Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271080807

Give Me Your Self: Gifts are Liked More When


They Match the Givers Characteristics

Article in Journal of Consumer Psychology January 2015


DOI: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006

CITATIONS READS

4 109

3 authors:

Gabriele Paolacci Laura Straeter


Rotterdam School of Management Erasmus University Rotterdam
18 PUBLICATIONS 1,868 CITATIONS 1 PUBLICATION 4 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Ilona de Hooge
Wageningen University & Research
30 PUBLICATIONS 479 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Ugly foods and food waste in supply chains and consumer households. View project

Creating the perfect gift experience in online retailing View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ilona de Hooge on 03 February 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
JCPS-00463; No. of pages: 8; 4C:

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxx xxx

Research Report
Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match
the giver's characteristics
Gabriele Paolacci a,, Laura M. Straeter a , Ilona E. de Hooge b
a
Department of Marketing Management, Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
b
Department of Marketing and Consumer Behaviour, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands

Received 30 January 2014; received in revised form 18 November 2014; accepted 10 January 2015

Abstract

Research on gift giving has devoted considerable attention to understanding whether and how givers succeed in choosing gifts that match
recipients' tastes. On the contrary, this article focuses on how recipients' appreciation for a gift depends on the match between the gift and the
giver. Four studies demonstrate that recipients are particularly appreciative when they receive gifts that guratively match the giver, i.e., that
contain references to the giver's characteristics, because they perceive such gifts as more congruent with the giver's identity. This effect is not
conditional on inferences recipients might make about the giver's motivations or on whether recipients have a good relationship with the giver, but
relies on the match concerning core rather than peripheral characteristics of the giver. Importantly for our understanding of identity-based
motivation, these ndings demonstrate in a gift-giving context that identity-congruence not only drives consumer behavior, but is also appreciated
in other people.
2015 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Gift giving; Gift evaluation; Identity; Identity-congruence; Identity-based motivation

On occasions such as celebrations and anniversaries, we often characteristics of the giver. Four studies show that gifts that
find ourselves wondering what gifts we could buy to make our merely contain aesthetic references to the giver's core character-
friends and relatives happy. Past research has identified many istics (e.g., the giver's passions) are appreciated more by recipients,
characteristics that affect recipients' satisfaction with the gifts because they are perceived as more congruent with the giver's
they receive. Typically, gift-giving researchers have investigated identity. These findings have theoretical and practical implications
how gift appreciation depends on the fit between the gift and the for identity-based motivation and gift giving, and suggest new
recipient's tastes and desires (Gino & Flynn, 2011; Ward & research questions about how identity-congruence affects con-
Broniarczyk, 2011) and on the relationship between the giver sumer behavior.
and the recipient (Belk, 1976; Ruth, Otnes, & Brunel, 1999).
In contrast, the current paper focuses on how the relationship
Theoretical background
between the giver and the gift affects recipients' appreciation
for the gifts they receive. We propose that recipients like a gift
We propose that recipients particularly appreciate gifts that
differently depending on whether or not it matches the giver, i.e.,
match the giver, i.e., that contain references to the giver's
on whether or not the gift contains references to the core
characteristics, because recipients perceive such gifts as congru-
Corresponding author at: Department of Marketing Management, Rotterdam
ent with the giver's identity. Theoretically, this proposition builds
School of Management, Erasmus University, Burgemeester Oudlaan 50, 3062 PA on people's inherent preference for identity-congruence when
Rotterdam, The Netherlands. Fax: +31 10 408 90 11. they consider other people's expressions, and on the role of gifts
E-mail address: gpaolacci@rsm.nl (G. Paolacci). as expressions of the giver's identity.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
1057-7408/ 2015 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
2 G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx

Identity has been demonstrated to be a strong predictor of Gifts are no exception to this identity-based motivation
people's behavior. Both the personal and the social characteristics framework. Research on gift giving has long highlighted the
of people's identity (e.g., traits, group memberships) can determine role of gifts as a tool that allows givers to express their
people's goals and actions (Oyserman, 2009). Recently, the identities, and that self-presentation might be one of the most
identity-based motivation model articulated why and how people prominent symbolic functions of gift giving (Schwartz, 1967;
are motivated to act in ways that are congruent with their identity Wolfinbarger, 1990). In fact, the giver's self concept may often
(for reviews see Oyserman, 2007, 2009). Making choices that are be more predictive of the characteristics of a gift than the
consistent with one's identity feels right, both when the identity giver's concept of the receiver (Belk, 1979). In this work, we
is chronically salient and when it is temporarily cued. In the explore whether appreciating congruence in the expressions of
marketplace, individuals express their identity by preferring other people's identities translates into increased appreciation
products that convey who they are and the communities they among gift recipients for gifts that figuratively match the giver,
belong to (Belk, 1988; Berger & Heath, 2007; Escalas & Bettman, i.e., that contain references to core characteristics of the giver.
2003, 2005). Practically, consumers seek identity-congruence Note that the fact that the giver matters in how recipients
when making choices that are both central in their life (e.g., when evaluate a gift is not new to the gift giving literature; however,
they consider career opportunities; Cheryan, Plaut, Davies, & whereas previous research has focused on the relationship
Steele, 2009) and trivial (e.g., detergents; Forehand & Deshpande, between the giver and the recipient (Belk, 1976; Ruth et al.,
2001). 1999), we hypothesize that gift appreciation is also affected by
Especially because people expect identities to be stable over the relationship between the giver and the gift.
time, they not only use their own identities to guide their Consider the examples of Olivia and Jack, who are passionate
behaviors and decisions; they also evaluate other people's about Italian culture and horses respectively. We hypothesize that
behaviors and purchase decisions in relation to the actors' Olivia's and Jack's friends will particularly appreciate gifts that
identities (Belk, Mayer, & Bahn, 1982; Berger & Heath, 2007; contain references to Italy and horses (e.g., a mug or a mouse pad
Calder & Burnkrant, 1977). In particular, people seem to with Italy- and horses-related pictures), because such gifts are
appreciate consistency not only in their own attitudes and perceived as more congruent with Olivia and Jack. Of course, not
behaviors, but also in the attitudes and behavior of other people any reference to the giver will be perceived as congruent with the
(Cialdini, Trost, & Newsom, 1995; Fiske & Taylor, 1991; giver's identity. In particular, a non-core descriptor of the giver
Guadagno & Cialdini, 2010). Being inconsistent is perceived that is matched by the gift should not result in a gift that is
as a sign of being inauthentic and not true to one's self, perceived as particularly congruent with the giver, and therefore
traits that people consider to be inherently negative (Sheldon, should not result in increased appreciation. Using the examples
Ryan, Rawsthorne, & Ilardi, 1997). In terms of identity-based above, if Italy and horses were only incidentally related to Olivia
motivation, this stream of research suggests that consumers and Jack rather than being core descriptors of their identities (e.g.,
may value identity-congruent expressions both when they if Italy was the country of origin of a liked artist or a horse was
pertain to their own actions and when they pertain to others' portrayed on the cover of a liked book), Italy- and horses-related
actions. gifts should not result in increased appreciation.

Table 1
Means (and standard deviations) per condition for all studies.
Study 1 Match No match
Gift appreciation 5.83 (1.10) 5.27 (0.97)
Identity congruence 5.74 (1.04) 4.47 (1.52)

Study 2 Urban Landscape Control


Preference for landscape print 5.02 (2.91) 6.14 (2.79) 5.76 (2.87)
Landscape print relative congruence 1.72 (2.11) 1.27 (1.78) 0.03 (1.15)

Study 3 Good friend Acquaintance Disliked person


Match No match Match No match Match No match
Gift appreciation 5.26 (1.55) 4.53 (1.54) 4.79 (1.16) 4.19 (1.36) 4.11 (1.29) 3.00 (1.44)
Identity congruence 5.66 (1.19) 4.10 (1.54) 5.28 (1.20) 4.08 (1.57) 5.20 (1.14) 3.47 (2.14)

Study 4 Core match Non-core match No match


Gift appreciation 5.64 (1.08) 5.13 (1.34) 5.03 (1.45)
Identity congruence 6.16 (0.97) 5.45 (1.38) 5.11 (1.14)

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx 3

Study overview London (see Methodological Detail Appendix for full scenarios
of all studies).
We report four studies that tested whether gift recipients are After the scenario, participants answered five questions
particularly appreciative of gifts that figuratively match the about how much they appreciated the gift (adapted from Flynn
giver, and that together exclude explanations for this effect & Adams, 2009; Ward & Broniarczyk, 2011): How much
other than the perceived congruence between the gift and would you appreciate the gift?, How grateful would you feel
the giver's identity. Importantly, gifts in our studies are not for the gift?, How thankful would you feel for the gift?,
functionally related to the giver's characteristics (e.g., a mug How pleased would you feel about receiving the gift?, and
received by an amateur or professional craftsman) but merely How much do you like the gift? Participants answered on a
contain or not aesthetic references to the giver (e.g., a mug with 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very much) and the items were
a picture referencing the giver's country of origin). This rules averaged into one appreciation index ( = .89). Additionally,
out the alternative explanation that recipients are particularly we measured the perceived congruence of the gift with Robin's
appreciative of giver-matched gifts because they infer higher identity using three items (The gift reflects Robin's interests,
quality from the giver's (presumed) expertise. Study 1 shows The gift is related to Robin's personality, and The gift
that recipients appreciate a gift more when it matches the giver, resembles Robin; 1 = not at all, 7 = very much; = .88).
i.e., when it contains references to the giver's characteristics.
Study 2 decouples the gift from the act of giving and
Results and discussion
demonstrates that the attractiveness of giver-matched gifts
does not depend on recipients' inferences about the giver's
Supporting our hypothesis, gift appreciation positively
motivations. Study 3 reveals that recipients' preferences for
depended on whether the gift figuratively matched the giver
giver-matched gifts do not depend on how they feel towards the
(See Table 1 for the main statistics of all studies). Participants
giver, ruling out the alternative explanation that recipients
appreciated the Louvre mug more when Robin was passionate
appreciate giver-matched gifts more because these gifts will be
about France (M = 5.83, SD = 1.10) than when Robin was
reminders of people they like. Finally, Study 4 shows that
passionate about England (M = 5.27, SD = 0.97, t(71) =2.33,
recipients appreciate giver-matched gifts more only when the
p = .02, mean difference = 0.56, 0.08 b 95% CI b 1.04,
match concerns a core (as opposed to a more peripheral)
Cohen's d = 0.54). Moreover, the gift was perceived as more
descriptor of the giver. Confirming our theoretical account, in
congruent with the giver's identity in the match condition than in
each of our studies we find that the perceived congruence of the
the no match condition (M = 5.74, SD = 1.04 vs. M = 4.47,
gift with the giver's identity mediates the effect of matching on
SD = 1.52, t(71) = 4.151, p b .001, mean difference = 1.27,
gift appreciation.
0.67 b 95% CI b 1.87, Cohen's d = 0.97), and a bootstrapping
analysis with 5000 samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) showed that
Study 1
perceived identity congruence mediated the effect of condition on
recipients' appreciation (b = 0.1970, .0002 b 95% CI b 0.4758).
The goal of Study 1 was to demonstrate that recipients
Although the confidence interval barely excluded 0, the following
appreciate a gift more when the gift matches the giver, i.e., when
studies provide more solid evidence for the role of perceived
the gift contains references to the giver's core characteristics.
identity congruence.
Participants read a scenario about receiving a gift and indicated
These findings demonstrate that recipients like a gift more
how much they appreciated the gift. In order to keep the gift
when the gift figuratively matches the characteristics of the
constant across conditions, in all studies we manipulated the
giver, and suggest that this effect may be driven by recipients
relationship between the giver and the gift by changing the
perceiving the gift to be more congruent with the giver's
description of the giver.
identity. An identity-congruence explanation implies that the
additional attractiveness of giver-matched gifts should not
Method
depend on what recipients infer about the giver's motivations for
giving such a gift. In particular, recipients' increased appreciation
Seventy-three international students from Erasmus University
for giver-matched gifts should exist even when giver-matched
Rotterdam (30.1% females, Mage = 21.27, SDage = 1.97) partic-
gifts do not signal any benevolent motivation (e.g., to make the
ipated in this study in exchange for course credit. In a between
recipient knowledgeable of the giver's characteristics). To test
participants design, participants were randomly assigned to one
this, Study 2 employed a scenario in which givers offered both a
of two different scenarios. Participants read that as a gift for
gift that matched the giver's characteristics and a gift that did not.
graduating, their friend Robin gave them a mug depicting Cupid
and Psyche, a painting that is part of the Louvre museum's
collection in Paris. Critically, the scenario included a description Study 2
of Robin that slightly differed between conditions: Whereas in
the match condition Robin's description included that Robin In this study, participants expressed their preference between
likes rock music and is passionate about France, in particular two gift objects. We expected participants to be more attracted to
Paris, the parallel sentence in the no match condition read likes a gift when it matched the core characteristics of the giver than
rock music and is passionate about England, in particular when it did not.

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
4 G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx

Method other. As predicted, the perceived congruence with the giver's


identity of the landscape print relative to the urban print was higher
Three hundred two US residents recruited on Amazon in the landscape condition (M = 1.27, SD = 1.78) than in the
Mechanical Turk (Paolacci & Chandler, 2014; 39.7% females, control condition (M = 0.03, SD = 1.15, t(299) = 5.10, p b .001,
Mage = 29.65, SDage = 8.84) participated in this study in mean difference = 1.24, 0.83 b 95% CI b 1.65, Cohen's d =
exchange for $0.20. Participants read that their friend Robin had 0.83) and lower in the urban condition (M = 1.72, SD = 2.11)
moved and had left behind several items as gifts for friends, than in the control condition (t(299) = 7.17, p b .001, mean
including a print of the photograph NYTowers and Spotlights difference = 1.75, 1.28 b 95% CI b 2.22, Cohen's d =1.03).
(by Jerry Driendl) and a print of the photograph Sunbeams (Using a repeated measures analysis on the urban and
through the trees (by Ross Hoddinott). The prints were listed in landscape congruency scores instead of combining these two
this order without pictures. In a between-participant design, into a relative congruency index gives qualitatively identical
participants read one of three slightly different descriptions of results.) A bootstrapping analysis with 5000 samples based
Robin. In the urban (landscape) condition, the description on a multicategorical approach (Hayes & Preacher, 2013)
included Robin's passion for photographing urban sites (land- showed that the relative congruency index mediated the effect
scapes), i.e., Robin is passionate about photography, and of condition on recipients' preferences for prints (urban vs.
particularly about photographs of urban sites (landscapes). control: b = 0.5244, 0.9119 b 95% CI b .1929; landscape
Robin spends a lot of time taking photographs in urban areas vs. control: b = 0.3710, 0.1338 b 95% CI b 0.6496).
(of natural landscapes) and editing them. In the control In summary, the findings of Study 2 confirm that recipients
condition there was no reference to photography. After reading prefer gifts that match the giver because they perceive them as
the scenario, participants indicated which of the two prints they more congruent with the giver's identity. Importantly, in this
would pick on a 9-point scale (1 = definitely Driendl's urban study the giver merely left behind some objects as gifts
photograph print, 9 = definitely Hoddinott's landscape pho- while moving to a different country, and offered both a gift
tograph print). Additionally, participants rated the degree to that matched the giver and a gift that did not. This suggests that
which the urban print and the landscape print were congruent the preference for giver-matched gifts is not conditional on
with Robin's identity (same measures as Study 1; = .90 inferences that recipients might make about the givers'
and = .89, respectively). Theoretically, we expected prefer- motivations that may be associated with the act of giving
ence for the print across conditions to be mediated by how such a gift.
congruent a print was with Robin's identity relative to the other Another potential explanation for the attractiveness of giver-
print. Accordingly, we subtracted the urban print congruence matched gifts follows from the function of gifts as reminders of
rating from the landscape print congruence rating, and the giver (Areni, Kiecker, & Palan, 1998; Belk, Wallendorf, &
obtained an index of how congruent with Robin's identity Sherry, 1989). If recipients prefer gifts that will remind them of
was the landscape print relative to the urban print (a positive the giver, then the observed appreciation for giver-matched
congruency index indicating that the landscape print is perceived gifts might depend on their particular strength as memory cues
as relatively more congruent with the giver's identity than the for the giver. Study 3 tested the gifts-as-reminders account of
urban print). our effect by manipulating how recipients felt about the giver.
If recipients appreciate giver-matched gifts because these are
Results and discussion stronger reminders of the giver, then gift appreciation should
increase (relative to non-matched gifts) when the giver is a
Consistent with our proposition, participants were more close and liked friend but not when the giver is a disliked
attracted to the landscape (urban) print when Robin was passionate person. On the contrary, our identity-congruence account
about photographing natural landscapes (urban sites). A one-way predicts that the valence of the relationship between the giver
ANOVA on relative print preference with condition as and the gift has no influence on whether or not recipients are
independent variable (F(2, 299) = 4.00, p = .02) indeed more appreciative of gifts that match the giver. Because
showed that participants' preferences for the landscape print identity-congruence is a characteristic that consumers inherently
relative to the urban print were stronger in the landscape value in their expressions and in those of others, gift recipients
condition than in the urban condition (M = 6.14, SD = 2.79 vs. should appreciate a giver-matched gift more independent of how
M = 5.02, SD = 2.91, t(299) = 2.78, p b .006, mean differ- they feel about the giver.
ence = 1.12, 0.33 b 95% CI b 1.91, Cohen's d = 0.39),
with preferences in the control condition falling in between Study 3
(M = 5.76, SD = 2.87; control vs. urban, t(299) = 1.83, p =
.068, mean difference = 0.74, 0.06 b 95% CI b 1.54, Similar to Study 1, participants in Study 3 read a scenario
Cohen's d = 0.26; landscape vs. control, t(299) = 0.94, p = about receiving a gift that figuratively matched the giver or not,
.348, mean difference = 0.38, 0.40 b 95% CI b 1,16, Cohen's and indicated their appreciation for the gift. We tested whether
d = 0.13). a gifts-as-reminders account can explain the increased appre-
In addition, we looked at whether the effect of condition on ciation for a giver-matched gift by additionally manipulating
gift preference was explained by differences in the perceived whether the giver was a close friend, an acquaintance, or a
congruence with the giver's identity of one print relative to the disliked person.

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx 5

7
Method
6
One hundred eighty-three Dutch adults from the surround-
ings of the authors' institution participated voluntarily in this 5

online study (54.1% females, Mage = 35.96, SDage = 15.48). 4 Match


Participants imagined being promoted to a new job position and No match
receiving a mouse pad with ancient tribal images of the 3
South-African Lemba tribe as a small gift from their friend
2
Robin. Participants were randomly assigned to one condition of
a 2 (match: match vs. no match) 3 (liking for the giver: good 1
friend vs. acquaintance vs. disliked person) between-participant Good friend Acquaintance Disliked person
Note: Error bars represent 1 standard error.
design. Across the six scenarios, we varied whether the gift
matched the giver or not (Robin described as originally
Fig. 1. Gift appreciation as a function of match and liking for the giver. Note:
coming from South Africa and being passionate about Error bars represent 1 standard error.
African music vs. coming from Scotland and being
passionate about Scottish music) and how recipients felt
about the giver (Robin presented as good and close friend of
yours vs. an acquaintance of yours vs. a person that you
really do not like). We measured gift appreciation and SD = 1.44; t(177) = 3.09, p b .01, mean difference = 1.11,
perceived congruence of the gift with the giver's identity 0.42 b 95% CI b 1.80, Cohen's d = 0.82).
using the same items as in Study 1 ( = .94 and = .90 We tested whether perceived identity congruence (that was
respectively). higher in the match conditions than in the no match conditions,
M = 5.40, SD = 1.18 vs. M = 3.87, SD = 1.79, F(1, 177) =
43.57, p b .001, mean difference = 1.53, 1.09 b 95% CI b 1.97,
Results and discussion Cohen's d = 1.00) could explain the effect of match on recipients'
appreciation controlling for liking for the giver (we obtained the
Results supported our hypothesis that people like a gift same findings when we did not control for this variable). An
more when the gift matches the giver, independent of their analysis with 5000 bootstrapped samples (Preacher & Hayes,
feelings towards the giver. A two-way ANOVA on gift 2008) showed that perceived identity congruence mediated
appreciation with match and liking for the giver as independent the effect of match on recipient's appreciation (b = 0.4117,
variables showed a main effect of match (F(1, 177) = 15.09, 0.1603 b 95% CI b 0.7270).
p b .01, mean difference = 0.89, 0.46 b 95% CI b 1.32, Study 3 replicated the result that recipients appreciate a gift
Cohen's d = 0.59), such that the mouse pad was appreciated more when it figuratively matches the giver, and showed that
more when Robin had South African roots (M = 4.76, SD = this effect is not conditional on whether givers are liked. This
1.43) than when Robin had Scottish roots (M = 3.87, SD = demonstrates that the attractiveness of gifts that figuratively
1.57) (see Fig. 1). We also found a main effect of liking for the match the giver is not a byproduct of a preference for gifts
giver (F(2, 177) = 14.40, p b .01), such that gifts received that are better reminders of liked givers. Moreover, Study 3
from a good friend (M = 4.95, SD = 1.57) were appreciated confirmed that the effect of a givergift match is mediated by
more than gifts received from an acquaintance (M = 4.45, the perceived congruence of the gift with the giver's identity.
SD = 1.30, t(180) = 1.89, p = .06, mean difference = 0.50, One question that is important from both a theoretical and a
0.01 b 95% CI b 1.01, Cohen's d = 0.35), and gifts received practical perspective is whether recipients appreciate the match
from an acquaintance were appreciated more than gifts between the gift and any characteristic, experience, or trait of
received from a disliked person (M = 3.51, SD = 1.47; the giver, or whether matches need to concern core character-
t(180) =3.56, p b .01, mean difference = 0.94, 0.45 b 95% istics of the giver that define the identity of the giver in
CI b 1.43, Cohen's d = 0.68). Importantly, we found no the recipient's eyes. Study 4 provides support for the latter
interaction between match and liking for the giver (F b 1), proposition.
indicating that recipients' appreciation for matched gifts did
not depend on their feelings towards the giver. Simple
contrasts showed that matched gifts, relative to non-matched Study 4
gifts, were appreciated significantly more in the good friend
conditions (M = 5.26, SD = 1.55 vs. M = 4.53, SD = 1.54; In Study 4 we manipulated between participants whether the
t(177) = 1.99, p = .047, mean difference = 0.73, 0.05 b 95% gift matched or did not match the country of origin of the giver.
CI b 1.51, Cohen's d = 0.47), marginally more in the acquain- Moreover, we included a third condition in which the gift
tance conditions (M =4.79, SD = 1.16 vs. M = 4.19, SD =1.36; matched a less defining characteristic of the giver. We expected
t(177) = 1.65, p =.10, mean difference = 0.60, 0.03 b 95% that recipients would be more appreciative of a gift that
CI b 1.23, Cohen's d = 0.47), and significantly more in the matched the giver only when the match concerned a core
disliked person conditions (M = 4.11, SD = 1.29; vs. M = 3.00, characteristic of the identity of the giver.

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
6 G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx

Additionally, the no match between the gift and the giver in to be increased relative to when no match is present. Gift
this study was operationalized more clearly as lack of match appreciation was higher in the core match condition than in
rather than mismatch. One could argue that gifts in our previous the non-core match condition (t(297) = 2.76, p = .006, mean
no match conditions might be perceived as conflicting with difference = 0.51, 0.17 b 95% CI b 0.85, Cohen's d = 0.42).
the giver's identity (e.g., a South Africa-related gift perceived as Perceived congruence between the gift and Robin's identity
conflicting with the identity of a person who is passionate about was higher in the core match condition (M = 6.16, SD = 0.97)
Scotland), which might be possibly conducive to reacting than in both the no match condition (M = 5.11, SD = 1.14;
negatively to the gift. In Study 4 we removed potentially t(297) = 6.36, p b .001, mean difference = 1.05, 0.76 b 95%
mismatching aspects from our no match condition, showing more CI b 1.34, Cohen's d = 0.99) and the non-core match condi-
clearly that increased appreciation for a giver-matched gift can tion (M = 5.45, SD = 1.38; t(297) = 4.245, p b .001, mean
result from positive reactions compared to when a match is difference = 0.71, 0.38 b 95% CI b 1.04, Cohen's d = 0.60).
absent. A bootstrapping analysis with 5000 samples based on a
multicategorical approach (Hayes & Preacher, 2013) confirmed
Method that perceived congruence between the gift and Robin's identity
mediated the effect of matching the core characteristics of the
Three hundred US residents recruited on Amazon Mechanical giver on recipients' appreciation (core match vs. non-core match:
Turk (35.0% females, Mage = 29.64, SDage = 9.64) participated b = 0.1949, 0.3671 b 95% CI b .0846; core match vs. no
in this study in exchange for $0.20. Participants read a similar match: b = 0.2890, 0.4802 b 95% CI b 0.1348).
scenario to Study 3 in which they imagined receiving a mouse Study 4 replicated our previous findings using a no match
pad with ancient tribal images of the South-African Lemba tribe. condition that did not include information that could potentially be
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. perceived as conflicting with the giver's identity. This highlights
In the core match condition, Robin was described as originally that the effect derives from an increased appreciation for a gift that
coming from South Africa and being passionate about African matches the giver's characteristics as opposed to a decreased
music. In the non-core match condition, Robin's description appreciation for a gift that mismatches the giver's characteristics.
included Robin is very passionate about soccer and these days is Moreover, we show that in order to be particularly attractive to
very looking forward to the next World Cup (Robin still loves to recipients, gifts need to match a core characteristic of the giver. A
talk about the last World Cup in South Africa). Conceptually, gift that matches a non-core descriptor of the giver is not perceived
the non-core match condition differs from the core match as an expression that is particularly congruent with the giver's
condition because the match between the giver and the gift identity, and therefore does not result in increased appreciation. As
concerns an incidental descriptor of Robin (that the last World we elaborate in the General Discussion, this finding represents a
Cup took place in South Africa) rather than a core characteristic first step towards a more thorough understanding of the nature
of Robin's identity (Robin's origins). Finally, the no match of the match between gifts and givers that is appreciated by
condition did not include any mention of Robin's origins or recipients.
passions, thus avoiding any reference that could be perceived as
(congruent or) incongruent with Robin's identity. We hypothe- General discussion
sized that participants would be more appreciative of the mouse
pad in the core match condition than in the no match condition. Early research on gift giving highlighted the role of gifts as a
On the contrary, we expected no increase in appreciation for the tool that allows givers to express their identities (Belk, 1976;
mouse pad when the match concerned a non-core descriptor of Schwartz, 1967). We explored the consequences of doing so for
Robin relative to the no match condition. Gift appreciation and recipients' appreciation, and thus focused on a neglected driver
identity congruence were measured using the same items as in of recipients' satisfaction with the gifts they receivethe
Study 1 ( = .92 and = .89, respectively). relationship between the giver and the gift. Four studies showed
that gift recipients like a gift more when the gift figuratively
Results matches the giver, i.e., when it contains references to the giver's
characteristics. This tendency is not conditional on whether or
A one-way ANOVA with gift appreciation as dependent not the giver intentionally selected a giver-matched gift for the
variable revealed an effect of condition (F(2, 297) = 6.41, p = recipient (Study 2) or on whether or not the giver is a liked
.002). Consistent with our hypotheses and our previous results, gift person (Study 3), but depends on the gift matching core
appreciation was higher in the core match condition than in the no characteristics as opposed to incidental descriptors of the giver
match condition (M = 5.64, SD = 1.08 vs. M = 5.03, SD = 1.45, (Study 4). In all of our studies, we found mediation evidence
t(297) = 3.35, p = .001, mean difference = 0.61, 0.26 b 95% that giver-matched gifts are appreciated more because they are
CI b 0.96, Cohen's d = 0.48). On the contrary, gift appreciation perceived as more congruent with the giver's identity.
was not higher in the non-core match condition (M = 5.13, SD = We grounded our findings in an identity-based motivation
1.34) than in the no match condition (t(297) = 0.554, p = .580, framework (Oyserman, 2009). Consumers are consistently moti-
mean difference = 0.10, 0.29 b 95% CI b 0.49, Cohen's d = vated to express their identity, or the identity of the people they
0.07), confirming that the match between the gift and the giver would like to become, in their product choices. We have shown
needs to concern core characteristics of the giver for appreciation in a gift-giving context that people's inherent preference for

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx 7

identity-congruence deploys not only when people choose their appreciation (Gino & Flynn, 2011; Ruth et al., 1999), and our
own actions and expressions, but also when they evaluate other research certainly does not imply that givers should neglect the
people's actions and expressions. Future research should delve characteristics of the recipients. The goal of this article was to
more deeply into this possibility and investigate the general isolate the positive effect of matching the gift and the giver. When
circumstances in which consumers expect congruence between tradeoffs exist between matching the gift with the giver or with
other people's identities and expressions, and the consequences the recipient, which strategy is more appropriate might depend on
that observing such congruence (or the lack thereof) may have for characteristics of the giverrecipient relationship. Importantly, in
interpersonal judgments and relationships. Importantly, we tested such situations the cultural qualifications of our proposition may
our hypothesis using identities (e.g., country of origin) that are be even stronger, as in certain cultures adjusting to others is
chronically salient. Additional research should investigate whether normatively prescribed (e.g., East Asian, Markus & Kitayama,
our results generalize to identities of the actor that are typically 1994).
activated only in specific situations (Oyserman, 2009). More Finally, future research should overcome the limitations
generally, it is critical to understand which dimensions of the inherent in this short article. Our methodological choices
actor's identity are expected by the observer to be reflected in the reflected the goal of providing clean evidence of our proposed
actor's expressions, and whether such expectations are matched by effect and mechanism. Whereas scenario studies in which the
the actor's actual choices. giver's description was manipulated allowed us to isolate the
Importantly, preference for identity-congruent gifts may be effects of matching the gift with the giver, real gift exchanges
culturally dependent. Research has demonstrated that East Asians or surveys of previous experiences would speak to the real-
maintain a positive self-concept by seeking interpersonal harmony, world prevalence of our findings. Moreover, using simple gifts
something that is easier to achieve by being flexible with respect to with merely aesthetic manipulations made for a conservative
the environment rather than by being congruent with one's identity test of our effects. However, future research should explore
(Markus & Kitayama, 1994). As a result, East Asian consumers do the attractiveness of matching the giver with gifts of higher
not value consistency in others' self-concepts as much as North monetary or symbolic value, and employ richer characteriza-
Americans do (Heine, 2001; Suh, 2002), suggesting that gifts that tions of the giver's identity.
match the giver may not be particularly appreciated relative to gifts
that do not. Moreover, recent research on gift giving suggested Conclusion
that Asians are more likely to attribute gift giving to self-serving
motives than North Americans (Shen, Wan, & Wyer, 2011). Gifts Research in gift giving has traditionally emphasized the need
that match the characteristics of the giver may exacerbate these for givers to accurately predict recipients' tastes. We have shown
attributions, ultimately translating into lower gift appreciation. that recipients' appreciation for a gift also depends on whether the
Future research should explore the cultural antecedents of gift matches the giver: Givers can increase recipients' happiness
recipients' preferences for gifts that match the giver. by choosing gifts that contain references to their own character-
Notwithstanding the many open questions, our research istics, because such gifts are perceived as more identity-
already has actionable implications for gift giving. Previous congruent. Future research should investigate how givers should
research has documented several instances in which people balance the goals of matching the gift with their own and
overly focus on themselves when attempting to predict others' recipients' identities, whether the preference we documented
reactions (Epley, Keysar, Van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004). for products that are congruent with other people's identity
Egocentric biases might underlie several tendencies among generalize outside gift giving, and whether cultural differences
gift givers that ultimately translate into suboptimal recipient qualify such results.
happiness, such as privileging gift attributes like monetary
value and exclusivity (Flynn & Adams, 2009; Teigen, Olsen, & Appendix A. Supplementary data
Solas, 2005) and stigmatizing regifting (Adams, Flynn, &
Norton, 2012). The present research, however, documents a Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
route for givers to make recipients happy that does not rely on http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006.
correctly predicting recipients' preferences, and suggests that
some egocentrism might in fact be beneficial. By gifting people References
with objects that match their own, core characteristics, givers
can increase recipients' appreciation, due to recipients valuing Adams, G. S., Flynn, F. J., & Norton, M. I. (2012). The gifts we keep on giving:
the congruence of the gift with the giver's identity. This result Documenting and destigmatizing the regifting taboo. Psychological
Science, 23, 11451150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612439718.
holds across different relationships between the giver and the Areni, C. S., Kiecker, P., & Palan, K. M. (1998). Is it better to give than to
recipient (e.g., when they are acquaintances), suggesting that receive? Exploring gender differences in the meaning of memorable gifts.
such strategy might prove particularly valuable when the giver Psychology and Marketing, 15, 81109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-
lacks information about the recipient's tastes. 6793(199801)15:1b81::AID-MAR6N3.0.CO;2-J.
A practical aspect to investigate is the conditions under Belk, R. W. (1976). It's the thought that counts: A signed digraph of gift giving.
Journal of Consumer Research, 3, 155162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/
which givers should privilege matching gifts with their own 208662.
characteristics rather than with the recipients'. Including Belk, R. W. (1979). Gift-giving behavior. In J. N. Sheth (Ed.), Research in
recipients' likes and tastes in gifts has positive effects on gift Marketing, Vol. 2. (pp. 95126). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
8 G. Paolacci et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2015) xxxxxx

Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer and Statistical Psychology, 67(2), 120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.
Research, 15, 139168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209154. 12028.
Belk, R., Mayer, R., & Bahn, K. (1982). The eye of the beholder: Individual Heine, S. J. (2001). Self as cultural product: An examination of East Asian and
differences in perceptions of consumption symbolism. In A. Mitchell (Ed.), North American selves. Journal of Personality, 69, 881906. http://dx.doi.
Advances in Consumer Research (pp. 523530). Ann Arbor, MI: Association org/10.1111/1467-6494.696168.
for Consumer Research. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective:
Belk, R. W., Wallendorf, M., & Sherry, J. F. (1989). The sacred and the profane Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology
in consumer behavior: Theodicy on the odyssey. Journal of Consumer Bulletin, 20, 568579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205013.
Research, 16, 138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209191. Oyserman, D. (2007). Social identity and self-regulation. In A. Kruglanski, &
Berger, J., & Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others: Identity T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 432453) (Second
signaling and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 121134. Edition ). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/519142. Oyserman, D. (2009). Identity-based motivation: Implications for action-readiness,
Calder, B. J., & Burnkrant, R. E. (1977). Interpersonal influence on consumer procedural-readiness, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
behavior: An attribution theory approach. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 250260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2009.05.008.
4, 2938. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208676. Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the Turk: Understanding mechanical
Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient turk as a participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
belonging: How stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer 23(3), 184188.
science. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 10451060. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016239. for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models.
Cialdini, R. B., Trost, M. R., & Newsom, J. T. (1995). Preference for consistency: Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.
The development of a valid measure and the discovery of surprising behavioral 40.3.879.
implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 318328. Ruth, J., Otnes, C., & Brunel, F. (1999). Gift receipt and the reformulation of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.2.318. interpersonal relationships. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 385402.
Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209546.
as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Schwartz, B. (1967). The social psychology of the gift. The American Journal
Psychology, 87, 327339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327. of Sociology, 73, 111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/224432.
Escalas, J. E., & Bettman, J. R. (2003). You are what they eat: The influence of Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Rawsthorne, L. J., & Ilardi, B. (1997). Trait self
reference groups on consumer connections to brands. Journal of Consumer and true self: Cross-role variation in the Big-Five personality traits and its
Psychology, 13, 339348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1303_14. relations with psychological authenticity and subjective well-being. Journal
Escalas, J. E., & Bettman, J. R. (2005). Self-construal, reference groups, and of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 13801393. http://dx.doi.org/10.
brand meaning. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 378389. http://dx.doi. 1037/0022-3514.73.6.1380.
org/10.1086/497549. Shen, H., Wan, F., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2011). Cross-cultural differences in the
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York, NY: refusal to accept a small gift: The differential influence of reciprocity norms
McGraw-Hill. on Asians and North Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Flynn, F. J., & Adams, G. S. (2009). Money can't buy love: Asymmetric beliefs 100, 271281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021201.
about gift price and feelings. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being.
45, 404409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.003. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 13781391. http://dx.
Forehand, M. R., & Deshpande, R. (2001). What we see makes us who we doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378.
are: Priming ethnic self-awareness and advertising response. Journal of Teigen, K. H., Olsen, M. V. G., & Solas, O. E. (2005). Giver-receiver
Marketing Research, 38, 336348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.38.3. asymmetries in gift preferences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44,
336.18871. 125144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466604X23428.
Gino, F., & Flynn, F. J. (2011). Give them what they want: The benefits of Ward, M. K., & Broniarczyk, S. M. (2011). It's not me, it's you: How gift
explicitness in gift exchange. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, giving creates giver identity threat as a function of social closeness. Journal
47, 915922. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.015. of Consumer Research, 38, 164181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/658166.
Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2010). Preference for consistency and Wolfinbarger, M. F. (1990). Motivations and symbolism in gift-giving
social influence: A review of current research findings. Social Influence, 5, behavior. In M. E. Goldberg, G. Gorn, & R. W. Pollay (Eds.), Advances in
152163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15534510903332378. Consumer Research (pp. 699706). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Statistical mediation analysis with a Research.
multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical

Please cite this article as: Paolacci, G., et al., Give me your self: Gifts are liked more when they match the giver's characteristics, Journal of Consumer Psychology
(2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.01.006
View publication stats

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi