Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Journal of Applied Psychology

1978, Vol. 63, No. 1, 29-39

Motivation, Behavior, and Performance: A Closer Examination


of Goal Setting and Monetary Incentives
James R. Terborg and Howard E. Miller
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

Some experimenters often test predictions from theories of motivation using


performance outcomes as dependent variables. We argue that observable be-
haviors that are likely to be affected by motivation manipulations should be
used in combination with performance outcomes. Such procedures would be
sensitive to differential effects of manipulations on various behaviors and would
allow for investigation of relationships among behaviors and performance out-
comes. For this experiment, 60 males were hired to work individually on a
2-hour construction task. Subjects were assigned to one of two pay conditions
(piece-rate vs. hourly) and one of three goal-setting conditions (no goal, quan-
tity, or quality) resulting in a 2 X 3 crossed analysis of variance design. De-
pendent variables included three measures of effort, three measures of direction
of behavior, and both quantity and quality performance. Method of payment
affected quantity performance and effort. Goal setting affected quantity and
quality performance and direction of behavior. Implications for designing and
testing work motivation systems are discussed.

Motivation cannot be measured directly, Terborg, 1977). Although these other vari-
but is inferred from the arousal, amplitude, ables could be controlled experimentally or
persistence, and direction of behavior (Birch statistically, confounds may still exist. For ex-
& Veroff, 1966; Campbell, Dunnette, Lawler, ample, a batter may be able to hit the ball
& Weick, 1970; Campbell & Pritchard, 1976). hard with every attempt, but the velocity of
Examination of past research on employee the ball may be weakly related to an evalu-
motivation, however, shows a reliance on per- ated outcome such as a home run or a double
formance outcomes as surrogate measures of play (McGrath, 1976). Similarly, amplitude
motivation. of behavior may be weakly related to some
There are several problems with the use of measured performance outcome even when
performance outcomes as substitutes for be- ability is controlled. A third problem deals
havioral indices of motivation. First, per- with the complex relationships among specific
formance is multidimensional and the mea- behaviors emitted in response to some inde-
surement of one or two outcomes may incom- pendent variable and the links between these
pletely document any relationships that may behaviors and various performance outcomes.
exist. Second, performance is affected by such Hackman (1976) and Hackman and Morris
variables as ability and opportunity in addi- (1975) state that tasks vary on the degree to
tion motivation (Campbell & Pritchard, 1976; which changes in a behavior can bring about
changes in performance. Effort may be im-
portant on some tasks and direction may be
This study was supported by a grant from the Uni-
versity of Illinois Research Board. important on others, depending on critical
The authors are grateful to their supervisors (Dan task contingencies. Locke (1975) made a
Koys, Byron Matten, and Bill McFadden) and their similar point.
film raters (Pam Law and Becky White) for the long The choice, then, of dependent variables in
hours they spent working on this project.
Requests for -reprints should be sent to James R.
motivation research is especially important.
Terborg, Department of Psychology, University of The complexity of this choice is diagrammed
Illinois, Champaign, Illinois 61820. with an example in Figure 1. The example
Copyright 1978 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

29
30 JAMES R. TERBORG AND HOWARD E. MILLER

DOMAIN OF PERFORMANCE RELEVANT


DEPENDENT VARIABLES : OUTCOMES:
SELECTED BEHAVIORS SELECTED OUTCOMES

QUANTITY

EFFORT
MANIPULATION OF
QUALITY
THEORY SPECIFIC
INDEPENDENT QUANTITY
VARIABLES
1
DIRECTION
QUALITY

INDIVIDUAL CRITICAL
RESPONSE TASK
CAPACITY CONTINGENCIES

Figure 1. Hypothetical example of relationships among independent variables, motivation de-


pendent variables, and performance outcomes (Heavy lines indicate causal links).

describes a hypothetical set of relationships dependent variables and performance out-


among motivation independent variables, vol- comes regardless of choice and manipulation
untary behaviors that are used to infer mo- of independent variables.
tivation, and performance outcomes, which The research described below was designed
may result from those behaviors. First, the with these issues in mind. The intent was to
independent variable(s) specific to a theory examine behavioral and performance conse-
must be manipulated or measured. Second, quences associated with two widely used mo-
not every independent variable influences tivational theories/techniques: goal setting
every dependent variable with equal effect. and the use of money as a performance-con-
Third, appropriate performance outcomes tingent reward.
must be selected and measured. Fourth, de-
pending on existing critical task contingencies, Goal Setting
each motivation dependent variable will not
have equal effect on each performance out- Whether goal setting is a motivation theory
come. or technique (Locke, 1975), the performance
Turning to Figure 1, had a researcher results are clear. The implementation of dif-
chosen to measure only effort and quantity, a ficult and specific goals leads to performance
conclusion of no support would be reached. increments (Latham & Yukl, 1975; Locke,
Although effort could be correlated with 1975; Steers & Porter, 1974). These review-
quantity, neither would be affected through ers also state that future research is needed
manipulation of the independent variable. A that specifies how task goals become trans-
main effect might be found if quality had lated into effective performance. Both Locke
been measured, but there would be little co- (1975) and Latham and Yukl (1975) suggest
variation between effort and quality. Put an- that we have just begun to understand why
other way, empirical support for a valid the- goal setting affects employee motivation and
ory depends in part on (a) the strength of performance.
the manipulation, (b) the response capacities One approach to this question has con-
of individuals in some setting, (c) links be- sidered the motivational components of goal-
tween independent variables and various mo- directed behavior. Goal difficulty is thought
tivation dependent variables, (d) choice of to affect effort, since the more difficult the
motivation dependent variables to measure, goal, the greater effort the individual must ex-
(e) errors associated with various assessment pend to attain the goal (Locke, 1968). Al-
techniques, (f) critical task contingencies, (g) though relatively few studies have included
choice of performance-relevant outcomes, (h) separate measures of effort, the results are
errors associated with the assessment of these generally supportive (cf. Carroll & Tosi,
outcomes, and (i) interrelationships among 1970; Steers, 1975; Terborg, 1976). Goal
MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR, AND PERFORMANCE 31

specificity might affect direction of behav- It is unfortunate, however, that even though
ior. The management-by-objective literature performance strategies have been mentioned
points to the clarification of role requirements as determiners of performance, the impact of
as one means for improving performance reward systems on these behaviors has re-
(Latham & Yukl, 197S; Locke, 1975). Sup- ceived little attention. This omission is espe-
port, for this position is less well established cially relevant in those instances in which
because, as with effort, direction of behavior method of payment was found to affect per-
rarely has been assessed. The exceptions have formance, but not effort (Jorgenson et al.,
been laboratory studies or simulations which 1973). Terborg (1976), iw an exception,
did find support (Locke, Bryan, & Kendall, found no difference in direction of behavior
1968; Terborg, 1976). due to contingent versus noncontingent pay-
ment. Only one measure of behavior was used,
however, and search behavior was not as-
Money as a Contingent Reward sessed.
Even though theoretical controversy exists
as to the specific role of money in work mo- Research Objectives
tivation (cf. Hamner, 1974; Lawler, 1971;
Opsahl & Dunnette, 1966), a common theme Acceptance of performance goals that are
persists; motivation and performance should both specific and difficult and knowledge of
be greater when pay is contingent upon per- performance-pay contingencies provide cues
formance as opposed to when pay is not con- that help the worker define the task. It was
tingent on performance. argued, however, that these cues may have
Much of recent research on pay has at- differential effects on separate dimensions of
tempted to specify the relevant personality motivated behavior. Past research on goal set-
and situational variables that influence the ef- ting and pay systems generally has not con-
fectiveness of various incentive programs. It sidered these issues in detail. For example, a
is not our purpose to explore these issues. specific and difficult goal for quality perform1
Rather, a more basic question is put forth. ance may elicit different behaviors than a simi-
Given a multidimensional conceptualization of lar goal for quantity performance. Also, qual-
motivated behavior, what are some likely be- ity performance goals in combination with a
havioral outcomes associated with contingent pay system that rewards quantity perform-
reward pay systems? It is not entirely clear ance may represent conflicting cues if quality
that such systems always increase effort or and quantity are negatively related.
performance (cf. Jorgenson, Dunnette, & In an attempt to clarify some of these ques-
Pritchard, 1973; Opsahl & Dunnette, 1966; tions, we emphasized the operationalization
Pritchard, DeLeo, & Von Bergen, 1976). Fail- and measurement of motivation. Effort was
ure to obtain support often results in attribu- operationalized in two ways and assessed from
tions to measurement error, especially with three sources. Direction of behavior was op-
effort. erationalized in three ways. Finally, both
We can also speculate that performance- quality and quantity performance measures
contingent reward systems influence direction were obtained.
of behavior. Such rewards change the out- Such a design would allow for a more de-
comes associated with different levels of per- tailed examination of past results, which sug-
formance. Search behavior designed to dis- gest that goals should affect effort, direction
cover a satisfactory performance strategy may
of behavior, and performance and that per-
result. But if search behavior entails addi-
formance-pay contingencies should affect ef-
tional costs, other strategies may be evoked
from the beginning that preclude search be- fort and performance. Pay system predictions
havior. Contingencies associated with the task were not made for direction of behavior, since
may cue the use of a performance strategy little theoretical or empirical work presently
that previously has been found satisfactory. exists on this question.
32 JAMES R. TERBORG AND HOWARD E. MILLER

Method troduced as project supervisors. Each supervisor had


an equal number of subjects in each condition and
Subjects was responsible for conducting individual sessions.
Half of the subjects were paid on a piece-rate sys-
Subjects were recruited via posters and advertise- tem (40^ per model during 1 hour of assembly and
ments in a campus newspaper listing a 2-hour re- $2.50 per hour for pre- and postassembly interviews),
search project on the analysis of job behavior. The and half were paid by the hour ($2.50 per hour for
task was described as light assembly work, with pay all work). The value of the piece-rate manipulation
being approximately $2.50 per hour depending on was determined in pilot work such that if an indi-
what work was done. The nature of the task was vidual worked at an average pace, he could expect to
described along with the fact that behavior would be earn about $2.50 for the hour. This was explained
filmed. The first 60 respondents were scheduled for to subjects in the piece-rate condition. It also was
individual 2-hour sessions and were randomly as- stated that any partially finished models at the end
signed to conditions. Subjects ranged in age from 17 of the hour would be prorated on the basis of 10^
to 23 years (M = 19.8 years), and almost all were for each quarter of a model built. Criteria for par-
college students. tially built models were explained.
Subjects also were assigned to one of the three
Task goal conditions: quantity, quality, or no assigned
goal. In the quantity goal condition, subjects were
One goal of the present study was to focus atten- given the specific production goal of building at least
tion on the measurement of effort and direction of nine models during the hour. Pilot data indicated this
behavior. To this end, time-lapse photography was to be a difficult but attainable goal. In the quality
used to record individual behavior on film. This goal condition, subjects were given the specific qual-
would allow for the rating of relevant behaviors by ity goal of at least 10 out of 12 possible quality
independent observers who were blind to conditions. points. Quality was assessed on six dimensions '(base
A task was developed using Tinker Toys that re- flatness, structure squareness, moving part function,
quired the assembly of a complex model with several crank assemblage, joint assemblage, and fan blade
moving parts. This task was chosen since it was de- angle) with 0, 1, or 2 points being assigned 'as
termined that (a) the task was interesting, (b) qual- quality improved. Examples of quality levels were
ity and quantity of performance could vary and be shown; a card with descriptions and point values
measured, (c) behavior at the task could be filmed was placed on the work bench. Pilot work again
for meaningful analysis, and (d) different methods indicated this to be a difficult, but attainable goal.
or strategies for assembly were possible and in fact In the no-goal condition, no mention was made con-
employed. cerning quantity or quality with the exception that a
We introduced subjects to the task by stating our model had to stand and the crank had to work to be
desire to obtain a photographic record of their be- counted as complete. The result was a 2 X 3 crossed
havior. The project was explained as an attempt to design with 10 subjects per cell. Experimental condi-
determine if time-lapse photography could aid in the tions were counterbalanced over time so that parts
analysis of manual jobs. No attempt was made to fatigue would be spread across conditions.
conceal the camera. Rather, subject attention was Subjects reported individually to the research
focused on the importance of filming the work. The project room. The supervisor explained that the
camera was placed in front of a work bench ap- camera was set to expose one frame every 5 sec,
proximately 15 ft. (4.6 m) away. The presence of which resulted in 720 observations per hour. Earlier
the camera was reported not to have a negative ef- work indicated this would be satisfactory for film
effect. ratings. Subjects completed a background question-
Subjects stood at a work bench with parts bins naire and were tested on ability. They were then
and an example model. There was an oversupply of shown the model; method of payment and assigned
parts. The model was explained, and subjects were goals were described next. While the camera was
given an opportunity to examine it. They worked at being set, subjects were given time to familiarize
the task for 1 hour. A clock was set to 12:00 at the themselves with the model and the various parts.
start of the work period. Subjects were instructed to The supervisor remained in the room but away from
place assembled models on a shelf that was out of the work bench while subjects worked. At the end
camera range but within their view. Thus, subjects of the hour, various method checks were obtained in
were able to keep track of the time remaining and an interview and subjects were paid and partially de-
the number of models completed. A rest area with briefed. Full debriefings were made available at the
magazines, coffee, and soft drinks was provided. No end of the project.
subjects took breaks, but several did take drinks to
the work bench for consumption during the task.
Assessment of Variables
Conditions and Procedure Performance. Quantity was measured by counting
the number of completed and partially completed
The 60 subjects were divided among four advanced models. This variable had a range from 4.00 to 11.00,
undergraduate male research assistants who were in- a mean of 7.61, and a standard deviation of 1.67.
MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR, AND PERFORMANCE 33

Quality was measured by supervisor ratings of com- rected for the number of raters. Scores were stan-
pleted models. The reliability of supervisor quality dardized within raters and combined to yield one
ratings was r .81. This was computed from ratings score, which ranged from 2.25 to 6.71.
of 11 practice models. Average quality points per Two other operationalizations of this variable were
completed model was used as the measure of quality. assessed for exploratory purposes. Number of models
These scores had a range from 3.5 to 11.5, a mean of built at the start was determined from analysis of
7.36, and a standard deviation of 1.93. the films. This objective measure had a range from
Effort. Measures of effort were obtained from 1 to 9, a mean of 2.38, and a standard deviation of
self-ratings, supervisor ratings, and analyses of the 2.01. Number of different strategies used during the
films. The first two were made on an 11-point scale, hour made up the second exploratory measure. For
with high scores indicating greater effort. Supervisors example, a subject used two strategies if he built
were instructed to observe the subjects unobtrusively three models at a time and then switched to one
and to rate them on how hard they worked during model at a time for the rest of the period. This
the hour. This measure had a range from 3 to 11, a measure had a range from 1 to 3, a mean of 1.72,
mean of 7.45, and a standard deviation of 2.10. There and a standard deviation of .74.
were no mean differences across supervisors. Self-re-
port measures were collected in a postwork inter-
view. This measure had a range from 6 to 11, a
Results
mean of 8.32, and a standard deviation of 1.38.
Method Checks
Rating of the films involved the use of a modified
paired comparison procedure similar to that discussed
by McCormick and Bachus (1952). First, within
Ability. The time required for subjects to
each cell, the 10 subjects were rank ordered on the complete a one-hand and two-hand manipula-
basis of both quality and quantity performance, with tion of the Minnesota Rate of Manipulation
the top position indicating the best performance in Test (see Guion, 1965, for a description),
that cell. Paired comparisons were made across the plus a small Tinker Toy model was used to
six cells, using subjects with similar rank orders. This
procedure would minimize error due to within-cell index ability. The coefficient alpha on stan-
variance that would have occurred if random assign- dardized scores was computed to be r= .81.
ment had been used for the pairings. Ratings were Ability scores ranged from 4.52 to 7.31,
made by two trained observers who worked inde- with low scores indicating superior ability.
pendently and were blind to conditions. The pro-
cedure consisted of running two movie projectors at
There were no significant ability differences
identical speeds. The subjects' film images were pro- across cells.
jected side by side so that simultaneous observation Pay manipulation. Following the manipu-
of the paired subjects was possible for rating speed lation of pay but prior to working at the task,
of motions. Ratings were made after every 15 min of subjects responded to interview questions ask-
time worked so that four assessments were made for
each pair of subjects. To illustrate, if one subject was ing for instrumentalities between quantity
rated as exerting more effort than his comparison at and quality performance and pay. The ex-
each quarter hour, he received a score of 4 and the pected pay effect was observed, with subjects
other received a score of 0. The possible range of in the piece-rate condition reporting signifi-
scores was from 0 to 20, since each subject was in-
volved in five comparisons. Rater reliability was com-
cantly higher quantity-pay instrumentalities
puted to be r ~ .80 corrected for the two raters. and lower quality-pay instrumentalities than
Scores were standardized within .raters and combined subjects in the hourly condition. There were
to yield one score, which ranged from 2.92 to 3.93. no interactions or goal condition effects.
Effort then was operationalized in two ways: how Goal manipulations. All subjects who were
hard the subject worked and how fast the subject
moved. Assessments were obtained with multiple assigned goals agreed to them and restated
methods: structured observation of films and verbal them to the supervisor. Goal acceptance also
reports. In this way, construct validity was enhanced was determined in a posttask interview, with
by minimizing problems associated with monoopera- over 97% of the subjects who were assigned
tion bias and monomethod bias (Cook & Campbell, a goal reporting a goal.
1976).
Direction of behavior. Three operationalizations Another concern was the effect of the pay
of direction of behavior were taken from the films. manipulation on the frequency of self-set
Quality behavior was defined as the number of goals. Locke (1968) and Terborg (1976) have
frames of film the subject was observed to check on suggested that when pay is contingent on
the quality of his work, for example, operation of
the crank, straightening the base, and tightening
performance, people may be more likely to
joints on a completed model. The two raters worked choose goals on their own. Chi-square analy-
independently and had a reliability of r = .82 cor- sis indicated no significant pay effect for fre-
34 JAMES R. TERBORG AND HOWARD E. MILLER

quency of either type of self-set goal. Thus, goal difficulty. Subjects in the no-goal condi-
subsequent analyses with pay would not be tion reported their goals as less difficult than
confounded with frequency of self-set goals. did subjects in the other two conditions.
A final check was made using self-report There were no significant interactions or
measures of goal level (i.e., intended perform- method-of-payment effects.
ance) and goal difficulty for all subjects who In summary, the goal manipulations ap-
reported having a goal (i.e., self-set or as- peared to be successful. The only effects were
signed). If method of payment affected these due to goal manipulations, with the means
variables, then main effects for payment on for goal level and goal difficulty being in the
motivation and performance would be con- expected directions. Although there was evi-
founded with goal level and goal difficulty. dence of self-set goals, these were set at
Similarly, it is important to examine for in- lower levels, were rated as less difficult than
tended goal manipulation effects. when specific goals were assigned, and were
For the 51 subjects who had a quality goal not affected by method of payment.
(assigned plus self-set), there was a signifi-
cant effect for goal condition on level of set Relationships Among Dependent Variables
goal and on difficulty. Subjects in the as-
signed quality goal condition had a signifi- The intercorrelations among the variables
cantly higher goal level than did subjects in are presented in Table 1. Quantity perform-
the other two conditions. For quality goal dif- ance was significantly related to supervisor
ficulty, subjects in the no-goal condition rated and self-ratings of effort, quality behavior and
their goal less difficult than did subjects in number of models built at the start, and abil-
the other two conditions. There were no sig- ity (p < .01 for all). Quality performance
nificant interactions or method-of-payment ef- was significantly correlated only with ability
fects. (/X.01).
Examination of quantity goal level and goal Additional correlations not reported in
difficulty produced essentially similar results. Table 1 were computed using goal level and
For the 49 subjects who reported having a goal difficulty. For the 51 subjects who re-
quantity goal (assigned plus self-set), there ported having a quality goal, ratings of level
was no effect on goal level due to the manip- and difficulty were positively correlated (r =
ulations, but there was a significant effect on .38, p < .01). Quality goal level was nega-

Table 1
Correlation Matrix of Dependent Variables

Variable 1
Performance
1. Quantity
2. Quality -11
Effort
3. Supervisor 64** -03
4. Self 49** -02 50**
5. Film 13 01 53** 32*
Direction
6. Quality behavior -35** 19 05 -01 24
7. Number built at start 34** -08 18 08 04 -32**
8. Number different
strategies 21 20 08 06 00 -12 34**
9. Ability" -50* -36** -39** -21 -15 08 -21 -31*
Note. N = 60. Decimals have been omitted.
" Low scores reflect superior ability.
* p < .05, two-tailed test.
** p < .01, two-tailed test.
MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR, AND PERFORMANCE 35

Table 2
Effects of Method of Payment and Goal Setting on Performance

Independent variable
Method of payment Goal setting
Dependent
variable Hourly Piece rate No goal Quality Quantity
(performance
measure) M SD M SD p 2
o> M SD M SD M SD p u2

Quantity 7.2 1.8 8.0 1.4 .04 .05 7.7 1.8 7.0 1.4 8.2 1.6 .05 .06
Quality 7.6 1.9 7.1 2.0 .29 .00 7.0 2.1 8.4 2.1 6.7 1.2 .01 .04

lively correlated with both quantity perform- Quality performance was influenced by goal
ance (r = .35, p < .05) and number of manipulations, F(2, 54) = 4.66, p < .01, o>2 =
models built at the start (r - -.29, p < .05), .04, but was not influenced by method of
and was positively correlated with both qual- payment. Fisher's least significant difference
ity performance (r = .34, p < .05) and qual- test indicated that quality was significantly
ity behavior (r = .28, p < .05). No other cor- greater when quality goals were assigned than
relations were significant. in the other two conditions. There was no dif-
For the 49 subjects who reported having ference in quality performance between the
a quantity goal, ratings of level and difficulty no-goal or the quantity goal conditions. Again,
were positively correlated (r = .47, p < .01). there was no evidence of an interaction.
Quantity goal level also was correlated with
supervisor ratings of effort (r .28, p < .05) Effort
and quantity performance (r = .40, p < .01).
No other correlations were significant. The three operationalizations of effort were
These correlations provide some support analyzed using a multivariate analysis of vari-
for the notion that goal level covaries with ance (MANOVA). This technique was used
task performance. There also was evidence of because identical predictions would be made
relationships among goal level and goal dif- for each measure of effort and, as expected,
ficulty with measures of effort and direction they were intercorrelated with each other (see
of behavior. The results, however, were weak. Table 1). Evidence of a significant multivari-
ate effect would suggest the secondary use of
Performance ANOVA (Spector, 1977).
The results of the 2 X 3 MANOVA are
Quantity and quality performance data presented in Table 3. There was a significant
were analyzed separately using a 2 X 3 method of payment effect on the set of effort
crossed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with measures, multivariate F(3, 52) =3.72, p <
10 subjects per cell. The results are sum- .02. Table 3 shows that for each measure, ef-
marized in Table 2. Quantity performance was fort was greater when pay was contingent
significantly greater in the piece-rate condi- on performance. Univariate Fs indicated that
tion than in the hourly condition, F(l, 54) = conditions differed significantly on self-rat-
4.50, p < .04, to2 = .05. Quantity also was in- ings, F(l, 54) = 9.91, p < .003, >2 = .13, and
fluenced by assigned goals, F(2, 54) =3.07, on supervisor ratings, F(l, 54) = 6.22, p <
p < .05, io2 = .06. Fisher's least significant ,02, <o- = .08, but not on film ratings. There
difference test indicated that this effect was was no evidence of a goal-setting effect or an
due to the performance difference between interaction.
the quantity goal condition and the quality
goal condition, with significantly more models Direction of Behavior
being produced when quantity goals were as-
signed. There was no evidence of an interac- Number of models built at the start and
tion. number of strategies were analyzed with a
36 JAMES R. TERBORG AND HOWARD E. MILLER

Table 3
Effects of Method of Payment and Goal Setting on Effort

Independent variable
Method of payment Goal setting
Dependent
variable Hourly Piece rate No goal Quality Quantity
(effort
measure) M SD M SD p w 2
M SD M SD M SD P u2

Self-ratings" 7.8 1.4 8.8 1.2 .003 .13 8.2 1.4 8.0 1.4 8.8 1.3 .16 .00
Supervisor
ratings 6.8 2.3 8.1 1.8 .02 .08 7.0 2.1 7.4 1.9 8.0 2.3 .26 .00
Film ratings -.4 2.0 .4 1.7 .13 .00 -.1 1.5 .3 1.8 -.2 2.1 .72 .00

" Multivariate results: pay, F(3, 52) = 3.12, p < .02; goal setting, F(6, 104) = 1.42, p < .22; interaction,
F(6, 104) = .71, p < .65.

MANOVA. The third index, quality behavior, Discussion


was analyzed with an ANOVA. This was done
because a specific directional prediction could The results demonstrated once again that
be made for quality behavior based on the performance can be predicted from manipula-
goal manipulations, whereas the other two tion of performance-pay contingencies and
variables were included for exploratory pur- performance goals. More important, it was
poses. The results of all analyses are pre- shown that these motivation interventions
sented in Table 4. Goal setting significantly had differential effects on effort and direction
influenced quality behavior, F(2, 54) = 3.85, of behavior. Terborg (1976) reported similar
p < .03, <o2 = .09. Fisher's least significant findings on a cognitive task with different op-
difference test indicated greater attention to erationalizations of the same constructs.
quality in the quality goal condition than in Method of payment and goal setting inde-
either of the other two conditions. There was pendently affected motivation and perform-
no evidence of a method-of-payment effect or ance. The absence of any interactions repli-
an interaction. The MANOVA results for cated other work (London & Oldham, 1976;
number of models built at the start and num- Terborg, 1976) and supported the position
ber of strategies failed to reveal any signifi- that goal-setting procedures should not re-
cant effects. place financial incentives as a means for im-

Table 4
Effects of Method of Payment and Goal Setting on Direction of Behavior

Independent variable
Dependent Method of payment Goal setting
variable
(direction of Hourly Piece rate No goal Quality Quantity
behavior
2
measure) M SD M SD p w M SD M SD M SD P u2

Quality behavior .1 1.9 -.1 1.8 .53 .00 -.5 1.6 .9 2.2 -.3 1.3 .03 .09
Number built
at start" 2.0 1.5 2.7 2.4 .19 .00 2.7 2.2 2.0 1.7 2.6 2.2 .51 .00
Number of
strategies" 1.8 .8 1.6 .7 .40 .00 .1.6 .8 1.8 .8 1.8 .7 .61 .00

" Multivariate results on number built at start and number of strategies: pay, F(2, 53) = 1.93, p < .16;
goal setting, F(4, 106) = .70, p < .60; interaction, F(4, 106) = .11, p < .98.
MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR, AND PERFORMANCE 37

proving performance (Pritchard & Curts, ability and motivation. The ability composite
1973). These results do not conflict with was significantly correlated with both the ef-
Locke's (1968), since financial rewards were fort and direction composites (rs = .32 and
not contingent on goal attainment. .29, respectively, p < .05). Finally, a fourth
The inability of the film rating of effort to variable made up of the product of the other
differentiate groups significantly or to predict three failed to account for any additional
performance was unexpected. The measure variance beyond that explained by the linear
did correlate highly with other measures of combination (R2 change <.01). A different
effort, which supports the construct validity finding occurred when quality performance
of the measure. Admittedly post hoc, a pos- was examined. Depending on the order of
sible explanation may be that speed of motion entry into the equation, ability accounted for
represented an inappropriate operationaliza- 13% to 17% of the variance and the two
tion of effort for this task and that some other motivation composites combined predicted less
operationalization such as smoothness or ef- than 4%. The multiplicative term again
ficiency of motion would have been better. In failed to explain any additional variance (R2
any case, use of films for rating effort merits change < .01).
further attention. A question that stems from the above re-
The amount of variance in performance sults concerns the choice of particular behav-
that can be explained by motivation-depen- iors and performance outcomes that represent
dent variables presents a theoretical issue of the focus of motivation interventions. The
concern. The pattern of results could be con- two manipulations used in this study pre-
sidered weak given the nature of the task. It dicted 11 % of quantity performance variance.
is possible, however, to examine the relative Effort and direction of behavior predicted
contribution of ability, effort, and direction 26% of quantity performance variance after
of behavior for predicting performance. This the effects of ability were removed. In this
was done by constructing composite scale study, the usual practice of setting perform-
scores for the three constructs. Quantity per- ance goals and making rewards contingent on
formance then was regressed on the set of performance was followed. But, as stated in
scale scores. In one regression, ability was en- the introduction, perhaps attention should be
tered first followed by effort and direction. A directed toward observable behaviors that
second regression entered ability last. Ability imply motivation and that relate to perform-
alone produced a multiple correlation of .25, ance outcomes. Goals should be set for effort
which was significant. Inclusion of the two and direction in addition to performance; re-
motivation composites yielded a total mul- wards should be made contingent on all three
tiple correlation of .51, with an additional variables as well.
26% of variance being explained by effort We admit that in many cases performance
and direction of behavior. It should be noted can be measured more accurately than mo-
that these two composites were uncorrelated tivation. This should not be a problem, how-
(r = .04) and that each was significant re- ever, if motivation is operationalized in terms
gardless of order of entry. When the motiva- of observable behaviors that are under indi-
tion composites were entered first, they pro- vidual control.
duced a multiple correlation of .45, with abil- The shift from performance outcomes to
ity accounting for an additional 6% of the observable behaviors has several advantages.
variance. This suggests that on this task, These advantages occur when performance is
knowledge of individual motivation predicts (a) determined primarily by the situation,
a substantial amount of performance variance (b) temporally distant from immediate be-
which cannot be attributed to ability. It also havior, and (c) multidimensional and not
suggests that the practice of using the per- amenable to reliable and valid measurement.
formance residual, with ability removed, as The position being advanced should not be
an index of motivation may fail to consider confused with operant conditioning or behav-
the performance variance common to both ior modification. The only intended similarity
38 JAMES R. TERBORG AND HOWARD E. MILLER

lies in the emphasis on observable behaviors tive, does suggest possible directions for fu-
as appropriate dependent variables and in ture research. Greater attention must be di-
the de-emphasis on performance outcomes, rected toward the operationalization and mea-
which in turn result from those behaviors. surement of motivation as a multidimensional
A second theoretical issue concerns the construct. Independent variables in motiva-
complex relationships among motivation in- tion research should be examined more closely
terventions, task characteristics, and motiva- so as to understand better the motivational
tion dependent variables. Difficult goals on meaning of the manipulations. In other words,
simple tasks may cue subjects toward effort, do all manipulations of goal difficulty have
since these tasks have stronger effort-per- the same motivational impact? Finally, task
formance relationships. Many tasks employed processes need to be explored in more detail
in past research on goal difficulty could be with regard to understanding possible differ-
classified in this category. Difficult goals on ential effects of manipulations on dimensions
more complex tasks, which are high on path- of motivation. It would appear that the issues
goal multiplicity, may cue subjects toward surrounding motivation are more complex
various behavior directions such as generat- than they are usually presented.
ing, testing, and implementing alternative
strategies. Specific performance goals likewise References
may affect effort when a clear strategy for
task accomplishment exists; however, when Birch, D., & Veroff, J. Motivation: A study of ac-
tasks are more complex and nonprescribed, we tion. Belmont, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1966.
Campbell, J. P., Dunnette, M. D., Lawler, E. E., &
might expect to find goal specificity affecting Weick, K. E. Managerial behavior, performance,
direction of behavior. Frost and Mahoney and effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.
(1976) made a similar point. Campbell, J. P., & Pritchard, R. D. Motivation the-
A further complication arises when we con- ory in industrial and organizational psychology.
In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial
sider the possibility that a specific and diffi- and organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand Mc-
cult quality goal may provide different cues Nally, 1976.
(i.e., more specific information) than a spe- Carroll, S. J., & Tosi, H. L. Goal characteristics and
cific and difficult quantity goal. Quality goal personality factors in a management by objectives
program. Administrative Science Quarterly, 1970,
difficulty may be equated with quantity goal IS, 295-305.
difficulty on some dimension, such as percent- Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. The design and con-
age of goal accomplishment, yet the quality duct of quasi-experiments and true experiments in
goal may redefine the task in such a way that field settings. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook
of industrial and organizational psychology. Chi-
the task becomes more complex with regard cago: Rand McNally, 1976.
to path-goal multiplicity. Thus, to say that Frost, P. J., & Mahoney, T. A. Goal setting and the
goal difficulty affects effort or that goal spe- task process an interactive influence on individual
cificity affects direction may be incomplete, performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
since task characteristics (i.e., simple vs. com- Performance, 1976,17, 328-350.
Guion, R. M. Personnel testing. New York: Mc-
plex or quality performance vs. quantity per- Graw-Hill, 196S.
formance) and goal characteristics (i.e., dif- Hackman, J. R. Group influences on individuals. In
ficult vs. specific) may provide different cues M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial
with regard to effort and direction of behavior. and organizational Psychology. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1976.
The task used in the present study was Hackman, J. R., & Morris, C. G. Groups tasks, group
chosen because a variety of production meth- interaction process, and group performance effec-
ods were possible for both quality and quan- tiveness: A review and proposed integration. In L.
tity. The lack of goal effects on effort then Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1975.
could be due to cues provided by the task Hamner, W. C. Reinforcement theory and contin-
and the goals themselves, which may have em- gency management in organizational settings. In
phasized direction of behavior rather than H. L. Tosi & W. C. Hamner (Eds.) Organizational
behavior and management: A contingency ap-
effort. proach. Chicago: St. Clair Press, 1974.
The preceding discussion, although specula- Jorgenson, D. O., Dunnette, M. D., & Pritchard, R.
MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR, AND PERFORMANCE 39

D. Effects of the manipulation of a performance- and organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand Mc-
reward contingency on behavior in a simulated Nally, 1976.
work setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1973, Opsahl, R. L., & Dunnette, M. D. The role of
57, 271-280. financial compensation in industrial motivation.
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. A review of research Psychological Bulletin, 1966, 66, 94-118.
on the application of goal setting in organizations. Pritchard, R. D., & Curts, M. I. The influence of
Academy of Management Journal, 197S, 18, 224- goal setting and financial incentives on task per-
245. formance. Organizational Behavior and Human
Lawler, E. E. Pay and organizational effectiveness: Performance, 1973,10, 175-183.
A psychological mew. New York: McGraw-Hill, Pritchard, R. D., DeLeo, P. L., & Von Bergen, C.
1971. W. A field experimental test of expectancy-valence
Locke, E. A. Toward a theory of task motivation incentive motivation techniques. Organizational Be-
and incentives. Organizational Behavior and Hu- havior and Human Performance, 1976, IS, 355-406.
man Performance, 1968, 3, 157-189. Spector, P. E. What to do with significant multivari-
Locke, E. A. Personnel attitudes and motivation. In ate effects in MANOVA. Journal of Applied Psy-
M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual chology, 1977, 62, 158-163.
review of psychology. Palo Alto, Calif.: Annual Steers, R. M. Task-goal attributes, achievement, and
Reviews, 1975. supervisor performance. Organizational Behavior
Locke, E. A., Bryan, J. F., & Kendall, L. M. Goals and Human Performance, 1975,13, 392-403.
and intentions as mediators of the effects of mone-
Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. The role of task-goal
tary incentives on behavior. Journal of Applied
attributes in employee performance. Psychological
Psychology, 1968, 52, 104-121.
Bulletin, 1974, 81, 434-452.
London, M., & Oldhara, G. R. Effects of varying goal
types and incentive systems on performance and Terborg, J. R. The motivational components of goal
satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61,
1976,19, 537-546. 613-621.
McCormick, E. J., & Bachus, J. A. Paired comparison Terborg, J. R. Validation and extension of an indi-
ratings I. The effect on ratings of reductions in vidual differences model of work performance. Or-
the number of pairs. Journal of Applied Psychol- ganizational Behavior and Human Performance,
ogy, 1952, 36, 123-127. 1977,18, 188-216.
McGrath, J. E. Stress and behavior in organizations.
In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial Received February 28, 1977

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi