Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 7

Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Animal Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav

Sex-specic offspring discrimination reects respective risks and costs


of misdirected care in a poison frog
Eva Ringler a, b, *, Andrius Pasukonis c, Max Ringler b, Ludwig Huber a
a
Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University of Vienna, and University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
b
Department of Integrative Zoology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
c
Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

a r t i c l e i n f o
The ability to differentiate between one's own and foreign offspring ensures the exclusive allocation of
Article history: costly parental care to only related progeny. The selective pressure to evolve offspring discrimination
Received 21 October 2015 strategies is largely shaped by the likelihood and costs of offspring confusion. We hypothesize that males
Initial acceptance 24 November 2015 and females with different reproductive and spatial behaviours face different risks of confusing their own
Final acceptance 25 January 2016 with others' offspring, and this should favour differential offspring discrimination strategies in the two
Available online 11 March 2016 sexes. In the brilliant-thighed poison frog, Allobates femoralis, males and females are highly polygamous,
MS. number: 15-00898R terrestrial clutches are laid in male territories and females abandon the clutch after oviposition. We
investigated whether males and females differentiate between their own offspring and unrelated young,
Keywords: whether they use direct or indirect cues and whether the concurrent presence of their own clutch is
amphibians
essential to elicit parental behaviours. Males transported tadpoles regardless of location or parentage,
offspring discrimination
but to a lesser extent in the absence of their own clutch. Females discriminated between clutches based
parental care
sex differences
on exact location and transported tadpoles only in the presence of their own clutch. This sex-specic
tadpole transport selectivity of males and females during parental care reects the differences in their respective costs
of offspring confusion, resulting from differences in their spatial and reproductive behaviours.
2016 The Authors. Published on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour by Elsevier
Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

In species with parental care, the ability to recognize and Substantial tness benets of accurate offspring discrimina-
discriminate between one's own offspring and unrelated young can tion abilities can be expected particularly when the risk of mis-
have considerable tness consequences for both the caregiving directed care is high (i.e. the likelihood of mistaking unrelated for
parent and its progeny (Beecher, 1991; Sherman, Reeve, & Pfennig, one's own offspring, Westneat & Sherman, 1993). This is the case,
1997). As parental behaviours are often very costly, parents in for example, when offspring are highly mobile, when foreign
noncooperatively breeding species should ensure that care is progeny are in close spatial proximity, under polygamy or when
directed exclusively to their own progeny (Duckworth, Badyaev, & cuckoldry is common. Several mechanisms have been proposed
Parlow, 2003; Queller, 1997; Trivers, 1972; but see also Larsson, to explain how parents may differentiate between their own
m, & Forslund, 1995). Thus in several species males
Tegelstro offspring and unrelated young: recognition alleles, phenotype
adjust the intensity of care according to the level of perceived pa- matching, assortative learning or spatial recognition (Komdeur &
ternity (bluegill sunsh, Lepomis macrochirus: Neff, 2003; eastern Hatchwell, 1999; Sherman et al., 1997). Discrimination mecha-
bluebirds, Sialia sialis: MacDougall-Shackleton & Robertson, 1998; nisms are also classied regarding the use of direct or indirect
pumpkinseed sunsh, Lepomis gibbosus: Rios-Cardenas & Webster, cues: direct recognition refers to parents recognizing specic
2005; blue-footed boobies, Sula nebouxii: Osorio-Beristain & phenotypic characteristics of their young (chemical: Head,
Drummond, 2001; but see also Kempenaers, Lanctot, & Doughty, Blomberg, & Keogh, 2008; Neff, 2003; Neff &
Robertson, 1998). Sherman, 2005; acoustic: Kno rnschild & Von Helversen, 2008;
visual: Lahti & Lahti, 2002; Underwood & Sealy, 2006); indirect
recognition occurs if parents use contextual cues such as spatial
location, frequency of encounters, larval age or external cues
associated with an offspring's location (Bonadonna, Cunningham,
* Correspondence: E. Ringler, Messerli Research Institute, University of
Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University of Vienna, and University of
Jouventin, Hesters, & Nevitt, 2003; Lank, Bouseld, Cooke, &
Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, A-1210 Vienna, Austria. Rockwell, 1991; Mller & Eggert, 1990; Waldman, 1987). Par-
E-mail address: eva.ringler@vetmeduni.ac.at (E. Ringler). ents should follow the simplest set of rules to optimize costs and

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.008
0003-3472/ 2016 The Authors. Published on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY
license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
174 E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179

benets between two kinds of possible errors in offspring transported to widely dispersed water bodies up to a distance of
recognition: (1) caring for unrelated progeny and (2) rejecting 200 m (E. Ringler et al., 2013).
their own offspring as recipients of care (Trivers, 1974). For Considering the differential reproductive strategies and the
example, indirect rather than direct recognition is expected to unequal frequency of parental care in male and female
evolve when offspring are not likely to move and are deposited in A. femoralis, differences in offspring discrimination strategies
spatially discrete clusters or inside a parent's territory (Sherman between the sexes can be expected. As clutches are deposited in
et al., 1997; Waldman, 1987). Sex-specic differences in spatial male territories, males can generally assume that all clutches
behaviours (e.g. territoriality versus high mobility) and/or inside their territory are their own offspring, and might therefore
reproductive strategies (e.g. choosing versus advertising sex, use a simple discrimination rule such as all clutches inside my
parental care versus offspring desertion) might thus favour territory are mine. In contrast, females have their clutches
different offspring discrimination strategies in males and dispersed across multiple male territories, which, in general, will
females. also contain clutches of other females. Thus, if females transfer
Behavioural differences between males and females are com- tadpoles when the male disappears, they should be much more
mon features across most species and across social/environmental selective than males. Tadpole transport is likely to be costly for
contexts. For example, several studies have demonstrated sex dif- the carrying individual in terms of energy investment, predation
ferences in species recognition abilities, probably resulting from risk and lost potential mating opportunities. During times of
the differential costs of mismating and hybridization or sex-specic absence other males might also try to take over the territory,
risks of predation (Saetre, Kr al, & Bures, 1997; Svensson, Karlsson, resulting in serious ghts as soon as the former territory owner
Friberg, & Eroukhmanoff, 2007). Regarding offspring discrimina- returns (E. Ringler, M. Ringler & A. Pasukonis, personal obser-
tion, sex differences have been shown in the razorbill, Alca torda, in vation). Transport of unrelated offspring would impose these
which care by each parent takes place at different stages of costs on either sex, but without yielding any benets, and thus
offspring development (Insley, Paredes, & Jones, 2003). Studies on should be avoided. Specically, we asked whether males and
offspring discrimination have mostly focused on highly social females discriminate between their own offspring and unrelated
vertebrate species with prolonged and complex parental care young and whether they use direct or indirect cues when
(Komdeur & Hatchwell, 1999; Krause & Caspers, 2012), which at the transporting tadpoles. Furthermore, we tested whether parental
same time are considered to possess high cognitive abilities and behaviours are only elicited when an individual is predisposed to
learning capacities (Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Kummer, Daston, perform parental care by the presence of its own clutch.
Gigerenzer, & Silk, 1997; but see also Holekamp, 2007). Little is
known about offspring discrimination abilities in less social verte- METHODS
brates, such as amphibians (but see Poelman & Dicke, 2007;
Stynoski, 2009). While general kin discrimination and recognition We performed a behavioural experiment under controlled
mechanisms have been demonstrated for several amphibian spe- laboratory conditions from August 2014 to March 2015 in the
cies (Blaustein & Waldman, 1992; Waldman, 2005), the majority of animal care facilities at the University of Vienna. Both wild-
studies have focused on differential behavioural responses towards caught frogs (N 19) from French Guiana and our own captive-
kin and nonkin among amphibian larvae. In many animals, bred individuals (N 29) were used for the experiments (see
including amphibians, spatial and reproductive behaviours differ Table A1). All tested individuals were adult and had successfully
considerably between the sexes. In species with parental care, produced/sired a clutch previously.
differential likelihood and costs of misdirected care might thus
drive different offspring discrimination strategies in males and Ethical Note and Housing
females.
We tested this hypothesis in Allobates femoralis, a Neotropical All frogs used in this study are part of the ex situ laboratory
poison frog with sex-specic reproductive strategies and spatial population of the animal care facilities at the University of Vienna.
behaviour. Males defend territories of about 150 m2 (M. Ringler, Permissions for sampling and export of wild-caught frogs were
Ringler, Magan ~ a-Mendoza, & Ho dl, 2011) and announce territory obtained from the responsible French authorities (DIREN: Arrete n
ownership by a prominent advertisement call (Ho dl, Ame zquita, & 82 du 10.08.2012 and Arrete n 4 du 14.01.2013). All experimental
Narins, 2004; M. Ringler et al., 2011; M. Ringler, Ursprung, & Ho dl, procedures were in strict accordance with current Austrian law,
2009). Females occupy perches which are interspersed between approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Vienna, and
male territories (E. Ringler, Ringler, Jehle, & Hodl, 2012). Both sexes followed the ASAB/ABS guidelines for the treatment of animals in
are iteroparous and highly polygamous throughout the prolonged behavioural research and teaching. The experiments were nonin-
reproductive season (Ursprung, Ringler, Jehle, & Ho dl, 2011). Under vasive as they were based on behavioural observations alone and
optimal conditions in captivity females can produce a clutch every therefore do not fall under the Austrian Animal Experiments Act (x
8 days (Weygoldt, 1980). Courtship and mating occur in male ter- 2, Federal Law Gazette No. 114/2012).
ritories where terrestrial clutches are laid and fertilized in the leaf All experiments were performed in standard glass terraria of
litter (Montanarin, Kaefer, & Lima, 2011; E. Ringler et al., 2012; M. equal size (60  40 cm and 40 cm high) with identical equipment
Ringler et al., 2009; Roithmair, 1992). Females abandon the clutch and furnishing. The oor was covered with pebbles of expanded
and return to their perches immediately after oviposition; males clay, the back and side walls were covered with xaxim (plates
neither remain close to the clutches (i.e. egg guarding) nor provide made of dried tree fern stems) and cork mats, and the front was
any further care such as egg moistening or active predator defence. covered with fabric to prevent visual contact between neigh-
After 3 weeks of larval development the tadpoles are generally bouring terraria and disturbances during maintenance. All
transported by the father to nearby water bodies (E. Ringler, terraria contained half a coconut shell, a small plant and a branch
Pasukonis, Ho dl, & Ringler, 2013; Weygoldt, 1980). However, it as suitable shelters and calling positions. We provided oak leaves
has been shown that the mother takes over parental duties when as a substrate for oviposition, and a small glass bowl of 12 cm
the father disappears (E. Ringler, Pasukonis, Fitch, Huber, Ho dl, & diameter lled with approximately 35 ml of water for tadpole
Ringler, 2015). As soon as the parent positions itself on the clutch deposition. An automatic raining, heating and lighting system
the larvae wiggle onto the parent's back and are subsequently ensured standardized climatic conditions with similar
E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179 175

parameters to the natural conditions in French Guiana in all unrelated clutches, but transported their own clutches in 90% of
terraria. Frogs were fed with wingless fruit ies every second day. cases (9/10). Only in a single case (1/10) did a female transport both
Apart from the transfer of the mating partners to other terraria her own clutch and the unrelated one, while another female (1/10)
after oviposition in trials 2 and 3, no further disturbance refused to carry both clutches (Fig. 2b). Thus, in test 2 tadpole
happened during the experimental trials. transport of unrelated clutches also differed signicantly between
the sexes (Fisher's exact test: P 0.001). Females in general did not
Experimental Design transport tadpoles of unrelated clutches in tests 1 and 2 (no own
clutch: 0/10; with own clutch: 1/10). By contrast, in test 3 all fe-
Our experiment, with three test conditions, was designed to males (10/10) transported unrelated clutches when put in the
identify the use of direct and indirect cues for offspring discrimi- original location of their own clutch, while they did not transport
nation, as well as to determine whether the presence of a parent's any of their own clutches (0/10) that were moved from their orig-
own clutch is necessary to elicit parental care. In test 1 an unrelated inal location (Fig. 2c).
clutch (i.e. a clutch from another pair of frogs) was placed inside the
terrarium of an individual that had no clutch of its own at the time. DISCUSSION
In test 2 an unrelated clutch was added to the terrarium of an in-
dividual that already had its own clutch. The unrelated clutch was We found different offspring discrimination strategies in male
placed approximately 20 cm away from the parent's own clutch, and female A. femoralis during parental care. While males also
and the latter's location was not altered. In test 3 we replaced the transported unrelated tadpoles, females discriminated between
parent's own clutch with an unrelated clutch and moved the former their own offspring and unrelated young according to the exact
approximately 20 cm from the original location (see Fig. 1). We spatial location of the clutch.
matched the parent's own and unrelated clutches by develop- Males in general transported all tadpoles that were placed in-
mental stage (all between Gosner stages 13 and 17; Gosner, 1960) side their terraria. They even transported tadpoles in the absence of
and clutch size. Unrelated clutches did not differ from the parents' their own clutch (i.e. if they had not sired a clutch previously), but
own clutches in their number of tadpoles (own: mean - then they picked up unrelated tadpoles less often than males that
SD 14.2 4.3; unrelated: mean SD 14.7 5.9; paired t test: had their own clutch present. However, the fact that even 60% of
N 30, t29 0.44, P 0.663) in tests 2 and 3. those males without their own clutch transported unrelated tad-
As it is impossible to manipulate egg clutches directly without poles suggests a strong motivation in A. femoralis males to transport
substantial destruction, we always moved clutches together with all encountered larvae to water, at least all conspecic ones situated
the leaves on which they were deposited. In trial 2 the leaf with inside a male's territory.
the parent's own clutch was also slightly lifted and then placed In a recent study on tadpole transport behaviour in this spe-
back at its exact original location, to exclude handling biases. cies (E. Ringler et al., 2013) males were observed to occasionally
Individuals that participated in test 1 were kept isolated by transport clutches of neighbouring males (four of 119). These
removing the previous partner for at least 3 weeks prior to cases were probably caused by shifts in territory boundaries and
testing and until they had transported any remaining clutches. In show that, although rare, transport of unrelated tadpoles also
tests 2 and 3 we permitted pairs to mate and produce one clutch, occurs under natural conditions, at least in this study population
and a few days after oviposition the respective other partner was which had a density of about 23 males/ha (M. Ringler, Ho dl, &
removed and the unrelated clutch was added. To keep required Ringler, 2015). These eld observations are in line with the re-
sample sizes as small as possible, we started by conducting tests sults of the present study. We suggest that strong male territo-
1 and 2, while test 3 started only after we identied whether riality favoured the observed behavioural pattern in males. Males
males and females discriminate between their own and unre- actively defend their territories against male intruders (Narins,
lated clutches. Given that most males transported unrelated Grabul, Soma, Gaucher, & Ho dl, 2005; M. Ringler et al., 2011)
clutches in trials of both tests 1 and 2, we only used females in and clutches are exclusively laid within territory boundaries
test 3. All individuals were tested only once (N 46), or after a (Montanarin et al., 2011; Roithmair, 1992). Thus even when
break of at least 3 months before being tested again (N 2). clutches might have changed their position slightly due to
However, the latter two individuals were not used in the same naturally occurring disturbances, such as other animals tres-
test twice, neither were their data points directly compared in passing, males can still generally expect that all clutches inside
any statistical analysis. We checked terraria daily and recorded their territory are their own. Consequently, there is no need for
which clutches were transported to the water bodies. In cases males to discriminate between the clutches inside their territory.
where parents' own or unrelated clutches failed to develop, In turn, the costs of accidentally rejecting their own clutch would
suffered from fungus infection or occasional slug predation the presumably be higher than occasionally transporting unrelated
trial was stopped and excluded from further analyses. If clutches tadpoles (cf. brooding birds that accept parasitic eggs, Rothstein,
were not transported within 4 weeks after oviposition and dried 1975). However, the simple discrimination rule all clutches in-
up, this was counted as no transport. The ratios of successful side my territory are mine is only feasible when territories are
tadpole transport events versus no transport were then stable and shifts in boundaries are rare. We cannot exclude that
compared between tests and sexes, respectively, using Fisher's males actually recognize their own offspring but still decide to
exact test, which is particularly robust and conservative if sample transport all tadpoles they encounter. However, given the ex-
sizes are small. Alpha for rejection of null hypotheses was set a pected costs associated with tadpole transport in terms of energy
priori at P < 0.05. expenditure, predation risk, potential territory take-overs and
lost mating opportunities, we consider this scenario unlikely.
RESULTS Follow-up experiments in the eld are needed to corroborate
that males differentiate between clutches that are inside or
In test 1, 60% of males (6/10) but no female (0/10; Fig. 2a) outside their territory boundaries. Furthermore, future studies
transported tadpoles of unrelated clutches (Fisher's exact test: should investigate tadpole transport behaviour in natural pop-
P 0.011). In test 2, 90% of males (9/10; Fig. 2b) transported both ulations with high male densities, where territory shifts are
their own and unrelated clutches. Females did not transport probably more common.
176 E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179

(a) (b) (c)


Females
males

Original clutch location

Own clutch Unrelated clutch

Figure 1. Experimental design. (a) Test 1: unrelated clutches were placed inside the terrarium of males/females that had no own clutch at the same time; (b) test 2: unrelated
clutches were added to the terrarium of males/females that already had their own clutches; (c) test 3: unrelated clutches were added to the terrarium of females that already had
their own clutches, at the original location of the parent's own clutch, while the latter was moved approximately 20 cm from the original location.

Females only transported tadpoles when they had recently discrimination in A. femoralis (see also Schulte & Veith, 2014). In
produced a clutch and accepted clutches based on their exact recent studies, remarkable orientation and spatial learning abilities
location. Even their own clutches that were relocated by only 20 cm have been demonstrated for two dendrobatid species. In a visual
were not transported. As tadpoles cannot move between clutches, discrimination task Dendrobates auratus males used visual cues for
the exact spatial location of the clutch is probably sufcient to allow spatial orientation and were also able to update their visual asso-
mothers to approach their own offspring (cf. Sherman et al., 1997; ciations in a reversal learning task (Liu, Day, Summers, &
Waldman, 1987). The high polygyny and clutch deposition in the Burmeister, 2016). In A. femoralis males, the high accuracy and
males' territories can confront females with clutches of several precision during homing behaviour after experimental trans-
other females in close proximity to their own. The likelihood of location and the loss of this orientation ability in unfamiliar habitat
accidentally picking up the wrong clutch is therefore much higher indicate the relevance of spatial learning for exible navigation in
in females than in males. Consequently, establishing rules for their local area (Pasukonis et al., 2013; Pasukonis, Warrington,
offspring discrimination is more complex for females. Moreover, Ringler, & Ho dl, 2014). Males repeatedly commute between home
misdirected care can be expected to have differential costs to the territories and tadpole rearing sites and presumably possess very
sexes. As males generally transport any clutch inside their territory, detailed knowledge of the surrounding area. Our study suggests
transporting an unrelated clutch only leads to additional costs for that females also have very precise spatial knowledge and
the carrying male, but will hardly impact his remaining offspring. In remember the locations of specic clutches over the course of
contrast, females only transport tadpoles as a specic behavioural weeks. This is particularly surprising as females will probably have
response triggered by their partner's disappearance. Consequently, produced one or two further clutches with other males during the
the accidental transport of an unrelated clutch would automatically development of the initial clutch (E. Ringler et al., 2012) and as
result in their own clutch not being transported, as from the fe- female tadpole transport is rare in the eld (7%, E. Ringler et al.,
male's perspective her compensatory duties have already been 2013). In this context, future studies should test whether the
fullled. Thus, the costs of misdirected care are much higher for simultaneous presence of multiple clutches reduces a female's
female than male A. femoralis. relocation ability. Further studies are also needed to identify
Peterson (2000) suggested that if the probability of nest whether females learn and remember certain features in their
confusion is high, direct egg recognition should be favoured. In our complex and changing habitat and how they manage to relocate
study, female A. femoralis did not discriminate between clutches their clutches after several weeks.
based on specic clutch-related cues, but were spatially accurate Previous studies in other dendrobatid species have shown that
when transporting clutches. We speculate that direct clutch Ranitomeya amazonica (previously Dendrobates ventrimaculata in
recognition is not feasible in A. femoralis because of insufcient this location; Poelman & Dicke, 2007) males do not discriminate
phenotypic variation between clutches (cf. Tibbets & Dale, 2007) between offspring and unrelated young, whereas Oophaga pumilio
and the ontogenetic change from zygotes to the hatched tadpoles. females do (Stynoski, 2009). Mothers of O. pumilio provisioned
Although scent-based offspring discrimination mechanisms are tadpoles regardless of tadpole identity, but were highly sensitive to
known from many vertebrate species (Johnston, Muller-Schwarze, location as they did not provision tadpoles that were moved 2 cm to
& Sorensen, 1999; Yamazaki, Beauchamp, Curran, Bard, & Boyse, an adjacent cup. Stynoski (2009) hypothesized that the reason why
2000), apparently odour cues of tadpoles are not used for offspring O. pumilio, but not R. amazonica, discriminate between offspring
E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179 177

10 10 10
(a) (b) (c)
8 8 8
Frequency

6 6 6

4 4 4

2 2 2

Figure 2. Number of clutches transported by males and females in (a) test 1, (b) test 2 and (c) test 3. Green bars indicate the number of the parents' own clutches and red bars the
number of unrelated clutches that were transported by male and female A. femoralis. White bars indicate the number of clutches that were not transported.

and unrelated young, is that O. pumilio invest more in their The high and low selectivity in males and females, respectively,
offspring, both transporting and feeding tadpoles, than regarding tadpole transport reect differences in the most reliable
R. amazonica which only transport tadpoles. Our ndings do not and efcient solutions for differentiating between their own and
support the idea that in A. femoralis offspring discrimination has unrelated tadpoles in the two sexes. Future studies should inves-
evolved in association with high levels of parental investment. tigate how different uncertainties, such as stability of male terri-
Offspring discrimination seems to be linked to the risk of choosing tories or higher levels of polygyny, inuence discrimination
the wrong clutch (e.g. when clutches of different individuals are strategies in males and females.
found in close spatial proximity), supporting the hypothesis that
offspring discrimination strategies are shaped by the likelihood and Acknowledgments
costs of misdirected care (cf. Beecher, 1991). In fact, this hypothesis
is also corroborated by the ndings of Stynoski (2009) and Poelman This study was nanced by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF)
and Dicke (2007), as female O. pumilio home ranges overlap, through the project P24788-B22 (PI E.R.). E.R. was funded by a
whereas male R. amazonica defend well-dened reproductive Hertha Firnberg fellowship from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF)
territories. T699-B24. A.P. was funded by the doctoral programme Cognition
In some species, offspring recognition is not used until some and Communication W1234-G17 (speaker Thomas Bugnyar).
minimum level of parental investment has occurred (Lefevre, Thanks to Rosanna Mangione for assistance with performing the
Montgomerie, & Gaston, 1998; Mateo, 2006; Mller & Eggert, experiments, to Nadja Kavcik-Graumann for help with designing
1990). In the present study males even transported tadpoles gures, to Corsin Mller and Barbara Fischer for valuable comments
when they had no clutches of their own, while females did so only on the manuscript and to Gesche Westphal-Fitch for language
when they had recently produced a clutch. Parental behaviour is editing. The authors declare no conict of interest.
presumably strongly inuenced by the hormonal status of an in-
dividual (Hunt, Hahn, & Wingeld, 1999; Kindler, Bahr, Gross, & References
Philipp, 1991; Neumann, 2008). Our ndings suggest that females
are predisposed to perform parental care only after oviposition and Beecher, M. D. (1991). Successes and failures of parenteoffspring recognition sys-
tems in animals. In P. H. Hepper (Ed.), Kin recognition (pp. 94e124). Cambridge,
probably also use temporal information for assessing parentage of a U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
given clutch. In turn, the continuous calling of A. femoralis males Blaustein, A. R., & Waldman, B. (1992). Kin recognition in anuran amphibians. An-
during the rainy season might induce and maintain a certain hor- imal Behaviour, 44, 207e221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(92)90027-7.
Bonadonna, F., Cunningham, G. B., Jouventin, P., Hesters, F., & Nevitt, G. A. (2003).
monal state in males, where they remain in a reproductive mode Evidence for nest-odour recognition in two species of diving petrel. Journal of
which includes mating but also tadpole transport. However, the Experimental Biology, 206(20), 3719e3722. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00610.
slightly lower tadpole transport rate in males that had not recently Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social complexity and
the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford
sired a clutch indicates hormonal and/or motivational changes are
University Press.
also induced by siring a clutch in males. Further studies, using Duckworth, R. A., Badyaev, A. V., & Parlow, A. F. (2003). Elaborately ornamented
larger sample sizes and possibly also hormonal analyses, are males avoid costly parental care in the house nch (Carpodacus mexicanus): a
proximate perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 55(2), 176e183.
needed to more accurately investigate motivational changes ac-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0671-7.
cording to tadpole transport induced by courtship and mating in Gosner, K. L. (1960). A simplied table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with
males and females. notes on identication. Herpetologica, 16, 183e190.
Head, M. L., Doughty, P., Blomberg, S. P., & Keogh, J. S. (2008). Chemical mediation of
reciprocal mothereoffspring recognition in the southern water skink (Eulam-
Conclusions prus heatwoli). Austral Ecology, 33(1), 20e28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-
9993.2007.01785.x.
Hodl, W., Ame zquita, A., & Narins, P. M. (2004). The role of call frequency and the
We have shown that different offspring discrimination strate-
auditory papillae in phonotactic behavior in male dart-poison frogs Epi-
gies have evolved in male and female A. femoralis, probably as a pedobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A,
response to different risks of misdirected care between the sexes. 190(10), 823e829. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00359-004-0536-1.
178 E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179

Holekamp, K. E. (2007). Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis. Trends in Poelman, E. H., & Dicke, M. (2007). Offering offspring as food to cannibals: ovipo-
Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 65e69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.003. sition strategies of Amazonian poison frogs (Dendrobates ventrimaculatus).
Hunt, K. E., Hahn, T. P., & Wingeld, J. C. (1999). Endocrine inuences on parental Evolutionary Ecology, 21(2), 215e227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10682-006-
care during a short breeding season: testosterone and male parental care in 9000-8.
Lapland longspurs (Calcarius lapponicus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, Queller, D. C. (1997). Why do females care more than males? Proceedings of the
45(5), 360e369. Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264(1388), 1555e1557. http://dx.doi.org/
Insley, S. J., Paredes, R., & Jones, I. L. (2003). Sex differences in razorbill Alca torda 10.1098/rspb.1997.0216.
parenteoffspring vocal recognition. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206(1), Ringler, M., Ho dl, W., & Ringler, E. (2015). Populations, pools, and peccaries:
25e31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00072. simulating the impact of ecosystem engineers on rainforest frogs. Behavioral
Johnston, R. E., Muller-Schwarze, D., & Sorensen, P. W. (1999). Advances in chemical Ecology, 26(2), 340e349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru243.
signals in vertebrates. New York, NY: Plenum. Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., Ho dl, W., & Ringler, M. (2015).
Kempenaers, B., Lanctot, R. B., & Robertson, R. J. (1998). Certainty of paternity and Flexible compensation of uni-parental care: female poison frogs take over when
paternal investment in eastern bluebirds and tree swallows. Animal Behaviour, males disappear. Behavioral Ecology, 26(4), 1219e1225. http://dx.doi.org/
55(4), 845e860. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0667. 10.1093/beheco/arv069.
Kindler, P. M., Bahr, J. M., Gross, M. R., & Philipp, D. P. (1991). Hormonal regulation of Ringler, E., Pasukonis, A., Hodl, W., & Ringler, M. (2013). Tadpole transport logistics
parental care behavior in nesting male bluegills: do the effects of bromocriptine in a Neotropical poison frog: indications for strategic planning and adaptive
suggest a role for prolactin? Physiological Zoology, 310e322. plasticity in anuran parental care. Frontiers in Zoology, 10(1), 67. http://
Kno rnschild, M., & Von Helversen, O. (2008). Nonmutual vocal motherepup dx.doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-10-67.
recognition in the greater sac-winged bat. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 1001e1009. Ringler, E., Ringler, M., Jehle, R., & Hodl, W. (2012). The female perspective of mating
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.018. in A. femoralis, a territorial frog with paternal care e a spatial and genetic
Komdeur, J., & Hatchwell, B. J. (1999). Kin recognition: function and mechanism in analysis. PLoS One, 7(6), e40237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/
avian societies. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 14(6), 237e241. http://dx.doi.org/ journal.pone.0040237.
10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01573-0. Ringler, M., Ringler, E., Magan ~ a-Mendoza, D., & Ho dl, W. (2011). Intrusion experi-
Krause, E. T., & Caspers, B. A. (2012). Are olfactory cues involved in nest recognition ments to measure territory size: development of the method, tests through
in two social species of Estrildid Finches? PLoS One, 7(5), e36615. http:// simulations, and application in the frog Allobates femoralis. PLoS One, 6(10),
dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036615. e25844. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025844.
Kummer, H., Daston, L., Gigerenzer, G., & Silk, J. (1997). The social intelligence hy- Ringler, M., Ursprung, E., & Ho dl, W. (2009). Site delity and patterns of short- and
pothesis. In P. Weingart, P. Richerson, S. D. Mitchell, & S. Maasen (Eds.), Human long-term movement in the brilliant-thighed poison frog Allobates femoralis
by nature: Between biology and the social sciences (pp. 157e179). Hillsdale, NJ: (Aromobatidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(9), 1281e1293. http://
Lawrence Erlbaum. dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-009-0793-7.
Lahti, D. C., & Lahti, A. R. (2002). How precise is egg discrimination in weaverbirds? Rios-Cardenas, O., & Webster, M. S. (2005). Paternity and paternal effort in the
Animal Behaviour, 63(6), 1135e1142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3009. pumpkinseed sunsh. Behavioral Ecology, 16(5), 914e921. http://dx.doi.org/
Lank, D. B., Bouseld, M. A., Cooke, F., & Rockwell, R. F. (1991). Why do snow geese 10.1093/beheco/ari076.
adopt eggs? Behavioral Ecology, 2(2), 181e187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ Roithmair, M. E. (1992). Territoriality and male mating success in the dart-poison
beheco/2.2.181. frog, Epipedobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae, Anura). Ethology, 92(4), 331e343.
Larsson, K., Tegelstro m, H., & Forslund, P. (1995). Intraspecic nest parasitism and http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1992.tb00970.x.
adoption of young in the barnacle goose: effects on survival and reproductive Rothstein, S. I. (1975). Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition: do birds know their
performance. Animal Behaviour, 50(5), 1349e1360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ own eggs? Animal Behaviour, 23, 268e278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0003-
0003-3472(95)80050-6. 3472(75)90075-5.
Lefevre, K., Montgomerie, R., & Gaston, A. J. (1998). Parenteoffspring recognition in Saetre, G. P., Kral, M., & Bures, S. (1997). Differential species recognition abilities of
thick-billed murres (Aves: Alcidae). Animal Behaviour, 55(4), 925e938. http:// males and females in a ycatcher hybrid zone. Journal of Avian Biology, 259e263.
dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0626. Schulte, L. M., & Veith, M. (2014). Is the response of a Neotropical poison frog
Liu, Y., Day, L. B., Summers, K., & Burmeister, S. S. (2016). Learning to learn: (Ranitomeya variabilis) to larval chemical cues inuenced by relatedness? Her-
advanced behavioural exibility in a poison frog. Animal Behaviour, 111, petological Journal, 24(3), 189e192.
167e172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.018. Sherman, P. W., Reeve, H. K., & Pfennig, D. W. (1997). Recognition systems. In
MacDougall-Shackleton, E. A., & Robertson, R. J. (1998). Condence of paternity and J. R. Krebs, & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioral ecology: An evolutionary approach
paternal care by eastern bluebirds. Behavioral Ecology, 9(2), 201e205. http:// (pp. 69e96). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell Scientic.
dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/9.2.201. Stynoski, J. L. (2009). Discrimination of offspring by indirect recognition in an egg-
Mateo, J. (2006). Development of individually distinct recognition cues. Develop- feeding dendrobatid frog, Oophaga pumilio. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1351e1356.
mental Psychobiology, 48(7), 508e519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.20156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.002.
Montanarin, A., Kaefer, I. L., & Lima, A. P. (2011). Courtship and mating behaviour of Svensson, E. I., Karlsson, K., Friberg, M., & Eroukhmanoff, F. (2007). Gender differ-
the brilliant-thighed frog Allobates femoralis from Central Amazonia: implica- ences in species recognition and the evolution of asymmetric sexual isolation.
tions for the study of a species complex. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 23(2), Current Biology, 17(22), 1943e1947. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.038.
141e150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2011.554884. Tibbets, E. A., & Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: it is good to be different.
Mller, J. K., & Eggert, A. K. (1990). Time-dependent shifts between infanticidal and Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22(10), 529e537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
parental behavior in female burying beetles a mechanism of indirect mother- j.tree.2007.09.001.
offspring recognition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27(1), 11e16. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.),
Narins, P. M., Grabul, D. S., Soma, K. K., Gaucher, P., & Ho dl, W. (2005). Cross-modal Sexual selection and the descent of man (pp. 136e179). Chicago, U.S.A.: Aldine.
integration in a dart-poison frog. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conict. American Zoologist, 14(1), 249e264.
of the United States of America, 102(7), 2425e2429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/icb/14.1.249.
pnas.0406407102. Underwood, T. J., & Sealy, S. G. (2006). Inuence of shape on egg discrimination in
Neff, B. D. (2003). Decisions about parental care in response to perceived paternity. American robins and gray catbirds. Ethology, 112(2), 164e173. http://dx.doi.org/
Nature, 422(6933), 716e719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature01528. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01143.x.
Neff, B. D., & Sherman, P. W. (2005). In vitro fertilization reveals offspring recog- Ursprung, E., Ringler, M., Jehle, R., & Ho dl, W. (2011). Strong male/male competition
nition via self-referencing in a sh with paternal care and cuckoldry. Ethology, allows for nonchoosy females: high levels of polygynandry in a territorial frog
111(4), 425e438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01075.x. with paternal care. Molecular Ecology, 20(8), 1759e1771. http://dx.doi.org/
Neumann, I. D. (2008). Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05056.x.
behaviours in both females and males. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 20(6), Waldman, B. (1987). Mechanisms of kin recognition. Journal of Theoretical Biology,
858e865. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01726.x. 128(2), 159e185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5193(87)80167-4.
Osorio-Beristain, M., & Drummond, H. (2001). Male boobies expel eggs when pa- Waldman, B. (2005). Kin recognition in amphibians. In P. G. Hepper (Ed.), Kin
ternity is in doubt. Behavioral Ecology, 12(1), 16e21. recognition (pp. 162e219). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Pasukonis, A., Ringler, M., Brandl, H. B., Mangione, R., Ringler, E., & Ho dl, W. (2013). Westneat, D. F., & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Parentage and the evolution of parental
The homing frog: high homing performance in a territorial dendrobatid frog behavior. Behavioral Ecology, 4(1), 66e77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/
Allobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae). Ethology, 119(9), 762e768. http:// 4.1.66.
dx.doi.org/10.1111/eth.12116. Weygoldt, P. (1980). Zur Fortpanzungsbiologie von Phyllobates femoralis (Bou-
Pasukonis, A., Warrington, I., Ringler, M., & Ho dl, W. (2014). Poison frogs rely on lenger) im Terrarium. Salamandra, 16(4), 215e226.
experience to nd the way home in the rainforest. Biology Letters, 10(11), Yamazaki, K., Beauchamp, G. K., Curran, M., Bard, J., & Boyse, E. A. (2000). Paren-
20140642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0642. teprogeny recognition as a function of MHC odortype identity. Proceedings of
Peterson, M. G. (2000). Nest, but not egg, delity in a territorial salamander. the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(19),
Ethology, 106(9), 781e794. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00584.x. 10500e10502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.180320997.
E. Ringler et al. / Animal Behaviour 114 (2016) 173e179 179

APPENDIX

Table A1
Detailed list of experimental trials

Trial Date Test Individual Sex Origin Own clutch No. of tp own No. of tp unrelated TT own TT unrelated

1 29 Jul 2014 2 femo07 M W 1 10 8 1 1


2 29 Jul 2014 2 femo25 M W 1 9 9 1 1
3 29 Jul 2014 2 femo40 M C 1 13 16 1 1
4 31 Jul 2014 1 femo05 M W 0 N/A 15 N/A 1
5 31 Jul 2014 2 femo03 M W 1 10 9 1 1
6 01 Aug 2014 1 femo13 M W 0 N/A 12 N/A 1
7 04 Aug 2014 1 femo10 F W 0 N/A 16 N/A 0
8 05 Aug 2014 1 femo28 F W 0 N/A 9 N/A 0
9 06 Aug 2014 1 femo04 F W 0 N/A 14 N/A 0
10 13 Aug 2014 1 femo22 M W 0 N/A 16 N/A 1
11 13 Aug 2014 1 femo23 M W 0 N/A 12 N/A 1
12 28 Aug 2014 2 femo06 F W 1 14 13 1 0
13 28 Aug 2014 2 femo24 M W 1 18 13 1 1
14 28 Aug 2014 2 femo43 M C 1 12 15 1 1
15 03 Sept 2014 2 femo47 F C 1 9 24 1 0
16 05 Sept 2014 1 femo51 M C 0 N/A 12 N/A 1
17 08 Sept 2014 2 femo16) F W 1 17 9 1 0
18 01 Oct 2014 2 femo32 F W 1 12 11 0 0
19 07 Oct 2014 2 femo29) F W 1 20 15 1 0
20 07 Oct 2014 2 femo30 F W 1 14 15 1 0
21 07 Oct 2014 2 femo39 F C 1 12 20 1 0
22 20 Oct 2014 2 femo55 F C 1 14 22 1 0
23 30 Oct 2014 1 femo78 F C 0 N/A 14 N/A 0
24 30 Oct 2014 1 femo81 F C 0 N/A 10 N/A 0
25 30 Oct 2014 1 femo86 F C 0 N/A 23 N/A 0
26 11 Nov 2014 2 femo66 F C 1 10 8 1 0
27 11 Nov 2014 2 femo35 M W 1 17 13 1 1
28 11 Nov 2014 2 femo45 M C 1 11 15 0 0
29 25 Nov 2014 1 femo68 F C 0 N/A 11 N/A 0
30 10 Dec 2014 1 femo12 F W 0 N/A 13 N/A 0
31 10 Dec 2014 1 femo42 M C 0 N/A 21 N/A 0
32 10 Dec 2014 1 femo44 M C 0 N/A 21 N/A 0
33 10 Dec 2014 1 femo59 M C 0 N/A 19 N/A 1
34 15 Dec 2014 1 femo74 M C 0 N/A 10 N/A 0
35 19 Dec 2014 2 femo49 F C 1 19 11 1 1
36 19 Dec 2014 2 femo48 M C 1 13 13 1 1
37 30 Dec 2014 1 femo02 M W 0 N/A 17 N/A 0
38 12 Jan 2015 3 femo16) F W 1 10 15 0 1
39 12 Jan 2015 3 femo29) F W 1 6 14 0 1
40 12 Jan 2015 3 femo57 F C 1 15 12 0 1
41 12 Jan 2015 3 femo63 F C 1 14 11 0 1
42 12 Jan 2015 3 femo73 F C 1 21 14 0 1
43 15 Jan 2015 1 femo67 F C 0 N/A 10 N/A 0
44 29 Jan 2015 1 femo76 F C 0 N/A 14 N/A 0
45 29 Jan 2015 3 femo75 F C 1 17 29 0 1
46 02 Feb 2015 3 femo53 F C 1 25 18 0 1
47 02 Feb 2015 3 femo69 F C 1 20 33 0 1
48 05 Feb 2015 3 femo80 F C 1 18 15 0 1
49 23 Feb 2015 2 femo84 M C 1 13 11 1 1
50 17 Mar 2015 3 femo83 F C 1 13 10 0 1

Trial: consecutive trial number; date: date when experiment was started; individual: ID of the tested individual, asterisks indicating multiply tested individuals; sex:
M male, F female; origin: w wild-caught individual, c captive-bred individual; own clutch: presence of the parent's own clutch, 0 no, 1 yes; no. of tp own: number
of tadpoles in the parent's own clutch; no. of tp unrelated: number of tadpoles in the unrelated clutch; TT own: tadpole transport of the parent's own clutch, 0 no, 1 yes; TT
unrelated: tadpole transport of the unrelated clutch, 0 no, 1 yes.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi