Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 6

Long-Term Average Spectrum in Screening of Voice

Quality in Speech: Untrained Male


University Students
Timo Leino

Summary: Voice quality has mainly been studied in trained speakers, singers, and dysphonic patients. Few studies
have concerned ordinary untrained university students voices. In light of earlier studies of professional voice users,
it was hypothesized that good, poor, and intermediate voices would be distinguishable on the basis of long-term average
spectrum characteristics. In the present study, voice quality of 50 Finnish vocally untrained male university students was
studied perceptually and using long-term average spectrum analysis of text reading samples of one minute duration.
Equivalent sound level (Leq) of text reading was also measured. According to the results, the good and ordinary voices
differed from the poor ones in their relatively higher sound level in the frequency range of 13 kHz and a prominent peak
at 34 kHz. Good voices, however, did not differ from the ordinary voices in terms of the characteristics of the long-term
average spectrum (LTAS). The strength of the peak at 34 kHz and the voice-quality scores correlated weakly but sig-
nificantly. Voice quality and alpha ratio (level difference above and below 1 kHz) correlated likewise. Leq was signif-
icantly higher in the students with good and ordinary voices than in those with poor voices. The connections between
Leq, voice quality, and the formation of the peak at 34 kHz warrant further studies.
Key Words: Voice qualityLTAS.

INTRODUCTION discriminate satisfactorily between various articulatory set-


Sound quality is defined as the attribute of sound that makes tings.23 He concluded that LTAS may show better glottal
two sounds of the same pitch and loudness dissimilar.1,2 By than resonatory characteristics of voice quality. Lofqvist et
definition, then, quality, loudness, and pitch are distinct percep- Mandersson28 found substantial differences in the LTAS spec-
tual attributes even though a change in one may affect another. tra recorded from the same subjects during the same day,
The physical correlate of sound quality is sound energy distribu- which they supposed to impair the usefulness of LTAS for
tion along the frequency range. Long-term average spectrum screening purposes. However, their recordings were made before
(LTAS) analysis provides a means of viewing the average fre- and after a working day requiring a lot of speaking. Substantial
quency distribution of the sound energy in a continuous speech differences in LTAS have been reported after vocal loading.2931
sample. The method yields information of the general voice Encouraging results on the suitability of LTAS for differenti-
quality, if the duration of the sample is long enough to avoid ation of various voice qualities have been reported for both
the effects of individual speech sounds. Li et al3 regard a sample normal and disordered voices.11,13,25
of 3040 seconds long enough to study voice quality, whereas To obtain some objectivity in setting goals for vocal training
Majewski et al4 recommend a sample of at least 1 minute. and for evaluating its effects, Leino used LTAS to study the dif-
LTAS has been used to study differences between normal and ferences between good and poor speaking voices of vocally
pathological voices58 and between various vocal patholo- trained professional male actors.32,33According to the results
gies.913Among normal-voiced subjects LTAS has been used LTAS distinguished between good, fairly good, rather poor,
to study individual and gender, age and language related differ- and poor voice qualities. Poor speaking voices had relatively
ences.4,1420 Characteristics of vocal expression of emo- lower sound level in the frequency range of 13 kHz, and
tions21,22 and acoustic differences between specific voice good voice quality differed from fairly good and rather poor
qualities2325 have also been studied with this method. More- voice qualities by having a more prominent peak between 3
over, LTAS has been used to investigate singing voice, for exam- and 4 kHz. This peak seemed to be a kind of an actors formant
ple, differences between voice categories and various singing (ref. singers formant34). Perceptual evaluation of voice quality
styles.26,27 correlated with the level of the peak.
The suitability of LTAS for vocal screening has also been The present study investigates the voice quality of normal-
questioned. In the study by Kitzing,24 the differences between voiced, vocally untrained male university students.
the LTAS of perceptually very different voice qualities were in
the magnitude of only 3%6%. Nor did Nolan find LTAS to
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects and recording
Accepted for publication March 26, 2008. Text reading samples of 50 Finnish male normal-voiced vocally
From the Department of Speech Communication and Voice Research, University of
Tampere, Tampere, Finland.
untrained university students (2027 years, mean 22 years; no
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Timo Leino, Department of Speech acting students included) were recorded in the same sound-
Communication and Voice Research, University of Tampere, Kalevantie 4, FIN-33014
Tampere, Finland. E-mail: timo.leino@uta.fi
treated studio using a digital recorder and a Bruel and Kjaer mi-
Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 6, pp. 671-676 crophone (4165) at a distance of 40 cm from the subjects
0892-1997/$36.00
2009 The Voice Foundation
mouth. The speakers were asked to read the same prose extract
doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2008.03.008 in their habitual way, using natural loudness, that is, neither too
672 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 6, 2009

soft nor with extra effort, and with neutral expression, thus, no region and the fundamental frequency range was measured by
artistic performances were allowed. No external monitoring the subtraction L1L0 (see Figure 1). This level difference
was provided to control for F0 or sound level, because this would has been found to correlate negatively with the perception of
have impaired the naturalness of the speech. The recordings breathiness (hypofunctional voice production35). That level dif-
were perceptually monitored by the researchers, and the subjects ference may also be affected by intensity and F0. In addition to
were asked to repeat the task if some of the requirements re- LTAS measurements, alpha ratio9 was also calculated with In-
ported above were not met. The duration of each sample was telligent Speech Analyser by subtracting the Leq of the range of
over one minute. The samples were calibrated for sound pres- 50 Hz1 kHz from that of the range of 15 kHz.
sure level measurements using a sine wave generator and a level The text reading samples were divided into different voice
meter (Bruel and Kjaer Frequency Analyzer 2120). quality classes according to the points given in the listening
evaluation (poor mean points 3 to 1.2, good: +0.8to
Listening evaluation +2.6, intermediate: 0.8 to +0.4). The statistical significance
The reading samples were evaluated by two experienced profes- of the differences between the voice quality groups was studied
sional voice trainers and five students of vocology. The listeners by carrying out Students unpaired t-tests and nonparametric
rated the voice quality using a scale 3, 2, 1, 0, +1, +2, +3 MannWhitney U tests. To illustrate the results, the LTAS of in-
(3 very poor, +3 excellent). All subjects had training in dividual samples were averaged over voice quality classes with
perceptual analysis of voices, which had been given in the a custom-made computer program (developed by Heikki Ala-
same department. According to the terminology used in the per- talo, DSP-Systems).
ceptual training, voice quality is one of the three basic character-
istics of voice together with pitch and loudness. Listening tests
RESULTS
were carried out in a sound-treated studio. The samples were
played back with a high-quality loudspeaker (Genelec Biamp Test results
1019A). The listeners participated in the test in two groups to al- Interrater and intrarater reliability of the listeners evaluations
low each subject to be seated sufficiently close (at 2 m) to the was satisfactory (Cronbachs alpha 0.89, Spearmans Corre-
loudspeaker, directly in front of it. The whole 1-minute samples lation r 0.75, P < 0.001, respectively). There was a positive
were presented. Leq was set to be the same in each sample on the correlation between the points given for voice quality and the
listening tape. The Intelligent Speech Analyser (ISA) signal average sound energy level in different frequency ranges
analysis system, developed by Raimo Toivonen, M.Sc. Eng., (Spearman correlation coefficient r 0.39, P 0.005 for 1
was used in this standardization. First the true Leq of each sam- 2 kHz, r 0.40, P 0.004 for 23 kHz, and r 0.33,
ple was measured. Then one of the samples was chosen as the P 0.018 for 34 kHz). L1L0 difference did not correlate
reference and the Leq of all the other samples were set up to significantly with the perceptual evaluation. Alpha ratio also
be the same as that of the reference by increasing or decreasing correlated with voice quality (r 0.31, P 0.032).
their level by the amount needed. The average points given in the listening evaluation allowed
a grouping of the voice samples into 14 good voices, 14 poor
Spectrum analysis voices, and 22 intermediate voices. Figure 2 compares the aver-
LTAS was made with a real-time spectrum analyzer (Hewlett- age LTAS of the students good, poor, and intermediate voices.
Packard signal analyzer 3561A). A 400-point linear narrow The poor voices differed from the good voices by having rela-
band fast Fourier transform (FFT) analysis was used. Earlier tively less sound energy at the range of 13 kHz. The good voi-
findings33 have shown that broadband analysis does not differ- ces also had a more prominent peak at 34 kHz. The
entiate sufficiently between voice qualities. intermediate voices did not differ from the good ones in terms
The frequency span was 10 kHz; fast display mode was of the LTAS characteristics. The analyses were made using
used since it provides with real-time registration up to
7.5 kHz. Voiceless segments were left out of the analysis
by using an analogous s-gate. Pauses were excluded by using
the amplitude trigger of the analyzer. The time record length
was 40 millisecond, and the overlap was 15%. The display
resolution was 25 Hz. The flat top window with the fre-
quency bandwidth of 98 Hz was used because it is practically
ripple free (<0.01 dB) in the passband, and thus gives the
best amplitude accuracy. This characteristic makes Flat Top
window suitable, for instance, for calibration purposes.
The LTAS were normalized for the strongest peak and com-
pared to each other according to the level differences between
frequency ranges. The fact that relative levels were compared
instead of absolute levels made the subjects comparable to FIGURE 1. Schematic picture of measurement of the level differ-
each other regardless of the absolute Leq values of their sam- ence L1L0 between the F1 variation range (L1) and the F0 variation
ples. In addition, the level difference between the first formant range (L0) in LTAS. In this case the difference is +2.5 dB.
Timo Leino Students Voices 673

professional actors.33 Good voices were characterized by


a less steep spectral slope and a more prominent peak at 3
4 kHz. This peak, however, was not as prominent in the good
students voices as in the best voices of the actors.33 For the stu-
dents the level of the 3.5 kHz peak was on the average 28 dB,
relative to the strongest peak in the LTAS. According to earlier
results, the level of this peak was on the average 20 dB for the
best voices of the actors. The correlation between the strength
of the peak and the perceptual voice quality was low in students,
whereas according to earlier results it was moderate in actors.33
Good voices did not differ from the ordinary voices among the
FIGURE 2. Average LTAS for the male university students good
students. This acoustic finding is in accordance with voice
voices (N 14, dark line), poor voices (N 14, grey line), and inter-
trainers perception that clearly good voices are quite rare
mediate voices (N 22, dotted line).
among vocally untrained university students. Among young
a 10 kHz frequency range, but in figures the spectra are com- students this may be due to social pressure not to be different
pared in the range of 05 kHz as no significant differences from the others.
were found above 5 kHz. LTAS illustrates both phonatory and resonatory characteris-
Table 1 shows the statistical results. It can be seen that the tics. However, Nolan23 has found that LTAS is more capable
difference between the good and the poor voices was significant of distinguishing between phonatory than resonatory differ-
in all other ranges studied except for 23 kHz and 45 kHz. The ences in voice qualities. The level differences between fre-
difference between the good and the intermediate voices, in- quency ranges in the LTAS reflect the slope of the source
stead, was nonsignificant in all ranges. L1L0 level difference spectrum, which in turn is related to the glottal closing speed.
did not distinguish the three voice quality groups either (good Increased closing speed gives a less tilting slope.36 The spectral
voices: mean 0.64 dB, SD 3.6; poor voices: mean 2.11 dB, tilt is known to decrease together with vocal intensity and effort
SD 3.6 and intermediate voices: mean 0.27 dB, SD 4.2. Differ- increase. This is due to the fact that the higher overtones gain
ences between the groups were nonsignificant). more in intensity than the lower ones when the overall intensity
Table 2 summarizes the results of Leq measurements. Stu- is increased.37 In the present study the students with good and
dents with good voices had significantly higher Leq than those ordinary voices used somewhat more intense voice than the
with poor voices, while students with intermediate voices did students with poor voices (mean difference 1.9 dB between
not differ significantly from those with good voices. good and intermediate voices and 5.1 dB between good and
poor voices).
DISCUSSION Sound level and voice quality seem to be interrelated. In gen-
Differences in the LTAS of the voice quality groups among eral, sound level tends to be somewhat higher after vocal exer-
students were basically similar to those reported earlier for cising or a longer voice training period.38,39 This may suggest

TABLE 1.
Differences between the average LTAS and Alpha ratio (level difference above and below 1 kHz): Leq
(15 kHz)Leq (50 Hz1 kHz) of good voices (N 14), poor voices (N 14), and intermediate voices (N 22)
of Finnish male university students (N 50 in total)
01 kHz 12 kHz 23 kHz 34 kHz 45 kHz Alpha
Good
Mean 7.6 dB 18.5 dB 27.5 dB 32.2 dB 42.4 dB 11.3
SD 1.8 3.3 4.0 4.1 2.8 (15.3 to 8.1)
Poor
Mean 9.8 dB 22.0 dB 30.0 dB 36.5 dB 44.5 dB 13.3
SD 2.1 3.5 2.5 4.1 5.0 (18.3 to 8.7)
Intermediate
Mean 8.3 dB 18.2 dB 26.5 dB 31.6 dB 42.3 dB 10.5
SD 2.1 4.3 5.7 6.0 3.8 (15.5 to 5.9)
Significance of differences
Good/poor P 0.006 P 0.012 NS P 0.015 NS P 0.043
Good/intermediate; NS NS NS NS NS NS
Mean and standard deviations are given for the averages of the normalized amplitude values in different frequency ranges. Statistical significance: MannWhit-
ney U test.
NS nonsignificant P > 0.05.
674 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 6, 2009

TABLE 2.
Differences between Leq of the students with good voices (N 14), poor voices (N 14),
and intermediate voices (N 22)
Good Intermediate Poor
Mean 72.8 dB 70.9 dB 67.7 dB
SD 2.7 3.6 4.0

Significance of differences
Good/poor P 0.001 Good/intermediate; NS Intermediate/poor; P 0.019
Statistical significance: MannWhitney U test.
NS nonsignificant P > 0.05.

that subglottic pressure is higher after training. We may ask formant. According to the results of Iivonen and Laukkanen,45
whether the only difference between speakers with good voices the fourth formant for a male speaker of Finnish was located at
and those with not so good voices is related to the habitual loud- the range 3,3603,800 Hz depending on both the vowel and its
ness of speechand could thus be abolished by merely asking duration. Thus, the vowel-related changes in the frequency of
the speakers with poor voices to speak more loudly. On the F4 were only minor. This suggests that F4 is not very heavily
other hand, somewhat higher sound level could also be a sign dependent on articulation and thus most likely is not very lan-
of improved vocal function per se. For instance, there are results guage dependent either. A fairly strong peak at 3.5 kHz can
suggesting that the glottal spectrum tilts less for trained singers also be seen in the LTAS of speakers of different languages: De-
than for untrained subjects.38 This could result from a higher jonckere published such an LTAS in a French speaking subject,6
glottal closing speed or a more favorable input impedance of Frkjr-Jensen and Prytz9 obviously from samples in Danish
the vocal tract, for example, epilaryngeal narrowing.40 A faster and Nolan from a sample in English.23 Nawka found this
glottal closing speed could show a more adequate adduction peak in good voices among German speakers.46 This peak,
and/or improved mobility of the vocal folds. The latter could however, is not an absolute prerequisite of a good voice quality,
possibly result from a more economic activity ratio between cri- and sometimes it can also be seen in not so good voices,47 for
cothyroid and thyroarytenoid muscles.41 The strength of the 3 example, in those with vocal fry.23
4 kHz peak compared to the strongest spectral peak (F0 or F1) The level difference L1L0 has been found to correlate neg-
is naturally to some extent related to overall spectral slope, but atively with the perception of breathiness (hypofunctional voice
the prominence of that peak in the good voices of the actors production35). Furthermore, the level difference has been found
does not seem to be merely a result of a less tilting spectral to rise after voice training while the perceptual voice quality
slope. According to the earlier studies by Leino,33 it was not also became tighter (ie, more hyperfunctional).39 According
the level differences between frequency ranges but the promi- to the results of the present study, L1L0 difference was on av-
nence of the 3.5 kHz peak in the average LTAS that distin- erage positive for the good and ordinary voices and negative for
guished the actors good voices from the fairly good and the poor voices. This suggests that the relative level of the F0
rather poor voices. Below 3 kHz the relative sound level of was lower and voice production thus most likely tighter in the
the rather poor voices was the same and that of the fairly students with good and ordinary voices compared to those
good voices was even higher than that of the good voices. In in- with poor voices. This may be related to greater loudness or oth-
dividual LTAS, it has been possible to see that the peak at erwise tighter adduction. Bele has also found a greater L1L0
3.5 kHz, the actors formant, can be almost as strong or in and stronger sound level in actors compared to teachers.47 In the
some cases even stronger than the preceding peak in the range present study, however, L1L0 difference did not differentiate
of 23 kHz.42 This suggests that the peak is not only a result of significantly between voice qualities of normal voiced un-
strong voice source overtones in general but of a special reso- trained students. This may be due to the fact that not so good
nance type, most likely related to the actors technique of pro- voice may be either more hypofunctional or more hyperfunc-
jecting. The region above 3 kHz is supposed to be more prone tional than the optimum and thus at group level the differences
to show resonatory than phonatory characteristics.24 The results in this parameter will cancel each other out.
of Hurme43 also showed that when the speakers increased voice
intensity (556575 dB) in text reading (30 seconds), the level
differences between frequency ranges in the LTAS (strongest CONCLUSIONS
peak set as zero) changed substantially more below 3 kHz In vocally untrained male students speaking samples, those
than above it. At 4 kHz the change was nonsignificant. This which were perceived as representing a good voice quality dif-
was also demonstrated in Nordenberg and Sundberg.44 fered from those perceived as representing a poor voice quality
The clear valleys separating the 3.5 kHz peak from its sur- by having a relatively higher sound level between 1 and 3 kHz
roundings suggest that it is formed by a formant or a cluster in the LTAS and a more prominent peak at 34 kHz. Good voi-
of formants. This peak may basically be related to the fourth ces did not differ from intermediate voices. Leq was
Timo Leino Students Voices 675

significantly lower in samples evaluated as poor in voice qual- 18. Harmegnies B, Landercy A. Intra-speaker variability of the long term speech
ity. The level difference between the regions of the fundamental spectrum. Speech Commun. 1988;7:81-86.
19. Pavlovic V, Rossi M, Espesser R. Statistical distributions of speech of var-
and the first formant did not differentiate between voice quali-
ious languages. J Acoust Soc Am. 1990;88(Suppl 1):176.
ties. 20. Byrne D, Dillon H, Tran K, et al. An international comparison of long-term
The peak at 34 kHz (actors formant) cannot result from average speech spectra. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994;96:2108-2120.
louder voice alone but seems to imply some resonatory phe- 21. Williams CE, Stevens KN. Emotions and speech: some acoustical corre-
nomena as well. The effects of Leq and resonance characteris- lates. J Acoust Soc Am. 1972;52:1238-1250.
22. Pittam J, Gallois C, Callan V. The long-term spectrum and perceived emo-
tics on the LTAS characteristics of trained and untrained good
tion. Speech Commun. 1990;9:177-187.
voices need to be studied further. 23. Nolan F. The Phonetic Bases of Speaker Recognition. Cambridge, MA:
Cambridge University Press; 1983.
Acknowledgments 24. Kitzing P. LTAS criteria pertinent to the measurement of voice quality.
J Phon. 1986;14:477-482.
The author is grateful to Mr. Jussi Helin for technical assistance 25. Pittam J. Discrimination of five voice qualities and prediction to perceptual
ratings. Phonetica. 1987;44:38-49.
in the analyses and to Mrs. Virginia Mattila, M.A., for language
26. Dmitriev L, Kiselev A. Relationship between the formant structure of dif-
correction of the manuscript. Professors Johan Sundberg and ferent types of singing voices and the dimension of supraglottal cavities.
Anne-Maria Laukkanen are acknowledged for their valuable Folia phoniatr. 1979;31:238-241.
comments. 27. Rossing T, Sundberg J, Ternstrom S. Acoustic comparison of voice use in
solo and choir singing. J Acoust Soc Am. 1986;79:1975-1981.
REFERENCES 28. Lofqvist A, Mandersson B. Long-time average spectrum of speech and
1. Ansi S. USA standard: Acoustical terminology (s 1.1). New York: Ameri- voice analysis. Folia Phoniatr. 1987;39:221-229.
can National Standards Institute, Inc.; 1960. 29. Novak A, Dlouha O, Capoca B, Vohradnik M. Voice fatigue after theatre
2. Titze IR. Principles of voice production. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice performance in actors. Folia Phoniatr. 1991;43:74-78.
Hall; 1994. 30. Rantala L, Paavola L, Korkko P, Vilkman E. Working-day effects on the
3. Li K-P, Hughes GW, House AS. Correlation characteristics and dimension- spectral characteristics of teaching voice. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 1998;50:
ality of speech spectra. J Acoust Soc Am. 1969;46:1019-1025. 205-211.
4. Majewski W, Rothman H, Hollien H. Acoustic comparisons of American 31. Jonsdottir V, Laukkanen A-M, Siikki I. Changes in teachers voice quality
English and Polish. J of Phon. 1977;5:247-251. during a working day with and without electric sound amplification. Folia
5. Gauffin J, Sundberg J. Clinical applications of acoustic voice analysis Phoniatr Logop. 2003;55:267-280.
acoustical analysis, results and discussion. In: NH. Buch, ed. Proceedings 32. Leino T. The spectral characteristics of good voice. In: Finnish. Licentiate
of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics Congress Thesis in Logopedics. Helsinki: University of Helsinki, Department of Pho-
1518 August 1977, Copenhagen, Denmark. Herning: Organizing Commit- netics; 1976.
tee of the Congress, 1978:489502. 33. Leino T. Long-term average spectrum study on speaking voice quality in
6. Formby C, Monsen RB. Long-term average speech spectra for normal and male actors. In: Friberg A, Iwarsson J, Jansson E, Sundberg J, eds,
hearing-impaired adolescents. J Acoust Soc Am. 1982;71:196-202. SMAC93, Proceedings of the Stockholm Music Acoustics Conference,
7. Hurme P, Sonninen A. Normal and disordered voice quality: listening tests July 28August 1, 1993, No 79. Stockholm: The Royal Swedish Academy
and long-term spectrum analyses. In: Hurme P, ed, Papers in Speech Research, of Music; 1994:206-210.
Vol. 6. Jyvaskyla: Department of Communication, University of Jyvaskyla; 34. Sundberg J. Articulatory interpretation of the singing formant. J Acoust Soc
1985:49-72. Am. 1974;55:838-844.
8. Dejonckere PH. Acoustic analysis of voice production. Essai de synthese 35. Laukkanen A-M, Vintturi J, Vilkman E, Sala E, Siikki I, Lukkarila P. Per-
dans une optique clinique. Acta Oto-Rhino-Laryngologica Belgica. 1986; ceptual, acoustic and self-reported correlates of vocal loading. In: Proceed-
40:377-385. ings of the XXVth world congress of the International Association of
9. Frkjr-Jensen B, Prytz S. Registration of voice quality. Bruel Kjr Tech Logopedics and Phoniatrics in Montreal.
Rev. 1976;3:3-17. 36. Gauffin J, Sundberg J. Data on the glottal voice source behavior in vowel
10. Prytz S. Long-time-average-spectra (LTAS) analyses of normal and patho- production. In: Speech Transmission Laboratory, Quarterly Progress and
logical voices. In: NH. Buch, ed. Proceedings of the International Associ- Status Report, 23. Stockholm: Royal Institute of Technology; 1980. 61
ation of Logopedics and Phoniatrics Congress 1518 August 1977, 70.
Copenhagen, Denmark. Herning: Organizing Committee of the Congress, 37. Fant G. Speech Sounds and Features. Cambridge, MA: The MIT (Massa-
1978: 459475. chusetts Institute of Technology) Press; 1973.
11. Wendler J, Doherty ET, Hollien H. Voice classification by means of long- 38. Wedin S, Leanderson R, Wedin L. Evaluation of voice training. In: N.H.
term speech spectra. Folia phon. 1980;32:51-60. Buch, ed. Proceedings of the International Association of Logopedics
12. Hammarberg B, Fritzell B, Gauffin J, Sundberg J, Wedin L. Perceptual and and Phoniatrics Congress 1518 August 1977, Copenhagen, Denmark.
acoustic correlates of abnormal voice qualities. Acta otolaryngologica. Herning: Organizing Committee of the Congress, 1978:361381.
1980;90:441-451. 39. Laukkanen A-M, Syrja T, Laitala M, Leino T. Effects of two-month vocal
13. Dejonckere PH, Villarosa D. Long-term spectrum analysis of the voice. exercising with and without spectral biofeedback on student actors speak-
Comparaison de voix normales et de voix alterees par differentes categories ing voice. Log Phon Vocol. 2004;29:66-76.
de pathologies laryngees. Acta Oto-Rhino Laryngologica Belgica. 1986;40: 40. Titze IR, Story BH. Acoustic interactions of the voice source with the lower
426-435. vocal tract. J Acoust Soc Am. 1997;101(4):2234-2243.
14. Benson R, Hirsh I. Some variables in audio spectrometry. J Acoust Soc Am. 41. Titze IR, Talkin DT. A theoretical study of the effects of various laryngeal
1953;25:499-505. configurations on the acoustics of phonation. J Acoust Soc Am. 1979;66(1):
15. Pruzansky S. Pattern matching procedure for automatic talker recognition. 60-74.
J Acoust Soc Am. 1963;35:354-358. 42. Leino T, Karkkainen P. On the effects of vocal training on the speaking
16. Niemller A, McCormick L, Miller J. On the spectrum of spoken English. J voice quality of male student actors. In: Elenius K, Branderud P, eds,
Acoust Soc Am. 1974;55:461. Proceedings of the XIIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences,
17. Kiukaanniemi H, Siponen P, Mattila P. Individual differences in the long- Stockholm, Sweden 1319 August, 1995, Vol. 3 of 4. Stockholm: De-
term speech spectrum. Folia phoniatrica. 1982;34:21-28. partment of Speech Communication and Music Acoustics, Royal Insti-
676 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 6, 2009

tute of Technology and the Department of Linguistics, Stockholm Uni- and Phonetics 4, Series B: Phonetics, Logopedics and Speech Communica-
versity; 1995:496-499. tion, 5. Helsinki: Department of Phonetics, University of Helsinki; 1993:
43. Hurme P. Acoustic Studies of Voice Variation. Doctoral thesis. In: Jyvaskyla 29-54.
Studies in Communication, 7. Jyvaskyla: University of Jyvaskyla; 1996. 46. Nawka T, Anders LC, Cebulla M, Zurakowski D. The speakers formant in
44. Nordenberg M, Sundberg J. Effect on LTAS of vocal loudness variation. male voices. J Voice. 1997;11(4):422-428.
Logop Phoniatr Vocol. 2004;29:183-191. 47. Bele IV. Professional Speaking Voice: A Perceptual and Acoustic Study of
45. Iivonen A, Laukkanen A-M. Explanations of the qualitative variation of Male Actors and Teachers Voices. Doctoral dissertation. University of
Finnish vowels. In: Iivonen A, Lehtihalmes M, eds. Studies in Logopedics Oslo; 2002.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi