Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Journal of Alzheimers Disease 49 (2016) 10651074 1065

DOI 10.3233/JAD-150686
IOS Press

Social Cognition Deficits: The Key


to Discriminate Behavioral Variant
Frontotemporal Dementia from Alzheimers
Disease Regardless of Amnesia?
Maxime Bertouxa,b,c, , Leonardo Cruz de Souzab,d , Claire OCallaghane , Andrea Grevef ,
Marie Sarazinb,g , Bruno Duboisb,c and Michael Hornbergera
a Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
b Institut de la Memoire et de la Maladie dAlzheimer, Pitie-Salpetriere Hospital, Paris, France
c Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epiniere, UMRS 975 INSERM, Paris, France
d Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo-Horizonte, Brazil
e Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
f MRC Cognition & Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
g Neurologie de la Memoire et du Langage, Universite Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cite, INSERM UMR S894,

Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne, Paris, France

Accepted 27 September 2015

Abstract. Relative sparing of episodic memory is a diagnostic criterion of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD).
However, increasing evidence suggests that bvFTD patients can show episodic memory deficits at a similar level as Alzheimers
disease (AD). Social cognition tasks have been proposed to distinguish bvFTD, but no study to date has explored the utility of
such tasks for the diagnosis of amnestic bvFTD. Here, we contrasted social cognition performance of amnestic and non-amnestic
bvFTD from AD, with a subgroup having confirmed in vivo pathology markers. Ninety-six participants (38 bvFTD and 28 AD
patients as well as 30 controls) performed the short Social-cognition and Emotional Assessment (mini-SEA). BvFTD patients
were divided into amnestic versus non-amnestic presentation using the validated Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test
(FCSRT) assessing episodic memory. As expected, the accuracy of the FCSRT to distinguish the overall bvFTD group from AD
was low (69.7%) with 50% of bvFTD patients being amnestic. By contrast, the diagnostic accuracy of the mini-SEA was high
(87.9%). When bvFTD patients were split on the level of amnesia, mini-SEA diagnostic accuracy remained high (85.1%) for
amnestic bvFTD versus AD and increased to very high (93.9%) for non-amnestic bvFTD versus AD. Social cognition deficits
can distinguish bvFTD and AD regardless of amnesia to a high degree and provide a simple way to distinguish both diseases at
presentation. These findings have clear implications for the diagnostic criteria of bvFTD. They suggest that the emphasis should
be on social cognition deficits with episodic memory deficits not being a helpful diagnostic criterion in bvFTD.

Keywords: Alzheimers disease, amnesia, differential diagnosis, frontotemporal dementia, episodic memory, neuropsychology,
social-cognition

INTRODUCTION
Correspondence to: Dr. Maxime Bertoux, Department of

Clinical Neurosciencs, Herchel Smith Building, Forvie Site, Adden-


Relative sparing of episodic memory remains a diag-
brookes hospital, Cambridge, UK. Tel.: +44 1223542527; Fax: +44 nostic feature of behavioral variant frontotemporal
33142167504; E-mail: mb2044@medschl.cam.ac.uk. dementia (bvFTD) and is heralded as the neuropsy-

ISSN 1387-2877/16/$35.00 2016 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved
1066 M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD

chological gold standard to distinguish bvFTD from frontal/fronto-temporal hypoperfusion at SPECT scan
Alzheimers disease at presentation [1, 2]. However, and normal CSF biomarker profile as defined by
increasing evidence suggest that bvFTD patients can p-Tau/A42 ratio lower than 0.21 when a LP was per-
show episodic memory deficits [3], with a subgroup formed (n = 17/38, 45%). We included patients with
of bvFTD patients being impaired to a similar level as memory impairment if the other core diagnostic crite-
Alzheimers disease, even in biologically and patho- ria of bvFTD were present. Two patients had a genetic
logically confirmed cases [4, 5]. Similarly, on a neural mutation (1 GRN, 1 MAPT).
level, memory-related structures of the limbic system Thirty healthy controls were selected according to
are found to be affected up to a similar degree as the following criteria: normal scores at the MMSE and
Alzheimers disease in bvFTD [3, 68]. the FAB, no depression, and no history of psychiatric
By contrast, social cognition assessments have or neurological conditions. Controls were matched to
emerged as powerful new tools to distinguish both dis- patients on age and education.
eases in a clinical setting, when cerebrospinal fluid Importantly, all patients were followed-up over at
(CSF) biomarkers or amyloid imaging are not avail- least three years. The clinical progression of every
able [911]. However, it is currently not clear whether patient was in favor of the initial diagnosis. We did not
the high sensitivity and specificity for social cognition include participants who presented with the following:
deficits in bvFTD holds regardless of their amnestic (1) clinical or neuroimaging evidence of focal lesions,
impairment when compared to Alzheimers disease as (2) severe cortical or subcortical vascular lesions on
previous study only investigated social cognition in brain MRI, (3) severe depression, or (4) motor neuron
non-amnestic bvFTD. In others words, in case of severe disease.
episodic memory deficits, is assessment of social cog-
nition able to discriminate between the two diseases? Measurement of CSF biomarkers
The current study addresses this question by con-
trasting social cognition performance of biologically CSF samples were collected by LP and analyzed for
confirmed amnestic versus non-amnestic bvFTD as total tau, tau phosphorylated at threonine 181 (p-Tau),
well as Alzheimers disease patients and healthy con- and A42 using a double-sandwich enzyme-linked
trols. We hypothesized that social cognition deficits can immunosorbent assay (ELISA) method (Innogenet-
distinguish bvFTD from Alzheimers disease regard- ics, Gent, Belgium). Assays were conducted at the
less of amnesia. Metabolic Biochemistry Department of the Pitie-
Salpetriere Hospital, as described elsewhere [12].
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Neuropsychological assessment
Participants
All patients underwent a neuropsychological assess-
Ninety-six subjects were selected from the database ment that included the Mini-Mental State Exam
of the Memory and Alzheimer Institute of the (MMSE) [14], the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB)
Pitie-Salpetriere Hospital from September 2005 to [15], the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test
June 2012. Twenty-eight typical Alzheimers dis- (FCSRT) [16], the mini-SEA [9], semantic and
ease patients were selected according to the revised morphologic verbal fluencies, digit spans, and a
NINCDS-ADRDA criteria [1]. Among them, thir- picture-naming task in order to identify semantic mem-
teen Alzheimers disease patients underwent a lumbar ory deficits. In addition, bvFTD patients were tested
puncture (LP) showing biological evidence of the with the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS) and
Alzheimers disease pathophysiological process from the modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST)
their CSF biomarker profile defined by a p-Tau/A42 [17, 18].
ratio greater than 0.21 [12].
Thirty-eight bvFTD patients met the following Assessment of episodic memory (FCSRT)
inclusion criteria: prominent changes in personality
and social behavior according to the core clinical The FCSRT is based on a semantic cueing method
diagnostic criteria for probable FTD [13], clinical that controls for effective encoding of the list of words
progression consistent with the diagnosis of bvFTD and facilitates retrieval by semantic cueing [16]. Imme-
(therefore excluding so-called FTD phenocopies), diate cued recall was tested in a first phase in order
frontal/fronto-temporal atrophy at MRI scan, and/or to control for encoding (16 written words presented
M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD 1067

in groups of 4 4, maximum score = 16). Then, the groups based on the total recall score of the FCSRT,
memory phase was performed in three successive recall resulting in A-bvFTD (amnesic bvFTD; n = 19; 50%)
trials. Each recall trial included (1) a free recall attempt and nonA-bvFTD (non-amnesic bvFTD; n = 19; 50%).
consisting of spontaneous recall of as many items The normative data of the FCSRT were employed to
as possible, then (2) a cued recall attempt using an compare the total recall score of each patient to its age
orally presented semantic category for items that were and educational matched normative group and, consec-
not spontaneously retrieved by the patient. The same utively, abnormal scores were defined as scores below
semantic cues given in the initial encoding stage were the 10th percentile [20]. Data of the FCSRT for the
used. This provided (1) a free recall score and (2) a bvFTD group and result of this analysis are presented
total (free + cued) recall score (/48). Then, after an in the Supplementary Material 1.
interval of 30 minutes, a last recall trial was performed,
providing (3) a delayed total recall score (/16). Statistical analysis

Social cognition & Emotional Assessment Data were analyzed using SPSS20 (SPSS Inc.,
(mini-SEA) Chicago, IL). Prior to any analysis, variables were plot-
ted and checked for normality of distribution using
The mini-SEA taps into social cognition and emo- the Shapiro-Wilk test. Parametric data were compared
tion disturbances. It consists of two subtests and across the four groups (controls, Alzheimers disease,
provides two weighted composite scores: (1) a facial A-bvFTD, nonA-bvFTD) via ANOVA, followed by
emotion recognition test, scored from 0 to 15, in which Students t-test. Non-parametric data were analyzed
participants must identify the emotion expressed in a by Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA followed by the Mann-
photograph of a face (happiness, surprise, neutral, sad- Whitney test for two-by-two comparisons. Cohens d
ness, disgust, anger, and fear); (2) a shortened version effect-size was computed for all comparisons. Correla-
of the Faux-Pas Recognition Test [19], scored from 0 tions were analyzed using Spearman rank coefficient.
to 15, which evaluates theory of mind, where partici- Bonferronis correction for multiple measures was
pants must detect and explain social faux-pas through applied for all analyses. Logistic stepwise regression
short stories. The overall mini-SEA composite score analysis (using the Enter method) and Area Under the
is calculated by adding the two subscores, and scored Curve were processed in order to determine the accu-
from 0 to 30. More details about the administration racy of the mini-SEA to classify each patient in its
procedure were presented before in a previous study correct (bvFTD or Alzheimers disease) group.
[39].
RESULTS
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and
patient consent Demographics, clinical characteristics, and
neuropsychological scores
Controls were included in the INSERM RBM-05-15
study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee The three groups (controls, Alzheimers disease,
of the Pitie-Salpetriere hospital. Participants provided bvFTD) were not significantly different with regard
written informed consent before participating. For all to age, gender, and educational level (Table 1). Patient
patients, the biological and clinical data were generated groups did not differ on duration of disease. Not sur-
during routine clinical work-ups and were retrospec- prisingly, MMSE and FAB scores were significantly
tively extracted for the purpose of this work. According lower in the bvFTD and Alzheimers disease groups
to French legislation, explicit informed consent was compared to controls (p < 0.107 ). No difference in
waived as patients and their relatives were informed the MMSE score was observed between bvFTD and
that individual data might be used in retrospective clin- Alzheimers disease, as well as for digit spans (for-
ical research studies. ward and backward), semantic fluency, and picture
denomination task. FAB and morphological fluency
Denition of bvFTD subgroups scores were significantly lower in bvFTD compared
to Alzheimers disease (p < 0.001). No difference was
In order to test the ability of the mini-SEA to dis- observed between bvFTD patients who underwent LP
tinguish amnestic bvFTD patients from Alzheimers or had a genetic confirmation (n = 21/38) and those
disease, the bvFTD group was divided in two sub- who did not (n = 17/38) for any clinical features and
1068 M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD

Table 1
Mean (SD) scores for Alzheimers disease, behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), and control groups on demographics, general
cognitive tests, and clinical data
Demographics and clinical data Alzheimers disease bvFTD Controls
n 28 38 30
Mean age at test, y 70.3 (11.1) 66.6 (9.3) 67.2 (8.7)
Education, y 11.0 (3.6) 10.9 (3.8) 10.7 (3.7)
Disease duration, y 3.5 (2.8) 2.7 (1.8)
Gender, M/F 16/12 24/14 15/15
MMSE (/30) 24.3 (2.7) 23.4 (3.4) 29.0 (0.9)
FAB (/18) 14.9 (2.0) 12.1 (3.3) 17.1 (1.0)
Patients with LP, n 13 19
Patients with genetic mutation, n 0 2
CSF biomarkers
CSF A42 311.2 (122.1) 422.7 (144.1)
CSF Tau 580.4 (255.3) 241.2 (108.4)
CSF p-Tau 88.1 (32.3) 39.1 (16.3)
CSF Tau/A42 2.18 (0.9) 0.84 (0.3)
CSF p-Tau/A42 (cut-off = 0.21) 0.32 (0.1) 0.09 (0.04)
MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; LP, lumbar puncture; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers.
Significant difference compared to bvFTD. Significant difference compared to Controls. Significant difference compared to Alzheimers

disease.

Table 2 WCST, FAB, verbal fluency), language (picture nam-


Demographics data and neuropsychological scores for A-bvFTD and ing), or working memory (forward/backward digit
nonA-bvFTD
span) between A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD patients.
A-bvFTD nonA-bvFTD
A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD were, respectively, 9 and
n 19 19 12 to have a diagnosis confirmation (CSF exclud-
Mean age at test, y 66.5 (8.8) 66.6 (10.1)
Education, y 10.4 (4.2) 11.4 (3.5) ing Alzheimers disease, or genetic mutation). Within
Disease duration, y 2.7 (1.8) 2.8 (1.9) bvFTD subgroups (A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD), there
Gender, M/F 11/8 13/6 was no difference on any demographic, clinical, or cog-
MMSE (/30) 22.5 (3.8) 24.3 (2.8)
nitive measures between patients with and without LP
FAB (/18) 11.3 (3.7) 13.0 (2.6)
Executive Neuropsychological scores (Supplementary Material 2 and 3).
MDRS (/144) 116.3 (16.3) 124.4 (11.5)
Verbal Fluency (morphologic) 4.4 (3.5) 7.3 (4.1)
Verbal Fluency (semantic) 9.9 (3.3) 13.4 (4.8) Episodic memory scores
mWCST category (/6) 2.3 (1.2) 2.8 (4.9)
mWCST perseveration errors 9.2 (7.4) 7.2 (4.3)
mWCST attentional errors 3.4 (2.3) 3.1 (4.3) At the group level, bvFTD patients had significantly
Picture naming (%) 94.0 (5.2) 97.0 (4.2) higher free recall (p = 0.104 ; d = 0.79), total recall
Digit span forward 4.9 (1.0) 5.7 (1.5) (p < 104 ; d = 0.83), and delayed total recall scores
Digit span backward 3.1 (0.8) 3.6 (1.5) (p < 105 ; d = 0.97) than Alzheimers disease patients
MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; FAB, Frontal Assessment (Fig. 1). bvFTD and Alzheimers disease patients did
Battery; MDRS, Mattis Dementia Rating Scale; mWCST, modified
Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; mini-SEA, abbreviated version of the
not differ on encoding score. Each FCSRT score of
Social cognition and Emotional Assessment. Mean (SD). each patient was compared to its age and educa-
tional matched normative group. bvFTD patients were
47.4% (n = 18/38), 65.8% (n = 25/38), 50% (n = 19/38),
neuropsychological scores (Supplementary Material and 42.1% (n = 16/38) to have a score below nor-
2). The same result (i.e., no difference for any features mative scores for, respectively, encoding, free recall,
or scores) was observed between Alzheimers disease total (free+cued) recall and delayed total recall at
patients who underwent LP (n = 13/28) and those who the FCSRT. Alzheimers disease patients were 35.7%
did not (n = 15/28) (Supplementary Material 4). (n = 10/28, 1 missed data), 85.7% (n = 24/28), 85.7%
Demographics, clinical characteristics, and neu- (n = 24/28), 75% (n = 21/28) to have a score below nor-
ropsychological executive scores of A-bvFTD and mative scores for, respectively, encoding, free recall,
nonA-bvFTD are presented in Table 2. There was no total (free+cued) recall, and delayed total recall at
significant difference in age, gender, education, disease the FCSRT (Supplementary Material 1, Supplemen-
duration, MMSE, executive cognitive scores (MDRS, tary Table 1). Binary logistic regression using the Enter
M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD 1069

ROC Curve
100% 1.0

80% 0.8

Sensitivity
0.6
60%

0.4
40%

0.2
20%

0.0
0% 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
AD bvFTD
1 - Specificity

Fig. 1. Episodic memory & social cognition in bvFTD and Alzheimers disease. Performance (percentage of correct performance) at the FCSRT
and the mini-SEA in Alzheimers disease (AD) and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) (left graph) and ROC curve for the
FCSRT (lower curve) and the mini-SEA (upper curve) for the diagnostic distinction between bvFTD and AD.

100%
patients for each memory scores (Table 3, Fig. 2),
although they had significantly lower encoding
(p < 0.104 ; d = 1.58), free recall (p < 0.105 ;
80% d = 0.88), total recall (p < 0.107 ; d = 1.21), and
delayed total recall (p < 0.107 ; d = 0.87) scores than
60% nonA-bvFTD patients. The nonA-bvFTD patients
had higher encoding (p < 0.104 ; d = 1.40), free
recall (p < 0.106 ; d = 1.48), total recall (p < 0.107 ;
40%
d = 1.67), and delayed total recall (p < 0.107 ;
d = 1.51) than Alzheimers disease patients. Results
20% were similar when analyses were restricted to bvFTD
who underwent LP or had genetic mutation and
there were also no differences in the results when
0%
AD AbvFTD nonAbvFTD contrasting bvFTD who underwent LP or had genetic
mutation and bvFTD with clinical diagnosis only,
Fig. 2. Episodic memory & social cognition in Alzheimers dis- or when the analyses were restricted to bvFTD with
ease and in the amnestic and non-amnestic presentation of bvFTD. clinical diagnosis only (Supplementary Material 2
Performance (percentage of correct performance) at the FCSRT and
the mini-SEA in Alzheimers disease (AD) and amnestic (A-bvFTD) and 3).
and non-amnestic (nonA-bvFTD) behavioral variant frontotemporal All these analyses were replicated using gender,
dementia (bvFTD). age then duration of disease as covariates. No effect
of these variables was observed and therefore, results
did not change. In addition, gender effect was specifi-
method using the FCSRT correctly classified bvFTD
cally assessed using direct comparison between males
or Alzheimers disease with 69.7% of accuracy. AUC
and females in each group and no differences were
for this test (bvFTD versus Alzheimers disease) was
observed.
0.773. ROC curve for the FCSRT is displayed on Fig. 1.
The overlap between bvFTD and AD on the FCSRT
total recall score was 53%. Social cognition and emotional assessment
At the subgroup level, the ANOVA showed signifi-
cant difference between the three patient groups for all At a group level, compared to controls, bvFTD
memory scores. More precisely, as expected, A-bvFTD patients had significantly lower scores in both the
patients performed similarly to Alzheimers disease reduced Faux-pas test and the emotion recognition
1070 M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD

Table 3
Mean (SD) Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT) and mini-SEA scores for Alzheimers disease, amnestic (A-bvFTD), or non-
amnestic (nonA-bvFTD) behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and controls
FCSRT scores Alzheimers disease A-bvFTD nonA-bvFTD Controls
Encoding (/16) 13.3 (2.4) 11.8 (2.9) 15.2 (0.9)
Free recall (/48) 9.8 (5.7) 11.8 (8.7) 18.3 (5.8)
Total recall (/48) 27.8 (11.3) 30.8 (12.6) 42.5 (5.2)
Delayed total recall (/16) 7.9 (4.7) 10.4 (4.8) 13.9 (3.1)

mini-SEA scores
Total (/ 30) 24.3 (2.9)& 16.3 (3.7) 16.4 (3.7) 25.8 (1.8)&
Faux-pas (/15) 13.0 (1.7)& 9.2 (1.9) 8.2 (2.7) 13.2 (1.5)&
Emotion recognition (/15) 11.3 (1.7)& 7.0 (2.7) 8.1 (2.2) 12.6(1.1) &
Significant difference compared to Controls. Significant difference compared to nonA-bvFTD. Significant difference compared to Alzheimers

disease. & Significant difference compared to A-bvFTD.

subtests (all ps < 0.107 , with respectively d = 2.27 was observed for the later. A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD
and d = 2.61) and therefore a lower total mini-SEA patients also had significantly lower Faux-pas (respec-
total score (p < 0.107 ; d = 3.27). Alzheimers disease tively, p < 0.106 ; d = 2.10 and p < 0.106 ; d = 2.15)
patients had a lower emotions recognition score and emotions recognition scores than Alzheimers
(p < 0.104 ; d = 0.91) but showed no significant dif- disease patients (respectively, p < 0.106 ; d = 1.92
ference with controls on the Faux-pas test score and and p < 0.105 ; d = 1.65), as well as a lower mini-
on the total mini-SEA score, although a trend was SEA total score (respectively, p < 0.107 ; d = 2.40 and
observed for the later (Fig. 1; Supplementary Table 2). p < 0.107 ; d = 2.38). A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD did
Compared to bvFTD, Alzheimers disease patients not significantly differ on these measures. Results were
had higher total mini-SEA (p < 107 ; d = 2.41), Faux- similar when analyses were restricted to bvFTD who
pas (p < 107 ; d = 2.09), and emotions recognition underwent LP or had genetic mutation and there were
(p < 107 ; d = 1.90) scores. Logistic regression was also no differences in the results when contrasting
able to classify patients into bvFTD or Alzheimers dis- bvFTD who underwent LP or had genetic mutation and
ease in 87.9% of cases when using the mini-SEA total bvFTD with clinical diagnosis only, or when the anal-
score and in 89.2% or 76.9% in using either the reduced yses were restricted to bvFTD with clinical diagnosis
Faux-pas or the emotions recognition score. The results only (Supplementary Material 2 and 3).
were similar when the analyses were restricted to the Similarly to the analyses conducted on FCSRT
patients with CSF/genetic data (Supplementary Mate- scores, analyses for the mini-SEA were replicated
rial 2 and 3). The overlap between bvFTD and AD on using gender, age, then duration of disease as covari-
the mini-SEA score was inferior to 11%. ates. No effect of these variables was observed and
AUC for the mini-SEA (Alzheimers disease versus therefore, results did not change. In addition, gender
bvFTD) was 0.949. ROC curve for the mini-SEA is effect was specifically assessed using direct compari-
displayed on Fig. 1. son between males and females in each group and no
At the subgroup level (Table 3), ANOVA showed differences were observed.
significant differences between the groups. Compared
to controls, A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD groups had Accuracy of the mini-SEA to distinguish A-bvFTD
significantly lower scores in both the Faux-pas com- or nonA-bvFTD from Alzheimers disease
ponent (all p values <0.107 ; respectively, d = 2.37 and
d = 2.60) and the Emotion recognition component (all When bvFTD patients were divided on the basis of
p values <0.107 ; respectively, d = 2.71 and d = 2.56) the presence of episodic amnesia, the mini-SEA has
and therefore a lower total mini-SEA score (all p an accuracy of 85.1% to distinguish A-bvFTD patients
values <0.107 ; respectively, d = 3.25 and d = 3.22). from Alzheimers disease and 93.9% to distinguish
Alzheimers disease patients had a lower emotions nonA-bvFTD from Alzheimers disease.
recognition score (p < 0.104 ; d = 0.91) but showed no Finally, in order to confirm the discriminative power
significant difference with controls on the Faux-pas test of the mini-SEA, we conducted logistic regression
score and on the total mini-SEA score, although a trend analyses using independent random samples from
M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD 1071

the initial dataset for mini-SEA and FCSRT (Total power to differentiate the amnestic form of bvFTD
recall) scores. They are presented in the Supplemen- from Alzheimers disease [3, 5].
tary Material and showed very similar results, therefore By contrast, during the last decade, there has been
confirming the sample-based results. increasing evidence for the ability of social cognition
assessment to distinguish bvFTD from other diseases
Correlation analyses and specifically from Alzheimers disease [23], as it
taps into ventral and rostral parts of the medial pre-
Age was set as a nuisance variable in correlations frontal cortex [24, 25], which are specifically damaged
analyses. In Alzheimers disease, the FAB was signifi- in bvFTD [26], even at the early stages of the dis-
cantly correlated to the total mini-SEA score (R = 0.60) ease [27]. However, the utility of social cognition tasks
and the FCSRT free recall (R = 0.51). The MMSE was to differentiate amnestic bvFTD from Alzheimers
also correlated to the FCSRT encoding score (R = 0.47) disease has not been investigated before. Our find-
and the emotion recognition (R = 0.47). The digit-span ings show that regardless of the presence of episodic
(forward) was correlated to the emotion recognition amnesia, the mini-SEA can distinguish bvFTD from
(R = 0.51) and the mini-SEA scores (R = 0.64). In Alzheimers disease to a high degree, with a classifica-
bvFTD, the MMSE was significantly correlated to the tion power of 87.9% at group level and, more precisely,
FAB (R = 0.62) and the digit-span (forward) (R = 0.48). an accuracy of 85.1% and 93.9% to respectively distin-
No other significant correlation was observed. guish A-bvFTD and nonA-bvFTD from Alzheimers
disease. By comparison, the FCSRT lacked of power
to distinguish bvFTD from Alzheimers disease as it
DISCUSSION was able to classified only 69.7% of patients. The over-
lap between both groups (53%) was too high to allow
Our results clearly show that social cognition an accurate distinction, although bvFTD obtained bet-
can discriminate biologically confirmed bvFTD and ter performances than AD. By contrast, the overlap
Alzheimers disease to a high degree. More impor- between AD and bvFTD using the mini-SEA was low
tantly, social cognition deficits are unrelated to the (11%).
level of amnesia in bvFTD and thus may provide a Research on social cognition benefits from the
uniquely sensitive and specific cognitive marker for increasing recognition that social cognitive processes
the detection of the underlying pathology. are crucial for human interactions and adequate social
In more details, the preservation of episodic mem- adaptation. A growing number of tests are available to
ory in bvFTD has been recently challenged by an assess the deficits in this domain, which all have dif-
increasing number of independent studies showing ferent psychometric properties [28]. Theory of mind
that bvFTD patients can present with similar lev- assessments are particularly useful for capturing the
els of amnesia as Alzheimers disease [4, 5], with cognitive deficits related to the behavioral symptoma-
both manifesting a combination of frontally medi- tology of bvFTD [29], but a consensus is needed
ated and storage-based memory impairment. Although amidst the numerous available tests. Recently, Bora
previous studies have suggested that prefrontal cor- and colleagues provided crucial findings by conduct-
tex degeneration might be the greatest determinant ing a meta-analysis across theory of mind studies in
of amnesia in bvFTD [21, 22], more recent evi- bvFTD and Alzheimers disease in order to determine
dence suggest that bvFTD patients also show severe the sensitivity and specificity of theory of mind tasks
atrophy of the medial temporal lobes, including the evaluating different processes such as faux-pas recog-
hippocampus as well as the entire Papez circuit [4, 6] nition, sarcasm detection, false belief, and reading the
One of the only studies that cross-correlated episodic mind in the eyes [30]. Besides replicating previous
memory performance with grey matter intensity in findings by showing that theory of mind deficits could
bvFTD and Alzheimers disease showed that posterior accurately differentiate bvFTD from Alzheimers dis-
parietal and cingulate regions were implicated exclu- ease, the results showed that faux-pas and sarcasm
sively in Alzheimers disease while temporal poles and tests have the greatest discriminatory potential between
medial frontal regions were involved specifically in both diseases [31]. Moreover, social adaptation also
bvFTD [8]. Although the profile of cortical involve- relies on the accurate recognition of others emotional
ment in episodic amnesia is different in bvFTD and expressions, a critical process that allows adjusting
Alzheimers disease, current available episodic mem- ones behavior during a social interaction [32]. This
ory assessments (i.e., words-list based) may lack of process is also impaired in bvFTD and relatively spared
1072 M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD

in AD during the early stages of the diseases [3335]. criteria for bvFTD proposes the presence of executive
This highlights the importance of assessing emotion deficits with relative sparing of memory and visuospa-
recognition concurrently with theory of mind. Histor- tial functions as neuropsychological features of bvFTD
ically, neuropsychological testing of social cognition [13], without any reference to social cognition tests.
relied on long and experimental tasks, which are not Considering the increasing evidence of episodic mem-
always feasible in a clinical setting. The mini-SEA has ory impairment in bvFTD, and the diagnostic value of
been designed to provide a quick and easy way to assess tests that tap into emotional and social abilities, it may
theory of mind and emotional recognition through be valuable to propose these tests as clinical markers
revised and shortened versions of the faux-pas and for bvFTD diagnosis.
facial emotion recognition tests [19, 36]. This test has Although these results are in accordance with pre-
been linked, in bvFTD, to grey matter degeneration and vious studies about the clinical relevancy to use
perfusion decrease in rostral medial prefrontal cortex social cognition tests to discriminate bvFTD from
[37, 38], and has been shown to accurately distinguish Alzheimers disease [23, 28, 30], it is important to con-
bvFTD from Alzheimers disease and depression [9, sider that they could lack of power to distinguish the
39]. minority of bvFTD patients that have an Alzheimers
Our findings have strong implications on a disease underlying pathology. Because it taps into
biomarker level. Indeed, current diagnosis of spo- fronto-medial dysfunctions, the mini-SEA has shown
radic bvFTD remains challenging, as no biomarkers to be impaired in those very specific cases [42]. Fur-
exist to diagnose the disease. The episodic memory thermore, social-cognition performance could be also
problems in bvFTD further complicate the picture. lower in severe Alzheimers disease cases, as both the-
CSF biomarkers and amyloid imaging showed robust ory of mind and emotion recognition performance have
results for identifying Alzheimers disease relative to been shown to decrease over the course of Alzheimers
controls or patients suffering from FTD [40]. How- disease as a consequence of a more general cognitive
ever these investigations rely either on a LP, an deterioration [30, 34], which was highlighted in this
invasive exam for patients that requires a day of hos- study by the correlation between general cognition and
pitalization, or expensive neuroimaging requiring the social-cognition performance in Alzheimers disease.
production of short-life radioisotopes which require a However, we believe that these findings have critical
cyclotron and therefore cannot be performed outside implication on the clinical distinction of bvFTD and
of expert-centers. Similarly, radiological observations Alzheimers disease in bringing evidence that social
of hippocampal volumes have been proposed as a cognition could accurately distinguish bvFTD from
promising specific biomarker for Alzheimers disease, Alzheimers disease, regardless of amnesia.
but recent studies challenged this finding, showing
that bvFTD could present with a similar degree of
atrophy [4, 6]. Short cognitive tests, such as the mini- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
SEA, could therefore provide a simple, inexpensive,
non-invasive and efficient way to distinguish both We are thankful to Aurelie Funkiewiez for her
diseases at presentation, when facing to a patient involvement in the acquisition of clinical data, as
with an episodic amnesia that could be an indicator well as to Foudil Lamari for performing the CSF
or Alzheimers disease or bvFTD. This might be in biomarkers measurements. We also thank Celine
particular relevant for the detection of specific pathol- Chamayou, Virginie Czernecki, Richard Gnassounou,
ogy (tau, TDP-43) in bvFTD. For example, a recent Elodie Guichart-Gomez, Valerie Hahn-Barma, Dalila
study by the Genetic Frontotemporal dementia Initia- Samri, and Christina Rogan as well as Marina Agen
tive (GENFI), a multi-center study on presymptomatic and Mary Rouille from the Alzheimer Institute (Pitie-
FTD, has shown that genetic predisposed tau patients Salpetriere) for their help in the acquisition of clinical
(MAPT) show severe hippocampal atrophy already data.
up to 10 years before diagnosis [41]. Thus, detection Maxime Bertoux is supported by a Marie
of memory problems in addition to social cognition Skodowska-Curie Fellowship awarded by the Euro-
deficits might be a potential cognitive marker for tau- pean Commission. Claire OCallaghan is supported by
bvFTD. a National Health and Medical Research Council Neil
On the other hand, the present results raise the ques- Hamilton Fairley postdoctoral fellowship.
tion about the specificity of the current framework for Authors disclosures available online (http://j-alz.
the diagnosis of bvFTD. The International consensus com/manuscript-disclosures/15-0686r1).
M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD 1073

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Rosen H, Prioleau-Latham CE, Lee A, Kipps CM, Lillo


P, Piguet O, Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD, Fox NC,
Galasko D, Salmon DP, Black SE, Mesulam M, Weintraub S,
The supplementary material is available in the elec- Dickerson BC, Diehl-Schmid J, Pasquier F, Deramecourt V,
tronic version of this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/ Lebert F, Pijnenburg Y, Chow TW, Manes F, Grafman J, Cappa
JAD-150686. SF, Freedman M, Grossman M, Miller BL (2011) Sensitivity
of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of
frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134, 2456-2477.
REFERENCES [14] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini-mental
state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of
[1] Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger- patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189-198.
Gateau P, Cummings J, Delacourte A, Galasko D, Gauthier S, [15] Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B (2000) The
Jicha G, Meguro K, Obrien J, Pasquier F, Robert P, Rossor M, FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55,
Salloway S, Stern Y, Visser PJ, Scheltens P (2007) Research 1621-1626.
criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimers disease: Revising the [16] Van der Linden M (2006) Levaluation des troubles de la
NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol 6, 734-746. memoire Presentation de quatre tests de memoire episodique
[2] Piguet O, Hornberger M, Mioshi E, Hodges JR (2011) (avec leur etalonnage), Solal, Paris.
Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: Diagnosis, [17] Mattis S (1976) Mental status examination for organic mental
clinical staging, and management. Lancet Neurol 10, syndrome in the elderly patients. In: Geriatrics Psychiatry:
162-172. A Handbook for Psychiatrists and Primaty Care Physicians,
[3] Hornberger M, Piguet O, Graham AJ, Nestor PJ, Hodges Bellak L, Karasu T, eds. Grune & Stratton, New York,
JR (2010) How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral pp. 77-121.
variant frontotemporal dementia? Neurology 74, 472-479. [18] Nelson HE (1976) A modified card-sorting test sensitive to
[4] Hornberger M, Wong S, Tan R, Irish M, Piguet O, Kril frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12, 313-324.
J, Hodges JR, Halliday G (2012) In vivo and post-mortem [19] Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT (1998) Frontal lobe
memory circuit integrity in frontotemporal dementia and contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci 10,
Alzheimers disease. Brain 135, 3015-3025. 640-656.
[5] Bertoux M, de Souza LC, Corlier F, Lamari F, Bottlaender M, [20] Amieva H, Carcaillon L, Rouze LAlzit-Schuermans P, Miller
Dubois B, Sarazin M (2014) Two distinct amnesic profiles in X, Dartigues JF, Fabrigoule C (2007) Cued and uncued mem-
behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Biol Psychiatry ory tests: Norms in elderly adults from the 3C epidemiological
75, 582-588. study. Rev Neurol 163, 205-221.
[6] de Souza LC, Chupin M, Bertoux M, Lehericy S, Dubois [21] Thomas-Anterion C, Jacquin K, Laurent B (2000) Differential
B, Lamari F, Le Ber I, Bottlaender M, Colliot O, Sarazin M mechanisms of impairment of remote memory in Alzheimers
(2013) Is hippocampal volume a good marker to differen- and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
tiate Alzheimers disease from frontotemporal dementia? J 11, 100-106.
Alzheimers Dis 36, 57-66. [22] Pennington C, Hodges JR, Hornberger M (2011) Neural corre-
[7] Wong S, Flanagan E, Savage G, Hodges JR, Hornberger M lates of episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal
(2014) Contrasting prefrontal cortex contributions to episodic dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 24, 261-268.
memory dysfunction in behavioural variant frontotemporal [23] Elamin M, Pender N, Hardiman O, Abrahams S (2012)
dementia and Alzheimers disease. PLoS One 9, e87778. Social cognition in neurodegenerative disorders: A systematic
[8] Irish M, Hornberger M, El Wahsh S, Lam BY, Lah S, Miller review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 83, 1071-1079.
L, Hsieh S, Hodges JR, Piguet O (2014) Grey and white mat- [24] Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ (2009) Are there theory of mind
ter correlates of recent and remote autobiographical memory regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature.
retrievalinsights from the dementias. PLoS One 9, e113081. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 2313-2335.
[9] Bertoux M, Funkiewiez A, OCallaghan C, Dubois B, Horn- [25] Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain: A
berger M (2013) Sensitivity and specificity of ventromedial meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 829-858.
prefrontal cortex tests in behavioural variant frontemporal [26] Schroeter ML, Raczka K, Neumann J, von Cramon DY
dementia. Alzheimers Dement 9(5 Suppl), S84-S94. (2008) Neural networks in frontotemporal dementia a meta-
[10] Lavenu I, Pasquier F, Lebert F, Petit H, Van der Linden M analysis. Neurobiol Aging 29, 418-426.
(1999) Perception of emotion in frontotemporal dementia and [27] Seeley WW (2009) Frontotemporal dementia neuroimaging:
Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 13, 96-101. A guide for clinicians. Front Neurol Neurosci 24, 160-167.
[11] Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, Erzinclioglu S, Martin L, [28] Henry JD, Phillips LH, von Hippel C (2014) A meta-analytic
Baron-Cohen S, Hodges JR (2002) Theory of mind in patients review of theory of mind difficulties in behavioural-variant
with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimers frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 56, 53-62.
disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain 125, [29] Sarazin M, Dubois B, de Souza L, Bertoux M (2012) Should
752-764. the Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment replace stan-
[12] de Souza LC, Lamari F, Belliard S, Jardel C, Houillier dard neuropsychological tests for frontotemporal dementia?
C, De Paz R, Dubois B, Sarazin M (2011) Cerebrospinal Expert Rev Neurother 12, 633-635.
fluid biomarkers in the differential diagnosis of Alzheimers [30] Bora E, Walterfang M, Velakoulis D (2015) Theory of
disease from other cortical dementias. J Neurol Neurosurg mind in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and
Psychiatry 82, 240-246. Alzheimers disease: A meta-analysis. J Neurol Neurosurg
[13] Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer Psychiatry 86, 714-719.
JH, Neuhaus J, van Swieten JC, Seelaar H, Dopper EG, [31] Bertoux M, Hornberger M. Try to see it my way Which
Onyike CU, Hillis AE, Josephs KA, Boeve BF, Kertesz A, theory of mind tests best distinguish bvFTD and AD? J Neurol
Seeley WW, Rankin KP, Johnson JK, Gorno-Tempini ML, Neurosurg Psychiatry 86, 706.
1074 M. Bertoux et al. / Social-Cognition & Amnesia in bvFTD

[32] Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J (1994) Emotion- Emotional Assessment (SEA) differentiates frontotemporal
related learning in patients with social and emotional changes dementia and major depressive disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg
associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psy- Psychiatry 83, 411-416.
chiatry 57, 1518-1524. [40] Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Cummings JL, Dekosky
[33] Eckart JA, Sturm VE, Miller BL, Levenson RW (2012) Dimin- ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Delacourte A, Frisoni G, Fox NC,
ished disgust reactivity in behavioural variant frontotemporal Galasko D, Gauthier S, Hampel H, Jicha GA, Meguro K,
dementia. Neuropsychologia 50, 786-790. OBrien J, Pasquier F, Robert P, Rossor M, Salloway S,
[34] Goodkind MS, Sturm VE, Ascher EA, Shdo SM, Miller BL, Sarazin M, de Souza LC, Stern Y, Visser PJ, Scheltens P
Rankin PL, Levenson RW (2015) Emotion recognition in (2010) Revising the definition of Alzheimers disease: A new
frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimers disease: A new lexicon. Lancet Neurol 9, 1118-1127.
film-based assessment. Emotion 15, 416-427. [41] Rohrer JD, Nicholas JM, Cash DM, van Swieten J, Dopper
[35] Bertoux M, de Souza LC, Sarazin M, Funkiewiez A, Dubois E, Jiskoot L, van Minkelen R, Rombouts SA, Cardoso MJ,
B, Hornberger M (2015) How preserved is emotion recogni- Clegg S, Espak M, Mead S, Thomas DL, De Vita E, Masellis
tion in Alzheimer disease compared with behavioral variant M, Black SE, Freedman M, Keren R, MacIntosh BJ, Rogaeva
frontotemporal dementia? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 29, E, Tang-Wai D, Tartaglia MC, Laforce R Jr, Tagliavini F,
154-157. Tiraboschi P, Redaelli V, Prioni S, Grisoli M, Borroni B,
[36] Ekman P, Friesen WV (1975) Pictures of facial affect. Con- Padovani A, Galimberti D, Scarpini E, Arighi A, Fumagalli
sulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA. G, Rowe JB, Coyle-Gilchrist I, Graff C, Fallstrom M, Jelic V,
[37] Bertoux M, Volle E, de Souza LC, Funkiwiez, Dubois B, Stahlbom AK, Andersson C, Thonberg H, Lilius L, Frisoni
Habert MO (2014) Neural correlates of the mini-SEA (Social GB, Pievani M, Bocchetta M, Benussi L, Ghidoni R, Fin-
cognition and Emotional Assessment) in behavioral variant ger E, Sorbi S, Nacmias B, Lombardi G, Polito C, Warren JD,
frontotemporal dementia. Brain Imaging Behav 8, 1-6. Ourselin S, Fox NC, Rossor MN (2015) Presymptomatic cog-
[38] Bertoux M, Volle E, Funkiewiez A, de Souza LC, Leclercq D, nitive and neuroanatomical changes in genetic frontotemporal
Dubois B (2012) Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment dementia in the Genetic Frontotemporal dementia Initiative
(SEA) is a marker of medial and orbital frontal functions: (GENFI) study: A cross-sectional analysis. Lancet Neurol 14,
A voxel-based morphometry study in behavioral variant of 253-262.
frontotemporal degeneration. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 18, [42] de Souza LC, Bertoux M, Funkiewiez A, Samri D, Azuar
972-985. C, Habert M-O, Kas A, Lamari F, Sarazin M, Dubois B
[39] Bertoux M, Delavest M, de Souza L, Funkiewiez A, Lepine JP, (2013) Frontal presentation of Alzheimers disease. Dement
Fossati P, Dubois B, Sarazin M (2012) Social cognition and Neuropsychol 7, 66-74.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi