Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 7

This article was downloaded by: [190.55.198.

179]
On: 15 May 2013, At: 13:40
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Personality Assessment


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

Toward a Process-Based Framework for Classifying


Personality Tests: Comment on
a
Robert F. Bornstein
a
Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies, Adelphi University
Published online: 05 Dec 2007.

To cite this article: Robert F. Bornstein (2007): Toward a Process-Based Framework for Classifying Personality Tests: Comment
on , Journal of Personality Assessment, 89:2, 202-207

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223890701518776

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to
anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should
be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,
proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 89(2), 202207
Copyright 
C 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Toward a Process-Based Framework for Classifying


Personality Tests: Comment on Meyer
and Kurtz (2006)
Robert F. Bornstein
Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies
Adelphi University

Meyer and Kurtz (2006) argued that the longstanding psychological test labels objective and
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

projective have outlived their usefulness, and invited further work focusing on alternative
terms for these measures. This Comment describes a framework for classifying personality
tests based on the psychological processes that occur as people respond to test stimuli. Because
an attribution process is involved in responding to both types of measures, those instruments
formerly called objective tests are labeled self-attribution tests, and those formerly classi-
fied as projective tests are labeled stimulus-attribution tests. The possibility of extending
the process-based framework beyond personality, to psychological tests in general, is also
discussed. Clinical and empirical implications of a process-based framework are considered.

Meyer and Kurtz (2006) argued persuasively that the long- Meyer and Kurtz (2006) offered a range of alternative
standing psychological test labels objective and projec- labels to replace the terms objective and projective, con-
tive have outlived their usefulness, for two reasons. First, cluding their editorial by inviting further work on the topic.
these labels are misleading. As Meyer and Kurtz noted, ob- This Comment contributes to that ongoing effort by outlin-
jective tests hardly can be considered objective measures of ing a framework for classifying personality tests based on the
the psychological constructs they purport to assess; on the psychological processes that take place during testing (i.e.,
contrary, scores on these tests are affected by myriad threats the cognitive and affective dynamics that occur as the testee
to external validity, including respondent bias, subtle con- perceives, evaluates, and responds to test stimuli).
text/setting effects, and scorer error (Allard, Butler, Faust, &
Shea, 1995; Masling, 2002). Moreover, evidence supporting
the role of projection in shaping responses to projective
tests is inconclusive at best (Exner, 1989; Weiner, 2003); A PROCESS-BASED FRAMEWORK FOR
extant data suggest only that a projection-like dynamic may CLASSIFYING PERSONALITY TESTS
influence certain types of responses to ambiguous test stimuli
under certain limited circumstances (Bornstein, 2007). Because an attribution process is involved in responding to
The terms objective and projective are not only sci- both types of measures, what formerly were known as objec-
entifically inaccurate, but problematic from a professional tive tests might better be labeled self-attribution tests; what
standpoint as well. As Meyer and Kurtz (2006) pointed out, traditionally have been called projective tests are more ac-
the term objective carries with it an array of unwarranted curately labeled stimulus-attribution tests. In the following
positive connotations, seeming to imply objectivity in pre- sections I describe key features of these two types of tests in
diction when in fact the term refers only to the mechanical the context of a process-based framework.1
nature of test scoring. Conversely, because of its association
with Freudian theory and a plethora of flawed Rorschach
1 I use the term attribution in its broadest form here, not only to denote the
writings that have appeared over the decades, the term pro-
jective has acquired such a negative halo that even selective causal inferences that people draw regarding various internal experiences
and external events (Buehner & McGregor, 2006), but also to include the
and biased critiques of the validity of projective tests are neurocognitive mechanisms through which people automatically attribute
readily accepted by members of the professional community meaning to stimuli whose purpose and identity are unclear (see Kensinger
(see, e.g., Wood, Nezworski, & Stejskal, 1996). & Schacter, 2006).
BORNSTEIN ON MEYER & KURTZ 203

Self-Attribution Tests to that item even in the absence of anger-related memories if


the goal is to appear unhealthy rather than healthy.)
Instruments that traditionally have been identified as objec-
tive tests (e.g., the NEO Personality Inventory; Costa & Stimulus-Attribution Tests
McCrae, 1985) typically take the form of questionnaires
wherein people are asked to (1) acknowledge whether or Measures that require people to interpret ambiguous stimuli
not each of a series of trait-descriptive statements is true of may be characterized as stimulus-attribution tests, because
them; or (2) rate the degree to which these statements describe regardless of whether one is confronted with an inkblot and
them accurately. As McClelland, Koestner, and Weinberger asked What might this be? or with a drawing or painting
(1989) pointed out, such measures assess self-attributed and asked to describe the events depicted thereinthe fun-
traits, motives, emotions, and need statescharacteristics damental task is to attribute meaning to a stimulus that can
that a person acknowledges as being representative of his or be interpreted in multiple ways. This attribution process oc-
her day-to-day functioning and experience. Thus, these mea- curs in much the same way as the attributions that each of us
sures reasonably may be described as self-attribution tests. makes dozensperhaps hundredsof times each day as we
In responding to self-attribution test items people typi- navigate the ambiguities of the social world (e.g., when we
cally turn their attention inward to determine if a given test attempt to interpret our friends failure to greet us as we pass
statement captures some aspect of their feelings, thoughts, on the street; see Bornstein, 2007, for a detailed discussion
motives, or behaviors. Given the dynamics of memory and of this attribution process).3
memory distortion, coupled with peoples reliance on various Does the stimulus-attribution label suggest that projection
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

judgment heuristics to make decisions regarding self-relevant is never involved in responding to ambiguous test stimuli
events and experiences (Loftus & Davis, 2006; Schwarz like inkblots? Not at all. First, it is important to recognize
et al., 1991) however, self-attribution test scores will not nec- that all projections are in fact attributions. This is true re-
essarily yield accurate information regarding trait-related be- gardless of whether one ascribes to the traditional Freudian
haviors and experiences. Moreover, given recent research on view (which conceptualizes projection as a mechanism for
content nonresponsiveness (e.g., random or markedly acqui- disavowing unwanted traits, thoughts, and emotions; Freud,
escent responding) it is clear that mostbut not alltestees 1896/1962), a contemporary psychodynamic view (which
genuinely attempt to engage in and respond to these test conceptualizes projection as a method of avoiding responsi-
items via an introspective process (see Gallen & Berry, 1996; bility for negative events and protecting self-esteem; Cramer,
Graham, 2000). 2000), or the socialcognitive view (which conceptualizes
Self-attribution tests may be further divided into two cat- projection as a cognitive distortion wherein the degree to
egories: those wherein the person is asked to judge the ap- which others share ones views is overestimated; Newman,
plicability of a test item describing a longstanding pattern, Duff, & Baumeister, 1997). When a person concludes in the
and those wherein they are asked to judge the applicabil- absence of compelling evidence that their boss dislikes them,
ity of a test item describing behavior or experiencing in the their neighbor is out to get them, or their extreme political
here-and-now. For the most part trait-focused measures (e.g., beliefs actually are shared by many, that person is engag-
the trait version of Spielbergers [1989] State-Trait Anxi- ing in an attribution (or, more accurately, a misattribution)
ety Inventory [STAI]) involve retrospection, whereas state- process.
focused measures (e.g., the state version of the STAI) in- Note that all these inaccurate inferences reasonably might
volve introspection aimed at elucidating current, ongoing be labeled projections. They are, but they are also attributions,
experience.2 and in this context it is useful to recognize that attribution is
Although responses to self-attribution tests typically begin a broader and more encompassing term than projection. All
with introspection (and sometimes retrospection), another, projections represent attributions, but not all attributions (or
very different processdeliberate self-presentationoften misattributions) are necessarily projections.
follows. Consider how a person might respond to the state- Finally, as is true of self-attribution tests, responses to
ment, I often have difficulty controlling my anger. Even if stimulus-attribution tests often are modified by peoples ef-
one can recall numerous instances of angry outbursts, one still forts to present themselves in a positive or negative light.
might choose to respond No to that item to present oneself in As Exner (1991) and Weiner (2003) pointed out, people typ-
a positive light. (Conversely, one might choose to respond Yes ically generate many more Rorschach responses than they

2 In lieu of asking the respondent to report longstanding behavior or 3 In conceptualizing the psychological processes that occur as people

current functioning, some self-attribution test items ask the respondent to respond to stimulus-attribution tests it is important to note that responses to
speculate regarding attitudes, abilities, or preferences (e.g., I would make a ambiguous test stimuli are not determined exclusively (or even primarily)
good leader, I would rather be a physician than an attorney). With respect by personality characteristics of the testee but by stimulus characteristics as
to psychological process these are more closely aligned with trait than state well, and it is possible to study empirically the impact of stimulus pull on
test items. responses to stimulus-attribution test items (Weiner, 2003).
204 BORNSTEIN

ultimately verbalize; deliberate self-presentation (including Representations (QSDOR), which asks the respondent to
censorship of certain percepts) limits the number and type of generate descriptions of key introjects (e.g., mother, father,
responses articulated. The same is true of responses to other self, God) that are then scored along various structural and
stimulus-attribution tests. Moreover, given the demand char- content-based dimensions. Put another way, in contrast to
acteristics of most psychological testing situations, this cen- stimulus-attribution tests such as the Rorschach and TAT
sorship represents an appropriate self-presentation strategy wherein testees describe stimuli whose essential properties
and a positive prognostic sign. Although research indicates were determined a priori and which are physically present
that it is more difficult to deliberately fake ones answers during testing, in constructive tests the stimulus exists only
to stimulus-attribution tests than self-attribution tests (Born- in the mind of the respondent (e.g., a maternal or paternal
stein, Rossner, Hill & Stepanian, 1994), self-presentation introject).
clearly plays a role in shaping responses to both types of Continuing through Table 1, observational measures (as
measures. often are used to quantify behavior in hospitals, class-
rooms, shopping malls, and other settings) may be dis-
tinguished from informant-report tests (wherein data are
BEYOND PERSONALITY: EXTENDING THE derived from knowledgeable informants descriptions or
PROCESS-BASED FRAMEWORK TO OTHER ratings). Although in both cases judgments are made by
PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS an individual other than the person being evaluated, dif-
ferent processes are involved in generating these judg-
A process-based framework is useful in contrasting the men- ments, with observational measures based on direct obser-
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

tal activities that occur as people respond to self-attribution vation and immediate recording of behavior, and informant-
and stimulus-attribution tests; this framework may be use- report tests based on informants retrospective, memory-
ful for classifying other types of psychological tests as well. derived conclusions regarding characteristics of the target
Table 1 extends the process-based framework to the broader person.4
array of assessment tools used by psychologists today, group-
ing these instruments into six categories based on the men-
tal activities and behaviors involved in responding to these DO THE ADVANTAGES OF A
tests. PROCESS-BASED FRAMEWORK OUTWEIGH
Thus, in addition to self-attribution and stimulus- ITS DISADVANTAGES?
attribution tests there exist performance-based tests, which
include the Bender (1938) Visual-Motor Gestalt Test (when In discussing limitations in the current terminology used
used as a neurological screen), the Implicit Association Test to classify personality tests Meyer and Kurtz (2006) raised
(Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005), and various intelli- the possibility thatbecause any framework for grouping
gence and neuropsychological measures. Within the process- psychological tests into categories is likely to be less than
based framework performance-based tests are distinguished perfectperhaps assessment psychologists should simply
from stimulus-attribution tests because different processes identify each test by name and forego any attempt to create an
and tasks are involved, a distinction not captured by cur- overarching classification scheme. Although identifying tests
rent test labeling approaches (see Meyer et al., 2001; Meyer by name rather than by category has the advantage of elimi-
& Kurtz, 2006). Whereas performance-based tests require nating ambiguity that might arise when tests are grouped and
the respondent to perform structured behavioral tasks (e.g., labeled, a process-based framework for classifying psycho-
copy figures from cards, assemble jigsaw puzzles) with per- logical tests offers several advantages in this regard.
formance evaluated according to predefined scoring crite-
ria, respondents scores on stimulus-attribution tests like the
Rorschach and Thematic Apperception Test (TAT; Murray, 4 Certain types of measures (e.g., Q-sorts) may be included in more than

1943) are derived from open-ended descriptions and elabo- one category, depending upon whether they involve self-description (i.e.,
rations of test stimuli. self-attribution), or description by a familiar other (in which case they rep-
resent informant reports). Similarly, most archival data can be classified
In the process-based framework constructive tests also
either as a form of observational measure (if data initially were based on
are distinguished from stimulus-attribution tests, because direct observation, e.g., nurses notes of patient behavior), or as a form
constructive tests require respondents to createliterally of informant report (if data consist of the summary conclusions or judg-
to constructnovel products (e.g., drawings, written de- ments of knowledgeable others, e.g., end-of-semester evaluations provided
scriptions) with minimal guidance from the examiner and no by teachers or academic advisors). Finally, whereas structured interviews
represent self-attribution tests (akin to verbal questionnaires; see Rogers,
test stimulus physically present. Machovers (1949) Draw-
2003), unstructured and semistructured interviews whose focus is deter-
a-Person (DAP) test would fall into this category, as would mined largely by the clinicians inferences and assessment goals are best
Blatt, Chevron, Quinlan, Schaffer, and Weins (1988) mea- considered techniques for gathering and synthesizing clinical data, not as
sure of Qualitative and Structural Dimensions of Object formal psychological tests.
BORNSTEIN ON MEYER & KURTZ 205

TABLE 1
A Process-Based Framework for Classifying Psychological Tests

Test category Key characteristics Representative tests

Self-Attribution Test scores reflect the degree to which the person attributes NEO Personality Inventory
various traits, feelings, thoughts, motives, behaviors, or Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality
experiences to him- or herself. Disorders
Stimulus-Attribution Person attributes meaning to an ambiguous stimulus, with Rorschach Inkblot Method
attributions determined in part by stimulus characteristics and Thematic Apperception Test
in part by the persons cognitive style, motives, emotions, and
need states.
Performance-Based Test scores are derived from persons unrehearsed performance Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
on one or more structured tasks designed to tap on-line Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test
behavior and responding.
Constructive Generation of test responses requires person to create or construct Draw-a-Person Test
a novel image or written description within parameters defined Qualitative and Structural Dimensions of Object Relations
by the tester.
Observational Test scores are derived from observers ratings of persons Spot Sampling
behavior exhibited in vivo, or in a controlled setting. Behavior Trace Analysis
Informant-Report Test scores are based on knowledgeable informants ratings or SWAP-200
judgments of a persons characteristic patterns of behavior and Informant-Report version of the NEO Personality Inventory
responding.
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

The Process-Based Framework Facilitates Test based framework helps link personality assessment to re-
Score Integration search in other areas of psychological science (e.g., cognitive,
social). Only when assessment research is embedded in ideas
Because the process-based framework makes explicit the and findings from mainstream psychology can we evaluate
mental activities that occur as people respond to test stim- rigorously the external validity of our hypotheses and mod-
uli, this framework facilitates meaningful integration of els by scrutinizing the fit of these hypotheses and models
scores on different types of measures in the clinical set- with those of neighboring subfields. Psychologists in other
ting. For example, studies have shown that changes in de- areas have often utilized concepts from assessment research
pressive symptoms affect both self-attributed and stimulus- (e.g., internal reliability, discriminant validity) to improve
attributed dependency scores, whereas more minor vari- the psychometric properties of their tests and measures; the
ations in mood states affect stimulus-attributedbut not process-based framework can help assessment psychologists
self-attributeddependency (Bornstein, Bowers, & Bonner, use findings from other subfields (e.g., the dynamics of im-
1996; Hirschfeld, Klerman, Clayton, & Keller, 1983). It may plicit and explicit memory, the dispositional and contextual
be that the increased cognitive load (and diminished atten- variables that influence causal attributions) to understand
tional capacity) associated with a significant elevation in more completely the psychological processes that underlie
depression is sufficient to alter self-attributed dependency responses to different assessment tools.
scores while more minor variations in mood state are not.
Conversely, laboratory evidence suggests that the manner
in which the test is introduced has a significant impact Psychologists Are Going to Group and Label
on self-attributedbut not stimulus-attributeddependency Tests Anyway
(Bornstein et al., 1994).
Thus, in both research and clinical settings the process- People classify things into categories and assign labels to
based framework can help elucidate the dispositional and these categories; it is one of many strategies we use to pro-
situational factors that lead to test score inconsistencies, sug- cess, encode, and store information more efficiently. Regard-
gesting variables worthy of further study as we seek to under- less of whether these things consist of dogs, foods, works
stand the processes that dissociate scores on tests that ostensi- of art, or modes of transportation, studies suggest that once
bly assess the same underlying construct (see also Bornstein, people have developed some degree of familiarity with the
2002, for examples of other moderating variables). members of a group they intuitively divide members of that
group into subgroups based on properties of the individual
The Framework Helps Link Personality group members (Corter & Gluck, 1992; Rosch, 1975).
Assessment to Other Domains of Psychological Thus, if assessment psychologists did not derive overar-
Science ching frameworks and terminologies for classifying psycho-
logical tests, those who use, study, or critique these tests
By specifying the mental activities that occur as people re- would do it anyway. In this respect it is better that an orga-
spond to different types of psychological tests the process- nizing framework be made explicit (and the logic underlying
206 BORNSTEIN

the framework spelled out in detail) than that multiple con- Bornstein, R. F. (2007). Might the Rorschach be a projective test after all?
trasting frameworks and labels emerge in isolation among Social projection of an undesired trait alters Rorschach Oral Dependency
scores. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 354367.
different segments of the psychological community.
Bornstein, R. F., Bowers, K. S., & Bonner, S. (1996). Effects of induced
mood states on objective and projective dependency scores. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 67, 324340.
CONCLUSION Bornstein, R. F., Rossner, S. C., Hill, E. L., & Stepanian, M. L. (1994).
Face validity and fakability of objective and projective measures of de-
pendency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 363386.
The process-based framework readily suggests possibilities Buehner, M. J., & McGregor, S. (2006). Temporal delays can facilitate
for studies focusing on convergences and divergences be- causal attribution: Toward a general timeframe bias in causal induction.
tween scores from tests in different categories (Bornstein, Thinking and Reasoning, 12, 353378.
2002); this framework also can form a conceptual founda- Corter, J., & Gluck, M. (1992). Explaining basic categories: Feature pre-
tion for intracategory test score comparisons (e.g., contrasts dictability and information. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 291303.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory
of results from state versus trait measures of a motive or affect manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
state, comparisons of results produced by inkblot interpre- Cramer, P. (2000). Defense mechanisms in psychology today: Mechanisms
tations versus thematic storytelling). Such studies will not for adaptation. American Psychologist, 55, 637646.
only help clinicians and researchers understand the factors Exner, J. E. (1989). Searching for projection in the Rorschach. Journal of
that lead to divergences between findings obtained within Personality Assessment, 53, 520536.
Exner, J. E. (1991). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system: Vol. 2, Inter-
a particular test category, but also may create a context pretations 2nd ed. New York: Wiley.
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

for research exploring the range of situational and dispo- Freud, S. (1962). Further remarks on the neuro-psychoses of defense. In J.
sitional factors that combine to affect intracategory test score Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works
consistency. of Sigmund Freud (vol. 3, pp. 159188). London: Hogarth. (Original work
Finally, it is worth noting that in certain respects the published 1896.)
Gallen, R. T., & Berry, D. T. R. (1996). Detection of random responding in
process-based framework for classifying psychological tests MMPI-2 protocols. Assessment, 3, 171178.
is analogous to the grouping of psychological syndromes into Graham, J. R. (2000). MMPI-2: Assessing personality and psychopathology
overarching categories (e.g., mood disorders, anxiety disor- (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
ders, personality disorders) in the DSM-IV. Although both Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., & Keller, M. B. (1983).
organizing schemes are imperfect, and one could argue that Personality and depression: Empirical findings. Archives of General Psy-
chiatry, 40, 993998.
certain tests (or syndromes) should be categorized or labeled Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Neural processes underlying
differently, in both cases the organizing framework facili- memory attribution on a reality-monitoring task. Cerebral Cortex, 16,
tates clinical work and empirical study. Like the grouping of 11261133.
syndromes in the DSM series, the proposed system for classi- Loftus, E. F., & Davis, D. (2006). Recovered memories. Annual Review of
fying psychological tests will evolve over time, improving as Clinical Psychology, 2, 469498.
Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing of the human
new information emerges regarding various assessment tools. figure: A method of personality investigation. Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
Masling, J. M. (2002). Speak, memory, or goodbye Columbus. Journal of
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Personality Assessment, 78, 430.
McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-
attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690
I thank Violeta Bianucci, Daniel Freeman, Mark Hilsenroth, 702.
Michelle Sonnenberg, and all those who participated in the Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R.
JPA review process for their helpful comments on earlier R., Eisman, E. J., Kubiszyn, T. W., & Reed, G. M. (2001). Psychological
versions of this article. testing and psychological assessment: A review of evidence and issues.
American Psychologist, 56, 128165.
Meyer, G. J., & Kurtz, J. E. (2006). Advancing personality assessment
terminology: Time to retire objective and projective as personality
REFERENCES test descriptors. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 223225.
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test Manual. Cambridge,
Allard, G., Butler, J., Faust, D., & Shea, M. T. (1995). Errors in hand- MA: Harvard University Press.
scoring objective personality tests. Professional Psychology: Research Newman, L. S., Duff, K. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). A new look at de-
and Practice, 26, 304308. fensive projection: Thought suppression, accessibility, and biased person
Bender, L. (1938). A visual-motor gestalt test and its clinical use. New York: perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 9801001.
American Orthopsychiatric Association. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding
Blatt, S. J., Chevron, E. S., Quinlan, D. M., Schaffer, C. E., & Wein, S. J. and using the Implicit Association Test: Method variables and construct
(1988). The assessment of qualitative and structural dimensions of object validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166180.
representations (rev. ed.). Unpublished research manual, Yale University, Rogers, R. (2003). Standardizing DSM-IV diagnoses: The clinical appli-
New Haven, CT. cations of structured interviews. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81,
Bornstein, R. F. (2002). A process dissociation approach to objective- 220225.
projective test score interrelationships. Journal of Personality Assessment, Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal
78, 4768. of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192233.
BORNSTEIN ON MEYER & KURTZ 207

Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, G., & Robert F. Bornstein
Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies
availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
195202.
212 Blodgett Hall
Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State-trait Anxiety Inventory: A comprehensive Adelphi University
bibliography. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Garden City, NY 11530
Weiner, I. B. (2003). Principles of Rorschach interpretation (2nd ed.). Email: bornstein@adelphi.edu
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Wood, J. M., Nezworksi, M. T., & Stejskal, W. J. (1996). The Comprehensive
System for the Rorschach: A critical evaluation. Psychological Science, Received January 23, 2007
7, 310. Revised June 15, 2007
Downloaded by [190.55.198.179] at 13:40 15 May 2013

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi