Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 4

908

pain and right loin pain both predict renal colic, but pain in
both loins is likelier to arise from urinary tract infection.
Results from the French collaborative study of this Diagnosis
approach showed a modest rise.9
Further modifications to bayesian systems might raise
VISUOSPATIAL NEGLECT: UNDERLYING
their accuracy towards that of clinicians, but are unlikely to
FACTORS AND TEST SENSITIVITY
do more. Such systems seem merely to rehash the clinicians
own opinion, losing accuracy in the process. Computers will
P. W. HALLIGAN J. C. MARSHALL
not become diagnosticians in their own right until they
D. T. WADE
acquire the deep knowledge of anatomy, physiology, and
pathology that underpins the simplest surgical decision. Rivermead Rehabilitation Centre and Neuropsychology Unit,
This implies using techniques of artificial intelligence,
University Department of Clinical Neurology, Radcliffe Infirmary,
though these have been unpromising to date. Oxford
The present trial was potentially biased towards the
retrospective mode, as database cases were included in that Summary Visuospatial neglect, a frequent
accuracy series; but in fact computer accuracy was very consequence of unilateral (usually right-
similar in the two modes. Future programs can and should hemisphere) stroke, is associated with poor functional
therefore be tested on existing patient archives before "going recovery and in many patients is resistant to remedial
live". Retrospective trials can predict gains in prospective treatment. Studies of the nature and prevalence of the
accuracy through better mathematical methods, new disease disorder have been hindered by problems of definition and
categories, and new database probabilities. They cannot assessment. In this study 80 unselected stroke patients were
predict gains through greater user-friendliness or new assessed for the presence and severity of neglect on the
symptoms and signs not previously recorded. behavioural inattention test. The six subtests of this battery
The present study was not designed to assess the relative all intercorrelated highly, and a subsequent factor analysis
contribution of structured forms, computer diagnosis, and showed that all tests loaded significantly on one underlying
audit feedback to surgical decision making. However, it does factor. The construct of neglect as defined by performance
strongly favour the null hypothesis that the computer on the battery is therefore robust. Nonetheless, the
diagnosis contributes precisely nothing. Only bayesian individual tests differed substantially in their sensitivity.
methods have been studied, and only the acute abdomen. Star cancellation was the most sensitive measure of neglect
Yet this is the area for which the greatest claims have been and correctly diagnosed all patients whose aggregate score
made, and in which studies are easiest to conduct. Its failure on the full battery fell below that of the control population.
here must seriously undermine its credibility in other areas.
Might exposure to computer diagnosis have increased the
clinicians accuracy? A large effect is unlikely; moreover, INTRODUCTION
early trials demonstrated that structured records and audit
feedback can be implemented without computers on site.5 THE term visuospatial neglect is used to describe the
behaviour of neurological patients who, after brain damage,
Though the results are negative, the implications are
considerable. Computer-aided diagnosis is not yet suitable appear to be unaware of visual stimuli situated on the side
for clinical practice. "Live" prospective trials should be opposite the lesion. Although described by Zingerle in
conducted only with systems shown in retrospective 1913,1 the disorder has remained obscure within neurology
matched trials to do significantly better than clinicians. But and until lately has had little influence on
greatest research effort, and any government funding, neuropsychological thought.2,3 Clinically, neglect has been
should now be devoted to the low-cost innovations of singled out as a negative prognostic variable that has
structured forms and audit feedback. profound effects on functional rehabilitation after stroke.4,5
Accurate information on the prevalence and nature of this
common neuropsychological disorder is necessary in
I thank the members of the Lothian CADA group for their support during
these studies, in particular Mr A. A. Gunn (consultant at Bangour and
planning rehabilitation facilities. However, progress
towards the effective treatment of neglect has been
Chairman) and Mr I. M. C Macintyre and Dr K. Little (consultants at Leith
and RIE). hampered by problems of definition and assessment.6
Despite the expansion of work on the clinical
manifestations and underlying mechanisms of visual
REFERENCES
neglectthere remains disagreement about the prevalence
1. de Dombal FT, Leaper DJ, Staniland JR, et al. Computer aided diagnosis of acute
abdominal pain. Br Med J 1972; ii 9-13.
of the disorder. Over the past decade, reported frequencies
2. de Dombal FT, Leaper DJ, Horrocks JC, et al. Human and computer aided diagnosis have ranged from less than 2 % to 88 % in patients with right
of abdominal pain: further report with emphasis on performance of clinicians. unilateral stroke.8,9 These large discrepancies have been
Br Med J 1974; i: 376-80.
3. Sutton GC. Computer aided diagnosis: a review. Br J Surg 1989; 76: 82-85. attributed partly to variation in the number and type of tests
4. Horrocks JC, McCann AP, Staniland JR, et al. Computer aided diagnosis: description used to assess visual neglect.9,10 Many different
of an adaptable system and operational experience with 2034 cases. Br Med J 1972;
ii: 5-9. cancellation,l1 copying,12 and visual search tasks13 are
5. Gunn AA. The diagnosis of acute abdominal pain with computer analysis. J R Coll presently used in clinical practice.
Surg Edin 1976; 21: 170-72., The lack of consensus about the assessment of neglect has
6. de Dombal FT. The OMGE acute abdominal pain survey. Progress report 1982.
Scand J Gastroenterol 1984; 19 (suppl 95): 28-40 made it difficult to compare different studies of the
7. Graham DF, Wyllie FJ. Prediction of gallstone pancreatitis by computer. Br Med J
disorder.9 There has been a largely unquestioned
1979; i: 515-17.
8. Adams ID, Chan M, Clifford PC, et al. Computer aided diagnosis of acute abdominal assumption that the various assessment procedures used all
pain: a multicentre study. Br Med J 1986; 293: 800-04.
9. Seroussi B and ARC and AURC Cooperative Group. Computer aided diagnosis of
provide measures of the same underlying deficit.1O,14 Few
acute abdominal pain when taking into account interactions. Meth Inf Med 1986; studies, however, have specified the degree to which the
25: 194-98. various conventional tests of neglect correlate. 15 Many are
909

unstandardised and may well differ in their sensitivity to TABLE III-VARIMAX ROTATED FACTOR MATRIX

detect visual neglect.16,17


We have examined the performance of an unselected
group of stroke patients on six traditional neglect tests.18
Each of these tests (or variants) has been used previously to
measure unilateral "visual neglect"-a conceptual construct
which refers to the deficits observed in brain-damaged
patients who fail to notice, orient, or otherwise respond to
stimuli located on one side of space. A consistent pattern of
impairment across these tests would provide evidence
supporting the underlying construct and the implicit TABLE IV-TEST SENSITIVITY
assumption that such tests are measuring aspects of the same
underlying deficit.
PATIENTS AND METHODS

80 patients with a diagnosis of unilateral stroke were recruited


from admissions to Rivermead Rehabilitation Centre, Oxford.
Patients (54 with right and 26 with left brain damage) were
diagnosed according to physical signs and history; all were right
handed and capable of understanding the task requirements. The *Defined by aggregate score from all six tests < 130/146; n = 30.
mean time since the onset of stroke was 85-5 (SD 740; range

12-366) days. On admission each patient underwent a complete


neurological examination. The presence of neglect is defined as the omission of any major
A comprehensive visual neglect battery of six pencil and paper
tests from the behavioural inattention test (BIT)1$was administered part of a lateralised subcomponent of the figures; the maximum
score is 4.
to all patients. Eye and head movements were not restricted during
In the line bisection test patients are presented with three
testing, but moving the stimulus paper was not permitted; test horizontal (204 mm) black lines in a stepwise arrangement across
material was centred on the patients midsagittal plane.
The first test, line crossing, adapted from Albert," consists of 40 the page. The test is scored by measuring the extent to which the
short lines randomly positioned over the page. The patient is asked patients bisection mark deviates from true centre on each line; the
to cross out all the lines; the maximum score is 36 (the four central
maximum score is 9.
lines are not scored). For the representational drawing test the patient draws, from
In the letter cancellation test 170 upper-case letters arranged in memory, a clock-face, a man or woman, and a butterfly. Scoring is
five rows are presented. 40 letter targets comprising 40% of the total the same as for figure copying, with a maximum of 3.
stimuli are positioned so that approximately equal numbers appear The diagnosis of visual neglect was calculated from normative
on either side of the midplane. The patient is instructed to cross out
data on 50 age-matched control subjects. 1.8 The presence of neglect
on each of the six tests was defined as the score 1 point or more
all the target stimuli; the maximum score is 40.
The stimuli in the star cancellation test are 52 large stars, 13 below the lowest score achieved by any normal control subject.
letters, 10 short words, interspersed with 56 smaller stars. The Aggregate performance on the six tests was used to assess the
patient is instructed to cross out all the small stars. Two of the presence and extent of visual neglect; the cutoff for normality was
central small stars are used for demonstration, and the maximum 130/146. Patients characteristics are given in table 1.
score is thus 54.
The method of analysis used was principal components analysis.
For figure and shape copying the patient copies three outline This process involved the examination of a correlation matrix to
determine which variables co-vary and the subsequent combination
drawings of a four-pointed star, a cube, a daisy, and a group of three of these variables into meaningful factors or traits. In this way,
simple geometric shapes presented on a separate stimulus sheet. evidence for a hierarchical factor structure can be used to support
the case for a robust measure of the constructs being tested. Pearson
TABLE I-DISTRIBUTION OF NEGLECT AMONG 80 STROKE PATIENTS
I I product moment correlations were calculated for all six test
measures. The resulting correlation matrix, and correlations with

age and days since onset of stroke, are given in table u.

RESULTS

Because of the large number of comparisons, the


significance level was set at 0 001. All correlations between
tests were positive and significant; performance on all the
neglect tests was statistically independent of age and time
I I since onset (although the values were, as expected, all

TABLE II&mdash;CORRELATION MATRIX


negative). By means of the BMDP computer package, a
principal component factor analysis was carried out on the
data. The analysis extracts the orthogonal linear
combinations that account for the largest proportion of the
total variance. The rotated factor matrix showed a single
factor that accounted for 72 6% of the total variance
explained (table m). No other factor accounted for any
significant proportion of variance. This single factor had
substantial loadings from all six neglect tests. The results of
the principal components analysis thus provide evidence for
the homogeneity of these visual neglect tests. In other
words, patient performance on the set of six visuospatial
910

The subject is further complicated by the fact that some


behavioural neurologists advocate the use of visual double
simultaneous stimulation (in the absence of primary sensory
loss) as a clinically useful method for diagnosing visual
neglect.23,24 Yet the relation between the elicited response of
extinction and the more flagrant symptoms of visual neglect
is unclear, so the equivalence of these measures remains
open to question.2s In extinction, failure to detect a stimulus
depends on the simultaneous presentation of a second
competing stimulus, whereas visual neglect occurs with a
full range of head and eye movements and does not require
competing stimuli or fixation of gaze. Unlike florid visual
neglect, extinction is not readily apparent in spontaneous
behaviour.26 It is associated with damage of both left and
Star cancellation test (BIT).
right hemispheres, whereas neglect of the left side of space
Dimensions of original are 298 x 208 mm. Reproduced by permission of after right brain damage is very much more common than
Thames Valley Test Company.
the opposite. Reports of double dissociations suggest
caution in equating the two disorders.14,27
Since the manifestation of neglect is variable, many
tasks clearly shows that all the tests are measuring aspects of
the same construct, clinically described as visual neglect. investigators have thought that more than one test should be
Nonetheless, there were some discrepancies in the used; several tests are more likely to uncover evidence of
homogeneity of the tests (table m). Representational neglect than a single test. Our study shows that, if only one
test is used, star cancellation from the BIT is an extremely
drawing loaded only 0-75, whereas figure copying had the sensitive measure of neglect. It lends itself to very detailed
highest loading of 0-92.
The reason for including all 80 patients in the factor experimental analysis28 and is well-adapted to rapid,
accurate diagnosis. Nonetheless, 1 of the patients within our
analysis is that visual neglect is not an all or none
phenomenon.l9 Although some of the stroke patients scored large sample had a perfect performance on star cancellation
as well as the controls, the scores of the majority, though (54/54), but a score of only 30/40 on letter cancellation; the
worst control scored 33/40 on that test. A full examination of
above the designated cutoff, showed omissions on some
tests. For the purpose of examining the relative sensitivities any patient with suspected visual neglect must accordingly
use a wide range of different tests.
of the six tests, only the 30 patients who were classified as
showing neglect by the operational definition of an aggregate This work was supported by grants from the Chest, Heart, and Stroke
score less than 130/146 were included. Association, and the Medical Research Council.
Using the respective cutoff score for each individual test Correspondence should be addressed to P. W. H., Rivermead
we compared detection performance on each test with that Rehabilitation Centre, Abingdon Road, Oxford OX1 4XD.
of the diagnosis achieved with the six-test aggregate cutoff
score. There was substantial variation in sensitivity (table REFERENCES

iv). Star cancellation (see accompanying figure) was the 1. Zingerle H. Ueber Storrungen der Wahrnemung de eigenen Koerpers be: organischen
most sensitive test, eliciting evidence of visual neglect in all Gehirnerkrankungen. Monatsch Psychiatrie Neuro 1913; 34: 13-36.
2. De Renzi E. Disorders of space exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley, 1982.
cases diagnosed on the basis of aggregate score. Drawing 3. Bisiach E, Berti A. Dyschiria: an attempt at its systematic explanation. In. Jeannerod
from memory (representational drawing test), although M, ed. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect
Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1987: 183-201.
popular as a clinical bedside test, failed to identify a 4. Adams GF, Hurwitz LJ. Mental barriers to recovery from stroke. Lancet 1963; ii
substantial majority (63%) of neglect patients in our 533-37.
5. Wade DT, Langton Hewer R, Skilbeck CE, David RM. Stroke: a critical approach to
(non-acute) sample. In groups including acute patients, diagnosis, treatment and management. London: Chapman and Hall, 1985.
drawing from memory may be a more sensitive measures 6. Caplan B. Stimulus effects in unilateral neglect? Cortex 1985; 21: 69-80.
7. Jeannerod M, ed. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial
Likewise, line crossing (Alberts test) and line bisection were neglect. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1987.
insensitive tests that correctly diagnosed little more than half 8. Prescott RJ, Garraway WM, Akhtar AJ. Predicting functional outcome following
of the patients. acute stroke using a standard clinical examination. Stroke 1982; 13: 641-47.
9. Fullerton KJ, McSherry D, Stout RW. Alberts test: a neglected test of perceptual
neglect. Lancet 1986; i: 430-32.
DISCUSSION 10. Ogden JA. The neglected left hemisphere and its contribution to visuospatial neglect.
In: Jeannerod M, ed. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial
This analysis suggests that visuospatial neglect is, to a neglect. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1987: 215-33.
11. Albert M. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 1973; 23: 658-64.
large extent, a single phenomenon, and that different tests of 12. Oxbury JM, Campbell DC, Oxbury SM. Unilateral spatial neglect and impairment of
it have differing sensitivities. In the absence of any widely spatial analysis and visual perception. Brain 1974; 97: 551-64.
13. Gainotti G, DErme P, Monteleone D, Silveri MC. Mechanisms of unilateral neglect
accepted operational definition, reports of the frequency of in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. Brain 1986; 109: 599-612
visual neglect must be considered within the context of the 14. Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of spatial neglect in humans. In: Jeannerod M, ed.
particular criteria and tests used. The most important factor Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. Amsterdam:
Elsevier, 1987: 235-58.
that has contributed to the variation reported is the range of 15. Sunderland A. Cognitive aspects of visual neglect. Doctoral thesis, Brunel University,
different tests used to assess neglect.9,14,21 For example, Hier 1984.
16. Colombo A, De Renzi E, Faglioni O. The occurrence of visual neglect in patients with
and colleagues21 reported a frequency of 85 % in 41 patients unilateral cerebral disease. Cortex 1976; 12: 221-31.
with right-hemisphere brain damage seen within 7 days of 17. Sunderland A, Wade DT, Langton Hewer R. The natural history of visual neglect
stroke based on omissions in copying the complex Rey after stroke. Int Disability Studies 1987; 9: 60-65.
18. Wilson B, Cockbum J, Halligan PW Behavioural inattention test. Titchfield, Hants
figure. Vallar and Perani,22 studying 110 patients with Thames Valley Test Company, 1987
right-hemisphere damage of equivalent aetiology, found a 19. Weinstein EA, Friedland RP, eds. Hemi-inattention and hemispheric specialisation
New York: Raven Press, 1977.
prevalence of 43% based on a simple cancellation task in
patients seen within a mean of 6 7 days since onset.
911

and married, the solitary and the gregarious, those in


Point of View humble or abject conditions, and those in apparently
buoyant or agreeable ones. "Is there a single person to whom
you can openly confide?" If the armour of stoicism and pride
THE RELIEF OF COMMUNICATION be gently removed, comes the quiet and sorrowful reply
"No". Let there be no confusion. This is not a medical
WILLIAM G. PICKERING problem but a human one with a human solution. The
requirements for a confidant are availability and empathy.
7 Moor Place, Gosforth, Newcastle upon Tyne NE3 4AL When depth answers resonatingly unto depth, the relief is
sublime and restorative. The quality is instinctive rather
WHY aregeneral practitioners attending to the than teachable and, in my judgment, as likely to be found in a
populations personal problems and anxieties in ever
hairdresser or shopkeeper as in a doctor.
increasing numbers,l when a stringent financial climate The new tendency to employ "counsellors" in general
suggests that their resources would be better channelled to
medical need? There is not a shred of evidence that doctors practice suggests that doctors are either experiencing
are more able at handling non-medical problems than self-doubt about their skills in dealing with non-medical
need or are being asked to do more than they can cope with.
laymen. Part of the reason lies in the escape of "hands-on" Even for the doctor with special empathy a ten-minute
medicine to hospitals, the resolve of GPs to retreat
consultation is scant time for heart-felt deliverances to be
therefrom, and the numerical growth of GPs. Thus
disburdened, GPs are available to tackle a wider range of decently handled. One thing that distinguishes him or her
human need. At the same time, traditional fountains of from non-medical confidants is the possession of blunt and
succour have decayed or been switched off. Religion is out of expensive instruments to terminate the consultation. Even
those doctors best equipped to soothe turbulent souls may,
vogue; families, wisdom, and generations are wont to
scatter; there exists a cult of the individual, not of the for lack of time and energy, reach for the pen. The resultant
community. These are ideal ambient conditions for the prescriptions, investigations, and referrals carry the tacit
sickness born of isolation to flourish nicely; and, with the message "I see, I feel, I care, but I cant help: please go".
current fashion of seeking counsel at the surgery, the Normal people with normal worries that have no outlet are
National Health Service is fast becoming an integral part of thus transmuted, at some expense, into patients.
the populations modus vivendi rather than a service The essential difference between non-medical problems
primarily reserved for those requiring a medical opinion. and medical problems is that thorough ventilation heals the
The old idea of medical omnipotence, not always former but not the latter; and medical intervention is
discouraged by the profession, is only part of the story. The indicated in the latter and contraindicated in the former. We
public are ducking the needs of their fellows. Rather than general practitioners should consistently rebut rather than
receive a proposed confidence from friend or associate, how reinforce the notion that non-medical need is part of our
often people interpose hastily, "Oughtnt you to see domain. The sage who declared that a trouble shared is a
someone?"-a thin euphemism for "Get thee to a doctor". trouble halved had ordinary people in mind as sharers. If
Do such persons deem it unseemly to listen? Are they disused, the thousand natural healing influences of
indifferent? Or do they truly believe that doctors alone hold instinctive, unwitting, and everyday counsellors will remain
the sovereign remedies? We cannot exactly know; but it is wastefully inactive, eventually to disappear.
simple to divine that they are of no earthly use to a friend ill
with the need to unburden the heart.
So, propelled by a desperate urge to communicate, they Round the World
try the National Health Service. A common ploy is to
camouflage the complaint with the decorations of sickness.
Another is the frequent involuntary elevation of a minor From our Correspondents
medical trouble to one of extravagant urgency and
New Zealand
importance which, had it occurred in a composed
individual, might scarcely have been noticed. PATIENT ADVOCATE AT AUCKLANDS NATIONAL
How often are the simplest things in life the most essential WOMENS HOSPITAL
and the most easily overlooked. Without communication THE first patient advocate appointed under the
how "imprisoned and tortured"2 may the heart become. Yet recommendations of the Report of the Cervical Cancer Inquiryl
confidants do not abound and communication starvation is began work at Aucklands National Womens Hospital in mid-
indiscriminate: it is found in the young and old, the single September. Judge Dame Silvia Cartwright, who led the inquiry,

1. Morbidity statistics from general practice; third national study. London: HM 1. Committee of Inquiry into Allegations Concerning the Treatment of Cervical Cancer
Stationery Office, 1986. at National Womens Hospital and into Other Related Matters. Report of the
2. Thackeray WM. Vanity fair. London: Bradbury and Evans, 1848. cervical cancer inquiry. Auckland, 1988.

20. Schenkenberg T, Bradford DC, Ajax ET. Line bisection and visual neglect in patients 24. Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E. Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman
with neurological impairment. Neurology 1980; 30: 509-17. KM, Valenstein E, eds. Clinical neuropsychology. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford
21. Hier DB, Mondlock J, Caplan LR. Behavioural abnormalities after right hemisphere University Press, 1985: 243-93.
stroke. Neurology 1983; 33: 337-34. 25. Schwartz AS, Marchok PL, Kreinick GJ, Flynn RE. The asymmetric lateralization of
22 Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after tactile extinction in patients with unilateral cerebral dysfunction Brain 1979; 102:
right hemisphere stroke 669-84.
lesions: a clinical/CT scan correlation study m man. Neuropsychologia 1986; 24:
26. Critchley M. The parietal lobes. New York: Haffner, 1953.
609-22.
27. Barbieri C, De Renzi E. Patterns of neglect dissociation. Behav Neurol (in press).
23. Mesulam M-M Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: FA Davis 28. Marshall JC, Halligan PW. Does the midsagittal plane play any privileged role in left
Company, 1985.
neglect? Cogn Neuropsychol 1989; 6: 403-22.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi