Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49:10 (2008), pp 11181128 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01955.

Pre- and postnatal influences on preschool


mental health: a large-scale cohort study
Monique Robinson,1,2 Wendy H. Oddy,1,3 Jianghong Li,1 Garth E. Kendall,5
Nicholas H. de Klerk,1 Sven R. Silburn,4 Stephen R. Zubrick,4
John P. Newnham,6 Fiona J. Stanley,1 and Eugen Mattes1
1
Telethon Institute for Child Health Research, Centre for Child Health Research, The University of Western Australia,
Perth, Australia; 2School of Psychology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia;
3
School of Public Health, Curtin University of Technology, Perth, Australia; 4Centre for Developmental Health, Curtin
University of Technology, Perth, Australia; 5School of Nursing and Midwifery, Curtin University of Technology, Perth,
Australia; 6School of Womens and Infants Health, The University of Western Australia at King Edward
Memorial Hospital, Perth, Australia

Background: Methodological challenges such as confounding have made the study of the early deter-
minants of mental health morbidity problematic. This study aims to address these challenges in
investigating antenatal, perinatal and postnatal risk factors for the development of mental health
problems in pre-school children in a cohort of Western Australian children. Methods: The Raine Study
is a prospective cohort study of 2,868 live born children involving 2,979 pregnant women recruited at
18 weeks gestation. Children were followed up at age two and five years. The Child Behaviour Checklist
(CBCL) was used to measure child mental health with clinical cut-points, including inter-
nalising (withdrawn/depressed) and externalising (aggressive/destructive) behaviours (n = 1707).
Results: Multinomial logistic regression analysis showed that the significant risk factors for behaviour
problems at age two were the maternal experience of multiple stress events in pregnancy (OR = 1.20,
95% CI = 1.06, 1.37), smoking during pregnancy (OR = 1.30, 95% CI = 1.06, 1.59) and maternal
ethnicity (OR = 3.34, 95% CI = 1.61, 6.96). At age five the experience of multiple stress events (OR =
1.17, 95% CI = 1.08, 1.27), cigarette smoking (OR = 1.19, 95% CI = 1.03, 1.37), male gender (OR = 1.43,
95% CI = 1.02, 2.00), breastfeeding for a shorter time (OR = .97, 95% CI = .94, .99) and multiple baby
blues symptoms (OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 1.02, 1.14) were significant predictors of mental health prob-
lems. Conclusions: Early childhood mental health is significantly affected by prenatal events in
addition to the childs later environment. Interventions targeting adverse prenatal, perinatal and
postnatal influences can be expected to improve mental health outcomes for children in the early
years. Keywords: Mental health, pre-school children, behavioural development, Raine Study, CBCL.

The World Health Organization (WHO) estimates a findings are complicated by methodological difficul-
worldwide prevalence of mental health morbidity in ties (Geddes & Lawrie, 1995; Kessler, 2000; Susser,
children and adolescents of approximately 20% and Brown, & Matte, 1999). Geddes and Lawrie (1995)
identifies mental health in the early years as a key found in their meta-analysis that obstetric compli-
target for intervention strategies (World Health cations are potentially responsible for a two-fold
Organization, 2001). It is well established that increase in the later development of early-onset
mental health in early childhood has a significant schizophrenia, although the authors do note poss-
influence on subsequent mental health outcomes ible sampling bias in existing research. Susser and
(Preski & Walker, 1997). Poor mental health in early colleagues (1999) also reviewed the literature linking
childhood also has a negative impact on the childs prenatal insults with the later development of
physical health and school achievements (Sawyer schizophrenia and noted that the study of the early
et al., 2001). Therefore the early risk factors associ- determinants of mental health is potentially riddled
ated with mental health morbidity in pre-school with methodological challenges, such as confound-
children is a vital area of empirical enquiry both in ing bias and crude measures of exposures.
its own right and as a prerequisite for the develop- There has been a lesser focus on the early deter-
ment of effective preventions to improve mental minants of adverse behavioural outcomes in
health in children and adults. children and young adults, and such research is not
There is growing evidence that prenatal experi- immune from these same methodological conun-
ences can influence later psychological outcomes drums. Limited associations were found in a previ-
(Pennington, 2002). The pregnancy and postpartum ous prospective pregnancy study that examined a
periods have been recognised in relation to risk variety of pre- and perinatal risk factors and child
factors on the causal pathways for serious mental behaviour at age five (OCallaghan, Williams,
illnesses such as schizophrenia; however, these Andersen, Bor, & Najman, 1997). The authors
observed that only male gender, more antenatal
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. admissions and cigarette smoking during pregnancy
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA
Early life determinants of mental health in the pre-school years 1119

were significant predictors of either externalising or using a valid and reliable measure of child behav-
internalising behaviour in the adjusted analysis; iour, the Child Behaviour Checklist (Achenbach,
however, the use of a modified measure of child 1991). Understanding the influence of early deter-
behaviour may have influenced the absence of fur- minants can inform the appropriate targeting of
ther significant associations. Another study reviewed resources, especially during pregnancy, and the
pre- and perinatal risk factors for child behavioural development of effective clinical interventions to
problems, specifically attention deficit hyperactivity assist in lessening the social, emotional and
disorder (ADHD); however, the authors of this study economic burden of child mental health problems.
predominantly looked at the effects of prenatal
maternal substance use and stress and did not in-
clude other types of child behavioural problems Method
(Linnet et al., 2003). Some relevant research has
Study population
used retrospective data collection. Allen, Lewinsohn,
and Seeley (1998) conducted a comprehensive The study is based on data from the Western Australian
analysis and presented some striking findings as to Pregnancy Cohort Study, commonly known as the
the role of adverse pre- and perinatal events, such as Raine Study. The Raine Study is a longitudinal study
prenatal stress and premature birth, and the following women who were recruited at or around
18 weeks gestation (n = 2979) through the public
development of psychopathology in adolescence.
antenatal clinic at King Edward Memorial Hospital
However, the analysis is based on the maternal ret-
(KEMH) and nearby private clinics in Perth, Western
rospective recall of events that occurred between 14 Australia, from May 1989 through to November 1991.
and 18 years previously and hence the reliability of The enrolment methods are reported elsewhere (Newn-
the results is called into question (Allen et al., 1998). ham, Evans, Michael, Stanley, & Landau, 1993).
Putting such methodological issues aside, it is Briefly, to be eligible for enrolment, the women were
clear from the existing research that there are spe- required to have a gestation of between 16 and
cific negative behavioural sequelae associated with 20 weeks, sufficient English language skills, an expec-
specific pre- and perinatal events such as maternal tation to deliver at KEMH, and an intention to reside in
characteristics (Fergusson & Woodward, 1999), Western Australia to allow for future follow-up of their
socioeconomic hardship (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & child. At birth, a total of 2,868 live born children (96%)
were available for follow-up. All follow-ups of the study
Klebanov, 1994), stress experience (Rice, Jones, &
families were approved by the Human Ethics Commit-
Thapar, 2007), obstetric complications (OCallaghan
tee at KEMH and/or Princess Margaret Hospital for
et al., 1997), and cigarette smoking (Martin, Dom- Children in Perth.
browski, Mullis, Wisenbaker, & Huttunen, 2006). It
is also clear that risk factors for child behaviour
problems, such as those listed above, do not exist Measures Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL)
within a vacuum but rather exert their influence Child mental health was measured at age two by the
from within a complex network of biological, socio- Child Behaviour Checklist for Ages 23 (CBCL/23).
economic and psychological influences. The CBCL/23 is a 99-item, empirically validated
Given the importance of these methodological measure of child behaviour by parent report (Achen-
considerations, it is essential to unpack the rela- bach, Edelbrock, & Howell, 1987). At age five the
tionships between antenatal, perinatal and postna- children were assessed using the CBCL for Ages 418
tal risk and later mental health outcomes through (CBCL/418). The CBCL/418 is a 118-item commonly
used dimensional measure of child behaviour during
complex analysis of a longitudinal nature (Angold &
the previous six months and shows good internal
Egger, 2007; Kessler, 2000). What is currently
reliability and validity in a number of population set-
lacking is a comprehensive analysis that takes into tings (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & McConaughy,
account the multiple risk factors for behavioural 1997). A clinical calibration with Australian children
problems in early childhood and analyses these demonstrated moderately high sensitivity (83% overall)
multiple factors together within a prospective preg- and specificity (67% overall) to a clinical diagnosis, and
nancy cohort to gain a better understanding of which good testretest reliability (Zubrick et al., 1997). The
factors are of particular importance in terms of the three year predictive validity of the CBCL/23 for
development of psychopathology. CBCL/418 outcomes across both sexes is r = .49,
In this study we aim to prospectively examine a indicating good predictive power (Achenbach et al.,
comprehensive range of antenatal, perinatal and 1987).
Both the CBCL/23 and CBCL/418 produce a
postnatal risk factors and their impact on problem
T-score for total behaviour, in addition to apportioning
behaviours in early childhood using complete
factors into clinical domains and internalising and
behavioural data from the Western Australian Preg- externalising behaviour sub-scales (Achenbach, 1991).
nancy Cohort (Raine) Study. This analysis is an The withdrawal, somatic and anxious/depressed
improvement on previous research outlined above by domains are grouped together to determine a total score
using prospectively collected data from a large for internalising behaviours, which are associated with
population-based sample, analysing a multitude of depression, anxiety, and withdrawal (Achenbach &
factors together to minimise confounding bias, and McConaughy, 1997). The domains of delinquency and
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1120 Monique Robinson et al.

aggression are grouped to determine a score for exter- problems, problems with children, job loss (involun-
nalising behaviours, which are implicated in the psy- tary), partners job loss (involuntary), money problems,
chopathology of mental health disorders such as residential move or other stressful event. The question
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and on the 18 weeks gestation questionnaire asked whether
conduct disorder (Achenbach & McConaughy, 1997). any of the events had been experienced since becoming
As recommended by the CBCL manual, we applied pregnant and, on the 34 weeks gestation questionnaire,
clinical cut-off scores to the total CBCL T-scores to whether any of the events had been experienced within
obtain a binary variable indicative of overall mental the last four months, ensuring that the same event was
health morbidity, and internalising and externalising not counted twice. We examined the number of stress
morbidity (Achenbach, 1991; Drotar, Stein, & Perrin, events experienced at 18 weeks and 34 weeks gestation
1995). We chose to use clinical cut-point variables and given their near identical impact on mental health
rather than continuous scores as we were examining morbidity we created a variable representing the
the development of mental health problems and there- cumulative number of stressful events experienced
fore required a clinical point of definition for morbidity. since becoming pregnant.
In this study, a T-score of greater than 60 indicated
mental health morbidity, consistent with CBCL norms Perinatal determinants. We examined gestational age
(Achenbach & McConaughy, 1997). A variable with four at birth in weeks as a continuous variable. The gender
levels was created to indicate the presence of mental of the child (male or female) was also included in our
health morbidity at age two only, age five only, and both model, in addition to how many siblings the child had at
age two and age five as an indicator of ongoing problems the time of birth (continuous). Apgar scores five minutes
in comparison with the non-clinical sample. after birth were collected.

Postnatal determinants. Breastfeeding was exam-


Explanatory variables
ined as a continuous variable representing the total
Antenatal determinants. A number of sociodemo- duration of breastfeeding in months after collating
graphic influences in the antenatal period were exam- information from the feeding questionnaire at ages one,
ined, including maternal age at conception, maternal two and three. Postpartum mood disturbance was
education, maternal ethnicity and maternal psychiatric measured using an index of 16 postnatal feelings on the
history. Maternal education at the time of birth was Blues Questionnaire, completed by mothers after birth
included as a continuous variable representing the and while still in hospital. These 16 items were a
highest completed year of secondary education. Mater- summation of the 22 items on the Blues Scale devel-
nal ethnicity was originally recorded in the dataset as oped by Kennerley and Gath (1989), a robust and valid
Caucasian, Aboriginal, or other (9%), which predomi- measure of baby blues symptoms immediately post-
nantly consisted of Asian mothers. Aboriginal mothers partum (Henshaw, 2003). The number of blues feelings
(2.2% of cohort) were excluded from the analysis due to was included as a continuous variable because previ-
missing CBCL data. Maternal psychiatric history (2.4% ous research has shown that women experiencing
of the cohort) was also not included in the model for the higher levels of baby blues symptoms in the first days
same reason. after birth are more likely to develop postnatal depres-
We examined maternal smoking during pregnancy sion later (Henshaw, Foreman, & Cox, 2004).
with a continuous variable representing increasing
cigarettes smoked per day, measured at approximately
Procedure
18 weeks gestation. Preliminary analysis showed that
the number of cigarettes smoked per day by 34 weeks Informed consent to participate in the study was
gestation was strongly correlated with the number obtained from the mother of each child at every follow-
of cigarettes smoked at 18 weeks gestation (r = .866, up. Data regarding social, economic, and demographic
p < .001). We therefore used the amount of cigarettes characteristics and the experience of life stress events
smoked at 18 weeks gestation to represent smoking were collected at enrolment (18 weeks gestation) and at
frequency during pregnancy. Data on alcohol con- 34 weeks gestation. Perinatal data were collected at
sumption during pregnancy was also collected at 18 birth, baby blues symptoms in hospital shortly after
and 34 weeks gestation. birth, and infant feeding information was collected at
Total family income during pregnancy and the pres- one, two and three years of age. The study children and
ence of the childs biological father in the family home their families were followed-up at two and five years of
during pregnancy (yes or no) were also considered. age by questionnaire, which included sociodemo-
Family income was dichotomised according to mini- graphic and behavioural information.
mum household income levels, or the poverty line (less The aim of the follow-ups was to collect data as close
than $24,000 per annum compared with $24,000 per as possible to the intended age, although the range of
annum or greater), defined by the Australian govern- ages being examined increased as the study continued.
ment and utilised in similar population studies (Sawyer All cohort studies have problems from loss to follow-up
et al., 2001). although the attrition rate in our study was likely to be
Mothers were asked at 18 weeks and 34 weeks ges- non-random. At age two, 69% children participated in
tation whether or not they had experienced any of 10 the follow-up (M age = 26 months, SD = 1.9) while 76%
major life stress events selected from the 67-item life participated at age five (M age = 71 months, SD = 2.6).
stress inventory developed by Tennant and Andrews Teenage and young mothers, those who did not live with
(1976). These were: pregnancy problems, death of a the childs biological father at birth, those who experi-
close friend or relative, separation or divorce, marital enced high levels of stress and those whose child had a
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Early life determinants of mental health in the pre-school years 1121

lower gestational age were less likely to remain in the Table 1 Frequency characteristics for the cohort
study at two and five years, potentially having an effect
Continuous variables N M SD
on our observed results (Li et al., 2008). However, given
that the initial cohort over-represented socially-disad- Maternal Age at Conception* 1701
vantaged women, this loss to follow-up reduces the bias years 28.41 5.65
and may have increased the generalisability of the Stress Events in Pregnancy 1578
study. number of events 2.08 1.97
Gestational Age 1703
weeks 38.84 2.09
Breastfeeding Durationl 1661
Statistical analysis months 7.89 7.06
Correlations between risk factors were calculated [see Baby blues 1466
addendum] and frequency data analysed (Table 1). number of blues symptoms 2.94 2.98
Maternal Education* 1702
Univariate multinomial logistic regression analysis
highest school year 10.98 1.06
was initially performed to determine the relationships
between explanatory variables and total mental health Categorical variables N n %
morbidity (Table 2), and internalising and externalis-
CBCL Year 2 Morbidity Only 1707
ing problems (not presented). The four-tiered nominal
Total 88 5.2
outcome was no mental health morbidity and mor- Internalising 77 4.5
bidity at age two only, age five only, and at both ages Externalising 118 6.9
(all odds ratios have the children with no mental CBCL Year 5 Morbidity Only 1707
health morbidity as the denominator). The pre- Total 233 13.6
liminary univariate analysis showed no relationship Internalising 240 14.1
between alcohol consumption in pregnancy or Apgar Externalising 221 12.9
scores measured five minutes after birth and the CBCL Year 2 & 5 Morbidity Combined 1707
CBCL outcomes, therefore these variables were not Total 107 6.3
included. All explanatory variables were then block Internalising 57 3.3
Externalising 113 6.6
entered into a multivariable multinomial logistic
Maternal Ethnicity* 1706
regression model for total behavioural morbidity other 147 8.6
(Table 3) and internalising and externalising morbidity Gender of Child 1703
(Table 4). We tested for two-way interaction effects male 889 52.2
between the variables that were shown to be the most female 814 47.8
significant in the multinomial logistic regression Smoking in Pregnancy*,b 1707
analysis; however, there were no more significant none 1322 77.4
results than would be expected by chance and there- 110 daily 228 13.4
fore these interactions were not included in the final 1120 daily 132 7.7
model. We used SPSS Version 15.0 in our data ana- 21 +daily 25 1.5
Low Family Income in Pregnancy* 1631
lysis (SPSS Inc., 2006).
<$24,000 per annum 437 26.8
Father Living with Family* 1706
no 163 9.6
Number of siblingsa,b 1707
Results none 835 48.9
Prevalence 1 516 30.2
2 262 15.4
The frequency distributions of the outcome and 3+ 94 5.5
explanatory variables are presented in Table 1 by
Missing cases not included in analysis unless stated otherwise,
continuous and categorical status. Eleven and a half valid percentages used.
percent of two-year-old children had CBCL T-scores *Measured at 18 weeks gestation.

above the clinical cut-point for total behavioural Measured at 18 and 34 weeks gestation.
l
morbidity, while 20% of five-year-old children had Measured at age one, two and three years.

CBCL T-scores above the clinical cut-point. Over 6% Measured in hospital in days immediately following birth.
Measured at birth.
had a clinically significant T-score (60) at both ages, b
Included in regression analyses as continuous variable.
so 5% of two-year-olds and 14% of five-year-olds had
one episode of morbidity (Table 1). Externalising
problems were relatively more common than inter- below the poverty line, 10% of mothers did not live
nalising problems at age two and at both ages, while with the biological father of their child, and the mean
internalising problems were more common at age number of stress events experienced in pregnancy
five. The mean maternal age at conception was was two (SD = 1.97). The mean gestational age in
28 years (SD = 5.65), and 38% of mothers had left weeks was 39 (SD = 2.09), and 52% of children born
secondary education by the completion of year 10. were male. In our cohort 49% of births were firstborn
Almost 9% of mothers were of other ethnicity than children, the mean length of breastfeeding was eight
Caucasian or Aboriginal, and 23% smoked at least months (SD = 7.06), and the average number of baby
one cigarette per day during pregnancy. In our blues symptoms experienced in the immediate
cohort 27% of mothers had a total family income postpartum was three (SD = 2.98).
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1122 Monique Robinson et al.

Table 2 Univariate multinomial logistic regression analysis

Morbidity Year 2 Only Morbidity Year 5 Only Morbidity Year 2 &


Variables OR# (95% CI) OR# (95% CI) Year 5 OR# (95% CI)

Antenatal determinants
Maternal Age at Conception (years) .93** (.90, .97) .95** (.92, .97) .93** (.90, .96)
Maternal Education (highest school year) .77* (.63, .94) .89 (.78, 1.02) .73** (.61, .88)
Maternal Ethnicity (other) 2.54** (1.42, 4.51) .84 (.49, 1.45) 1.56 (.84, 2.87)
Smoking in Pregnancy (cigarettes per day) 1.30** (1.18, 1.52) 1.23** (1.10, 1.37) 1.19* (1.02, 1.39)
Low Family Income in Pregnancy (<$24,000pa) 2.00** (1.27, 3.16) 1.67** (1.23, 2.26) 3.65** (2.42, 5.51)
Father Living with Family (no) 1.68 (.84, 3.35) 2.62** (1.75, 3.92) 4.53** (2.81, 7.31)
Stress Events in Pregnancy (number of events) 1.23** (1.11, 1.37) 1.23** (1.15, 1.32) 1.41** (1.29, 1.55)
Perinatal determinants
Gestational Age (weeks) .96 (.88, 1.06) 1.04 (.97, 1.12) .89** (.83, .96)
Gender of Child (male) 1.60* (1.03, 2.48) 1.26 (.95, 1.67) 1.18 (.80, 1.75)
Number of siblings (total at birth) .86 (.68, 1.09) .82* (.70, .95) .69** (.54, .89)
Postnatal determinants
Breastfeeding Duration (months) .97 (.94, 1.01) .96** (.94, .98) .96* (.90, .99)
Baby blues (number of symptoms) 1.05 (.97, 1.14) 1.08** (1.03, 1.14) 1.20** (1.13, 1.28)

* significant at .05 level.


** significant at .005 level.
#
odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals.

Table 3 Multivariable multinomial logistic regression analysis

Morbidity Year 2 Only Morbidity Year 5 Only Morbidity Year 2 &


Variables OR# (95% CI) OR# (95% CI) Year 5 OR# (95% CI)

Antenatal determinants
Maternal Age at Conception (years) .95 (.901.01) .98 (.951.02) .97 (.921.02)
Maternal Education (highest school year) .94 (.721.23) 1.05 (.88, 1.25) 1.06 (.801.39)
Maternal Ethnicity (other) 3.34** (1.616.96) .97 (.511.86) 1.98 (.904.40)
Smoking in Pregnancy (cigarettes per day) 1.30* (1.061.59) 1.19* (1.031.37) 1.02 (.821.27)
Low Family Income in Pregnancy (<$24,000pa) 1.64 (.892.99) 1.12 (.741.70) 2.68** (1.474.88)
Father Living with Family (no) .64 (.231.78) 1.61 (.892.89) 2.07 (1.205.13)
Stress Events in Pregnancy (number of events) 1.20** (1.061.37) 1.17** (1.081.27) 1.29** (1.151.45)
Perinatal determinants
Gestational Age (weeks) .89 (.771.03) 1.03 (.931.14) .82** (.72.94)
Gender of Child (male) 1.59 (.932.70) 1.43* (1.021.2.00) .90 (.541.50)
Number of siblings (total at birth) .88 (.65, 1.19) .91 (.75, 1.10) .69* (.50, .97)
Postnatal determinants
Breastfeeding Duration (months) .99 (.951.03) .97* (.94.99) 1.00 (.961.04)
Baby blues (number of symptoms) .98 (.901.08) 1.08** (1.021.14) 1.15** (1.061.23)

* significant at .05 level.


** significant at .005 level.
#
odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals.

odds ratio increased in the adjusted analysis (OR =


Multinomial logistic regression analysis
3.34, p < .005), and was significant for internalising
Antenatal determinants. A younger maternal age behaviours rather than externalising behaviours.
(years) was significantly associated with an increase Increasing numbers of cigarettes smoked per day
in child mental health morbidity in all outcome cat- during pregnancy was significantly predictive of
egories in the univariate analysis (Table 2). However, mental health morbidity for all outcomes in the
these results were not significant in the adjusted unadjusted results, and was significant at age two
analysis (Table 3) except for externalising behaviour (OR = 1.30, p < .05) and age five years (OR = 1.19,
(Table 4) at age two (OR = .95, p < .05). Similarly, the p < .05) in the multivariable results for total
univariate relationship between increasing years of behaviour, for both internalising and externalising
maternal secondary education and fewer behavio- behaviour at age two years, and externalising
ural problems was no longer significant in the mul- behaviour only at age five years. A family income
tivariable analysis for any of the behavioural below the poverty line during pregnancy was
outcomes. Mothers from non-Caucasian back- strongly linked with the development of child mental
grounds showed a strong relationship with mental health problems in the univariate analysis; however,
health morbidity at age two in the univariate it was significant for total behavioural morbidity only
regression analysis (OR = 2.54, p < .005), and this at both ages in the multivariable results (OR = 2.68,
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Early life determinants of mental health in the pre-school years 1123

p < .005). A low family income was predictive of in-

(1.053.33)

(1.101.38)

1.08* (1.001.16)
.85 (.63, 1.14)
(.871.31)
(.901.00)
(.701.18)
(.372.58)

(.913.72)

1.14 (.701.87)

.98 (.941.02)
Year 2 & Year5
OR# (95% CI)

.84** (.74.95)
ternalising morbidity at two years (OR = 2.43,
Morbidity

p < .05) and externalising morbidity at two years (OR


= 2.09, p < .005) and at both ages combined (OR =

1.87*
1.07

1.23**
.91
.98

1.84
.95* 1.87, p < .05). The absence of the biological father in
the family home showed some relationships with
total behavioural morbidity in the univariate results,
Externalising Morbidity

(1.171.54) though was significant only for internalising mor-

1.05 (1.001.11)
(.86, 2.02)

.87 (.71, 1.07)


(.981.18)
(.931.00)
(.771.10)
(.421.69)

(.491.78)

1.02 (.921.14)
1.29 (.911.82)

1.00 (.981.03)
Morbidity Year

bidity at age five in the adjusted analysis. Increasing


5 Only OR#
(95% CI)

numbers of stressful events experienced during


pregnancy was significantly predictive of mental
health morbidity at age two (OR = 1.20, p < .005),
1.34**
1.32

1.08
.97
.92
.84

.94

age five (OR = 1.17, p < .005), and at both ages (OR =
1.29, p < .005) in both the adjusted analysis (re-
ported) and the unadjusted analysis. More stress
(1.021.49)
(1.283.43)

1.60* (1.012.54)
.97 (.75, 1.25)
(.991.24)
(.851.37)
(.883.58)

(.221.41)

.94 (.821.08)

.98 (.941.01)
1.00 (.921.08) experience was associated with internalising
Morbidity Year

(.91.99)
2 Only OR#

morbidity at age five years (OR = 1.15, p < .005), and


(95% CI)

externalising morbidity at both ages two and five


years (OR = 1.23, p < .005).
2.09**

1.11
1.08
1.78
1.23*

.56
.95*

Perinatal determinants. Within the perinatal period,


a low gestational age in weeks at birth was a
(1.97, 1.18)

1.20** (1.101.32)

significant risk factor for total behavioural morbidity


(.77, 1.38)
(.93, 1.06)
(.53, 1.02)

.75 (.50, 1.11)


Year 2 & Year 5

(.984.51)

(.991.35)
(.142.05)

.88 (.741.04)
.68 (.351.34)

1.04 (.991.08)
OR# (95% CI)

at both ages two and five years prior to (OR = .89,


Morbidity

p < .005) and following inclusion in the multivariable


model (OR = .82, p < .005). The analysis of internal-
Table 4 Multivariable multinomial logistic regression analysis internalising and externalising morbidity

4.47**
1.03
2.10

1.16
.99
.73

.53

ising and externalising outcomes showed that lower


gestational age at birth was associated with exter-
nalising problems rather than internalising prob-
Internalising Morbidity

lems. Male children were more likely to show clinically


(1.06, 1.25)
(1.04, 3.23)

1.81** (1.29, 2.54)

1.09** (1.03, 1.14)

*significant at .05 level, **significant at .005 level, #odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals.
(.83, 1.14)
(.79, 1.84)
(.97, 1.04)
(.92, 1.32)
(.57, 1.93)

.99 (.90, 1.09)

.98 (.96, 1.01)


Morbidity Year

significant overall behaviour at age two in the uni-


.77* (.63, .95)
5 Only OR#
(95% CI)

variate analysis (OR = 1.60, p < .05), and at age five


in the multivariable model (OR = 1.43, p < .05).
Male children also had clinically significant internal-
.97
1.21

1.15**
1.01
1.10
1.05

1.83*

ising behaviour at age five years (OR = 1.81, p < .005)


and externalising behaviour at age two years (OR =
1.60, p < .05). The more siblings in the family at
(1.02, 1.55)
(1.26, 4.68)
(1.06, 6.14)

(.94, 1.24)
(.90, 1.02)
(.63, 1.14)

(.35, 2.31)

.93 (.79, 1.10)


1.08 (.61, 1.92)
.79 (.56, 1.12)

1.02 (.98, 1.06)


1.04 (.95, 1.15)

birth, the less likely a child was to exhibit behavio-


Morbidity Year
2 Only OR#

ural problems at age five and at both ages in the


(95% CI)

univariate analysis, while this result existed only


for the both ages category in the adjusted model
1.26*
2.43*
2.55*

1.08
.96
.85

.90

(OR = .69, p < .05). Having more siblings was


protective for internalising morbidity at age five
(OR = .77, p < .05).
Low Family Income in Pregnancy (<$24,000pa)

Stress Events in Pregnancy (number of events)

Postnatal determinants. A longer duration of


Smoking in Pregnancy (cigarettes per day)
Maternal Education (highest school year)

breastfeeding (in months) was significantly related to


fewer child mental health problems at age five (OR =
Baby blues (number of symptoms)
Maternal Age at Conception (years)

Number of siblings (total at birth)

.96, p < .005) and at both ages (OR = .96, p < .05) in
Breastfeeding Duration (months)

the unadjusted results. When breastfeeding length


Father Living with Family (no)

was included in the multivariate analysis, it


Maternal Ethnicity (other)

remained protective for child mental health prob-


Gestational Age (weeks)
Gender of Child (male)
Antenatal determinants

Postnatal determinants

lems at age five only (OR = .97, p < .05) and was not
Perinatal determinants

significant for internalising or externalising behav-


iour. An increasing number of baby blues symp-
toms in the immediate postpartum was a risk factor
for child mental health morbidity at age five
Variables

(OR = 1.08, p < .005) and at both ages (OR = 1.15,


p < .005) for both the adjusted (reported) and
unadjusted analysis. More baby blues symptoms
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1124 Monique Robinson et al.

were also predictive of increased internalising mor- problems (Logan & Spencer, 1996). Smoking
bidity at age five (OR = 1.09, p < .005) and both ages behaviours are closely linked with sociodemographic
(OR = 1.20, p < .005) and externalising morbidity at status, including low income, and have been sus-
age five only (OR = 1.08, p < .05). pected as a proximal risk factor reflecting the larger
influence of socioeconomic status on mental health
outcomes (Logan & Spencer, 1996). However, we did
not find significant interaction effects in our study
Discussion
for antenatal maternal smoking, indicating that
Our study aimed to comprehensively examine the smoking is not primarily influencing child mental
antenatal, perinatal and postnatal determinants health status in a proxy capacity. As recently noted
of mental health morbidity in the pre-school years. by Button et al. (2007), it is clear that a direct
In this analysis we have looked at multiple risk fac- influence is present between smoking in pregnancy
tors for mental health morbidity in one model to and psychological problems, but what remains un-
minimise the effects of confounding bias that have known is the mechanism through which this influ-
limited previous research. We have also used an ence operates.
empirically validated outcome measure to examine Maternal ethnicity (non-Caucasian) was signific-
child mental health, and we have conducted our antly associated with overall child mental health at
analyses within a large prospective pregnancy cohort age two in the adjusted analysis, in particular for
to enhance the reliability of the findings and elimin- internalising problems where significant odds ratios
ate problems associated with retrospective recall. were evident at age two and at both ages two and five.
In all three outcome categories the experience of This may reflect the barriers faced by women from
multiple stress events in pregnancy was predictive of minority ethnic groups in accessing support for
a clinically significant T-score for total behaviour preschool children. Mothers from culturally and
after adjustment for multiple other risk factors, and linguistically diverse backgrounds also face barriers
the effects were equally balanced across internalis- other than socioeconomic resources, such as lan-
ing and externalising sub-scales. This finding guage, lack of social capital and discrimination, and
supports the link between stress experience in these factors in turn may affect child mental well-
pregnancy and fetal neuro-development that has being in the early years (Wen, 2007). The majority of
been previously found (Rice et al., 2007; Wadhwa et the sample of other ethnicity was comprised by
al., 2001), but adds to the literature in revealing that Asian mothers and previous research has found that
the strength of this relationship persists even in the Asian populations are usually less likely to experi-
light of other major influences on child mental health ence the same disparities related to child outcomes
morbidity. Clearly this factor is of great significance as other ethnic groups, though our finding would
in establishing the epidemiology of behavioural suggest that some difficulties or disparities do in-
morbidity in early childhood. One hypothesis sug- deed exist (Wen, 2007).
gests that this relationship is mediated by premature For the remaining antenatal risk factors, we found
exposure to cortisol, which may prepare the fetus for a number of significant results. Early childhood has
a world the mother perceives as difficult and hence been identified as the developmental stage where
inattention and externalising behaviour may repre- family income has the greatest effect on child devel-
sent the childs behavioural adaptation to a stressful opment (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Our study
world (Crowther et al., 2007; French, Hagan, Evans, showed that economic hardship in pregnancy was a
Mullan, & Newnham, 2004). Reporting a greater strong predictor of total mental health morbidity at
incidence of stress may also reflect maternal mental both ages two and five, and also had a significant
health status. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Par- effect on internalising behaviour at age two and
ents and Children (ALSPAC), a large-scale prospect- externalising behaviour at age two and at both ages.
ive cohort study, examined the influence of clinically A younger maternal age was a significant predictor of
significant maternal anxiety during pregnancy on externalising morbidity at age two and at both
child mental health outcomes at age four and found ages two and five; however, maternal age was not
a significant relationship between high levels of significant in the multivariable model. The correla-
prenatal anxiety and poorer mental health outcomes tions between maternal age and other variables are
in the early years (OConnor, Heron, Golding, likely to explain this result, with maternal age sig-
Beveridge, & Glover, 2002). nificantly correlated with all other variables except
Maternal smoking during pregnancy was a signi- the gender of the child.
ficant risk factor for later child behavioural problems We saw a protective effect of increasing years of
at age two and at age five, in particular externalising high school education experienced by the mother;
behaviour which is consistent with the existing however, this disappeared probably due to co-action
literature (Button, Maughan, & McGuffin, 2007; of other predictors such as stress, smoking and low
Maughan, Taylor, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2004). In terms of income. Specifically, the absence of the biological
general health outcomes, prenatal smoking may be father in the family home in pregnancy was predict-
the single most preventable cause of child health ive of internalising morbidity at age five in the
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Early life determinants of mental health in the pre-school years 1125

adjusted analysis, though not significant in the baby blues symptoms in the immediate postnatal
adjusted models for overall behaviour. It is period to be highly predictive of persistent mental
suggested that being a single parent is associated health morbidity in the pre-school years, and
with poorer child mental health outcomes and that strongest with later internalising morbidity but still
the younger a child is when their parents separate, influential with externalising behaviour at both
the worse the mental health outcome with children ages. This suggests that withdrawal and anxious/
whose parents have been separated since birth or depressed symptomatology are the specific
before to be at the greatest risk (Clarke-Stewart, behavioural consequences of maternal postpartum
Vandell, McCartney, Owen, & Booth, 2000). How- blues. Given the positive association between mul-
ever, numerous factors, such as antisocial behaviour tiple baby blues symptoms and clinical post-
of the father, mediate this relationship (see Jaffee, partum depression (Henshaw et al., 2004), our
Moffitt, Caspi, & Taylor, 2003) and may be respon- findings suggest that early intervention with
sible for the opposing directions of effect seen in the mothers experiencing multiple baby blues symp-
multivariable model. toms may be a strategy to prevent the later devel-
Three perinatal risk factors were explored within opment of mental health problems in offspring.
this study. A lower gestational age in weeks at birth
was predictive of the continuity of mental health
Strengths and limitations
morbidity in early childhood, particularly external-
ising morbidity, at ages two and five and is congruent The main strength of our study was the use of a large
with retrospective findings that children born pre- pregnancy cohort followed to age five years that
term are more likely to experience early mental provided information relating to a wide range of
health morbidity (Zubrick et al., 2000). Our results biomedical, sociodemographic and psychosocial
show that babies of a younger gestational age at factors collected from 18 weeks gestation and pro-
birth may face significant mental health challenges spectively throughout early childhood, thus allowing
throughout the pre-school years in addition for multiple risk factor analysis at several critical
to known physical health challenges (Aday, 1994). developmental stages. The large sample size enabled
In our study, male children showed more behav- rigorous analysis and generalisability for other
ioural morbidity than female children, with signi- populations of mothers and children showing similar
ficant results for overall behaviour and internalising characteristics. Research on the risk factors associ-
behaviour at age five, and significantly more exter- ated with poor child mental health outcomes to date
nalising behaviour at age two. Other studies have has predominantly been cross-sectional in nature, or
found male children to be more at risk of external- based on retrospective recall of data, both potentially
ising behaviour than female children (Baillargeon et unreliable strategies that can dilute the potency of
al., 2007; OCallaghan et al., 1997), and it is thought findings (Allen et al., 1998; Kraemer et al., 1997).
that externalising behaviour in boys is more salient Our use of the CBCL, a well-established measure
to mothers than in girls (Najman et al., 2001). of mental health morbidity, is a particular strength
Having more siblings was related to better overall of the study, as the CBCL has shown good internal
mental health status in our study at both ages two consistency in the diagnosis of child psychopatho-
and five, and was significantly predictive of less logy in many previous reports (Bird, Gould, Rubio-
internalising morbidity at age five only. The impor- Stipec, & Staghezza, 1991). The CBCL was shown to
tant role that siblings play in a childs social envir- be comparable to the DISC diagnostic schedule
onment has been noted previously (Leventhal & (based on DSM-III), suggesting that the CBCL is as
Brooks-Gunn, 2000), and having siblings at the time good at identifying children with psychiatric mor-
of birth has been associated with externalising bidity as a time-consuming diagnostic interview
behaviour such as physical aggression (Tremblay et (Jensen et al., 1996). Similar results have been
al., 2004). The results from this study show that found with Western Australian children (Zubrick
having more siblings is protective for mental health et al., 1995). Thus the use of the CBCL allows our
status, although more so for internalising behaviour findings to be compared with those of other studies
than for externalising behaviour. describing trajectories of childhood mental health
Finally, we examined two risk factors from the problems and behavioural psychopathology.
postnatal period. Breastfeeding for a longer period The use of maternal self-report for the CBCL as
showed a significant protective effect initially in the our primary outcome measure has been validated
univariate analysis which remained for total (Warnick, Bracken, & Kasl, 2007), although child
behaviour T-scores above the clinical cut-point at behaviour ratings may have been affected by the
age five only but otherwise was overshadowed by mothers own experience of stress and depressive
other sociodemographic risk factors. symptomatology postnatally (Najman et al., 2001).
The link between maternal depressive symptoms Our use of mothers ratings of baby blues symptoms
immediately postpartum and later child psycho- in the immediate postpartum period (within days)
pathology has long been recognised (Phillips & and measures of child behaviour at age two and age
OHara, 1991). We found the presence of multiple five make it problematic to presume any inverse
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1126 Monique Robinson et al.

causal relationship between earlier maternal mood Correspondence to


and later CBCL responses.
Monique Robinson, Telethon Institute for Child
Health Research, PO Box 855, West Perth, WA 6872
Australia; Tel: +61 8 9489 7913; Fax: +61 8 9489
Conclusion
7700; Email: moniquer@ichr.uwa.edu.au
Our study addresses the major methodological
problems experienced by previous research into the
early determinants of mental health morbidity in
early childhood. We have shown that particular risks
References
present in the ante-, peri- and postnatal periods Achenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior
have a significant independent influence on pre- Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: Univer-
school child mental health, particularly stress, sity of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
smoking, low income in pregnancy and multiple Achenbach, T.M., Edelbrock, C., & Howell, C.T. (1987).
baby blues symptoms immediately after birth. Our Empirically based assessment of the behavioural/
emotional problems of 2- and 3-year-old children.
study contributes to a more complete understanding
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 629650.
of the extent to which early determinants account for
Achenbach, T.M., & McConaughy, S.H. (1997). Empir-
the population prevalence and continuity of mental ically based assessment of child and adolescent
health morbidity, as well as their potential as targets psychopathology (2nd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA:
for prevention. Sage Publications.
Aday, L.A. (1994). Health status of vulnerable popula-
tions. Annual Review of Public Health, 15, 487509.
Supporting information Allen, N.B., Lewinsohn, P.M., & Seeley, J.R. (1998).
Prenatal and perinatal influences on risk for psycho-
Additional supporting information may be found in pathology in childhood and adolescence. Develop-
the online version of this article: ment and Psychopathology, 10, 513529.
Table 1 Univariate multinomial logistic regression Angold, A., & Egger, H.L. (2007). Preschool psycho-
analysis of stress events at 18 and 34 weeks gestation pathology: Lessons for the lifespan. Journal of Child
and total morbidity Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 961966.
Baillargeon, R.H., Normand, C.L., Seguin, J.R., Zocco-
Table 2 Univariate multinomial logistic regres-
lillo, M., Japel, C., Perusse, D., et al. (2007). The
sion analysis of stress events at 18 and 34 weeks
evolution of problem and social competence behav-
gestation and internalising and externalising iors during toddlerhood: A prospective population-
morbidity based cohort survey. Infant Mental Health Journal,
Table 3 Correlations between explanatory vari- 28, 1238.
ables using Pearsons r Bird, H.R., Gould, M.S., Rubio-Stipec, M., & Staghezza,
Table 4 Significant two-way interaction effects as B.M. (1991). Screening for childhood psychopatho-
a result of testing for interactions between explana- logy in the community using the Child Behavior
tory variables in the multivariable model Checklist. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Please note: Wiley-Blackwell are not responsible and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 116123.
for the content or functionality of any supporting Button, T.M., Maughan, B., & McGuffin, P. (2007). The
relationship of maternal smoking to psychological
materials supplied by the authors. Any queries
problems in the offspring. Early Human Development,
(other than missing material) should be directed to
83, 727732.
the corresponding author for the article. Clarke-Stewart, K., Vandell, D.L., McCartney, K., Owen,
M.T., & Booth, C. (2000). Effects of parental separa-
tion and divorce on very young children. Journal of
Acknowledgements Family Psychology, 14, 304326.
Crowther, C.A., Doyle, L.W., Haslam, R.R., Hiller, J.E.,
The Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort Study is Harding, J.E., & Robinson, J.S. (2007). Outcomes at
funded by the Raine Medical Research Foundation, 2 years of age after repeat doses of antenatal cor-
the National Health and Medical Research Council ticosteroids. New England Journal of Medicine, 357,
(NHMRC) of Australia, the Telstra Foundation and 11791189.
the Western Australian Health Promotion Founda- Drotar, D., Stein, R.E., & Perrin, E.C. (1995). Methodo-
tion. Dr W.H. Oddy is funded as an NHMRC Popu- logical issues in using the Child Behavior Checklist
lation Health Research Fellow. Dr E. Mattes was and its related instruments in clinical child psycho-
logy research. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
funded by an NHMRC General Practice Fellowship
24, 184192.
and a Capacity Building Grant in Population Health Duncan, G.J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family pov-
Research. We would like to acknowledge Peter erty, welfare reform, and child development. Child
Jacoby for statistical advice, the research assistants Development, 71, 188196.
for their ongoing commitment to data collection, and Duncan, G.J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P.K.
the study families for their continuing participation (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood
in the study. development. Child Development, 65, 296318.
 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Early life determinants of mental health in the pre-school years 1127

Fergusson, D., & Woodward, L. (1999). Maternal age conduct problems: Testing genetic and environmental
and educational and psychosocial outcomes in early explanations of the association. Archives of General
adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- Psychiatry, 61, 836843.
atry, 40, 479489. Najman, J., Williams, G., Nikles, J., Spence, S., Bor, W.,
French, N.P., Hagan, R., Evans, S.F., Mullan, A., & OCallaghan, M., et al. (2001). Bias influencing
Newnham, J.P. (2004). Repeated antenatal cortico- maternal reports of child behaviour and emotional
steroids: Effects on cerebral palsy and childhood state. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
behavior.[see comment]. American Journal of Obstet- 36, 186194.
rics and Gynecology, 190, 588595. Newnham, J.P., Evans, S.F., Michael, C.A., Stanley,
Geddes, J.R., & Lawrie, S.M. (1995). Obstetric compli- F.J., & Landau, L.I. (1993). Effects of frequent ultra-
cations and schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. British sound during pregnancy: A randomised controlled
Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 786793. trial. Lancet, 342, 887891.
Henshaw, C. (2003). Mood disturbance in the early OCallaghan, M.J., Williams, G.M., Andersen, M.J.,
puerperium: A review. Archives of Womens Mental Bor, W., & Najman, J.M. (1997). Obstetric and
Health, 6(Suppl. 2), s33s42. perinatal factors as predictors of child behaviour at
Henshaw, C., Foreman, D., & Cox, J. (2004). Postnatal 5 years. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 33,
blues: A risk factor for postnatal depression. Journal 497503.
of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynecology, 25, OConnor, T., Heron, J., Golding, J., Beveridge, M., &
267272. Glover, V. (2002). Maternal antenatal anxiety and
Jaffee, S.R., Moffitt, T.E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2003). childrens behavioural/emotional problems at
Life with (or without) father: The benefits of living 4 years: Report from the Avon Longitudinal Study of
with two biological parents depend on the fathers Parents and Children. British Journal of Psychiatry,
antisocial behavior. Child Development, 74, 109 180, 502508.
126. Pennington, B.F. (2002). The development of psycho-
Jensen, P.S., Watanabe, H.K., Richters, J.E., Roper, M., pathology: Nature and nurture. New York: The Guil-
Hibbs, E.D., Salzberg, A.D., et al. (1996). Scales, ford Press.
diagnoses, and child psychopathology: II. Comparing Phillips, L.H., & OHara, M.W. (1991). Prospective study
the CBCL and the DISC against external valida- of postpartum depression: 4 year follow-up of
tors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, women and children. Journal of Abnormal Psycho-
151168. logy, 100, 151155.
Kennerley, H., & Gath, D. (1989). Maternity blues. I. Preski, S., & Walker, L.O. (1997). Contributions of
Detection and measurement by questionnaire. British maternal identity and lifestyle to young childrens
Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 356362. adjustment. Research in Nursing & Health, 20, 107
Kessler, R.C. (2000). Psychiatric epidemiology: Selected 117.
recent advances and future directions. Bulletin of the Rice, F., Jones, I., & Thapar, A. (2007). The impact of
World Health Organization, 78, 464474. gestational stress and prenatal growth on emotional
Kraemer, H.C., Kazdin, A.E., Offord, D.R., Kessler, R.C., problems in offspring: A review. Acta Psychiatrica
Jensen, P.S., & Kupfer, D.J. (1997). Coming to terms Scandinavica, 115, 171183.
with the terms of risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, Sawyer, M.G., Arney, F.M., Baghurst, P.A., Clark, J.J.,
54, 337343. Graetz, B.W., Kosky, R.J., et al. (2001). The mental
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neigh- health of young people in Australia: Key findings
borhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood from the Child and Adolescent Component of the
residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psycho- National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being.
logical Bulletin, 126, 309337. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
Li, J., Kendall, G.E., Henderson, S., Downie, J., Lands- 35, 806814.
borough, L., & Oddy, W.H. (2008). Maternal psycho- SPSS Inc. (2006). SPSS 15.0 for Windows. Chicago.
social wellbeing in pregnancy and breastfeeding Susser, E.B., Brown, A., & Matte, T.D. (1999).
duration. Acta Paediatrica, 97, 221225. Prenatal factors and adult mental and physical
Linnet, K.M., Dalsgaard, S., Obel, C., Wisborg, K., health. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 326
Henriksen, T.B., Rodriquez, A., et al. (2003). Maternal 334.
lifestyle factors in pregnancy risk of attention deficit Tennant, C., & Andrews, G. (1976). A scale to measure
hyperactivity disorder and associated behaviors: the stress of life events. Australian and New Zealand
review of the current evidence. American Journal Journal of Psychiatry, 10, 2732.
of Psychiatry, 160, 10281040. Tremblay, R.E., Nagin, D.S., Seguin, J.R., Zoccolillo,
Logan, S., & Spencer, N. (1996). Smoking and other M., Zelazo, P.D., Boivin, M., et al. (2004). Physical
health related behaviour in the social and environ- aggression during early childhood: Trajectories and
mental context. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 74, predictors. Pediatrics, 114, E43E50.
176179. Wadhwa, P.D., Culhane, J.F., Rauh, V., Barve, S.S.,
Martin, R.P., Dombrowski, S.C., Mullis, C., Wisenbak- Hogan, V., Sandman, C.A., et al. (2001). Stress,
er, J., & Huttunen, M.O. (2006). Smoking during infection and preterm birth: A biobehavioural per-
pregnancy: Association with childhood temperament, spective. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology,
behavior, and academic performance. Journal of 15(Suppl. 2), 1729.
Pediatric Psychology, 31, 490500. Warnick, E.M., Bracken, M.B., & Kasl, S. (2007).
Maughan, B., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T.E. Screening efficiency of the Child Behavior Check-
(2004). Prenatal smoking and early childhood list and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire:

 2008 The Authors


Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1128 Monique Robinson et al.

A systematic review. Child and Adolescent Mental Zubrick, S., Silburn, S., Garton, A.F., Burton, P.R.,
Health, Online Early Articles. Dalby, R., Carlton, J., et al. (1995). Western Austra-
Wen, M. (2007). Racial and ethnic differences in general lian Child Health Survey: Developing health and
health status and limiting health conditions among well-being in the nineties. Perth, Western Australia:
American children: Parental reports in the 1999 Australian Bureau of Statistics and the Telethon
national survey of Americas families. Ethnicity and Institute for Child Health Research (ISBN 0 642
Health, 12, 401422. 20754 2).
World Health Organization. (2001). World health report: Zubrick, S., Silburn, S., Gurrin, L., Teoh, H., Shepherd,
2001: Mental health: New understanding, new hope. C., Carlton, J., et al. (1997). Western Australian Child
Geneva: World Health Organization. Health Survey: Education, health and competence.
Zubrick, S., Kurinczuk, J., McDermott, B., McKelvey, Perth, Western Australia: Australian Bureau of Sta-
R., Silburn, S., & Davies, L. (2000). Fetal growth and tistics and the Telethon Institute for Child Health
subsequent mental health problems in children aged Research (ISBN 0 642 17239 0).
4 to 13 years. Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 42, 1420. Manuscript accepted 14 April 2008

 2008 The Authors


Journal compilation  2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi