and penalization will
of moral outrage in response to an intentional violation of a be greater for voluntarily childfree women than for voluntarily
prescriptive stereotype, it is reasonable to expect that people childfree men. However, recall that Prentice and Carranza
might also experience moral outrage in response to voluntarily (2002) found that interest in children was rated as highly de-
childfree women and men because having an interest in sirable for both genders and for people in general, with aver-
children is a social prescription. Indeed, Thompson (1974) age ratings well above the scale midpoint. Thus, targets gen-
reviewed not only the psychological literature, but also the der was included for exploratory purposes as a potential mod-
sociological and demographic literatures, and she concluded erator of the predicted main effects of targets parenthood
that, in the United States, if not around the world, having status on both psychological fulfillment and moral outrage.
children is a moral imperative. If, as Thompson suggested, Despite its exploratory nature, the inclusion of targets gender
parenthood is a moral imperative, then voluntarily choosing as a variable in the present research was especially important
not to accept this responsibility should evoke moral outrage given that some of the previous studies of the stigmatization of
among perceivers. Furthermore, according to the model of childfree people (Mueller and Yoder 1997) focused exclusive-
retributive justice (Darley and Pittman 2003), as well as find- ly on female targets, thereby leaving a significant gap in our
ings by Okimoto and Brescoll (2010), this moral outrage knowledge of gender-role expectations regarding parenthood.
should help account for any unfavorable evaluations of vol-
untarily childfree targets. Such findings would perhaps ex-
plain why perceptions of voluntarily childfree women and Method
men have changed so little over time and despite the increas-
ing typicality of their parenthood decision in the United States. Participants and Design
The present investigation uniquely attempts to provide empir-
ical support of Thompsons parenthood-as-moral-imperative Participants were 204 introductory psychology students at a
hypothesis. large U.S. Midwestern university. They were recruited via the
departmental online participant management system for a
study advertised as examining peoples ability to make accu-
rate predictions of the future. Participants were compensated
The Present Study with credit toward their course research requirement. Data
from seven participants were excluded because those partici-
In the present research, participants were randomly assigned pants incorrectly responded to a manipulation check regarding
to evaluate a male or female married target who had chosen to targets parenthood status. The remaining participants includ-
have two children or no children. Participants then provided ed 147 women, 49 men, and one participant who did not
their perceptions of the targets psychological fulfillment and report his or her gender. Upon examination by cell, there were
adjustment using items adapted from previous studies of too few men to provide sufficient power to analyze participant
childfree individuals. Participants also reported any feelings gender as a predictor variable. However, excluding men from
of moral outrage they had in reaction to the target. the analyses did not change the results. Participants average
Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401 397

age was 20.61 years (SD = 4.10 years). Most of the partici- have no (two) children and when surveyed in 2005 they
pants self-identified as White (n = 133), 20 as Black, 13 as had stuck with this decision.
Asian, 9 as Hispanic or Latino, 7 as Middle Eastern, 11 as
other or multiracial, and 4 did not report their race or ethnicity. Consistent with the cover story, participants then answered
The experiment involved a 2 (target gender: male vs. fe- questions supposedly designed to determine how accurately
male) 2 (target parenthood status: 0 vs. 2 children) they could predict the targets responses from their most recent
between-subjects design. Thus, participants were randomly alumni survey. These questions pertained to perceptions of the
assigned to read about one of four targets who varied only in targets psychological fulfillment or adjustment. Specifically,
terms of their gender and parenthood status. participants responded to seven items, using 7-point Likert-
type scales from 1 (extremely unlikely) to 7 (extremely likely),
to indicate the likelihood that the target has the potential to be
Procedure and Measures a good parent, is satisfied in their marital relationship, has a
partner who is satisfied in their relationship, is satisfied with
Participants completed the experiment individually in a labo- their decision regarding children, has a partner who is satisfied
ratory. After obtaining their informed consent, an experiment- with their decision regarding children, has a relationship likely
er led participants to believe that they were taking part in a to end in divorce (reverse-scored), and is satisfied with their
study about peoples sense of intuition and how accurately life overall. These items were adapted from previous research
they can predict the future. Participants first completed a filler examining perceptions of targets as a function of their parent-
task in which the experimenter recorded their guesses regard- hood status (Jamison et al. 1979; Shields and Cooper 1983).
ing ten coin tosses. The next task (the task of interest to my Items were scored such that higher scores indicated greater
study) involved making predictions about the life of a gradu- perceived psychological fulfillment; given their internal con-
ate of the participants own university. The experimenter led sistency ( = .82) as well as factor analytic evidence indicating
participants to believe that the target had been part of a longi- they formed a unitary construct, the items were averaged to
tudinal alumni survey since his/her graduation over 10 years create a perceived psychological fulfillment scale (M = 5.29,
ago, providing data on several occasions to the schools alum- SD = .89).
ni office. Thus, the experimenter ostensibly could compare Participants next completed five items (embedded among
participants predictions with actual data from the (in reality, filler positive affect items to disguise the study purpose)
fictitious) student. The purpose of this cover story was to assessing their moral outrage toward the target; items were
enhance participants engagement in the study, given that the randomized for each participant. Specifically, participants in-
task involved evaluating a person whom participants would dicated to what extent the target made them feel disapproval,
not meet. Moreover, leading participants to believe that data angry, outraged, annoyed, and disgusted using 5-point scales
were collected from their fictitious peer across multiple time from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). These items were adapted
points reinforced the perception that the childfree targets par- from research examining moral outrage toward power-
enthood status was not temporary. This point is important seeking women (Okimoto and Brescoll 2010). Given their
given some previous findings suggesting reduced backlash acceptable reliability ( = .76) and results of a principal com-
when the targets childfree status was assumed to be short- ponents analysis, these items were averaged to create a moral
term (Koropeckyj-Cox et al. 2007), as well as previous qual- outrage scale (M = 1.27, SD = .49).
itative reports of expectations that voluntarily childfree targets Finally, participants completed demographic items and ma-
will later change their minds (Mueller and Yoder 1999). nipulation checks to ensure they attended to the targets gen-
Participants next read a paragraph about the alleged former der and the targets parenthood status. Upon participants
student. The paragraph included the experimental manipula- completion of all experimental tasks, the experimenter
tions of target gender (male vs. female) and target parenthood awarded their research credit and debriefed participants using
status (0 vs. 2 children). Participants were randomly assigned a funnel procedure with increasingly targeted probes to check
to read one of four versions of this description of the target: for suspicion. No participant independently expressed doubt
about the credibility of the alumni survey cover story or could
James (Jennifer) lives in Columbus, Ohio. He (She) fully articulate the true purpose of the research.
graduated from [name of university] with a degree in
biology in December 2002. After graduating, he (she)
worked as a pharmaceutical sales rep to pay back some Results
student loans, but after a couple of years he (she) decid-
ed that career wasnt a good fit and investigated other Recall that Hypothesis 1 predicted that women would view
options. In summer 2003, James (Jennifer) married his adults who have chosen not to become parents as less psycho-
(her) college girlfriend (boyfriend). They decided to logically fulfilled or more poorly adjusted than those who
398 Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401

have chosen to be parents. Participants perceived target ful- accounted for approximately 28.81% of the variance in tar-
fillment scores were submitted to a two-way between-subjects gets perceived fulfillment. In short, the previously observed
analysis of variance (ANOVA). Results revealed a main effect finding that people who choose not to have children are per-
of targets parenthood status, such that childfree targets ceived as psychologically unfulfilled is partially explained by
(M = 5.00, SD = .84) were perceived to be significantly less perceivers moral outrage in reaction to them.
psychologically fulfilled than targets with two children
(M = 5.62, SD = .83), F(1, 193) = 27.00, p < .001 , d = .74. No
other results approached significance (Fs < 1). Thus, findings Discussion
support Hypothesis 1.
If having children is a moral imperative, as Thompson Relative to targets who had chosen to have two children, vol-
(1974) suggested, then people who choose not to have chil- untarily childfree women and men were penalized by per-
dren should elicit greater moral outrage than people who ac- ceivers in the present research. Specifically, they were per-
cept this moral responsibility (Hypothesis 2). Analyses indeed ceived as leading less fulfilling lives than do people who
revealed the expected main effect of target parenthood status, had chosen to have children. Moreover, their decision to forgo
thereby supporting this hypothesis. Participants reported sig- parenthood, arguably individuals most personal choice,
nificantly greater moral outrage toward targets who had cho- evoked moral outrageanger, disgust, and disapproval.
sen to have no children (M = 1.37, SD = .57) than toward tar- Moral outrage in turn served as a mechanism by which tar-
gets who had chosen to have two children (M = 1.16, gets parenthood status affected their perceived psychological
SD = .33), F(1, 193) = 10.11, p < .01, d = .45. No other effects fulfillment.
approached significance (Fs < 1). Findings from this research provide the first empirical sup-
Although findings thus far are consistent with the notion of port for the hypothesis of parenthood as a moral imperative.
parenthood as a moral imperative, a stronger test of the im- As Thompson (1974, p. 96) argued, there exists within the
portance of moral outrage as an explanatory mechanism for United States a cultural expectation that is held so strongly
the less favorable evaluation of voluntarily childfree people and so widely that it is an imperative: BOne shouldor
would be to determine whether it helps account for the ob- musthave children.^ She further suggested that the impera-
served direct effect of target parenthood status on perceived tive is so well internalized that there are almost certainly social
fulfillment. Thus, data were submitted to Hayes (2013) sanctions for those who violate the norm. In other words, not
PROCESS mediation analysis, using a bootstrapping proce- having children is seen not only as atypical but also as wrong.
dure to estimate 95% confidence intervals based on 10,000 Backlash theory (Rudman and Fairchild 2004) provides a
samples to determine whether indirect effects likely differ theoretical framework with which social sanctions for volun-
from zero in the overall population. Confidence intervals that tarily childfree people can be better understood. Specifically,
contain zero indicate that the indirect effect does not signifi- given cultural stereotypes that prescribe an interest in children
cantly differ from zero and therefore does not mediate the for both women and (to a somewhat lesser extent) men
relationship. (Prentice and Carranza 2002), perceivers feel justified in neg-
As shown in Fig. 1, moral outrage was implicated as a atively evaluating those who violate their expected parental
significant mediator. The indirect effect of parenthood status roles by choosing not to have children. Moreover, the model
through moral outrage was .16 (SEboot = .06, 95% CI [.06, of retributive justice suggests that when someone intentionally
.31]), thereby providing support for Hypothesis 3. Together, commits a wrongdoing, the resulting response is moral out-
targets parenthood status and participants moral outrage rage and a motive is to punish (Darley and Pittman 2003). The

Fig. 1 Mediation model


illustrating direct and indirect
Moral Outrage
effects of target parenthood status
on perceived psychological
fulfillment. All relationships are
statistically significant at p < .01
-.21 -.76

Target Parenthood Status


(0 = no children, Perceived Fulfillment
1 = two children) .46
Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401 399

fact that voluntarily childfree targets in the present research experimental realism. Nonetheless, future research should not
evoked moral outrage and figurative punishment in the form be limited to experiments with fictitious targets.
of a predicted unhappy life is consistent with research on Given the study comprised a single experiment, future re-
retributive justice as well as the hypothesis of parenthood as search is needed to ensure findings are not limited to this
a moral imperative. sample or methodology. Indeed, reflective of the population
It is noteworthy that voluntarily childfree male and female from which it was obtained, the present sample comprised
targets were stigmatized equally in the present research. primarily White women in their early 20s. Although previous
Although much of the extant literature on this subject has research (e.g., Jamison et al. 1979; Shields and Cooper 1983)
focused on women (e.g., Mueller and Yoder 1997, 1999), has not yielded many participant gender differences in percep-
studies that have included male targets have not yielded dra- tions of voluntarily childfree targets, a more diverse sample
matic differences in how they are perceived. For example, would be informative, particularly of potential gender differ-
Jamison et al. (1979) found that participants ascribed similar ences in the strength of perceived moral obligation to become
traits (e.g., selfish, poorly adjusted) to voluntarily childfree a parent for male versus female targets. That said, Prentice and
men and women and that, if anything, men were evaluated Carranzas (2002) examination of gender stereotype prescrip-
somewhat more negatively than were women. Indeed, inves- tions and proscriptions noted significant participant gender
tigations of stereotypic gender prescriptions suggest that both target gender interactions for only 4 of the 100 traits they
women and men are prescribed to be interested in children, examined across two studies: anxious, extroverted, playful,
although this prescription is somewhat more relaxed for men and controlling. These traits do not seem to be particularly
(Prentice and Carranza 2002). Again, these findings point to- relevant for evaluating parents versus non-parents.
ward parenthood as an imperative rather than as typical choice Furthermore, participant gender effects are noticeably absent
and speak to the common socialization of boys and girls in the in U.S. studies of backlash toward targets who deviate from
United States to aspire to become parents. prescribed gender roles (Rudman and Fairchild 2004;
My findings extend previous research on the stigmatization Rudman and Glick 1999, 2001). Collectively, these studies
of voluntarily childfree people by identifying an important point toward probable consistency in the ways women and
mechanism through which stigmatization occurs. Although men might view voluntarily childfree people. Perhaps a more
previous studies (e.g., Kopper and Smith 2001) examined fruitful path for discovering meaningful individual differences
some negative affective reactions as a function of parenthood in perceptions of people who are voluntarily childfree would
decisions, none focused specifically on moral outrage and involve an examination of participants values. For example,
none tested for its potential mediating role. The present re- might individual differences in endorsement of values such as
search thus takes us beyond documenting and describing this tradition and self-direction (Schwartz et al. 2012) predict the
form of social stigma and helps clarify why it occurs. degree of backlash and moral outrage toward voluntarily
childfree people?
The present findings open the door for further research to
Limitations and Future Research Directions specify additional antecedents and consequences of biases
against those who are voluntarily childfree. For example, mor-
The present study is, of course, not without limitations. al outrage is often associated with denial of humanity (Haslam
Participants evaluated one individual who was presented only and Loughnan 2014); perhaps having children is seen as so
via a paragraph-long description. Despite obtaining moderate- fundamentally part of human nature that those who opt out are
ly strong effect sizes in the present study, mean levels of moral thought to be somehow not quite human. Such a perception
outrage were small overall. One could therefore criticize this almost certainly would have implications not only for how
Bpaper person^ approach, but it is noteworthy that findings childfree people are evaluated, but also for how they are treat-
corroborated those of previous studies that varied widely in ed. Indeed, much of the extant literature on the stigmatization
their methods, and they are consistent with the self-reported of voluntarily childfree women and men has focused on ste-
experiences of actual individuals (Berdahl and Moon 2013; reotypes and attitudes (e.g., Jamison et al. 1979; Shields and
Mueller and Yoder 1999). Arguably, moral outrage in re- Cooper 1983); few studies have obtained evidence of outright
sponse to real and/or known targets may have been even stron- discrimination toward those individuals. One exception is a
ger than those observed herein. The major advantage of the study by Berdahl and Moon (2013), who found, in a sample of
present studys methodology is that participants could be ran- U.S. working adults, that women without children reported
domly assigned to read identical descriptions of people that more workplace harassment than did women who were
varied only in terms of the targets gender and parenthood mothers. More research is needed to identify other forms of
status (i.e., the variables of interest). Additionally, leading par- mistreatment as well as psychological consequences for those
ticipants to believe they were predicting the life experiences of who violate parenthood norms. For example, does the societal
actual graduates of their university likely enhanced the studys pressure on childfree women and men have negative
400 Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401

implications for health and well-being? There is much to learn backlash; in this case, moral outrage and expectations of poor
about this relatively unexamined, but growing, demographic. psychological fulfillment and adjustment. The present re-
Given that parenthood is a social and moral prescription, search merely scratches the surface in terms of describing
not merely a typical choice, findings observed over the past the continued stigmatization of voluntarily childfree women
three decades in the United States regarding the stigmatization and men, who are, by many personal accounts, often ostra-
of voluntarily childfree people seem unlikely to change. Any cized and led to believe they are selfish and defective (Sandler
further downturn in the U.S. birthrate may affect the perceived and Witteman 2013). Indeed, some writers have blamed vol-
typicality of voluntary childlessness but not likely the per- untarily childfree people for the destruction of the U.S. econ-
ceived moral obligation of parenthood. Future research might omy and culture (Last 2013). The present research helps shine
manipulate perceived threats, such as that of overpopulation, a light on why such stigmatization of voluntarily childfree
to determine the boundaries of this very robust bias. people occurs and just how serious its implications are.

Practice Implications Acknowledgments The author would like to thank Megan Dumka,
Scott Hall, Haley Kolp, Kassi Rains, Stacy Slavings, Kelsey Stephens,
Practically speaking, the present findings have some troubling Jessica Thomas, and Logan Wikoff for their assistance with data
potential implications for how people transition to parenthood. collection.
For example, the present findings, obtained with college stu-
dents in the Midwestern United States, suggest that many
young people view children as a necessary ingredient for ful- References
filling lives. Thus, they may feel tremendous pressure to have
children, not only from others as this literature suggests Allen, T. D. (2006). Rewarding good citizens: The relationship between
citizenship behavior, gender, and organizational rewards. Journal of
(Mueller and Yoder 1999), but also internally. Ironically, these
Applied Social Psychology, 36, 120143. doi:10.1111/j.0021-9029.
perceptions have absolutely no basis in reality. Meta-analyses 2006.00006.x.
reveal that parents report significantly less marital satisfaction Balbo, N., & Barban, N. (2014). Does fertility behavior spread among
than do non-parents, and as their number of children increases, friends? American Sociological Review, 79, 412431. doi:10.1177/
marital satisfaction decreases (Twenge et al. 2003). These 0003122414531596.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice
findings are thought to result from the lack of freedom and
Hall.
independence that parenthood brings, as well as role conflicts Bandura, A., & Huston, A. C. (1961). Identification as a process of inci-
and strain. In other words, the transition to parenthood is not dental learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63,
the recipe for happiness it is thought to be, and the moral 311318. doi:10.1037/h0040351.
imperative to become parents might push women and men Berdahl, J. L., & Moon, S. H. (2013). Workplace mistreatment of middle
class workers based on sex, parenthood, and caregiving. Journal of
into making a choice that leads to relationship conflict. Social Issues, 69, 341366. doi:10.1111/josi.12018.
The moral imperative to have children may also have prac- Burgess, D., & Borgida, E. (1999). Who women are, who women should
tical implications for young peoples academic success. In one be: Descriptive and prescriptive gender stereotyping in sex discrim-
study, U.S. female college students who were primed with ination. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 5, 665692. doi:10.
parenthood imagery (e.g., a crib) exhibited lower implicit 1037/1076-8971.5.3.665.
CBS/AP (2014, May 7). Dropping birth rates threaten global economic
identification with academics compared with those who were growth. Retrieved from http://www.cbsnews.com/news/dropping-
primed with college education imagery (e.g., cap and gown; birth-rates-threaten-global-economic-growth/
Devos et al. 2007). Such findings suggest that parenthood and Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of
education are, at least implicitly, perceived to be a zero-sum normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce
littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social
game, with potentially serious implications for students like-
Psychology, 58, 10151026. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015.
lihood of success. opur, Z., & Koropeckyj-Cox, T. (2010). University students percep-
tions of childless couples and parents in Ankara, Turkey. Journal of
Conclusions Family Issues, 31, 14811506. doi:10.1177/0192513X10361577.
Darley, J. M., & Pittman, T. S. (2003). The psychology of compensatory
and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7,
The present research provides the first known empirical evi-
324336. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_05.
dence that parenthood is viewed by some as a moral impera- Devos, T., Viera, E., Diaz, P., & Dunn, R. (2007). Influence of mother-
tive, consistent with Thompsons (1974) assertion. Despite the hood on the implicit academic self-concept of female college stu-
fact that women and men, with increasing frequency in the dents: Distinct effects of subtle exposure to cues and directed think-
U.S., are delaying the parenthood decision or are opting out of ing. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22, 371386.
doi:10.1007/BF03173433.
parenthood entirely (Livingston and Cohn 2010; Martin et al. Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and
2015), the cultural prescription to have children persists. As infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399423.
with any injunctive norm, violators are subjected to social doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045.
Sex Roles (2017) 76:393401 401

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi- prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: 26, 269281. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066.
Guilford Press. Riessman, C. K. (2000). Stigma and everyday resistance practices child-
Heilman, M. E., & Chen, J. J. (2005). Same behavior, different conse- less women in South India. Gender and Society, 14, 111135. doi:
quences: Reactions to mens and womens altruistic citizenship be- 10.1177/089124300014001007.
havior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 431441. doi:10.1037/ Roe v. Wade, 410 U.S. 113 (1973).
0021-9010.90.3.431. Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad
Jamison, P. H., Franzini, L. R., & Kaplan, R. M. (1979). Some assumed hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt,
characteristics of voluntarily childfree women and men. Psychology anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divin-
of Women Quarterly, 4, 266273. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1979. ity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574586.
tb00714.x. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574.
Kane, E. W. (2006). BNo way my boys are going to be like that!^ Parents Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to counterstereotypic
responses to childrens gender nonconformity. Gender and Society, behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance.
20, 149176. doi:10.1177/0891243205284276. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 157176. doi:10.
Kopper, B. A., & Smith, M. S. (2001). Knowledge and attitudes toward 1037/0022-3514.87.2.157.
infertility and childless couples. Journal of Applied Social Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash
Psychology, 31, 22752291. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001. toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder,
tb00175.x. gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and
Koropeckyj-Cox, T., Romano, V., & Moras, A. (2007). Through the lens Social Psychology, 77, 10041010. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.
of gender, race, and class: Students perceptions of childless/ 1004.
childfree individuals and couples. Sex Roles, 56, 415428. doi:10. Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and
1007/s11199-006-9172-2. backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743
762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239.
Last, J. V. (2013). What to expect when no ones expecting: Americas
Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012).
coming demographic disaster. New York: Encounter Books.
Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hier-
Livingston, G., & Cohn, D. (2010). Childlessness up among all women;
archy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of
Down among women with advanced degrees. Pew Research Center.
Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 165179. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.
Retrieved from http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2010/06/25/
2011.10.008.
childlessness-up-among-all-women-down-among-women-with-
Sandler, L., & Witteman, K. (2013). None is enough. Time, 182(7), 38
advanced-degrees/
46.
Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Osterman, M. J. K., Curtin, S. C., & Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Vecchione, M., Davidov, E., Fischer, R.,
Mathews, T. J. (2015). Births: Final data for 2013. National Vital Beierlein, C., Konty, M. (2012). Refining the theory of basic
Statistics Reports, 64, 165. individual values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). When men 103, 663-688. doi:10.1037/a0029393.
break the gender rules: Status incongruity and backlash against mod- Shields, S. A., & Cooper, P. E. (1983). Stereotypes of traditional and
est men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 11, 140151. doi:10. nontraditional childbearing roles. Sex Roles, 9, 363376. doi:10.
1037/a0018093. 1007/BF00289671.
Mueller, K. A., & Yoder, J. D. (1997). Gendered norms for family size, Starrels, M. E., & Holm, K. E. (2000). Adolescents plans for family
employment, and occupation: Are there personal costs for violating formation: Is parental socialization important? Journal of
them? Sex Roles, 36, 207220. doi:10.1007/BF02766268. Marriage and Family, 62, 416429. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.
Mueller, K. A., & Yoder, J. D. (1999). Stigmatization of non-normative 2000.00416.x.
family size status. Sex Roles, 41, 901919. doi:10.1023/ Thompson, V. D. (1974). Family size: Implicit policies and assumed
A:1018836630531. psychological outcomes. Journal of Social Issues, 30, 93124. doi:
Okimoto, T. G., & Brescoll, V. L. (2010). The price of power: Power 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1974.tb01757.x.
seeking and backlash against female politicians. Personality and Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Foster, C. A. (2003). Parenthood and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 923936. doi:10.1177/ marital satisfaction: A metaanalytic review. Journal of Marriage
0146167210371949. and Family, 65, 574583. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00574.x.
Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes: A
shouldnt be, are allowed to be, and dont have to be: The contents of multination study. Newbury Park: Sage.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi