Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

a r t ic l e s

Separate elements of episodic memory subserved by


distinct hippocampalprefrontal connections
Gareth R I Barker1, Paul J Banks1, Hannah Scott2, G Scott Ralph3, Kyriacos A Mitrophanous3,
Liang-Fong Wong2, Zafar I Bashir1, James B Uney2 & E Clea Warburton1
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

Episodic memory formation depends on information about a stimulus being integrated within a precise spatial and temporal
context, a process dependent on the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Investigations of putative functional interactions
between these regions are complicated by multiple direct and indirect hippocampalprefrontal connections. Here application of a
pharmacogenetic deactivation technique enabled us to investigate the mnemonic contributions of two direct hippocampalmedial
prefrontal cortex (mPFC) pathways, one arising in the dorsal CA1 (dCA1) and the other in the intermediate CA1 (iCA1). While
deactivation of either pathway impaired episodic memory, the resulting pattern of mnemonic deficits was different: deactivation
of the dCA1mPFC pathway selectively disrupted temporal order judgments while iCA1mPFC pathway deactivation disrupted
spatial memory. These findings reveal a previously unsuspected division of function among CA1 neurons that project directly to the
mPFC. Such subnetworks may enable the distinctiveness of contextual information to be maintained in an episodic memory circuit.

Remembering a past episode or event depends on the successful recall projections from the hippocampus to the mPFC or by those from the
of information, not only about what happened but also where and when mPFC back to the medial temporal lobe (including the hippocam-
it happened1. The hippocampus is critical for episodic memory; specifi- pus) via polysynaptic pathways. The present study therefore addressed
cally, it is responsible for integrating these different types of informa- several questions. First, we determined the necessity of a hippocam-
tion (what, where and when)24. However, successful episodic memory palmPFC interaction for episodic memory retrieval, using an animal
also requires the hippocampus to operate in concert with areas of the model. Second, we examined whether the interaction was mediated
neocortex57, and of these, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is of especial by information transfer from the hippocampus to the mPFC via
interest8. Clinical studies have shown that lesions in the PFC disrupt the direct CA1mPFC connections.
episodic memory911, associative learning12 and memory for tempo- The first approach taken to investigate the importance of
ral order13. Functional imaging has revealed activation in the PFC hippocampalmPFC interactions in episodic memory was discon-
during episodic memory encoding and retrieval14. Further, there are nection of these two regions in rats using temporary lesions of the
reports of coactivation of the PFC and hippocampus during episodic hippocampus and mPFC. Episodic memory in rats was tested using
memory15 and electrophysiological recording studies in rodents have an episodic-like memory task, previously shown to be disrupted by
revealed increased coherence between the hippocampus and prefrontal bilateral hippocampal lesions19. The principle behind the disconnec-
cortex during spatial learning16. Together, such findings support the tion technique is that it prevents two regions from interacting in either
hypothesis that a functional interaction between the hippocampus and cerebral hemisphere by producing unilateral dysfunction of one of the
prefrontal cortex is critical for episodic memory. However, the precise regions in one hemisphere (for example, in the hippocampus) and
neural pathways that support such an interaction and the directional- unilateral dysfunction of the other region (for example, the mPFC) in
ity of the interaction are difficult to address through fMRI studies or the opposite hemisphere. If memory performance depends upon an
conventional lesion studies and hence remain poorly understood. interaction between the two regions, disconnecting the circuit in each
Animals, including rodents, encode and retrieve robust episodic- hemisphere should result in a deficit. To address the second question
like memories17,18 by forming associations between an object and of the role of direct CA1mPFC projections in episodic memory,
the location (where) and/or occasion (when) it was last encountered, we used a new pharmacogenetic technique to selectively and revers-
and performance of such episodic-like memory tasks have been ibly deactivate these pathways.
shown to be impaired by lesions of the hippocampus and mPFC19.
In rodents, a direct hippocampal projection to the mPFC arises from RESULTS
the CA1 subfield but, in addition, there are multiple indirect pro- Disconnecting hippocampus and PFC disrupts episodic memory
jections between the hippocampus and mPFC2022. Consequently, Ten rats had cannulae implanted into both the hippocampus and
it is not clear whether episodic memory function is maintained by mPFC. To disconnect the hippocampus and mPFC, NBQX, an AMPA

1School of Physiology, Pharmacology and Neuroscience, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK. 2School of Clinical Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK.
3Oxford BioMedica (UK) Ltd Medawar Centre, Oxford, UK. Correspondence should be addressed to E.C.W. (e.c.warburton@bristol.ac.uk).

Received 21 September 2016; accepted 1 December 2016; published online 9 January 2017; doi:10.1038/nn.4472

242 VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 nature NEUROSCIENCE


a r t ic l e s

receptor antagonist, was infused unilaterally into the mPFC in one a Sample phase 1 Sample phase 2 Test phase
hemisphere and unilaterally into the hippocampus in the opposite NL-TD FL-TR
hemisphere (NBQX contralateral) in half the animals. In the other
half (control group), NBQX was infused unilaterally into both regions 1h 1h

in the same hemisphere (NBQX ipsilateral). One week later the ani-
mals were retested using a crossover design, such that each animal NL-TR FL-TD
served as its own control. All infusions occurred before the memory
test phase (see Online Methods). b
The episodic-like memory task exploits rats spontaneous prefer-
ence for novel or less recently encountered stimuli or locations com-
pared to familiar or more recently encountered ones. This preference
is expressed behaviorally as an increase in the amount of exploration c ***
directed toward the less familiar stimulus23. The task thus assesses the *
animals ability to recall the temporal occurrence (when) and spatial ** NBQX ipsi
context (where) of a previously encountered object (what). In the ** NBQX contra
two sample phases of the task, rats explored two different objects in 0.6

different locations. After a retention delay, the test phase was con-

Proportion of time exploring


2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

ducted: all four objects were presented, but with the location of one **
0.4
object from each sample phase switched, and exploration of all four
objects was measured (Fig. 1a). Successful memory of all types of
information (what, where, when) will result in a pattern of preferential 0.2
exploration such that most exploration is directed to the object in a
novel location (NL) not recently encountered (temporally distant;
TD); that is, the novel locationtemporally distant object (NL-TD). 0.0
Exploration of the familiar locationtemporally distant object NL-TD NL-TR FL-TD FL-TR

(FL-TD) or the novel locationtemporally recent object (NL-TR) is Object

less than for the NL-TD object, while the least exploration is directed d ** **
0.6
to the familiar locationtemporally recent object (FL-TR)18.
Histological analyses confirmed the location of the cannula tips 0.4 NBQX ipsi
Discrimination ratio

in the mPFC or dorsal hippocampus (Fig. 1b). Animals in the NBQX contra
0.2
NBQX ipsilateral condition (n = 10) showed the expected pattern of
exploration (NL-TD > (FL-TD, NL-TD) > FL-TR) described above. 0.0
In contrast, animals in the NBQX contralateral condition (n = 10) were
0.2
significantly impaired in their ability to recall both when and where a
specific object had been previously encountered. A two-way repeated- 0.4
measures ANOVA with treatment and object as factors revealed a Where When
Memory type
significant treatment by object interaction (F(3,27) = 11.15, P = 0.001;
Fig. 1c). Analysis of the time spent exploring each of the objects in Figure 1 Episodic memory depends on a functional interaction between
the test phase revealed that the NBQX ipsilateral group spent a sig- the hippocampus and mPFC. (a) Scheme of the episodic memory task.
nificantly greater proportion of time exploring the NL-TD object than In each sample phase each animal is allowed to explore two different
objects each located in a unique position in the arena. In the test phase
the three other objects (NL-TR P = 0.006; FL-TD P = 0.014; FL-TR
the animal is presented with all four objects, but the location of one
P = 0.001), while the NBQX contralateral group showed no differences object from each sample phase is changed such that each object has a
in object exploration (NL-TD versus FL-TR P = 0.307; all other com- particular spatial-temporal association. (b) Cannula locations in mPFC
parisons P = 1.0). Finally, one-way ANOVA of the two memory com- and hippocampus. (c) Episodic memory performance was significantly
ponents (location and temporal) confirmed that the NBQX ipsilateral impaired in the NBQX contralateral group, in comparison to the NBQX
group had significantly higher levels of discrimination (location F(1,9) ipsilateral group (two-way ANOVA treatment by object interaction
= 43.21, P = 0.001; temporal F(1,9) = 10.55, P = 0.010) compared to the F(3,27) = 11.15, P = 0.001; main effect of object F(3,27) = 5.42,
P = 0.005). (d) Disconnection of the mPFC and hippocampus significantly
contralateral group (Fig. 1d). Total object exploration in any phase of impaired both the location and temporal components of episodic-like
the procedure did not differ between treatments, and thus two-way memory (one-way ANOVA). Thus NBQX ipsilaterally infused animals
ANOVA of object exploration across sample phase 1 and 2 revealed showed significantly higher levels of discrimination for both memory
no significant interaction between sample phase and treatment components compared to the NBQX contralaterally infused ones. Data
(F(1,9) = 0.65, P = 0.44) (Supplementary Table 1). These results reveal presented as mean s.e.m. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
that a rats ability to recall both the spatial and temporal information
concerning an objects prior occurrence relies on simultaneous neural input to mPFC. Anatomical tracing studies reveal a topographical
activity in the hippocampus and mPFC and a functional interaction organization of the hippocampalmPFC pathway along its dorso-ven-
between these two regions. tral and transverse axis, with efferents arising in the posterior region
Given the requirement for simultaneous information processing in of the dorsal hippocampus and in the intermediate hippocampus21,24.
the hippocampus and mPFC for episodic memory task performance, In light of this distribution of CA1 to mPFC neurons, we decided to
we next investigated the role of direct anatomical connections between test whether both projections (dorsal CA1 region to mPFC; dCA1
these two regions and the directionality of information processing; mPFC; intermediate CA1 to mPFC; iCA1mPFC) are involved in
that is, whether episodic memory depends on direct hippocampal episodic-like memory formation.

nature NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 243


a r t ic l e s

a Deactivation of CA1 pyramidal neurons by Daun02


To disrupt neural activity in the dCA1mPFC and iCA1mPFC
projections, we employed a new pharmacogenetic technique, a modi-
fication of the Daun02 inactivation method described previously25,26.
These reports have shown that it is possible to identify and manipulate
40 **
** neuronal activity by directing expression of the reporter gene lacZ to
** specific neuronal populations and then administering the prodrug
30 ** Daun02 via intracerebral cannulae. Daun02 is converted into dau-
norubicin by -d-galactosidase (-gal), the protein product of lacZ
No. of APs

20 ** (ref. 27), which results in a decrease in neuronal activity (deactiva-


tion) and disruption of behavior25,26.
To confirm that Daun02 attenuates activity of CA1 neurons, we
10
first expressed lacZ to induce -gal expression bilaterally in CA1
(see Online Methods). In each animal Daun02 was delivered into
0 CA1 in one hemisphere and vehicle into the other hemisphere, and
100 150 200 250 300
Current injection (pA)
horizontal hippocampal slices were prepared 3 d later to perform
in vitro whole-cell recordings. In CA1 neurons from the Daun02 hem-
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

b 45
** isphere, compared to neurons from the vehicle hemisphere (Fig. 2 and
Table 1), there was a decrease in the number of action potentials in
50
response to depolarizing voltage steps (Fig. 2a; main effect of treat-
AP threshold (mV)

Vehicle
ment F(1,33) = 12.83, P = 0.001), an increase in action potential thresh-
Daun02
55 old (Fig. 2b; F(1,33) = 7.68, P = 0.009) and an increase in the magnitude
of the afterhyperpolarizing potential (Fig. 2c; main effect of treatment
60 F(1,33) = 11.30, P = 0.002). Collectively, these effects will reduce the
excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons and provide a possible physi-
65
ological explanation for how connectivity between regions may be
Vehicle Daun02 disrupted by Daun02.
c
Infusate spread through dorsal and intermediate hippocampus
Next we confirmed that it was possible to restrict the spread of infu-
sate to posterior dorsal and intermediate hippocampus (dHPC and
iHPC, respectively). Infusions of fluorophore-conjugated musci-
5 ** ** **
mol into the dorsal CA1 revealed drug spread in dorsal and medial
* areas of CA1 that extended from 3.6 mm to 5.6 mm relative to
4
** bregma along the anteriorposterior axis and 1.0 mm to 3.4 mm from
mAHP amplitude (mV)

3 the midline along the mediolateral axis (Supplementary Fig. 1).


Calculation of the total area affected was 0.45 mm3. There was further
2 drug spread into the dentate gyrus and the overlying somatosensory
cortex; however, as these areas did not express -gal because they
1 lacked direct projections to mPFC (Fig. 3b), there is unlikely to be an
effect of Daun02 on neurons in these areas. Infusions into the inter-
0
5 10 15 20 25
mediate CA1 resulted in drug spread in posterior and lateral areas of
No. of APs CA1 (Supplementary Fig. 1) that extended from 5.2 mm to 7.0 mm
relative to bregma along the anterior posterior axis and from 3.8 mm
Figure 2 Daun02 attenuates activity of CA1 pyramidal neurons.
to 5.8 from the midline along the mediolateral axis. In total, the
(a) CA1 pyramidal neurons fire significantly fewer action potentials (APs)
in response to 500-ms current injections when treated with Daun02
infusion spread across an area of 0.35 mm 3. There was further drug
compared to vehicle (two-way ANOVA main effect of treatment F(1,33) = 12.83, spread into the dentate gyrus and the overlying posterior parietal
P = 0.001; main effect of current injection F(3.9, 1.8) = 778.61, and auditory cortical regions, again areas that did not show expres-
P = 0.0001; interaction between treatment and current injection F(3.9,1.8) = sion (Fig. 3c). Notably, the results reveal non-overlapping patterns of
1.47, P = 0.239). Inset shows representative traces following 100 pA fluorescence around the dorsal and intermediate infusion sites. In this
injection (scale bars: 200 ms, 20 mV). (b) The AP threshold of pyramidal context we considered the boundaries of these regions to be in line
cells treated with Daun02 was significantly more depolarized than that of
vehicle-treated neurons (one-way ANOVA F(1,33) = 7.68, P = 0.009).
with those described by Dong et al.28, where at the anterior level of the
(c) Five to 25 brief (2 ms) current injections, each resulting in a hippocampus (in our study in rats, 4.55.6 mm posterior to bregma)
single action potential, were applied at 50 Hz and resulted in medium the border between the dorsal and intermediate CA1 is parallel to the
afterhyperpolarizing potentials (mAHP) that were significantly larger in ventral edge of the lateral blade of the dentate gyrus.
Daun02-treated neurons than in vehicle-treated (two-way ANOVA main
effect of treatment F(1,33) = 11.30, P = 0.002; main effect of AP number Deactivation of projections to mPFC impairs memory
F(2.0, 66.8) = 8.61, P = 0.0001; interaction between treatment and AP
To assess the behavioral consequences of selective deactivation of
number F(2.0, 66.8) = 4.47, P = 0.015). Insets show representative mAHPs
following 10 APs (scale bars: 200 ms, 2 mV). Vehicle: N = 17 cells the dCA1mPFC and iCA1mPFC projections, a VSV-G/rabies-G
from 10 slices from 4 animals, Daun02: 18 cells from 10 slices from fusion envelope proteinpseudotyped equine infectious anemia virus
4 animals. Data presented as mean + s.e.m. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01. (EIAV)-based lentiviral vector expressing lacZ was injected into the

244 VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 nature NEUROSCIENCE


a r t ic l e s

Table 1 Intrinsic membrane properties of CA1 pyramidal neurons treated with vehicle or Daun02
Condition
Parameter Vehicle Daun02 F value P value
Resting membrane potential (mV) 75.6 0.4 73.7 0.6 F(1,33) = 6.92 0.013
Input resistance (M) 78.9 4.9 70.6 5.5 F(1,33) = 0.27 0.27
Time constant (ms) 18.7 0.7 17.5 1.2 F(1,33) = 0.72 0.40
Sag (%) 19.0 0.8 21.0 1.2 F(1,33) = 1.84 0.18
AP threshold (mV) 58.3 0.7 55.2 0.9 F(1,33) = 7.68 0.009
AP peak (mV) 34.7 1.6 33.4 2.6 F(1,33) = 0.19 0.67
AP width (mV) 0.75 0.02 0.75 0.03 F(1,33) = 0.04 0.84
AP maximum rate of rise (V s1) 516 28 482 27 F(1,33) = 0.76 0.39
For all parameters, vehicle n = 17, Daun02 n = 18. Statistical values reported for one-way between-subjects ANOVA; significant results are in bold. Data presented as mean
s.e.m. AP, action potential.

mPFC, resulting in retrograde transport of EIAV vector to the nucleus (Fig. 3a left). Daun02 was infused into the hippocampal subregions
and the subsequent expression of lacZ in a number of cell popula- via intracerebral cannulae aimed at the dCA1 (n = 12) or iCA1
tions that project directly to the mPFC, including the hippocampus (n = 12) to deactivate neurons expressing -gal in the direct
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

a EIAV-lacZ
EIAV-lacZ
Infusion of Daun02

mPFC HPC

b CT 4.5 4.8 5.2 5.6

CA1
DG

CA3

1,000 m

c 6.0 CT 6.3
CT
6.7 CT 7.0

CA1
DG

d *
** e * f *** g
0.8 0.8
***
*** ***
Discrimination ratio

Discrimination ratio
Proportion of time

**
Proportion of time

0.6 0.5 *
* ** 0.6 *** 0.5
exploring

exploring

0.4 0.4 *
0.0 0.0
0.2 0.2

0.0 0.5 0.5


0.0
NL-TD NL-TR FL-TD FL-TR Where When NL-TD NL-TR FL-TD FL-TR Where When
Object Memory type Object Memory type
Vehicle Daun02

Figure 3 Selective deactivation of the dCA1mPFC projection disrupts the temporal component of episodic memory while selective deactivation of
the iCA1mPFC projection disrupts the spatial component of episodic-like memory. (a) Expression of the lacZ construct in mPFC (left) and diagram
of the injection site of the viral construct and cannula placement (right). A VSV-G/rabies-G fusion envelope proteinpseudotyped lentiviral EIAV vector
expressing the reporter gene lacZ injected into the mPFC transduces neurons in anatomically connected regions, including the hippocampus.
Bilateral cannulae in the hippocampus enable the direct infusion of Daun02 to selectively deactivate the hippocampalmPFC projection as indicated.
(b,c) Histological sections showing cannula tract (CT) and transduced neurons in dCA1 (b) and iCA1 (c). Hippocampal areas CA3 and dentate gyrus
(DG) are also shown. Numbers refer to relative position from bregma; scale bars, 1,000 m. (d) Episodic memory was significantly disrupted by
deactivation of the direct dCA1mPFC projection (two-way ANOVA treatment-by-object interaction F(3,30) = 3.06, P = 0.043; main effect of object
F(3,30) = 14.93, P = 0.001; n = 11). (e) Analysis of location and temporal order components of the episodic memory performance by one-way ANOVA
shows that deactivation of the dCA1mPFC projection impaired discrimination of the temporal component (P = 0.213) and enhanced discrimination
of the location component (P = 0.013). (f) Episodic memory was significantly disrupted by deactivation of the CA1iHPC projection (two-way ANOVA
treatment-by-object interaction (F(3,33) = 8.35, P = 0.001; main effect of object F(3,33) = 65.03, P = 0.001; n = 12). (g) One-way ANOVA of the
location and temporal order components of the episodic memory performance shows that deactivation of the iCA1mPFC projection significantly
impaired the location (P = 0.001) but not the temporal component (P = 0.217). Data presented as mean s.e.m. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

nature NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 245


a r t ic l e s

Table 2 Object exploration in the episodic-like memory task after deactivation of either the direct dCA1mPFC projection or the direct
iCA1mPFC projection
Exploration in sample phase (s)
Experiment Condition Phase 1 Phase 2 Exploration in test phase (s)
dCA1mPFC Vehicle 88.3 11.2 83.4 11.0 46.9 4.7
Daun02 88.5 8.8 83.7 7.4 50.9 4.5
iCA1mPFC Vehicle 93.9 6.2 72.6 4.2 54.3 2.9
Daun02 99.0 4.3 77.8 6.6 51.1 4.8
Two-way ANOVA of object exploration levels across sample phases 1 and 2 revealed no significant interaction between sample phase and treatment (dHPCmPFC F(1,10) = 0.01,
P = 0.93; iHPCmPFC F(1,11) = 0.08, P = 0.99). Further, there was no significant difference in overall object exploration levels in the test phase in either group (dHPCmPFC
F(1,10) = 0.55, P = 0.48; iHPCmPFC F(1,11) = 0.35, P = 0.56). Data presented as mean s.e.m.

hippocampalmPFC pathways (Fig. 3c,d) while leaving other order aspect of the episodic-like memory task. Thus, in the dCA1
neuronal populations unaffected. deactivation group, post hoc analyses revealed that the Daun02 treated
Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA of episodic memory perform- animals showed a significant increase in exploration of the recently
ance following infusion of Daun02 into the dorsal or intermediate encountered object in a novel spatial location (NL-TR) (P = 0.037)
hippocampus revealed significant treatment-by-object interactions compared to that in control animals, indicating that the Daun02 ani-
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

in both conditions (dCA1mPFC, Fig. 3d, F(3,30) = 3.06, P = 0.043, mals detected the novel spatial location of the object irrespective of
n = 11; iCA1mPFC, Fig. 3f, F(3,33) = 8.35, P = 0.001, n = 12). Further the recent exploration of that object. In addition, the Daun02 group
post hoc comparisons revealed that deactivation of the dCA1mPFC showed significantly less exploration of the temporally distant object
pathway disrupted the animals ability to discriminate the temporal in a familiar location (FL-TD condition) compared to the control

a Sample phase Test phase

1h

b Sample phase 1 Sample phase 2 Sample phase 3 Sample phase 4 Test phase

1h 1h 1h 1h

c
1h 1h 1h 1h

d 1.0
e 1.0 f 1.0
***
***
***
Discrimination ratio
Discrimination ratio

Discrimination ratio

0.5 0.5
0.5

0.0 0.0 0.0

0.5 0.5 0.5


Spatial temporal order Object-in-place Object Spatial Object-in-place Object Spatial
temporal temporal temporal temporal
NBQX contra order order order order
NBQX ipsi
Vehicle Daun02

Figure 4 Deactivation of the dCA1mPFC projection selectively impaired object temporal order memory whereas selective deactivation of the
iCA1mPFC projection impaired object-in-place memory. (a) Object-in-place task with a 1-h retention delay. (b) Object temporal order task. (c) Spatial
temporal order task. (d) Spatial temporal order memory was significantly impaired following infusion of NBQX into the mPFC and hippocampus in
opposite hemispheres (NBQX contra) compared to infusions into the mPFC and hippocampus in the same hemisphere (NBQX ipsi) (one-way ANOVA;
P = 0.001). (e) Deactivation of the dCA1mPFC pathway by Daun02 impaired object temporal order performance (one-way ANOVA, P = 0.001; n = 12)
without affecting object-in-place (one-way ANOVA, P = 0.833; n = 11) or spatial temporal order memory (one-way ANOVA, P = 0.626; n = 11).
(f) Deactivation of the iCA1mPFC projection by Daun02 produced a selective impairment in object-in-place performance (one-way ANOVA,
P = 0.001; n = 12) but not object temporal order (one-way ANOVA, P = 0.383; n = 12) or spatial temporal order (one-way ANOVA, P = 0.774; n = 12).
Data presented as mean s.e.m. ***P < 0.001.

246 VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 nature NEUROSCIENCE


a r t ic l e s

Table 3 Object exploration in the object-in-place, object temporal order or spatial temporal order task was not affected following
deactivation of either the direct dCA1mPFC projection or the direct iCA1mPFC projection
Experiment Task Condition Exploration in sample phases (s) Exploration in test phase (s)
Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3 Phase 4
dCA1mPFC Object-in-place Vehicle 91.8 4.8 42.2 2.5
Daun02 92.5 3.7 41.3 4.5
Object temporal order Vehicle 64.3 5.4 55.7 4.2 60.4 8.3 64.2 8.3 33.8 3.5
Daun02 67.0 5.4 51.9 4.5 64.6 7.5 60.3 4.3 26.1 2.6
Spatial temporal order Vehicle 45.3 3.5 38.5 5.5 23.2 4.0 37.9 5.6 20.0 3.0
Daun02 47.8 6.2 35.8 4.6 37.5 7.1 43.0 6.2 22.1 2.3
iCA1mPFC Object-in-place Vehicle 88.8 3.9 36.8 2.0
Daun02 86.6 4.1 35.8 3.6
Object temporal order Vehicle 67.0 6.2 54.3 5.4 48.8 3.9 50.2 3.5 31.8 4.0
Daun02 70.6 4.7 56.7 3.2 48.8 4.0 53.8 4.2 29.1 3.1
Spatial temporal order Vehicle 48.1 4.5 44.0 5.7 35.8 3.9 35.3 3.3 29.7 3.5
Daun02 43.9 4.3 38.1 2.1 35.8 3.7 40.3 2.8 26.0 2.1
Object-in-place sample phase exploration dHPCmPFC F(1,10) = 0.04, P = 0.85; iHPCmPFC F(1,11) = 0.22, P = 0.65; test phase exploration dHPCmPFC F(1,10) = 0.06,
P = 0.82; iHPCmPFC F(1,11) = 0.06, P = 0.81); object temporal order sample phase exploration dHPCmPFC F(3,33) = 0.20, P = 0.90; iHPCmPFC F(3,33) = 0.09, P = 0.97),
test phase exploration dHPCmPFC (F(1,11) = 2.96, P = 0.11) or iHPCmPFC (F(1,11) = 0.6, P = 0.45); spatial temporal order sample phase exploration dHPCmPFC
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

F(3,33) = 1.26, P = 0.31; iHPCmPFC F(3,33) = 0.99, P = 0.42); test phase exploration dHPCmPFC (F(1,11) = 0.33, P = 0.58) and iHPCmPFC (F(1,11) = 0.95, P = 0.35).
Data presented as mean s.e.m.

group (Fig. 3d, P = 0.001). Separate calculations of the discrimina- order memory and spatial temporal order memory selectively
tion ratios for the temporal and location components of the episodic (Fig. 4ac). Previous studies have shown that object-in-place and
memory revealed no significant difference between the Daun02 and object temporal order memory depend on a hippocampalmPFC
control animals in the performance of the temporal component of the interaction29, and here we confirmed that spatial temporal order
task (Fig. 3e, F(1,10) = 1.77, P = 0.213), but a significant enhancement memory was also disrupted by disconnection of the hippocampus
in the performance of the location component of the task (Fig. 3e, and mPFC (Fig. 4d, one-way ANOVA F(1,9) = 24.14, P = 0.001, n = 10).
F(1,10) = 9.02, P = 0.013). Further analyses comparing the discrimi- Further analyses revealed that the ipsilaterally infused animals showed
nation ratios against chance performance revealed that the control a significant preference for exploring the location the object occupied
animals showed significant discrimination of both the location and earlier in the sequence (t(9) = 7.93, P = 0.001) whereas contralater-
temporal components (location t(10) = 3.71, P = 0.004; temporal t(10) ally infused animals explored both locations occupied by the object
= 4.07, P = 0.002) while the Daun02 animals showed significant dis- equally (t(9) = 0.83, P = 0.427) (Fig. 4d) Overall object exploration
crimination of the location component (t(10) = 6.05, P = 0.001) but levels were unaffected (Supplementary Table 2). Deactivation of the
not the temporal component (t(10) = 0.30, P = 0.771). dCA1mPFC projection significantly impaired performance in the
Post hoc analyses of the effects of disruption of the iCA1mPFC object temporal order task (Fig. 4e, one-way ANOVA main effect of
pathway revealed impairments in the spatial aspect of episodic-like drug, F(1,11) = 51.75, P = 0.001, n = 12) whereas object-in-place and
memory. Thus, in the iCA1mPFC deactivation group, the Daun02- spatial temporal order memory were unaffected (Fig. 4e, one-way
treated animals showed a significant decrease in their exploration of ANOVA object-in-place F(1,10) = 0.05, P = 0.833; spatial temporal
the recently encountered object in a novel spatial location (NL-TR, order F(1,10) = 0.25, P = 0.626; n = 11 for both). In direct contrast,
P = 0.018) compared to control animals and an increase in explora- selective deactivation of the iCA1mPFC projection significantly
tion of the temporally distant object in a familiar location (P = 0.001), impaired object-in-place performance (Fig. 4f, one-way ANOVA
indicating that following Daun02 treatment the animals could not main effect of drug, F(1,11) = 38.41, P = 0.001, n = 12) but not object
discriminate the change in the position of the object in the arena temporal order or spatial temporal order (Fig. 4f, one-way ANOVA
(Fig. 3f). Further separate one-way ANOVA of the location and tem- object temporal order F(1,11) = 0.83, P = 0.383; spatial temporal order
poral components (Fig. 3g) confirmed that there was a significant F(1,11) = 0.009, P = 0.774, both n = 12). Overall object exploration in
difference in the discrimination performance of the location compo- the sample or test phases of any of the tasks was not affected by Daun02
nent between the control and Daun02 treated animals (F(1,11) = 18.17, infusions into either the dHPC or iHPC (Table 3). Deactivation of the
P = 0.001) but no significant difference in the discrimination of dCA1mPFC projection or iCA1mPFC projection had no effect
the temporal component (F(1,11) = 1.71, P = 0.217). Total object on performance of a hippocampus-independent object recognition
exploration in any phase of the procedure, under either treatment, memory tasks29 (Fig. 5a,b,d,e) or a hippocampus-dependent object
did not differ (Table 2). location task29 (Fig. 5ce).
These results confirm a selective requirement for the dCA1mPFC
Distinct pathways mediate temporal order and place memory projection in the processing of object temporal order information and
In the episodic memory task, exploration of the four objects cannot for the iCA1mPFC projection in the processing of object spatial
be considered independent, as less exploration of one object may be information. That deactivation of the dCA1mPFC or iCA1mPFC
either a cause or consequence of more exploration of another object. projections did not affect spatial temporal memory reveals that this
In this study, disruption of the dCA1mPFC pathway reduced explo- process is critically dependent on alternative routes between the hip-
ration of the FL-TD object and increased exploration of the NL-TR pocampal and mPFC projections. Finally, as object location and object
object, and significantly disruption of the iCA1mPFC pathways recognition were not impaired and overall exploration levels in any
produced the opposite pattern. Hence the next experiment further of the tasks was not affected (Supplementary Table 3), our results
examined this dissociation using a battery of behavioral tasks to assess cannot be accounted for by a nonspecific disruption of hippocampal
object spatial memory (using an object-in-place task), object temporal function nor a general reduction in sensitivity to novelty.

nature NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 247


a r t ic l e s

a Sample
phase 1
Sample
phase 2
Sample
phase 3
Sample
phase 4
Test
phase
information appear to be distinct in CA1. The segregation of inputs
to the mPFC may allow further cognitive flexibility in the top-down
1h 1h 1h 1h processing of the mPFC; specifically, in the construction of contextual
representations used to guide subsequent retrieval. Indeed, functional
interaction between the hippocampus and mPFC has been implicated
b Sample Test in both encoding and retrieval8, but the relatively poor temporal spe-
phase phase cificity of the Daun02 technique means that it is not possible in the
1h
present study to draw conclusions concerning the relative contribu-
tion of the dCA1mPFC and iCA1mPFC pathways to these two
processes. The application of optogenetic techniques with more pre-
c Sample Test cise temporal control will enable such questions to be explored.
phase phase
Our results are consistent with neuronal recording studies show-
4h
ing that the CA1 contains neurons that encode temporal and spatial
information separately3133, but given that these studies have con-
fined their recording to the dorsal CA1, the anatomical distribution
of these different neuronal populations has yet to be fully described.
d 1.0 e 1.0
How might the topographical representation of object novelty and
Discrimination ratio

Discrimination ratio
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

spatial novelty arise? One possible explanation may be in the diversity


0.5 0.5
of inputs from the entorhinal cortex that terminate directly or indi-
rectly in CA1. Distal CA1that is, CA1 closest to the subiculum, the
0.0 0.0
regions targeted by the dCA1 cannulae in the present experiment
Recognition Location Recognition Location receives a direct input from the lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC)3436.
Memory type Memory type Functionally, the distal CA1 is critical for discrimination of the relative
Vehicle Daun02 novelty of objects37, and direct inputs from LEC to CA1 are impor-
Figure 5 Deactivation of either the dCA1mPFC or iCA1mPFC tant for recency memory38, consistent with the finding in the present
projection did not alter either object recognition or object location study that the dCA1mPFC projection facilitates object temporal
memory. (a) Object recognition task based on object temporal order task. order memory. The intermediate hippocampus, here shown to
(b) Object recognition task based on object-in-place task. (c) Object mediate object-spatial discrimination, receives input from cells in
location task with a 4-h retention delay. (d) Deactivation of the direct the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) that are highly tuned to spa-
dCA1mPFC projection did not affect object recognition (one-way ANOVA
F(1,10) = 0.49, P = 0.501; n = 11) or object location performance (one-
tial information39, and functionally the intermediate hippocampus
way ANOVA F(1,11) = 0.05, P = 0.829; n = 12). (e) Deactivation of the controls precise place learning40. Hence our data extend the notion
direct iCA1mPFC projection did not affect object recognition (one-way of a topographically organized corticohippocampal processing sys-
ANOVA F(1,11) = 0.04, P = 0.854; n = 12) or object location performance tems for object spatial and object temporal order memory beyond the
(one-way ANOVA F(1,11) = 0.01, P = 0.909; n = 12). Data presented as hippocampus to the mPFC.
mean + s.e.m. Given the necessity for a functional hippocampusmPFC interac-
tion for spatial temporal order memory, it was intriguing that selec-
DISCUSSION tive deactivation of either of the direct CA1mPFC pathways had no
The construction of an episodic memory requires that informa- effect on performance of this task. This apparent dissociation suggests
tion about an event (for example, an encounter with a stimulus) be that the information processing demands of the spatial temporal task
integrated with both the spatial and temporal context in which the are such that other routes between the hippocampus and mPFC are
encounter occurred. Here we have shown for the first time, to our critical. While there are no direct projections from the mPFC back to
knowledge, in a rodent model that the retrieval of episodic mem- the hippocampus, the mPFC may exert top-down control of the hip-
ory requires a functional interaction between the hippocampus and pocampus indirectlyfor example, via the LEC41 or via the nucleus
mPFC, consistent with evidence that spatio-temporal contextbased reuniens (NRe) of the thalamus22. Indeed, recent studies have revealed
object memory formation is achieved through mPFChippocampal a role for the NRe in contextual memory42, and we have found that
interactions29,30. Using a new pharmacogenetic technique to produce selective excitotoxic lesions of the NRe impair spatial temporal mem-
neuronal deactivation, we next demonstrated that inputs to the mPFC ory (G.R.I.B. and E.C.W., unpublished data). Further experiments
from dCA1 and iCA1 separately process the temporal aspects and spa- are now required to trace the precise anatomical pathways through
tial aspects of episodic-like memory, respectively. Hence information which the NRe may affect hippocampal and mPFC processing. It is
concerning an objects temporal and spatial attributes, which both also possible that the spatial temporal order task requires further hip-
contribute to episodic memory formation, is mediated by separate pocampal cortical interactions not explored here. In this task the same
direct CA1mPFC pathways. object is encountered in different locations within the same arena, and
Our results dissociated the contributions of dCA1 and iCA1 to hence the task depends on processing of competing local and global
object spatial and object temporal memory in an episodic memory cues. Evidence suggests that the CA1 receives local cue information,
task, but crucially also in a series of behavioral tasks that investigated such as that associated with the object and its immediate location
each memory dimension separately. Single-item object recognition in the arena, from LEC via CA3 (refs. 43,44), while global cue infor-
and object location memory were both unaffected, and hence the mation, such as that associated with the experimental room or the
dCA1mPFC and iCA1mPFC pathways have distinct and hierar- testing arena, is provided directly to CA1 from the MEC 45. For the
chical roles in relaying object associations. These results also extend resolution of conflict between the local cue and global cue informa-
theoretical accounts of the neural basis of episodic memory integration tion, as might be required during the spatial temporal task, ongo-
to include a role for the mPFC, as object-spatial and object-temporal ing interactions between CA1 and MEC are necessary44,45, but not

248 VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 nature NEUROSCIENCE


a r t ic l e s

direct interactions between the hippocampus and mPFC. Further 4. Eichenbaum, H.T. Memory Systems (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA,
1994).
studies will be required to examine this hypothesis. 5. Dickerson, B.C. & Eichenbaum, H. The episodic memory system: neurocircuitry and
The present demonstration of a separation of mnemonic processing disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 86104 (2010).
in the hippocampus fits with current models of functional segrega- 6. King, D.R., de Chastelaine, M., Elward, R.L., Wang, T.H. & Rugg, M.D. Recollection-
related increases in functional connectivity predict individual differences in memory
tion in the primate and rodent brain along the longitudinal axis of accuracy. J. Neurosci. 35, 17631772 (2015).
the hippocampus46. Models initially suggesting a sharp delineation 7. Watrous, A.J., Tandon, N., Conner, C.R., Pieters, T. & Ekstrom, A.D. Frequency-
between a dorsal cognitive and ventral emotional hippocampus 47 specific network connectivity increases underlie accurate spatiotemporal memory
retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 349356 (2013).
have been refined and now include multiple functional subdivisions 8. Preston, A.R. & Eichenbaum, H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in
of the hippocampus4850 based on gene expression and electrophysiol- memory. Curr. Biol. 23, R764R773 (2013).
9. Wheeler, M.A., Stuss, D.T. & Tulving, E. Frontal lobe damage produces episodic
ogy, as well as anatomical connections. This topographical division memory impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1, 525536 (1995).
of hippocampus function originating in parahippocampal cortex is 10. Duarte, A., Ranganath, C. & Knight, R.T. Effects of unilateral prefrontal lesions on
now clearly shown to extend beyond CA1 to mPFC, and our finding familiarity, recollection, and source memory. J. Neurosci. 25, 83338337
(2005).
of a separation of function in the output pathways of CA1 further 11. Nolde, S.F., Johnson, M.K. & Raye, C.L. The role of prefrontal cortex during tests
suggests that integration of information for episodic memory is not of episodic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 399406 (1998).
purely a function of the hippocampus. 12. Petrides, M. Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal-
and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 23, 601614 (1985).
The demonstration of parallel processing networks for the spatio- 13. Ekstrom, A.D., Copara, M.S., Isham, E.A., Wang, W.C. & Yonelinas, A.P. Dissociable
temporal context of an object from CA1 to mPFC thus addresses the networks involved in spatial and temporal order source retrieval. Neuroimage 56,
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

18031813 (2011).
nature of information processing via hippocampalmPFC interac- 14. Buckner, R.L., Kelley, W.M. & Petersen, S.E. Frontal cortex contributes to human
tions, as well as broader issues concerning the structure of episodic memory formation. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 311314 (1999).
memory formation and retrieval. Our results support the view that 15. Barredo, J., ztekin, I. & Badre, D. Ventral fronto-temporal pathway supporting
cognitive control of episodic memory retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 25, 10041019
the hippocampus is critical for the formation of representations of (2015).
an objects spatial and temporal context, and hence the hippocampus 16. Benchenane, K. et al. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing
acts as a key hub for episodic memory. However, episodic memory in the hippocampal- prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 66, 921936
(2010).
performance also depends on these representations being relayed to 17. Dere, E., Huston, J.P. & De Souza Silva, M.A. Integrated memory for objects, places,
the mPFC via functional subnetworks that enable differentiation of and temporal order: evidence for episodic-like memory in mice. Neurobiol. Learn.
Mem. 84, 214221 (2005).
the spatial and temporal contexts in which an item is represented. 18. Good, M.A., Barnes, P., Staal, V., McGregor, A. & Honey, R.C. Context- but not
Thus our data provide insight into the complexity of hippocampal familiarity-dependent forms of object recognition are impaired following excitotoxic
mPFC interactions. Such organization has not to our knowledge been hippocampal lesions in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 218223 (2007).
19. DeVito, L.M. & Eichenbaum, H. Distinct contributions of the hippocampus and
previously described and may lead to a new understanding of the medial prefrontal cortex to the what-where-when components of episodic-like
anatomy of episodic memory. memory in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 215, 318325 (2010).
20. Cond, F., Maire-Lepoivre, E., Audinat, E. & Crpel, F. Afferent connections of the
medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and subcortical afferents. J. Comp.
Methods Neurol. 352, 567593 (1995).
Methods, including statements of data availability and any associated 21. Hoover, W.B. & Vertes, R.P. Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial
prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct. Funct. 212, 149179 (2007).
accession codes and references, are available in the online version of 22. Varela, C., Kumar, S., Yang, J.Y. & Wilson, M.A. Anatomical substrates for direct
the paper. interactions between hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the thalamic
nucleus reuniens. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 911929 (2014).
Note: Any Supplementary Information and Source Data files are available in the 23. Ennaceur, A. One-trial object recognition in rats and mice: methodological and
online version of the paper. theoretical issues. Behav. Brain Res. 215, 244254 (2010).
24. Jay, T.M. & Witter, M.P. Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents
Acknowledgments in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of anterograde transport of
We thank J. Robbins for help with the experiments, L. Barnes (Oxford BioMedica) Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J. Comp. Neurol. 313, 574586 (1991).
25. Koya, E. et al. Targeted disruption of cocaine-activated nucleus accumbens neurons
for help with the vector plasmids, J.T. Brown (Exeter University) and C.A. Booth
prevents context-specific sensitization. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 10691073 (2009).
(Bristol University) for providing MATLAB scripts, M.W. Brown for comments 26. Cruz, F.C. et al. New technologies for examining the role of neuronal ensembles in
and discussions on the manuscript and A. Doherty for assistance with preparation drug addiction and fear. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 743754 (2013).
of the figures. The work was supported by the Biotechnology and Biology 27. Farquhar, D. et al. Suicide gene therapy using E. coli beta-galactosidase. Cancer
Sciences Research Council grants BB100310X/1 to E.C.W., J.B.U. and L.-F.W. and Chemother. Pharmacol. 50, 6570 (2002).
BB/L001896/1 to Z.I.B. and E.C.W. 28. Dong, H.-W., Swanson, L.W., Chen, L., Fanselow, M.S. & Toga, A.W. Genomic-
anatomic evidence for distinct functional domains in hippocampal field CA1.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 1179411799 (2009).
E.C.W., G.R.I.B., Z.I.B., P.J.B. and J.B.U. contributed to the study design; G.R.I.B., 29. Barker, G.R.I. & Warburton, E.C. When is the hippocampus involved in recognition
E.C.W., and H.S. contributed to the behavioral experiments and data collection; memory? J. Neurosci. 31, 1072110731 (2011).
30. Navawongse, R. & Eichenbaum, H. Distinct pathways for rule-based retrieval
J.B.U., L.-F.W., G.S.R., and K.A.M. designed, optimized and provided the viral
and spatial mapping of memory representations in hippocampal neurons.
constructs; G.R.I.B. conducted the surgery; P.J.B. performed and analyzed the J. Neurosci. 33, 10021013 (2013).
electrophysiology experiments. E.C.W. and G.R.I.B. wrote the manuscript. All 31. MacDonald, C.J., Carrow, S., Place, R. & Eichenbaum, H. Distinct hippocampal
authors discussed and commented on the manuscript. time cell sequences represent odor memories in immobilized rats. J. Neurosci. 33,
1460714616 (2013).
COMPETING FINANCIAL INTERESTS 32. Kraus, B.J., Robinson, R.J. II, White, J.A., Eichenbaum, H. & Hasselmo, M.E.
The authors declare no competing financial interests. Hippocampal time cells: time versus path integration. Neuron 78, 10901101
(2013).
Reprints and permissions information is available online at http://www.nature.com/ 33. Eichenbaum, H. Time cells in the hippocampus: a new dimension for mapping
reprints/index.html. memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 732744 (2014).
34. Witter, M.P., Wouterlood, F.G., Naber, P.A. & Van Haeften, T. Anatomical organization
1. Tulving, E. Organisation of Memory (Academic, New York, 1972). of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 911, 124
2. Diana, R.A., Yonelinas, A.P. & Ranganath, C. Imaging recollection and familiarity (2000).
in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 35. Naber, P.A., Lopes da Silva, F.H. & Witter, M.P. Reciprocal connections between
379386 (2007). the entorhinal cortex and hippocampal fields CA1 and the subiculum are in register
3. Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A.P. & Ranganath, C. The medial temporal lobe and with the projections from CA1 to the subiculum. Hippocampus 11, 99104
recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123152 (2007). (2001).

nature NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 249


a r t ic l e s

36. Knierim, J.J., Neunuebel, J.P. & Deshmukh, S.S. Functional correlates of the lateral 43. Lee, I., Yoganarasimha, D., Rao, G. & Knierim, J.J. Comparison of population
and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference coherence of place cells in hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3. Nature 430,
frames. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369, 20130369 (2013). 456459 (2004).
37. Ito, H.T. & Schuman, E.M. Functional division of hippocampal area CA1 via 44. Neunuebel, J.P., Yoganarasimha, D., Rao, G. & Knierim, J.J. Conflicts between local
modulatory gating of entorhinal cortical inputs. Hippocampus 22, 372387 and global spatial frameworks dissociate neural representations of the lateral and
(2012). medial entorhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 92469258 (2013).
38. Kinnavane, L., Amin, E., Horne, M. & Aggleton, J.P. Mapping parahippocampal 45. Knierim, J.J. & Neunuebel, J.P. Tracking the flow of hippocampal computation:
systems for recognition and recency memory in the absence of the rat hippocampus. Pattern separation, pattern completion, and attractor dynamics. Neurobiol. Learn.
Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 37203734 (2014). Mem. 129, 3849 (2016).
39. Henriksen, E.J. et al. Spatial representation along the proximodistal axis of CA1. 46. Poppenk, J., Evensmoen, H.R., Moscovitch, M. & Nadel, L. Long-axis specialization
Neuron 68, 127137 (2010). of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 230240 (2013).
40. Bast, T., Wilson, I.A., Witter, M.P. & Morris, R.G.M. From rapid place learning to 47. Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E.I. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus.
behavioral performance: a key role for the intermediate hippocampus. PLoS Biol. Hippocampus 8, 608619 (1998).
7, 07300746 (2009). 48. Fanselow, M.S. & Dong, H.-W. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally
41. Apergis-Schoute, J., Pinto, A. & Par, D. Ultrastructural organization of distinct structures? Neuron 65, 719 (2010).
medial prefrontal inputs to the rhinal cortices. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 135144 49. Strange, B.A., Witter, M.P., Lein, E.S. & Moser, E.I. Functional organization of the
(2006). hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 655669 (2014).
42. Xu, W. & Sdhof, T.C. A neural circuit for memory specificity and generalization. 50. Igarashi, K.M., Ito, H.T., Moser, E.I. & Moser, M.-B. Functional diversity along the
Science 339, 12901295 (2013). transverse axis of hippocampal area CA1. FEBS Lett. 588, 24702476 (2014).
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

250 VOLUME 20 | NUMBER 2 | FEBRUARY 2017 nature NEUROSCIENCE


ONLINE METHODS and then counterstained with eosin and coverslipped with Vectashield (Vector
Subjects. All experiments were conducted in naive adult male Lister Hooded Laboratories, Burlingame, California).
rats (Charles River, UK, weighing 300350 g at the start of the experiments).
The animals were housed in pairs under a 12-h/12-h light/dark cycle (light phase Infusate spread through dorsal and intermediate CA1. Fluorophore-conju-
18:006:00 h). Behavioral training and testing were conducted during the dark gated muscimol (FCM) (muscimol, BODIPY TMR-X conjugate; Invitrogen
phase of the cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum throughout the UK) was infused to establish that the dorsal and intermediate CA1 infusions
experiment. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with United targeted distinct neuronal populations within the hippocampus. Animals
Kingdom Animals Scientific Procedures Act (1986) and associated guidelines. (n = 3) were implanted with bilateral guide cannula (as outlined in surgery)
All efforts were made to minimize any suffering and the number of animals used. targeting either the dorsal or intermediate CA1 region of the hippocampus (one
Each rat was randomly allocated to an experimental group before surgery. in each hemisphere). Animals were allowed 2 weeks to recover from surgery.
The infusion procedure was identical to that used for all other infusions (see
Stereotaxic surgery for cannula implantation. Each rat was anesthetized with infusion procedure); briefly, 0.5 l of FCM (0.5 mg/ml, 5% DMSO) was infused
isoflurane (induction 4%, maintenance 23%) and secured in a stereotaxic frame into each hemisphere at a rate of 0.25 l/min. Twenty minutes after the start of
with the incisor bar set at 3.3 mm below the interaural line. Four stainless steel the infusion the animals were anesthetized with Euthetal (Rhne Mrieux) and
guide cannulae (26 gauge, Plastics One, Bilaney, UK) were implanted bilaterally transcardially perfused (as described under Histology). Brains were sectioned
into the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex of each rat through burr (40 m), mounted onto slides, counterstained with DAPI and coverslipped with
holes in the skull at the following coordinates relative to the skull at bregma: Vectashield (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, California). Sections were imaged
hippocampus anteriorposterior (AP) 4.3 mm, mediallateral (ML) 2.5 mm, using a Leica DM500b microscope and Leica DFC300FX camera. Fluorescence
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

dorsalventral (DV) 2.8 mm from dura; medial prefrontal cortex AP +3. 2 mm, images of FCM spread were overlaid onto images of DAPI stain to allow ana-
ML 0.75 mm, DV 3.5 mm. The cannulae were anchored to the skull by stain- tomical localization of the FCM. Area of the hippocampus filled with FCM was
less steel skull screws (Plastics One, Bilaney, UK) and dental acrylic. Following measured (Qwin, Leica) every 160 m along the anteriorposterior axis of the
surgery, each animal was given fluid replacement therapy (5 ml saline, s.c.) and hippocampus and the total volume of the hippocampus filled with FCM was
analgesia (0.05 ml Vetgesic, i.m.), and was housed individually for 1 week after calculated. In addition, the extent of FCM spread in the anteriorposterior and
surgery. Animals were subsequently housed in pairs for the duration of the experi- the mediolateral axes was assessed for each infusion site.
ments. The animals were allowed to recover for at least 14 d before habituation to
the testing arena began. Between infusions, 33-gauge obturators (Plastics One, Infusion procedure. General infusion procedures were performed as previously
Bilaney, UK) were used to keep the cannulae patent. described52,53. NBQX (Ascent Scientific) dissolved in sterile 0.9% saline solu-
tion was infused at 1 mM per side at a volume of 0.5 l, over a 2-min period
Vector constructs. Lentiviral vectors based on the equine infectious anemia virus immediately before the test phase. Daun02 (Ascent Scientific custom synthesis)
(EIAV) were produced by transient transfection of human embryonic kidney was dissolved in a 45% wt/vol (2-hydroxypropryl)--cyclodextrin (Sigma) and
293T cells with three plasmids ((i) vector genome, encoding the lacZ gene, (ii) 2% DMSO solution (Fisher Scientific), and a volume of 0.5 l at 4 mg/ml was
optimized gag-pol packaging component and (iii) the pseudotyping plasmid infused over a 2-min period. Thus each hippocampus received 2 g of Daun02.
encoding a VSV-G/rabies-G fusion envelope glycoprotein) using Lipofectamine Vehicle infusion consisted of a 45% wt/vol (2-hydroxypropryl)--cyclodextran
2000 (Invitrogen, UK) according to the manufacturers instructions. Cell super- and 2% DMSO solution. Daun02 was infused 3 d before the start of behavioral
natants were harvested 2448 h after transfection and concentrated 2,000-fold testing, as described previously25.
using two centrifugation steps, a low-speed centrifugation at 6,000g for 16 h
at 4 C and ultracentrifugation at 50,000g for 90 min at 4 C. The vectors were Behavioral protocols (Figs. 14). Exploration occurred in a wooden open-
resuspended in a buffer containing tromethamine, NaCl, sucrose and man- topped arena 90 100 cm with walls 50 cm high and was video recorded for sub-
nitol. The titer of the vesicular stomatitis virus VSV-G/rabies-G pseudotyped sequent analysis. The stimuli presented were objects composed of Duplo blocks
EIAV-lacZ viral vector as determined by an integration (DNA) titer assay was (Lego UK Ltd, Slough, UK) that varied in shape, color and size (9 8 7 cm
4 108 transducing units/mL. to 25 15 10 cm) too heavy for the animal to displace.
Habituation. For 4 d before the commencement of behavioral testing the ani-
Stereotaxic surgery for deactivation of specific projections. Rats were anesthe- mals were placed in the arena for 5 min.
tized and secured in a stereotaxic frame as described above. Viral particles were Episodic memory task (Fig. 1a). The task involved three phases, two sample
delivered bilaterally into the medial prefrontal cortex (AP +3.2 mm, ML 0.5 mm, phases and one test phase, with 1 h delay between each phase. In each sample
DV 4.3 mm) in 2.0 l per hemisphere at a rate of 200 nL/min. Cannulae phase the animals explored two different objects in a unique location in the arena
were implanted to target either the dCA1, n = 12 (AP 4.3 mm, ML 2.5 mm, for 10 min. In the test phase (5 min) animals were presented with all four objects
DV 2.6 mm) or the iCA1, n = 12 (AP 6.3 mm, ML 5.3 mm, DV 4.0 mm) and but one object from each sample phase had switched location and thus possesses
secured to the skull as described. Animals were allowed to recover for 5 weeks a unique spatial-temporal representation: novel locationtemporally distant
before behavioral testing commenced. (NL-TD); familiar locationtemporally distant (FL-TD); novel location
temporally recent (NL-TR); familiar locationtemporally recent (FL-TR).
Histology. Following completion of the experiments, each rat was anesthetized with The time spent exploring each of the objects was recorded.
Euthetal (Rhne Mrieux) and perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered Object-in-place memory task (Fig. 4a). This task comprised a sample and test
saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde. After removal the brain was post-fixed phase separated by a 1-h delay. In the sample phase the rats explored four different
in paraformaldehyde for either 2 h (X-gal histochemistry) or 24 h (cresyl violet objects for 5 min. In the test phase (3 min), two of the objectsfor example, B
staining) before being transferred to 30% sucrose in 0.2 M phosphate buffer for and Dexchanged positions. The time spent exploring the two objects that had
48 h. Sections were either incubated in reaction buffer for X-gal histochemistry changed position was compared to the time spent exploring the two objects that
(see below) or stained with cresyl violet to verify cannula locations against standardized had remained in the same position.
sections of the rat brain51. Any mounting artifacts were removed from the Object temporal order memory task (Fig. 4b). This task involved four sample
histology images (Fig. 3b,c). phases (S1S4) and a test phase, each separated by 1 h. In each sample phase
the rats explored two copies of the sample object for 4 min. Different objects
X-gal histochemistry. Coronal sections (50 m) were incubated in the X- were presented in each sample phase. In the test phase (3 min) the rats were
gal reaction buffer (5 mM potassium ferricyanide (K3Fe(CN)6), potassium presented with objects from S2 and S3 and the time spent exploring each object
ferrocyanide (K4Fe(CN)6), 2 mM magnesium chloride (MgCl2), CA-630 was recorded.
0.02%, sodium deoxycholate (C24H39NaO4) 0.01%, and phosphate-buffered Spatial temporal order memory task (Fig. 4c). This task was identical to the
saline (PBS) for 6 h at 37 C. Sections were washed three times with PBS object temporal order memory task, except that the rats were exposed to the
and mounted onto chrom-alum-coated slides. Slides were left to dry and then same object in a series of different spatial locations in four sample phases (S14).

doi:10.1038/nn.4472 nature NEUROSCIENCE


In the test phase (3 min) the rats were presented with two copies of the object, and recordings were made from CA1 pyramidal neurons that were either GFP-
one in the S2 location and the other in the S3 location, and the time spent explor- positive or in a region of dense GFP labeling so as to maximize the probability
ing each object was recorded. that recorded cells expressed -gal. CA1 pyramidal neurons, selected on the basis
Object recognition task. To assess recognition memory the dCA1 group were of location of the soma in the stratum pyramidale and pyramidal morphology
tested for recognition memory performance in the temporal order memory task under oblique infrared imaging, were patch-clamped using 26 M borosilicate
(Fig. 5a). They were presented with the object presented in sample phase 2 and glass (GC150F-10; Harvard Apparatus) electrodes filled with potassium gluco-
a novel object. In the iCA1 group novel object recognition was tested in a modi- nate-based solution (120 potassium gluconate, 10 KCl, 40 HEPES, 0.5 EGTA,
fied version of the object-in-place task (Fig. 5b): four objects were presented in 0.3 Na-GTP, 2 Mg-ATP, 1 MgCl2, 2 NaCl; pH 7.25, 295 mOsm). Current-clamp
the sample phase, and two objects were replaced by novel objects at test; thus recordings were obtained using an Axon Multiclamp 700B amplifier, pClamp 10
the iCA1 group were presented with two novel objects and two familiar objects. acquisition software, filtered at 4 KHz and digitized at 100 kHz (Digidata 1322A;
As for the other tasks, the time spent exploring each object was recorded. Molecular Devices). Following recording of the resting membrane potential cur-
Object location task (Fig. 5c). In the sample phase the rat was exposed to two rent was injected such that membrane potential was 70 mV following post hoc
objects for 3 min. After a delay of 4 h the rat was place back in the arena, which subtraction of the liquid junction potential. To assess neuronal firing, cells were
contained an identical object from the sample phase in the same position as in injected with 500-ms pulses ranging from +100 to +300 pA steps (Fig. 2a and Table 2).
the sample phase and a fourth identical object in a novel location. The time spent To measure afterhyperpolarizations, a series (525) of brief (2 ms) +2,000 pA
exploring each object was recorded. steps were injected at a frequency of 50 Hz such that each pulse resulted in a single
action potential (Fig. 2c). To measure subthreshold membrane properties (Fig. 2b
Experimental design. A within-subject design was used to assess the effect of and Table 2), a 100 pA current injection was given for 500 ms.
2017 Nature America, Inc., part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.

disconnecting the hippocampus and mPFC or deactivation of a specific pathway


on each memory task tested. The cohort of animals that undertook the NBQX Statistical analysis. The sample size for each experiment was determined by
disconnection study was tested in the episodic-like memory task and the spa- previous studies conducted in both our and other laboratories. Power calcula-
tial temporal order task. Both cohorts that undertook the deactivation studies tions on previously reported data52,53 collected in our laboratory suggested that
(dCA1mPFC and iCA1mPFC) performed each of the memory tasks to achieve a power of 0.8, a group size of eight was required. Larger sample sizes
described. Each experiment consisted of two trials separated by a minimum of 7 were used to allow for maintenance of power should animals be excluded for
d. Each animal received either a control infusion (NBQX ipsilateral or vehicle) in cannula misplacement or blockage.
one of the trials and a treatment infusion (NBQX contralateral or Daun02) in the Memory performance between groups was compared using ANOVA analy-
other. Choice of infusate for each experiment was determined randomly by the ses using SPSS (IBM). Statistical analyses were designed using an assumption
person conducting the infusions. Infusates were counterbalanced across animals of normal distribution and similar variance, but this was not formally tested.
so that equal numbers of animals received control and treatment infusions in Performance in the episodic-like memory task was compared using a two-
each run of a task. Within each behavioral experiment the location and/or order way repeated-measures ANOVA with treatment and object as factors; post hoc
of object presentation was also counterbalanced. comparisons used a Bonferroni correction. Performance in all other tasks used
was compared using a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA with treatment as
Assessment of exploration in behavioral tasks. For all the spontaneous explora- the factor. In addition, to test whether each group of animals could significantly
tion tasks, the proportion of time each animal spent exploring each object was discriminate between objects or pairs of objects in each task, the discrimination
analyzed. Exploratory behavior was strictly defined as the animal directing its ratios of each condition was compared to zero (chance performance) using a one-
nose toward the object at a distance of <2 cm and was scored by the experimenter sample t-test (two-tailed). Cannula blockage resulted in the loss of an animal from
blind to the infusion status of each animal. the dCA1mPFC group before the object recognition memory test (as indicated
In the episodic memory task, for analysis of the location memory component the by fewer degrees of freedom in the quoted statistical tests). The significance of
following formula was used (NL-TD + NL-TR) (FL-TD + FL-TR)/(NL-TD + NL-TR the results was accepted at P < 0.05.
+ FL-TD + FL-TR). To analyze the temporal component, the following formula was Electrophysiological recordings were imported into MATLAB using code
used: (NL-TD + FL-TD) (NL-TR + FL-TR)/(NL-TD + NL-TR + FL-TD + FL-TR). from SourceForge (https://sourceforge.net/projects/libaxon/) and analyzed using
For the object-in-place, object temporal order and spatial temporal order tasks, code kindly provided by Jon T. Brown, Exeter University, UK (Supplementary
object discrimination was determined using a discrimination ratio, calculated as Software; see refs. 54,55 for details; values reported in Table 1 are subtracted sag
the difference in time spent by each animal exploring the novel compared to the and steady-state Rinput) and statistical tests using SPSS (IBM). Neuronal firing
familiar object divided by the total time spent exploring all objects. rates and afterhyperpolarization amplitude between groups were compared using
a two-way mixed-design ANOVA with treatment as a between-subjects factor
Acute slice preparation. Four animals were injected with 2 l of virus con- and current injection (neuronal firing) or number of action potentials (after-
taining equal parts EIAV-lacZ and EIAV-GFP into iCA1 implanted with bilat- hyperpolarization) as within-subject factors. Action potential threshold and all
eral cannulae in iCA1 as described above. Following at least 5 weeks recovery, of the intrinsic membrane properties were compared with a one-way between-
each animal was given an infusion of Daun02 into one hemisphere and vehicle subjects ANOVA with treatment as the factor.
into the other. Three days later animals were anesthetized with 4% isoflurane A Supplementary Methods Checklist is available.
and decapitated. Brains were rapidly removed and placed in 4 C oxygenated
(95% O2, 5% CO2) sucrose solution (in mM: 189 sucrose, 10 d-glucose, Data and code availability. The data that support the findings of this study are
26 NaHCO3, 3 KCl, 5 MgSO4, 0.1 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4). Horizontal hippocam- available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The code used
pal slices 350 m thick were prepared using a vibratome (7000smz-2, Campden to analyze the electrophysiological data is available in Supplementary Software.
Instruments). Slices were kept such that each hemisphere was separate and slices
were kept in order of being cut in 34 C aCSF (124 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 3 KCl,
51. Swanson, L.W. Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain (Elsevier, 1992).
1.4 NaH2PO4, 1 MgSO4, 10 d-glucose and 2 CaCl2) for 30 min and then at room 52. Barker, G.R.I. et al. The different effects on recognition memory of perirhinal kainate
temperature for at least another 30 min before use. The experimenter was blind and NMDA glutamate receptor antagonism: implications for underlying plasticity
to which hemisphere had received Daun02 and which vehicle. mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 26, 35613566 (2006).
53. Barker, G.R.I. & Warburton, E.C. NMDA receptor plasticity in the perirhinal and
prefrontal cortices is crucial for the acquisition of long-term object-in-place
Electrophysiology. Hippocampal slices were placed in a submerged recording associative memory. J. Neurosci. 28, 28372844 (2008).
chamber and perfused with 34 C aCSF at 2 ml min1. A cannula tract was observ- 54. Kerrigan, T.L., Brown, J.T. & Randall, A.D. Characterization of altered intrinsic
able in stratum pyramidale of some slices; experiments were performed from excitability in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells of the A-overproducing PDAPP
mouse. Neuropharmacology 79, 515524 (2014).
slices where cannula tracts were visible or from immediately adjacent slices only
55. Booth, C.A., Brown, J.T. & Randall, A.D. Neurophysiological modification of CA1
so as to ensure the cells recorded from had been previously exposed to Daun02 pyramidal neurons in a transgenic mouse expressing a truncated form of disrupted-
or vehicle. Fluorescence imaging was used to visualize cells expressing GFP in-schizophrenia 1. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 10741090 (2014).

nature NEUROSCIENCE doi:10.1038/nn.4472

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi