Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 12

6

Making Space for Children in Archaeological


Interpretations
Jane Eva Baxter
DePaul University

ABSTRACT
Childhood is a prolonged period of dependence during which children mature physically and acquire the cultural
knowledge necessary to become accepted members of a society. Members of every culture create and define “chil-
dren” through the process of socialization, whereby children are taught “acceptable” roles, practices, beliefs, and
values by their families, peers, and communities. Socialization creates a culturally specific framework for children’s
behavior, including their use of space. The process of socialization also relies on material culture as a means to
symbolically reinforce messages about proper behaviors, roles, and values. These factors make it possible to study
the process of socialization through the archaeological record. A recent comparative study of archaeological data
from five 19th-century domestic sites has been used (1) to demonstrate empirically that children produce structured
artifact distributions in the archaeological record and (2) to demonstrate that behavioral patterns and artifact types
may be used to investigate how children were socialized in past cultures. The results of this research have wide-
ranging implications for studying the distribution of material culture and interpreting site-formation processes at
both historic and prehistoric sites. This chapter presents an overview of this research with an emphasis on the theo-
retical and methodological basis for linking childhood socialization to the material record. Particularly, this chapter
introduces a methodology for recovering and interpreting evidence related to children in behavioral, nonmortuary
contexts in the archaeological record. While the case study is from 19th-century America, the methodologies are
presented to emphasize their applicability in other archaeological contexts, both historic and prehistoric.
Keywords: children, space, socialization, 19th century, America

A rchaeologists study not only the types of material cul-


ture encountered at archaeological sites but also the spa-
tial distribution of those materials. Interest in site-formation
(1987:85) has noted that the effects of children’s behavior
on archaeological sites are still poorly understood; yet at the
same time, children form a major part of the social units doc-
processes and the study of the distribution of artifacts in the umented at most archaeological sites. Children compose a
archaeological record have long been considered ways to un- significant demographic component of most human groups,
derstand human behavior in the past (Binford and Binford and in some cases children are the majority of a group’s
1966; Kent 1984; Schiffer 1976, 1987). More recent in- members (Hiner and Hawes 1985:xiv; Kelly 1995:207–208).
terest in cultural landscapes has expanded discussions of Reconstructing cultural landscapes and site-formation pro-
space, place, and behavior to include important social vari- cesses without children as cultural actors, therefore, rep-
ables such as status (Harrington 1989; Rotman and Nassaney resents a serious gap in archaeological interpretations of
1997; Stine 1990), ethnicity (Cheek and Frielander 1990; the past. These sentiments have been echoed recently by
Stine 1990), and gender (Gibb and King 1991). Andrew Chamberlain, who wrote, “children contribute to
All of these studies share one thing in common: they fo- the archaeological record whether or not we are competent
cus exclusively on the behaviors and lives of adults. Schiffer to recognize them” (Chamberlain 1997:249).

Archeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association, Vol. 15, pp. 77–88, ISSN 1551-823X.  C 2006 by the American Anthro-

pological Association. All rights reserved. Permissions to photocopy or reproduce article content via www.ucpress.edu/journals/rights.htm.
78 Jane Eva Baxter

Archaeological competency for recognizing children’s (Hammond and Hammond 1981). While these studies un-
behavior in nonmortuary contexts has been limited because doubtedly have identified certain aspects of children’s behav-
of two primary factors. The first is a general bias of West- ior, the problem is the appropriateness of such experiments
ern scholarship that characterizes children’s activities as as an analogy for archaeological contexts. Archaeologists
unimportant and peripheral to concerns of mainstream re- generally do not excavate the historical or prehistoric equiv-
search (Baxter 2005; Kamp 2001; Sofaer Derevenski 1994, alent of a vacant lot but rather focus on domestic settings,
1997, 2000). The second is a corpus of literature present- where different sets of social expectations and activities and
ing ethnoarchaeological and experimental data that suggest behaviors would be in place. Observations of children in
children’s behavior is unpatterned and therefore unknowable nondomestic settings do not offer insights as to how chil-
in the archaeological record (see below). dren use and distribute material culture in and around their
The research presented here actively questions this lat- homes.
ter assertion and develops an archaeological methodology The second issue is that these studies used very lim-
for identifying children’s behavior in the distribution of ma- ited numbers of observations as the basis for their conclu-
terials at archaeological sites (Baxter 2000). By focusing on sions. Patterning, random or otherwise, cannot adequately
children’s behavior and resulting artifact distributions rather be assessed after only a single play episode or test, and such
than material culture alone, this approach to the archaeology limited observations do not provide an adequate basis to
of childhood offers a methodological and theoretical basis interpret the archaeological record. This latter problem is
for studying children in sedentary societies in a variety of shared by most ethnoarchaeological studies that have ad-
temporal and geographic settings. dressed children’s behavior.
Ethnoarchaeological studies used to determine the
“randomizing” nature of children’s behavior were not de-
Rethinking Previous Work: Children as signed expressly with children’s behavior in mind, and thus
“Randomizing” and “Distorting” Factors observations of children’s behaviors were not systematic
or comprehensive. The portions of these studies that do
Archaeological literature linking the behavior of chil- refer to children have been used in a manner that David
dren to the distribution of artifacts in the archaeological and Kramer (2001:14, 16) have called “the cautionary tale,”
record has been in existence since the 1970s. This literature wherein ethnographic information acts as a spoiler to more
is composed of studies that used ethnoarchaeological obser- traditional and conventional archaeological interpretations.
vation (Bonnichsen 1973; Deal 1985; Hayden and Cannon The first such tale noted that children would take items
1983; Schiffer 1987; Stevenson 1990; Watson 1979) and ex- from their “proper places” or places of adult use or discard
perimental studies (Hammond and Hammond 1981; Wilk and move them to other locations. The fact that children’s
and Schiffer 1979) to study site-formation processes. These behavior altered the material expressions of adult behav-
studies did not focus specifically on children, with the excep- iors made children a “distorting factor” rather than active
tion of that of Hammond and Hammond (1981). Generally, members of the social unit under observation. The second
these studies were larger ethnoarchaeological analyses fo- “cautionary tale” addressed children’s often atypical, uncon-
cusing on other aspects of behavior and site formation that ventional, or unexpected uses of material culture. When chil-
also briefly reported the activities of children. This research dren were observed using material culture differently than
has led to an assumption that children have a randomizing adults, the behavior was described as random and defying
and/or distorting effect on artifact distributions that makes interpretation. Both of these tales were adopted into archae-
it virtually impossible for archaeologists to study children in ology in a widespread manner, and both reflect the biases
behavioral contexts. against children as important social actors.
It is both necessary and possible to question this assump-
tion based on the nature of the studies used in its formulation.
The experimental studies that focused on children’s behavior
and resulting artifact distributions are problematic for two The Socialization of Children and Their Use
main reasons: (1) the studies focused on children’s behav- of Space
ior away from the home and (2) the studies used the results
of single observations or experiments as the basis for their Critiques of previous research call into question
conclusions. long-held assumptions about children’s behavior and the
Archaeologists have conducted studies of children’s be- archaeological record. Theoretical discussions of child-
havior as mapping exercises in vacant lots (Wilk and Schiffer hood socialization further suggest that children’s behavior
1979) and as observations of children’s behavior in gardens should produce patterned distributions of artifacts in the
Making Space for Children 79

archaeological record that reflect cultural norms, beliefs, and and by discouraging or prohibiting them from others (Kent
practices. 1984:1; Spencer et al. 1989:107–108).
Scholars of childhood share the belief that childhood Hence, as children begin to explore their environment
is a social category defined differently in each cultural set- they do not encounter spaces that are culturally “neutral,” but
ting (Baxter 2005; Kamp, this volume; Keith, this volume). rather each space is given a cultural context and meaning that
Particularly, social historians have noted that attitudes, defi- shapes the types of behaviors and activities that take place
nitions, and behaviors of children are culturally and histori- there (Kent 1990; Rapoport 1990; Thomas, this volume).
cally situated and change in relation to the prevailing social, Rapoport (1990:10) notes that it is possible to understand
political, and economic trends in society. As historian Karin directly the relationship between behaviors, the expressions
Calvert has noted: of cultural ideals, and the use of space in a particular cultural
setting. Along similar lines, other researchers have noted that
Members of any society carry within themselves a work- divisions of age, sex, and social position are reproduced in
ing definition of childhood, its nature, limitations, and
duration. They may not explicitly discuss this definition, spatial divisions and act as a reflection of culturally segre-
write about it, or even consciously conceive of it as an gated behaviors and the cultural ideals that underlie those
issue, but they act upon their assumptions in all of their social divisions (e.g., Moore 1986).
dealings with, fears for, and expectations of their chil- The types of spatial knowledge that are developed in a
dren. Every culture defines what it means to be a child, child are dependent upon the types of culturally sanctioned
how children should look and act, what is expected of
them, and what is considered beyond their capabilities.
experiences, activities, and opportunities that are presented
[Calvert 1992:3] to them as they interact with their environment (Gauvin
1992:27). As adults prescribe and proscribe the use of cer-
Members of every culture create and define children tain areas for different children’s activities, they are shaping
through the process of socialization. This socialization pro- children’s perceptions of the world around them and influ-
cess transforms a newborn child into a social person who encing where children will spend their time and engage in
is capable of interacting with others. Socialization is carried particular activities.
out by individuals and organizations that impart messages to As the use of space is heavily influenced by cultural fac-
children through a variety of techniques, including tutoring tors, and as children’s relationships with their environment
and lecturing on certain subjects, rewarding and punishing are regulated as an integral part of their social development,
to reinforce certain attitudes and behaviors, and generating it should be expected that children would not use space in a
opportunities that expose or restrict children to certain expe- random fashion. Rather, children’s behaviors should demon-
riences (Damon 1988). However, the process of socialization strate regularities and patterning that reflect the social norms
often is more passive and experiential in nature as “those and guidelines for children’s behavior and use of space in a
who know the social rules of a group or a culture display particular cultural setting. While children’s behaviors, like
this knowledge repeatedly in their everyday activities . . . those of individuals of other socially constructed categories
expert knowers perforce generate examples of the pertinent (e.g., adult gender), are not strictly governed by their so-
databases all the time” (Gelman et al. 1991:250). Through cial category, they are guided by these culturally prescribed
these various methods and messengers children are encul- roles to a sufficient degree that these behaviors should be
turated and socialized in all aspects of their culture and are reflected in the distribution of material culture encountered
taught the norms, values, and behaviors deemed acceptable in the archaeological record (Gibb and King 1991).
or desirable in various social settings.
Many of these behaviors and expectations concern the
use of space. The use of space is culturally specific, so much
so that people often express uneasiness and disorientation Children and Space in Domestic Settings:
in situations in which spatial behavioral patterns differ from A Generalized Model
those to which they are accustomed (Kent 1984:1). As Von
Bruck notes, “The meaning of spatial order is inextricably Children’s development is shaped by both social and
linked to the interpretations given to it by historically situated ontological factors (Kamp, this volume; Perry, this volume),
social actors in a specific context of practice” (Von Bruck and childhood socialization is only one way that children
1997:142). Children are taught the acceptable locations for learn about their environment. Many developmental factors
different behaviors, such as eating, sleeping, work, and play. relating to children and their environments have been iden-
Locations deemed acceptable for these activities often vary tified cross-culturally, making it possible to generate some
by age and gender. Adults socialize children in the use of general ideas about children’s activities in domestic settings.
space by encouraging or restricting them to certain areas These general trends in children’s behavior may be used to
80 Jane Eva Baxter

develop testing strategies that will increase the likelihood


of encountering the remains of children’s activities in the
archaeological record.
Two major studies of children’s interactions with the
environment have discovered that children use play as a
mechanism to gain experience with their environment and to
learn about the world around them (Hart 1979; Moore 1986;
see Thomas, this volume). The exploration of the physical
world is an important component of this learning experience.
Robin Moore has noted that “children continue to assert their
urge to engage with the environment, to investigate and test
its possibilities, and to try things they haven’t tried before”
(Moore 1986:11). This sentiment is echoed by Roger Hart,
who wrote, “All children have the urge to explore the land-
scape around them, to learn about it, and to invest it with
meaning both shared and private” (Hart 1979:3).
Both of these researchers conducted descriptive studies
of children’s behavior in relation to the everyday environ-
ment. These studies were designed to assess the cognitive
and spatial properties of the physical environment and the
Figure 6.1. Idealized ranges of children’s play at domestic sites
development of place experience in children. The two studies
(after Hart 1979 and Moore 1986).
were conducted in a suburban community in Britain (Hart
1979) and in a more rural suburban community in New
England (Moore 1986). Although limited to industrialized consent and visibility and the desire to explore the immedi-
settings, these two large studies provide a consistent picture ate environment of the neighborhood. These two factors are
of how children use and perceive space around their homes central in the studies used as the basis for this general model
independent of their specific cultural setting. The recent time of children’s behavior.
frame of these studies, however, does present two particular Data for these two studies were collected only on the
issues for consideration. basis of firsthand exploration. The child and the researcher
The first consideration is that over the past three hundred walked through the neighborhood together and discussed the
years children’s play has become increasingly centered on the types of activities that took place in different areas. Children
home (Chawla 1994; Sutton-Smith 1994). Hence, a mod- were also asked to draw maps of their neighborhood, identi-
ern study of children’s behavior presents a more domestic- fying the places they frequented. Observations made by the
centric view of children’s play than may have been typical children during trips taken in vehicles or on walks when par-
in earlier time periods. The increase in the domestic focus ents or other adults were present were not collected as data.
of children’s play does not necessarily undermine the effi- While each researcher came up with different explana-
cacy of this general model for studying children’s behavior tory categories for their findings, both researchers identified
in household or community settings. It should be noted, how- a general, three-tiered model of “zones” or “ranges” of chil-
ever, that there are other possible ranges of children’s behav- dren’s behavior (Figure 6.1). In each model, the child’s home
ior that are not accounted for in a domestic-centric model serves as the center of all activities, with concentric ranges
such as found in Hart’s and Moore’s studies. radiating outward. Behavioral ranges encompass areas of
A second consideration is the effect that industrial, play, leisure places, and the pathways that connect them.
built environments have on children’s behavior. Community Children’s activities are not distributed evenly throughout
spaces, such as playgrounds, sports fields, and parks are of- these ranges but rather take place at relatively discrete lo-
ten designed to be locations of children’s play and this could cations within each range. Also, pathways used by children
be considered a barrier to using these studies. However, ex- often are not adult-determined routes but rather are chil-
tensive research has shown that children’s behavior varies dren’s short cuts that take them past more “interesting” fea-
little when these types of communal play spaces are brought tures of the neighborhood. The researchers also noted that
into neighborhoods (e.g., de Connick-Smith 1990; Francis these ranges are as much defined by adult prescriptions and
1985; Frost 1989; van Andel 1985). Instead, children’s be- proscriptions for children’s activities as by the children’s be-
havior has a greater tendency to be structured by parental haviors themselves.
Making Space for Children 81

Moore (1986) explains the three ranges of children’s to aid in the identification of artifacts and the interpreta-
behavior on the basis of time available for play activities. In tion of identified behavior. The second was the selection
Moore’s model, the innermost zone around the home is the of sites conducive to identifying children’s behavior. Third,
“habitual range” that is accessible to children on a daily basis a sampling strategy was developed to recover the remains
for short periods of time. The intermediate or “frequented” of children’s behavior, and, finally, analytical methods were
range is a less accessible area used only when larger blocks developed to search for patterning in children’s artifacts.
of time are available. The outermost zone or “occasional
range” is visited rarely on specialized excursions or outings. Creating a Social Context
The occasional range is unique in that only specific areas
within the zone are selected for children’s activity. These A fruitful first step in the archaeological study of child-
areas are not interconnected by pathways within the zone hood is to create a social context that reconstructs the cultural
but rather are visited as back-and-forth trips between the and social circumstances operating in a particular time and
home and the specific play location. place. Such a context acts as a framework both for identify-
In Hart’s (1979) model, interaction within the three ing children’s material culture and for interpreting children’s
zones of behavior is determined by the need to gain parental behavior as reflected in artifact distributions. Specifically, a
consent. Ranges are defined by a variety of factors, accord- social context makes it possible to generate expectations for
ing to Hart, including topography, visibility, and the pres- the nature and location of children’s activities in the archae-
ence/absence of dangerous features. The innermost zone in ological record by identifying potential roles, behaviors, and
this model is identified as the “free range” or the area where expectations for children in a particular cultural setting. Ide-
children can go without asking or telling a parent prior to ally, such a social context includes four main categories of
each excursion. The intermediate zone is the “range with per- information. These are
mission” for which parental consent must be gained before
1. The identification of which members of a social group
children can venture into play areas there. The outermost
are defined as children,
zone is defined as the “range with other children” for which
2. The roles and behaviors that are expected of children in
children need to negotiate the nature, duration, and location
a particular setting,
of play activities with their parents.
3. The children’s physical environment, including the mate-
Both researchers also noted that age was the primary
rial items selected for children’s use and for child rearing,
factor determining the extent of each range, with range
the architecture of the home, and the places delineated for
boundaries expanding outward as children matured. Age
children’s play outside of the home, and
also was a factor in the types of experiences and actions that
4. The social environment of the children, including family
took place in each range. Many areas that had been among
size and structure and the racial, ethnic, and class com-
a child’s favorite at an early age were later abandoned as
position of their community.
their ability to explore new areas increased. However, with
almost every child studied, “As the full potential of newly Information for developing a social context can come
discovered places is explored in depth, a sense of attachment from a variety of sources. My research used primary and sec-
and meaning arises. In some special places, the process can ondary historical data to create a social context for studying
go on for years with layers of successive play episodes accu- childhood in 19th-century America. However, the ability to
mulating on the landscape” (Moore 1986:19). Many of these study children archaeologically is not necessarily limited to
children’s “special places” were areas unused or abandoned the historic period. In prehistoric cases, both ethnographic
by adults, making them the exclusive domain of children and mortuary data may serve as fruitful sources of informa-
(Baxter 2000, 2005; Sobel 1990, 1993; Wilkie 2000). tion for creating a social context.

Site Selection
Identifying Children’s Behavior in the
Archaeological Record: A Brief Example The remains of children’s behavior are present at most
archaeological sites, but the selection of appropriate sites is
Theories of socialization and generalized models were still an essential component of investigating children’s be-
combined to develop a testing strategy to identify patterning havior archaeologically. The idea that children’s behavior
in children’s behavior in the archaeological record (Baxter produces identifiable patterns in the archaeological record
2000). This testing strategy involved four critical compo- was evaluated using archaeological data from five sites rep-
nents. The first was the development of a social context resenting a variety of domestic settings that were occupied
82 Jane Eva Baxter

Figure 6.2. Sites used in this study (after Baxter 2000).

between 1820 and 1900 (Figure 6.2). These sites are as how ideals of 19th-century childhood were translated in a
follows: variety of domestic settings, while strengthening general in-
ferences about children’s behavior.
1. Schyler Mansion in Albany, New York, which operated Specific sites were selected on the basis of four main
as an orphanage during the late 19th and early 20th cen- criteria in addition to their dates of occupation: (1) census
turies and was an institution associated with social reform or other documentary sources for each site were available to
operated by the Daughters of Charity. verify the presence of children; (2) each site had been sam-
2. The Felton Farmhouse in Westland, Michigan, an early pled systematically across the entire site area, enabling the
and modest farmstead dating from 1850 through the late reconstruction of overall patterns of yard use and the identi-
1920s. fication of features and activity areas; (3) each site provided
3. The William Conner House in Fishers, Indiana, an upper- adequate evidence to reconstruct 19th-century domestic ar-
class rural residence of one of the most prominent pio- chitecture, either through the presence of a standing structure
neers in Indiana. or through a combination of historic documents, archaeolog-
4. Orange Grove Plantation in Jefferson Parish, Louisiana, ical excavation, and the presence of comparable structures in
a successful sugar plantation that operated from the late the vicinity; and (4) each site had been subjected to little or
18th century until emancipation. no postdepositional disturbance since its 19th-century occu-
5. O’Brien and Costello’s Bar and Shooting Gallery in Vir- pation, making it possible to assess accurately the location
ginia City, Nevada, a saloon and boardinghouse in a very of features and activity areas in the archaeological record.
rough part of town that operated during the later years of
the 19th century.
Sampling Strategy
These sites represent a wide range of contemporaneous
domestic settings, including households of different socioe- Each of the sites was sampled using a combination of
conomic means, ethnic backgrounds, and geographic loca- extensive systematic shovel probe survey of the remaining
tions. The use of a variety of domestic site types enabled 19th-century yard and larger excavation units placed in areas
inter-site as well as intra-site comparisons of material culture of interest (Figure 6.3). This sampling provided coverage of
types and distributions associated with children. Inter-site all three “zones” or “ranges” of children’s behavior concep-
comparisons facilitated culturally specific interpretations of tualized in the urban planning models. Such an approach
Making Space for Children 83

Figure 6.3. Archaeological site map of the Felton Farmhouse site as a graphic example of the sampling strategy used at all five sites.
Solid black dots are shovel tests, numbered squares are excavation units. Outlines show the location of former and extant structures on
the property.

also allowed for the identification and investigation of spe- fore the density contours represent the relative concentra-
cific activity areas within the yard area and the opportunity tion of artifacts, rather than differences in excavated volume
to assess the location of children’s activities relative to other (Baxter 2000).
yard features. Children’s artifacts were identified using 19th-century
merchant catalogs and ladies’ publications. These historical
Analytical Methods sources depict and discuss appropriate toys and clothing for
children and often make suggestions for how the toys should
The data gathered from these excavations were ana- be used and by whom. Children’s artifacts used in this study
lyzed using both graphic and statistical methods. For each included marbles, tops, toy soldiers, dolls and doll acces-
site, a contour map was generated that represented the rela- sories such as tea sets, as well as clothing items specific
tive artifact densities in the overall 19th-century assemblage to children’s outfits of the period. Counts of children’s arti-
(Figure 6.4). Each contour map was created using data from facts were superimposed on the contour maps to illustrate
excavated 19th-century contexts and excluded artifacts clas- their spatial distribution across the site and the relationship
sified as structural remains (see South 1977). Structural re- between the distribution of children’s artifacts and the dis-
mains, such as brick, mortar, nails, and window glass, are tribution of the total assemblage. Because children’s artifact
found most often as dense concentrations that reflect the assemblages tend to be numerically small, such a visual as-
presence of a structure and distort distributions associated sessment provides a useful means to identify patterns that
with other yard activities. These contour maps were gener- could not be revealed by statistical methods. Children’s ar-
ated using a standard artifact frequency (number of artifacts tifacts are relatively abundant at the sites selected for this
per unit/excavated volume of unit) rather than artifact counts. study; however, they are still found in small numbers (be-
This standard artifact frequency provided a correction for tween 37 and 153 artifacts). These quantities of artifacts are
different unit sizes excavated at the same site, and there- adequate to perform certain statistical analyses and make up
84 Jane Eva Baxter

Figure 6.4. Contour map of relative artifact densities at the Felton Farmhouse with locations of children’s artifacts superimposed as
diamonds. A similar map was generated for each site in the study.

a large enough sample to assess reliably their distributions this study. Artifacts identified as children’s and child-related
using visual techniques. in historical sources were patterned in ways that were both vi-
The second component of this analysis was statistical. sually identifiable and statistically significant (Baxter 2000).
A Kruskal-Wallis test, a nonparametric analysis of variance The results of this study pointed to the issue of site size and
that tests distributions against a theoretical equal population ranges of children’s behavior when searching for children in
distribution, was used. The Kruskal-Wallis test was used to behavioral contexts.
test whether the differences in the samples can be explained The largest site area analyzed in this study was Orange
by simple random error and tested the null hypothesis that Grove Plantation, which still has over 34,000 square me-
children’s artifacts were distributed randomly across the site ters of remaining lands, which have been extensively sur-
area. To perform this test each site was divided into multi- veyed. Attempts to analyze children’s artifact distributions
ple regions that corresponded to subjectively defined areas over the entire site area failed because of the size of the sam-
around a domestic structure (e.g., front yard, back yard, side pled area. Children’s artifacts were clustered in proximity
yard). These regions were not defined on the basis of arti- to the domestic structures at the plantation, pointing to the
fact distributions but instead used clues from the particu- culturally bounded nature of children’s play spaces and ac-
lar landscape and built environment, as well as patterns of tivity areas even on a large site. When the sample universe
site layouts documented for 19th-century homesteads (Moir was redefined to the areas around the domestic structures,
1987; Rotenizer 1993; Rotman and Nassaney 1997; Stewart- patterning was readily discerned in the children’s artifact
Abernathy 1986). To account for the different number of test distributions.
units and different unit sizes among regions the standard ar- The smallest site in the sample was a saloon and lodg-
tifact frequency (artifacts per cubic meter) was used (Baxter ing house in Virginia City, Nevada, and this was the only site
2000; Figure 6.5). where there was no identifiable patterning in the children’s
artifact distributions. This site also was the only one in the
Results and Conclusions: Children’s Behavior study where no actual “yard” areas were present, and the
and the Archaeological Record areas available for children’s activity outside the home were
bounded by other cultural features such as adjacent struc-
This research strategy was successful in identifying pat- tures and roadways. The lack of available yard space and the
terned behaviors of children at four of the five sites used in resulting spatial constraints on children’s activities appear to
Making Space for Children 85

Figure 6.5. Site map of the Felton Farmhouse site showing regions. This same analytical process was done for each of the five sites in
the study.

have resulted in children using all available areas around the Figures 6.3–6.5. Artifacts attributable to children are super-
home for their play activities. imposed on a contour map showing the overall distribution
Clear evidence for the patterning of children’s artifacts of artifacts. These artifact locations are represented by di-
did emerge at the remaining three sites, as well as in the amonds, and numbers adjacent to the diamonds represent
area around the domestic structure at Orange Grove Plan- the quantity of children’s artifacts found at each location. A
tation. These patterns also appear to be independent of the visual inspection of these figures shows that children’s arti-
overall distribution of artifacts, suggesting these patterns are facts are found only in selected regions of the site. Nearly
not simply the result of postdepositional processes operat- 60 percent of the children’s artifacts come from a 20-by-20-
ing on the entire assemblage. Hence, children’s behavior is meter area in the northern portion of the farmyard near the
not “randomizing” or “distorting” but rather is patterned and garden and animal pen. Two concentrations of children’s ar-
identifiable in the archaeological record. tifacts are also noted, one directly adjacent to the north side
The identification of a structured pattern is not an expla- of the farmhouse and another in the extreme northwest cor-
nation for a distribution but rather an aid to interpret the cul- ner of the sample universe where three children’s artifacts
tural processes that produced the pattern (Hodder and Orton were found in a single shovel test and another in an adjacent
1976:30). One fruitful way to interpret children’s behaviors unit. Almost all of the children’s artifacts are found within
in the archaeological record is to return to the social context the “habitual” and “general” ranges of children’s play in the
created for a particular study. Artifact distributions should generalized model and therefore conform to the idea that
reflect not only children’s physical capabilities but also the most children’s activity would take place closer to the home.
types of activities considered permissible and appropriate Children’s artifacts have a high degree of overlap with
for children in a particular cultural setting. adult artifacts and areas of household activity. The majority
A brief example of how such an interpretation can of children’s artifacts were also identified in areas that could
illuminate children, their lives, and their relationships to easily be viewed from the Felton home, and therefore most
others comes from the Felton Farmhouse data depicted in children’s activity could have been supervised directly or
86 Jane Eva Baxter

indirectly by adults. The association between children’s arti- childhood over the past several years. Tom Rocek, Jay
facts and the garden and animal pen suggests that these areas Johnson, and the reviewers from the Archeology Division of
may have been associated with tasks that were allocated to the AAA were all immensely helpful in guiding me though
children as their contribution to the household. Work and the process of this volume and providing suggestions for this
play may have been combined as children tended crops and manuscript.
animals for their family’s consumption. Children in these
locations also would have been able to study adult activi- References
ties taking place around them and through this process of
observation learned about the daily operations of a farming Baxter, Jane Eva
household (see Keith, this volume). 2000 An Archaeology of Childhood: Children and Mate-
There are places where adult artifacts occur indepen- rial Culture in 19th Century America. Ph.D. disser-
dently of children’s materials as well. These areas include tation, Department of Anthropology, University of
the yard areas around the barn, herb garden, and root cellar. Michigan. Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfilms
It may be possible that children did not assist their parents International.
with activities that occurred in these areas or that certain 2005 The Archaeology of Childhood: Children, Gender,
aspects of these yard features were considered dangerous or and Material Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira
inappropriate for children. Press.
The two areas where children’s artifacts are found inde-
pendent of adult artifacts are also significant. The artifacts Binford, Lewis R., and Sally Binford
found just to the north side of the Felton home are in an area 1966 A Preliminary Analysis of Functional Variability
of otherwise low artifact density that does not appear to have in the Mousterian of LeVallois Facies. American
been the location of any significant household activities. The Anthropologist 68(2):238–295.
second concentration, in the northwest corner of the sample
universe, was located in an area that was under orchard culti- Bonnichsen, Robson
vation in the 19th century. The first such concentration may 1973 Millie’s Camp: An Experiment in Archaeology.
be evidence for children playing in a location where they World Archaeology 4(3):277–291.
were not underfoot but still could be supervised by adults
Calvert, Karin Lee Fishbeck
inside and around the home. The second location, however,
1992 Children in the House: The Material Culture of
is well outside any “habitual” range and is completely out of
Early Childhood, 1600–1900. Boston: Northeast-
sight from the Felton house and the areas of the active yard.
ern University Press.
This small concentration of artifacts in the former orchard
may represent a secret or special place of childhood where Chamberlain, Andrew T.
children went to play away from adult supervision. 1997 Commentary: Missing Stages of Life—Towards
The ability to identify patterns in the distribution of the Perception of Children in Archaeology. In
children’s artifacts is important, as it unlocks the potential Invisible People and Processes: Writing Gen-
to interpret children’s behavior through the archaeological der and Childhood into European Archaeology.
record, as well as the ability to relate children’s activities to Jenny Moore and Eleanor Scott, eds. Pp. 248–250.
other household activities. Combining archaeological test- Leicester: University of Leicester Press.
ing with contextualizing evidence enables the identification
of these patterns, as well as the opportunity to offer expla- Chawla, Louise
nations for how and why they occurred. These relationships 1994 Childhood’s Changing Terrain: Incorporating
suggested by patterned distributions of cultural materials Childhood Past and Present into Community Eval-
demonstrate that an understanding of children is not periph- uation. Childhood 4:221–233.
eral to mainstream research interests but is yet another lens
through which archaeologists can come to understand the Cheek, Charles D., and Amy Frielander
dynamics of past social groups. 1990 Pottery and Pig’s Feet: Space, Ethnicity, and Neigh-
borhood in Washington, D.C., 1880–1940. Histor-
Acknowledgments ical Archaeology 24(1):34–60.

I would like to thank the many people who have en- Damon, William
couraged and assisted in my work on the archaeology of 1988 Socialization and Individuation. In Childhood and
Making Space for Children 87

Socialization. G. Handel, ed. Pp. 3–10. New York: Social, Economic, and Ideological Analyses. His-
Aldine de Gruyter. torical Archaeology 23(1):1–44.

David, Nicholas, and Carol Kramer Hart, Roger


2001 Ethnoarchaeology in Action. Cambridge: 1979 Children’s Experience of Place. New York:
Cambridge University Press. Irvington.

Deal, Michael Hayden, Brian, and Aubrey Cannon


1985 Household Pottery Disposal in the Maya High- 1983 Where the Garbage Goes: Refuse Disposal in the
lands: An Ethnoarchaeological Interpretation. Maya Highlands. Journal of Anthropological Ar-
Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 4:243– chaeology 2:117–163.
291.
Hiner, N. Ray, and Joseph M. Hawes
de Connick-Smith, Ning 1985 Growing Up in America: Children in Historical Per-
1990 Where Should Children Play? City Planning from spective. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Knee-Height Copenhagen 1870–1920. Children’s
Hodder, Ian, and Clive Orton
Environments Quarterly 7(4):54–61.
1976 Spatial Analysis in Archaeology. New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Francis, Marc
1985 Children’s Use of Open Space in Village Homes.
Kamp, Kathryn
Children’s Environments Quarterly 1(4):36–38.
2001 Where Have All the Children Gone? The Archae-
ology of Childhood. Journal of Archaeological
Frost, Joe L. Method and Theory 8(1):1–34.
1989 Play Environments for Young Children in the
USA: 1800–1990. Children’s Environments Quar- Kelly, Robert L.
terly 6(4):17–24. 1995 The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-
Gatherer Lifeways. Washington, DC: Smithsonian
Gauvin, Mary Institution Press.
1992 Sociocultural Influence on the Development of
Spatial Thinking. Children’s Environments Quar- Kent, Susan
terly 9(1):27–36. 1984 Analyzing Activity Areas: An Ethnoarchaeological
Study in the Use of Space. Albuquerque: University
Gelman, R., C. Massey, and M. McManus of New Mexico Press.
1991 Characterizing Supporting Environments for Cog- 1990 Activity Areas and Architecture: An Interdisci-
nitive Development: Lessons from Children in a plinary View of the Relationship between the Use
Museum. In Perspectives on Socially Shared Cog- of Space and Built Domestic Environment. In Do-
nition. L. Resnick, J. Levine, and S. Teasley, eds. mestic Architecture and the Use of Space: An In-
Pp. 226–256. Washington, DC: American Psycho- terdisciplinary, Cross-Cultural Study. S. Kent, ed.
logical Association. Pp. 1–8. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gibb, James, and Julia King Moir, Randall


1991 Gender, Activity Areas, and Homelots in the 17th 1987 Farmstead Proxemics and Intrasite Patterning. In
Century Chesapeake Region. Historical Archaeol- Historic Buildings, Material Culture, and People on
ogy 25(4):109–131. the Prairie Margin. D. Jurney and R. Moir, eds. Pp.
229–237. Dallas: Archaeology Research Program
Hammond, G., and N. Hammond Institute for the Study of Earth and Man at Southern
1981 Child’s Play: A Distorting Factor in Archaeological Methodist University.
Distribution. American Antiquity 46:634–636.
Moore, Robin
Harrington, Faith 1986 Childhood’s Domain: Play and Place in Child De-
1989 The Archaeological Use of Landscape Treatment in velopment. Dover, NH: Croom Helm.
88 Jane Eva Baxter

Rapoport, Amos South, Stanley


1990 Systems of Activities and Systems of Settings. In 1977 Method and Theory in Historical Archaeology.
Domestic Architecture and the Use of Space: An New York: Academic Press.
Interdisciplinary, Cross-Cultural Study. S. Kent,
ed. Pp. 9–20. Cambridge: Cambridge University Spencer, Christopher, Mark Blades, and Kim Morsley
Press. 1989 The Child in the Physical Environment. New York:
John Wiley and Sons.
Rotenizer, David
1993 In the Yard: An Examination of Spatial Organiza- Stevenson, Marc
tion and Subdivision of Activity Areas on Rural 1990 Beyond the Formation of Hearth-Associated Arti-
Farmsteads of the Upland South. In Proceedings of fact Assemblages. In The Interpretation of Archae-
the Tenth Symposium on Ohio Valley Urban and ological Spatial Patterning. E. Kroll and T. Price,
Historic Archaeology. Tennessee Anthropological eds. Pp. 101–119. New York: Plenum.
Association, Miscellaneous Paper 16. A. Young and
C. Faulkner, eds. Pp. 1–21. Knoxville: Tennessee Stewart-Abernathy, L.
Anthropological Association. 1986 Urban Farmsteads: Household Responsibilities in
the City. Historical Archaeology 20(2):5–15.
Rotman, Deborah, and Michael Nassaney
Stine, Linda F.
1997 Class, Gender, and the Built Environment: De-
1990 Social Inequality and Turn-of-the-Century Farm-
riving Social Relations from Cultural Landscapes
steads; Issues of Class, Status, Ethnicity, and Race.
in Southwest Michigan. Historical Archaeology
Historical Archaeology 24(4):37–49.
31(3):39–51.
Sutton-Smith, Brian
Schiffer, Michael 1994 Does Play Prepare for the Future? In Toys, Play
1976 Behavioral Archaeology. New York: Academic
and Child Development. J. H. Goldstein, ed. Pp.
Press. 136–146. Berkeley: University of California Press.
1987 Formation Processes of the Archaeological
Record. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico van Andel, Joost
Press. 1985 Effects on Children’s Behavior of Physical Changes
in a Leiden Neighborhood. Children’s Environ-
Sobel, David ments Quarterly 1(4):46–54.
1990 A Place in the World: Adults’ Memories of Child-
hood’s Special Places. Children’s Environments Von Bruck, Gabriel
Quarterly 7(4):5–12. 1997 A House Turned Inside Out: Inhabiting Space in a
1993 Children’s Special Places: Exploring the Role of Yemeni City. Journal of Material Culture 2(2):139–
Forts, Dens, and Bush Houses in Middle Child- 172.
hood. Tucson: Zephyr.
Watson, Patty Jo
Sofaer Derevenski, Joanna 1979 Archaeological Ethnography in Western Iran.
1994 Where Are the Children? Accessing Children in Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology, No. 57.
the Past. Archaeological Review from Cambridge Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
13(2):7–20.
1997 Engendering Children: Engendering Archaeology. Wilk, Richard, and Michael Schiffer
In Invisible People and Processes: Writing Gen- 1979 The Archaeology of Vacant Lots in Tucson,
der and Childhood into European Archaeology. Arizona. American Antiquity 44:530–536.
Jenny Moore and Eleanor Scott, eds. Pp. 192–202.
London: Leicester University Press. Wilkie, Laurie
2000 Not Merely Child’s Play: Creating a Historical Ar-
Sofaer Derevenski, Joanna, ed. chaeology of Children and Childhood. In Children
2000 Children and Material Culture. New York: and Material Culture. J. Sofaer Derevenski, ed. Pp.
Routledge. 100–114. New York: Routledge.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi