Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

Degree of competition at feeding differentially affects behavior and

performance of group-housed growing-finishing pigs


of different relative weights1

L. Georgsson2 and J. Svendsen

Department of Agricultural Biosystems and Technology,


Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, SE-230 53 Alnarp, Sweden

ABSTRACT: The effect of competition at feeding on part of their daily feed intake was consumed during
specific categories of growing-finishing pigs was studied the nighttime hours (P < 0.001) when only one feeder
in order to provide an explanation for the increased was available rather than two feeders. The large pigs
variation in performance within pens previously seen ate more during the restricted period (P < 0.01) and
in highly competitive feeding environments. Individual had a lower carcass meat percentage (P < 0.05) in the
feed intake and feeding behavior, performance, and pens with one feeder. Some differences in feeding be-
health were compared for relatively small, medium, and havior between the two levels of competition were seen
large pigs in pens of 16. Pens contained either one or for all categories of pigs. No difference in health was
two dry feeders during a period of ad libitum eating observed between pigs in the one- and two-feeder pens
followed by a period of restricted feeding. Computerized for any size category of pigs. In conclusion, the inability
feeders that registered the time and amount of feed of the small pigs to get access to feed in combination
consumed by each individual were used. Seven replicate with overeating by the largest individuals caused the
groups on each of the two treatments (a total of 224 variation in performance seen within pens with a high
pigs) were studied. In the pens with only one feeder, level of competition at feeding (one feeder for 16 pigs).
the small pigs ate less (P < 0.05) and tended to have a From a welfare point of view, feeding systems causing
lower daily weight gain (P < 0.10) than the small pigs a high level of competition may be detrimental when
in pens with two feeders. However, they had a better considering all individual pigs in pens, even when it is
feed conversion efficiency (P < 0.05). The small pigs also possible to achieve acceptable production results on
had a different eating pattern. A considerably larger average.

Key Words: Competitive Ability, Feed Conversion Efficiency, Feed Intake, Feeding Behavior, Pigs

2002 American Society of Animal Science. All rights reserved. J. Anim. Sci. 2002. 80:376383

Introduction would be acceptable. Other studies of the number of


pigs per feeder space have also failed to demonstrate
In 1992 the Danish National Committee for Pig Pro- any effects on the performance of the pigs (Hansen et
duction published a report (Nielsen, 1992) in which they al., 1982; Nielsen et al., 1995). However, recent studies
found that there were no negative effects on perfor- (Botermans and Svendsen, 2000; Georgsson and Svend-
mance of having only one feeder, compared to three, sen, 2001) have demonstrated that a highly competitive
for 14 growing-finishing pigs. It was speculated that feeding environment (one feeder for a group of 16 pigs)
even a slightly higher number of pigs per feeding place causes a large variation in daily weight gain and carcass
meat percentage within growing-finishing pigs. These
effects can, without considering the difference in the
1
This work was supported by the Swedish Council for Forestry average daily weight gain, make it difficult for efficient
and Agricultural Research and the South of Sweden Agricultural pig production due to the resulting prolonged emptying
Research Programme. The authors would like to thank Torbjorn Hell-
strom and Mats Olsson for technical assistance and excellent care of time of the units. Animal welfare may also be a concern;
the pigs. The authors would also like to thank the staff at INSENTEC the results of Botermans et al. (2000a) indicated more
(Marknesse, The Netherlands) for technical support whenever skin injuries and more forced withdrawals from the
needed. feeder in the highly competitive environment.
2
Correspondence: P.O. Box 59 (phone: +46 40 415113; fax: +46 40
464241; E-mail: lotta.georgsson@jbt.slu.se).
In the present study, the effect of competition on
Received April 19, 2001. specific individual group-housed growing-finishing pigs
Accepted September 14, 2001. was studied to determine the reason for the increased

376
Competition affects pigs differentially 377
variation previously observed (Botermans and Svend- space feeders per pen for a group of 16 pigs. Seven
sen, 2000; Georgsson and Svendsen, 2001). The perfor- replicates were carried out in sequence, each with one
mance, feed intake, feeding behavior, and health of pen equipped with a single computerized feeder and
small, medium, and large pigs were compared between one pen with two computerized feeders (IVOG stations,
pens with one or two feeding places for 16 growing- Insentec). The feeders automatically registered time
finishing pigs. Individual feed intake and feeding be- and weight of the feed trough when a pig with an ear
havior were recorded using computerized feeding sta- transponder entered and exited the feeder. The gates
tions (IVOG system, Insentec, Marknesse, The Neth- of the feeders provided the eating pig with protection
erlands). for the head and neck. For a more detailed description
of the feeders see de Haer et al. (1992) and Georgsson
Materials and Methods and Svendsen (2001). In a previous paper we have re-
ported that using an IVOG station gave the same pro-
Animals and Management duction results, measured as daily weight gain and vari-
The investigation was carried out at Odarslov re- ation within pens, as conventional feeders did but with
search farm (Department of Agricultural Biosystems a slightly poorer feed conversion efficiency (Georgsson
and Technology, Swedish University of Agricultural and Svendsen, 2001). In the present study, data were
Sciences) in a closed herd of about 50 sows. All pigs collected every day from introduction until 3 to 6 d
were reared at the facility and were of mixed breeding before delivery to slaughter. The investigation was car-
([Yorkshire Swedish Landrace] Hampshire). All pigs ried out over a 3-yr period and pigs were introduced
had an individual identity assigned from birth. Piglets during all seasons. The feeders were placed in a corner
with undocked tails were reared as intact litters and of the pen such that the eating pig and the activity
weaned at 5 wk of age. The male piglets were castrated around the feeders did not disturb the pigs in the resting
before 2 wk of age. At 12 wk of age (approximately area of the pen. In the pen with two feeders the feeders
25 kg live weight), they were moved into the growing- were placed side by side, resulting in a distance of about
finishing unit and mixed to form groups of 16 with 70 cm between the feed troughs. This distance was
a maximum within-pen variation in weight, sex, and enough to prevent one pig from occupying both feeders
genetic background (SD in body weight at introduction at the same time.
was approximately 5 kg). The sex distribution was the At introduction, the 16 pigs in each pen were assigned
same in both systems and for all weight categories. The a category for statistical purposes according to relative
pens consisted of a solid lying area with an adjustable initial weight so that the five lightest pigs in a pen
area between 0.33 m2 and 0.57 m2 per pig and a slatted formed the group of small pigs, the five heaviest formed
dunging area of 0.45 m2 per pig, resulting in a total the group of large pigs, and the six remaining pigs
area per pig between 0.78 m2 and 1.02 m2. The size of consequently formed the group of medium-sized pigs.
the pen was adjusted to the size of the pigs in two steps They did not change groups during the study under
during the growing-finishing period. The adjustments any circumstances.
were made approximately 2 wk after introduction and
at the onset of restricted feeding around d 40 after Observations
introduction and were made at the same time for all
pens. The lying area was cleaned daily, if necessary, The computerized feeders automatically registered
and about 2 kg of chopped straw was provided daily on the feed intake of each individual pig (de Haer et al.,
the lying area of the pen. The building had windows 1992). Obviously false data were corrected manually.
allowing natural daylight. Artificial light was used dur- When false registrations were made (e.g., when the pigs
ing maintenance hours, weighing, and other handling put a foot in the trough or hit the trough too hard in
of the pigs. For further details regarding housing see attempts to get access to the feed), the feed intake for
Georgsson and Svendsen (2001). that particular visit was corrected by subtracting the
The growing-finishing pigs were fed dry, crushed pel- weight of the feed trough at the beginning of the suc-
lets (crumbs) of a standard commercial diet (Slaktsvins- ceeding visit from the weight of the feed trough at the
foder, Singel pelletskross, Lantmannen, Sweden; con- end of the preceding visit. In cases in which it was
tent per kg: metabolizable energy, 12.4 MJ; crude pro- impossible to calculate the correct feed intake for a visit
tein, 146 g; and lysine, 8.4 g). They were given ad (due to a number of consecutive false recordings), the
libitum access to feed until the pigs had reached an average of the feed intake for the 2 d prior to the day
average live weight of 65 kg. After that the pigs were in question was used to make a reasonable estimation
fed restrictedly, on a pen basis, 2.65 kg/d per pig until of the feed intake. The feeders provided information
slaughter. The pigs had constant access to four drinking that enabled calculation of the following variables for
valves per pen, all located in the dunging area. each individual pig: number of visits per day (24 h),
daily feed intake (ADFI), amount of feed consumed per
Experimental Design
visit, daily eating time, length of time per visit, number
A total of 224 pigs were used in this experiment, of visits during the night (12 h, 2000 to 0800), feed
which consisted of two treatments, one or two single- intake during the night, eating time per night, percent-
378 Georgsson and Svendsen

age of feed consumed during the night, daily nonfeeding and five from the two-feeder pens, all in the first two
time in the feeder (time with the head in the feeder replicates. At most, three pigs from the same pen were
without feed consumption), number of nonfeeding visits removed. In seven cases the reason for removal was
(visits to the feeder without feed consumption), and tail-biting; four of these were the tail-biters (these were
eating rate (calculated from [total feed per day]/[total physically healthy pigs) and three were tail-bitten. The
daily eating time]). All the above variables were calcu- tail-biters and tail-bitten pigs were removed promptly
lated for each week for the individual pigs. These data to prevent the behavior from spreading within the pen
were then used to calculate the results for the ad libitum and to other pens. One pig in a two-feeder pen was
(growing) period and the period of restricted feeding excluded due to lack of data about initial weight.
(finishing) separately. There were significant interaction effects between
All pigs were weighed once a week and 3 to 6 d before initial weight group and treatment for most variables
delivery to slaughter. The final weight for the pigs in of the study. For ADG, ADFI, feed per visit, and percent-
all pens was calculated from the carcass weight at age of feed consumed at night the interaction was sig-
slaughter, using a dressing percentage of 74%. These nificant during both the ad libitum and the restricted
figures were used to calculate daily weight gain (ADG). feeding periods. For other variables, G/F, time per day,
For feed conversion efficiency (G/F)(g live weight gain/ feed at night, and visits at night, the interaction was
kg feed), the observed live weight at the last weighing significant for only one of the periods (see Table 1).
before slaughter was used because there were no obser- The performance of the small pigs in the pens was
vations of individual feed intake after that because the impaired when only one feeder was used for 16 pigs.
transponders were removed. The individual commer- The ADG of the small pigs in the one-feeder pens was
cial carcass meat percentage was measured using a 90 g less than that of the small pigs in the two-feeder
Hennessy grading probe and recorded at slaughter. pens (Figure 1). The difference was seen for both the
For each pen, performance was calculated for the ad libitum and the restricted feeding periods (P <
small, medium, and large pigs as a group based on the 0.10)(Table 2). The medium-sized pigs showed no differ-
data from the individual pigs for the growing period, the ence in ADG, but there was a tendency for the large
finishing period, and for the entire growing-finishing pigs to have a higher ADG in the one-feeder pens than
period. As for all animals in the research herd, any in the two-feeder pens.
health problem or other treatment was noted for each The small pigs in pens with one feeder also had a
individual (Svendsen et al., 1988). Any remark from significantly lower ADFI than the small pigs in the pens
the body and organ inspection at slaughter was also with two feeders. During the ad libitum period, the
noted for each individual pig (later called slaughter ADFI differed significantly between the animals in one-
notations). For each pen the percentage of animals in and two-feeder pens for the small pigs, whereas during
each of the weight groups with health problems or the restricted period the small pigs in the one-feeder
slaughter notations was calculated. pens only tended to have a lower ADFI. No differences
Statistics in feed intake could be detected for the medium-sized
pigs at the two levels of competition. The large pigs had
All performance and behavioral results were calcu- a significantly higher ADFI of 35 g/d more in the one-
lated as means for the respective weight groups in the feeder pens during restricted feeding, which resulted
pens. Interaction effects between weight group (small, in a 20-g higher ADFI for the entire growing-finishing
medium, or large) and treatment (one or two feeders) period even though there was no difference during the
for the variables measured in the study were tested ad libitum period.
using a split-plot model (with treatment as main plot The G/F was significantly better for the small pigs
and weight group as subplot) (Proc GLM of SAS; SAS with only one feeder for the whole period and during
Inst. Inc., Cary, NC). Because significant interaction the ad libitum period, whereas there was no difference
effects were found for most variables (Table 1), only during the period of restricted feeding between small
comparisons within each weight category were made pigs in pens with one or two feeders. No significant
in the further analysis. Analysis of variance was used differences in G/F were detected for the medium and
to detect treatment differences between pigs of the same large pigs.
weight category, where replicate was always used as a The carcass meat percentage for the large pigs was
block-effect in the model. For diseases and slaughter a whole percentage unit lower for the animals in the
notations, the data were nonnormal and Wilcoxons one-feeder pens, whereas the small pigs in the one
nonparametric test (SAS Inst. Inc.) was used to test feeder pens tended to have a higher meat percentage
differences between the two levels of competition for than those in the two-feeder pens (Figure 2).
each weight group. Differences were considered signifi- The feeding behavior of the individual pigs in the size
cant when P < 0.05.
categories was registered and is shown in Table 3 and
Results Figure 3. With two feeders, the number of feeding visits
increased significantly for all pigs except for the me-
There were eight pigs removed from the study (from dium-sized pigs during the ad libitum period. The
a total of 224 pigs), three from the one-feeder pens amount of feed consumed per visit was generally higher
Competition affects pigs differentially 379
Table 1. The P-values for the interaction effect of number of feeders (one or two) and
relative weight group in the pen (small, medium, or large)
on the variables of the present study
P-value for the interaction (no. of feeders weight group)a

Variable Ad libitum period Restricted period Total period

Initial weight 0.15 0.0012


Daily weight gain 0.0067 0.046 0.0049
Daily feed intake 0.0008 0.0031 0.0002
Feed conversion efficiency 0.016 0.60 0.055
Visits/day 0.47 0.68 0.47
Feed/visit 0.0005 0.0002 0.0001
Time/day 0.045 0.099 0.027
Time/visit 0.0002 0.0027 0.0001
Feed at night 0.58 0.0072 0.11
Percentage of feed intake at night 0.0001 0.0009 0.0002
Time at night 0.018 0.0002 0.0039
Visits at night 0.31 0.0006 0.046
Eating rate 0.28 0.20 0.36
Meat percentage 0.025
a
The interaction was tested in a split-plot design.

when only one feeder was available for the large pigs, when there were two feeders (12.7 vs 7.3 visits/d and
whereas the small and medium-sized pigs only in- 10.5 vs 4.6 visits/d for the medium and large pigs, re-
creased the amount of feed eaten per visit when they spectively).
were fed restrictedly. The amount of time spent eating No differences in the total number of health problems
per 24 h was greater for the small and medium-sized or any category of health problem (diarrhea, tail-biting,
pigs when they had access to two feeders. Conversely, lameness, muscle disease, or other disease) were ob-
the large pigs did not show any tendency to spend more served between the one- and two-feeder pens for any
time eating when there were two feeders available in of the size categories of pigs.
the pen. During the ad libitum period, the duration of In general, the number of slaughter notations was
each visit was longer only for the large pigs in the one- low compared to the average for the whole slaughter-
feeder pens; the small and medium-sized pigs had the house (about 8 vs 17 to 18%). No differences between
same visit duration in both treatments. During the re- the two levels of competition were detected for any of
stricted period, all categories of pigs had longer visits the weight categories. The most common notation was
where there was only one feeder. for arthritis or periarthritis, which was noted for about
When allowed to eat ad libitum, pigs in all categories 2.4% of the pigs.
consumed more feed during the night (2000 to 0800)
when only one feeder was available. The difference ap- Discussion
peared to be greatest for the small pigs (Figure 3). Obvi-
ously, the pigs were able to eat more during the night- In the present study, the feeding behavior and perfor-
time in the one-feeder pens than in the two-feeder pens mance of individual group-housed pigs was studied. An
when fed restrictedly because the troughs were emptied interaction effect between the weight group at introduc-
earlier in the two-feeder pens. The same pattern as for tion and number of feeders was found for most variables
nightly feed intake was observed for the time spent in the study. This showed that competition affects the
eating during the night. small and the large pigs differently, and it would there-
The rate at which the pigs consumed feed (eating fore not be suitable to study the effects of competition
rate) did not increase at all for the small or large pigs on pen means. Because of this interaction, the results
with increased competition. There was a tendency for for pigs of the same size category in two levels of feeding
the medium-sized pigs to have a higher eating rate with competition were compared. It was shown that the rela-
only one feeder. The nonfeeding time in the feeder, tively small pigs in pens with a high level of competition
during which the pig occupied the feeder without consumed less feed than the relatively small pigs in
eating, and the number of nonfeeding visits per day pens with a lower level of competition. This appeared
were the same at the two levels of competition for all to be the primary explanation for the greater variation
categories of pigs when they were allowed to eat ad in performance seen among pigs subjected to a high
libitum. Nonfeeding time in the feeder did not differ level of competition for feed (Hansen et al., 1982; Boter-
between the treatments during restricted feeding. The mans and Svendsen, 2000; Georgsson and Svendsen,
medium and large pigs did have a higher number of 2001). In addition, the relatively large pigs in pens with
nonfeeding visits per day during restricted feeding a high level of competition (access to one feeder) con-
380 Georgsson and Svendsen

pense of the smaller pigs in the pens (for a review on


social facilitation see Hansen et al., 1982). This resulted
in a numerically higher ADG for the large pigs in the
one-feeder pens but also a considerably poorer meat
percentage and numerically poorer feed conversion ef-
ficiency. One reason for overeating might be a desire
to show superior rank in the group (Hansen and Ha-
gels, 1980; Hansen et al., 1982). This phenomenon
should not be possible in pens with multiple feeding
places, in which one pig cannot guard all the available
feed. Ten pigs with one feeder should therefore not be
regarded to be the same situation as 20 pigs with two
feeders where one pig cannot occupy all the feeders.
In a study of group-housed sows (Brouns and Ed-
wards, 1994) it was found for sows eating ad libitum
that the low-ranking animals could achieve feed intake
comparable to that of higher-ranking sows by changing
their feeding strategy to eating smaller but more fre-
quent meals and spending a lot of time waiting for
access to the feed. However, with restricted feeding,
the low-ranking sows did not manage to get access to
the feed and thus gained less weight than the high-
ranking animals. In the present study, it was clear
that competition affected the feeding behavior of the
growing-finishing pigs in general, with the most evident
change seen among the smallest pigs in the pens. In
pens with two feeders all pigs ate a little less than
one-third of the ADFI during the 12 nighttime hours.
Furthermore, in the pens with only one feeder the large
pigs also ate about one-third of their feed at night. Thus,
it may be assumed that one-third can be considered as
a normal or a preferred level of nightly feed intake
under the circumstances of the present trial. This is in
good agreement with the diurnal pattern of feed intake
that has been demonstrated in group-housed pigs in
several investigations (de Haer and Merks, 1992; Young
and Lawrence, 1994; Botermans et al., 2000a) and in
individually housed pigs (de Haer and Merks, 1992;
Nielsen et al. 1996a). The much higher level of feed
Figure 1. The daily weight gain, daily feed intake, and
consumed during the night by the small pigs (more than
feed conversion efficiency for the three size categories of
half of their ADFI) must therefore be considered to be a
pigs in pens with one or two feeders for 16 pigs. Values
result of a forced change in their eating pattern. Despite
are averages over the whole growing-finishing period.
this change, the small pigs were unable to compensate
Error bars represent the SE. The P-values are for the com-
for the high level of competition. The small pigs reached
parison between the same size category at the two differ-
a point at which they could not cope with the situation
ent levels of competition.
by altering their behavior, which is a criterion for a
state of stress in the motivational theory of stress as
given by Jensen and Toates (1997).
sumed more feed than those experiencing a lower level Increased eating rate has been considered to be a
of competition (access to two feeders). The large pigs sign of social constraint (Nielsen, 1999). In a study such
in the present study probably had access to as much as ours, when the competition for feed increases a
feed as they pleased in both systems, because the feed higher rate of eating would be expected. Interestingly,
was restricted on pen basis and not individually. neither the large nor the small pigs showed any ten-
The assertion that the large pigs were free to eat as dency to have different eating rates in the two treat-
they preferred in pens with one feeder was supported ments. This would not be so surprising for the large
by the fact that they did not spend more time eating pigs because they were expected to have high positions
when provided with two feeders, in contrast to the small in the hierarchy and thereby had the privilege of eating
and medium-sized pigs. The large pigs were apparently in their preferred manner. For the small pigs, the result
stimulated by the competition to eat more at the ex- was a little more intriguing. One theory could be that
Competition affects pigs differentially 381
Table 2. Performance of growing-finishing pigs, categorized according to relative initial weight, during the periods
of eating ad libitum and restricted feeding in pens with one or two feeders for 16 pigs (means)
Ad libitum period (2868 kg) Restricted period (68110 kg)

Item 1 Feeder 2 Feeders SE P-value 1 Feeder 2 Feeders SE P-value

Small pigs
No. of pigs 34 32 34 32
No. of groups 7 7 7 7
Initial weight, kg 21.5 22.4 0.31 0.096 55.0 60.9 2.03 0.082
Daily weight gain, g/d 680 779 31 0.067 761 830 22 0.064
Daily feed intake, kg/d 1.55 1.98 0.10 0.025 2.29 2.62 0.10 0.066
Feed conversion efficiency, g/kg 439 392 7.8 0.0056 341 338 5.6 0.77
Medium-sized pigs
No. of pigs 41 42 41 42
No. of groups 7 7 7 7
Initial weight, kg 27.6 27.3 0.33 0.46 68.7 68.1 1.24 0.74
Daily weight gain, g/d 834 831 20 0.93 883 861 29 0.62
Daily feed intake, kg/d 2.12 2.14 0.067 0.85 2.77 2.80 0.077 0.84
Feed conversion efficiency, g/kg 394 390 6.1 0.65 306 323 6.5 0.12
Large pigs
No. of pigs 34 33 34 33
No. of groups 7 7 7 7
Initial weight, kg 34.3 33.6 0.53 0.38 78.7 75.4 1.29 0.12
Daily weight gain, g/d 899 862 23 0.30 940 872 34 0.21
Daily feed intake, kg/d 2.40 2.31 0.067 0.38 3.15 2.79 0.067 0.0086
Feed conversion efficiency, g/kg 370 373 9.9 0.81 303 318 10 0.36

small pigs in the treatment with two feeders ate at result in a higher output of enzymes from the exocrine
their maximal capacity and therefore were unable to pancreas. Frequent small meals have also been ob-
increase this rate when subjected to an even higher served to lead to a higher lean tissue content of the
level of competition. The tendency observed for the me- carcass (de Haer et al., 1993; Ramaekers et al., 1999).
dium-sized pigs to increase their eating rate when only In the present study only the large pigs had a higher
one feeder was present supported this theory. These meat percentage when the group had access to two
animals were put under a higher social pressure and feeders instead of one. To some extent, pigs changing
were able to respond to a certain extent. feeders within the same meal could explain the higher
As in the study by Nielsen et al. (1996b), a higher number of visits to the feeders observed in the two-
number of visits to the feeder was seen with less compe- feeder pens.
tition. This could be expected to lead to an effect on A common husbandry practice used by stockpersons
digestion; Botermans et al. (2000b) have demonstrated to cope with production systems that provide a highly
that many small meals instead of one large meal per day competitive environment for the pigs is to sort them at
introduction to form groups that are as homogenous as
possible. This is done to reduce the diverging effect of
the competition by avoiding the presence of a group of
small pigs in the pens. However, this procedure can
add another strain on the pigs. According to Rushen
(1987) a large variation in body size in a pen is prefera-
ble to a homogenous group in order to reduce fighting by
facilitating a stable, easily distinguishable hierarchy.
When dealing with conditions leading to high competi-
tion by making homogenous groups, we might be adding
another strain and creating an even more stressful en-
vironment for the pigs. Environmental stressors can
have additive negative effects on the animals (Hyun et
al., 1998), and thus, sorting pigs into homogenous
groups may not be a good management practice when
Figure 2. The carcass meat percentage of the three size there is competition to feed. It probably is better to
categories of pigs in pens with one or two feeders for 16 provide more feeding places.
pigs. Error bars represent the SE. The P-values are for In conclusion, previous reports have shown that a
the comparison between the same size category at the highly competitive environment results in a large varia-
two different levels of competition. tion in daily weight gain among the pigs in the pens.
382 Georgsson and Svendsen

Table 3. Feeding behavior of the three size categories of pigs in pens with 16 pigs and one or two dry feeders;
data (means and SE) are presented for the ad libitum and restricted periods separately
Ad libitum period (2868 kg) Restricted period (68110 kg)

Item 1 Feeder 2 Feeders SE P-value 1 Feeder 2 Feeders SE P-value

Small pigs
Visits/d, no. 38.7 52.6 2.32 0.0054 25.4 40.9 2.43 0.0041
Feed/visit, g 55.7 46.0 5.04 0.22 112 75.9 8.90 0.028
Time/d, min.s 71.4 92.3 4.51 0.023 57.4 64.2 1.46 0.038
Time/visit, s 134 113 12.0 0.25 153 104 11.3 0.022
Feed at night, g 784 581 34.1 0.0057 654 116 71.6 0.0018
Time at night, min.s 41.1 28.6 1.18 0.0006 24.1 5.26 2.27 0.0016
Visits at night, no. 12.3 12.8 0.583 0.54 7.01 4.30 0.778 0.049
Eating rate, g/min 22.4 22.4 0.978 0.96 41.0 41.6 1.98 0.84
Medium-sized pigs
Visits/d, no. 33.8 40.7 2.24 0.074 20.7 34.1 2.83 0.015
Feed/visit, g 83.1 67.2 5.68 0.096 161 99.2 12.4 0.013
Time/d, min.s 79.1 92.2 13.2 0.029 60.4 66.3 1.54 0.076
Time/visit, s 166 157 11.8 0.59 202 134 16.3 0.025
Feed at night, g 814 632 37.3 0.014 505 145 78.0 0.017
Time at night, min.s 35.1 29.3 1.13 0.016 15.3 5.34 2.19 0.023
Visits at night, no. 9.15 9.98 0.433 0.23 3.88 3.45 0.525 0.58
Eating rate, g/min 27.3 24.2 0.915 0.051 45.8 42.1 1.47 0.13
Large pigs
Visits/d, no. 25.0 34.7 1.98 0.013 15.8 28.6 2.36 0.088
Feed/visit, g 130 80.3 6.32 0.0014 265 118 20.9 0.026
Time/d, min.s 84.2 86.4 2.03 0.46 63.4 61.2 2.56 0.60
Time/visit, s 241 164 14.0 0.0080 305 152 19.0 0.0013
Feed at night, g 816 680 25.4 0.0091 402 150 42.1 0.0055
Time at night, min.s 31.4 26.5 0.52 0.0079 11.0 5.09 1.13 0.015
Visits at night, no. 6.37 9.07 0.520 0.011 2.55 3.50 0.402 0.14
Eating rate, g/min 29.0 27.5 0.744 0.19 48.5 44.4 1.81 0.16

The variation cannot be explained simply by the fact altering their feeding behavior but they are still not
that the small pigs in the pen eat less and therefore able to achieve the feed intake and performance of small
have a poor growth rate. The higher feed intake of the pigs in a less-competitive environment and their wel-
large pigs also plays a role. It is also clear that the fare is thereby compromised. To maximize performance
small pigs try to compensate for the competition by and animal welfare, feeding systems must be designed
to facilitate feed consumption for the low-ranking pigs
and prevent the high-ranking ones from excessively
occupying the feeder.

Implications

Because of the negative effects of highly competitive


feeding systems on the welfare and production of rela-
tively small pigs, these systems should be avoided. Poor
performance of a few pigs in a pen with a good average
production still results in long emptying times of the
buildings or sections and longer intervals between
batches, ultimately affecting production economics.
When pigs are fed restrictedly, a trough with enough
space for all pigs to eat at the same time is recom-
mended. When feeding pigs unrestrictedly, feeders can
Figure 3. The relative feed intake during night as a be a good alternative provided that there are not too
percentage of the feed intake over the whole 24 h during many pigs per feeding place.
the period of eating ad libitum for the three size categories
of pigs in pens with one or two feeders for 16 pigs. Error Literature Cited
bars represent the SE. The P-values are for the comparison
between the same size category at the two different levels Botermans, J. A. M., L. Georgsson, B. R. Westrom, A.-C. Olsson, and J.
of competition. Svendsen. 2000a. Effect of feeding environment on performance,
Competition affects pigs differentially 383
injuries, plasma cortisol and behaviour in growing-finishing Hyun, Y., M. Ellis, G. Riskowski, and R. W. Johnson. 1998. Growth
pigs: Studies on individual pigs housed in groups. Acta Agric. performance of pigs subjected to multiple concurrent environ-
Scand. 50:250262. mental stressors. J. Anim. Sci. 76:721727.
Botermans J. A. M., M. S. Hedemann, M. Sorhede-Winzell, C. Erlan- Jensen, P., and F. M. Toates. 1997. Stress as a state of motivational
son-Albertsson, J. Svendsen, L. Evilevitch, and S. G. Pierzynow- systems. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 53:145156.
ski. 2000b. The effect of feeding time (day versus night) and Nielsen, B. L. 1999. On the interpretation of feeding behaviour mea-
feeding frequency on pancreatic secretion in pigs. J. Anim. Phys- sures and the use of feeding rate as an indicator of social con-
iol. Anim. Nutr. 83:2435. straint. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 63:7991.
Nielsen, B. L., A. B. Lawrence, and C. T. Whittemore. 1995. Effects
Botermans, J. A. M., and J. Svendsen. 2000. Effect of feeding environ-
of group size on feeding behaviour, social behaviour and perfor-
ment on performance, injuries and behaviour in growing-finish-
mance of growing pigs using single-space feeders. Livest. Prod.
ing pigs: Group-based Studies. Acta Agric. Scand. 50:237249. Sci. 44:7385.
Brouns, F., and S. A. Edwards. 1994. Social rank and feeding behav- Nielsen, B. L., A. B. Lawrence, and C. T. Whittemore. 1996a. Effects
iour of group-housed sows fed competitively or ad libitum. Appl. of individual housing on the feeding behaviour of previously
Anim. Behav. Sci. 39:225235. group housed growing pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 47:149161.
de Haer, L. C. M., and J. W. M. Merks. 1992. Patterns of daily food Nielsen, B. L., A. B. Lawrence, and C. T. Whittemore. 1996b. Feeding
intake in growing pigs. Anim. Prod. 54:95104. behaviour of growing pigs using single or multi-space feeders.
de Haer, L. C. M., J. W. M. Merks, H. G. Kooper, G. A. J. Buiting, Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 47:235246.
and J. A. van Hattum. 1992. A note on the IVOG-stations: A Nielsen, N-P. 1992. Enkeltdyrsautomater og simple trfoderauto-
station to record the individual food intake of group-housed mater til slagtesvin. Den rullande afprvning. No. 225. Danske
growing pigs. Anim. Prod. 54:160162. Slagterier, Copenhagen, Denmark [in Danish].
de Haer, L. C. M., P. Luiting, and H. L. M. Aarts. 1993. Relations Ramaekers, P. J. L., J. W. G. M. Swinkels, J. H. Huiskes, and M. W.
among individual (residual) feed intake, growth performance A. Verstegen. 1999. Eating traits in relation to performance and
and feed intake pattern of growing pigs in group housing. Livest. carcass traits of restrictedly fed group-housed finishing pigs.
Prod. Sci. 36:233253. Anim. Sci. 68:399405.
Rushen, J. 1987. A difference in weight reduces fighting when unac-
Georgsson, L., and J. Svendsen. 2001. One or two feeders for groups
quainted newly weaned pigs first meet. Can. J. Anim. Sci.
of 16 growing-finishing pigseffects on health and production.
67:951960.
Acta Agric. Scand. (In press). Svendsen, J., A.-C. Olsson, and D. Rantzer. 1988. Productivity and
Hansen, L. L., and A. M. Hagels. 1980. A general survey of environ- the occurrence of disease through to slaughter in pigs with and
mental influence on the social hierarchy function in pigs. Acta without reduced vitality or physical handicap at birth. Rep. 62.
Agric. Scand. 30:388392. Swed. Univ. Agric. Sci., Dept. Farm Buildings, Lund, Sweden.
Hansen, L. L., A. M. Hagels, and A. Madsen. 1982. Behavioural Young, R. J., and A. B. Lawrence. 1994. Feeding behaviour of pigs
results and performance of bacon pigs fed ad libitum from one in groups monitored by a computerized feeding system. Anim.
or several self-feeders. Appl. Anim. Ethology 8:307333. Prod. 58:145152.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi