Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

Review Article

Autism & Developmental Language


Impairments
SLP-educator classroom collaboration: Volume 2: 117
! The Author(s) 2017
A review to inform reason-based practice Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.
nav
DOI: 10.1177/2396941516680369
journals.sagepub.com/home/dli
Lisa MD Archibald
The University of Western Ontario, Canada

Abstract
Background and aims: Increasingly, speech language pathologists are engaging in collaborative classroom services with
teachers and other educators to support children with developmental language disorder and other communication
impairments. Recent systematic reviews have provided a summary of only a small fraction of the available evidence
and recommended the use of reason-based practice in the absence of a sufficient empirically driven evidence base.
The purpose of this paper was to provide a broad (but critical) review of the existing evidence.
Main contribution: Papers were gathered through review of reference lists in the recent systematic reviews and other
published works, as well as general internet searches. A total of 49 papers were identified either reporting empirical
evidence pertaining to SLP-educator collaborative classroom activities, empirical evidence pertaining to consultative
services, classroom instruction, or small group intervention in the classroom, or providing information, discussion,
surveys, or reviews related to the topic. Evidence pertaining to vocabulary, oral language, phonological awareness,
curriculum-based language, and written language were summarized together with qualifications based on elements of
the research design.
Conclusion and implications: Although much of the evidence must be interpreted with considerable caution, the
present review is informative for clinicians looking to adopt a reason-based approach to practice.

Keywords
Classroom, consultation, developmental language impairment (DLI), education, specific language impairment (SLI)

Increasingly, speech language pathologists (SLPs) are Association (ASHA)s Ad Hoc Committee on
engaging in collaborative classroom services with tea- the Roles and Responsibilities of School-based SLPs
chers and other educators to support children with (2010) describing collaboration with educators as
developmental language disorder (DLD) and other a responsibility of SLPs. Nevertheless, the
communication impairments. Importantly, educators research-based evidence for the eectiveness of a
and SLPs have dierentbut complimentary skills collaborative approach to service delivery has been
and knowledge, which lays the groundwork for an labeled inadequate (Cirrin et al., 2010). Indeed,
important partnership. Collaboration, by denition, Cirrin et al. suggested that clinicians rely on reason-
refers to working together to achieve shared goals, based practice and their own data to guide service
and there can be no doubt that SLPs, teachers, and delivery decisions. In order to assist clinicians in
other school educators share common goals in provid- this regard, the present paper provides a broader over-
ing educational access to children with communication view of the available evidence than has been included
impairments generally, and DLD specically. The in recent systematic reviews constrained by specic
need for SLP-educator collaboration is well recognized inclusion criteria (Cirrin et al., 2010; McGinty &
with the American Speech-Language-Hearing Justice, 2006).

Corresponding author:
Lisa MD Archibald, School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Elborn College, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario,
Canada N6G 1H1.
Email: larchiba@uwo.ca

Creative Commons Non Commercial CC-BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribu-
tion-NonCommercial 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits non-commercial use, repro-
duction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open
Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
2 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

SLP service delivery in schools and Responsibilities of School-based SLPs, 2010). It fol-
In a school environment, SLPs employ a wide range of lows from this notion that the classroom itself would be
service delivery models depending on a variety of fac- the natural context in which to address educationally
tors. Suleman et al. (2014) described services that lar- related communication goals. It is important to note,
gely happen outside of the classroom such as removing however, that this view is not universally held. It has
individuals or small groups from the classroom for been suggested that learning may be optimized through
intervention sessions (pull out) or that involve the judicious use of interventions outside the classroom in
SLP indirectly aecting the childs educational program controlled environments (Lindsay & Desforges, 1986;
by providing modeling or coaching to relevant educa- Lindsay & Dockrell, 2002). This latter view emphasizes
tors in the use of strategies to promote specic skills the importance of individualized solutions for optimiz-
(consultation). Collaborative SLP-educator class- ing intervention for children with DLD.
room-based services would include the SLP providing In parallel with the shifts in speech language path-
support to identied students while the teacher instructs ology concerning generalization and functional goals,
the whole class, with the SLP teaching particular cur- several driving forces out of education have served to
riculum content falling within the expertise of the SLP encourage SLP-educator classroom collaboration. One
such as phonological awareness, or the SLP and teacher factor is the movement towards an inclusive framework
engaging in team-teaching of content by jointly deliver- for individuals with disabilities requiring that persons
ing a lesson (Suleman et al., 2014; Prelock, Miller, & with disabilities receive the support necessary to facili-
Reed, 1995; see also Dohan & Schulz, 1998). tate their eective education as part of the general edu-
cation system (United Nations Convention on the
Rights of Persons with Disabilities, 2006). According
Why classroom-based services?
to this view, children with disabilities should have the
Over recent decades, there has been growing interest in opportunity to be educated in the Least Restrictive
SLP-educator classroom collaboration for many rea- Environment (LRE), that is, in the general classroom
sons including learning theory, functional goal setting, along with non-disabled peers to the greatest extent
inclusive educational philosophies, dierentiated possible (Individuals with Disabilities Education Act
instruction in the classroom, and response to interven- of 1990, 1990). The notion that all students should
tion. In the eld of speech language pathology, the have equal access to quality education is incorporated
concept of generalization of speech therapy gains widely in governmental education reports around the
made in-clinic to out-of-clinic environments has been world (e.g., Australias Education for All National
an ongoing concern across a range of disorder areas Review, 2015; Ontarios Learning for All, 2013; UKs
including treatment targeting aphasia (Thompson, Department of Education white paper, Education
1989), stuttering (Finn, 2003), speech sound disorders Excellence Everywhere, 2016; US Equity and
(Dunn, 1983), and DLD (Peterson, 2007). In response Excellence Commissions, For Each and Every Child,
to such concerns, there has been a move to more natur- 2013). For children with DLD, educational activities,
alistic treatment that occurs in the target individuals themselves, may pose a barrier to access because they
regular environmental setting and incorporates inciden- often involve complex oral and written language
tal communication drivers as they occur (e.g., Kaiser, (Bauer, Iyer, Boon, & Fore, 2010). Due to the growing
1993; Sundberg & Partington, 1998). For school-based pressures for students to learn the skills and knowledge
SLPs, there has been growing interest in classroom- needed to function in todays world, it has been argued
based services as a way of providing intervention directly that educational curriculums are becoming increasingly
in the setting in which the developing skills are needed rigorous and dependent on a students deep under-
(Pershey & Rapking, 2003; Prelock, 2000). It has been standing, reasoning, and problem solving (Capacity
suggested that the authenticity of the setting promotes Building Series, 2013). As a result of the complexity
faster generalization (McGinty & Justice, 2006). of the materials, the need to provide classroom support
Together with the potential benets to generalization to facilitate curriculum access for children with DLD
through classroom-based services, SLPs are interested has become increasingly important (Nippold, 2010).
in pursuing functional communication goals that help More recently, there has been growing recognition of
the child perform specic activities required in daily the need for dierentiated instruction in the classroom.
life. Functional goals in an educational context relate In simple terms, dierentiation means tailoring instruc-
to academic, social, emotional, and vocational progress tion to meet individual needs. The idea is to provide
(Ehren, 2000). As a result, school-based SLPs are dierent avenues to learning depending on the individ-
encouraged to incorporate communication goals that ual needs of dierent students in the classroom such as
will facilitate school success for children with DLD providing reading materials at varying readability
(e.g., ASHAs Ad Hoc Committee on the Roles levels, manipulatives, choice in assignments, and
Archibald 3

engaging work contexts (Tomlinson, 2000). In princi- special education at tier 3. In this system, the focus of
pal, there is much to admire about the theory related to special service provision moves away from costly and
dierentiated instruction, but in practice, the demands time-consuming individual assessments to progress
of implementing dierentiated instruction in a busy monitoring for all students implemented throughout
classroom of 2030 students with multiple needs is con- the tiers with movement to higher tiers based on docu-
cerning (Bauer et al., 2010; Myhill & Warren, 2005). In mented lack of progress. Formal comprehensive assess-
a 2008 nationwide survey of 900 teachers by the ments may not occur until the child is considered
Fordham Institute, 84% reported that dierentiated appropriate for tier 3 services. The potential role of
instruction was dicult to implement. Some evidence SLPs working in collaboration with educators in RTI
consistent with this view comes from a study on scaf- was discussed in a special topics issue of Topics in
folding, the temporary support provided for the Language Disorders (Ehren & Nelson, 2005; Graner,
completion of a task otherwise too challenging for a Faggella-Luby, & Fritschmann, 2005; Troia, 2005).
learner. Silliman, Bahr, Beasman, and Wilkinson At tier 1, SLPs may assist with the design of progress
(2000) compared use of directive scaolding involving monitoring and high quality instruction incorporating
direct teaching of concepts with corrective feedback to language in the curriculum (e.g., Justice, McGinty,
student responses and supportive scaolding involving Guo, & Moore, 2009); at tier 2, SLPs may collaborate
cues to elicit reasoning and contributions to extend in the planning, implementation, and progress monitor-
emerging understanding. Scaolding by a general edu- ing of supplemental instruction for learners struggling
cator and special education teacher conducting emer- in language- and literacy-related aspects of the curricu-
gent reading groups including two children with DLD lum (e.g., Koutsoftas, Harmon, & Gray, 2009), and at
and two typically developing children was observed tier 3, SLPs will be involved in providing specialized
over 13 sessions. Results revealed that more than assessment and treatment for children with DLD and
99% of all scaolding employed was categorized as dir- other communication disorders.
ective for all participants. The researchers concluded
that instruction was undierentiated.
Why might teachers struggle to implement dieren-
The challenge of change
tiated instruction in a large classroom (Bauer et al., In addition to the alignments with current devel-
2010; Myhill & Warren, 2005)? First, frequency: The opments in the elds of education and speech lan-
sheer number of demands for dierentiation among a guage pathology outlined above, SLP-educator
classroom of children with multiple needs might exceed classroom collaboration aords many opportunities
that which can be eectively delivered by a single class- for growth (Ehren, 2000; Throneburg, Calvert, Sturm,
room teacher. Secondly, the teacher might lack the Paramboukas, & Paul, 2000; Wilcox, Kouri, & Caswell,
expertise to provide eective dierentiation for children 1991): Teachers have the opportunity to observe and
with particular needs such as those with DLD. In both reinforce strategies being taught, and SLPs gain a
cases, the presence of a SLP in the classroom can pro- greater understanding of the skills the child needs to
vide needed and necessary assistance. The SLP in the succeed in the classroom, curriculum and social context
classroom not only provides another pair of hands to (Nippold, 2011). For children receiving services, in-
help with the work, but also brings expert understand- class intervention means that valuable instructional
ing of DLD permitting more eective implementation time is not missed and aords greater opportunities
of a dierentiated instruction framework (Palinscar, for generalization. As well, at-risk children who
Collins, Marano, & Magnusson, 2000). Importantly, would not otherwise receive SLP services may benet
no one person or profession has sucient expertise to from the enhanced language environment achieved
execute all of the functions associated with providing through the collaboration.
educational services to all children in the classroom Despite the potential benets, there are barriers to
(Hadley et al., 2000). By working together, an eective establishing SLP-educator classroom collaborations as
SLP-educator collaboration has the potential to sup- well. SLPs and educators must achieve a shared under-
port more students more eectively in the classroom. standing of their respective roles and expertise as a
Hand-in-hand with the principal of dierentiated necessary and rst step to building a collaborative rela-
instruction has come the notion of response to inter- tionship. Educators must acknowledge the added value
vention (RTI), a multi-tiered instructional approach to an SLP can bring to their educational context, and
the early identication and support of students with SLPs must be able to maintain their intervention
special needs. RTI incorporates high-quality instruc- focus (Ehren, 2000; Hartas, 2004; Ritzman, Sanger, &
tion in the general education classroom followed by Coufal, 2010). SLPs can help to establish a rm starting
more intense interventions for struggling learners invol- point for collaboration by shifting from the more
ving supplemental learning activities at tier 2 and traditional mindset of providing teacher training
4 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

to partnering with teachers and adopting language classroom-based services for children ages 28 years,
consistent with the latter view. and measured outcomes related to vocabulary
Of course, establishing a collaborative relationship (Throneburg et al., 2000; Valdez & Montgomery,
takes time, time for both planning and implementation, 1997; Wilcox, Kouri, & Casell, 1991). The authors
which can be another signicant barrier. If resources reported converging evidence (2/3 studies) of a benet
are very scarce, SLPs and educators might lack su- to collaborative classroom-based services over pull-out
cient time to jointly plan and carry out eective collab- services when addressing vocabulary goals. Caution
orative teaching (Bauer et al., 2010; Hartas, 2004; was recommended in interpreting the results given the
Throneburg et al., 2000). Consider also, the potential small sample sizes, large condence intervals for eect
impact of insucient resources on the quality of con- sizes, lack of blinding of assessors, and absence of del-
sultative approaches in which the SLP provides indirect ity checks in some studies. An additional six articles
services by training educators to incorporate communi- comparing service delivery models but not meeting
cative strategies for target children. Inadequate access the review criteria were listed in the appendix.
to consultation might result in little-to-no educator In a more recent review (2010), Cirrin et al. reviewed
uptake of appropriate strategies. Another concern ve studies that addressed the inuence of SLP service
arises regarding identifying someone available to delivery model (including frequency and intensity) for
work with a child in the classroom who has the neces- children ages 511 years on a range of speech, language,
sary knowledge and skills to implement programming and educational outcomes (Boyle, McCartney, Fobes,
suggestions from the SLP (Law, Lindsay, Peacey, & OHare, 2007; Kohl, Wilcox, & Karlan, 1978;
Gascoigne, Solo, Radford, & Band, 2002). A potential Throneburg et al., 2000; Howlin, 1981; Bland &
example of such constraints comes from a series of Prelock, 1995). Of the included studies, three addressed
studies by Boyle and McCartney and colleagues report- vocabulary outcomes and three, language and literacy
ing a manualized language therapy program for pri- outcomes more broadly. Given the small number of
mary school children with DLD delivered to small studies, the authors reported that no conclusions
groups by SLPs (Boyle, McCartney, Forbes, & regarding service delivery were justied and clinicians
OHare, 2007), speech and language assistants (Boyle, were encouraged to rely on reason-based practice and
McCartney, OHare, & Forbes, 2008), or by main- their own data to guide service delivery decisions.
stream school sta through consultation with the Articles (n 39) that did not t criteria (e.g., partici-
SLPs (McCartney, Boyle, Ellis, Bannatyne, & pants outside of age range, no identied speech or lan-
Turnbull, 2011). Expressive language gains were guage impairment, service not clearly delivered by SLP)
reported in the rst two, but not the latter study. In were listed in the appendix. Comparing across the two
the case of the consultancy approach (McCartney et al., reviews (McGinty & Justice, 2006; Cirrin et al., 2010)
2011), mainstream sta failed to implement the amount with regards to vocabulary outcomes, only the
of language-learning activity required to adhere to the Throneburg et al. study was common to both and
therapeutic program possibly because the sta simply both reviews rated this study highly. However, the nd-
did not have sucient resources to manage the added ings for the two additional studies included by Cirrin
demands of providing the intervention program. et al. were either unclear (Kohl et al., 1978) or reported
Indeed, there is evidence that SLP consultation may no dierences based on service delivery (Boyle et al.,
not necessarily lead to changed teacher behaviour 2007).
(Noell & Witt, 1999), that teachers may not report These systematic reviews (Cirrin et al., 2010;
beneting from SLP consultations (Dockrell & McGinty & Justice, 2006) involving 7 studies in total
Lindsay, 2001), and may have little access to SLPs for lead to the inescapable conclusion that the evidence-
consultation (Baxter, Brookes, Bianchi, Rashid, & Hay, base regarding school-based SLP service delivery
2009). The bottom line is SLP-educator collaboration models is insucient. It is also true, however, that
will be ineectual (or nonexistent) if the partners lack these reviews included only 7 of the 44 unique studies
the necessary time or other resources to implement ser- related to the topic identied in the search process
vices appropriately. (across both studies removing duplicates). Of course,
a rigorous evidence-based review must exclude studies
Evidence Based Reviews of Service that fail to address the specic question under consid-
eration. For the present purposes, however, a more
Delivery eclectic approach was adopted and all research address-
In the past decade, two systematic reviews have exam- ing classroom-based SLP collaborative or consultative
ined SLP school-based service delivery for young chil- services was considered relevant and potentially
dren. In 2006, McGinty and Justice reviewed three important for guiding and encouraging discussion
studies that directly compared pull-out and regarding current practice. The comprehensive search
Archibald 5

strategy included a review of the 44 papers identied in additional weekly 15-minute small group pull-out ses-
the systematic reviews, gathering relevant papers iden- sion. Results revealed a signicant advantage for the
tied in study reference lists, and completing general collaborative co-teaching approach over either of the
internet searches on the topic of SLP-educator/teacher other conditions for children with speech and language
classroom/collaboration. A total of 49 published papers needs (n 32). When considering all participants
were gathered on the topic, of which 14 reported empir- (n 177), greater gains were observed in either class-
ical evidence pertaining to SLP-educator collaborative room-based condition compared to the pull-out condi-
classroom activities, 14 reported empirical evidence tion. The authors suggested that the collaboration
pertaining to consultative services, classroom instruc- might have fostered greater sharing between the teacher
tion, or small group intervention in the classroom, and SLP leading to more carry over of activities for the
and 21 were information, discussion, survey, or students who needed it most (i.e., those with speech and
review papers (see coding notation in References1). language needs). Limitations to implementation include
the considerable SLP planning time required for the
SLP-Educator Classroom collaboration, and the lack of measurement regarding
Collaboration: Evidence generalization.
The corroborating evidence from the remaining
and Discussion ve studies that targeted vocabulary is summarized in
Vocabulary. Six studies were identied that focused on Table 1. The two studies targeting at-risk children of
vocabulary intervention and outcomes in developmen- 56 years (Ellis et al., 1995; Hadley et al., 2000) both
tal language disorder (DLD)-educator classroom col- compared classroom level intervention to a business-
laborations (see Table 1). The studies varied in terms as-usual classroom and either provided direct in-
of the target population (preschool, DLD: Wilcox classroom joint teaching (Hadley et al.) or consultative
et al., 1991; Valdez & Montgomery, 1997; kindergarten, services through suggestions for classroom teaching of
at risk: Ellis, Schlaudecker, & Regimbal, 1995; Hadley target vocabulary/concepts (Ellis et al.). Greater gains
et al., 2000; kindergarten-gr. 3, typical & DLD: were noted for the experimental classroom in both
Throneburg et al., 2000; secondary school in areas of cases, however generalization was observed by Hadley
socioeconomic disadvantage: Murphy, Franklin, et al. on standardized vocabulary measures but not by
Breen, Hanlong, McNamara, Bogue & James, 2016) Ellis et al. on a study-specic measure. In studies com-
and methodological rigor. Nevertheless, all incorpo- paring classroom and individual/pull-out treatment for
rated some randomization (at individual, Wilcox preschool children with DLD, Wilcox et al. (1991)
et al.; classroom, Throneburg et al.; Hadley et al.; observed no group dierences on a study-specic meas-
and school levels, Murphy et al.; note though, Ellis ure but a classroom advantage on a generalization
et al. relied on random assignment to classes completed measure, and Valdez and Montgomery (1997) found
by the school), demonstrated no baseline dierences (or no group dierence on a standardized expressive lan-
accounted for dierences if they occurred), and guage measure but a pull-out advantage on a receptive
reported on data lost to attrition (or had no attrition). language measure. Murphy et al. (2016) reported raw
These three quality indicators are considered necessary but not standard score increases on 2/5 standardized
for a randomized clinical trial to provide strong evi- vocabulary/word knowledge measures for 128 adoles-
dence of causality by the What Works Clearinghouse cents attending schools in low income areas compared
(U.S. Department of Education, 2013). Of note, how- to those in business-as-usual matched classrooms
ever, acceptable reliability (Throneburg et al.; Hadley (n 75) suggesting some generalization after a tea-
et al.; Wilcox et al.), delity (Murphy et al.), and blind- cher-implemented classroom adaptation of Joes
ing (Murphy et al.) were incorporated in only some of (2011) Vocabulary Enrichment Programme (VEP). In
these studies. this study involving consultative services, the SLP pro-
Throneburg et al. (2000) compared growth on a vided 5 hours of teacher training, and the intervention
study-specic measure of targeted vocabulary in 12 was completed by the teacher in 24, 40-minute class
classrooms with one each of kindergarten, gr. 1, 2, periods. Taken together, this evidence provides compel-
and 3 receiving services under 1 of 3 conditions: (1) ling evidence that targeted vocabulary can be eectively
Pull-out Weekly 50-minute small-group or individual taught through SLP-educator collaboration in the
sessions held outside the classroom for 12 weeks. (2) classroom. However, evidence regarding generalization
Classroom-based Weekly 40-minute SLP-delivered is largely equivocal.
whole class language lessons for 12 weeks with add- Oral Language. Five studies were identied that
itional weekly 15-minute small group pull-out session. focused on aspects of oral language and involved
(3) Collaborative Weekly 40-minute SLP-teacher SLP-educator collaboration (see Table 1). Three of
planned and team taught lessons for 12 weeks with the studies addressed narrative language and
6
Table 1 Evidence summary for SLP-educator consultative or collaborative partnerships targeting language goals in the classroom
Study Sample Intervention groups Results Critical analysis Effect sizea

Vocabulary
Throneburg et al., K-gr. 3, 4 classes each Collaboration joint planning Greater gain on targeted Random assignment at class level for Children without S&L
2000 Collaboration (n 74; (40 min/wk); team taught 5 tar- vocabulary for collaboration & 2/3 groups (not collaborative); needs:
12 S&L) gets/wk in 40 min weekly session classroom than pull out for all baseline equivalence of groups -Collaboration4pull-out,
Classroom (n 60; for 12 wks plus 15 min weekly kids; greater gain for children demonstrated; no attrition d 1.3
11 S&L) small group pull out session with speech and language reported; acceptable reliability; -Classroom4pull-out,
Pull-out (n 43; 9 S&L) Classroom SLP taught same needs in collaboration than fidelity not reported; no blinding d 1.1
vocabulary on same schedule but either classroom or pull-out Children with S&L needs:
teacher not involved plus 15 min -Collaboration4pull-out,
weekly small group pull out ses- d 0.7
sion
Pull-out 50 min/wk; target vocabu-
lary & other goals
Hadley et al., 2000 K-gr. 1, at-risk Collaboration joint planning (1 hr/ Standardized vocabulary scores Random assignment at class level; Receptive vocabulary,
2 collaborative classes wk); 20 words/concepts each wk; higher for collaboration than baseline equivalence of groups d 0.45; expressive
(n 46) SLP 2.5 days/wk in class including control demonstrated; 13/99 participants vocabulary, d 0.42
2 control classes (n 40) 25 min/wk small group on *see also phonological awareness lost to attrition; reliability (stan-
phonological awareness section dardized tests only) & fidelity not
Control paraprofessional on same reported; no blinding
schedule
Ellis et al., 1995 K, at-risk Consult SLP and teachers selected Scores higher for consult vs. School assigned children randomly to Data unavailable
1 consult class (n 20) concepts; teachers targeted con- control for target concepts; kindergarten classes, but experi-
1 control class (n 20) cept 80 min/wk; SLP provided no difference on untrained mental teacher interested in col-
ideas in weekly meetings; Control concepts laboration; statistical analysis
business-as-usual controlled for preexisting group
difference in concept knowledge;
no attrition reported; reliability
(standardized tests only) & fidelity
not reported; no blinding
Wilcox et al., 1991 Preschool, DLD Play-based interactive modeling of 10 No difference on target words; Random assignment to groups; Data unavailable
10 in classroom target words; team implementa- classroom intervention higher baseline equivalence demon-
10 individual tion in classroom; minimum 10 scores than individual on gen- strated; no attrition reported;
models of target for 24 sessions eralization measure acceptable reliability; fidelity not
reported; no blinding
Valdez & Preschool, DLD 90 min, 1/wk for 6 months No group differences in gain on Random assignment to groups; Data unavailable
Montgomery, 1997 20 in classroom Collaboration: joint identification of CELF total and expressive baseline equivalence demon-
20 pull-out goals, planning, implementation language score; greater strated; 1 child lost from pull-out
Both interventions targeted concept receptive language score gain condition; reliability (standardized
development for group receiving pull-out tests only) & fidelity not reported;
no blinding
(continued)
Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
Table 1 (continued)
Study Sample Intervention groups Results Critical analysis Effect sizea
Archibald

Murphy et al., Gr. 7-8 (11-13 year olds), Consult teacher training by SLP in Raw scores higher for experi- Random assignment at the school Significant effects,
2016 low income 2 sessions of 2.5 hrs for VEP mental group on 2/5 standar- level; baseline equivalence d 0.15-0.22; non-
7 Experimental class adapted for classroom. 12 topics dized vocabulary measures demonstrated; 11 participants significant effects, d -
(n 128) each covered in 2 40-min classes including 1 expressive & 1 with missing data not included; 0.001-0.12 (exception,
5 Business-as-usual by English teacher (sometimes receptive measure. Standard/ fidelity monitored; reliability d 0.25 for 1 measure
(n 75) team taught with resource tea- scaled scores no group (standardized tests only) not with discrepant SDs)
cher) in term 1 differences reported; some blinding
Business-as-usual English class in
term 1 & intervention in term 2
Narrative Language / Expressive Language
Gillam et al., 2014 Gr. 1, low & high risk Experimental SLP provided narra- Narrative gains in experimental Recruitment tactic not specified; Narrative gain vs.
1 Experimental class tive language instruction in class- group were clinically signifi- baseline equivalence demon- matched control: high
(n 10/11) room 30 min, 3 times/wk for 6 cant for high risk group strated; 3 lost to follow up from risk, d 0.93; low risk,
1 Business-as-usual wks Targeted vocabulary gains in control group; acceptable reliabil- d 0.42; vocabulary
(n 7/12) Business-as-usual student SLP experimental with low risk ity; fidelity monitored; blinding fain vs. matched con-
assisted teacher on same schedule group showed greatest gains trol: high risk,
d 0.94; low risk,
d 2.43
Spencer et al., 2015 Preschool, at-risk Experimental whole class narrative Story retell and comprehension Random assignment at class level; Reported d ranged 0.39-
2 Experimental classes language program: model, ges- score gains higher for experi- baseline equivalence demon- 0.56 for significant &
(n 36) tures, retell mental than control group strated; missing data at follow up 0.1-0.15 for nonsignifi-
2 Business-as-usual 4x/wk for 3 wks, 15-20 min each No difference on story (n 3) set to group follow up cant effects
(n 35) production mean; acceptable reliability; fidel-
ity monitored; blinding
McIntosh et al., 2007 Preschool, low income Experimental SLP designed & tea- Experimental higher than control Random assignment to classes; After follow up, d 0.42
1 Experimental class cher implemented 10 wks on standardized language test baseline equivalence not demon- compared to 0.25
1 Control class phonological awareness (term 1) at term 3 follow up but not strated; attrition not reported; immediately post &
(n 97, total) & 10 wks general language (term directly after intervention reliability (standardized test only) 0.2-0.21 at pretests
2) tasks for books chosen by the & fidelity not reported; no
teacher. SLP & teacher meetings 4 blinding
times/term.
Control not reported (assumed
business as usual)
Smith-Lock et al., School for DLD, 5 year Experimental 3 expressive goals: Experimental higher than control Random assignment to groups; Treated, d 0.65;
2013 olds SLP in classroom 1/wk for 1 hour, group gain on treated but not baseline equivalence demon- untreated, d -0.18
Experimental (n 22) 8 wks; class lesson, then 3 small untreated grammatical targets strated; 6 participants with miss-
Control (n 18) groups led by teacher, assistant & ing data not included but did not
SLP differ from remainers; acceptable
Control business as usual with reliability; fidelity monitored;
focus on comprehension some blinding
(continued)
7
8
Table 1 (continued)
Study Sample Intervention groups Results Critical analysis Effect sizea

Motsch et al., 2008 School for DLD, 8-10 Experimental 6 wks daily incorp- Gains higher for experimental Included all students meeting Data unavailable
years old (in Germany) oration of training on grammatical than control on trained tar- screening cut off at school; base-
23 Experimental classes targets focusing on context with gets, but performance did not line equivalence demonstrated; 12
(n 63) well-controlled, known vocabu- reach mastery participants not included due to
22 Control classes lary missing data; no blinding
(n 63) Control individualized targets
incorporated as possible
Phonological Awareness
Hadley et al., 2000 K-gr. 1, at-risk *See vocabulary section Collaborative group gains higher *See vocabulary section Significant effects:
2 collaborative classes than control group for trained d 0.28-1.17; non-
(n 46) & untrained phonological significant (only
2 control classes (n 40) awareness tasks (including rhyme): d 0.17
more challenging tasks)
McIntosh et al., 2007 Preschool, low income *See narrative language / expressive Experimental higher than control *See narrative language / expressive Immediately post inter-
1 Experimental class language section on standardized phonological language section vention, d 0.53-0.77;
1 Control class awareness measure post remaining data not
(n 97, total) intervention reported
Koutsoftas et al., 2009 Low income preschools Tier 2 2x/wk, 6 wks, 20-25 min Single subject responses to probe Recruited from 5 classrooms in Reported individual
34 low scorers on a each, teacher or SLP, small groups tasks 71% with med-large schools participating in early effects, d 0.6-1.9
phonological aware- in classroom; scripted instruction treatment effects reading partnership; single subject
ness measure in targeting initial sound awareness design (individual baselines); attri-
January tion not reported; acceptable
reliability; fidelity monitored; no
blinding
van Kleek et al., 1998 School for children with Phonological awareness activities at Experimental scores higher than Recruited from 2 classrooms; base- Data unavailable
communication dis- centres to which children rotated control 95% confidence inter- line equivalence not demon-
orders (10-15 min) for 12 wks in each of val on measures phonological strated; attrition, reliability &
2 groups of 8 (3-4; 5-6 2 terms (1-rhyme; 2-phonological awareness but not rhyme fidelity not reported; no blinding
years old); 8 historical awareness)
control data
Curriculum-Based Language (School Age)
Bland & Prelock, 1996 Gr.1-4 classrooms Collaboration interdisciplinary Measured Fall and Spring for 3 Recruited from children receiving Data unavailable
Collaborative (n 7 training (7 sessions of 2 hours), years! therapy in school, group depend-
DLD) planning (30 min/wk) to establish No group differences in number ent on class assignment; baseline
Pull-out (n 7 DLD) common curriculum and commu- of different words and utter- equivalence not demonstrated
nication goals, team teaching ance length but indicated in some analyses;
(SLP-educator) 30-45 min/wk Higher gains for collaborative vs. attrition, reliability & validity not
Pull out 1-2 times/wk, total 30- pull-out in number of intelli- reported although project dir-
45 min, incorporated academic gible utterances and complete ector checks conducted; no
vocab utterances blinding
(continued)
Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
Table 1 (continued)
Study Sample Intervention groups Results Critical analysis Effect sizea
Archibald

Kaufman et al., 1994 Gr. 3, typical Experimental LIC program plus Experimental better than control Recruitment tactic not specified; Significant effects,
1 Experimental class communication skills unit by SLP group on measures of statistical analysis accounted for d 0.83-1.74; non-
(n 16) & teacher, 1/wk for 3 wks, 45 min identifying poorer quality pretest performance; attrition, significant effects,
1 Business-as-usual con- each; focused on adequacy of explanations, & 4/4 measures fidelity not reported; acceptable d 0.01-0.78
trol (n 16) explanations to peers/adults of providing better reliability; no blinding
justifications
Starling et al., 2012 Secondary school Collaboration 10 weekly, 50 min Trained teachers higher levels of Schools (n 2) randomly assigned to Teacher levels of use,
7 teachers/21 DLD col- meetings with SLP targeting use than untrained teachers group; baseline equivalence d 1.2-4.2; Student
laborative training modifying language of instruction for modifications demonstrated; data missing for 1 outcomes ranged
6 teachers/22 DLD wait (oral & written) DLD students of trained vs. participant from each group d 0.67-1.05 for sig-
condition Wait group did not receive inter- untrained teachers better (n 2); acceptable reliability; nificant & 0.2-0.25 for
vention until after study written expressive and listen- fidelity monitored; blinding nonsignificant effects
ing comprehension (WIAT-III);
no difference on oral expres-
sion & reading comprehension
Drew, 1998 Summer school, 32 poor Everyone Can Read designed & 22 improved reading age by more All summer school students Data unavailable
readers, 6-10 years implemented by SLP: phonics- than 6 months; 4 - no benefit; recruited; single group design (no
Small groups based, sight words, repetition, older children benefited more control); attrition, reliability,
pleasure of reading (11-12 hours fidelity not reported; no blinding
total)
Farber & Klein, 1999 6 schools, K & gr. 1 Experimental MAGIC; SLP and Gains greater for experimental Schools recruited regionally, all stu- Data unavailable
12 Experimental classes teacher, 2.25 hours/wk; weekly 1 than control group on listen- dents in classes recruited, classes
(n 319) hour planning meetings; goals: ing comprehension and writ- randomly assigned to group; Post-
12 Control classes improve literacy, increase oral ing; reading approached test only design (no pretest
(n 253) language, improve communication significance scores); attrition, fidelity no
No difference in speaking reported; acceptable reliability; no
blinding
Curriculum-Based Language / Emergent Literacy (Preschool)
Wilcox et al., 2011 Speech and language Experimental TELLs; code-focused Gains greater for experimental Random assignment at class level; Reported d ranged 0.47-
needs; 3-5 year olds (phonological awareness, alpha- greater than control group on statistical analyses accounted for 1.32 for significant & -
Random assignment; bet, print concepts, writing) & vocabulary, sentence length, pretest differences; 3 classes not 0.22-0.39 for nonsigni-
unbalanced oral language (vocabulary, sen- phonological awareness included withdrew; acceptable ficant effects
19 Experimental classes tence length and complexity); 12 No difference on sentence com- reliability; fidelity monitored;
(n 80) biweekly themes, all day, all year; plexity, print concepts some blinding
10 Business-as-usual training 22 hours, 30 min.
(n 38) weekly in-class mentor (SLP)
support
Justice et al., 2010, Typical; 3-5 years old Experimental Read It Again!, 30 wk Gains greater for experimental Random assignment at class level; Significant effects,
2009 11 Experimental classes curriculum, 2 times/wk for 20- than control group on lan- baseline equivalence demon- d 0.15-0.82; non-
(n 66) 30 min; 1.5 days training tea- guage (grammar; vocabulary) strated; 12-21% missing data significant effect,
9 Business-as-usual chers, SLP, assistants; whole class and emergent literacy (print managed with intent-to-treat d 0.19 (for measure
(n 72) focus on narrative, vocab, print and phonological awareness) analysis; acceptable reliability; with large SDs)
awareness, phonological No difference on alphabet fidelity monitored; blinding
9

awareness
(continued)
10 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

Effect size: Cohens d calculated on mean differences in selected comparisons based on published data, or converted from t-values or eta-squared values for group gain comparisons, or as reported for

Growth by Improving Communication; TELL: Teaching Early Language and Literacy; VEP: Vocabulary Enrichment Programme; CELF: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; WIAT-III: Wechsler
K: kindergarten; gr.: grade; S&L: children with speech and language needs; wk: week; min: minute; hr: hour; CBL: Curriculum-Based Language; LIC: Language-in-Classroom; MAGIC: Maximizing Academic
d 1.9; childrens lan-
populations of varying risk level (at risk; low income;
low/high risk) in either preschool (Spencer, Petersen,

Teacher utterances,
Effect sizea

Slocum, & Allen, 2015; McIntosh, Crosbie, Holm,

guage, d 1.8
Dodd, & Thomas, 2007) or gr. 1 (Gillam et al., 2014),
and the remaining two compared classroom-based ser-
vices to business-as-usual classrooms in schools for
children with DLD (5 year olds: Smith-Lock et al.,
2013; 810 year olds: Motsch et al., 2008). With regards
to methodological rigor, only two of these studies
strated; no attrition; acceptable

incorporated any randomization of participants


reliability; fidelity monitored;
baseline equivalence demon-
Random assignment to groups;

(McIntosh et al., 2007; and at the classroom level:


Critical analysis

Spencer et al.), all (but one: McIntosh et al., 2007)


between-group studies demonstrated baseline equiva-
lence of groups, but none specically addressed attri-
tion. Further, the majority (exceptions: Motsch et al.;
blinding

McIntosh et al., 2007) incorporated some blinding of


assessors, and Gillam et al. reported treatment delity
checks. It is clear that caution is warranted in interpret-
ing the results from this group of studies. Note that one
study (Gallagher & Chiat, 2009) comparing direct
untrained teachers in making
Greater gains for trained than

print references, and child

group intervention (96 hours) and classroom-based ser-


using decontextualized

vices (11 hours) for preschool children was not included


in the review due to the highly discrepant dierence in
Results

hours of service across the treatment conditions.


Gillam et al. (2014) identied children in two grade 1
language

classrooms as either high or low risk based on a cut-o


standard score of 90 on the Test of Narrative Language
(Gillam & Pearson, 2004). For the experimental class-
room (low risk: n 10; high risk: n 11), the SLP pro-
vided 30-minute, full class narrative language
Control 2 professional training

Hanen program (4 workshops


development: ABC & Beyond

with SLP; 3 classroom visits;

instruction sessions targeting story grammar and


Experimental Professional

related vocabulary, elaboration, and independent story-


Intervention groups

telling 3 times per week for 6 weeks. In the business-as-


usual classroom, a student SLP assisted the teacher on
video feedback)

activities of the teachers choice on the same schedule as


the SLP intervention in the experimental class. Study
specic outcome measures included a narrative probe,
days

and a vocabulary probe specic to the targeted story


grammar. Growth at posttest was assessed relative
to the 95% condence interval around the pretest
Individual Achievement Test; SDs: standard deviations

scores. Although higher scores on the narrative probe


each recruited 3-4 kids

gains compared across groups in the original paper

at posttest were observed for both the high and low risk
trained, 10 control;
Educator groups: 10

experimental but not control groups overall, only the


Sample

high-risk experimental group showed a clinically signi-


cant change reected by posttest scores in excess of the
pretest score 95% condence interval. In fact, posttest
scores did not dier for the high vs. low risk groups
who completed the narrative language intervention. In
the case of the vocabulary probe, the pattern was
Table 1 (continued)

opposite: Although both clinically signicant, the postt-


Girolametto et al.,

est gains were much larger for the low- than high-risk
experimental groups and not observed in the control
groups. The authors argued that classroom-based nar-
2012

rative language with embedded vocabulary instruction


Note:
Study

can lead to clinically signicant change in narrative


a
Archibald 11

language and vocabulary, but opportunities for skills). In addition to vocabulary for Hadley et al.
vocabulary review may be insucient for learning in (2000) and narrative/expressive language for
children with the lowest language skills. McIntosh et al. (2007), these studies also targeted
The corroborating evidence for the remaining four phonological awareness and so were included in this
studies targeting oral language is summarized in group. Neither of the other studies (Koutsoftas et al.,
Table 1. Similar to Gillam et al. (2014), Spencer et al. 2009; van Kleek et al., 1998) incorporated randomiza-
(2015) compared a whole class preschool narrative lan- tion, demonstrated baseline equivalence, or considered
guage program implemented in 15-20 minute lessons, attrition (although it did not appear that there was data
3 times per week for 4 weeks (n 36) to a business-as- loss due to attrition). Koutsoftas et al. incorporated a
usual classroom (n 35) at a Head Start preschool. multiple baseline design, and treatment delity checks,
Signicantly greater gains in the experimental group but assessors also acted as interventionists. van Kleek
were found on program-specic outcome measures of employed a historical control group, did not report
narrative recall and story comprehension, but not story delity checks, and did not have blinded assessors.
generation. It was suggested that the lack of a change Hadley et al. (2000) compared outcomes from two
on the expressive measure could have been related to kindergarten-gr.1 classes (n 46) receiving SLP-educa-
the low dose of intervention (4 weeks). In another study tor collaborative classroom-based services targeting
of preschool children (n 97) attending school in a low vocabulary and rhyme/initial sound awareness to two
income area, McIntosh et al. (2007) reported standar- kindergarten-gr. 1 business-as-usual comparison classes
dized test score gains at 3-month follow up but not (n 40). In the collaborative program, SLPs and edu-
immediately post intervention compared to a busi- cators met weekly over the academic year for joint
ness-as-usual classroom for those in an experimental planning and the SLP worked in the classroom 2.5
classroom receiving a SLP-designed and teacher imple- days per week to facilitate language and lead one
mented 10-week classroom-based language intervention weekly 25-minute small-group rhyme/initial sound
focused on story retelling, categorization, and following awareness activity centre to which all students rotated.
directions. One additional study by Khan and Paddick A paraprofessional assisted the classroom teacher in the
(2014) provided a description of a similar program business-as-usual classroom on the same schedule. Pre/
adapted for First Nations children but was not included post phonological awareness skills were measured in
in the review due to the lack of empirical data reported. September and April of the academic year using
The two small-scale studies implemented in schools grade-level subtests from the Phonological Awareness
for children with DLD reported highly similar results and Literacy Screening (PALS; Swank et al., 1997).
(Smith-Lock et al., 2013; Motsch et al., 2008). In both The PALS measured skills targeted (rhyme, initial
cases, expressive grammatical targets were the focus of sound awareness, letter-sound knowledge) and not tar-
SLP classroom-based interventions for 3-6 weeks in an geted in the intervention (syllable and phoneme dele-
experimental classroom and outcomes on study-specic tion). Results revealed signicantly greater gains for the
measures were compared to a business-as-usual class- experimental vs. comparison groups for all phono-
room. Results revealed greater gains for the experimen- logical awareness subtests except rhyme reecting
tal than control groups on treated but not untreated changes in both trained and untrained skills after a
targets (Smith-Lock et al.) and without attaining mas- high dose of intervention.
tery of the skill (Motsch et al.). Taken together, these Table 1 summarizes the corroborating evidence from
studies are highly suggestive of an overall benet to the the remaining three studies. Prior to their language
narrative language skills of young children of direct intervention and with the same participants, teachers
classroom teaching of narrative skills (Gillam et al.; in the McIntosh et al. (2007) study implemented a 10-
Spencer et al.). For specic expressive language goals, week phonological awareness intervention designed by
however, the ndings are more guarded (Smith-Lock the SLP. Signicant gains favouring the experimental
et al.; Motsch et al.): Despite positive change, children group on a standardized measure of phonological
with DLD may fail to reach mastery of an expressive awareness were observed at post intervention. The
language skill presented solely through whole class remaining two studies showed positive benets of
teaching. It may be that a hybrid approach involving small group phonological awareness intervention over
some small group/individual intervention followed by 6 weeks in low-income preschoolers (Koutsoftas et al.,
classroom support would increase the impact of inter- 2009) and over 12 weeks in each of 2 academic terms in
ventions addressing specic oral language targets. school age children with DLD (van Kleek et al., 1998).
Phonological Awareness. Four papers (see Table 1 The study by Koutsoftas et al. is unique in that it incor-
for summary) were identied that focused specically porated an RTI approach: All preschoolers were
on phonological awareness classroom-based interven- screened in January of the academic year after four
tion (without incorporating other literacy-related months of the schools universal instruction
12 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

incorporating phonological awareness. Low scorers on To develop and implement the Language-in-the-
the January phonological awareness screen (n 34) Classroom curriculum, SLPs and educators completed
completed 20-25-minute small group initial sound transdisciplinary training, met weekly to establish
awareness activities in the classroom 2 times per week common goals and activities, and co-taught weekly in
for 6 weeks conducted by the SLP or teacher. On study- the classroom for 3045 minutes. Language samples
specic treatment probes, 71% of participants showed collected in the Fall and Spring of each academic year
intervention and post-intervention gains relative to for three years were coded for number of dierent
baseline performance reecting medium-to-large words, mean length of utterance, utterance complete-
eects. Taken together, these studies provide compel- ness, and utterance intelligibility. For the latter two
ling evidence for benets of classroom and small group measures only, signicantly higher scores using non-
phonological awareness interventions. The ndings also parametric statistics were found for the experimental
indicate that SLPs can be instrumental in providing group. The authors argued that the advantage of the
such interventions. classroom-based services for discourse skills rather than
Curriculum-based Language. A total of 8 studies expressive form was consistent with the focus of the
were found reporting curriculum level SLP-educator Language-in-the-Classroom program on communica-
partnerships aimed at enriching language models and tion eectiveness. In another of Prelocks studies
improving access to the curriculum through language (Kaufman et al., 1994), in a classroom already imple-
(see Table 1). Of these, 4 involved SLP-educator class- menting the Language-in-the-Classroom curriculum,
room collaborations for elementary school children an additional 3-week communication skills unit was
(Bland & Prelock, 1996; Kaufman, Prelock, Weiler, implemented involving SLP delivery of a once weekly
Creaghead, & Donnelly, 1994; Drew, 1998; Farber & 45-minute whole class session about adequacy of peer-
Klein, 1998), 1 involved SLP-educator consultative ser- or adult-directed explanations. Compared to a busi-
vices for secondary school students (Starling et al., ness-as-usual control class (n 16), the experimental
2012), and 3 involved SLP-educator consultative ser- class (n 16) improved on their ability to identify
vices for preschool children (Girolametto et al., 2012; poor quality explanations and provide justications.
Justice et al., 2010; Wilcox et al., 2011). All of the stu- Farber and Klein (1999) reported an SLP-educator
dies compared experimental and control groups except collaboration implementing a kindergarten-gr.1 curri-
one (Drew), but only 2 incorporated full randomization culum-based language and literacy program across
with either a balanced (Girolametto et al.) or unba- 6 schools over the academic year entitled, Maximizing
lanced design (Wilcox). Two additional studies incor- Academic Growth by Improving Communication
porated some randomization: Starling et al. at the (MAGIC). For 12 experimental classes (n 319), the
school level, and Farber and Klein at the whole class SLP and teacher met for weekly planning meetings
level. Baseline equivalence (Starling et al.; Girolametto and conducted weekly 2.25-hour co-teaching sessions
et al.; Justice et al.; Wilcox et al.) and attrition (Starling aimed at increasing oral language, improving commu-
et al., Girolametto et al.; Justice et al.; Wilcox et al.) nication, and improving literacy in the classroom. On a
were reported consistently for the studies involving pre- program-specic outcome measure, higher scores were
school children and secondary school students only. As observed for listening comprehension and writing but
well, blinding of assessors (Girolametto et al.; Justice not reading or spoken language for the experimental
et al.; Wilcox et al.; Starling et al.), reliability of out- group compared to business-as-usual control classes
come measures (Farber & Klein; Starling et al.; (n 253). Drew (1998) also reported a SLP-educator
Girolametto et al.; Justice et al.; Kaufman et al.; collaboration of a 12-hour summer program for poor
Wilcox et al.), and treatment delity (Starling et al.; readers aged 6-10 years targeting phonics, sight read-
Girolametto et al.; Justice et al.; Wilcox et al.) were ing, and reading for pleasure (see also, Fleming &
reported consistently for the studies involving pre- Forester, 1997, for a descriptive report of a similar pro-
school children and secondary school students only. It gram). Greater than 6-months age-equivalent reading
is clear that considerable caution is warranted in inter- growth was found for 69% of participants. Taken
preting these studies, especially those involving elemen- together, these studies provide some suggestive evi-
tary school children. dence of the benets of SLP-educator collaboration
In a small scale study of children with DLD in in improving oral language in kindergarten-to-gr.
grades 1 through 4, Bland and Prelock (1996) compared 4 classrooms.
language sample characteristics of 7 children receiving Starling et al. (2012) reported an interesting consul-
3045-minute pull-out speech and language sessions 1-2 tative-collaboration with secondary school teachers
times per week incorporating academic curriculum to 7 aimed at improving access of children with DLD to
children in whose class a transdisciplinary Language- the teachers instructional language. A group of 7 tea-
in-the-Classroom approach was being implemented. chers with at least 1 child with DLD in the classroom
Archibald 13

(DLD: n 21; 12-14 years old) participated in 10 SLP- Writing. Nelson and colleagues (Nelson & Van
delivered 50-minute weekly sessions aimed at reducing Meter, 2006; Nelson, Bahr, & Van Meter, 2004) have
the information processing load of oral and written championed a SLP-educator classroom collaboration
language instructions, and simplifying and explaining approach to addressing written expression. Their
unfamiliar vocabulary. The control group (who work provides case summaries, and some preliminary
received the same training after the study) was com- results for class groups. Nelsons Writing Lab Approach
prised of 6 teachers and 22 children with DLD in the involves regular SLP-teacher planning meetings, and
same age range. Reported level of use of the strategies joint written language support to the whole class 3
was higher for the trained than untrained teachers. times per week for a class period (45-60 minutes) in
Signicantly greater increases on standardized meas- the school computer room. Curriculum-based writing
ures of listening comprehension and written expression is targeted involving recursive writing (planning, orga-
(but not oral expression and reading comprehension) nizing, drafting, revising, editing, publishing, present-
were found for the children with DLD whose teachers ing), authentic projects, and oral and written language.
did, compared to did not, receive the training. The positive results reported provide practice-based
Although just one study, these ndings are compelling evidence for SLP-educator classroom collaboration
as a demonstration of eective SLP-educator collabor- around written expression in grades 1-5 (see Table 1).
ation with adolescent students with DLD.
Finally, the three studies implementing curriculum-
based language and literacy programs in preschools
Summary
(Girolametto et al., 2012; Justice et al., 2010; Wilcox School-based SLPs have the responsibility to support
et al., 2011) reported positive results. For the two stu- the academic, social, emotional, and vocational pro-
dies implementing a general use program for all chil- gress of children with communication disorders
dren either over 30 weeks with 11 experimental (n 66) (Ehren, 2000). In order to achieve this, SLPs are
and 9 business-as-usual (n 72) classes (Justice et al.) increasingly working collaboratively with teachers and
or with 10 trained (39 children) and 10 untrained (37 other school educators in the classroom, an approach
children) educators in workshops (4), class visits (3), serving to bring together respective expertise to achieve
and video feedback (Girolametto et al.), signicantly a more eective educational program for target chil-
higher language and emergent literacy skills on standar- dren and others. Establishing such collaborative rela-
dized (Justice et al.) or rating measures (Girolametto tionships can be challenging, but the importance of
et al.) were found for experimental compared to control success in this area cannot be underestimated.
groups. Wilcox et al.s Teaching Early Literacy and Unfortunately, the optimal service delivery option for
Language (TELLS) program was implemented for pre- school-based SLP provision remains poorly understood
school children with speech and language needs in 19 based on recent systematic reviews (Cirrin et al., 2010;
classes (n 80) compared to 10 business-as-usual McGinty & Justice, 2006) leading to the recommenda-
classes (n 38). In the TELLS program, 12 biweekly tion that clinicians rely on reason-based practice and
themes focusing on oral language and emergent literacy their own data to guide service delivery decisions
were taught by the teacher throughout the day after 22- (Cirrin et al.). Reason-based practice involves the imple-
hours of training and with the SLP providing 30-min- mentation of scientic thinking in practice (Stanovich &
utes of weekly, in-class (mentor) support. Standardized Stanovich, 2003). For example, SLPs use an empirical
language and emergent literacy measures revealed sig- approach when they form hypotheses about a childs
nicantly higher vocabulary, sentence length, and language processing struggles, implement and evaluate
phonological awareness (but not sentence complexity the eectiveness of a candidate strategy, and modify
or print concepts) for the experimental compared to strategies based on observations. No more applicable
control groups. As well, the experimental classes were to teaching than speech language pathology, Pearson
found to be more language rich on a rating measure of (1999) stated, We have a professional responsibility to
the classroom language and literacy environment. forge best practice out of the raw materials provided by
Compared to previous ndings of low use of language our current and most valid reading of research . . . (p.
strategies (Bickford-Smith et al., 2005), high use of dir- 245). In reason-based practice, approaches theoretically
ective language, and insucient language models for linked to a research-base are implemented with context-
language growth in preschools (Turnbull et al., 2009), ual sensitivity, and evaluated to determine the quality
ndings such as these studies involving SLP-educator and impact of the program (Stanovich & Stanovich,
collaboration in preschools lead Justice et al. (2009) to 2003). Such an approach is especially important when
conclude that the presence of a SLP-educator collab- the evidence base is lacking, as is clearly the case with
orative framework may be necessary to achieve high regards to service delivery options for school-based
quality language and literacy instruction. speech language pathology services. Given the highly
14 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

variable conditions across school systems, classrooms, References


professionals and students, it could be argued that the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2010).
evidence-base can never be expected to adequately cover Roles and responsibilities of Speech-Language
all eventualities making reason-based practice a perpet- Pathologists in schools. Ad Hoc Committee on the Roles
ual responsibility for the SLP. and Responsibilities of the School-Based Speech-Language
The present review summarized available evidence Pathologist.
c
descriptively in relation to SLP-educator classroom- Bauer, K. L., Iyer, S. N., Boon, R. T., & Fore, C. (2010). 20
based services targeting vocabulary, oral language, ways for classroom teachers to collaborate with Speech-
Language Pathologists. Intervention in School and Clinic,
phonological awareness, curriculum-based language,
45, 333337.
and writing. Consistent with the ndings from the sys- c
Baxter, S., Brookes, C., Bianchi, K., Rashid, K., & Hay, F.
tematic reviews (Cirrin et al., 2010; McGinty & Justice, (2009). Speech and language therapists and teachers work-
2006), there is reasonably compelling evidence that tar- ing together: Exploring the issues. Child Language
geted vocabulary and phonological awareness can be Teaching and Therapy, 25, 215234.
eectively taught through SLP-educator collaboration Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., & Kucan, L. (2013). Bringing
in the classroom. For oral language, evidence for an words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction: Second edition.
overall benet from direct classroom teaching of nar- New York, NY: Guilford.
b
rative skills is highly suggestive, although weaker out- Bickford-Smith, A., Wijjayatilake, L., & Woods, G. (2005).
comes for specic expressive language targets suggests a Evaluating the effectiveness of an early years language
hybrid approach involving both small group/individual intervention. Educational Psychology in Practice, 21,
161173.
intervention and classroom support might be most a
Bland, L.E., & Prelock, P.A. (1996). Effects of collaboration
benecial. With regards to the benets of a curricu-
on language performance. Journal of Childrens
lum-based consideration of language, the most compel- Communication Development, 17, 3137.
ling evidence exists for preschool programs. Cautious b
Boyle, J., McCartney, E., Forbes, J., & OHare, A. (2007). A
optimism is warranted for studies aimed at curriculum- randomized controlled trial and economic evaluation of
based language and writing for elementary and second- direct versus indirect and individual versus group modes
ary school students. It is hoped that this evidence, taken of speech and language therapy for children with primary
together, will inform the reason-based approach to language impairment. Health Technology Assessment, 11,
SLP-educator collaborations aimed at nding the best 1158.
b
service solutions for students with DLD and other com- Boyle, J., McCartney, E., OHare, A., & Forbes, J. (2008).
munication impairments. Predictors of responsivity to language intervention:
Findings from a RCT. In: 2008 British Psychological
Society Developmental Section Annual Conference.
Acknowledgements
Capacity Building Series. (May 2013). Inquiry-based learning.
This review article is based on a presentation by the author Secretariat Special Edition, #32. Student Achievement
at the 2016 School Services Symposium of the Ontario
Division, Ontario Ministry of Education. Retrieved from
Association of Speech Language Pathologists and
http://www.edu.gov.on.ca/eng/literacynumeracy/inspire/
Audiologists (OSLA), and was informed by an
@WeSpeechies twitter week based on the theme of SLP-edu- research/CBS_InquiryBased.pdf
c
cator collaboration curated by the author. Cirrin, F.M., Schooling, T.L., Nelson, N.W., Diehl, S.F.,
Flynn, P.F., Staskowski, M., Torrey, T.Z., &
Adamczyk, D.F. (2010). Evidence-based systematic
Declaration of conflicting interests
review: Effects of different service delivery models on
The author(s) declared no potential conicts of interest with communication outcomes for elementary school-age chil-
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this dren. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
article. 41, 223264.
Dixon, D. A. (2016). Missed session. . . Whats really missing?
Funding ASHA Leader, 21, 3031.
c
The author(s) received no nancial support for the research, Dockrell, J.E., & Lindsay, G. (2001). Children with specific
authorship, and/or publication of this article. speech and language difficultiesthe teachers perspective.
Oxford Review of Education, 27, 369394.
c
Dohan, M., & Schulz, H. (1998). The speech-language
Note pathologists changing role: Collaboration within the
1. Superscript alphabets in the References section indicate that classroom. Journal of Childrens Communication
the study was coded as (a) addressing classroom collabor- Development, 20, 918.
a
ation with empirical evidence, (b) addressing consultative Drew, M.F. (1998). Speech and language therapist-teacher
services, classroom instruction, or small group intervention collaboration in a literacy summer school. International
outside of the classroom with empirical evidence, and (c) Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
providing information, discussion, survey, or review data. 33(supp.): 581586.
Archibald 15

Dunn, C. (1983). A framework for generalization in disor- Joffe, V. (2011). Vocabulary Enrichment Programme. Milton
dered phonology. Journal of Childhood Communication Keynes: Speechmark.
Disorders, 7, 4658. Justice, L.M., McGinty, A., Guo, Y., & Moore, D. (2009).
c
Ehren, B.J. (2000). Maintaining a therapeutic focus and Implementation of responsiveness to intervention in early
sharing responsibility for student success; Keys to in-class- education settings. Seminars in Speech & Language, 30,
room speech-language services. Language, Speech, and 5974.
b
Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 219229. Justice, L.M., McGinty, A.S., Cabell, S.Q., Kilday, C.R.,
Ehren, B.J., & Nelson, N.W. (2005). The responsiveness to Knighton, K., & Huffman, G. (2010). Language & literacy
intervention approach and language impairment. Topics in curriculum supplement for preschoolers who are academi-
Language Disorders, 25, 120131. cally at risk: A feasibility study. Language, Speech, and
b
Ellis, L., Schlaudecker, C., & Regimbal, C. (1995). Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 161178.
Effectiveness of a collaborative consultation approach to Justice, L.M., Schmitt, M.B., Murphy, K.A., Pratt, A., &
basic concept instruction with kindergarten Children. Biancone, T. (2014). The robustness of vocabulary inter-
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26, vention in public schools: Targets and techniques
6974. employed in speechlanguage therapy. International
a
Farber, J.G., & Klein, E.R. (1999). Classroom-based assess- Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49,
ment of a collaborative intervention program with kinder- 288303.
garten and first grade students. LSHSS, 30, 8391. Kaiser, A. P. (1993). Parent-implemented language interven-
Finn, P. (2003). Addressing generalization and maintenance tion: An environmental system perspective. Enhancing
of stuttering treatment in the schools: A critical look. childrens communication: Research foundations for inter-
Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 153164. vention, 2, 6384.
c a
Fleming, J., & Forester, B. (1997). Infusing language Kaufman, S.S., Prelock, P.A., Weiler, E.M., Creaghead,
enhancement into the reading curriculum of disadvan- N.A., & Donnelly, C.A. (1994). Metapragmatic awareness
taged adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing of explanation adequacy: Developing skills for academic
Services in Schools, 28, 177180. success from a collaborative communication skills unit.
b
Gallagher, A.L., & Chiat, S. (2009). Evaluation of speech Language, Speech, and Hearning Services in Schools, 25,
and language therapy interventions for pre-school children 174180.
c
with specific language impairment: a comparison of out- Khan, N., & Paddick, S. (2014). First nations and Metis
comes following specialist intensive, nursery-based and no early literacy and language enrichment program.
intervention. International Journal of Language and Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and
Communication Disorders, 44, 616638. Audiology, 38, 194205.
a a
Gillam, S.L., Olszewski, A., Fargo, J., & Gillam, R.B. Koutsoftas, A.D., Harmon, M.T., & Gray, S. (2009). The
(2014). Classroom-based narrative & vocabulary instruc- effect of Tier 2 intervention for phonemic awareness in a
tion: Results of an early-stagge, nonrandomized compar- Response-to-Intervention model in low-income preschool
ison study. American Journal of Speech Language classrooms. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Pathology, 45, 204219. Schools, 40, 116130.
Gilliam, R.B., & Pearson, N.A. (2004). Test of Narrative Kohl, F.L., Wilcox, B.L., & Karlan, G.R. (1978). Effects of
Language: Examiners Manual. Austin, TX: Pro-ed. training conditions on the generalization of manual signs
b
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2012). with moderately handicapped students. Education and
Facilitating emergent literacy: Efficacy of a model that Training of the Mentally Retarded, 13, 327335.
c
partners speech-language pathologists & educators. Law, J., Lindsay, G., Peacey, N., Gascoigne, M., Soloff, N.,
American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 21, Radford, J., & Band, S. (2002). Consultation as a model
4763. for providing speech and language therapy in schools: A
Graner, P.S., Faggella-Luby, M.N., & Fritschmann, N.S. panacea or one step too far? Child Language Teaching and
(2005). An overview of responsiveness to intervention: Therapy, 18, 145163.
What practitioners ought to know. Topics in Language Lindsay, G., & Desforges, M. (1986). Research Supplement:
Disorders, 25, 93105. Integrated nurseries for children with special educational
a
Hadley, P.A., Simmerman, A., Long, M., & Luna, M. needs. British Journal of Special Education, 13, 6366.
c
(2000). Facilitating language development for inner-city Lindsay, G., & Dockrell, J. (2002). Meeting the needs of
children: Experimental evaluation of a collaborative, class- children with speech language and communication needs:
room-based intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing a critical perspective on inclusion and collaboration. Child
Services in Schools, 31, 280295. Language Teaching and Therapy, 18, 91101.
c b
Hartas, D. (2004). Teacher and speech-language therapist McCartney, E., Boyle, J., Ellis, S., Bannatyne, S., &
collaboration: Being equal and achieving a common Turnbull, M. (2011). Indirect language therapy for chil-
goal? Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20, 3354. dren with persistent language impairment in mainstream
Howlin, P. (1981). The results of a home-based language primary schools: Outcomes from a cohort intervention.
training programme with autistic children. British International Jouranl of Language & Communication
Journal of Disorders of Communication, 16, 7388. Disorders, 46, 7482.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act of 1990, 20 U.W.C. McIntosh, B., Crosbie, S., Holm, A., Dodd, B., & Thomas, S.
(1990). (2007). Enhancing the phonological awareness and
16 Autism & Developmental Language Impairments

language skills of socially disadvantaged pre-schoolers: An program. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
interdisciplinary programme. Child Language Teaching Schools, 26, 286292.
c
and Therapy, 23, 267866. Ritzman, M.J., Sanger, D., & Coufal, K.L. (2006). A case
c
McGinty, A.S., & Justice, L.M. (2006). Classroom-based study of a collaborative speech-language pathologist.
versus pull-out language intervention: An examination of Communication Disorders Quarterly, 27, 221231.
b
the experimental evidence. EBP Briefs, 1, 326. Roberts, J.E., Prizant, B., & McWilliam, R.A. (1995). Out-
b
Motsch, J., & Riehemann, S. (2008). Effects of Context- of-class versus in-class service delivery in language inter-
Optimization on the acquisition of grammatical case in vention: Effectis of communication interactions with
children with specific language impairment: An experi- young children. American Journal of Speech-Language
mental evaluation in the classroom. International Journal Pathology, 4, 8794.
b
of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 683698. Silliman, E.R., Bahr, R., Beasman, J., & Wilkinson, L.C.
Murphy, A., Franklin, S., Breen, A., Hanlon, M., (2000). Scaffolds for learning to read in an inclusion class-
McNamara, A., Bogue, A., & James, E. (2016). A whole room. LSHSS, 31, 265279.
a
class teaching approach to improve the vocabulary skills Smith-Lock, K.M., Leitao, S., Lambert, L., & Nickels, L.
of adolescents attending mainstream secondary school, in (2013). Effective intervention for expressive grammar in
areas of socioeconomic disadvantage. Child Language children with specific language impairment. International
Teaching and Therapy, 116. Journal of Language & Comunication Disorders, 48,
c
Myhill, D., & Warren, P. (2005). Scaffolds or straitjackets? 265282.
a
Critical moments in classroom discourse. Educational Spencer, T.D., Peterson, D.B., Slocum, T.A., & Allen, M.M.
Review, 57, 5569. (2015). Large group narrative intervention in Head Start
Nelson, N.W., Bahr, C.M., & Van Meter, A.M. (2004). The preschools: Implications for response to intervention.
writing lab approach to language instruction and interven- Journal of Early Childhood Research, 13, 196217.
tion. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Stanovich, P.J., & Stanovich, K.E. (2003). Using research and
a
Nelson, N.W., & Van Meter, A.M. (2006). Partnerships for reason in education: How teachers can use scientifically
literacy in a writing lab approach. Topics in Language based research to make curricular and instructional deci-
Disorders, 26, 5569. sions. RMN Research Corporation: Portsmounth, NH.
c b
Nippold, M.A. (2010). Back to school: Why the speech-lan- Starling, J., Munro, N., Togher, L., & Arciuli, J. (2012).
guage pathologist belongs in the classroom. Language, Training secondary school teachers in instructional lan-
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 377378. guage modification techniques to support adolescents
c
Nippold, M.A. (2011). Language intervention in the class- with language impairment: A randomized controlled
room: What it looks like. Language, Speech, and Hearing trial. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
Services in Schools, 42, 393394. 43, 474495.
c
Noell, G.H., & Witt, J.C. (1999). When does consultation Suleman, S., McFarlane, L., Pollock, K., Schneider, P.,
lead to intervention implementation? Critical issues for Leroy, C., & Skoczylas, M. (2014). Collaboration: More
research and practice. The Journal of Special Education, than working together: An exploratory study to deter-
33, 2935. mine effect of interprofessional education on awareness
b
Palincsar, A.S., Collins, K.M., Marano, N.L., & and application of models of specialized service delivery
Magnusson, S.J. (2000). Investigating the engagement by student speech-language pathologists and teachers.
and learning of students with learning disabilities in Canadian Journal of Speech Language Pathology and
guided inquiry science teaching. Language, Speech, and Audiology, 37, 298307.
Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 240251. Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching lan-
Pearson, P.D. (1999). A historically based review of prevent- guage to children with autism and other developmental dis-
ing reading difficulties in young children. Reading abilities. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts Inc.
Research Quarterly, 34, 231246. Swank, L., Invernizzi, M., & Juel, C. (1997). Phonological
c
Pershey, M.G., & Rapking, C.I. (2003). A survey of colla- Awareness and Literacy Screening. Part I: Phonological
borative speech-language service delivery under large case- Awareness. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia
load conditions in an urban school district in the United Press.
Sates. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Thompson, C. (1989). Generalization research in aphasia.
Audiology, 27, 211220. Clinical Aphasiology, 18, 195222.
a
Peterson, P. (2007). Promoting generalization and mainte- Throneberg, R.N., Calvert, L.K., Sturm, J.J., Paramboukas,
nance of skills learned via natural language teaching. A.A., & Paul, P.J. (2000). A Comparison of Service
The Journal of Speech and Language Pathology Applied Delivery Models Effects on Curricular Vocabulary Skills
Behavior Analysis, 2, 97138. in the School Setting. American Journal of Speech
Prelock, P.A. (2000). Multiple perspectives for determining Language Pathology, 9, 1020.
the roles of speech-language pathologists in inclusionary Tomlinson, C.A. (2000). Reconcilable differences? Standards-
classrooms. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in based teaching and differentiation. Educational
Schools, 31, 213218. Leadership, 58, 611.
c
Prelock, P.A., Miller, B.L., & Reed, N.L. (1995). Troia, G.A. (2005). Responsiveness to intervention: Roles
Collaborative partnerships in a language in the classroom for Speech-Language Pathologists in the prevention and
Archibald 17

a
identification of learning disabilities. Topics in Language Van Kleeck, A., Gillam, R.B., & McFadden, T. (1998). A
Disorders, 25, 106119. study of classroom-based phonological awareness training
Turnbull, K.P., Anthony, A.B., Justice, L., & Bowles, R. for preschoolers with speech and/or language disorders.
(2009). Preschoolers exposure to language stimulation in American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 7, 6576.
a
classrooms serving at-risk children: The contribution of Wilcox, M.J., Kouri, T.A., & Caswell, S.B. (1991). Early
group size and activity context. Early Education & language intervention: A comparison of classroom and
Development, 20, 5379. individual treatment. American Journal of Speech
United Nations Convention on the Rights of Persons with Language Pathology, 1, 4960.
b
Disabilities. (2006). Retrieved from http://www.un.org/dis- Wilcox, M.J., Gray, S.I., Guimond, A.B., & Lafferty, A.E.
abilities/convention/conventionfull.shtml (2011). Efficacy of the TELL language and literacy curri-
U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education culum for preschoolers with developmental speech and/or
Sciences, What Works Clearinghouse. (2013, March). language impairment. Early Childhood Research
What Works Clearinghouse: Procedures and Standards Quarterly, 26, 278294.
Handbook (Version 3.0). Retrieved from http://what- Zentall, S.S., & Lee, J. (2012). A reading motivation interven-
works.ed.gov tion with differential outcomes for students at risk for
a
Valdez, F.M., & Montgomery, J.K. (1997). Outcomes from reading disabilities, ADHD, and typical comparisons:
two treatment approaches for children with communica- Clever is and clever does. Learning Disability
tion disorders in head start. Journal of Childrens Quarterly, 35, 248259.
Communication Development, 18, 6571.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi