Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17
European Journal of Personality, Vol. 10, 61-77 (1996) Evidence for the Big Five in analyses of familiar English personality adjectives GERARD SAUCIERt Califomia State University, San Bernardino, CA, USA and LEWIS R, GOLDBERG University of Oregon and Oregon Research Institute, Eugene, OR, USA Abstract Studies of the natural language are a printe source of the Big-Five model, yet the factor ‘analysis of @ large, representative, and non-clustered set of English-language ‘personality adjectives in a large sample has not yet been published. In order to test the hypothesis that finding the Big Five depends on biasing the variable selection with ‘an investigator's preferred non-familiar terms, we present the factor analysis of 435 familiar adjectives in a combined sample (N=699) of 507 self- and 392 peer ratings. The five-factor solution reproduced the Big Five with high clarity, demonstrating generally very high correlations with Goldberg's adjective markers of the Big Five. The Intellect factor had a more moderate correlation, due to its de-emphasis of the ‘creativity components of Factor V, a phenomenon that may occur commonly with the lexical Intellect factor. INTRODUCTION ‘An increasing number of researchers have adopted the view that phenotypic personality variation is most meaningfully distinguished in terms of five broad orthogonal factors, labeled the Big Five (Digman, 1990; Goldberg, 1990, 1993; John, 1990; Ostendorf, 1990). These factors demonstrate robustness not only within data sets (see e.g. Ostendorf, 1990), and between self and peer ratings (e.g. Goldberg, 1990; Ostendorf, 1990), but even between languages. That is, ‘emic’ or within-language ‘This work was supported by grant MH-49227 from the National Institute of Mental Health, U. S. Public Health Service. ACorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerard, Saucier, Department of Psychology, California State University San Bernardino, 5500 University Parkway, San Bernardino, CA 92407-2397, USA. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to gsaucier@wiley.csusb edu, CCC 0890-2070/96/010061-17 Received 16 October 1995 © 1996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 18 December 1995 62 G. Saucier and L. R. Goldberg studies of personality descriptors from lexical sources, using exploratory factor analysis, generally converge on these five factors. Convergence has so far been clearest in studies utilizing German (Ostendorf, 1990), Czech (Hrebickova, Ostendorf and Angleitner, 1995), and Dutch (De Raad, Mulder, Kloosterman and Hofstee, 1988); convergence was also quite strong in studies of Hungarian (De Rad and Szirmak, 1994), Russian (Shmelyov and Pokhil’ko, 1993), and Filipino (Church, Reyes, Katigbak and Grimm, 1995). The present report addresses and documents the lexical Big Five in English. ‘Although five-factor models have been developed, following a partially independent course, in the questionnaire domain (McCrae and Costa, 1987), and in studies of scales developed by Cattell (Digman and Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Norman, 1963; Tupes and Christal, 1961), lexical studies provide the most powerful evidence in support of the Big Five, Personality-questionnaire items sample from the ‘population’ of phrases and sentences, a virtually infinite population that cannot be representatively sampled. Commonly used single words descriptive of personality are a more finite population, more amenable to representative sampling. Adjectives are particularly advantageous because they richly include both desirable and undesirable attributes, whereas personality type-nouns (at least in English) make reference to relatively fewer desirable attributes. ‘A representative sampling of personality-descriptive terms, especially adjectives, might yield a representative sampling of personality attributes. This rather straightforward psychometric rationale is consonant with the lexical hypothesis, which states that (j) the most distinctive, significant, and widespread phenotypic attributes tend to become encoded as single words in the conceptual reservoir of language, and (ii) the degree of representation of an attribute in language tends to correspond to the relative importance of the attribute (Saucier and Goldberg, 1995). Because expert personality-attribute distinctions might go beyond those emphasized in everyday transactions (Block, 1995), representative sampling by way of the lexical approach can provide necessary, though not sufficient, features for an optimal representation of personality attributes (Goldberg and Saucier, 1995). Lexical studies of English-language personality descriptors began with Allport and Odbert (1936); they extracted 17954 ‘trait names’ from an unabridged dictionary and provided a four-column classification of these terms, with approximately 4500 terms falling into their ‘stable trait” column. About three decades later, Norman (1967) retraced the footsteps of Allport and Odbert, examining a new edition of the same unabridged dictionary and extracting 171 more trait terms. Using various criteria—difficulty, ambiguity, metaphoricalness, and reference to states, activities, social evaluations, and overt characteristics rather than traits—Norman’s four-person research team produced a list of 2797 terms for stable traits, including 608 ‘prime terms’ of particularly low difficulty, colloquiality, and awkwardness. Goldberg (1976; 1982) culled Norman's 2797-term stable-trait pool to devise a set of 1710 terms by eliminating nouns, as well as adjectives that were difficult, that referred to ‘nonhuman and/or evil motives or behaviors’ (Goldberg, 1982, p. 209), or that were considered ambiguous or intuitively unsvitable. A further culling on grounds of ambiguity, difficulty, slanginess, sex-linkage, over-evaluation, metaphoricalness, and redundancy left 492 trait terms. Although they were never analyzed as a distinct variable selection, these 492 ‘Norman-Goldberg’ terms form Personality adjective Big Five 63 the major part of a 540-adjective inventory that Goldberg and colleagues drew on for major studies of the Big-Five structure (Goldberg, 1990; 1992; Hofstee, De Raad and Goldberg, 1992; Saucier, 1992a) and the development of adjective measures of the Big Five using adjective clusters (Goldberg, 1990) and single (unipolar) adjectives (Goldberg, 1992). Saucier (1994a) developed a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar- adjective measure. However, both the 492- and $40-term sets include some terms that are relatively unfamiliar to many subjects (e.g. Imperturbable, Indefatigable). What comes out of a factor analysis depends largely on what goes into it, and Block (1995) has pointed to the hazards of experts inadvertently ‘prestructuring’ their data sets. If such prestructuring were to occur in lexical studies, it would be exhibited partly in the retention of certain variables despite their low frequency of use in everyday transactions; in other words, the expert may add certain ‘expert variables’, or cluster the variables in a biased manner, and thereby affect the results. One useful corrective for this inadvertent trend is to collect data on the familiarity of the terms from a sample of judges. Eliminating the less familiar terms should skim off the ‘expert’ variables and reduce the potential for bias in variable selection. If finding the Big Five is dependent on investigators’ prestructuring their data sets, as Block (1995) has suggested, the Big Five should not emerge clearly to the extent that variable selection is left in the hands of judges other than the investigator, and there is no clustering of terms. In this study, we test this hypothesis. We delineate the most familiar subset within the sets of 492 and 540 adjectives, and examine the factor structure of this unclustered subset. If the ‘expert-bias hypothesis’ is correct, the Big- Five factor structure will be less clear in this subset than in the larger sets that contain possible ‘expert bias’. METHOD The familiar subset of adjectives We first utilized familiarity ratings of the 540 adjectives provided by eight men and 17 women, students in an undergraduate psychology course at an ethnically diverse university in the western United States (Saucier, 1995). The 540 adjectives were included in a collection of 843 ‘stable-trait terms,’ including also Norman's 608 prime terms for stable traits, plus a few other adjectives included in neither list but studied elsewhere (Saucier, 1994b; Saucier and Goldberg, 1994). The terms were rated on a 0-9 rating scale, with 0 indicating that the judge had no knowledge of the term, | indicating it was ‘Never’ used, and 9 indicating it was used ‘Extremely often’ to describe a person. The interrater alpha coefficient was 0.96 (mean interrater correlation =0.51) for these ratings. To enhance comparisons with a particularly influential and thorough previous study, we sought a set of variables of approximately equal size to Ostendorf’s (1990) prime set of 430 German disposition adjectives. Beginning with the 492 Norman~ Goldberg trait terms, we removed those 57 adjectives with a mean familiarity rating (in the work of Saucier (1995)) of less than 3.25, yielding a more compact and familiar set of 435 adjectives.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi