Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect
Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 57 67
www.elsevier.com/locate/comppsych

Changes of explicitly and implicitly measured self-esteem in the treatment


of major depression: Evidence for implicit self-esteem compensation
Ingo Wegener a,, Franziska Geiser a , Susanne Alfter a , Jan Mierke b , Katrin Imbierowicz a ,
Alexandra Kleiman a , Anne Sarah Koch a , Rupert Conrad a
a
Department of Psychosomatics, University Hospital Bonn, Sigmund-Freud-Str. 25, 53105 Bonn, Germany
b
(r)evolution GmbH, Heussallee 12, 53113 Bonn, Germany

Abstract

Background and objectives: Self-esteem has been claimed to be an important factor in the development and maintenance of depression.
Whereas explicit self-esteem is usually reduced in depressed individuals, studies on implicitly measured self-esteem in depression exhibit a
more heterogeneous pattern of results, and the role of implicit self-esteem in depression is still ambiguous. Previous research on implicit self-
esteem compensation (ISEC) revealed that implicit self-esteem can mirror processes of self-esteem compensation under conditions that
threaten self-esteem. We assume that depressed individuals experience a permanent threat to their selves resulting in enduring processes of
ISEC. We hypothesize that ISEC as measured by implicit self-esteem will decrease when individuals recover from depression.
Methods: 45 patients with major depression received an integrative in-patient treatment in the Psychosomatic University Hospital Bonn,
Germany. Depression was measured by the depression score of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D). Self-esteem was
assessed explicitly using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) and implicitly by the Implicit Association Test (IAT) and the Name Letter
Test (NLT).
Results: As expected for a successful treatment of depression, depression scores declined during the eight weeks of treatment and explicit
self-esteem rose. In line with our hypothesis, both measures of implicit self-esteem decreased, indicating reduced processes of ISEC.
Limitations: It still remains unclear, under which conditions there is an overlap of measures of implicit and explicit self-esteem.
Conclusions: The results lend support to the concept of ISEC and demonstrate the relevance of implicit self-esteem and self-esteem
compensation for the understanding of depression.
2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction criticized for at least two reasons: First, participants have to


be motivated to disclose their genuine attitudes, e.g.,
Major depressive disorder is known to be the most individuals may give biased answers due to reasons of
prevalent mental disorder. About 5% of the population suffer social desirability. Second, they must also be able to access
from depression [1], while approximately one fifth develop the construct that they are asked about, e.g., some may
one or more episodes of major depression in their life [2]. misattribute emotions and thus cannot answer questions
Self-esteem has been claimed to be an important factor in aiming at the reasons of their emotions correctly. The
the development and maintenance of depression [3]. Studies implicit measurement of an attitude aims at overcoming
show self-esteem scores to be reduced in depressed these limitations by the following principles: (1) the
individuals [4,5]. The majority of these studies employ participant's awareness of the fact that this specific attitude
self-report questionnaires to measure self-esteem. However, is measured may be reduced, (2) the participant may not have
although widely used, self-report data have longtime been conscious access to the attitude in focus, and (3) the
participant's control over the outcome of the measurement
may be limited [6]. Thus, implicit measures might be less
Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 228 287 16299; fax: +49 228 287 biased by deliberate attempts to conceal the attitude and that
15382. they might even reflect attitudes of which the respondent is
E-mail address: i.wegener@gmx.de (I. Wegener). not aware (p. 401) [7].
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.12.001
0010-440X/ 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
58 I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767

Although measures of implicit self-esteem were repeatedly suicidal ideation was not controlled in other studies on
criticized to have only low to modest reliability and insufficient implicit self-esteem in depressed samples and as a
convergent validity [8,9], several studies obtained results consequence we do not know to what extent the results are
indicating that implicit self-esteem comprises information that affected by suicidal ideation. Gemar et al. [24] observed
goes beyond the information provided by explicit measures of higher implicit self esteem (IAT) in formerly depressed than
self-esteem [10,11]. Bosson et al. [8] concluded that among in never depressed and currently depressed participants, but
measures of implicit self-esteem, reliability was best for the after negative mood was induced, implicit self-esteem of
Implicit Association Test (IAT) [12] and the Name-Letter Test the formerly depressed dropped to the level of the never
(NLT) [13,14]. Subsequently, the majority of empirical research depressed and the currently depressed. This pattern of results
relies on these measures [9] (see method section for a detailed was replicated by Franck et al. [25]. However, Franck et al.
description of both measures). [26] observed no differences in implicit self-esteem using the
Dual process accounts to cognitive vulnerability to depres- NLT in currently, formerly, and never depressed individuals.
sion [15,16] suggest that two processing systems determine how Nevertheless, implicit self-esteem predicted future depres-
an event is interpreted. The automatic or implicit system sive symptomatology. Risch et al. [27] accounted for the
operates by automatically activating memory concepts in an number of depressive episodes and observed that implicit
effortless and unintentional manner and without charging self-esteem (IAT) was the same for never depressed and
cognitive resources. The deliberate or explicit system is remitted patients with recurrent depressive episodes, which
characterized by effortful, intentional resource consuming both had higher implicit self-esteem than first-onset
processing. Haeffel et al. [16] assume that life events first depressive patients and currently depressive patients with
trigger a rapid, automatic, and unintentional response that may recurrent depressive episodes.
activate negative self-schemas. In a second step this interpre- Taken together, the most frequent finding is that currently
tation may be reinterpreted by explicit processes. Whereas some depressed and non-depressed samples do not differ with
researchers presume that the main cause of cognitive vulnera- respect to implicit self-esteem [21,25,26], although implicit
bility lies within negative implicit self-schemas [17], other self-esteem has also been found to be reduced [27] or
investigators assume that explicit cognition can be the source of elevated [21] in currently depressed individuals compared to
vulnerability to depression [18]. non-depressed. Furthermore, implicit self-esteem can be
Haeffel et al. [16] tested the predictions of dual process moderated by suicidal ideation [23] and history of depression
models and observed in their first study, that only in depressed [27] or remitted [27,24,25] patients, but see Franck
participants with lower implicit self-worth (IAT) experi- et al. [26] for contrary results. Moreover, implicit self-esteem in
enced immediate emotional distress after a failure feedback. remitted patients is affected by mood induction [24,25]. Despite
In their second study, using a prospective design, they found the stable finding of reduced explicit self-esteem in depressed
that implicit self-worth (IAT) as well as explicit cognitive populations, reduced implicit self-esteem seems not reliably
styles interacted with negative life events in predicting later associated with clinical depression in cross-sectional studies.
depression. When entered simultaneously into a regression A possible explanation for these heterogeneous results may be
model, only explicit self-worth interacted significantly. The compensatory responses to self-threatening situations, as will be
authors argue that implicit self-worth affects immediate outlined in the following paragraphs.
distress, whereas explicit cognitions determine the long-term Based on research demonstrating that implicit self-esteem
risk to depression. Also supporting the role of explicit improves after participants have been confronted with self-
processing, Steinberg et al. [19] found implicit self-esteem as threatening situations, several authors proposed that self-
measured by IAT to predict depressiveness only for defensive processes can be triggered under conditions of
individuals with depressogenic cognitive style. threat to the self and that these processes can compensate
Taking a closer look at implicit self-esteem in depressed potential loss in self-esteem [10,28,29]. They argue that
individuals reveals a mixed picture (see DeHart et al. [20] for measures of implicit self-esteem are particularly sensitive to
a more detailed review): De Raedt et al. [21] observed these compensatory processes. The studies that focused on the
implicit self esteem in currently depressed individuals to be consequences of self-threatening situations in healthy individ-
as high as in non-depressed controls using the IAT (study 1) uals used different ways to manipulate self-threat. For example,
and the NLT (study 2). In study 3 they observed higher Jones et al. [29] observed enhanced implicit self-esteem after
implicit self-esteem for depressed compared to non- participants with high explicit self-esteem wrote about an aspect
depressed participants using the Extrinsic Affective Simon of themselves they wish to change but not when they were asked
Task (EAST, cf. De Houwer [22]). Accounting for suicidal to write about a positive aspect of themselves or a self-irrelevant
ideation in depressed individuals, Franck et al. [23] find topic. Other studies yielded analogous effects on the liking
lower implicit self-esteem (IAT) in a depressed sample of others with similar names [30] or choosing brands with
without suicidal ideation than in a non-depressed group as names resembling their own name [31] after participants had
well as in depressed individuals with suicidal ideation. performed a self-threatening writing task. Improved implicit
Implicit self-esteem of depressed with suicidal ideation self-esteem was also observed after negative life events [28] and
was as high as in the non-depressed group. Unfortunately, social rejection [10].
I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767 59

Rudman et al. [10] propose an automatic self-esteem consequence, ISEC may yield particularly high levels of
defense mechanism called implicit self-esteem compensation implicit self-esteem. Furthermore ISEC may also explain the
(ISEC) that automatically regulates self-esteem whenever the frequent finding that implicit self-esteem is generally
self is seriously threatened. They argue that given the high positive, even in samples with major depression. For
frequency of threats to the self in everyday life, the idea of a example the performance of currently depressed participants
process that buffers self-esteem would be compelling. They in the IAT reveals stronger associations for self-positive and
propose that ISEC is an automatic self-regulatory mecha- other-negative than for self-negative and other-positive
nism that countervails self-decrementing situations in an combined trials [21,27]. Also the NLT is positive for
effortless manner. This account may explain why implicit depressed individuals as they prefer letters from their own
self-esteem is not reduced in depressed samples as reviewed name over other letters [21,26]. Given the more negative self-
above. Based on the findings that healthy individuals employ schemas that are observed in depressed individuals, one may
this mechanism when confronted with ego-threats, it makes expect negative scores for these measures of implicit self-esteem.
sense to assume a similar mechanism in depressed The frequently observed positive implicit self-esteem in
individuals: depressed individuals reveal low explicit self- depressed samples may also be a consequence of ISEC, because
esteem [4,5], adverse self-schemas [3234] and overestimate depressed individuals are in particular need of processes that
the frequency of future life events [35]. They are prone to compensate the omnipresent threats to their self.
disadvantageous attribution styles [36] as they attribute In the present study we examine the role of implicit
negative life events internally but positive life events self-esteem compensation for depressed individuals by
externally (see Sweeney et al. [37] for a meta-analytic focusing on the change of explicit and implicit self-esteem
review of 104 studies) thereby experiencing more self- during the course of treatment of inpatients with major
threatening events [3840]. Given this daily struggle with depression. Because many studies demonstrated that explicit
threatening events on the one hand, and the results of self-esteem improved as a consequence of the successful
elevated implicit self-esteem after manipulations of threats to therapy of depression [4,5] we accordingly expect to
the self [10,29] on the other hand, we expect implicit replicate this pattern in the present study (hypothesis 1). If,
self-esteem compensation to be a most relevant process in in contrast to explicit self-esteem, implicit self-esteem
depression. ISEC may thus be a reason, why depressed compensation is protecting the self of depressed individuals
individuals reveal consistently reduced levels of explicit but against daily threats as suggested above, one would suppose
not implicit self-esteem as reviewed above. The heteroge- a particularly high implicit self-esteem at the beginning of
neous results concerning the relative extent of implicit treatment which, as protective processes decrease during
self-esteem of depressed in comparison to healthy individ- recovery, decreases as patients recover from depression. We
uals may be a consequence of ISEC protecting the depressed thus expect implicit self-esteem to decline during the course
self from adverse threats. Implicit self-esteem compensation of treatment (hypothesis 2).
may often reach or sometimes even prevail the level of
healthy controls. Within the dual process framework ISEC
can be understood as a mechanism stabilizing implicit 2. Method
self-esteem. In this conception implicit self-esteem may 2.1. Participants
fluctuate in response to self-threatening events and thus
exhibit a state component [28]. In dual process accounts for 51 consecutive inpatients of the Psychosomatic Univer-
the vulnerability to depression, implicit self-esteem is often sity Hospital Bonn, Germany, diagnosed with major
understood as rapid and automatic processing mode that depression participated in the present study. Main reason
develops over a long period [15] and is based on stable for hospitalization was limited functioning in everyday life
memory constructs [16]. However, there is evidence that that did not improve sufficiently during out-patient treat-
implicit self-esteem can be affected by classical conditioning ment. All had been diagnosed by an experienced psycho-
[41], induction of negative mood [24,25], daily negative therapist of the Psychosomatic University Hospital
events [28], or after threats to the self [10,2831]. DeHart Outpatient Unit in an approximately two hour diagnostic
and Pelham [28] argue that implicit self-esteem has trait as and anamnestic interview using DSM-IV-TR [42] diagnostic
well as state aspects and propose that implicit self-esteem has criteria. Included were participants aged from 20 to 65 years.
a trait level around which the state level can fluctuate. If Exclusion criteria were a comorbid psychotic episode or
depressed individuals possess a low level of implicit limited German language skills that were insufficient to fill
self-esteem, ISEC may represent such a fluctuation that in the questionnaires. However, no participants had to be
can elevate implicit self-esteem to a level observed in excluded. Patients were asked to complete the Implicit
non-depressed controls. This may explain, for example, why Association Test [12] on a computer and afterwards to fill in
depressed individuals with suicidal ideation, but not without several questionnaires for a first time during the first week
suicidal ideation, revealed higher implicit self-esteem than and for a second time during the last week of their inpatient
non-depressed [23]. Self-threat is presumably extremely treatment (see below). During the treatment, six patients
pronounced in individuals with suicidal ideation and, as a dropped out. In two cases, patients left hospital a few days
60 I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767

before the date of planned discharge due to organizational sensitivity, and specificity were sufficient [48]. In the present
reasons. In one case the treatment was discontinued because study Cronbach's of .79 for anxiety and .82 for depression
the patient with comorbid anorexia nervosa was not fulfilling were observed at the beginning of the treatment, whereas the
her weight gain contract for more than three weeks. One Cronbach's were .81 and .86 for anxiety and depression,
patient discontinued the treatment because of difficulties in respectively, at the end of the treatment.
emotion regulation when confronted with annoying behavior
of some other patient. One patient had somatic complications 2.2.3. Implicit Association Test (IAT)
and one patient discontinued her treatment when she realized The Implicit Association Test for self-esteem was
that a former coworker of her had been admitted to the developed by Greenwald and Farnham [12] as an implicit
treatment program. measure of self-esteem on the basis of the procedure of the
At the beginning of the treatment, there were no standard IAT [49]. In this computerized speed categorization
differences between drop outs and completers with respect task, the participants' task is to assign words of the categories
to age (t(50) = 0.02, p = .98), gender ( 2 (1) = 0.27, p = self versus other and positive versus negative by pressing
.61), depression measured by HADS-D score (t(49) = 1.77, one of two response keys. Usually participants perform
p = .08), explicit self-esteem (t(49) = 0.03, p = .98), or better in compatible trials, where self-related words (e.g.,
implicit self-esteem (t(50) = 0.28, p = .78 for the IAT score me, my) are mapped on the same key as positive words
and t(48) = 1.60, p = .12 for the Name Letter Test). Among (e.g., luck, pleasure) and other-related words (e.g., it,
the 45 participants who completed questionnaires at the the) on the same key as negative words (e.g., poison,
beginning and end of treatment there were 36 females (80%) betrayal). If self-related words are assigned to the same key
and 9 males (20%). Average age was 35.24 years (SD = as negative words and other-related words to the same key as
11.73), 27 (60%) had completed high school, seven (16%) positive words, participants perform worse (incompatible
had a university degree. At the entry of the treatment 13 trials). It is assumed that the faster reaction times for
(29%) received no psychotropic medication, 23 (51%) compatible than for incompatible trials reflect stronger
received antidepressants, 10 (22%) sedatives, and 4 (9%) associations for the materials mapped on the same keys in
antipsychotics. Psychiatric comorbidities were personality the compatible trials. If the self is associated with positive
disorders (n = 20), eating disorders (n = 15), anxiety rather than negative concepts, as one would expect in case of
disorders (n = 9), somatoform disorders (n = 4), and positive self-esteem, participants should answer faster if they
posttraumatic stress disorder (n = 1). are required to press the same key for me and pleasure
(compatible trial) than for me and betrayal (incompatible
2.2. Measures trial). In order to reduce the influence of training effects,
2.2.1. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) Greenwald [49] suggested using different kinds of blocks
Explicit self-esteem was measured using the German consisting of training trials. In the present study the trials
translation by von Collani and Herzberg [43] of the were presented in blocks of 48 trials each. It was balanced
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale [44]. Von Collani and between patients whether they had to work on compatible or
Herzberg criticized the psychometric properties of one item incompatible blocks first. The IAT procedure consisted of
of an earlier German version of the RSES [45]. They 672 trials grouped in five phases, including training blocks:
translated this item more closely to Rosenberg's English 1. Two blocks with self- and other-related words only, 2.
version and obtained an improved corrected item-total- Two blocks with positive and negative words only, 3. Four
correlation for this item. The German RSES comprises 10 items mixed blocks (these blocks were all compatible or
that are 4-point Likert-scaled from 0 (does not apply at all) to incompatible, depending on balancing condition), 4. Two
3 (applies completely). Von Collani and Herzberg observed blocks with self- and other-related words only, but with
Cronbach's of .84 and .85 in two independent samples. In switched key assignment, 5. Four mixed blocks using this
the present study, we observed a Cronbach's of .87 at the switched assignment. The dependent variable was calculated
beginning as well as at the end of treatment. from the two phases with mixed blocks (phases 3 and 5)
according to the algorithm suggested by Greenwald et al.
2.2.2. Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [50], whereas phases 1, 2, and 4 served as training blocks
Depressive and anxious symptomatology was assessed only. Stimulus words were presented in black letters on a
using the German version [46] of the Hospital Anxiety and light gray background in a rectangle of 20 mm 120 mm
Depression Scale [47]. Respondents are asked to indicate on a 17 inch display. Response stimulus interval was set to
how they have been feeling over the past week on a 4-point 250 ms. The response keys were to be pressed by different
Likert scale. The questionnaire consists of a 7-item subscale hands, and matching of stimulus categories to the dominant
measuring anxious and a 7-item subscale measuring versus non-dominant hand was counterbalanced across
depressive symptoms. The latter scale was used in the patients. Responses were accepted as soon as a stimulus
analyses reported in the present study. Based on a review of was visible.
747 studies using the HADS, it has been stated that the two The IAT is the most frequently used measure of implicitly
factor structure was supported by the data and that reliability, measured self-esteem [9]. Although studies on the IAT were
I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767 61

quite promising at the beginning [12,51], more recent anorexia nervosa additionally approved a weight gain
overviews compiled more mixed results [9,52]. contract and had to maintain a food diary. Similar integrative
treatment conditions are common in German psychosomatic
2.2.4. Name-Letter Test (NLT) hospitals and have repeatedly been shown to provide good
The Name-Letter Test [13,14] is another frequently used effectiveness [57,58]. Moreover, the specific treatment
task that aims at assessing self-esteem implicitly. The idea conditions at the Psychosomatic University Hospital Bonn
behind this measure is that a person's self-esteem affects the were demonstrated to be effective [59,60].
extent of liking the letters of one's own name. Indeed it is a
stable finding that people like their initials more than other 2.4. Procedure
letters [53]. Although it has been questioned if this
preference can serve as a measure of self-esteem [9,53], All patients participated during the first and the last week
there is evidence that NLT comprises information that goes of their in-patient treatment at the Psychosomatic University
beyond explicit self-esteem [28,54,55]. In the present study Hospital, Bonn, Germany, on a voluntary basis. Patients
participants were asked to evaluate all letters of the alphabet were first administered an Implicit Association Test for self
with regard to how much they like each letter of the alphabet esteem [12] on a computer and afterwards they were asked to
on a 9-point Likert scale. Letters were presented in fill in several questionnaires including Rosenberg Self-
randomized order that was generated for every patient and Esteem Scale [43] and the Name-Letter-Test [13,14]. The
for each time of measurement anew. To avoid artifacts sessions took about two hours at beginning as well as at end
because letters that are generally evaluated more favorable of treatment.
may be more frequently found as name letters, the evaluation
of a participant's name letter is usually corrected by 2.5. Statistical analysis
calculating the difference of this evaluation and the average
evaluation of this letter by all other participants that do not For the statistical analyses IBM SPSS version 20 for
have this letter in their name. However, Albers et al. [56] Windows was used. Alpha error level was set to .05. Power
argued and demonstrated that this score is still confounded analyses with g*power [61,62] for the matched t-tests used
with the general liking of letters of the participant. They below with a sample size of 45 to test for medium effects
propose an alternative name-letter score that is calculated as resulted in a power of (1 ) = .95. Because one patient did
the average evaluation of someone's names letters minus the not fill in the HADS properly at the end of treatment and one
weighted average evaluation of letters that are not part of the other patient did not complete the ratings for the NLT at the
person's name and the weighted average evaluation of the end of treatment, the sample size is accordingly diminished
same letters rated by participants not possessing these letters for the statistical tests on these variables.
in their names. Albers et al. [56] found significant
correlations of this score with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem 2.6. Ethical statement
Scale and with internal but not external narcissism. In the All patients participated on a voluntary basis and signed
present study, we use this improved scoring algorithm of the informed consent forms. Permission to conduct the study
Name-Letter Test. was granted by the ethics committee of the University of
2.3. Treatment conditions Bonn, Germany (Lfd.Nr. 175/09).

The mean duration of the inpatient treatment in the


present study was 55.69 days (SD = 10.76). Patients 3. Results
participated four times a week in a 1.5 hours psychodynamic
group therapy. In addition they received one individual Comparisons of explicitly and implicitly measured self-
psychodynamic therapy session and two 1.5 hours group esteem as well as depression at beginning and completion of
sessions of concentrative movement therapy every week. treatment are presented in Table 1. Differences were tested
Concentrative movement therapy aims at making biograph- by matched pairs t-tests revealing significantly increasing
ical material topic by using experiences emerging from explicit self-esteem, while implicit self-esteem and depres-
movement work. It is based on psychodynamic and Gestalt sion decrease during the course of treatment. Spearman's
principles. Furthermore, patients joined a weekly cognitive- Rho correlations between the two points of measurement
behavioral role-play group and a weekly psychodynamic art indicate comparably high stability of implicitly measured
therapy session lasting 1.5 hours each. Finally, they self-esteem, implying some extent of reliability inherent in
participated in progressive muscle relaxation groups twice these measures. Note that the means of IAT and NLT scores
a week. Four patients with panic disorder and/or agoraphobia are significantly larger than zero at both times of
and one patient with a specific phobia in addition joined a measurement (all p's b .001), indicating a positive implicit
manual based cognitive-behavioral therapy with ten indi- self-esteem. This means, e.g. for the Name Letter Test, that
vidual sessions including exposure therapy focusing panic patients evaluated the letters of their own name generally
symptoms and avoidant behavior. The four patients with more positive than did patients that did not have these letters
62 I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, matched pairs t-statistics, effect sizes, and correlations of depression, explicit, and implicit self-esteem.
Beginning of End of Comparisons of beginning vs. end
treatment treatment of treatment
Mean SD Mean SD T-value Cohen's d Spearman's Rho (t1-t2)
Explicitly measured depression
HADS-D 10.73 4.39 5.95 4.17 7.11 1.07 .42
Explicitly measured self-esteem
RSES 13.47 6.97 18.78 6.29 4.64 0.69 .30
Implicitly measured self-esteem
IAT 0.64 0.38 0.55 0.32 2.15 0.32 .62
NLT 2.84 0.98 1.95 0.93 6.04 0.91 .59
RSES = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; IAT = Implicit Association Test; NLT = Name Letter Test; HADS-D = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
depression score; t-tests were conducted with df = 43 for NLT and HADS-D and df = 44 for all other variables.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.

in their names, but this positive evaluation decreased across explicit self-esteem, involving higher cognitive processes
the course of treatment. [68,69]. Thus, self-esteem is built on (at least) two modes of
The Spearman's Rho correlations of the three measures of information processing [70,71]. Implicit associations with the self
self-esteem are presented in Table 2 for both points of are thought to be more primitive and develop earlier compared to
measurement separately. Neither the correlations of the two their explicit counterparts, being closely connected to early social
implicitly measured self-esteem scores nor the correlations interactions [8,54,67]. Obviously, cognitive restructuring during
of implicitly and explicitly measured self-esteem are psychotherapy primarily addresses reflective self-evaluation,
statistically significant. However, correlations of explicit which involves higher cognitive processes [7274] and is closely
self-esteem and depression scores reveal a strong negative associated with explicit self-esteem [68,69].
relationship mirroring that the more depressed individuals To shed more light on factors that affect explicit and
reported lower explicit self-esteem, whereas no significant implicit self-esteem, the present study examined the changes
correlations were observed for depression scores and of explicitly and implicitly measured self-esteem across the
implicitly measured self-esteem. course of an inpatient treatment in individuals with major
depression. This specific treatment has been demonstrated to
reduce symptom distress in depressed individuals [59,60]. In
4. Discussion line with these previous results, we observed significantly
decreasing HADS-D scores across the course of treatment in
Current psychological theory defines self-esteem as a the present study. Consequently, we were able to study
person's overall appraisal of his or her own worth. The changes in self-esteem over an eight week time interval
self-concept is what we think about the self; self-esteem, the with significantly decreasing depressiveness. We used the
positive or negative evaluation of the self, is how we feel about Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale as an explicit measure of self-
it (p. 107) [63]. Since psychological theory distinguishes esteem and, both, the Implicit Association Test for self-
between automatic and unconscious self-evaluation on the one esteem and the Name Letter Test as implicit measures of
hand and a conscious and more reflective self-evaluation on self-esteem. All three measures are most frequently used in
the other it is useful to distinguish between two different self-esteem research [9].
constituents of self-esteem [64]: firstly, implicit self-esteem As self-esteem is an important factor in the development
involving mainly automatic processes [6567] and secondly, and maintenance of depression [3], our first hypothesis stated

Table 2
Correlations of explicit and implicit self-esteem and depression scores.
Correlations at the beginning of treatment Correlations at the end of treatment
RSES IAT NLT HADS-D RSES IAT NLT HADS-D
RSES 1.00 .03 .18 .55 1.00 .03 .21 .63
IAT 1.00 .09 .13 1.00 .00 .10
NLT 1.00 .05 1.00 .19
HADS-D 1.00 1.00
RSES = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; IAT = Implicit Association Test; NLT = Name Letter Test; HADS-D = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
Depression Score.
p b .001.
I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767 63

that explicit self-esteem will rise during the successful reaction is not activated in any case, but only in cases, when
treatment of depression. In support of this hypothesis, we a perception of a serious self-threat makes it necessary. To
observed a significant increase of explicit self-esteem over put it in other words there is an individually specific
time. Furthermore, we argued that (1) increased implicit threshold, above which the initiation of ISEC takes place.
self-esteem has been observed after manipulations of threat The extent of subjectively experienced self-threat is
[10,29], which has been proposed to be a result of implicit determined by several factors:
self-esteem compensation [10] and (2) depressed individuals
are prone to dysfunctional self-schemas [32,33] and First, the external situation, which can be perceived as
attribution styles [36,37] which is why everyday life bears secure and supporting or challenging and hostile [75].
frequent threats to their self. Based on this reasoning we Thus, for example, at the beginning of a psychiatric
hypothesized that a reduction of depression during treatment inpatient treatment patients often experience therapeutic
is accompanied by declining processes of implicit self- circumstances as far more challenging and threatening as
esteem compensation. In line with this second hypothesis, in the further course of treatment.
both implicit measures of self-esteem decreased significantly Second, the global cognitive vulnerability of an individ-
across the course of treatment. ual, which is determined by the mental representation of
These results support the notion that ISEC may buffer explicit and implicit attitudes [76]. The implicit attitudes
depressed individuals' self-esteem. As reviewed above, are mainly automatic and affect driven, the explicit
depressed individuals reveal lower explicit, but equal attitudes belong to the rational system being deliberative
implicit self-esteem in comparison to non-depressed controls and duty driven [70,77,78]. Both systems interact and
[21,25,26], although also higher [21] or lower [27] implicit influence behavior to maintain the adequate level of
self-esteem has been observed. ISEC can account for these self-esteem. This highly individual level depends on the
unexpected and heterogeneous results as it may boost the stability of explicit self-esteem, the awareness of current,
state component of implicit self-esteem in depressed to a past or future threats to self-esteem, the management of
level comparable to non-depressed. Depending on the previous self-threats and the associated implicit affective
intensity of the threat as well as the level of depression, it experiences. The likelihood of feeling seriously chal-
may not reach or even pass by the level of implicit lenged will be greater if explicit self-esteem is fragile
self-esteem in non-depressed. For these studies demonstrat- [10,79,80], more threatening situations have been expe-
ing a dissociation of implicit and explicit self-esteem in rienced or are anticipated, and management has been
depressed but not in non-depressed using a cross-sectional unsuccessful.
design, one cannot rule out that sample characteristics not Third, the type of self-threat, which may be relevant or
directly linked to depression may be responsible for irrelevant for subjective self-schemas. For example, an
differences in depressed and non-depressed. Because the individual defining him- or herself mainly by academic
present study is first to demonstrate a dissociation of explicit performance will be more vulnerable to poor grades
and implicit self-esteem depressed patients in one single compared to an individual, defining him- or herself by
sample over time, we can exclude that differences in sample physical appearance [81].
characteristics are mainly responsible for the observed
dissociation. The complex interplay between these factors determines,
Note that both implicit scores were positive at the whether the critical threshold is reached and ISEC is
beginning and end of treatment, indicating a generally initiated. The results of Franck et al. [23] corroborate this
positive implicit self-esteem. In case of the IAT these model as depressed individuals with suicidal ideation, that
positive scores denote that patients made fewer mistakes we would expect to exhibit stronger ISEC, revealed higher
when positive and self-related words as well as negative and implicit self-esteem than those without suicidal ideation.
other-related words were combined than in conversed Furthermore, in remitted depressed patients [25], the past
assignments. For the Name Letter Test the positive values experience of low self esteem, poor self esteem management,
indicate that patients evaluated letters that are part of their and the anticipation of possible recurrence can enhance the
name more positive than do other patients that do not have likelihood of ISEC. On the other hand, the experience of
these letters in their name. These results are in line with three or more depressive episodes [27] may lead to a change
previous studies that found positive values of implicit of implicit appraisal, in the sense that even though several
self-esteem in IAT [2127] and NLT [21,26] and may also threatening situations have been experienced, repeated
be a consequence of ISEC. successful management makes it less threatening and
However, in the context of contradicting results of activation of ISEC less likely. The decrease of implicit
previous studies it is necessary to draw a more elaborate self-esteem after mood induction in formerly depressed
picture of the possible mechanisms of ISEC, which can be patients [25] can be explained by the fact that the recall of
located within the framework of dual process account. ISEC sad experiences is not synonymous with the recall of
is conceptualized as an automatic and unconscious reaction self-threatening experiences. Distraction by other experi-
to severe challenges to self-esteem [10]. That means, this ences may disengage ISEC.
64 I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767

Our findings can also be seen within the framework of the results of meta-analyses that find very small to non-
dual-process account without the assumption of implicit existent correlations between implicit and explicit measures
self-esteem compensation. Dual-process accounts usually of self-esteem as well as between different measures of
assume that explicit processing is resource consuming, but implicit self-esteem [8,9].
that the processing rules can be learned relatively fast, After all, the strong negative correlation of explicit
whereas implicit processing is effortless but has to be self-esteem with depression corroborated the validity of the
acquired via repetitive experiences [15,16,77]. While Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Although we did not find
research has focused on the implicit and explicit vulnerabil- significant correlations of implicit and explicit measures, we
ity to depression, little is known about the contributions of observed strong correlations across time for each of the
these processes in the treatment of and recovery from implicit measures which lends support to a reasonable extent
depression. If explicit strategies are mainly responsible for of reliability and stability. An alternative explanation for the
recovery from depression, explicit self-esteem should rise decreasing implicit self-esteem may be that reduced
during the treatment, while implicit self-esteem should rise if depression in our study did influence processing speed
a change in the implicit self-schema is responsible for which may have influenced the reaction times in the IAT.
improvements. In this line of reasoning, the present results However, this explanation cannot account for the results
can be interpreted as supporting the role of explicit of the Name-Letter Task.
processing in the recovery from depression, because explicit, In conclusion it is rather unlikely that implicit and explicit
but not implicit self-esteem is rising across the course of measures mirror the same aspects of self-esteem, because our
therapy. If changes in implicit processing indeed require data showed a clear dissociation of both measures. If indeed
repetitive experiences, the present results suggest that much implicit measures of self-esteem are affected by processes
more experiences have to be made in order to improve of self-esteem compensation, one cannot expect a strong
implicit self-schemata than our eight week treatment could correlation with explicit self-esteem. An individual with high
provide. Still, in the present study, implicit self-esteem did self-esteem in a situation that does not threaten the self
not only fail to increase, but it decreased significantly. Such will reveal high explicit but low implicit self-esteem. If
impairments in implicit processing may be a consequence of this individual is exposed to a self-threatening situation, the
changes in well-learned implicit memory structures. Indeed threat to self-esteem will automatically be compensated for,
it has been demonstrated that learning changed implicit and high implicit and high explicit self-esteem will be
representations usually leads to reduced output [82,83]. As observed. Despite the dissociation of implicit and explicit
an example, imagine changes to a well learned mostly measures of self-esteem, we do not assume that both
automatic and effortless process like driving a car. If you are procedures are affected by completely disjunct processes,
used to drive a car with manual gear shift, changing to a car because some studies find significant correlations between
with automatic gearbox may temporarily cause difficulties, both approaches. Moreover, explicit measures of self-esteem
deceleration, and mistakes, although an automatic gear have been demonstrated to increase under conditions of
system may be regarded as easier to learn for a beginner. threat [8486], but also the reversed pattern was observed
Likewise, a depressed individual that makes experiences of [87,88]. There seems to be a considerable overlap of, but also
self-efficacy due to therapeutic interventions may have to significant differences between implicit and explicit measures of
integrate this new information in a less favorable implicit self-esteem. More research is needed to specify, which processes
self-schema. This may cause a weakening of old represen- affect each of the two measures under which conditions.
tations in the beginning yielding impaired performance in A major limitation of the present work is the absence of a
tasks like the IAT or NLT. Thus one may expect that positive non-depressed control group. Hence we do not know
experiences can cause changes to a less positive implicit whether implicit and explicit self-esteem is higher, lower
self-concept thereby interfering with the well learned more or on an equal level compared to a healthy control group.
automatic processing until the changes have been consoli- Nevertheless, the dissociation of implicit and explicit
dated. In the light of this interpretation, it is primarily explicit measures of self-esteem remains a significant and valid
processing that is responsible for the immediate recovery during result, even without control group. A second limitation is
the treatment of depression, while implicit processing may that we did not use a measure of self-threat that would allow
possess relevance for later phases of stabilization. This to corroborate our assumption of elevated self-threat in our
alternative explanation may be regarded as more parsimonious depressed sample. However, it is very difficult to measure
because it does not require the assumption of self-defensive self-threat in all its significant facets, as it embraces relevant
needs or processes. On the other hand, this account has aspects not consciously retrievable. A third limitation is that
difficulties explaining positive implicit self esteem in depressed we failed to observe a significant correlation of the two
individuals, which has been found in several studies [21,2426]. measures of implicit self-esteem. Also other studies using
With regard to our findings, some methodological issues more than one measure of implicit self-esteem found that
should be discussed. Neither at the beginning nor at the end these different measures of implicit self-esteem were affected
of treatment any of the three measures of self-esteem similarly, although they have repeatedly been demonstrated
correlated significantly with each other. This complies with to be unrelated [42,89,90]. One may speculate that different
I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767 65

implicit measures of self-esteem tap different aspects of implicit [16] Haeffel GJ, Abramson LY, Brazy P, Shah J, Teachman B, Nosek B.
self-esteem or that methodological differences of the tasks (e.g., Explicit and implicit cognition: a preliminary test of a dual-process
theory of cognitive vulnerability. Behav Res Ther 2007;45:1155-67.
fast vs. slow evaluations in IAT vs. NLT) hinder correlations to [17] Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Hankin BL, Haeffel GJ, MacCoon DG,
reach significance, especially in smaller samples. Gibb BE. Cognitive vulnerability-stress models of depression in a
self-regulatory and psychobiological context. In: Gotlib IH, &
Hammen CL, editors. Handbook of depression. New York: Guilford;
2002. p. 268-94.
5. Conclusions [18] Scher CD, Ingram RE, Segal ZV. Cognitive reactivity and vulnera-
bility: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive
The findings underline the importance of implicit self- diatheses in unipolar depression. Clin Psychol Rev 2005;25:487-510.
[19] Steinberg JA, Karpinski A, Alloy LB. The exploration of implicit
esteem compensation in depressed patients as a relevant aspect of self-esteem in vulnerability-stress models of depression. Self
implicit mechanism for stabilization that may undergo Identity 2007;6:101-17.
specific changes during treatment. Within the dual process [20] DeHart T, Pea R, Tennen H. The development of explicit and implicit
account for cognitive vulnerability in depression, ISEC can self-esteem and their role in psychological adjustment. In: Zeigler-Hill
explain previously not well understood findings such as a V, editor. Self-esteem: current issues in social psychology. New York:
Psychology Press; 2013. p. 99-123.
higher implicit self-esteem in depressed patients. Measures [21] De Raedt R, Schacht R, Franck E, De Houwer J. Self-esteem and
of implicit self-esteem may help to gain more insight into depression revisited: implicit positive self-esteem in depressed
these processes of protecting self-esteem, thereby hopefully patients? Behav Res Ther 2006;44:1017-28.
paving the way to optimize the treatment of depression. [22] De Houwer J. The extrinsic affective Simon task. Exp Psychol
2003;50:77-85.
[23] Franck E, De Raedt R, Dereu M, Van den Abbeele D. Implicit and
explicit self-esteem in currently depressed individuals with and without
suicidal ideation. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2007;38:75-85.
References [24] Gemar MC, Segal ZV, Sagrati S, Kennedy SJ. Mood-induced changes
on the Implicit Association Test in recovered depressed patients.
[1] Murphy JM, Laird NM, Monson RR, Sobol AM, Leighton AH. A 40- J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110:282-9.
year perspective on the prevalence of depression: The Stirling County [25] Franck E, De Raedt R, De Houwer J. Activation of latent self-schemas
Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:209-15. as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: the potential role of
[2] Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters implicit self-esteem. Cogn Emot 2008;22:1588-99.
EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV [26] Franck E, De Raedt R, De Houwer J. Implicit but not explicit self-
disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen esteem predicts future depressive symptomatology. Behav Res Ther
Psychiatry 2005;62:593-602. 2007;45:2448-55.
[3] Sowislo JF, Orth U. Does low self-esteem predict depression and [27] Risch AK, Buba A, Birk U, Morina N, Steffens MC, Stangier U.
anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull Implicit self-esteem in recurrently depressed patients. J Behav Ther
2013;139:213-40. Exp Psychiatry 2010;41:100-206.
[4] Battle J. Relationship between self-esteem and depression. Psychol [28] DeHart T, Pelham BW. Fluctuations in state implicit self-esteem in
Rep 1978;42:745-6. response to daily negative events. J Exp Soc Psychol 2007;43:157-65.
[5] Orth U, Robins RW. Understanding the link between low self-esteem [29] Jones JT, Pelham BW, Mirenberg MC, Hetts JJ. Name letter
and depression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22:455-60. preferences are not merely mere exposure: implicit egotism as self-
[6] De Houwer J. What are implicit measures and why are we using them? regulation. J Exp Soc Psychol 2002;38:170-7.
In: Wiers RW, & Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of implicit cognition [30] Jones JT, Pelham BW, Carvallo M, Mirenberg MC. How do I love
and addiction. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2006. p. 11-28. thee? Let me count the Js: implicit egotism and interpersonal attraction.
[7] De Houwer J, De Bruycker E. The implicit association test outperforms J Pers Soc Psychol 2004;87:665-83.
the extrinsic affective Simon task as an implicit measure of inter- [31] Brendl CM, Chattopadhyay A, Pelham BW, Carvallo M. Name letter
individual differences in attitudes. Br J Soc Psychol 2010;46:401-21. branding: valence transfers when product specific needs are active.
[8] Bosson JK, Swann WB, Pennebaker JW. Stalking the perfect measure J Consum Res 2005;32:405-15.
of implicit self-esteem: the blind men and the elephant revisited? J Pers [32] Beck AT. Depression: clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects.
Soc Psychol 2000;79:631-49. New York: Harper & Row; 1967.
[9] Buhrmester MD, Blanton H, Swann Jr WB. Implicit self-esteem: [33] Beck AT. Cognitive models of depression. J Cogn Psychother 1987;1:5-37.
nature, measurement, and a new way forward. J Pers Soc Psychol [34] Halvorsen M, Wang CE, Richter J, Myrland I, Pedersen SK, Eisemann
2011;100:365-85. M, et al. Early maladaptive schemas, temperament and character traits
[10] Rudman LA, Dohn MC, Fairchild K. Implicit self-esteem compensa- in clinically depressed and previously depressed subjects. Clin Psychol
tion: automatic threat defense. J Pers Soc Psychol 2007;93:798-813. Psychother 2009;16:394-407.
[11] Spalding LR, Hardin CD. Unconscious unease and self-handicapping: [35] Strunk DR, Lopez HL, DeRubeis RJ. Depressive symptoms are
behavioral consequences of individual differences in implicit and associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events.
explicit self-esteem. Psychol Sci 1999;10:535-9. Behav Res Ther 2006;44:875-96.
[12] Greenwald AG, Farnham SD. Using the Implicit Association Test to [36] Abramson LY, Metalsky FI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: a
measure self-esteem and self-concept. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;79:1022-38. theory based subtype of depression. Psychol Rev 1989;96:358-72.
[13] Nuttin JM. Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter [37] Sweeney PD, Anderson K, Bailey S. Attributional style in depression a
effect. Eur J Soc Psychol 1985;15:353-61. meta-analytic review. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;50:974-91.
[14] Nuttin JM. Affective consequences of mere ownership: the name letter [38] Crocker J, Karpinski A, Quinn DM, Chase S. When grades determine
effect in twelve European languages. Eur J Soc Psychol 1987;17:381-402. self-worth: consequences of contingent self-worth for male and
[15] Beevers CG. Cognitive vulnerability to depression: a dual process female engineering and psychology majors. J Pers Soc Psychol
model. Clin Psychol Rev 2005;25:975-1002. 2003;85:507-16.
66 I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767

[39] Crocker J, Sommers S, Luhtanen R. Hopes dashed and dreams therapy outcome. A replicated and enhanced study. Psychother Res
fulfilled: contingencies of self-worth in the graduate school admissions 2005;15:357-65.
process. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2002;28:1275-86. [60] Wegener I, Alfter S, Geiser F, Liedtke R, Conrad R. Schema change
[40] Ingram RE, Ritter J. Vulnerability to depression: cognitive reactivity without schema therapy: the role of early maladaptive schemata for a
and parental bonding in high-risk individuals. J Abnorm Psychol successful treatment of major depression. Psychiatry 2013;76:1-17.
2000;109:588-96. [61] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses
[41] Dijksterhuis A. I like myself but I don't know why: enhancing implicit using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses.
self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. J Pers Soc Psychol Behav Res Methods 2009;41:1149-60.
2004;86:345-55. [62] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible
[42] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and
of Mental Disorders DSM-IV-TR. Washington: American Psychi- biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007;39:175-91.
atric Association; 2000. [63] Smith ER, Mackie DM. Social psychology. Philadelphia: Psychology
[43] von Collani G, Herzberg PY. Eine revidierte Fassung der deutsch- Press; 2007.
sprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefhl von Rosenberg (A revised [64] Zeigler-Hill V. Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-
version of the German adaptation of Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale). esteem: implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. J Pers
Z Diff Diagn Psychol 2003;24:3-7. 2006;74:119-44.
[44] Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: [65] Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-
Princeton University Press; 1965. esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev 1995;102:4-27.
[45] Ferring D, Filipp SH. Messung des Selbstwertgefhls: Befunde zu [66] Koole SL, Dijksterhuis A, van Knippenberg A. What's in a name:
Reliabilit, Validitt und Stabilit der Rosenberg- Skala. Diagnostica implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. J Pers Soc Psychol
1996;42:284-92. 2001;80:669-85.
[46] Herrmann C, Buss U, Snaith RP. HADS-D Hospital Anxiety and [67] Pelham BW, Hetts JJ. Implicit and explicit personal and social identity:
Depression Scale Deutsche Version: Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung toward a more complete understanding of the social self. In: Tyler T, &
von Angst und Depressivitt in der somatischen Medizin. Bern, Kramer R, editors. The psychology of the social self. Mahwah:
Switzerland: Huber; 1995. Lawrence Erlbaum; 1999. p. 115-43.
[47] Zigmond SA, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. [68] Brown JD. Self-esteem and self-evaluation: feeling is believing. In:
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;67:361-70. Suls J, editor. Psychological perspectives on the self, vol. 4. Hillsdale:
[48] Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the Erlbaum; 1993. p. 27-58.
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: an updated literature review. [69] Kernis MH. Toward a conceptualization of optimal self-esteem.
J Psychosom Res 2002;52:69-77. Psychol Inq 2003;14:1-26.
[49] Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK. Measuring individual [70] Smith ER, DeCoster J. Dual-process models in social and cognitive
differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. J Pers psychology: conceptual integration and links to underlying memory
Soc Psychol 1998;74:1464-80. systems. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2000;4:108-31.
[50] Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the [71] Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. A model of dual attitudes.
implicit association test: 1. An improved scoring algorithm. J Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2000;107:101-26.
Psychol 2003;85:197-216. [72] Garratt G, Ingram RE, Rand KL, Sawalani G. Cognitive processes in
[51] Greenwald AG, Banaji MR, Rudman LA, Farnham SD, Nosek BA, cognitive therapy: evaluation of the mechanisms of change in the
Mellott DS. A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self- treatment of depression. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2007;14:224-39.
esteem, and self-concept. Psychol Rev 2002;109:3-25. [73] Linden DE. How psychotherapy changes the brainthe contribution
[52] Hofmann W, Gawronski B, Gschwendner T, Le H, Schmitt M. A meta- of functional neuroimaging. Mol Psychiatry 2006;11:528-38.
analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and [74] Linden DE. Brain imaging and psychotherapy: methodological
explicit self-report measures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2005;31:1369-85. considerations and practical implications. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin
[53] Krizan Z, Suls J. Are implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem Neurosci 2008;258:71-5.
related? A meta-analysis for the NameLetter Test. Pers Individ Differ [75] Rudman LA. Sources of implicit attitudes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci
2008;44:521-31. 2004;13:80-3.
[54] Koole SL, Pelham BW. On the nature of implicit self-esteem: the case of [76] Rudman LA, Fairchild K. Reactions to counterstereotypic behavior:
the name letter effect. In: Spencer JS, Fein S, Zanna MP, & Olson JM, The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance. J Pers Soc
editors. Motivated social perception: the Ontario symposium, vol. 9. Psychol 2004;87:157-76.
Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. p. 93-116. [77] DeCoster J, Banner MJ, Smith ER, Semin GR. On the inexplicability
[55] Shimizu M, Pelham BW. The unconscious cost of good fortune: of the implicit. Differences in the information provided by implicit and
implicit and explicit self-esteem, positive life events, and health. explicit tests. Soc Cogn 2006;24:5-21.
Health Psychol 2004;23:101-5. [78] Smith ER, DeCoster J. Associative and rule-based processing: a
[56] Albers LW, Rotteveel M, Dijksterhuis AJ. Towards optimizing the connectionist interpretation of dual-process models. In: Chaiken S, &
Name Letter Test as a measure of implicit self-esteem. Self Identity Trope Y, editors. Dual process theories in social psychology. New
2009;8:69-77. York: Guilford; 1999. p. 323-36.
[57] Haase M, Frommer J, Franke GH, Hoffmann T, Schulze-Muetzel J, [79] Roberts JE, Monroe SM. A multidimensional model of self-esteem in
Jger S. From symptom relief to interpersonal change: treatment depression. Clin Psychol Rev 1994;14:161-81.
outcome and effectiveness in inpatient psychotherapy. Psychother Res [80] Roberts JE, Monroe SM. Vulnerable self-esteem and depressive
2008;18:615-24. symptoms: prospective findings comparing three alternative concep-
[58] Salzer S, Streeck U, Jaeger U, Masuhr O, Warwas J, Leichsenring F, et al. tualizations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992;62:804-12.
Der Zusammenhang von interpersonalen Subtypen und der Vernderung [81] Rudman LA, Goodwin SA. Gender differences in automatic ingroup
interpersonaler Probleme am Beispiel stationrer Psychotherapiepatien- bias: why do women like woman more than men like men. J Pers Soc
ten (The relationship between interpersonal subtypes and the modifica- Psychol 2004;87:494-509.
tion of interpersonal problems in psychotherapy patients). Z Psychosom [82] Ikier S, Yang L, Hasher L. Implicit proactive interference, age, and automatic
Med Psychother 2010;56:191-206. versus controlled retrieval strategies. Psychol Sci 2008;19:456-61.
[59] Geiser F, Imbierowicz K, Conrad R, Liedtke R. Turning against self [83] Kushner M, Cleeremans A, Reber A. Implicit detection of event
and its relation with symptom distress, interpersonal problems and interdependencies and a PDP model of the process. Proceedings of the
I. Wegener et al. / Comprehensive Psychiatry 58 (2015) 5767 67

13th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale: [87] Leary MR, Baumeister RF. The nature and function of self-esteem:
Erlbaum; 1991. p. 215-20. sociometer theory. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental
[84] Baumeister RE, Jones EE. When self-presentation is constrained by the social psychology, vol. 32. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000. p. 1-62.
target's knowledge: consistency and compensation. J Pers Soc Psychol [88] Williams KD, Cheung CKT, Choi W. Cyberostracism: effects of being
1978;36:608-18. ignored over the Internet. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;79:748-62.
[85] Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T. Compensatory self-inflation: a response to the [89] Baccus JR, Baldwin MW, Packer DJ. Increasing implicit self esteem
threat to self-regard of public failure. J Pers Soc Psychol 1985;49:273-80. through classical conditioning. Psychol Sci 2004;15:498-502.
[86] Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S, Arndt J, Schimel J. Why do [90] Pelham BW, Koole SL, Hardin CD, Hetts JJ, Seah E, DeHart T.
people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol Gender moderates the relation between implicit and explicit self-
Bull 2004;130:435-68. esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol 2005;41:84-9.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi