In this article we suggest that events and contexts relevant to the initiation and regulation of inten-
tional behavior can function either to support autonomy (i.e., to promote choice) or to control
behavior (i.e., to pressure one toward specific outcomes). Research herein reviewed indicates that
this distinction is relevant to specific external events and to general interpersonal contexts as well as
to specific internal events and to general personality orientations. That is, the distinction is relevant
whether one's analysis focuses on social psychological variables or on personality variables. The
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
research review details those contextual and person factors that tend to promote autonomy and those
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
that tend to control. Furthermore, it shows that autonomy support has generally been associated
with more intrinsic motivation, greater interest, less pressure and tension, more creativity, more
cognitive flexibility, better conceptual learning, a more positive emotional tone, higher self-esteem,
more trust, greater persistence of behavior change, and better physical and psychological health
than has control. Also, these results have converged across different assessment procedures, different
research methods, and different subject populations. On the basis or these results, we present an
organismic perspective in which we argue that the regulation of intentional behavior varies along a
continuum from autonomous (i.e.. self-determined) to controlled. The relation of this organismic
perspective to historical developments in empirical psychology is discussed, with a particular empha-
sis on its implications for the study of social psychology and personality.
For several decades American psychology was dominated by sequences of behavior to expectations about future conse-
associationist theories. Assuming that behavior is controlled by quences of behavior. The concept of intentionality (Lewin,
peripheral mechanisms, these theories held that the initiation 1951) became important because behavior, whether implicitly
of behavior is a function of stimulus inputs such as external or explicitly, was understood in terms of people's intentions to
contingencies of reinforcement (Skinner, 1953) or internal act in a way that would yield certain outcomes.
drive stimulations (Hull, 1943) and that the regulation of behav- Within the concept of intentionality, however, a further dis-
ior is a function of associative bonds between inputs and behav- tinction can usefully be made. Some intentional behaviors, we
iors that develop through reinforcement processes. With that suggest, are initiated and regulated through choice as an expres-
general perspective, the central processing of information was sion of oneself, whereas other intentional behaviors are pres-
not part of the explanatory system, so concepts such as in- sured and coerced by intrapsychic and environmental forces
tention were considered irrelevant to the determination of be- and thus do not represent true choice (Deci & Ryan, 1985b).
havior. The fo'rmer behaviors are characterized by autonomous initia-
During the 1950s and 1960s, associationist theories gave way tion and regulation and are referred to as self-determined; the
to cognitive theories in which the processing of information was latter behaviors are characterized by heteronomous initiation
assumed to play an important role in the determination of be- and regulation and are referred to as controlled.1
havior. On the basis of this assumption, the initiation of behav- We shall argue that the distinction between self-determined
ior was theorized to be a function of expectations about behav- and controlled behaviors has ramifications for the quality of ac-
ior-outcome contingencies and of the psychological value of tion and experience and is relevant to the study of both social
outcomes (e.g., Atkinson, 1964; Tolman, 1959; Vroom, 1964), contexts and personality.
and the regulation of behavior was seen as a process of compar-
ing one's current state to a standard (i.e., the desired outcome)
Intentionality and Autonomy
and then acting to reduce the discrepancy (e.g., Kanfer, 1975;
Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960). Thus, the cognitive per- An intention is generally understood as a determination to
spective shifted the focus of analysis from the effects of past con- engage in a particular behavior (Atkinson, 1964). In the cogni-
tive theories of motivation and action (e.g., Heider, 1960;
Lewin, 1951; Tolman, 1959), which have their roots in Gestalt
psychology, having an intention implies personal causation and
This work was supported in part by Grant HD 19914 from the Na-
tional Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the Human is equivalent to being motivated to act. Intentions are said to
Motivation Program at the University of Rochester.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ed-
ward L. Deci or to Richard M. Ryan, Human Motivation Program, ' Like most dichotomies in psychology, being self-determined versus
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, New controlled is intended to describe a continuum. Behaviors can thus be
York 14627. seen as being more or less self-determined.
1024
AUTONOMY SUPPORT VERSUS CONTROL 1025
derive from one's desire to achieve positively valent outcomes pist's authority is behaving intentionally, but until the action is
or avoid negatively valent ones. self-initiated and grasped as one's own solution it would not be
Using an intentional perspective, psychologists working in a characterized as autonomous.
neo-operant reinforcement tradition have emphasized that peo- When autonomous, people experience themselves as initia-
ple's beliefs about whether certain behaviors are reliably related tors of their own behavior; they select desired outcomes and
to desired outcomes are of central import. An abundance of choose how to achieve them. Regulation through choice is char-
research has shown, for example, that when a situation is struc- acterized by flexibility and the absence of pressure. By contrast,
tured so that outcomes are independent of behaviors (Seligman, being controlled is characterized by greater rigidity and the ex-
1975) or when people have a generalized belief that behaviors perience of having to do what one is doing. There is intention,
and outcomes are independent (Rotter, 1966), nonintentional- but lacking is a true sense of choice. When controlled, people
ity and maladaptation are likely to result However, believing are, in the words of deCharms (1968), "pawns" to desired out-
that behaviors are reliably related to outcomes is not enough to comes, even though they intend to achieve those outcomes.
ensure a high level of motivation and adaptation. People must
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
also believe that they are sufficiently competent to execute the Initiation and Regulation of Behavior
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
on interpersonal or social contexts, showing not only that gen- contingent on doing the activityhave been consistently and
eral contexts can have either an autonomy-supportive or a con- reliably shown to undermine intrinsic motivation, presumably
trolling functional significance, but also that this varied func- because their controlling function is salient. The effects of per-
tional significance has predictable effects on people's experi- formance-contingent rewardsthose given for attaining a spec-
ence, attitudes, and behavior within those settings. ified level of good performanceare more complicated. Be-
When the autonomy supportive versus controlling distinc- cause they inherently provide positive competence feedback,
tion was initially made (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1980; Deci, the appropriate comparison condition is one that conveys the
Schwartz, Sheinman, & Ryan, 1981), it was hypothesized that same feedback without a reward. When such comparisons have
autonomy-supportive events and contexts would maintain or been made, performance-contingent rewards have generally
enhance intrinsic motivation and that controlling events and been found to undermine intrinsic motivation, although they
contexts would undermine intrinsic motivation. Because in- have sometimes been shown to maintain or enhance intrinsic
trinsic motivation has been so widely explored as the dependent motivation when the controlling aspect is minimized and com-
variable in studies of autonomy-supportive versus controlling petence cues are emphasized (Harackiewicz, Manderlink, &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
events and contexts, the effect of an event or context on intrinsic Sansone, 1984).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
motivation can be used as one criterion for classifying whether To summarize, many studies have shown that rewards, on
that event or context tends to be experienced as autonomy sup- average, undermine people's intrinsic motivation. It appears,
portive or controlling. Thus, within the reviews of research on therefore, that rewards tend to be experienced as controlling,
external events and on interpersonal contexts, we will first pres- which of course makes sense, as rewards are typically used to
ent studies that used intrinsic motivation as a dependent vari- induce or pressure people to do things they would not freely do.
able, so as to specify the average functional significance of par- When people behave in the presence of reward contingencies,
ticular events or contexts. Then, within each of the two reviews, the rewards tend to have a functional significance of control,
we will move on to studies that have explored the relation of thus representing an external event that restricts self-determi-
those factors to other variables so as to explicate empirically nation, although under certain circumstances they can be used
the concomitants and consequences of self-determined versus to support self-determination.
controlled behavior. Threats and deadlines. Using a modified avoidance condi-
tioning paradigm, Deci and Cascio (1972) found that subjects
who solved interesting puzzles to avoid an unpleasant noise
External Events: Autonomy Supportive or Controlling
demonstrated less subsequent intrinsic motivation for the activ-
The term event refers to a specifiable occurrence or condition ity than did subjects who solved the puzzles without the threat
relevant to the initiation and regulation of behavior. The offer of noise. Amabile, DeJong, and Lepper (1976) found that the
of a reward, for example, is an event, as is an instance of compe- imposition of a deadline for the completion of an interesting
tence feedback, a demand, a deadline, and an opportunity for activity also decreased subjects' intrinsic motivation for that ac-
choice. The most frequently studied events have been rewards, tivity. It appears, therefore, that these events, like rewards, tend
though many others have also been explored. In this section, to be experienced as controlling and thus to diminish people's
studies of the effects of various events on intrinsic motivation self-determination.
will be reviewed so as to allow each event to be classified as Evaluation and surveillance. Other experiments have indi-
tending to be either autonomy supportive or controlling. cated that the mere presence of a surveillant or evaluator, even
Rewards. Dozens of studies have explored the effects of re- without rewards or aversive consequences, can be detrimental to
wards on intrinsic motivation. These have included monetary intrinsic motivation and thus, we suggest, to self-determination
payments (Deci, 1971), good-player awards (Lepper, Greene, & more generally. Lepper and Greene (1975), for example, found
Nisbett, 1973), food (Ross, 1975), and prizes (Harackiewicz, that surveillance by a video camera undermined the intrinsic
1979). In general, rewards have been found to undermine in- motivation of children, and Plant and Ryan (1985) found the
trinsic motivation. When people received rewards for working same result for college students. Pittman, Davey, Alafat, Weth-
on an interesting activity, they tended to display less interest in erill, and Kramer (1980) reported that in-person surveillance
and willingness to work on that activity after termination of the also undermined intrinsic motivation.
rewards than did people who had worked on the activity without Harackiewicz et al. (1984) found that subjects who were told
receiving a reward. This phenomenon, labeled the undermining that their activity would be evaluated displayed less subsequent
effect (Deci & Ryan, 1980), has been most reliably obtained intrinsic motivation than did subjects who were not told this,
when rewards were expected (Lepper et al., 1973), salient (Ross, even though the evaluations were positive. Smith (1974) found
1975), and contingent on task engagement (Ryan, Mims, & the same results for intrinsic motivation to learn. Similarly,
Koestner, 1983). Benware and Deci (1984) and Maehr and Stallings (1972) have
Ryan et al. (1983) pointed out that when rewards are differ- found that learning in order to be tested or externally evaluated
ently structured, they have discernibly different effects. The au- has detrimental effects on intrinsic motivation for learning.
thors provided a taxonomy of reward structures and related it to The effects of evaluation and surveillance are not surprising,
reward effects. Their review indicated that task-noncontingent as both are integral to social control. These events tend to limit
rewardsthose that are given independent of task engage- self-determination and thus reduce intrinsic motivation even
mentwere least likely to undermine intrinsic motivation be- when they are not accompanied by explicit rewards or punish-
cause the reward is not given for doing the activity and thus is ments.
not salient as a control. Task-contingent rewardsthose made Choice. Autonomy-supportive events are defined as those
AUTONOMY SUPPORT VERSUS CONTROL 1027
that encourage the process of choice and the experience of au- measure have found that postexperimental interest-enjoyment
tonomy. The one type of event that both fits the definition and is higher following autonomy-supportive events than following
has been shown, on average, to enhance intrinsic motivation is controlling events (e.g., Enzle & Ross, 1978).
the opportunity to choose what to do. Creativity. Amabile (1979) reported that subjects who were
Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith, and Deci (1978) found told that their work would be evaluated produced artistic col-
that when college student subjects were given a choice about lages that were rated as less creative than those produced by
which puzzles to work on and about how much time to allot to subjects who did not expect evaluations. Similar effects were
each, they were more intrinsically motivated during a subse- found for surveillance (see Amabile, 1983). Furthermore, when
quent period than were no-choice subjects in a yoked compari- children competed for a reward, they produced less creative col-
son group. The provision of choice enhanced their intrinsic mo- lages than those produced in a noncompetitive condition
tivation. Swann and Pittman (1977) reported similar results in (Amabile, 1982), and when children contracted for rewards
an experiment with children. they were also less creative (Amabile, Hennessey, & Grossman,
Positive feedback. The event of positive competence feed- 1986). Additionally, Kruglanski, Friedman, and Zeevi (1971)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
back has been widely studied as it relates to intrinsic motiva- found that when subjects who wrote stories were rewarded with
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
tion.3 Several studies have found that it increased intrinsic mo- the opportunity to engage in an interesting activity in the future,
tivation (Blanck, Reis, & Jackson, 1984; Boggiano & Ruble, their stories were judged to be less creative than the stories of
1979; Vallerand & Reid, 1984), although this has occurred only subjects who were not rewarded. In sum, events that are typi-
under certain circumstances (Fisher, 1978; Ryan, 1982) or for cally controlling appear to affect creativity negatively, whereas
certain kinds of people (Boggiano & Barrett, 1985; Deci, Cas- events that are more autonomy supportive seem to promote
cio, & Krusell, 1975; Kast, 1983). Taken together, the studies creativity.
indicate that positive competence feedback neither supports au- Cognitive activity. Results similar to those for creativity have
tonomy nor controls behavior per se. It can enhance intrinsic been reported for cognitive flexibility. McGraw and McCullers
motivation by affirming competence (e.g., Harackiewicz, (1979) found that monetarily rewarded subjects had a more
Manderlink, & Sansone, in press) because intrinsic motivation difficult time breaking set when doing Luchins-type (1942)
is based in the need for competence as well as the need for self- water-jar problems than did nonrewarded subjects. Benware
determination, although it will do so only when the sense of and Deci (1984) reported that evaluative tests impaired college
competence is accompanied by the experience of self-determi- students' conceptual learning in addition to undermining their
nation (Fisher, 1978; Ryan, 1982).4 But it can also undermine intrinsic motivation. Grolnick and Ryan (1987) found impair-
intrinsic motivation by being experienced as a form of interper- ments in conceptual learning of fifth-grade subjects who
sonal control (Ryan et al., 1983). The Harackiewicz, Abrahams, learned material under a controlling-evaluative condition rather
and Wageman (1987) article in this special section focuses on than an autonomy-supportive one. It appears that when cogni-
the issue of competence, whereas our article focuses on self- tive activity is controlled, it is more rigid and less conceptual,
determination. perhaps with a more narrow focus, than when it is self-deter-
mined.
Effects and Correlates of Autonomy-Supportive Versus Emotional tone. Garbarino (1975) studied fifth- and sixth-
grade girls who were rewarded with movie tickets for teaching
Controlling Events
younger girls how to do a sorting task. He reported that the
The studies just reported used intrinsic motivation as the pri- rewarded tutors were more critical and demanding than were
mary dependent variable and were used to help classify events nonrewarded tutors. In a complementary study, children in-
as tending to be either autonomy supportive or controlling. It is duced to interact with another child in order to play with a nice
interesting to note that more of the events manipulated in these game had less positive impressions of that other child than did
experiments were experienced as controlling than as autonomy children who had not been focused on the incentive (Boggiano,
promoting. This makes sense, however, because autonomy must Klinger, & Main, 1985). Controlling events, it seems, tend to
emanate from oneself and can therefore only be facilitated by induce a negative emotional tone and a less favorable view of
contextual events, whereas control is something that can be others in that situation.
done to people by contextual events and is therefore more easily Maintenance of behavior change. Rewards have also been
evidenced. We shall now address additional effects of these au- studied as they relate to the persistence of behavior change fol-
tonomy-supportive versus controlling events to begin explicat- lowing the termination of treatment conditions. A study by
ing the qualities of self-determined versus controlled behaviors. Dienstbier and Leak (1976) of a weight-loss program, for exam-
Interest-enjoyment. Along with the free-choice measure of ple, indicated that although rewards facilitated weight loss, then-
intrinsic motivation, self-reports of interest are often obtained. termination led to much of the lost weight's being regained.
Ryan et al. (1983) reported a correlation of .42 between the
behavioral measure of intrinsic motivation and self-reports of 3
Negative feedback has also been studied and has been found to re-
interest, and Harackiewicz (1979) reported a correlation of .44
duce intrinsic motivation; however, we interpret these decreases as re-
between intrinsic motivation and expressed enjoyment. Al-
sulting from the feedback's being experienced as amotivating rather
though research has not always found these strong correlations than controlling.
(see Ryan & Deci, 1986), self-reports of interest-enjoyment do 4
In cognitive evaluation theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985b), inputs that
appear to be related to intrinsic motivation. Furthermore, nu- both affirm competence and promote self-determination are referred to
merous studies that have not used the free-choice, behavioral as informational.
1028 EDWARD L. DECI AND RICHARD M. RYAN
When behavior is controlled by events such as rewards, the Events and interpersonal contexts. Earlier, we saw that some
behavior tends to persist only so long as the controlling events events tend to be experienced as supporting self-determination
are present. In terms of effective behavior change in therapeutic and others tend to be experienced as controlling, and now we
settings, the implication is that behavior change brought about have seen that contexts can also be characterized as tending ei-
through salient external controls is less likely to persist follow- ther to support autonomy or to control. A few studies have ex-
ing the termination of treatment than is change that is brought plored the effects of the same event in different experimentally
about more autonomously. Behavior and personality change created contexts.
will be maintained and transferred, we have argued, when the In one study, Ryan et al. (1983) explored contextual influ-
change is experienced as autonomous or self-determined (Deci ences on the effects of performance-contingent rewards: those
&Ryan, I985b). rewards that people receive for attaining a specified level of good
To summarize, behavior undertaken when the functional sig- performance. Previous research had shown that these rewards
nificance of events is autonomy supportive has been related to generally undermined the intrinsic motivation of their recipi-
greater interest, more creativity, more cognitive flexibility, bet- ents relative to that of subjects who received no rewards but
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
ter conceptual learning, a more positive emotional tone, and got the same performance feedback that was inherent in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
more persistent behavior change than has behavior undertaken performance-contingent rewards. This means, in essence, that
when the functional significance of events is controlling. Thus the reward itself tends to be controlling unless its evaluative
far, research has related these motivationally relevant depen- component is removed (Harackiewicz et al., 1984). Further-
dent variables primarily to the events of rewards and evalua- more, however, the positive feedback that is conveyed by the
tion. reward can enhance intrinsic motivation by affirming one's
competence.
Interpersonal Contexts: Autonomy Supportive Ryan et al. argued that the effect of a performance-contingent
reward could be significantly affected by the way it is conveyed,
Versus Controlling
in other words, by the interpersonal context within which it is
In the preceding discussion we described research on specific received. Two groups of college student subjects received perfor-
events relevant to the initiation and regulation of behavior. Nu- mance-contingent rewards. Those in one group were told that
merous other studies have focused on interpersonal contexts they would receive a $3 reward if they "performed well," and
rather than specific events. For example, in interpersonal situa- those in the other group were told that they would receive a $3
tions the general ambience can tend either to support autonomy reward if they "performed well, as you should" Following each
or to control behavior. We now turn to that research on interper- of three puzzles, subjects received positive feedback that was in
sonal contexts. We begin, of course, with studies in which in- line with the initial induction. For example, half were told, ""You
trinsic motivation was the dependent measure, because those have done well," and the other half were told, "%u have done
are the ones that we use to establish the usefulness of the distinc- well, just as you should." Then, at the end of the performance
tion. period, subjects were given the reward either "for doing well"
Studies of autonomy-supportive versus controlling contexts or "for doing well and performing up to standards." It was ex-
have been of two types. Some are correlational field studies in pected, of course, that words like should and standards would
which the functional significance of the context is measured and serve to create a controlling context and lead the subjects to
related to motivationally relevant variables of people in those experience the rewards as controlling. Results revealed a sig-
contexts. The others are laboratory experiments in which nificant difference between the intrinsic motivation of the two
events such as rewards or feedback are administered within ex- groups of subjects. Those who received rewards in an auton-
perimentally created autonomy-supportive versus controlling omy-supportive context were more intrinsically motivated than
contexts, were those who received rewards in a controlling context. In
General contexts. In one field study (Deci, Nezlek, & Shein- other words, the interpersonal context within which the event
man, 1981), teachers and children in fourth- through sixth- (i.e., the reward) was administered affected the functional sig-
grade classrooms were subjects. The researchers used a psycho- nificance of the event.
metric instrument to measure individual teachers' orientations The Ryan et al. (1983) results are consistent with others re-
toward supporting children's autonomy versus controlling chil- ported by Harackiewicz (1979), who also found significant
dren's behavior. They reasoned that teachers oriented toward differences between the intrinsic motivation of two groups of
supporting autonomy would tend to create a classroom context high school subjects receiving performance-contingent rewards.
that promoted self-determination, whereas those oriented to- She had made one administration of the rewards less control-
ward control would tend to create a controlling context for the ling by allowing subjects to self-monitor their performance
children. The researchers then assessed the intrinsic motivation against a table of norms, and these subjects were more intrinsi-
of children in the classrooms by using Matter's (1981) measure cally motivated than others who were not allowed to self-
and found a strong positive correlation between teachers' auton- monitor.
omy support and children's intrinsic motivation. In another In another study, Ryan (1982) argued that positive compe-
study, Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, and Ryan (1981) analyzed tence feedback, which is not inherently either autonomy sup-
changes in children's intrinsic motivation from the second day portive or controlling, will be differentially interpreted as auton-
of school to the end of the second month. They found that chil- omy promoting or controlling depending on the nature of the
dren of autonomy-supportive teachers became more intrinsi- interpersonal context within which it is embedded. College stu-
cally motivated relative to children of control-oriented teachers. dent subjects received positive feedback, which either was made
AUTONOMY SUPPORT VERSUS CONTROL 1029
controlling through the use of additional words such as should about a neutral classroom scene using a projective technique.
(e.g., "Excellent, you did just as you should") or was noncon- The researchers then rated the stories for thematic content. Re-
trolling. Again, results revealed that the subsequent intrinsic sults revealed that children who perceived their own classrooms
motivation of subjects who received positive feedback in an au- to be autonomy promoting wrote about teachers who sup-
tonomy-supportive context was significantly greater than that ported autonomy and children who were more self-determined,
of subjects who received it in a controlling context. whereas children who perceived their classrooms to be control-
Finally, Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, and Holt (1984) argued ling wrote stories with control themes. Furthermore, children
that it is even possible to constrain behavior in a way that will who wrote about controlling classrooms projected more aggres-
tend to be experienced as noncontrolling. In a field experiment sion into the classrooms than did children who portrayed the
with first- and second-grade children, limits were set regarding classroom as less controlling. Contrary to the common view
the children's being neat while painting a picture. Limits seem that controls should be used to curb aggression, these results
to be controlling by nature, yet they may be perceived as less suggest that the aggression of children may be linked to their
controlling if they are set in a way that minimizes the use of feeling controlled.
control-related locution and acknowledges the probable con-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
flict between what the limits require and what the person would between interpersonal contexts in work organizations and the
want to do. The importance of the last point is that this ac- attitudes and perceptions of employees in those environments.
knowledgement conveys an appreciation of the perspective of To do this, they developed a psychometric instrument to assess
the actor, thus decreasing his or her experience of being con- managers' orientations toward supporting autonomy versus
trolled. As expected, Koestner et al. found that children who controlling behavior. The instrument was conceptually analo-
received noncontrolling limits maintained their intrinsic moti- gous and structurally similar to the measure of teachers' orien-
vation for painting (it did not differ from a no-limits compari- tations used by Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, and Ryan (1981).
son group), whereas those who received controlling limits The most salient finding in this study was that managers' orien-
showed significantly less intrinsic motivation. tations were strongly related to subordinates' level of trust. Sub-
ordinates with control-oriented managers had less trust in the
Other Effects of Autonomy-Supportive Versus corporation and its top management than did those with auton-
Controlling Contexts omy-supporting managers. This was particularly interesting be-
cause the data were collected in a large, geographically dis-
The studies just reviewed all used the dependent variable of
persed corporation, where most subjects had never met the cor-
intrinsic motivationassessed by the free-choice method, by
porate officers. Although the data were merely correlational,
self-reports of interest-enjoyment, or by Barter's (1981) ques-
they suggest that the interpersonal context created by one's im-
tionnaire measure for childrento establish that the functional
mediate manager may affect one's feelings and attitudes not
significance of interpersonal contexts can be either more auton-
only about the immediate environment but also about the
omy supportive or more controlling. Numerous studies of au-
whole organization.
tonomy-supportive versus controlling contexts have used other
Creativity and spontaneity. Koestner et al. (1984) used
dependent variables. Ryan et al. (1983), for example, also as-
Amabile's (1983) consensual assessment system to rate the
sessed experiences of pressure and tension and found, as one
paintings of 6- and 7-year-old children who had been given ei-
would expect from the definition of control, that subjects in a
ther autonomy-supportive or controlling limits. Results indi-
controlling context experienced greater pressure and tension
cated that children who painted with autonomy-oriented limits
than did those in a noncontrolling context.
were judged to have more creative and technically better paint-
Self-esteem, perceived competence, and aggression. In the
ings than were children who painted with controlling limits.
classroom studies reported earlier, the children also completed
The former children also showed greater spontaneity and less
Harter's (1982) measure of perceived cognitive competence and
self-esteem. Deci, Nezlek, and Sheinman (1981) reported sig- constriction in their paintings than did the latter. Whether an
nificant positive correlations between teachers* autonomy sup- event itself or the context within which an event occurs tends to
port and children's perceived cognitive competence and self- have a controlling functional significance, the behavior associ-
esteem. Furthermore, Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, and Ryan ated with it is likely to be less creative and more constricted.
(1981) reported that children in autonomy-supportive class- Creativity, it seems, is fostered by events and contexts that sup-
rooms increased in perceived competence and self-esteem dur- port autonomy.
ing the first 2 months of a school year relative to children in Preference for choice. In an experiment by Haddad (1982),
controlling classrooms. Finally, a study by Ryan and Grolnick 10- and 11-year-old children worked with age-appropriate ana-
(1986) found positive correlations between children's percep- grams. Half the children were given positive feedback with con-
tions of the classroom as being autonomy supportive and their trolling locution, and half were given noncontrolling positive
own perceived cognitive competence and self-esteem. With feedback. Subsequently, the children were told that they would
these three different research strategies, researchers found that be doing four more anagrams. Furthermore, they were told thai
when the interpersonal context of children's learning was au- they could select none, some, or all four of the ones they would
tonomy supportive, the children perceived themselves to be work on and that the experimenter would select the rest. Results
more competent in their cognitive activity and felt better about of the study indicated that when children had been controlled,
themselves than when the context was controlling. they said they wanted to make fewer of the choices than when
Ryan and Grolnick (1986) also had the children create stories they had not, though this was primarily true for girls. It seems
1030 EDWARD L. DECI AND RICHARD M. RYAN
that the girls, when they were controlled, became more prone encouraged them to take initiative, to be more autonomous and
to allowing others to make their future choices for them. self-determining. This can be contrasted with a study by Schulz
Behavior. Deci, Spiegel, Ryan, Koestner. and Kauffman (1976) in which elderly residents were given control over the
(1982) did a study of teaching behavior in which they created a hours they would be visited by volunteers in a visitation pro-
more autonomy-supportive versus a more controlling context gram. That intervention did not, however, encourage autono-
for subjects whose job it was to teach other subjects how to solve mous initiation and self-determination. The results did indicate
spatial relations puzzles. The controlling context was created short-term positive health effects for having control over out-
by emphasizing to the teachers that it was their responsibility comes, but a follow-up study (Schulz & Hanusa. 1978) showed
to see to it that their students performed up to high standards that after the visitation program was terminated, the subjects
in the puzzle solving. This was expected to be experienced by who had had control over outcomes evidenced significant de-
the teachers as pressure toward particular outcomes and thus to clines in health. Apparently, it is only when people learn to ex-
have a functional significance of control. perience their environment as supporting self-determination,
The 20-min teaching sessions that followed were tape-re- only when they become more autonomous (rather than merely
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
corded and subsequently analyzed by raters. The analyses re- perceiving that they have control over outcomes), that there will
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
vealed remarkable differences in the behavior of the two groups be long-term positive effects on their health.
of teachers. Those who taught in a controlling context made All of the research thus far reported has focused on the effects
about three times as many utterances, and many more of their of inputs from the environment, whether specific events or in-
utterances tended to be directives and to contain such control- terpersonal contexts. From these social psychological investiga-
ling words or phrases as should, have to, must, and ought to than tions, there is indication that when contextual factors function
was the case for those who taught in a less controlling context. to support autonomy rather than to control, people tend to be
In addition, raters judged those who taught in the controlling more intrinsically motivated, more creative, more cognitively
context to be more controlling in their teaching behavior than flexible, more trusting, more positive in emotional tone, and
those who taught in the less controlling context. more healthy; they tend to have higher self-esteem, perceived
Teachers who had received the controlling induction pro- competence, and preference for choice; their behavior tends to
ceeded from one puzzle to another, giving the solutions, as if be appropriately persistent and to be less controlling; and they
rote memorization of solutions to specific problems was the project less aggression. We turn now to studies that have fo-
route to learning problem solving. Teachers in the other group cused on person variables rather than contextual variables:
allowed their students to experiment with their own solutions. studies that are considered more in the province of personality.
These teachers gave hints, but they seldom gave solutions. As a
result of the different teaching, the students performed differ- Person Factors
ently. Those with controlling teachers assembled twice as many Two sets of studies have focused on person factors. The first
puzzles as those with autonomy-supporting teachers, but they is composed of laboratory experiments on intrapersonal events
independently solved only one fifth as many puzzles. or statesperson processes such as ego involvementthat can
In sum, the results suggest that when people are pressured be characterized as being either autonomy supportive or con-
to make others perform, they themselves tend to become more trolling. The second is composed of individual difference stud-
controlling. That in turn has negative consequences for the self- ies that focus primarily on causality orientations, which are
determination of people they relate to. people's tendencies to orient toward events and contexts that
Health. Langer and Rodin (1976) reported a study of the in- are autonomy supportive and those that are controlling.
stitutionalized aged in which an ambience that promoted self-
determinationwhat we call an autonomy-supportive inter- Intrapersonal Events: Autonomy Versus Control
personal contextwas created for some of the residents. The Many of the inputs relevant to the initiation and regulation
intervention included a meeting devoted to discussing the resi- of behavior are intrapsychic and can be independent of external
dents' taking greater responsibility for themselves (vs. telling circumstances. Imagine, for example, a colleague who is lying
them that they would be well cared for by the staff), the oppor- on the beach with his or her mind idly wandering. An idea for a
tunity to make choices about when they would attend a movie new experiment spontaneously occurs to the person, so with
(vs. being assigned a time), and being given the gift of a plant excitement he or she begins to design the experiment. The event
that they were responsible to care for (vs. being given a plant that prompted the behavior was an internal, cognitive-affective
that the staff would take care of for them). event that could be characterized as autonomous. But one could
Results of this study indicated that those elderly residents in easily imagine the person, while on vacation, designing an ex-
the context that emphasized self-determination improved on periment out of an internal obligation, with the pressured feel-
both questionnaire and behavioral measures of well-being rela- ing that he or she has to do an experiment to prove his or her
tive to those who lived in a context that did not. In an 18-month worth. This event would also be intrapersonal, but it would be
follow-up study, Rodin and Langer (1977) reported that there controlling. We predict that the consequences of these two types
were still significant differences in well-being such that those of internal events, which prompted the same overt behaviors,
residents whose self-determination had been supported were would be quite different and would have parallels to the conse-
healthier than the other residents. quences of the two types of external events.5
The Langer and Rodin study is often discussed as a study of
5
control over outcomes; however, it went beyond merely provid- Internal events can also be amotivating, though again they are not
ing control. The intervention not only gave residents control; it germane to the current discussion.
AUTONOMY SUPPORT VERSUS CONTROL 1031
Although this hypothesis has received less empirical atten- tion leading to the lowest level of intrinsic motivation and the
tion than the hypotheses discussed earlier, several studies have mirror condition to the next lowest. Both self-consciousness
supported it. Ryan (1982) argued that the state of ego involve- groups differed significantly from the non-self-consciousness,
ment as described by Sherif and Cantril (1947), a condition comparison group.
where people's self-esteem is hinged on performance, leads the Plant and Ryan (1985) had also premeasured subjects on
people to pressure themselves in a way similar to the way exter- public self-consciousness (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975),
nal forces can pressure them. He suggested that this type of ego the dispositional tendency to view oneself as if through the eyes
involvement is controlling and will thus undermine self-deter- of another. They found that this variable was also negatively
mination. In his study, college students worked on hidden-fig- related to intrinsic motivation, presumably because the nature
ures puzzles. Half of them were told that hidden-figures perfor- of one's self-focus is directly related to the relative autonomy ol
mance reflects creative intelligence and as such is used in some behavioral regulation, with public self-consciousness relating to
IQ tests. These subjects, being students in a competitive univer- a more controlling form of self-focus. The theoretical links be-
sity, were expected to become quite ego involved and thus to be tween aspects of public self-consciousness, conformity, and so-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
internally controlling. The other subjects were given a more cial control await further explication.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
such as "I'd find it valuable to . . ." or "I'd be interested in Behaviors, Attitudes, and Emotions
. . ." Accordingly, we predict that the qualities associated with
Causality orientations have also been correlated with a vari-
external controlling events and with external autonomy-sup-
ety of behavioral and attitudinal measures. King (1984) used
portive events will also be associated with their intrapsychic
the autonomy scores of 50 people who were scheduled for vol-
counterparts.
untary cardiac surgery to predict the extent to which they
would view the experience as a challenge rather than a threat.
She found that the higher the patients' autonomy scores, the
Causality Orientations
more their preoperative attitudes involved challenge rather than
threat and the more their postoperative attitudes were positive.
Elsewhere, we suggested that people have general orientations
In a spontaneous-learning study (Ryan, Connell, Plant, Rob-
regarding what they attend to and how they initiate and regulate
inson, & Evans, 1985), subjects who had completed the Causal-
their behavior (Deci & Ryan, 1985a). These orientations are
ity Orientations Scale read a passage and used the Differential
conceptualized with respect to the autonomy-control distinc-
Emotions Scale (Izard, Dougherty, Bloxom, & Kotsch, 1974) to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Type A coronary-prone behavior pattern (Jenkins, Rosenman, opportunity to make inputs. Control scores were also negatively
related to workers' satisfaction with job security and with the
& Friedman, 1967), which represents a pressured, competitive,
opportunity to make inputs.
ego-involved mode of behaving. It also correlated with public
To summarize, person factors, whether studied in terms of
self-consciousness (Fenigstein et al., 197S), which measures the
specific internal events using an experimental paradigm or in
tendency to view oneself as if from the outside. Parenthetically,
terms of general causality orientations using an individual
recall that Plant and Ryan (1985) reported a negative correla-
tion between public self-consciousness and intrinsic motivation difference paradigm, have been shown to be related to the dis-
tinction between self-determined versus controlled behavior in
for an experimental task. Finally, the control orientation was
moderately correlated with external locus of control (Rotter,
1966), although Rotter's external control is conceptually more 7
A third orientation, the impersonal orientation, refers to the ten-
similar to and empirically more strongly corrrelated with the dency to orient to amotivating inputs, in other words, the tendency to
third causality orientation, namely the impersonal orientation. experience oneself as being incompetent to attain desired outcomes.
AUTONOMY SUPPORT VERSUS CONTROL 1033
ways that parallel the relation of external events and contexts to In these reciprocal ways, whether someone in the environ-
the two types of behavior. As such, it seems that both contextual ment (e.g., a teacher or manager) attempts to control a person
and person factors can be analyzed in corresponding ways and (or to support a person's autonomy) or whether the person ori-
that the parallel findings from these analyses provide multi- ents toward others in the context who are controlling (or auton-
method validation of the self-determined versus controlled dis- omy supportive), transactional patterns develop between the
tinction. person and his or her social context.
From this perspective, one can see that the segregation of so-
Persons and Contexts cial psychological studies from personality studies is often arbi-
trary. The person is an active perceiver and an intentional be-
Much of the research related to this issue of autonomy and haver who acts in accord with a constructed view of the social
control in human behavior has focused on contextual factors. context. That construction is influenced by both contextual and
Yet the theory emphasizes that the functional significance of a personal factors and may in turn actually affect how the social
contextual factor, rather than its objective characteristics, is the context responds to the person.
critical consideration in predicting the effects of that factor.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
which case it would be autonomous), or one could be pressured positive feedback about their performance on it. For one group
toward a goal (in which case it would be controlled). the context was autonomy supportive, and for the other it was
This highlights an important definitional matter regarding controlling. Results indicated that the autonomy-supportive
intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. What distinguishes the context led to greater internalization of task value and greater
two is merely a Ideological aspect, whether the behavior is done persistence than did the controlling context and that internal-
for its inherent satisfaction (intrinsic) or is done in order to ob- ization was positively correlated with experienced self-determi-
tain a separable goal. Although this distinction has historical nation.
and practical importance (see Deci & Ryan, 1985b), it does not These studies suggest that extrinsically motivated behaviors
fully or adequately explicate the psychology of behavioral regu- can become self-determined through the process of integration
lation because extrinsic or goal-oriented activity can vary con- and that the integrative process itself depends on the context's
siderably in terms of the degree to which it is autonomously having an autonomy-supportive functional significance. In
regulated or controlled. such cases the behavior is still extrinsically motivated, however,
As an example, consider a person who derives considerable because the activity is still engaged in for reasons other than its
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
aesthetic pleasure from having a clean house but who does not inherent interest.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
latory factors are inside or outside the person. In motivational can therefore clarify how authority relationships influence indi-
terms, factors inside the person are always involved in inten- viduals' behavior, development, and experience. When consid-
tional behavior. However, all intentional behavior can be char- ered in terms of personality psychology, the autonomy-control
acterized as varying in the degree of relative autonomy, at one distinction is also very important for understanding behavior,
extreme having an external perceived locus of causality and at development, and experience. It helps to clarify individual
the other having an internal perceived locus of causality. For us, differences in selecting and responding to social situations, and
an internal perceived locus of causality describes the experience it adds a qualitative dimension to the psychology of self-control.
of an action's being one's own and being freely undertaken, The general framework offered herein thus highlights some
whereas an external perceived locus of causality describes the ways in which the enigma of human choice and autonomy can
experience of having to do something, of being compelled by be explored empirically to help explicate the dynamic interac-
heteronomous forces. Contextual factors as well as person fac- tion between persons and contexts.
tors can have either an autonomy-supportive or a controlling
functional significance and can therefore promote either an in- References
ternal or an external perceived locus of causality.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Deci, E. L., & Cascio, W. F. (1972, April). Changes in intrinsic motiva- Harackiewicz, J. (1979). The effects of reward contingency and perfor-
tion as a function of negative feedback and threats. Paper presented mance feedback on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and
at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston. Social Psychology, 37. 1352-1363.
Deci, E. L., Cascio, W. E, & Krusell, J. (1975). Cognitive evaluation Harackiewicz, J., Abrahams, S., & Wageman, R. (1987). Performance
theory and some comments on the Calder, Staw critique. Journal of evaluation and intrinsic motivation: The effects of evaluative focus,
Personality and Social Psychology, 31. 81-85. rewards, and achievement orientation. Journal oj'Personality and So-
Deci, E. L., Connell, 1. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1986). Self-determination in cial Psychology, 53, 1015-1023.
a work organization. Unpublished manuscript, University of Roches- Harackiewicz, J., Manderlink, G., & Sansone, C. (1984). Rewarding
ter, Rochester, NY. pinball wizardry: Effects of evaluation and cue-valence on intrinsic
Deci, E. L., Nezlek, J., & Sheinman, L. (1981). Characteristics of the interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 47, 287-300.
rewarder and intrinsic motivation of the rewardee. Journal of Person- Harackiewicz, J., Manderlink, G., & Sansone, C. (in press). Compe-
ality and Social Psychology, 40, 1-10. tence processes and achievement motivation: Implications for intrin-
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrin- sic motivation. In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement
sically motivated processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in exper- and motivation: A social-developmental perspective. New York: Cam-
imental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39-80). New York: Academic bridge University Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
adult surveillance and extrinsic rewards on children's intrinsic moti- Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1986). When free-choice behavior is not
vation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 479-486. intrinsically motivated: Experiments on internally controlling regula-
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining chil- tion. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester, Rochester,
dren's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the "overjus- NY.
tification" hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns in the class-
28, 129-137. room: Self-report and projective assessments of individual differences
Lewin, K. (1951). Intention, will, and need. InD. Rapaport(Ed.), Orga- in children's perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
nization and pathology of thought (pp. 95-153). New York: Columbia ogy, 50, 550-558.
University Press. Ryan, R. M., Mims, V., & Koestner, R. (1983). Relation of reward con-
Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. tingency and interpersonal context to intrinsic motivation: A review
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of and test using cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and
Einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54(6, Whole No. 248). Social Psychology, 45, 736-750.
Maehr, M. L., & Stallings, W. M. (1972). Freedom from external evalua- Sansone, C. (1986). A question of competence: The effects of compe-
tion. Child Development, 43, 177-185. tence and task feedback on intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality
McGraw, K. Q, & McCullers, J. C. (1979). Evidence of a detrimental and Social Psychology, 57,918-931.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Bien plus que des documents.
Découvrez tout ce que Scribd a à offrir, dont les livres et les livres audio des principaux éditeurs.
Annulez à tout moment.