Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

In the Eye of the Storm

Race and Genomics in Research and Practice

Vivian Ota Wang U.S. Department of Health and Human Services


Stanley Sue University of California, Davis

The difficulties of operationalizing race in research and National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
practice for social, behavioral, and genetic researchers (NHANES; U.S. Department of Health and Human Ser-
and practitioners are neither new nor related to recent vices, 1982), Framingham Heart Study (Dawber, Meadors,
genetic knowledge. For geneticists, the bases for under-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

& Moore, 1951; Kannel, 2004), United Kingdom Biobank


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

standing groups are clines, observed traits that gradually (Critchley & Capewell, 2003), and deCODE (Hakonarson,
change in frequency between geographic regions without Gulcher, & Stefansson, 2003; Nievergelt, Smith, Kohlen-
distinct identifiable population boundaries and population berg, & Schork, 2004) have or are being planned to
histories that carry information about the distribution of overcome some of these barriers, their use also has also
genetic variants. For psychologists, race may not exist or been limited by unavailable and unclear race or population
be a social and cultural construct associated with fluid inclusion criteria that make data from these large national
social inferences. Because definitions of populations and
data sets difficult to combine and compare (Collins, 2004).
race can be socially and biologically incongruent, the
In contrast, quantitative genetics arose in the 1920s to
authors suggest that geneticists and social and behavioral
scientists and clinicians attend to external validity issues by examine how genetic, environmental, and Genetic ! En-
operationalizing population and racial categories and vironmental interactions vary because of evolutionary, de-
avoiding race proxies for other biological, social, and velopmental, and environmental factors. For example,
cultural constructs in research designs, data analyses, some variations (polymorphisms) in DNA sequences are
and clinical practice. transmitted generation to generation over evolutionary time
scales, whereas some variants arose more recently. These

T
differences influence the quantity, timing, and effect of
he results of genomic discovery along with the protein activity during the developmental and physiologi-
promises of molecular medicine have energized the cal lifetime of the individual (e.g., Gray & Thompson,
cultural discourse about science and health. Al- 2004).
though confusion of this genetic twist for some people has Given the increasing crossover among the genetics,
been a naive faith (or dread) that genetics (the study of social, and behavioral research communities, the complex-
single genes and their effects), genomics (the study of gene ity of health and disease processes can be better contextu-
functions and interactions across the genome and environ- alized and understood than previously done. Geneticists
mental factors; Guttmacher & Collins, 2002), and biology can provide a genomic framework of mechanistic and
are interchangeable, not all biological differences are ge- evolutionary explanations of health and diseases, whereas
netic. Acquired or inherited biological differences can exist social and behavioral scientists can examine the complex
because of human population differences secondary to mi-
gration effects (e.g., genetic drifts, bottlenecks, selection)
and other factors such as occupation, nutrition, toxic expo- Vivian Ota Wang, Ethical, Legal, and Social Implications Program, Na-
tional Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health,
sures, fetal and neonatal development, and discrimination. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; Stanley Sue, Department
To unravel the complexity of health, disease, and behav- of Psychology, University of California, Davis.
iors, social, behavioral, and epidemiologic scientists have We thank Francis S. Collins, Lisa D. Brooks, Kevin O. Cokley,
studied disease etiologies and processes by investigating Morris W. Foster, Mark Guyer, Jean E. McEwen, and Joseph F. Rath for
genetic, environmental, and Genetic ! Environmental in- thoughtful comments on earlier versions of this article.
The views expressed in this article are those of the authors. No
teractions in twin, association, and epidemiologic-based official endorsement of the University of California, the National Human
studies, approaches used long before the advent of molec- Genome Research Institute, the National Institutes of Health, or the U.S.
ular biology (e.g., Heath et al., 2002; Jinks & Fulker, 1970; Department of Health and Human Services is intended or should be
Malhotra & Goldman, 1999; Plomin & Crabbe, 2000). inferred.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Viv-
Shortcomings to these approaches include false-positive ian Ota Wang, Ethical, Legal, and Social Implications Program, National
associations, recall bias, limited phenotypic and environ- Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, U.S.
mental exposure data, ascertainment bias of more severe Department of Health and Human Services, 5635 Fishers Lane, Suite
cases, underrepresentation of underserved populations, and 4076, MSC 9305, Bethesda, MD 20893-9305. E-mail: otawangv@
replication problems. Although large cohort studies mail.nih.gov

January 2005 American Psychologist 37


Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0003-066X/05/$12.00
Vol. 60, No. 1, 37 45 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.37
dictions) have resulted in potential and real inequitable
treatment of individuals and groups. Nevertheless, the de-
bate about the biological and social meanings of race
continues (Bhopal & Donaldson, 1998; Burchard et al.,
2003; Cooper, Kaufman, & Ward, 2003; Foster & Sharp,
2002; Phimister, 2003; Risch, Burchard, Ziv, & Tang,
2002; Sankar & Cho, 2002). On the one hand, abandoning
the variable of race because it has no biological or scientific
validity has been an appealing choice for some researchers
(Haga & Venter, 2003; Schwartz, 2001). On the other hand,
if race does not exist, how are pharmacogenomic claims of
inter- and intrapopulation differences in drug metabolism
and toxicity justified (Evans & Johnson, 2001; Wilson et
al., 2001)?
To avoid unnecessary and potential harmful social or
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

biological exaggerations of population differences, one


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

needs a shared understanding of how geneticists and social


and behavioral researchers define and use the concepts of
population and race. This task is particularly difficult for
both the social and behavioral scientists who are trying to
Vivian find ways of achieving greater definitional precision of race
Ota Wang without undervaluing the psychological and social signifi-
cance and the geneticists who are cautious of not overes-
timating biological or genetic contributions to health and
diseases in a race-conscious society.
relationships among genetics, biology, society, culture, in- In this article, we describe ways geneticists and psy-
trapsychic experiences, and behaviors. chologists understand these concepts. We propose that in-
However, race, genetics, and psychology have shared creasing external validity, the extent to which research
a tumultuous history (Guthrie, 1998; Jones, 1997). On the results can be applied to other people beyond the popula-
one hand, geneticists recognize that genes act in concert tion studied (population validity) or settings (ecological
with environmental factors and that a complete understand- validity; Campbell & Stanley, 1963; Cook & Campbell,
ing of health, disease, and behavioral processes requires 1979) will provide greater methodological consistency be-
multifactorial, multidisciplinary research designs and anal- tween these research communities. Ways of defining pop-
yses. However, they often focus their attention toward ulation categories and research design issues that avoid
genomics and biology, overlooking environmental, social, using race as proxy for other biological, social, and cultural
cultural, and psychological factors that are rudimentary for constructs are also discussed.
social and behavioral scientists. On the other hand, many
social and behavioral scientists have tended to disregard
Human Genetic Variation
genetics. For some, their skepticism is due to their inability The human genome is organized into 23 pairs of chromo-
to make conceptual or theoretical connections between somes. These chromosomes carry the instructions for mak-
human health and the in vitro and animal models often used ing proteins. Chromosomes are made of DNA and in turn
in genetics. Additionally, some are cautious about genetics are made of the smaller units called bases. Four bases,
because of their limited knowledge of biology. adenine, thymine, cytosine, and guanine, are specifically
There is also nervousness about biological determin- paired; an adenine with a thymine, and a cytosine with a
ism because the history of psychology awkwardly reminds guanine to form a double-stranded helix. The human ge-
psychologists how their predecessors contributed to the nome contains three billion base pairs and is estimated to
conflation of genetics and race-based phenotypes (observ- have 21,000 25,000 genes. The genetic sequence of any
able behaviors or traits such as intelligence and skin color) human is estimated to be 99.9% identical to any other
at the expense of other explanatory mechanisms (Duster, unrelated person (International Human Genome Sequenc-
2003; Guthrie, 1998). Yet others are more broadly con- ing Consortium, 2004).
cerned that focusing on genetics will divert attention from Of the roughly 20 million variable sites in the human
existing research efforts investigating reasons for health genome, the major proportion, 85%, accounts for within-
disparities by attributing poor health outcomes to genetics group genetic diversity, with the remaining 10% represent-
and thereby overemphasizing the potential of genetic re- ing variation between any two geographically distinct
search to alleviate health disparities (Sankar et al., 2004). groups, and 5% between-groups from different continents
Overall, the confluence of race, behavior, and genetics (Jorde, Watkins, Bamshad, Dixon, & Ricker, 2000;
has been particularly contentious because misleading social Lewontin, 1972; Marth et al., 2003). This genetic variation
and cultural inferences about biology and race especially in is indicative of recent modern human ancestry with an
the area of behaviors (e.g., violence, intelligence, and ad- initial low population size of about 20,000 people who

38 January 2005 American Psychologist


ically more closely related than are members of groups who
live greater distances apart. Some population groups arose
from a small number of people who expanded their geo-
graphic and social and cultural mores into other local and
distant populations. Because of their small numbers cou-
pled with a relatively endogamous lifestyle for large parts
of their social and cultural history, their genetic contribu-
tions have been disproportionately magnified in their pop-
ulation cluster (Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994).
Because of migration events, biological meanings of
genetic differences have been inferred in populations
(King, 2002; Schwartz, 2001). For example, founder ef-
fects have resulted in variation in the prevalence of genetic
variants and disease alleles such as Tay Sachs in Ashkenazi
Jewish people, cystic fibrosis in people of Northern Euro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pean ancestry (Arnason, Sigurgislason, & Benedikz, 2000;


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Lucotte & Hazout, 1995), and differences among popula-


tions in the frequencies of drug-metabolizing enzymes
leading to pharmacogenomic differences among individu-
als in metabolism, efficacy, and drug toxicity (Exner, Dries,
Stanley Domanski, & Cohen, 2001).
Sue
Looks or Drops
When we talk about the concept of race, most people believe that
shared genetic and physiological traits because of relative they know it when they see it but arrive at nothing short of
endogamy (preferential procreation of members of social confusion when pressed to define it. (Higginbotham, 1992, p. 253)
units within their own group because of social or cultural
norms) for prolonged time periods and a population expan- The power of racialized thinking is derived from social
sion antedating the migration of modern humans out of impressions that race is biological and inextricable from a
Africa about 100,000 years ago (Cavalli-Sforza, Menozzi, persons essential character thereby having scientific legit-
& Piazza, 1994). imacy (Delgado, 1995). Because ethnocentric assumptions
However, cultural and geographic barriers to endog- and stereotypes perpetuated by biology and race-based
amy are fragile. Here, physical anthropologists and genet- notions have been used to justify exploitation, slavery
icists agree that traits (e.g., skin color) do not cluster in practices, and population stereotypes about behaviors, it is
rigidly bounded populations but gradually change in fre- not necessarily the physical and biological realities but the
quency from one geographic region to another. This pattern social and personal significances attached to these features
of continuous variation is called clinal variation (Bamshad, that people find unsettling (Gould, 1994; Takaki, 1994).
Wooding, Salisbury, & Stephens, 2004). Even Charles Historically, race has been defined by morphological
Darwin (1871) recognized clinal variation, noting that characteristics, such as skull volume and size, skin color,
facial features, and other visible qualities that could be
there are no races without transitions to others; that every race metrically measured and cataloged (Gould, 1981; Guthrie,
exhibits within itself variations of color, of hair, of feature, and of 1998). Even now, people have been shown to place inor-
form, to such a degree as to bridge over to a large extent the gap dinate emphases on an overall physical gestalt of racial
that separates it from other races. It is asserted that no race is
homogeneous; that there is a tendency to vary. (p. 698)
characteristics (e.g., Black, White, Asian) and are often
unable to distinguish features that would help them recog-
Despite clinal variation, some human variation patterns nize as individuals people who belonged to racial groups
cluster, are more common in particular population groups, unlike their own (Levin, 2000). However, the practice of
and are biologically meaningful. For example, the ABO classifying groups on the basis of physical features is not
blood group is polymorphic in all populations. People new. For example, Linnaeuss (1758) Systema Naturae
belong to one of four blood types, A, B, AB, and O. Blood categorized people by physical features and geographic
types are inherited in all people and have observed popu- ancestry into four racial groups (Europeaus, Asiaticus,
lation frequency differences (e.g., in almost all populations, Americanus, and Africanus).
Type O is the most common type, with it being particularly Linnaeuss protege Blumenthal defined five races
common in some North and South American Indian pop- (Caucasian, Mongolian, Ethiopian, American, and Malay)
ulations; Lewontin, 1972). and has been suggested by Gould (1994) as proposing a
So why are some geographic regions perceived as race hierarchy of worth based on perceived beauty as
major human population groups? In brief, human genetic embodied in a Caucasian ideal (from the people from the
variation patterns are influenced by population and migra- Caucasoid mountain range that lies between the Black and
tion history. People from local population groups are typ- Caspian seas). All of these categories have been included in

January 2005 American Psychologist 39


nearly all subsequent race lists down to the present day behavior) also contribute to the confusion. Additionally,
(Risch et al., 2002; Rosenberg et al., 2002; D. W. Sue & intragroup variation is rarely measured and accounted for,
Sue, 2003). resulting in the tendency to ignore intragroup variation and
By the 19th century, researchers were discovering report overgeneralized group findings.
that racial differences were biological and that morals, Failure to acknowledge intragroup variation can lead
physical characteristics, intellectual capacity, and social to overgeneralized clinical and research conclusions and
differences were the consequence of blood or biology thus problems of external validity. For example, social and
(Gould, 1981; Guthrie, 1998; Smedley, 1999). Using state- behavioral researchers and clinicians often assume sample
of-the-art science of the time, these researchers found or group homogeneity because participants self-identify
evidence in biology, craniometry (measuring skulls to as- into a single forced-choice category (e.g., Asian) making it
sign intelligence levels), anthropology, and medicine. Then impossible to gauge intragroup variation. This in turn can
as today, researchers were unsuccessful in finding valid and lead them to erroneously conclude that a universal set of
reliable population-specific metrics due to clinal and intra- psychological qualities characterizes the population when
group variation. For example, skin color (Parra et al., 2003) comparisons are significant (Betancourt & Lopez, 1993;
and ancestral geography (Bamshad et al., 2004) can be Zuckerman, 1990). Not only are these generalizations more
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ambiguous proxies for genetic heritage. monolithic than in actuality (e.g., Asians are nonverbal and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Recent reports of human genetic variation research family oriented), the conclusions drawn may bear little
reifying race or population clusters as naturally occurring resemblance to possible unmeasured proximal processes
(Risch et al., 2002; Rosenberg et al., 2002), rather than that may better explain the observed phenomena (e.g.,
social and cultural constructions, has been occurring within cognitive complexity/simplicity; individuation/collectiv-
the context of an increasing number of behavioral genetics ism, introversion/extroversion). This is particularly trou-
claims related to mental illness (Chakravarti, 2002; Segu- blesome given psychologists training in identifying, mea-
rado, 2003), behaviors (Kluger, Siegfried, & Ebstein, 2002; suring, and using proximal factors such as intrapersonal
McGough et al., 2002; Rhee & Waldman, 2002), and (e.g., cognitive styles, racial identity), situational (e.g.,
genetic determinism. The meaninglessness of race also has family systems, socioeconomic status, racism), and affec-
been gaining visibility in the popular and scientific press tive variables (e.g., depression, anxiety) in research and
(Harpending & Cochran, 2002; Pinker, 2002). practice.
The issue of race and psychology again fills the pages S. Sue (1999) has argued that the state of psycholog-
of the American Psychologist over a decade after Yee, ical research is weakened by professional preferences and
Fairchild, Weizmann, and Wyatt (1993) urged the Ameri- a selective encouragement (e.g., by journal editors) of
can Psychological Association to address psychologys internal validity (e.g., the confidence that causal relation-
problems with race and produce guidelines for research and ships can be concluded between independent and depen-
publication. Many of the barriers to understanding race dent variables) over external validity as defining quality
they listed still exist (e.g., inadequate definitions for race, research. Although both forms of validity are important and
fears of genetic determinism, misrepresentation of genetic necessary, internal validity dominates. We suggest that
information, and organizational and professional inaction). internal and external validity can be balanced. Compro-
Therefore, we believe the task before psychologists is not mises to research and clinical integrity need not be made if
to increase the length of their list or get mired in the details psychologists are equally deliberative and thoughtful about
of how race should be defined but to advocate for inten- external and internal validity issues. If social and behav-
tionality and clarity when using race or population identi- ioral researchers and clinicians continue to rely on conve-
fiers in research. Questions of whether studies are unduly nience sampling and disregard the importance of external
racialized will largely depend on the questions being asked, validity and population identifiers, not only will the overall
the quality of the research, and the extent to which the clinical utility and research quality diminish because of
conclusions can be supported. We believe the faults usually intra- and intergroup variation confounds, spurious clinical
lie in problems of external validity where research findings judgments and research results will contribute to fears of
and clinical applications are overgeneralized because of genetic determinism, eugenics, and discrimination. Genet-
vague or unclear race or population descriptions and using icists and social and behavioral clinicians and researchers
race proxies in research designs, data analyses, and clinical must attend to conceptual clarity by operationalizing pop-
interventions. Psychologists are all in the eye of the storm. ulation and racial categories and avoiding race proxies for
other biological, social, and cultural constructs in research
Recommendations and clinical practice.
Demographic variables are an essential and frequently un-
Conceptual Clarity
examined part of the clinical and research enterprises. In its
worst form, race is arbitrarily defined (if at all) and incon- Researchers often assume that demographic categories
sistently used by researchers and clinicians because of their such as race reflect phenotypically identifiable and stable
use of unclear and interchangeable terminology such as qualities of people. However, great discrepancies in how
race, ethnicity, national origin, heritage, and ancestry. race is defined and used have made population or race-
Varying notions of what population and race represent based meta-analyses difficult. First, researchers frequently
(e.g., biological aspects, psychological aspects, language, fail to operationalize what they mean when using popula-

40 January 2005 American Psychologist


tion categories. Second, because race is often used as a is to be identified by grandparental geographic ancestry,
proxy for other factors, it is at times unclear what the race then depending on the populations history (e.g., patterns of
demographic variable is actually measuring (or not migration) his or her identity may be reflective of contem-
measuring). porary temporal, geographic, political, and cultural factors
Self-report is one way of ascertaining ancestry or race rather than biological or genetic ones.
to define the population of interest. Research participants Nevertheless, geneticists and social and behavioral
are typically asked to indicate their race (and/or ethnicity) scientists and clinicians should not be discouraged and
by choosing one of a mix of options that reflect the re- summarily drop race as a variable or scientific term because
searchers notions of race, ethnicity, national origin, or they believe it is hopelessly ambiguous or a politicized
ancestry. Because a persons self-reported identity incor- descriptor of human populations. Instead, it is precisely
porates a complex mix of biological, cultural, psychologi- because researchers and clinicians are subject to the same
cal, and behavioral factors not necessarily determined by intentional and unintentional biases that exist in society that
genotype or biology (Li, 2003), racial self-referents can be they must take particular care when describing and opera-
highly variable and arbitrary, varying as a function of time, tionalizing what they mean when using race as a research
history, law, politics, social context, and emotions. Conse- variable or demographic descriptor. Therefore, operation-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

quently, a person may define and respond differently at alizing race is as an important part of the phenomenological
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

different times to the same questions about his or her race. world of the researcher, clinician, and study participants as
Although some genetic researchers may be using multilo- the research question under investigation.
cus genotype clustering as a way of avoiding the uncer- Thus, researchers and clinicians alike will need to
tainty of a study participants self-reported identity, similar describe the assumptions and rationale underlying how
to the forced choice of self-report, how they set their their population or race labels are defined and used. As
parameters will determine the scale and number of popu- with any variable, psychologists should explain, define, and
lation clusters (Rosenberg et al., 2002). measure how they understand the variable of race and the
Ancestry, based on the lineage of progenitors who are rules they used to select and define the populations being
assumed to compose lines of descent, is another population studied. These efforts should meet similar standards of
identifier that has often been used in genetics and social and measurement and clarity as other constructs used in re-
behavioral research. Boyds (1950) forewarning more than search. Not only must researchers strive for conceptual
a half century ago about the danger of taking over the coherence and consistency, they will need to be thoughtful
common mans ideas of race and incorporating them into to a priori identify and include proxy variables into their
anthropological treatises (p. 453) is particularly cogent study design or clinical judgments rather than relying on
given that populations that participate in research studies generating post hoc hypotheses that should have been ini-
have been typically chosen because of convenience rather tially accounted for or measured. As in most psychosocial
than more technical criteria. So, is self- or investigator- research and practices, it is the psychological undercarriage
identified ancestral geography adequate as a population of the demographic indices (e.g., attitudes, dispositions)
identifier? that are of interest and not necessarily the biological or
Some researchers (e.g., Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994) social nature of the variables themselves. With this ap-
have presented dendrograms or tree diagrams equating proach, investigators will be able to investigate their ques-
their genetic data to colloquially defined races and believe tions with greater scientific rigor because they will have
that self- or investigator-identified ancestral geography is identified and included potential environmental and cul-
sufficient for defining populations. Other researchers have tural proxy variables as possible independent, moderating,
felt less confident of self- and investigator-identified an- or mediating study variables.
cestral geography as a proxy for population groups, espe-
Race as a Second-Order Construct
cially when phenotypic overlap, increased population in-
termixing, changes of colloquial usages, and clinal and As a second-order construct, race is often used as a proxy
intragroup variation decrease its theoretical and predictive for assumed biological, genetic, social, psychological, and
value and appeal. For a more detailed discussion of geo- other phenotypic factors including peoples beliefs about
graphic ancestry, genetics, and race, see Bamshad et al. ancestry, nationality, language(s)/accent, religion, skin
(2004). color, racial identity attitudes, and so forth. The danger of
If researchers choose to use ancestral geography as a using proxy variables lies in their indirect quality and
strategy for defining populations, they must explicitly op- attached assumptions. For example, when Crow (2002)
erationalize what they mean by ancestry and geography and recently stated that members of certain races make good
clearly state their underlying assumptions. This may be athletes but are less qualified to be physicians, he implied
particularly relevant if the investigator is identifying how athletic ability and intelligence were innate to certain pop-
contextual variables are cohort or multigenerational expe- ulations. Even with little to no supporting evidence that
riences (e.g., access to health and mental health services in complex traits and behaviors, such as diabetes, intelligence,
the context of discrimination and immigration laws). For criminality, athletic ability, and so forth, are genetic, bio-
example, given estimates that humans have been around for logical, or inherited (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994), these
approximately 100,000 200,000 years and assuming 20 assertions are difficult to idiomatically dispute because of
years as the measure of one human generation, if a person peoples social and racialized conditioning that reinforce

January 2005 American Psychologist 41


racial stereotypes as being true. Thus, population compar- descriptive analyses. As a starting point, researchers and
isons often exaggerate group differences at the expense of clinicians who rely on self- or investigator-reported racial
within-group variation and distribution overlaps. To mini- categories and attribute their research findings to race or
mize this unfounded biological default, psychologists will population should clearly operationalize what they mean by
need to expand their research design, analyses portfolio, race, population, or culture and include measures about the
and clinical decision making to include proxy variables. constructs underlying their population assumptions (e.g.,
So how can psychologists engage in research that collectivism, locus of control) rather than doing post hoc
combines genetic, biologic, environmental, and intrapsy- deductions. Additionally, population-based conclusions
chic factors without perpetuating unfounded racial stereo- should be considered only when other possible proxy or
types? They must begin using more multifactorial research confounding factors (e.g., socioeconomic status, insurance
designs and clinical data that accommodate complex, en- coverage, racial identity status) have been taken into ac-
vironment, and phenotype interactions. This information count. Unless this is done, comparative population studies
will enable researchers to realistically understand how in- can become particularly troublesome, as they may uninten-
teractions among biological, social, and physical environ- tionally reify biological determinism and perpetuate un-
ments can affect health and disease. Additionally, because founded stereotypes (Beutler, Brown, Crothers, Booker, &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of the de facto biological presumption conferred on inde- Seabrook, 1996).


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pendent variables in research designs, psychologists should Therefore, researchers and clinicians should not avoid
explore more stepwise and complex linear models and data using the variable of race because race in fact is important
analyses (e.g., structural equation modeling) that use race as a social and cultural construct. However, unless re-
as a moderating and/or mediating variable instead of as an searchers and clinicians use race judiciously, race can be
independent variable. This will allow for a realistic account misleading because of varying and inconsistent definitions
of how population variables moderate and mediate com- and uses across studies, invalid biological or social/cultural
plex systems (Kraemer, Stice, Kazdin, Offord, & Kupfer, assumptions, and misinterpretations of the proxy and/or
2001). Moderators are categorical (e.g., race, sex) or con- distal variable(s). Although it is true that researchers and
tinuous (e.g., income, degree of depression) variables that clinicians may want to use race as a gestalt or as a term
affect the direction or strength of the relationship between encompassing many variables associated with the experi-
the independent and dependent variables and address ques- ence of racial minorities (e.g., minority group status, stigma
tions of when or for whom a variable causes or predicts an because of skin color, experience with prejudice and dis-
outcome. Mediators are variables that account for or ex- crimination, etc.), the usefulness of race as a variable is an
plain the relationship between independent and dependent empirical question that should be dismantled and examined
variables and usually involve establishing how or why an in the following stages.
independent variable causes or predicts a criterion variable First, what is the definition and what are the advan-
(Baron & Kenny, 1986; Holmbeck, 1997). tages and limitations imposed by the definition? Second,
does the variable make a difference? Third, if differences
Race, Genetics, and Health are found, why do they exist? Are the racial explanations
Disparities consistent with what is allowable given ones definition?
Particular care must be taken to avoid using a socially
One way to investigate the issues of race is to be more based definition and invoking a genetic explanation (or vice
thoughtful about what race means as a distal or proximal versa) in the absence of strong and compelling evidence.
variable when doing research on population and health Additionally, if race is being examined as a proxy for other
disparities. By clarifying the roles and types of conclusions variables, the other variables should also be studied. Thus
that can be drawn, psychologists will find that interpreta- issues involving proxy variables, as well as moderators and
tions of research findings will be less confusing, especially mediators, should be examined in order to explain race
when biological and social experiences (e.g., phenotype- effects.
based cognitive distortions and stereotyping) are being
examined in the same research or clinical model. For Implications for Clinical Practice
example, race often is used as a distal variable. Unlike The dilemmas of using race in research are similar to those
proximal factors that may actually be responsible for dif- encountered in clinical, counseling, and professional prac-
ferences (e.g., acculturation), distal variables are furthest tice because clinicians definitions and assumptions about
from the point of causation, are typically descriptive (e.g., race influence diagnostics and clinical treatment plans. For
race, gender), and do not directly explain an observed example, how do clinician and client racial identities affect
phenomenon. Thus, psychologists need to understand the their assumptions about race? How do clients and clini-
research and clinical implications of using and interpreting cians racialized life experiences affect the therapeutic set-
race as a distal variable. To do this, they need to avoid ting and working alliance? How does a clinician compare
overreliance on descriptive analyses and distal explanations treatment efficacy that is found to be effective for one racial
and consider more theoretically driven proximal factors. group with another group?
Overall, to avoid contributing to and reifying existing Because treatment outcome disparities have been
notions of race as being biological and absolute, psychol- found for clients from various racial groups, guidelines
ogists need to explain group differences beyond simple have been established by the American Psychological As-

42 January 2005 American Psychologist


sociation (2002) to address how clients should be consid- on U.S. middle-class people of European ancestry. Al-
ered within their racial and cultural context and how cul- though the treatments may well be effective with different
turally competent skills should be acquired by clinicians. In populations, until research clearly demonstrates their effec-
particular, the guidelines raise the following important is- tiveness with different populations, clinicians should apply
sues pertinent in our discussion of professional practice and these interventions judiciously.
race:
1. Clinicians usually infer a clients racial group mem-
Final Thoughts
bership on the basis of their personal judgment of the How we define and discuss race has major biomedical,
clients physical appearance or by the clients self-report. psychosocial, and public health implications. Genetic, so-
Of critical importance is the clinicians ability to evaluate cial, and cultural interpretations of population and social
the clients psychological and pragmatic experiences of histories can be used in ways that intentionally or uninten-
being a member of a racial group. Just as researchers tionally lead to genetic discrimination, stigmatization, and
should understand the meaning of race in research studies, missed or delayed diagnoses. To these ends, how social,
clinicians should understand what race means for their behavioral, and genetic scientists and health service pro-
clients. viders understand how racism and race are used in research
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2. Understanding the meanings of race is critical for and practice continues to be investigated (Clark, Anderson,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

clinicians. Instead of solely placing race as belonging to Clark, & Williams, 1999; Krieger, Sidney, & Coakley,
their clients, clinicians must also be self-reflective of how 1998; Mays, Ponce, Washington, & Cochran, 2003; U.S.
their own racialized experiences have influenced their at- Department of Health and Human Services, 2001a, 2001b).
titudes and behaviors toward similar and different racial Overall, geneticists and social and behavioral scien-
groups compared with their own. tists and clinicians must recognize how their own unclear
3. Although most clinicians are unlikely to consider use of race creates havoc for scientific, clinical, and health
overt client behaviors, attitudes, and values as genetically policy enterprises. For example, identifying groups as pos-
or biologically determined, their dependence on cultural sessing unsubstantiated genetic, biological, social, or be-
issues as explaining behaviors may pose specific difficul- havioral characteristics can encourage erroneous assump-
ties. On the one hand, some clinicians may be prone to tions and stereotyping with pseudobiopsychosocial
overgeneralizations, racial mythology, and stereotyping be- precision. We must take responsibility for not perpetuating
cause they overemphasize cultural influences at the ex- ethnocentric assumptions clothed as scientific questions
pense of within-group heterogeneity (e.g., Asians are non- (Gould, 1981).
verbal and avoid eye contact). On the other hand, other For psychologists, covertly accepting a de facto bio-
clinicians may decontextualize their clients by deempha- logical imperative about race in research designs, data
sizing race and culture as relevant by treating them as analyses, and clinical applications and using race as a
generic humans or individuals in diagnostics and treatment proxy for other variables may continue to distract research
planning. Thus, the task for clinicians is to appreciate the and clinical efforts away from relevant variables that may
importance of race and culture and to determine how racial be pivotal in understanding people within their social,
and cultural factors operate and influence the treatment cultural, genetic, and environmental contexts. Adequate
context and their clients mental health. consideration of psychological, social, cultural, and politi-
4. In the mental health treatment context, it is impor- cal variables is critical for the successful integration of
tant to examine social, political, and racial group status. social, behavioral, and genetic information for research and
Culture refers to the behavior patterns, symbols, institu- practice.
tions, attitudes, values, and human products of a group or At some level, the current state of genetic research
society. Minority group status is a persons or groups allows the study of diseases in enough detail to move
position or power status. For example, racially identified beyond the naturenurture debate. It is now clearer how
minority groups such as people of African descent may DNA is both inherited and environmentally responsive. In
have cultural behavioral patterns that reflect not only their many ways, a persons attitudes and behaviors orchestrate
culture but also a social and political reaction to a history the interplay between inherited and environmental changes
of prejudice and discrimination. Thus, clinicians must be on the genome. One way geneticists and social and behav-
informed of their clients and own overt and covert cul- ioral scientists can begin collaborating is to develop inter-
tural, social, and political histories that may augment or disciplinary approaches to the study of race and health. For
jeopardize therapeutic alliances especially in treatment instance, the National Institutes of Healths roadmap initi-
interactions. atives for research are examples of removing barriers to
5. In research as well as when applied in clinical interdisciplinary research by encouraging collaborations in
settings, race when used as a demographic variable is often the development and implementation of long- and short-
used as a proxy for presumed cultural or minority group term training programs and curriculum development that sup-
experiences. Clinicians will need to distinguish and more port interdisciplinary research training for investigators at all
precisely understand racial, cultural group experiences of career levels. These efforts include exploratory centers for
clients. interdisciplinary research; training for a new interdisciplinary
6. Clinicians should be aware that research on the research workforce; supplements for methodological innova-
effectiveness of treatments has been primarily conducted tions in the behavioral and social sciences; interdisciplinary

January 2005 American Psychologist 43


health research training in behavior, environment, and bi- Bhopal, R., & Donaldson, L. (1998). White European, Western, Cauca-
ology; and curriculum and methodological development in sian, or what? Inappropriate labeling in research on race, ethnicity, and
health. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 13031307.
interdisciplinary research (see http://nihroadmap.nih.gov/). Boyd, W. C. (1950). Genetics and the races of man: An introduction to
Forty years ago, Snow (1964) described an abyss modern physical anthropology. Boston: Little, Brown.
between the natural sciences and humanities (and social Burchard, E. G., Ziv, E., Coyle, N., Gomez, S. L., Tang, H., Karter, A. J.,
and behavioral sciences) as two cultures and bemoaned the et al. (2003). The importance of race and ethnic background in bio-
medical research and clinical practice. New England Journal of Medi-
inability of members of either to communicate effectively cine, 248, 1170 1175.
with one another. He also suggested that molecular biology Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experi-
could be the bridge between the two cultures because this mental designs for research. Chicago: Rand-McNally.
branch of science is likely to affect the way in which men Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P., & Piazza, A. (1994). The history and
[and women] think of themselves more profoundly than geography of human genes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Chakravarti, A. (2002). A compelling genetic hypothesis for a complex
any scientific advance since Darwinsand probably more disease: PRODH2/DGCR6 variation leads to schizophrenia suscepti-
so than Darwin (p. 74). What Snow may not have envi- bility. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99,
sioned was the extent to which genetics would provide 4755 4756.
tools to help address questions posed by social and behav- Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999).
Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ioral scientists. American Psychologist, 54, 805 816.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

To this end, we hope that more social and behavioral Collins, F. C. (2004, May 27). The case for a US prospective cohort study
scientists will take up the challenge and investigate the of genes and environment. Nature, 429, 475 477.
relationships among genes, populations, and behavior. Ge- Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design
netic differences exist among individuals, and small differ- and analysis issues for field settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Cooper, R. S., Kaufman, J. S., & Ward, R. (2003). Race and genomics.
ences can have large effects on phenotypes and disease New England Journal of Medicine, 348, 1166 1170.
risk. Additionally, populations differ in the frequencies of Critchley, J. A., & Capewell, S. (2003). Prospective cohort studies of
many variants, and these can interact with environmental coronary heart disease in the UK: A systematic review of past, present
variables to contribute to differences in disease prevalence. and planned studies. Journal of Cardiovascular Risk, 10, 111119.
Crow, J. F. (2002, Winter). Unequal by nature: A geneticists perspective
Until population (and race) variables are more methodolog- on human differences. Daedalus, 81 88.
ically robust and used more consistently, social, behavioral, Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex.
and genetic research will be limited in answering questions London: J. Murray.
related to biological and psychological differences, treat- Dawber, T. R., Meadors, G. F., & Moore, F. E. J. (1951). Epidemiological
ment efficiency, diagnostic bias, and service delivery. approaches to heart disease: The Framingham Study. American Journal
of Public Health, 41, 279 286.
Regardless of whether scientists believe race is real or Delgado, R. (Ed.). (1995). Critical race theoryThe cutting edge. Phil-
just skin deep, race is personally, socially, and politically adelphia: Temple University Press.
important to millions of people who have nothing to do Duster, T. (2003). Backdoor to eugenics (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
with our scientific enterprise. Thus, perhaps the biggest Evans, W. E., & Johnson, J. A. (2001). Pharmacogenomics: The inherited
basis for interindividual differences in drug response. Annual Review of
challenge for understanding genetics and race for psychol- Genomics and Human Genetics, 2, 9 39.
ogists will be to disentangle not just the correlations of race Exner, D. V., Dries, D. L., Domanski, M. J., & Cohen, J. N. (2001). Lesser
with genetic variation and environmental exposures but response of angiotensin-converting-enzyme inhibitor therapy in black
adding the psychological and social factors to understand as compared with white patients with left ventricular dysfunction. New
the complexity of health and disease processes. If improved England Journal of Medicine, 344, 13511377.
Foster, M. W., & Sharp, R. R. (2002). Race, ethnicity, and genomics:
health is the objective, efforts should be placed where the Social classifications as proxies of biological heterogeneity. Genome
problems really lie. Research, 12, 844 850.
Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton.
Gould, S. J. (1994, November). The geometer of race. Discover, 85 89.
REFERENCES Gray, J. R., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Neurobiology of intelligence:
Science and ethics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 471 480.
American Psychological Association. (2002). Guidelines on multicultural Guthrie, R. (1998). Even the rat was whiteA historical view of psychol-
education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for ogy (2nd ed.). Needham, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
psychologists. Washington, DC: Author. Guttmacher, A. E., & Collins, F. S. (2002). Genomic medicineA
primer. New England Journal of Medicine, 347, 15121520.
Arnason, E., Sigurgislason, H., & Benedikz, E. (2000). Genetic homoge-
Haga, S. B., & Venter, J. C. (2003, July 25). FDA races in wrong
neity of Icelanders: Fact or fiction? Nature Genetics, 25, 373374.
direction. Science, 301, 466.
Bamshad, M., Wooding, S., Salisbury, B. A., & Stephens, J. C. (2004). Hakonarson, H., Gulcher, J. R., & Stefansson, K. (2003). deCODE ge-
Deconstructing the relationship between genetics and race. Nature netics, Inc. Pharmacogenomics, 4, 209 215.
Reviews Genetics, 5, 598 609. Harpending, H. C., & Cochran, G. (2002). In our genes. Proceedings of
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderatormediator variable the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 10 12.
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and Heath, A. C., Todorov, A., Nelson, E. C., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K.,
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- & Martin, N. G. (2002). Gene environment interaction effects on
ogy, 51, 11731182. behavioral variation and risk of complex disorders: The example of
Betancourt, H., & Lopez, S. R. (1993). The study of culture, ethnicity, and alcoholism and other psychiatric disorders. Twin Research, 5, 30 37.
race in American psychology. American Psychologist, 48, 629 637. Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curveIntelligence and
Beutler, L. E., Brown, M. T., Crothers, L., Booker, K., & Seabrook, M. K. class structure in American life. New York: Free Press.
(1996). The dilemma of factitious demographic distinctions in psycho- Higginbotham, E. B. (1992). African-American womens history and
logical research. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 64, metalanguage of race. Signs: A Journal of Women in Culture and
892902. Society, 17, 251274.

44 January 2005 American Psychologist


Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statis- (2004). Large-scale integration of human genetic and physical maps.
tical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from Genome Research, 14, 1199 1205.
the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Con- Parra, E. J., Amado, R. C., Lambertucci, J. R., Rocha, J., Antunes, C. M.,
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599 610. & Pena, S. D. (2003). Color and genomic ancestry in Brazilians.
International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium. (2004, November Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 177182.
6). Initial sequencing and analysis of the human genome. Nature, 409, Phimister, E. G. (2003). Medicine and the racial divide. New England
860 921. Journal of Medicine, 348, 10811082.
Jinks, J. L., & Fulker, D. W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature.
genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of human New York: Viking.
behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 311349. Plomin, R., & Crabbe, J. (2000). DNA. Psychological Bulletin, 126,
Jones, J. M. (1997). Prejudice and racism. San Francisco: McGraw-Hill. 806 828.
Jorde, L. B., Watkins, W. S., Bamshad, M. J., Dixon, M. E., & Ricker, Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental
C. E. (2000). The distribution of human genetic diversity: A compari- influences on antisocial behavior. A meta-analysis of twin and adoption
son of mitochondrial, autosomal, and Y-chromosome data. American studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490 529.
Journal of Human Genetics, 66, 979 988. Risch, N., Burchard, E., Ziv, E., & Tang, H. (2002). Categorization of
Kannel, W. B. (2004). Hypertension as a risk factor: The Framingham humans in biomedical research: Genes, race, and disease. Genome
contribution. In W. H. Birkenhager, J. I. S. Robertson, & A. Zanchetti Biology, 3(7), 112.
(Eds.), Handbook of hypertension (Vol. 22, pp. 129 142). Philadelphia: Rosenberg, N. A., Prichard, J. K., Weber, J. L., Cann, H. M., Kidd, K. K.,
Zhlvotovsky, L. A., & Feldman, M. W. (2002, December 20). Genetic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Elsevier.
structure of human populations. Science, 298, 23812385.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

King, T. E. (2002). Racial disparities in clinical trials. New England


Journal of Medicine, 346, 1400 1402. Sankar, P., & Cho, M. K. (2002, November 15). Genetics: Toward a new
Kluger, A. N., Siegfried, A., & Ebstein, R. P. (2002). A meta-analysis of vocabulary of human genetic variation. Science, 298, 13371338.
the association between DRD4 polymorphism and novelty seeking. Sankar, P., Cho, M. K., Condit, C. M., Hunt, L. M., Koenig, B., Marshall,
Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 712717. P., et al. (2004). Genetic research and health disparities. Journal of the
Kraemer, H. C., Stice, R., Kazdin, A., Offord, D., & Kupfer, D. (2001). American Medical Association, 291, 29852989.
How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and inde- Schwartz, R. S. (2001). Racial profiling in medical research. New England
pendent, overlapping and proxy risk factors. American Journal of Journal of Medicine, 244, 13921393.
Psychiatry, 158, 848 856. Segurado, R. (2003). Genome scan meta-analysis of schizophrenia and
bipolar disorder, Part III: Bipolar disorder. The American Journal of
Krieger, N., Sidney, S., & Coakley, E. (1998). Racial discrimination and
Human Genetics, 73, 49 62.
skin color in the CARDIA study: Implications for public health re-
Smedley, A. (1999). Race in North America: Origin and evolution of a
search. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1308 1313.
worldview (2nd ed.). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Levin, D. T. (2000). Race as a visual feature: Using visual search and
Snow, C. P. (1964). The two cultures and a second look. New York:
perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the
Cambridge University Press.
cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse. New
General, 129, 559 574.
York: Wiley.
Lewontin, R. (1972). The apportionment of human diversity. Evolutionary Sue, S. (1999). Science, ethnicity, and biasWhere have we gone wrong?
Biology, 6, 381391. American Psychologist, 54, 1070 1077.
Li, S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity Takaki, R. (1994). A different mirrorA history of multicultural America.
across levels: The interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind New York: Little, Brown.
and behavior across the lifespan. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 171194. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1982). A statistical
Linnaeus, C. (1758). Systemae naturae [The system of nature] (10th ed.). methodology for analyzing data from a complex survey: The first
Stockholm, Sweden: Laurentii Salvii, Holmiae. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (DHHS Publication
Lucotte, G., & Hazout, S. (1995). Geographic and ethnic distribution of No. PHS 82-1366). Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and
the more frequent cystic fibrosis mutations in Europe show that a Human Services Public Health Service, Office of Health Research,
founder effect is apparent for several mutant alleles. Human Biology, Statistics, and Technology, National Center for Health Statistics.
67, 562576. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2001a). Mental health:
Malhotra, A. K., & Goldman, D. (1999). Benefits and pitfalls encountered Culture, race, and ethnicityA supplement to Mental health: A report
in psychiatric association studies. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 544 550. of the Surgeon General [Executive summary]. Rockville, MD: Author.
Marth, G., Schuler, G., Yeh, R., Davenport, R., Agarwala, R., Church, D., U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2001b). Mental health:
et al. (2003). Sequence variations in the public human genome data Culture, race, and ethnicityA supplement to Mental health: A report
reflect a bottlenecked population history. Proceedings of the National of the Surgeon General [Full report]. Rockville, MD: Author.
Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 376 381. Wilson, J. F., Weale, M. E., Smith, A. C., Gratrix, F., Fletcher, B.,
Mays, V. M., Ponce, N. A., Washington, D. L., & Cochran, S. D. (2003). Thomas, M. G., et al. (2001). Population genetic structure of variable
Classification of race and ethnicity: Implications for public health. drug response. Nature Genetics, 29, 265269.
Annual Review of Public Health, 24, 83110. Yee, A. H., Fairchild, H. H., Weizmann, F., & Wyatt, A. E. (1993).
McGough, J. T., McCracken, T., MacPhie, I. L., Francks, C., Fisher, S. E., Addressing psychologys problems with race. American Psychologist,
Cantor, R. M., et al. (2002). Genetic linkage of attention-deficit/hyper- 48, 11321140.
activity disorder (ADHD) on chromosome 16P13 in a region implicated Zuckerman, M. (1990). Some dubious premises in research and theory on
in autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 959 963. racial differencesScientific, social, and ethical issues. American Psy-
Nievergelt, C. M., Smith, D. W., Kohlenberg, J. B., & Schork, N. J. chologist, 45, 12971303.

January 2005 American Psychologist 45

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi