Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 12

Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 24, No. 1, pp.

17-28, 1996
Copyright @ 1996 Elsevier Science Ltd
Pergamon Printed in the USA. All rights reserved
0047-2352/96 $15.00 + .00
0047-2352(95)00049-6

PUBLIC PERCEPTIONS OF STATE POLICE:


A N ANALYSIS OF I N D I V I D U A L - L E V E L
A N D C O N T E X T U A L VARIABLES

M A R K E . CORREIA, M I C H A E L D . REISIG, a n d NICHOLAS P. LOVRICH

Department of Political Science


Washington State University
Pullman, Washington 99164-4880

ABSTRACT

Research focusing on citizens'perceptions o f police has been widespread at the local level," however,
little attention has been devoted to state policing organizations. This focus is particularly interesting con-
sidering the enhanced role state police agencies have assumed in recent years. This study analyzes cate-
gorical and ordered categorical attitudinal data collected f r o m a statewide sample in the State o f
Washington. Among the findings reported are significant gender differences and the observation that
the number o f citations an individual has received affects his or her perceptions o f state police.

INTRODUCTION ing organizations, such as the internal struc-


ture of personnel (Weisheit, 1987), planning
State police agencies traditionally have been and management techniques in use (Verbeck,
oriented toward traffic enforcement on state Marshall, and O'Toole, 1991; Movius, 1988),
highways; however, many contemporary state training programs (Edwards, 1993), adminis-
police organizations have enlarged the scope trative decision-making models (Raub, 1988),
of agency operations to include a variety of ac- and assessments of services (Finn and McGillis,
tivities (U.S. Department of Justice, 1992). 1990). Despite the number and range of such
Many local and county police organizations studies and the importance of public contact
rely on the state police for criminal history and cooperation in performing police work ef-
records, crime evidence analysis, and joint op- fectively (Hahn, 1971), little empirical re-
erations in task forces. Although a few exist- search has investigated the consequences of
ing studies on policing agencies have analyzed expanded state police functions in a system-
public perceptions of state police (Hedgepeth, atic way (Preiss and Ehrlich, 1958; Gourley,
1970; Carter, 1985), most research endeavors 1966).
have assessed different features of state polic- By analyzing the public's perception of

17
18 M.E. CORREIA, M. D. REISIG, and N. P. LOVRICH

state police in a geographically heterogenous nority groups (see also Campbell and Schu-
state (i.e., mixed urban, suburban, and rural man, 1972; Furstenberg and Wellford, 1973).
settings) using individual-level and contextual To date, research has indicated a positive
variables employed in past research (Decker, relationship between age and attitudes toward
1981), this study attempts to provide impor- police. More specifically, young people appear
tant information that will help to develop po- to be less satisfied than their adult counterparts
licing policy that facilitates the building of a (Hadar and Snortum, 1975; Walker et al.,
cooperative relationship between the public 1972). In one of the only comprehensive anal-
and law enforcement. Further, in measuring yses focusing on state police, Preiss and
levels of citizen satisfaction with police, this Ehrlich (1958:130) found "a direct, almost lin-
study indirectly assesses individual perceptions ear relationship between age and the relative
concerning the legitimacy and authority of favorableness of image." Wilson (1985) argues
state government (Benson, 1981:46). that age may be more of a determinant of at-
titudes than ethnicity, gender, or socialization.
Individual-Level Variables Others note, however, that while age may be
related to individual perceptions, the relation-
Of the individual-level variables used to an- ship is unstable (Smith and Hawkins, 1973).
alyze citizen perceptions of police, race con- Although full consensus has yet to be
sistently has been shown to be a powerful reached, there appears to be reasonable em-
indicator. Many studies have revealed that pirical support for the relationship between
Caucasians tend to hold more positive views age and attitudes toward police. One explana-
of the police when compared to non-Cauca- tion offered for this relationship suggests that
sians (Smith and Hawkins, 1973). Hadar and younger individuals tend to value their free-
Snortum (1975) revealed important value dif- dom while other age groups are more security-
ferences showing that African Americans and and safety-oriented (Gaines, Kappeler, and
Hispanics were more dissimilar in values from Vaughn, 1994:343). Further, since younger
the police than Caucasians. In an analysis of people, especially those between eighteen and
Hispanic citizens in the State of Texas, Carter twenty-five years of age, are responsible for
(1985) found that Hispanics' expectations of a disproportionate amount of crime (Sagi and
police performance were not being met. He ar- Wellford, 1968; Wellford, 1973), these differ-
gued that these perceptions are a product of ences may be because younger people are in-
ineffective communication, cultural conflict, creasingly likely to have more negative contacts
and perceptions of discrimination. with police.
Although race and ethnicity are important Past research has found the variables gen-
factors, their influence may be more complex der and education to be unimportant factors
than originally thought. For example, Jacob in the assessment of attitudes of police. Con-
(1971) revealed considerable variation across cerning gender, Hadar and Snortum (1975) ar-
different ethnic groups (e.g., African Ameri- gue that although some differences between
can and Hispanic). These and other findings males and females existed regarding law en-
have led some to suggest that ethnicity is forcement policies, perceptions of the police
merely an indicator of neighborhood social- tended to be very similar. Preiss and Ehrlich
ization (Dunham and Alpert, 1988). Nonethe- (1958) found some gender differences, and
less, others maintain that differences in race concluded that women tend to hold an ideal-
result because minorities have a higher num- ized image of the police (i.e., service oriented)
ber of negative contacts with the police rela- while men tend to view police as primarily law
tive to Caucasians (Gaines, Kappeler, and enforcement oriented. Although some have
Vaughn, 1994). Overall, numerous studies found that younger men tend to hold the most
have revealed that Caucasians tend to view the negative attitudes (Wilson, 1985), in general,
police more positively when compared to mi- findings reveal little difference between men
Public Perceptions of State Police 19

and womens' perceptions (Winfree and Grif- related to his or her attitude toward the po-
fiths, 1977). lice. Radelet and Carter (1994:206) note that
the "biggest conflict between the police and the
Contextual Variables public occurs in the enforcement of traffic
laws." They further argue that this type en-
The nature of prior experiences with police counter usually results in negative attitudes to-
has an important impact on an individual's ward the police (see also Vedder and Keller,
perception of police. The salience of this fac- 1965). Smith and Hawkins (1973) found that
tor was highlighted by Mastrofski (1981), who receiving a traffic citation was not a signifi-
called for the inclusion of all possible clientele cant determinant. The authors concluded that
when evaluating police services. In one of the the experience during an encounter was more
earliest examinations of public attitude phe- important than the initiation of contact.
nomena, Wirths (1958:52) noted the impor- Following a review of the literature, it is
tance of police-citizen contacts by suggesting concluded that the most powerful individual-
that police officers "mold public attitudes." It level variables are most likely ethnicity and
is important to note, of course, that police- age, while the relationships concerning gender
citizen contacts differ significantly in charac- and education are problematic. Beyond these
ter; voluntary contacts (e.g., calls for service) individualqevel variables, the type of contact
versus involuntary contacts (e.g., traffic cita- and the experience (i.e., negative or positive)
tions) generate different consequences (Bercal, surrounding the contact are important. Simply
1970). Bordua and Tift (1971) found that put, those citizens with negative or involun-
those individuals who experienced involuntary tary contacts are more likely to hold negative
contact were more likely to hold lower evalu- perceptions of police, while positive encounters
ations of the police than those individuals who have little effect on enhancing one's attitude)
initiated contact by requesting service. Preiss
and Ehrlich (1958) found that individuals who
did not come into contact with police tended METHODS
to hold more favorable perceptions than those
who had limited contact. The authors also The results of a statewide random sample
found that those citizens who had numerous of 892 households, a sample that is represen-
negative contacts tended to have more favor- tative of the age, ethnicity, and class character-
able perceptions than those with only some istics of the statewide population, are reported
negative contact. More recent research chal- here. Dillman's (1978) Total Design Method
lenges this finding from the late 1950s. was the approach of data collection employed
Carter (1985) found that as the number of in this study. This process entails a pretesting
contacts with police increased, regardless of of the survey instrument, use of appropriate
the nature of that contact, public satisfaction graphics, and repeated mailings to nonrespon-
decreased. Beyond mere contact, Dean (1980) dents (four contacts per respondent). Citizens
argues that the degree of satisfaction experi- were surveyed in three mailing waves (and one
enced with the contact is important in deter- telephone follow-up) over a four-month pe-
mining an individual's attitude toward the riod during the fall of 1993, resulting in a re-
police. Dean (1980) found that those citizens sponse rate of 56 percent.
who viewed their contacts with the police as Due to the dichotomous nature of the en-
unfavorable tended to give the police low over- dogenous variable employed in this survey (i.e.,
all evaluations. In contrast, those citizens who positive versus less favorable perceptions of
viewed their encounters favorably tended to state police), log linear analysis (LOGIT) was
give the police high overall evaluations. employed using maximum likelihood estima-
In sum, the citizen's perception of his or her tion (MLE). Logit has been shown to be ap-
contact with the police has been shown to be propriate for categorical data analysis (Agresti,
20 M.E. CORREIA, M. D. REISIG, and N. P. LOVRICH

1990) and offers several advantages over tradi- follows: individuals 18-35, 36-50, 51-64, 65+
tional linear regression analysis. For example, years of age, with 18-35 as the reference cat-
m a n y of the requirements for linear analysis egory. 8 Educational level was dichotomized
(e.g., normal distributions, interval measures) into college graduates and non-college grad-
cannot be met when using categorical data uates, with non-college graduates represent-
(Aldrich and Nelson, 1984). The use of logit, ing the reference category. Lastly, ethnicity
in comparison to linear regression, provides was dichotomized into Caucasian (reference
a rigorous means of deriving the most effi- category) and non-Caucasian. Due to the low
cient estimates when the independent variables percentage of African Americans and Hispan-
are both categorical and ordered categorical. 2 ics in this sample, it was necessary to collapse
T-tests were employed to assess the signifi- all minority respondents (including Asians,
cance of the individual variables within the Pacific Islanders, and Native Americans) into
models and to reveal whether or not a variable a single category. 9
adds any significant likelihood to the overall Survey information also was collected con-
model. 3 Additionally, the probabilities for sev- cerning whether or not the respondent had re-
eral variables within the logit models were cal- ceived a warning or citation, the evaluative
culated to reveal important information. To context of the most recent such encounter, and
compare the individual models, the authors the number of citations an individual had re-
use the G 2 statistic calculated by the G L I M ceived over the last two years. To assess the
statistical package. 4 impact of receiving a warning rather than a ci-
The dependent variable measured the atti- tation, this variable was i n c l u d e d - with those
tude that an individual holds toward the state individuals having received a warning serving
police. After reading the mission statement, as the reference category. Those individuals
respondents were asked to indicate their level who received a citation rather than a warning
of agreement with the statement: "Overall, the were included in a variable concerning the
Washington State Patrol does a good job at number of citations received in the last two
p e r f o r m i n g its mission. ''5 Originally, this years, with no citations as the reference cat-
measurement incorporated a five-point Likert egory. Additionally, for those individuals who
scale. Due to the unequal distribution across received either a warning or a citation, a mea-
the five levels, however, it was necessary to surement was included to assess whether or
collapse those who strongly agree and agree not the officer was clear in his or her expla-
that the state police deserve a favorable over- nation of the traffic enforcement encounter.
all rating into one category and the remaining The reference category for this variable is
levels into a second category of less favorable composed of those individuals who believed
assessment. 6 This method allowed for an un- the officer's explanation for citation (or warn-
biased analysis of the predictors of individual ing) was clear (Table 1). Second, because sev-
attitudes toward the state police. eral authors have suggested that the way in
The predictor variables in this analysis in- which an individual comes into contact with
cluded those identified by previous studies the police is important in the development of
evaluating individual perceptions toward po- perceptions, it was necessary to differentiate
lice. Past research has shown that age and whether an individual was forced into contact
race/ethnicity are important indicators. Gen- (e.g., traffic stop) or initiated the contact (e.g.,
der also was included because Preiss and call for assistance). Subsequently, both the
Ehrlich (1958) found some variation in the at- type of contact and the individual's perception
titudes men and women hold toward state po- of that contact were investigated. 1
lice. Although education has been found to be
of little influence in attitudes toward city po-
RESULTS
lice, it was included in this study due to the
lack of research focusing on state police. 7 Three models were constructed to assess the
For this analysis, age was categorized as effects of the independent variables on percep-
Public Perceptions of State Police 21

TABLE 1

VARIABLE CODING AND DESCRIPTIVESTATISTICS

Variable Categories Values N %

Dependent
Perception of state police Less favorable 0 123 13.8
Positive perception 1 769 86.2
Independent
Individual level
Ethnicity Caucasian 1 817 92.8
Non-Caucasian 2 63 7.2
Age 18 to 35 1 181 20.3
36 to 50 2 344 38.6
51 to 64 3 192 21.5
65+ 4 175 19.6
Gender Male 1 648 73.4
Female 2 235 26.6
Education Non-college degree 1 554 62.7
College degree 2 329 37.3
Housing Rent 1 226 25.6
Own 2 655 74.4
Contextual
Received warning Yes 1 411 67.3
No 2 200 32.7
Number of citations None 1 721 80.8
1 or2 2 159 17.8
3+ 3 12 1.3
Type of explanation of warning or citation Clear 1 535 92.6
Unclear 2 43 7.4
Treatment of individuals who initiated contact Very satisfied 1 208 58.9
Somewhat satisfied 2 109 30.8
Somewhat dissatisfied 3 22 6.2
Very dissatisfied 4 14 4.0
Treatment of individuals who received a citation Fair 1 330 74.2
Unfair 2 115 25.8

Note: N = 892; missing data are excluded from these analyses.

tion of state police. Described below is the icant factor; females had a significant likeli-
main effects model, as well as two interaction hood of having less favorable perceptions of
models that were statistically significant when state police than males (T-value of 3.39). This
compared to the main effects model. Compar- finding is consistent with Preiss and Ehrlich's
isons were based on the scaled deviance ex- (1958) suggestion that men and women view
plained and the number of degrees of freedom state police differently. It may be that gender
lost. The best fit models were those that lost does not affect perceptions of police at the lo-
a significant amount of scaled deviance rela- cal level, but it does, in fact, affect attitudes
tive to minimal losses in degrees of freedom, at the state level. In terms of ethnicity, it was
yielding a significant G 2. found that non-Caucasians, relative to Cau-
casians, had a lower likelihood (T-value of
Main Effects Model 1.85) of positive perceptions. In combination,
these findings indicate that, in general, minor-
Many of the predictors in the main effects ities (i.e., women and ethnic minorities) have
model had significant T-values (see Table 2). a lower probability of having positive attitudes
Though previous research has found that gen- toward the state police.
der is not an important variable in determin- The data show that all age categories are
ing attitudes toward police, the present study's significant additions to the overall model
findings suggest that gender is a highly signif- (T-values of 3.73, 3.44, and 2.96). Overall, it
22 M . E . CORREIA, M. D. REISIG, and N. P. LOVRICH

TABLE 2
LOGIT RESULTS FOR CITIZEN PERCEPTIONSOF STATE POLICE

Model Main Effects Interaction # 1 Interaction #2

Gender(2) .7816** -.7488** --.8037 **


(.2301) (.2353) (.2333
Age(2) 1.096"* 1.138* 1.188"*
(.2933) (.5709) (.2982
Age(3) 1.303** .0792 1.337"*
(.3779) (.5656) (.3814
Age(4) 1.075** 1.754 1.164"*
(.3624) (1.123) (.3636
Ethnicity(2) -.6658 -,7315* --.7207*
(,3598) (.3682) (.3666
Education(2) - , 1699 - . 1767 --.4586
(,2292) (.2335) (.6351
Housing(2) -.0612 - . 1006 --.0524
(.2828) (.2894) (,2684
Warning(2) .3269 .3149 ,3670
(.3256) (.3346) (.3310)
Number(2) -.3389 -.3565 --.2834
(,2861) (.2967) (.2910)
Number(3) -.6982 -.5072 --.7304
(.7394) (.7886) (.7557)
Explanation(2) - 1.150" - 1.146" --1,165"
(.4233) (.4438) (.4308)
Citation(2) - 1.246 ** -2.008 ** --1.295**
(.3444) (,5859) (.3513)
Initiation(2) -1.380** -1.373** --1.904**
(.3988) (.4037) (.5102)
Initiation(3) - 1.749 * - 1.777 * --2.420**
(.6179) (.6434) (.7464)
Initiation(4) -2,624 ** -3.259 ** --3.186**
(.6798) (,7561) (.7827)
Age(2) Initiation(2) .1803
(.8049)
Age(3) * Initiation(3) 3.463**
(1.121)
Age(4) * Initiation(4) .3696
(1.350)
Education(2) * Initiation(2) 1.298
(.8258)
Education(2) * Initiation(3) 2.237
(1.525)
Education(2) * Initiation(4) 8.022
(15.86)
Intercept 2.856 3.150 2.944
(.4545) (.5603) (.5283)
- 2 Log-likelihood 589.3 572.9 576.9

*p < .05; **p < .01,


Note: Standard error for each variable is presented in parentheses,

appears that all age groups have a high like- explanation appears to lower the likelihood of
lihood of positive perceptions of police when a positive attitude toward the police (T-value
compared to those eighteen to thirty-five years of 2.72), which suggests that the manner in
of age; however, this likelihood decreases which the individual perceives himself or her-
somewhat as age increases. 1| self to be treated by the police significantly af-
Most contextual variables were significant fects his or her overall perception.
additions to the model. Receiving an unclear Though numerous studies have found that
Public Perceptions of State Police 23

receiving a traffic citation will not affect an TABLE 3


individual's perception, this analysis shows PROBABILITY ESTIMATES FOR SELECTED
that as the number o f traffic citations in- CONTEXTUAL VARIABLESa
creases, the likelihood of positive perceptions Female/ Male~
of state police decreases. Those individuals who Male Female Minority Minority
received one or two citations have a higher Init (2) .814 .667 .509 .693
likelihood of less favorable perceptions of the (3) .752 .578 .508 .609
police than those who have not received a ci- (4) .550 .365 .410 .395
tation. In sum, negative contact with the po- Expl .847 .716 .565 .740
lice (e.g., receiving a traffic citation) decreases Cit .830 .698 .540 .720
an individual's perception of the state police. Num (2) .910 .850 .745 .860
(3) .890 .794 .710 .810
In terms of whether or not the contact was
voluntary or involuntary, the results show ap = exp(a)/(1 + exp(a)).
that, regardless of the type of initiation, un-
satisfactory treatment of the individual de-
creases the likelihood of positive perceptions considered, female minorities have lower
of the state police. For example, the voluntary probabilities of positive perceptions than do
initiation of contact and satisfactory treatment male minorities, with the exception of minor-
decreased the likelihood that an individual ity males who reported they were very dissat-
would have a positive attitude toward the po- isfied with the context of their encounter.
lice (T-value of 3.46). As the satisfaction of Concerning the clarity of explanation, female
the contact decreased, so did the probability minorities have the lowest probability of pos-
of a positive perception, with those individu- itive perceptions. With but a few exceptions
als who were most dissatisfied having the low- (e.g., the findings concerning women), the
est likelihood of positive perceptions of the variables within the main effects model yield
police. results that are consistent with the literature
For those individuals who received traffic pertaining to local police.
citations, whether or not they believed them-
selves to be treated fairly had a significant ef- Interaction Models
fect on their perception. Receiving a citation
and holding a perception of unfair treatment The addition of two-way interaction terms
resulted in a decreased probability of having shows that several variables were significant
a positive perception (T-value of 3.62). Taken additions to the main effects model. ~2 The
together, the above findings suggest that, re- model that fit the interaction of age and cita-
gardless of the way in which the contact oc- tion constitutes a significant addition to the
curred, the manner in which the individual was main effects model, and it produces some in-
treated is very important. It also appears that teresting results. Primarily, the interaction of
any type of citizen contact with the state po- age and the perception of unfair treatment
lice has the tendency to lower an individual's while receiving a traffic citation does not de-
perception. crease the probability of having positive per-
Probability estimates for contextual vari- ceptions. In fact, individuals in the fifty-one
ables offer some additional insight. One find- to sixty-four age category who reported hav-
ing shows that as an individual's satisfaction ing been treated unfairly while receiving a traf-
with the context of the initiation decreases, the fic citation had an increased probability of
probability of positive perceptions decreases positive perceptions toward the police (T-value
as well. Within this context, however, it is im- of 3.08). Interactions concerning the other two
portant to note that males always maintain age categories were not significant, but they
higher perceptions when compared to females, too were in a positive direction. This analysis
even when the probability of positive percep- of interaction effects indicates that, regardless
tion decreases (see Table 3). When ethnicity is of the perception of treatment received, as age
24 M . E . CORREIA, M. D. RE1SIG, and N. P. LOVRICH

TABLE 4

MODEL DIAGNOSTICS

Model
Model Fitted Marginals L2 P Comparison z~L2

1 WAR + EXPL + NUM + AGE + HOUS + EDU


+ GEN + ETHN + CIT + INIT 589.3
(867) .04
(Model 1 ) plus age x initiation 572.9 1 + 2 16.4*
(861) .007
(Model 1) plus education x initiation 576.9 1 + 3 12.4*
(863) .996

*p < .05.
Note: Degrees of freedom for each model are presented in parentheses.

increases so do positive perceptions of state ond interaction model (i.e., model 3). Though
police. 13 the probabilities of the individual-level vari-
The interaction between education and ini- ables are quite similar to those in the main ef-
tiation (i.e., model 3) produced the best fit (see fects model, several differences exist among
Table 4). Although the fifty-one to sixty-four the contextual variables (Table 5). In particu-
age category was not significant in the previ- lar, all of the probabilities concerning citizen
ous model, this category becomes a significant initiation and satisfaction with police contact
addition to the present model (T-value of are lower in this model, with female minori-
3.50). In terms of the interaction itself, it ap- ties generating the lowest probabilities in
pears that an individual with a college degree nearly every category. For example, those fe-
who initiates contact with the police and is sat- male minorities who initiated contact and were
isfied with that contact has a high probability only somewhat satisfied with that encounter
(T-value of 3.69) of having positive percep- had an extremely low probability of positive
tions of the state police. This suggests that as perceptions ( p = .38). When minority females
one's level o f education increases, he or she is reported they were dissatisfied with the con-
more likely to hold positive perceptions to- tact, the probability of positive perceptions de-
ward the state police. This appears to be the creased dramatically ( p = .14). As before,
case to a point; however, when an individual minority females possess the lowest probabil-
becomes very dissatisfied with the manner in ities of positive assessment of any gender/eth-
which he or she was treated, the importance nic category, t4
of age becomes secondary to the context of the
encounter. It is interesting to note that this is
the only case in which an individual's educa-
tion represents a significant addition to the TABLE 5

model. College educated individuals initiate PROBABILITY ESTIMATES FOR MODEL #3 a


contact with the police more frequently than Female/ Male/
those with less education. These citizens, how- Male Female Minority Minority
ever, appear to consider other factors more
Init (2) .739 .559 .381 .579
important to the contact (e.g., the outcome) (3) .628 .431 .269 .451
than their overall treatment. (4) .440 .260 .146 .276
Expl .856 .726 .563 .742
Probability Estimates for Model 3 Cit .839 .700 .531 .717
Num (2) .935 .865 .757 .874
The authors analyzed the probabilities of (3) .897 .804 .666 .817

the most important variables within the sec- ap = exp(a)/(1 + exp(a)).


Public Perceptions of State Police 25

Model Comparison pie, it was revealed that ethnic minorities have


a lower probability of holding positive percep-
Several consistencies exist across the three tions of police. Some contradictory results,
models. The contextual variables initiation however, also were uncovered in this analysis.
and citation remained significant in the same In previous analyses, gender was found not to
direction- more specifically, the more unsat- have a significant effect on an individual's per-
isfied one is with the contact, the less likely he ception of police. This study indicates, how-
or she is to have positive perceptions. Simi- ever, that female respondents are less likely
larly, clarity of the officer's explanation re- than their male counterparts to hold positive
mained significant across the three models. perceptions toward state police.
Again, this finding indicates that officer con- Wilson (1985) contends that age is more of
duct during the encounter is important. Ad- a determinate than ethnicity and gender con-
ditionally, the number of traffic citations one cerning perceptions of police. These data do
receives appears to decrease the likelihood of not support Wilson's claim. In fact, this anal-
a positive perception in all three models. ysis is closely aligned with Smith and Hawkins
Therefore, one can conclude that the context (1973) in that age appears to have an incon-
in which police-citizen contact takes place sig- sistent effect on individual perceptions of po-
nificantly affects one's perception of the state lice. Finally, contrary to the findings of Preiss
police. and Ehrlich (1958), the present study found
Of the individual-level indicators, gender that an individual's level of education was not
and ethnicity were the only variables that were a significant determinant in his or her percep-
significant additions in all three models. More- tion of state police. While the earlier study
over, the relative strength remained somewhat found that those individuals with an educa-
constant: gender ranged in T-values from 2.96 tional level lower than grade school or higher
to 3.45 and ethnicity ranged from 1.83 to 1.98. than college had lower perceptions of the po-
The consistency of the effects of these two lice, this analysis indicates that educational
variables suggests that they are important in- level appears not to have a significant e f f e c t -
dicators of one's perception toward the state perhaps a result of the increased levels of ed-
police. Further, one of the age variables ucation throughout the citizenry since the late
(namely, age [3]) appeared to be somewhat un- 1950s.
stable, depending upon the interaction. Findings for contextual variables reveal that,
regardless of whether contact was voluntary
or involuntary, an individual's perception of
DISCUSSION unsatisfactory or unfair treatment decreased
the probability of positive perceptions. Fur-
Before possible conclusions are discussed, ther, an unclear explanation by police con-
it is important to note a few limitations of this cerning a citation or warning also decreased
research. First, this study only examines the the probability. This research suggests, then,
state policing organization of a single state; that there is an important relationship between
consequently, broad inferences should be the manner in which citizens are treated and
drawn with caution. Second, since the variables positive perceptions toward state police.
used in this study were adapted from research These findings have many relevant policy
focusing on local policing agencies, it is possible implications. One of the more important find-
that some important variables were inadver- ings in this study concerns the levels of posi-
tently omitted from the present a n a l y s i s - a tive perceptions toward state police held by
potential shortcoming that future research on women and ethnic minorities. Some have
state police may possibly uncover. Neverthe- stated that attitudes toward the police are as-
less, the preceding analysis provides a fair as- sociated with attitudes toward legal and po-
semblage of interesting information some of litical systems (Albrecht and Green, 1977).
which supports existing research. For exam- Similarly, Chackerian (1974) argues that high
26 M . E . C O R R E I A , M. D. REISIG, and N. P. L O V R I C H

evaluations o f police are related to perceived NOTES


levels of government access (i.e., high levels
1. The literature concerning local police also has
o f access equate to high evaluations of police). shown neighborhood culture to be an important determi-
Although avenues of access have improved in nant for public attitudes of police. This variable, how-
the last half century, in a historical context, ever, was not deemed suitable for an analysis of state
police by the authors.
women and minorities have and continue to
possess (although to a lesser degree) limited ac- 2. For a more complete discussion concerning the ad-
vantages of logit analysis in social science research, please
cess to government. Further, because the po- refer to Roncek (1991) and Demaris (1992).
lice are the "states most visible and obtrusive
3. In order to test if the missing data had a signifi-
agents of control" (Benson, 1981:46), it may cant effect, each variable contained a missing category.
be concluded that the low perceptions held by These categories were included in the analysis to assess
women and ethnic minorities are not necessar- their likelihood. In all cases, the missing categories were
not significant.
ily reflective of police, but are connected to the
larger political structure and the broader so- 4. The G 2 statistic, as calculated by the GLIM statis-
tical package, is equivalent to the scaled deviance o f the
cial forces surrounding government. model. In order to designate the better model, it is neces-
With this in mind, it appears as though any sary to consider the decrease in the G 2 relative to the loss
policing policy implemented on behalf of state in degrees of freedom. The better model will be the one
that has a significant decrease in the G 2 while losing only
police can, at best, have only an incremental minimal degrees of freedom.
effect on the perceptions of ethnic minorities
5. The Washington State Patrol mission statement
and women. In other words, the most prom- reads: "The Washington State Patrol shall serve the pub-
ising positive change in these groups' percep- lic by providing assistance, coordination, and delivery of
tions toward state police will most likely result law enforcement and support services for the safety and
protection of people and property" (Correia and Reisig,
not from changes in state policing policies, but 1994:1).
from broader and continued social change.
6. The less favorable category consisted of those re-
The importance of incremental change by spondents who answered "undecided," "disagree," or
way of reforms in policing policy should not "strongly disagree."
be discounted. For example, a number of po- 7. The survey did not include an accurate and direct
licing organizations throughout the country measure of socioeconomic status (SES) other than for-
have, in recent years, undertaken the task of mal education. In previous surveys conducted in this state,
an SES (occupation and income) measure was found to
racial and gender sensitivity training as part be unrelated to perceptions of state police.
of diversity enhancement and support efforts.
8. The methodology employed for this survey did not
These programs may produce some positive
include individuals younger than eighteen years of age.
results. This research shows that the context Age categories were collapsed to provide a more equal
of an encounter with state police is extremely distribution.
important. Accordingly, such training may 9. Although a separate analysis of each ethnic group
produce some increased levels of positive per- is preferred, the low percentage of ethnic minority respon-
ception of police and enhance citizen coop- dents did not allow for an adequate analysis. Before col-
lapsing the categories, however, each individual group was
eration in performing police functions within included in the model and, in all cases, the results were
those groups. It is the authors' contention, not significant.
however, that this change will likely be 10. For those individuals who initiated contact, a four-
l i m i t e d - t h o u g h not insignificant. point Likert scale was used to assess their satisfaction with
that encounter, with very satisfied representing the ref-
erence group. A dichotomous measure was employed to
assess the individual's perception of fairness in his or her
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS treatment in receiving a citation, with fair treatment used
as the reference category.
A n earlier version of this study was presented at the 11. Findings for the age variable, however, are less con-
1995 A n n u a l Meeting of the Academy of Criminal Jus- clusive than the results for ethnicity and gender. To gain
tice Sciences in Boston, Massachusetts. The authors would a full understanding of the dynamics of the effects o f age
like to thank David Carter and Jessie Tzeng for their help- on the dependent variable, individuals younger than eigh-
ful comments on earlier drafts of this article. teen years of age should be included in the analysis. Due
Public Perceptions of State Police 27

to sampling limitations, however, these individuals were Decker, S. (1981). Citizen attitudes toward the police.
excluded. Journal o f Police Science and Administration 9:80-87.
Demaris, A. (1992). Logit modeling: Practical applica-
12. Since the potential for multicollinearity was a con- tions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
cern, several diagnostic tests were conducted before the Dillman, D. (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: The to-
data was collapsed. Importantly, the Durbin-Watson test tal design method. New York: Wiley-lnterscience.
showed that multicollinearity was not a problem (2.00). Dunham, R., and Alpert, G. (1988). Neighborhood dif-
ferences in attitudes toward policing: Evidence for a
13. Interestingly, the age variable within the model is mixed-strategy model of policing in a multi-ethnic set-
inconsistent when compared to the main effects m o d e l - ting. The Journal o f Criminal Law and Criminology
that is, the 51-64 age category becomes insignificant, while 79:504-21.
the other two categories remain significant. This may be Edwards, T. (1993). State police basic training programs:
occurring due to the significance of that variable when An assessment of course content and instructional meth-
interacted with citation. It may be the case that this in- odology. American Journal o f Police 12:23-45.
teraction is reducing the significance of the variable within Finn, P., and McGillis, D. (1990). Public safety at the state
the main effects model. In terms of the other variables, level: A survey of major services. Journal of Police Sci-
those that were significant in the main effects model also ence and Administration 17:133-46.
were significant in this model. Furstenberg, F., and Wellford, C. (1973). Calling the po-
lice: The evaluation of police service. Law and Society
Review 7:393-406.
14. Since hierarchical logit analysis was employed, sev- Gaines, L., Kappeler, V., and Vaugbn, J. (1994). Polic-
eral other models were tested for significance, including ing in America. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Publishing
two two-way interactions. The results are not reported in Co.
this analysis because the models did not provide any ad- Gourley, D. (1966). Effective police organization and man-
ditional insight into citizen perceptions of state police. agement. Los Angeles: California State College at Los
Angeles.
Hadar, I., and Snortum, J. (1975). The eye of the be-
holder: Differential perceptions of police by the police
REFERENCES and the public. Criminal Justice and Behavior 2:37-54.
Hahn, H. (1971). The public and the police: A theoreti-
cal perspective. In Police in urban society, ed. H. Hahn.
Agresti, A. (1990). Categorical data analysis. New York: Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
John Wiley & Sons. Hedgepeth, M. (1970). A study o f high school students'
Albrecht, S., and Green, M. (1977). Attitudes toward the images o f police and police officers' perceived image
police and the larger attitude complex. Criminology in a selected city. Unpublished master's thesis, Pullman,
15:67-87. Washington, Washington State University.
Aldrich, J., and Nelson, F. (1984). Linear probability, Jacob, H. (1971). Black and White perceptions of justice
logit and probit models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage in the city. Law and Society Review 6:69-89.
Publications. Mastrofski, S. (1981). Surveying clients to assess police
Benson, P. (1981). Political alienation and public satis- performance: Focusing on the police-citizen encoun-
faction with police service. Pacific Sociological Review ter. Evaluation Review 5:397-408.
24:45-64. Movius, M. (1988). Reshaping the future of police pro-
Bercal, T. (1970). Calls for police assistance: Consumer ductivity. The Police Chief 55:31-33.
demands for governmental service. American Behav- Percy, S. (1986). In defense of citizen evaluations as per-
ioral Scientist 13:681-91. formance measures. Urban Affairs Quarterly 22:66-83.
Bordua, D., and Tift, L. (1971). Citizen interviews, or- Preiss, J., and Ehrlich, H. (1958). An examination o f role
ganizational feedback, and police-community relations theory: The case o f state police. Lincoln, NE: Univer-
decisions. Law and Society Review 6:155-82. sity of Nebraska Press.
Campbell, A., and Schuman, H. (1972). A comparison Radelet, L., and Carter, D. (1994). The police and the
of Black and White attitudes and experiences in the city. community. 5th ed. New York: Macmillan Publishing
In The end o f innocence: A suburban reader, ed. C. Co.
Haar. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foreman. Raub, R. (1988). Staffing a state police agency for ser-
Carter, D. (1985). Hispanic perception of police perfor- vice and patrol. Journal o f Police Science and Admin-
mance: An empirical assessment. Journal o f Criminal istration 16:255-63.
Justice 13:487-500. Roncek, D. (1991). Using logit coefficients to obtain the
Chackerian, R. (1974). Police professionalism and citi- effects of independent variables on changes in proba-
zen evaluations: A preliminary look. Public Adminis- bilities. Social Forces 70:509-18.
tration Review 34:141-48. Sagi, D., and Wellford, C. (1968). Age composition and
Correia, M., and Reisig, M. (1994). Public attitudes to- patterns of change in criminal statistics. Journal o f
ward the Washington State Patrol." Results from the Criminal Law, Criminology and Police Science 59:29-
1993 survey. Pullman, WA: Division of Governmental 35.
Studies and Services. Smith, P., and Hawkins, R. (1973). Victimization, types
Dean, D. (1980). Citizen ratings of the police: The dif- of citizen-police contacts, and attitudes toward the po-
ference contact makes. Law and Politics Quarterly lice. Law and Society Review 8:135-52.
2:445-71. U.S. Department of Justice (1992). Law enforcement man-
28 M . E . CORREIA, M. D. REISIG, and N. P. LOVRICH

agement and administrative statistics, 1990: Data for Weisheit, R. (1987). Women in the state police: Concerns
individual state and local agencies with 100 or more of- of male and female officers. Journal o f Police Science
ficers. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics. and Administration 15:137-44.
Vedder, C., and Keller, O. (1965). The police and middle- Wellford, C. (1973). Age composition and the increase in
class conflicts. Police 9:6-8. recorded crime. Criminology 11:61-70.
Verbeck, R., Marshall, J., and O'Toole, D. (1991). Stra- Wilson, J. (1985). Thinking about crime. New York:
tegic planning a valuable tool for OSP. The Police Chief Vintage.
58:34-38. Winfree, T., and Griffiths, C. (1977). Adolescent attitudes
Walker, D., Richardson, R., Williams, O., Denyer, T., toward the police. In Juvenile delinquency: Little
and McGaughey, S. (1972). Contact and support: An brother grows up, ed. T. Ferdinand. Beverly Hills, CA:
empirical assessment of public attitudes toward the po- Sage Publications.
lice and the courts. North Carolina Law Review 51: Wirths, C. (1958). The development of attitudes toward
43-79. law enforcement. Police 3:50-52.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi