Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Bird vision

1 Extraocular anatomy
The eye of a bird most closely resembles that of the rep-
tiles. Unlike the mammalian eye, it is not spherical, and
the atter shape enables more of its visual eld to be in fo-
cus. A circle of bony plates, the sclerotic ring, surrounds
the eye and holds it rigid, but an improvement over the
reptilian eye, also found in mammals, is that the lens is
pushed further forward, increasing the size of the image
on the retina.[2]

With forward-facing eyes, the bald eagle has a wide eld of


binocular vision.

Visual elds for a pigeon and an owl

Most birds cannot move their eyes, although there are ex-
Vision is the most important sense for birds, since good
ceptions, such as the great cormorant.[3] Birds with eyes
eyesight is essential for safe ight, and this group has a
on the sides of their heads have a wide visual eld, useful
number of adaptations which give visual acuity superior
for detecting predators, while those with eyes on the front
to that of other vertebrate groups; a pigeon has been de-
of their heads, such as owls, have binocular vision and can
scribed as two eyes with wings.[1] The avian eye resem-
estimate distances when hunting.[4] The American wood-
bles that of a reptile, with ciliary muscles that can change
cock probably has the largest visual eld of any bird, 360
the shape of the lens rapidly and to a greater extent than
in the horizontal plane, and 180 in the vertical plane.[5]
in the mammals. Birds have the largest eyes relative to
their size in the animal kingdom, and movement is con-
sequently limited within the eyes bony socket.[1] In ad-
dition to the two eyelids usually found in vertebrates, it
is protected by a third transparent movable membrane.
The eyes internal anatomy is similar to that of other ver-
tebrates, but has a structure, the pecten oculi, unique to
birds.
Some bird groups have specic modications to their vi- The nictitating membrane of a masked lapwing
sual system linked to their way of life. Birds of prey have
a very high density of receptors and other adaptations that The eyelids of a bird are not used in blinking. Instead
maximise visual acuity. The placement of their eyes gives the eye is lubricated by the nictitating membrane, a third
them good binocular vision enabling accurate judgement concealed eyelid that sweeps horizontally across the eye
of distances. Nocturnal species have tubular eyes, low like a windscreen wiper.[6] The nictitating membrane also
numbers of colour detectors, but a high density of rod covers the eye and acts as a contact lens in many aquatic
cells which function well in poor light. Terns, gulls and birds when they are under water.[7] When sleeping, the
albatrosses are amongst the seabirds which have red or lower eyelid rises to cover the eye in most birds, with the
yellow oil droplets in the colour receptors to improve dis- exception of the horned owls where the upper eyelid is
tance vision especially in hazy conditions. mobile.[8]

1
2 2 ANATOMY OF THE EYE

The eye is also cleaned by tear secretions from the the smaller. This may be because the smaller bird has to
lachrymal gland and protected by an oily substance from start the day earlier because of weight loss overnight.[11]
the Harderian glands which coats the cornea and prevents Overnight weight loss for small birds is typically 5-10%
dryness. The eye of a bird is larger compared to the size and may be over 15% on cold winter nights.[11] In one
of the animal than for any other group of animals, al- study, robins put on more mass in their dusk feeding when
though much of it is concealed in its skull. The ostrich nights were cold.[12]
has the largest eye of any land vertebrate, with an axial Nocturnal birds have eyes optimised for visual sensitivity,
length of 50 mm (2 in), twice that of the human eye.[1] with large corneas relative to the eyes length, whereas di-
Bird eye size is broadly related to body mass. A study urnal birds have longer eyes relative to the corneal diame-
of ve orders (parrots, pigeons, petrels, raptors and owls) ter to give greater visual acuity. Information about the ac-
showed that eye mass is proportional to body mass, but tivities of extinct species can be deduced from measure-
as expected from their habits and visual ecology, raptors ments of the sclerotic ring and orbit depth. For the latter
and owls have relatively large eyes for their body mass.[9] measurement to be made, the fossil must have retained its
Behavioural studies show that many avian species focus three-dimensional shape, so activity pattern cannot be de-
on distant objects preferentially with their lateral and termined with condence from attened specimens like
monocular eld of vision, and birds will orientate them- Archaeopteryx, which has a complete sclerotic ring but
selves sideways to maximise visual resolution. For a pi- no orbit depth measurement.[13]
geon, resolution is twice as good with sideways monocu-
lar vision than forward binocular vision, whereas for hu-
mans the converse is true.[1] 2 Anatomy of the eye

The European robin has relatively large eyes, and starts to sing
early in the morning.
Anatomy of the avian eye

The performance of the eye in low light levels depends The main structures of the bird eye are similar to those
on the distance between the lens and the retina, and small of other vertebrates. The outer layer of the eye consists
birds are eectively forced to be diurnal because their of the transparent cornea at the front, and two layers of
eyes are not large enough to give adequate night vision. sclera a tough white collagen bre layer which sur-
Although many species migrate at night, they often col- rounds the rest of the eye and supports and protects the
lide with even brightly lit objects like lighthouses or oil eye as a whole. The eye is divided internally by the lens
platforms. Birds of prey are diurnal because, although into two main segments: the anterior segment and the
their eyes are large, they are optimised to give maximum posterior segment. The anterior chamber is lled with a
spatial resolution rather than light gathering, so they also watery uid called the aqueous humour, and the posterior
do not function well in poor light.[10] Many birds have an chamber contains the vitreous humour, a clear jelly-like
asymmetry in the eyes structure which enables them to substance.
keep the horizon and a signicant part of the ground in The lens is a transparent convex or 'lens shaped body with
focus simultaneously. The cost of this adaptation is that a harder outer layer and a softer inner layer. It focuses the
they have myopia in the lower part of their visual eld.[1] light on the retina. The shape of the lens can be altered by
Birds with relatively large eyes compared to their body ciliary muscles which are directly attached to lens capsule
mass, such as common redstarts and European robins sing by means of the zonular bres. In addition to these mus-
earlier at dawn than birds of the same size and smaller cles, some birds also have a second set, Cramptons mus-
body mass. However, if birds have the same eye size but cles, that can change the shape of the cornea, thus giving
dierent body masses, the larger species sings later than birds a greater range of accommodation than is possible
3

for mammals. This accommodation can be rapid in some which have been proposed to absorb stray light entering
diving water birds such as in the mergansers. The iris is a the bird eye to reduce background glare. Slight warm-
coloured muscularly operated diaphragm in front of the ing of pecten oculi due to absorption of light by melanin
lens which controls the amount of light entering the eye. granules has been proposed enhance metabolic rate of
At the centre of the iris is the pupil, the variable circular pecten that is suggested to help increase secretion of nu-
area through which the light passes into the eye.[2][14] trients into vitreous, eventually to be absorbed by avas-
cular retina of birds for improved nutrition.[16] Exra-high
enzymic activity of alkaline phosphatase in pecten oculi
has been proposed to support high secretory activity of
pecten to supplement nutrition of retina.[17]
The choroid is a layer situated behind the retina which
contains many small arteries and veins. These provide
arterial blood to the retina and drain venous blood. The
choroid contains melanin, a pigment which gives the inner
eye its dark colour, helping to prevent disruptive reec-
tions.

Hummingbirds are amongst the many birds with two foveae 3 Light perception

The retina is a relatively smooth curved multi-layered


370 nm 445 nm 508 nm 565 nm
structure containing the photosensitive rod and cone cells 1.0

with the associated neurons and blood vessels. The den-


sity of the photoreceptors is critical in determining the
maximum attainable visual acuity. Humans have about
Absorbance

200,000 receptors per mm2 , but the house sparrow has


0.5
400,000 and the common buzzard 1,000,000. The pho-
toreceptors are not all individually connected to the optic
nerve, and the ratio of nerve ganglia to receptors is impor-
tant in determining resolution. This is very high for birds;
the white wagtail has 100,000 ganglion cells to 120,000 0.0

photoreceptors.[2] 330 nm 400 nm 500 nm 600 nm 700 nm

Ultraviolet
Rods are more sensitive to light, but give no colour infor-
mation, whereas the less sensitive cones enable colour vi-
sion. In diurnal birds, 80% of the receptors may be cones The four pigments in estrildid nches' cones extend the range of
colour vision into the ultraviolet.[18][19]
(90% in some swifts) whereas nocturnal owls have almost
all rods. As with other vertebrates except placental mam- There are two sorts of light receptors in a birds eye,
mals, some of the cones may be double cones. These can
rods and cones. Rods, which contain the visual pigment
amount to 50% of all cones in some species.[15] rhodopsin are better for night vision because they are sen-
Towards the centre of the retina is the fovea (or the less sitive to small quantities of light. Cones detect specic
specialised, area centralis) which has a greater density of colours (or wavelengths) of light, so they are more impor-
receptors and is the area of greatest forward visual acuity, tant to colour-orientated animals such as birds.[20] Most
i.e. sharpest, clearest detection of objects. In 54% of birds are tetrachromatic, possessing four types of cone
birds, including birds of prey, kingshers, hummingbirds cells each with a distinctive maximal absorption peak.
and swallows, there is second fovea for enhanced sideways In some birds, the maximal absorption peak of the cone
viewing. The optic nerve is a bundle of nerve bres which cell responsible for the shortest wavelength extends to the
carry messages from the eye to the relevant parts of the ultraviolet (UV) range, making them UV-sensitive.[21] In
brain and vice versa. Like mammals, birds have a small addition to that, the cones at birds retina are arranged
blind spot without photoreceptors at the optic disc, under in a characteristic form of spatial distribution, known as
which the optic nerve and blood vessels join the eye.[2] hyperuniform distribution, which maximizes its light and
The pecten is a poorly understood body consisting of color absorption. This form of spatial distributions are
folded tissue which projects from the retina. It is well only observed as a result of some optimization process,
supplied with blood vessels and appears to keep the retina which in this case can be described in terms of birds evo-
supplied with nutrients,[1] and may also shade the retina lutionary history.[22]
from dazzling light or aid in detecting moving objects.[2] The four spectrally distinct cone pigments are derived
Pecten oculi is abundantly lled with melanin granules from the protein opsin, linked to a small molecule called
4 3 LIGHT PERCEPTION

retinal, which is closely related to vitamin A. When the pigments in birds give increased discrimination.[20]
pigment absorbs light the retinal changes shape and alters Each cone of a bird or reptile contains a coloured oil
the membrane potential of the cone cell aecting neu- droplet; these no longer exist in mammals. The droplets,
rons in the ganglia layer of the retina. Each neuron in which contain high concentrations of carotenoids, are
the ganglion layer may process information from a num- placed so that light passes through them before reaching
ber of photoreceptor cells, and may in turn trigger a nerve the visual pigment. They act as lters, removing some
impulse to relay information along the optic nerve for fur- wavelengths and narrowing the absorption spectra of the
ther processing in specialised visual centres in the brain. pigments. This reduces the response overlap between pig-
The more intense a light, the more photons are absorbed
ments and increases the number of colours that a bird can
by the visual pigments; the greater the excitation of each discern.[20] Six types of cone oil droplets have been iden-
cone, and the brighter the light appears.[20]
tied; ve of these have carotenoid mixtures that absorb
at dierent wavelengths and intensities, and the sixth type
has no pigments.[25] The cone pigments with the lowest
maximal absorption peak, including those that are UV-
sensitive, possess the 'clear' or 'transparent' type of oil
droplets with little spectral tuning eect.[26]
The colours and distributions of retinal oil droplets vary
considerably among species, and is more dependent on
the ecological niche utilised (hunter, sher, herbivore)
than genetic relationships. As examples, diurnal hunters
like the barn swallow and birds of prey have few coloured
droplets, whereas the surface shing common tern has
a large number of red and yellow droplets in the dorsal
retina. The evidence suggests that oil droplets respond to
natural selection faster than the cones visual pigments.[23]
Even within the range of wavelengths that are visible to
humans, passerine birds can detect colour dierences that
humans do not register. This ner discrimination, to-
gether with the ability to see ultraviolet light, means that
many species show sexual dichromatism that is visible to
birds but not humans.[27]
Migratory songbirds use the Earths magnetic eld, stars,
the Sun, and other unknown cues to determine their mi-
gratory direction. An American study suggested that mi-
gratory Savannah sparrows used polarised light from an
area of sky near the horizon to recalibrate their mag-
netic navigation system at both sunrise and sunset. This
Diagram of a bird cone cell suggested that skylight polarisation patterns are the pri-
mary calibration reference for all migratory songbirds.[28]
By far the most abundant cone pigment in every However, it appears that birds may be responding to sec-
bird species examined is the long-wavelength form of ondary indicators of the angle of polarisation, and may
iodopsin, which absorbs at wavelengths near 570 nm. not be actually capable of directly detecting polarisation
This is roughly the spectral region occupied by the direction in the absence of these cues.[29]
red- and green-sensitive pigments in the primate retina,
and this visual pigment dominates the colour sensitivity
of birds.[23] In penguins, this pigment appears to have 3.1 Ultraviolet sensitivity
shifted its absorption peak to 543 nm, presumably an
adaptation to a blue aquatic environment.[24] There are two types of colour vision in birds: violet
The information conveyed by a single cone is limited: by sensitive (VS) and ultraviolet sensitive (UVS).[30] UVS
itself, the cell cannot tell the brain which wavelength of birds have a visual pigment in the cones of their retinas
light caused its excitation. A visual pigment may absorb that absorbs UV light, allowing them to see the ultravi-
two wavelengths equally, but even though their photons olet portion of the spectrum. The major clades of birds
are of dierent energies, the cone cannot tell them apart, that have UVS vision are Palaeognathae (ratites and tina-
because they both cause the retinal to change shape and mous), Charadriiformes (shorebirds, gulls, and alcids),
thus trigger the same impulse. For the brain to see colour, Trogoniformes (trogons), Psittaciformes (parrots), and
it must compare the responses of two or more classes of Passeriformes (perching birds, representing more than
cones containing dierent visual pigments, so the four half of all avian species).[30]
5

ultraviolet light (insects, reptiles, and crustaceans have


UVS vision as well), some predators of UVS birds can-
not see ultraviolet light. This raises the possibility that
ultraviolet vision gives birds a channel in which they
can privately signal, thereby remaining inconspicuous to
predators.[35] However, recent evidence does not appear
to support this hypothesis.[36]

4 Perception

4.1 Contrast sensitivity

Contrast is dened as the dierence in brightness be-


tween two stimuli, divided by the sum of brightness of
both stimuli. Contrast sensitivity is the inverse of the
smallest contrast that can be detected, a contrast sensi-
tivity of 100 means that the smallest contrast that can
be detected is 1%. Birds have comparably low con-
trast sensitivity to mammals. Humans have been shown
to detect contrasts as low as 0.5-1%,[37] whereas most
birds tested have require ca 10% contrast to show a be-
havioural response.[38][39][40] A contrast sensitivity func-
tion describes an animals ability to resolve detect the con-
The common kestrel, like other raptorial birds, have a very low
trast of patterns of dierent spatial frequency (detail).
sensitivity to UV light. For stationary viewing experiments the contrast sensitiv-
ity is highest for a medium spatial frequency and lower
for higher and lower spatial frequencies.[41]
UVS vision can be useful for courtship. Many birds show
plumage patterns in ultraviolet that are invisible to the
human eye; some birds whose sexes appear similar to the 4.2 Movement
naked eye are distinguished by the presence of ultravi-
olet reective patches on their feathers. Male blue tits
have an ultraviolet reective crown patch which is dis-
played in courtship by posturing and raising of their nape
feathers.[31] Male blue grosbeaks with the brightest and
most UV-shifted blue in their plumage are larger, hold the
most extensive territories with abundant prey, and feed
their ospring more frequently than other males do.[20]
The bills appearance is important in the interactions of
the blackbird. Although the UV component seems unim-
portant in interactions between territory-holding males, A red kite ying at a bird feeding station in Scotland
where the degree of orange is the main factor, the female
responds more strongly to males with bills with good UV- Birds can resolve rapid movements better than humans,
reectiveness.[32] for whom ickering at a rate greater than 50 light pulse
UVS vision may also give birds an advantage in foraging cycles per second appears as continuous movement. Hu-
for food. The waxy surfaces of many fruits and berries mans cannot therefore distinguish individual ashes of a
reect UV light that might advertise their presence.[20] uorescent light bulb oscillating at 60 light pulse cycles
Common kestrels may be able to locate the trails of voles per second, but budgerigars and chickens have icker or
visually. These small rodents lay scent trails of urine and light pulse cycles per second thresholds of more than 100
faeces that could reect UV light, making them visible light pulse cycles per second which is safe to strictly say
to the kestrels, particularly in the spring before the scent not Hertz A Coopers hawk can pursue agile prey through
marks are covered by vegetation.[33] However, this view woodland and avoid branches and other objects at high
has been challenged by the nding of low UV sensitivity speed; to humans such a chase would appear as a blur.[5]
in raptors and weak UV reection of mammal urine.[34] Birds can also detect slow moving objects. The move-
While birds are not unique in their ability to perceive ment of the sun and the constellations across the sky is
6 5 VARIATIONS ACROSS BIRD GROUPS

imperceptible to humans, but detected by birds. The abil-


ity to detect these movements allows migrating birds to
properly orient themselves.[5]
To obtain steady images while ying or when perched on
a swaying branch, birds hold the head as steady as possible
with compensating reexes. Maintaining a steady image
is especially relevant for birds of prey.[5]

4.3 Edges and shapes


When an object is partially blocked by another, hu-
mans unconsciously tend to make up for it and com-
plete the shapes (See Amodal perception). It has how-
ever been demonstrated that pigeons do not complete oc-
cluded shapes.[42] A study based on altering the grey level
of a perch that was coloured dierently from the back-
ground showed that budgerigars do not detect edges based
on colours.[43] Hawk-eyed is a byword for visual acuity

4.4 Magnetic elds the deep central fovea of raptors can create a telephoto
optical system,[49] increasing the size of the retinal im-
The perception of magnetic elds by migratory birds age in the fovea and thereby increasing the spatial resolu-
has been suggested to be light dependent.[44] Birds move tion. Behavioural studies show that some large eyed rap-
their head to detect the orientation of the magnetic tors (Wedge-tailed eagle, Old world vultures) and have ca
eld,[45] and studies on the neural pathways have sug- 2 times higher spatial resolution than humans, but many
gested that birds may be able to see the magnetic medium and small sized raptors have comparable or lower
elds.[46] The right eye of a migratory bird contains pho- spatial resolution.[50][51][52][53][54][55]
toreceptive proteins called cryptochromes. Light excites
these molecules to produce unpaired electrons that inter-
act with the Earths magnetic eld, thus providing direc-
tional information.[47][48]
fovea

5 Variations across bird groups


pecten
5.1 Diurnal birds of prey
The visual ability of birds of prey is legendary, and the
keenness of their eyesight is due to a variety of factors.
Raptors have large eyes for their size, 1.4 times greater
than the average for birds of the same weight,[9] and
the eye is tube-shaped to produce a larger retinal im- Each retina of the black-chested buzzard-eagle has two
age. The resolving power of an eye depends both on foveae[56]
the optics, large eyes with large appertures suers less
from diraction and can have larger retinal images due The forward-facing eyes of a bird of prey give binocular
to a long focal length, and on the density of receptor vision, which is assisted by a double fovea.[2] The raptors
spacing. The retina has a large number of receptors adaptations for optimum visual resolution (an American
per square millimeter, which determines the degree of kestrel can see a 2mm insect from the top of an 18m
visual acuity. The more receptors an animal has, the tree) has a disadvantage in that its vision is poor in low
higher its ability to distinguish individual objects at a dis- light level, and it must roost at night.[1] Raptors may have
tance, especially when, as in raptors, each receptor is to pursue mobile prey in the lower part of their visual
typically attached to a single ganglion.[1] Many raptors eld, and therefore do not have the lower eld myopia
have foveas with far more rods and cones than the hu- adaptation demonstrated by many other birds.[1] Scaveng-
man fovea (65,000/mm2 in American kestrel, 38,000 in ing birds like vultures do not need such sharp vision, so
humans) and this provides these birds with spectacular a condor has only a single fovea with about 35,000 re-
long distance vision. It is proposed that the shape of ceptors mm2 . Vultures, however have high physiological
5.3 Water birds 7

activity of many important enzymes to suit their distant


clarity of vision.[57]
Like other birds investigated raptors do also have
coloured oil droplets in their cones.[52][53][58] The gener- fovea
ally brown, grey and white plumage of this group, and
the absence of colour displays in courtship suggests that
colour is relatively unimportant to these birds.[2]
In most raptors a prominent eye ridge and its feathers ex- pecten
tends above and in front of the eye. This eyebrow gives
birds of prey their distinctive stare. The ridge physically
protects the eye from wind, dust, and debris and shields
it from excessive glare. The osprey lacks this ridge, al-
though the arrangement of the feathers above its eyes
[56]
serves a similar function; it also possesses dark feathers in Each owls retina has a single fovea
front of the eye which probably serve to reduce the glare
from the water surface when the bird is hunting for its
vironment. There are few coloured oil droplets, which
staple diet of sh.[5]
would reduce the light intensity, but the retina contains a
reective layer, the tapetum lucidum. This increases the
5.2 Nocturnal birds amount of light each photosensitive cell receives, allowing
the bird to see better in low light conditions.[2] Owls nor-
mally have only one fovea, and that is poorly developed
except in diurnal hunters like the short-eared owl.[59]
Besides owls, bat hawks, frogmouths and nightjars also
display good night vision. Some bird species nest deep
in cave systems which are too dark for vision, and nd
their way to the nest with a simple form of echolocation.
The oilbird is the only nocturnal bird to echolocate,[61] but
several Aerodramus swiftlets also utilise this technique,
with one species, Atiu swiftlet, also using echolocation
outside its caves.[62][63]

5.3 Water birds

A powerful owl photographed at night showing reective tapeta


lucida

Owls have very large eyes for their size, 2.2 times greater
than the average for birds of the same weight,[9] and posi-
tioned at the front of the head. The eyes have a eld over- Terns have coloured oil droplets in the cones of the eye to improve
lap of 5070%, giving better binocular vision than for di- distance vision
urnal birds of prey (overlap 3050%).[59] The tawny owls
retina has about 56,000 light-sensitive rods per square Seabirds such as terns and gulls that feed at the surface
millimetre (36 million per square inch); although earlier or plunge for food have red oil droplets in the cones of
claims that it could see in the infrared part of the spectrum
their retinas. This improves contrast and sharpens dis-
have been dismissed.[60] tance vision, especially in hazy conditions.[2] Birds that
Adaptations to night vision include the large size of the have to look through an air/water interface have more
eye, its tubular shape, large numbers of closely packed deeply coloured carotenoid
[23]
pigments in the oil droplets
retinal rods, and an absence of cones, since cone cells than other species.
are not sensitive enough for a low-photon nighttime en- This helps them to locate shoals of sh, although it is un-
8 7 NOTES

certain whether they are sighting the phytoplankton on by aerial predators. Two aspects of its optical structure
which the sh feed, or other feeding birds.[64] suggest that the eye of this species is adapted to vision at
Birds that sh by stealth from above the water have to night. In the shearwaters eyes the lens does most of the
correct for refraction particularly when the sh are ob- bending of light necessary to produce a focused image
served at an angle. Reef herons and little egrets appear to on the retina. The cornea, the outer covering of the eye,
be able to make the corrections needed when capturing is relative at and so of low refractive power. In a diur-
sh and are more successful in catching sh when strikes nal bird like the pigeon, the reverse is true; the cornea is
are made at an acute angle and this higher success may be highly curved and is the principal refractive component.
The ratio of refraction by the lens to that by the cornea is
due to the inability of the sh to detect their predators.[65]
Other studies indicate that egrets work within a preferred 1.6 for the shearwater and 0.4 for the pigeon; the gure
for the shearwater is consistent with that for a range of
angle of strike and that the probability of misses increase [68]
when the angle becomes too far from the vertical leading nocturnal birds and mammals.
to an increased dierence between the apparent and real The shorter focal length of shearwater eyes give them a
depth of prey.[66] smaller, but brighter, image than is the case for pigeons,
Birds that pursue sh under water like auks and divers so the latter has sharper daytime vision. Although the
have far fewer red oil droplets,[2] but they have special Manx shearwater has adaptations for night vision, the ef-
exible lenses and use the nictitating membrane as an ad- fect is small, and it is likely that these
[68]
birds also use smell
ditional lens. This allows greater optical accommodation and hearing to locate their nests.
for good vision in air and water.[7] Cormorants have a It used to be thought that penguins were far-sighted on
greater range of visual accommodation, at 50 dioptres, land. Although the cornea is at and adapted to swim-
than any other bird, but the kingshers are considered to ming underwater, the lens is very strong and can com-
have the best all-round (air and water) vision.[2] pensate for the reduced corneal focusing when out of
water.[59] Almost the opposite solution is used by the
hooded merganser which can bulge part of the lens
through the iris when submerged.[59]

fovea

6 See also
Visual system
pecten

7 Notes
[1] Gntrkn, Onur, Structure and functions of the eye in
Sturkie (1998) 118
Each retina of the Manx shearwater has one fovea and an elon-
gated strip of high photoreceptor density[56] [2] Sinclair (1985) 88100
[3] White, Craig R.; Day, N; Butler, PJ; Martin, GR;
Tubenosed seabirds, which come ashore only to breed and Bennett, Peter (July 2007). Bennett, Peter, ed.
spend most of their life wandering close to the surface of Vision and Foraging in Cormorants: More like
the oceans, have a long narrow area of visual sensitivity Herons than Hawks?" (PDF). PLoS ONE. 2 (7): e639.
on the retina[1] This region, the area giganto cellularis, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000639. PMC 1919429 .
has been found in the Manx shearwater, Kerguelen petrel, PMID 17653266.
great shearwater, broad-billed prion and common diving- [4] Martin, Graham R.; Katzir, G (1999). Visual elds in
petrel. It is characterised by the presence of ganglion cells short-toed eagles, Circaetus gallicus (Accipitridae), and
which are regularly arrayed and larger than those found in the function of binocularity in birds. Brain, Behaviour
the rest of the retina, and morphologically appear similar and Evolution. 53 (2): 5566. doi:10.1159/000006582.
to the cells of the retina in cats. The location and cellu- PMID 9933782.
lar morphology of this novel area suggests a function in
[5] Jones, Michael P; Pierce Jr, Kenneth E.; Ward, Daniel
the detection of items in a small binocular eld projecting (April 2007). Avian vision: a review of form and
below and around the bill. It is not concerned primarily function with special consideration to birds of prey
with high spatial resolution, but may assist in the detec- (PDF). Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 16 (2): 6987.
tion of prey near the sea surface as a bird ies low over doi:10.1053/j.jepm.2007.03.012.
it.[67]
[6] Williams, David L.; Flach, E (March 2003).
The Manx shearwater, like many other seabirds, visits its Symblepharon with aberrant protrusion of the nic-
breeding colonies at night to reduce the chances of attack titating membrane in the snowy owl (Nyctea scandiaca)"
9

(PDF). Veterinary Ophthalmology. 6 (1): 1113. [20] Goldsmith, Timothy H. (July 2006). What birds see
doi:10.1046/j.1463-5224.2003.00250.x. PMID (PDF). Scientic American: 6975. Archived from the
12641836. original (PDF) on April 10, 2011.

[7] Gill, Frank (1995). Ornithology. New York: WH Free- [21] Wilkie, Susan E.; Vissers, PM; Das, D; Degrip, WJ; Bow-
man and Co. ISBN 0-7167-2415-4. OCLC 30354617. maker, JK; Hunt, DM (1998). The molecular basis for
UV vision in birds: spectral characteristics, cDNA se-
[8] Beebe, C. William (1906). The bird: its form and func- quence and retinal localization of the UV-sensitive vi-
tion. Henry Holt & Co, New York. p. 214. sual pigment of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undula-
tus)". Biochemical Journal. 330 (Pt 1): 54147. PMC
[9] Brooke, M. de L.; Hanley, S.; Laughlin, S. B. (February
1219171 . PMID 9461554.
1999). The scaling of eye size with body mass in birds.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 266 (1417): 405412. [22] https://www.quantamagazine.org/
doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0652. PMC 1689681 . 20160712-hyperuniformity-found-in-birds-math-and-physics/

[10] Martin, Graham. Producing the image in Ziegler & [23] Varela, F. J.; Palacios, A. G.; Goldsmith T. M. Color
Bischof (1993) 524 vision of birds in Ziegler & Bischof (1993) 7794
[24] Bowmaker, J. K.; Martin, G. R. (January 1985). Visual
[11] Thomas, Robert J.; Suzuki, M; Saito, S; Tanda, S; New-
pigments and oil droplets in the penguin, Spheniscus hum-
son, Stuart E.; Frayling, Tim D.; Wallis, Paul D. (2002).
bolti". Journal of Comparative Physiology. 156 (1): 71
Eye size in birds and the timing of song at dawn.
77. doi:10.1007/BF00610668.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 269 (1493): 831837.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1941. PMC 1690967 . PMID [25] Goldsmith, T. H.; Collins, JS; Licht, S (1984). The
11958715. cone oil droplets of avian retinas. Vision Research. 24
(11): 16611671. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(84)90324-9.
[12] THOMAS, ROBERT (2002). Body mass regulation and PMID 6533991.
the daily singing routines of European robins. Animal
Behaviour. 63: 285295. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1926. [26] Vorobyev, M.; Osorio, D.; Bennett, A. T. D.; Marshall,
Retrieved 1 January 2015. N. J.; Cuthill, I. C. (3 July 1998). Tetrachromacy,
oil droplets and bird plumage colours (PDF). Jour-
[13] Hall, Margaret I. (June 2008). The anatomical re- nal of Comparative Physiology A. 183 (5): 621633.
lationships between the avian eye, orbit and sclerotic doi:10.1007/s003590050286.
ring: implications for inferring activity patterns in ex-
[27] Eaton, Muir D. (August 2005). Human vision fails to
tinct birds. Journal of Anatomy. 212 (6): 781794.
distinguish widespread sexual dichromatism among sexu-
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2008.00897.x. PMC 2423400
ally monochromatic birds. Proceedings of the National
. PMID 18510506. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
102 (31): 1094210946. doi:10.1073/pnas.0501891102.
[14] Sivak, Jacob G. (2004). Through the Lens Clearly: Phy-
logeny and Development. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. PMC 1182419 . PMID 16033870.
Sci. 45 (3): 740747. doi:10.1167/iovs.03-0466. PMID [28] Muheim, Rachel; Phillips, JB; Akesson, S (August 2006).
14985284. Polarized light cues underlie compass calibration in mi-
gratory songbirds (PDF). Science. 313 (5788): 837839.
[15] Nalbach Hans-Ortwin; Wolf-Oberhollenzer, Friedericke;
doi:10.1126/science.1129709. PMID 16902138.
Remy Monika. Exploring the image in Ziegler &
Bischof (1993) 2628 [29] Greenwood, Verity J.; Smith, EL; Church, SC; Partridge,
JC (2003). Behavioural investigation of polarisation sen-
[16] Bawa, S.R.; YashRoy, R.C. (1974). Structure and func- sitivity in the Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica)
tion of vulture pecten.. Acta Anatomica. 89: 473480. and the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris)". The Jour-
doi:10.1159/000144308. nal of Experimental Biology. 206 (Pt 18): 32013210.
doi:10.1242/jeb.00537. PMID 12909701.
[17] Bawa, S.R.; YashRoy, R.C. (1972). Eect of dark and
light adaptation on the retina and pecten of chicken.. Ex- [30] Odeen, Anders; Hastad, Olle (11 February 2013).
perimental Eye Research. 13: 9297. doi:10.1016/0014- The phylogenetic distribution of ultraviolet sensitiv-
4835(72)90129-7. ity in birds. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 13 (36).
doi:10.1186/1471-2148-13-36. PMC 3637589 . PMID
[18] Hart, NS; Partridge, J.C.; Bennett, A.T.D.; Cuthill, I.C.
23394614.
(2000). Visual pigments, cone oil droplets and ocu-
lar media in four species of estrildid nch (PDF). Jour- [31] Andersson, S.; J. Ornborg; M. Andersson (1998).
nal of Comparative Physiology A. 186 (78): 681694. Ultraviolet sexual dimorphism and assortative mating in
doi:10.1007/s003590000121. Archived from the original blue tits. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 265 (1395):
(PDF) on February 20, 2005. 44550. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0315.
[19] The eect of the coloured oil droplets is to narrow and [32] Bright, Ashleigh; Waas, Joseph R. (August 2002).
shift the absorption peak for each pigment. The absorp- Eects of bill pigmentation and UV reectance during
tion peaks without the oil droplets would be broader and territory establishment in blackbirds (PDF). Animal Be-
less peaked, but these are not shown here. haviour. 64 (2): 207213. doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3042.
10 7 NOTES

[33] Viitala, Jussi; Korplmki, Erkki; Palokangas, Plvl; [45] Mouritsen, H.; Feenders, G; Liedvogel, M; Kropp,
Koivula, Minna (1995). Attraction of kestrels to vole W (2004). Migratory birds use head scans to de-
scent marks visible in ultraviolet light. Nature. 373 tect the direction of the Earths magnetic eld
(6513): 42527. doi:10.1038/373425a0. (PDF). Current Biology. 14 (21): 19461949.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.10.025. PMID 15530397.
[34] Lind, Olle; Mitkus, Mindaugas; Olsson, Peter; Kelber,
Almut (2013-05-15). Ultraviolet sensitivity and colour [46] Heyers D, Manns M, Luksch H, Gntrkn O, Mourit-
vision in raptor foraging. The Journal of Experimental sen H (2007). Iwaniuk A, ed. A Visual Pathway Links
Biology. 216 (10): 18191826. doi:10.1242/jeb.082834. Brain Structures Active during Magnetic Compass Ori-
ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 23785106. entation in Migratory Birds. PLoS ONE. 2 (9): e937.
[35] Hastad, Olle; Visctorsson, Jonas; Odeen, Anders (March doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000937. PMC 1976598 .
28, 2005). Dierences in colour vision make passer- PMID 17895978.
ines less conspicuous in the eyes of their predators.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of [47] Shanor, Karen; Kanwal, Jagmeet (2009). Bats sing, mice
the United States of America. 102 (18): 63916394. giggle: revealing the secret lives of animals. Icon Books. p.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0409228102. PMC 1088363 . PMID 25. ISBN 1-84831-071-4. (Despite its title, this is written
15851662. by professional scientists with many references)

[36] Stevens, Martin; Cuthill, Innes. Hidden Messages: [48] Heyers, Dominik; Manns, M; Luksch, H; Gntrkn,
Are Ultraviolet Signals a Special Channel in Avian O; Mouritsen, H; Iwaniuk, Andrew (September 2007).
Communication?". BioScience. 57 (6): 501507. Iwaniuk, Andrew, ed. A Visual Pathway Links Brain
doi:10.1641/b570607. Structures Active during Magnetic Compass Orienta-
tion in Migratory Birds. PLoS ONE. 2 (9): e937.
[37] De Valois, Russel L; Morgan, Herman; Snodderly, Max
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000937. PMC 1976598 .
D (1974). Psychophysical studies of monkey vision -
PMID 17895978. Retrieved 2007-09-27.
III. Spatial luminance contras sensitivity tests of macaque
and human observers. Vision Research. 14 (1): 7581.
[49] Snyder, Allan W.; Miller, William H. (1978). Telephoto
doi:10.1016/0042-6989(74)90118-7.
lens system of falconiform eyes. Nature. 275: 127129.
[38] Harmening, Wolf M; Nikolay, P; Orlowski, J; Wagner, doi:10.1038/275127a0.
Hermann J (2009). Spatial contrast sensitivity and grat-
ing acuity of barn owls. Journal of Vision. 9 (7): 113. [50] Poitier, Simone; Bonadonna, Francesco; Kelber, Al-
doi:10.1167/9.7.13. mut; Duriez, Olivier (2016). Visual acuity in an op-
portunistic raptor, the chimango caracara (Milvago chi-
[39] Jarvis, John R; Abeyesinghe, Siobhan M; McMahon, mango)". Physiology & Behaviour. 157: 125128.
Claire E; Wathes, Christopher M (2009). Measuring and doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.01.032.
modelling the spatial contrast sensitivity of the chicken
(Gallus g. domesticus)". Vision Research. 49 (11): [51] Ganey, Matthew F; Hodos, William (2003). The vi-
14481454. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.019. PMID sual acuity and refractive state of the American kestrel
19272401. (Falco sparverius)". Vision Research. 43 (19): 2053
2059. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00304-3.
[40] Ghim, Mimi M; Hodos, William (2006). Spatial contrast
sensitivity of birds. Journal of Comparative Physiology [52] Reymond, Liz (1985). Spatial visual acuity of the ea-
A. 192 (5): 523534. doi:10.1007/s00359-006-0090-5. gle Aquila audax: a behavioural, optical and anatomical
[41] Uhlrich, Daniel J; Essock, Edward A; Lehmkuhle, investigation. Vision Research. 25 (10): 14771491.
Stephen (1981). Cross-species correspondence of spa- doi:10.1016/0042-6989(85)90226-3.
tial contrast sensitivity functions. Behavioural Brain Re-
search. 2: 291299. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(81)90013- [53] Reymond, Liz (1987). Spatial visual acuity of the fal-
9. con, Falco berigora: A behavioural, optical and anatomi-
cal investigation. Vision Research. 27 (10): 18591874.
[42] Sekuler AB, Lee JA, Shettleworth SJ (1996). Pigeons do doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90114-3.
not complete partly occluded gures. Perception. 25 (9):
11091120. doi:10.1068/p251109. PMID 8983050. [54] Fischer, Anna Barbara (1969). Laboruntersuchun-
gen und freilandbeobachtungen zum sehvermgen un-
[43] Bhagavatula P, Claudianos C, Ibbotson M, Srinivasan M der verhalten von altweltgeiern. Zoologische Jahrbche
(2009). Warrant E, ed. Edge Detection in Landing abteilung fr systematik (in German) (96): 81132.
Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus)". PLoS ONE. 4
(10): e7301. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007301. PMC [55] Martin, Graham (1986). Shortcomings of an eagles
2752810 . PMID 19809500. eye. Nature (319): 357.

[44] Mouritsen, Henrik; Gesa Feenders, Miriam Liedvogel, [56] Schematic diagram of retina of right eye, loosely based on
Kazuhiro Wada & Erich D. Jarvis (2005). Night- Sturkie (1998) 6
vision brain area in migratory songbirds. PNAS. 102
(23): 83398344. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409575102. PMC [57] Bawa, S.R.; YashRoy, R.C. Vulture retina enzyme dis-
1149410 . PMID 15928090. tribution and function. Neurobiology. 2: 162168.
11

[58] Sillman, A. J.; Bolnick, D. A.; Haynes, L. W.; Walter, A. Sturkie, P. D. (1998). Sturkies Avian Physiology.
E.; Loew, Ellis R. (1981). Microspectrophotometry of 5th Edition. Academic Press, San Diego. ISBN 0-
the photoreceptors of palaegnathous birds - the emu and 12-747605-9. OCLC 162128712.
the tinamou. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: 271
276. Ziegler, Harris Philip; Bischof, Hans-Joachim, eds.
(1993). Vision, Brain, and Behavior in Birds: A
[59] Burton (1985) 4448 comparative review. MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-
[60] Hecht, Selig; Pirenne, MH (1940). The sensibility of the 24036-X. OCLC 27727176.
nocturnal Long-Eared Owl in the spectrum (Automatic
PDF download). Journal of General Physiology. 23 (6):
709717. doi:10.1085/jgp.23.6.709. PMC 2237955 . 9 External links
PMID 19873186.
Robert G. Cook, ed. (2001). Avian Visual Cognition
[61] Cleere, Nigel; Nurney, David (1998). Nightjars: A Guide
to the Nightjars, Frogmouths, Potoos, Oilbird and Owlet-
(cyberbook). Tufts University; in cooperation with
nightjars of the World. Pica / Christopher Helm. p. 7. Comparative Cognition Press.
ISBN 1-873403-48-8. OCLC 39882046.

[62] Fullard, J. H.; Barclay; Thomas (1993). Echolocation in


free-ying Atiu Swiftlets (Aerodramus sawtelli)" (PDF).
Biotropica. 25 (3): 334339. doi:10.2307/2388791.
JSTOR 2388791. Retrieved 12 July 2008.

[63] Konishi, M.; Knudsen, EI (April 1979). The oilbird:


hearing and echolocation. Science. 204 (4391): 425
427. doi:10.1126/science.441731. PMID 441731.

[64] Lythgoe, J. N. (1979). The Ecology of Vision. Oxford:


Clarendon Press. pp. 180183. ISBN 0-19-854529-0.
OCLC 4804801.

[65] Lotem A; Schechtman E & G Katzir (1991). Capture of


submerged prey by little egrets, Egretta garzetta garzetta:
strike depth, strike angle and the problem of light re-
fraction (pdf). Anim. Behav. 42 (3): 341346.
doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80033-8.

[66] Katzir, Gadi; Lotem, Arnon; Intrator, Nathan (1989).


Stationary underwater prey missed by reef herons,
Egretta gularis: head position and light refraction at the
moment of strike (PDF). J. Comparative Physiology A.
165: 573576. doi:10.1007/BF00611243.

[67] Hayes, Brian; Martin, Graham R.; Brooke, Michael de L.


(1991). Novel area serving binocular vision in the retinae
of procellariiform seabirds. Brain, Behavior and Evolu-
tion. 37 (2): 7984. doi:10.1159/000114348.

[68] Martin, Graham R.; Brooke, M. de L. (1991). The


Eye of a Procellariiform Seabird, the Manx Shearwa-
ter, Punus punus: Visual Fields and Optical Struc-
ture. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution. 37 (2): 6578.
doi:10.1159/000114347.

8 References
Burton, Robert (1985). Bird Behaviour. London:
Granada Publishing. ISBN 0-246-12440-7.

Sinclair, Sandra (1985). How Animals See: Other


Visions of Our World. Beckenham, Kent: Croom
Helm. ISBN 0-7099-3336-3.
12 10 TEXT AND IMAGE SOURCES, CONTRIBUTORS, AND LICENSES

10 Text and image sources, contributors, and licenses


10.1 Text
Bird vision Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Bird_vision?oldid=776793984 Contributors: Shyamal, Jimfbleak, Andrewman327,
Raul654, Gilgamesh~enwiki, Bender235, Desultor, 99of9, Sabines Sunbird, Wtmitchell, Woohookitty, BD2412, Rjwilmsi, Nihiltres,
RussBot, Mithridates, Gaius Cornelius, Dugosz, SCZenz, Epipelagic, Asams10, BorgQueen, Sardanaphalus, SmackBot, Chris the speller,
Colonies Chris, Snowmanradio, Richard001, Dicklyon, Ravewolf, JMK, Danlev, ShelfSkewed, MaxEnt, Mojo Hand, Headbomb, Atavi,
Rie, Hassocks5489, R'n'B, Adamd1008, VolkovBot, The Sky May Be, Cloudswrest, Viridiavus~enwiki, Wingedsubmariner, Eddy-
carr, 4444hhhh, MeegsC, Strasburger, ImageRemovalBot, Marian Rusnak, Rotational, Alexbot, Arjayay, SchreiberBike, Thingg, Addbot,
DOI bot, SuperlativeHors, Luckas-bot, Yobot, Fraggle81, Julia W, Vincnet, AnomieBOT, Jim1138, Royote, JackieBot, Citation bot,
GrouchoBot, Sfoleor, Prari, LucienBOT, Citation bot 1, Pinethicket, Jonesey95, Tom.Reding, Trappist the monk, Vrenator, Zephyric,
Woodlot, FoxRaweln, DARTH SIDIOUS 2, RjwilmsiBot, EmausBot, John of Reading, Dcirovic, K6ka, RaymondSutanto, ClueBot NG,
Onano, Slartibartfastibast, Braincricket, Helpful Pixie Bot, BG19bot, Ccevo2011, BattyBot, Cyberbot II, ChrisGualtieri, Bw mutley, The-
JJJunk, SD5bot, Dexbot, Sigvit, Jochen Burghardt, Bernatel, Anrnusna, JaconaFrere, Monkbot, SantiLak, Parrispower, Lythronaxargestes,
Ccevol2014, Nowpedia, Ollind, CAPTAIN RAJU, The Garages Dragon, Bismvth, OlleLindiLund, Peter.olsson84, GreenC bot, Henryc-
murray, Kestrelooo, Blue Edits and Anonymous: 62

10.2 Images
File:Astronotus_ocellatus_-_closeup_(aka).jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/2/2d/Astronotus_
ocellatus_-_closeup_%28aka%29.jpg License: CC BY-SA 2.5 Contributors: Own work Original artist: Andr Karwath aka Aka
File:Bald_Eagle_at_The_National_Zoo.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/c/c1/Bald_Eagle_at_The_
National_Zoo.jpg License: CC BY-SA 2.0 Contributors: Flickr Original artist: Stevehdc from Washington DC, USA
File:BirdCone.png Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/2/29/BirdCone.png License: Public domain Contributors:
Own work Original artist: Jimfbleak
File:BirdVisualPigmentSensitivity.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/e/e8/BirdVisualPigmentSensitivity.
svg License: Public domain Contributors: Own work Original artist: L. Shyamal
File:Bird_blink-edit.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/d/d0/Bird_blink-edit.jpg License: CC BY-SA 3.0
Contributors: Own work Original artist: Toby Hudson
File:Birdeye.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/b/b7/Birdeye.jpg License: CC BY-SA 3.0 Contributors:
Transferred from en.wikipedia to Commons by jimfbleak. Original artist: Jimfbleak at English Wikipedia
File:Chileaneagleretina.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/0/0f/Chileaneagleretina.svg License: Public do-
main Contributors: Own work Original artist: Jimfbleak
File:Colibri-thalassinus-001-edit.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/d/db/Colibri-thalassinus-001-edit.
jpg License: CC-BY-SA-3.0 Contributors: Own work Original artist: Mdf, Edited by Laitche
File:Common_Kestrel_1.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/f/ff/Common_Kestrel_1.jpg License: CC-BY-
SA-3.0 Contributors: Transferred from en.wikipedia to Commons by Saperaud. Original artist: sannse
File:Crested_tern444_edit.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/1/12/Crested_tern444_edit.jpg License: CC-
BY-SA-3.0 Contributors: Own work Original artist: --Benjamint 05:18, 29 September 2007 (UTC), edited by Fir0002
File:Erithacus-OhWeh-006.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/0/0f/Erithacus-OhWeh-006.jpg License:
CC BY 2.5 Contributors: Own work Original artist: OhWeh
File:Fieldofview01.png Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/1/1b/Fieldofview01.png License: Public domain Con-
tributors: Transferred from en.wikipedia to Commons by Jimfbleak. Original artist: Jimfbleak at English Wikipedia
File:Folder_Hexagonal_Icon.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/en/4/48/Folder_Hexagonal_Icon.svg License: Cc-by-
sa-3.0 Contributors: ? Original artist: ?
File:Hawk_eye.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/3/3e/Hawk_eye.jpg License: CC BY 2.0 Contributors:
http://www.flickr.com/photos/jurvetson/162116759 Original artist: Steve Jurvetson
File:Lock-green.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/6/65/Lock-green.svg License: CC0 Contributors: en:File:
Free-to-read_lock_75.svg Original artist: User:Trappist the monk
File:Manxshearwaterretina.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/9/9e/Manxshearwaterretina.svg License:
Public domain Contributors: Own work Original artist: Jimfbleak
File:Milvus_milvus_-Laurieston,_Dumfries_and_Galloway,_Scotland_-feeding_station-8.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.
org/wikipedia/commons/c/cf/Milvus_milvus_-Laurieston%2C_Dumfries_and_Galloway%2C_Scotland_-feeding_station-8.jpg License:
CC BY 2.0 Contributors: Locked on Original artist: Mark Medcalf
File:Ninox_strenua_-Chatswood_West,_New_South_Wales,_Australia_-6April2009.jpg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.
org/wikipedia/commons/a/ad/Ninox_strenua_-Chatswood_West%2C_New_South_Wales%2C_Australia_-6April2009.jpg Li-
cense: Public domain Contributors: This le was derived from Powerful Owl - April 6 2009 Chatswood West edited..JPG: <a
href='//commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG' class='image'><img
alt='Powerful Owl - April 6 2009 Chatswood West edited..JPG' src='https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/7/75/
Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG/50px-Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG'
width='50' height='36' srcset='https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/7/75/Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_
Chatswood_West_edited..JPG/75px-Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG 1.5x, https://upload.wikimedia.
org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/7/75/Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG/100px-Powerful_Owl_-_April_
6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG 2x' data-le-width='1080' data-le-height='786' /></a>
Original artist: Powerful_Owl_-_April_6_2009_Chatswood_West_edited..JPG: Poyt448 Peter Woodard
10.3 Content license 13

File:Owlretina.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/7/7c/Owlretina.svg License: Public domain Contributors:


Own work Original artist: Jimfbleak
File:Portal-puzzle.svg Source: https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/en/f/fd/Portal-puzzle.svg License: Public domain Contributors: ?
Original artist: ?

10.3 Content license


Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 3.0

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi