Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 5

21/3/2017 TheBeautifulPowersofUnconsciousThought

Psychological Science Agenda (/science/about/psa/index.aspx) | October 2009


(/science/about/psa/2009/10/index.aspx)

SCIENCE BRIEFS

The Beautiful Powers of Unconscious


Thought
When you're faced with an important decision, people often
recommend you "sleep on it." Does science support that belief?

By Ap Dijksterhuis (http://www.apa.org/search.aspx?query=&fq=ContributorFilt:%22Dijksterhuis,
Ap%22&sort=ContentDateSort desc)

Ap Dijksterhuis obtained his PhD in 1996 at the Radboud University Nijmegen (at
that time still called University of Nijmegen). Between 1996 and 1999 he was a
Research Fellow of the Royal Dutch Academy of Arts and Sciences and in 2000
moved to the University of Amsterdam as an Associate professor. In 2002 he
became full professor and in 2006 moved back to the Radboud University
Nijmegen. In 2005, Ap won both the APA Award for Early Career Contributions and
the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology Kurt Lewin Award. In
that same year, he also received a Vici grant--the largest grant of the Dutch Science Foundation
(NWO)--to fund research on unconscious thought and decision making. In 2007 he published a
popular psychology book in Dutch called 'Het slimme onbewuste' ('The smart unconscious') which
became a bestseller. Recently, HP/De Tijd selected Ap as one of the top 100 of most inuential Dutch
people.

"When making a decision of minor importance, I have always found it advantageous to consider all
the pros and cons. In vital matters however...the decision should come from the unconscious, from
somewhere within ourselves."
Sigmund Freud

When you are facing an important decision, others will sometimes tell you to postpone your decision
and "sleep on it" rst. In my case it was often my grandmother who gave me this advice. It is a belief
many people intuitively share: It helps to put a problem aside for some time in order to arrive at a
better decision. Somehow, waiting seems to help us to dierentiate between the vital and the futile.
Postponing a decision helps us to base our decisions on the appropriate reasons.

But does this "folk belief" hold in a scientic experiment? A few years ago, we conducted an
experiment in which we had people choose between four hypothetical apartments. The information
was constructed in such a way that one of the four apartments was objectively more desirable than
the other three, in that it possessed more positive and fewer negative qualities. However, this was not
immediately evident as the apartments were described with a great deal of information. After our
experimental participants read all the information about the apartments, they chose their favorite one
http://www.apa.org/printthis.aspx 1/5
21/3/2017 TheBeautifulPowersofUnconsciousThought

either immediately or after a period of distraction during which they did some other things. Our
hypothesis was that the latter group would continue to "unconsciously think" about the apartments
while they were distracted. Indeed, our ndings showed that 37 % of the participants who decided
immediately chose the appropriate apartment, whereas 60 % of the unconscious thinkers chose the
best one (see Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006). Postponing a decision helps, even
if one does not consciously think about it anymore.
The next question was whether unconscious thought could be even more helpful than an equal
period of conscious thought. Traditionally, most scholars on decision making have assumed that
thorough conscious thought is the best strategy to arrive at sound decisions. This is without doubt
sometimes true, but as a general principle it needs to be qualied. We know that under some
circumstances (e.g., Wilson & Schooler, 1991), conscious thought deteriorates the quality of decisions.
In another experiment we conducted (Bos et al., 2009), our participants chose between six houses
that were on sale in our home city, Nijmegen. We simulated the website on which these houses were
advertised but removed the asking price. Our participants were given a few minutes time to navigate
our "website," and some participants were then given as much time as they wanted to think about the
houses, and to further browse through the information. Others were distracted for about 45 minutes
(they actually did other experiments) before they decided. Finally, participants chose their favorite
house and they were asked to estimate the asking price for each of the six houses based on the
information provided. The unconscious thinkers - that is, the ones that were distracted - performed
signicantly better than the conscious thinkers, a nding that has now been replicated a number of
times (see Strick et al., 2009, for a meta-analysis).
In other experiments (Dijksterhuis, Bos, van der Leij & van Baaren, 2009), we asked immediate
decision makers, conscious thinkers, and unconscious thinkers to predict the results of soccer
matches that were to be played in the near future. The accuracy of the predictions did not dier much
for people who didn't know much about soccer. For fans, however, the results did dier. Fans who
thought unconsciously made better predictions than fans who thought consciously or fans who
guessed immediately. Interestingly, for both immediate decision makers as well as for conscious
thinkers, knowledge of soccer did not correlate with the quality of the predictions. Only among
unconscious thinkers was this correlation obtained, indicating that the benets of expertise, at least
within the connes of the present paradigm, become apparent when one thinks unconsciously rather
than consciously.
Recently, the nding that unconscious thinkers outperform conscious thinkers and immediate decision
makers has been replicated in new and interesting domains. Whereas our original experiments were
mainly concerned with consumer decisions, it is now established that unconscious thought can
improve clinical diagnoses made by clinical psychology students (de Vries et al., 2009), judgments of
justice in hypothetical court cases (Ham, van den Bos & van Doorn, in press), and even moral
judgments (Ham & van den Bos, in press).
But why is unconscious thought so helpful? In our view, there are two reasons. The rst has to do with
the dierent processing capacities of conscious and unconscious processes. Consciousness works in
a serial fashion and has a small capacity. That is, it can only do one thing at a time, and it can only
work on a very limited amount of information. Unconscious processes have the capacity to work on
dierent things in parallel and can integrate a large amount of information. In a series of experiments,
we (Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren & van Baaren, 2006) tested whether the amount of information
involved in a decision determines which strategy - conscious thought or unconscious thought - is the
most fruitful. In these experiments, experimental participants had to choose their favorite among four
hypothetical cars. For some, the cars were only characterized by four attributes, while for other
participants the cars were characterized by twelve attributes. Indeed, conscious thinkers fared well
when they had to choose among cars described by only four attributes (they actually did a bit better
than unconscious thinkers), but they performed very poorly when they had to choose among the cars
http://www.apa.org/printthis.aspx 2/5
21/3/2017 TheBeautifulPowersofUnconsciousThought

with twelve attributes. The dierence in capacity was also suggested in another experiment
(Dijksterhuis, 2004), where we asked conscious and unconscious thinkers who had just chosen
between four apartments whether they had based their decision on just one or two attributes or on a
more holistic, global judgment. The majority of the conscious thinkers claimed to have followed the
rst strategy, whereas the unconscious thinkers had done the latter.

We (Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren & van Baaren, 2006) once interviewed people who had recently
purchased a consumer product. We asked whether they had thought a lot about it consciously, or
whether they had followed the "sleep on it" strategy. As it turned out, buyers of products that only
require limited information processing (CD's, clothes, small things such as shampoo), experienced
more post-choice satisfaction after conscious rather than unconscious thought, whereas buyers of
more complex products (cars, cameras, furniture) experienced more satisfaction after unconscious
thought.
The second major reason we oer for why unconscious thought is helpful is that it seems to be better
at weighting the relative importance of dierent attributes. People who judge a house know that a
large space as well as many electrical sockets in the kitchen are positive attributes, but they do well
to recognize that the rst attribute is far more important than the second. Obviously, it should receive
more weight in a decision.

The idea that unconscious processes may, at least sometimes, be more eective at weighting than
conscious processes is not new. It was already suggested by Freud and more recently by Wilson &
Schooler (1991). In their experiments, they showed that conscious thought may actually be detrimental
for the weighting process. In our own work, we obtained some suggestive evidence that unconscious
thought is eective at weighting. In one experiment (Dijksterhuis & van Olden, 2006), participants had
to choose one among ve art posters. They did this immediately after seeing the posters, after some
time of conscious thought, or after a period of unconscious thought. After they had chosen a poster
to take home with them, we also asked participants to what extent they liked each of the ve posters.
A few weeks later, we called our participants, and asked them how satised they were with their
choice, to what extent they experienced regret, and for how much money they would be willing to sell
their poster. As it turned out, unconscious thinkers were more satised and asked for a higher selling
price than other participants. Also, the degree to which they liked the chosen poster relative to the
ones not chosen correlated with their satisfaction. This was not true for conscious thinkers: The extent
to which they liked their poster after choosing it was not predictive of later satisfaction. This strongly
suggests they engaged in poor weighting by using the wrong reasons to choose.

Recently, we discovered that unconscious thought leads to an automatic weighting process that
continues - at least for a while - as time passes (Dijksterhuis, Bos & van Baaren, 2009). Assume you
have to choose between two cars, one with a few very important positive characteristics (good safety
record, good mileage) and many unimportant negative characteristics (not many cupholders, only
available in three colors), the other with many unimportant positive characteristics (enough
cupholders, available in many dierent colors), and few important negative characteristics (poor safety
record, not very good mileage). Most people would choose the former car over the latter, and if you
ask people immediately after processing information about such cars, they indeed show a mild
preference for the former car. However, after a period of unconscious thought, this preference for the
appropriate car has become much stronger. During unconscious thought, important matters become
more important, whereas unimportant matters become more unimportant.
Still, unconscious thought is not eective in all tasks. It computes rather rough preferences that allow
us to sense that we like one alternative better than another. However, it is not a precision instrument,
as it cannot actively use propositional rules such as are needed in arithmetic (try to calculate 26 x 23
without conscious thought - it's impossible). Consciousness, on the other hand, is capable of doing
just that--making it more precise. This is relevant for decision making, as it means that when we are

http://www.apa.org/printthis.aspx 3/5
21/3/2017 TheBeautifulPowersofUnconsciousThought

faced with a decision that involves little information and not much weighting--that is, a decision for
which the weaknesses of conscious thought are not very relevant--conscious thinkers can actually
outperform unconscious thinkers. Likewise, when we have to make a decision based on numbers and
calculations - such as in games - conscious thought usually outperforms unconscious thought. Payne
and colleagues (Payne, Samper, Bettman, & Luce, 2008) provided people with information about
various strategies in a game. The various alternatives were based on numerical values (as in returns
of stocks) and on probability. In such a case, conscious thinkers are better than unconscious thinkers.
We have learned - especially in the Western world - that conscious deliberation is the holy grail of
decision making. This idea needs to be revised. Sometimes conscious thought is more helpful, but
sometimes unconscious thought is better. The important avenue for future research is to establish the
circumstances when one strategy is better than the other. Perhaps because we have learned that
conscious deliberation is almost always good, some people are surprised by, or skeptical about,
ndings that unconscious thought is helpful, or more generally, that we can perform very useful
cognitive operations without conscious guidance. However, our evolutionary history should make
clear that it is not surprising at all. Decision making is much older than human consciousness as we
know it, and as with all such ancient abilities, we are generally quite good at them if we rely on our
unconscious.
Acknowledgement: This research was supported by a Vici grant from the Netherlands Organisation
for Scientic Research (NWO) (453-05-004). More information on our research program can be found
at www.unconsciouslab.com.
References
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., Bongers, K. C. A., van der Leij, A., Sjoerdsma, A., & van Baaren, R. B.
(2009). Complexity and unconscious thought. Manuscript in preparation.

De Vries, M., Holland, R. W., Witteman, C., Vente, F., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). Intuitive processes in
diagnostic decision-making: Classication after conscious versus unconscious processing.
Manuscript under review.
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think dierent: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development
and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 586-598.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M.W., Nordgren, L.F., & van Baaren, R.B. (2006). On making the right choice: The
deliberation-without-attention eect. Science, 311, 1005-1007.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M.W., & van Baaren (2009). Unconscious thought leads to automatic weighting.
Manuscript under review.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Van der Leij, A., & van Baaren, R. B. (2009). Predicting soccer matches
after unconscious and conscious thought as a function of expertise. Psychological Science, in press.

Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L.F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 1, 95-109.
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Olden, Z. (2006). On the benets of thinking unconsciously: Unconscious
thought increases post-choice satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 627-631.
Ham, J., & Van den Bos, K. (in press). On unconscious morality: The eects of unconscious thinking on
moral decision making. Social Cognition.
Ham, J., van den Bos, K. & Van Doorn, E. (in press). Lady Justice thinks unconsciously: Unconscious
thought can lead to more accurate justice judgments. Social Cognition.
Payne, J., Samper, A., Bettman, J.R. & Luce, M.F. (2008). Boundary conditions on unconscious thought
in complex decision making. Psychological Science, 19, 1118-1123.
http://www.apa.org/printthis.aspx 4/5
21/3/2017 TheBeautifulPowersofUnconsciousThought

Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M.W., Sjoerdsma, A., Van Baaren, R.B., & Nordgren, L.F. (2009). A meta-
analysis on unconscious thought eects. Manuscript in preparation. See www.unconsciouslab.com
(http://www.unconsciouslab.com/) .

Wilson, T.D. & Schooler, J.W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of
preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181-192.

Find this article at:


http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2009/10/sci-brief.aspx

http://www.apa.org/printthis.aspx 5/5

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi