Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 15

Journal of Early Intervention

http://jei.sagepub.com

Older Siblings' Use of Responsive Interaction Strategies and Effects on Their


Younger Siblings with Down Syndrome
Alacia Trent-Stainbrook, Ann P. Kaiser and Jennifer R. Frey
Journal of Early Intervention 2007; 29; 273
DOI: 10.1177/105381510702900401

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/29/4/273

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children

Additional services and information for Journal of Early Intervention can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jei.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/29/4/273

Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009


REGULAR ARTICLE

Older Siblings Use of Responsive Interaction


Strategies and Effects on Their Younger
Siblings with Down Syndrome
ALACIA TRENT-STAINBROOK, ANN P. KAISER AND
JENNIFER R. FREY
Vanderbilt University

The effects of an intervention designed to facilitate interactions between older typically developing
siblings and their younger siblings with Down syndrome were investigated. Older siblings were
taught to use two responsive interaction strategies through the use of written materials, modeling,
role play, and oral feedback. Following training, older siblings increased their use of mirroring and
verbal responding. Intentional communicative behaviors increased among their younger siblings.
One-month follow-up observations indicated that older siblings maintained their use of the
responsive interaction strategies, but the effects of the intervention did not appear to generalize to
an untrained setting. In an assessment of social validity, blind observers found sibling interactions to

appear more positive and reciprocal following intervention than during baseline.

Social interaction skills are prerequisite for them to become more responsive and less
communicating with others. While typically dominant and directive during interactions
developing children have many opportunities with their children. Studies examining the
for learning and using social skills in their effects of training parents consistently report
daily lives, children with developmental that mothers are capable of learning to use
delays might be unable to engage in such responsive interaction strategies and that
learning opportunities due to their less de- their use of these strategies promotes the
veloped social skills (Girolametto, 1988). language and social development of their
Social and communication skills in young young children with developmental delays
children with disabilities might be facilitated and disabilities (Girolametto, 1988; Kaiser et
by increasing opportunities for these children al., 1996).
to participate in social interactions and by Siblings have also been taught to imple-
improving the quality of naturally occurring ment responsive interaction strategies with
interactions. Responsive interaction strate- their younger siblings with Down syndrome
gies, for example, are effective for promoting (Trent, Kaiser, & Wolery, 2005). In that
social interactions and engagement between study, the effects of an intervention designed
young children with developmental delays to facilitate interactions between two older
and their interaction partners (Weiss, 1981; typically developing siblings and their youn-
Girolametto, 1988). Teaching parents to use ger siblings with Down syndrome were
responsive interaction strategies encourages investigated using a multiple baseline design

273
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
across behaviors. Following intervention, when their attention is focused on the actions
older siblings demonstrated the ability to of the sibling with a disability than when
use the
responsive interaction strategies their attention is focused primarily on their
during play and modest changes were ob- own actions and play agendas. Verbal
served in the communicative behaviors of responding was defined as contingent verbal
their siblings with Down syndrome. responsiveness and verbal commenting fol-
The primary purpose of this study was to lowing a pause in turn-taking. Contingent
replicate the study by Trent et al. (2005) on verbal responding balances sibling turns and
teaching older, typically developing siblings allows the child with a disability a number of
two responsive interaction strategies (mirror- opportunities to initiate and respond.
ing and verbal responding) in the context of
play sessions with their younger siblings with
Down syndrome. The second purpose was to METHOD
investigate the effects of the sibling-imple-
mented responsive interaction intervention Participants
on the intentional communicative behavior The participants in this study were three
of the children with Down syndrome. Trent sibling dyads, each including a younger
and colleagues investigated only the effects of sibling (hereafter referred to as younger
the intervention on the verbal behaviors of sibling) and an older typically developing
the children with Down syndrome. Because sibling (hereafter referred to as older sibling).
many children with disabilities often com- Demographic data on the older siblings and
municate with nonverbal forms of language, their younger siblings are shown in Table 1.
the current study included verbal and non- All sibling dyads met the prespecified criteria
verbal intentional communication measures for inclusion in the study. To be included,
of child communication. Finally, a third older siblings had to be between 6 and
12 years of age, willing to participate in the
purpose for conducting this study was to
extend the study by Trent et al. (2005) by study and sign an assent form. Younger
evaluating the effects of the intervention on siblings were required to be between the ages
both the older siblings and those with Down of 5 and 11 years and have significant
syndrome in an untrained, generalization language delays. Mothers of each sibling
setting (i.e., preparing a snack). As most dyad provided written consent for their own
siblings do not spend all of their time playing and their childrens participation in the
together, it was important to determine study.
whether the effects of the intervention Significant language delays of younger
generalized to other settings. siblings were documented by scores from
The responsive interaction intervention the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT;
used in this study was adapted from the Dunn & Dunn, 1997), Expressive Vocabulary
procedures used in previous studies (Kaiser Test (EVT; Williams, 1997), Preschool Lan-
& Delaney, 1998; Kaiser, Hancock, & guage Scale(PLS-3; Zimmerman, Steiner, &
Hester, 1998). The two features of responsive Pond, 1992), and the Leiter International
interaction, nonverbal mirroring and verbal Performance Scale - Revised (Leiter-R; Roid
responding, were selected for intervention as & Miller, 1997). These assessment measures
they appeared to be foundational approaches provided information about the childs re- I
for promoting reciprocal interactions. Mir- ceptive language skills, expressive language
roring, defined as the contingent imitation of skills, auditory and expressive communica-
nonverbal behavior, requires the older sibling tion skills, and nonverbal intellectual ability.
to attend to the nonverbal behaviors of the Additionally, younger siblings mean length
sibling with a disability. The older siblings of utterances (MLU) and diversity of vocab-
might be more likely to make activity- ulary were calculated from a 20-min language
relevant comments and contingent responses sample. Younger siblings scores on the

274
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
Table 1
Characteristics of Older Siblings and Younger Siblings at Baseline

Note. Leiter-R Leiter International Performance Scale - Revised; EVT


=
Expressive Vocabulary Test; PPVT
= =

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; PLS-3 Preschool Language Scale, Third Edition; MLU mean length of utterance
= =

based on a 20-min language sample; Diversity diversity of vocabulary based on a 20-min language sample; Scores on
=

EVT, PPVT, and PLS-3 are standard scores. *Leiter, EVT, and PLS-3 scores were not available for Younger Sibling 3.

assessment tools, MLU, and diversity of intervention sessions but were asked to
vocabulary are reported in Table 1. remain quiet throughout the session.
During training sessions, children in all
Trainers dyads played with toys provided by the
trainer. The older siblings chose the toys for
Training sessions were conducted by the first each session from a list of choices provided
author (Trainer 1) and a research assistant
(Trainer 2). Trainer 2 was trained by Trainer by the trainer. Toys and activities for the play
sessions met the following criteria: (a)
1 to implement the responsive interaction
allowed for two participants to play; (b)
intervention with siblings. Training consisted
were noncompetitive in nature (e.g., no
of weekly meetings that included discussion,
board games, video games, or card games);
coaching, and feedback plus observations of
Trainer 1 working with other sibling dyads. (c) could be used in the designated play area;
and (d) included at least two of each toy so
both siblings could have one. The most
Setting and Materials frequently selected activities were block play,
All observation and training sessions oc- dress-up, bubbles, play dough, painting,
curred in the homes of the participants. water play, and race cars.
Observations were videotaped with a digital
camera. Each session lasted 30 to 60 min and Design and Procedures
consisted of the trainer training and pro- We used a multiple baseline design across

viding feedback to the older sibling followed behaviors (McReynold & Kearns, 1983)
by a 10-min play segment; the 10-min play replicated across three sibling dyads. The
segment was videotaped for data collection. study was divided into four consecutive
Most sessions were conducted in the families conditions: (a) baseline, (b) nonverbal mir-
living room area. Televisions and radios were roring training, (c) verbal responsiveness
turned off and the children were asked to training, and (d) follow up. Each treatment
remain in the room they had selected during condition continued until the older siblings
experimental sessions. Generalization ses- data were higher than baseline levels for at
sions lasted 10 min and were held during least three consecutive sessions. Generaliza-
a snack time in the families kitchens. Parents tion probes occurred in baseline, mirroring,
and other siblings were permitted to watch and responding conditions.

275
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
Baseline. Baseline sessions were con- a 10-min session making it more difficult for
ducted twice each week at the childrens the older sibling to use the responsive
homes. During baseline observations, sib- interaction strategies. Furthermore, it often
lings were asked to play together for 10 min was difficult to make generalization sessions
with the toys provided by the trainer and to last a full 10 min (e.g., making a peanut
stay within the designated play area. No butter and jelly sandwich does not necessarily
other directions were given. The sessions require 10 min). To keep generalization
were similar to training sessions in that the session data comparable to treatment session
siblings were asked to stay in one room for data, the length of the session to be coded
the entire 10 min with the television and was kept at 5 min. After the play interaction,
radio off and with parents and other siblings the trainer provided positive and corrective
outside of the room. feedback to the older sibling while the older
Intervention. Intervention sessions were sibling, the younger sibling, and the trainer
conducted twice each week in the home and watched the video of the preceding play
each lasted 30 to 60 min. Intervention interaction. The session concluded with the
sessions were divided into three segments trainer and older sibling planning activities
during both conditions of intervention. The for the next intervention session.
first segment included either the trainer Older siblings were taught two responsive
teaching the older sibling one of the re- interaction strategies, nonverbal mirroring,
sponsive interaction strategies or reviewing and verbal responding. During mirroring
the previously taught strategies. The younger training, older siblings were taught to imitate
sibling was not present during this portion of the appropriate nonverbal behaviors of their
the intervention sessions. There were four younger siblings. To imitate the nonverbal
subcomponents to the teaching and review- play behaviors of the sibling with Down
ing portion of the intervention: (a) presenta- syndrome, older siblings had to use identical
tion of information by the trainer with the movements to manipulate separate but sim-
use of a Responsive Interaction Pictorial ilar toys as those manipulated by the younger
Manual developed for this project and avail- sibling. This contingent imitation could
able from the first author, (b) opportunity occur with or without an accompanying
for the older sibling to discuss the procedures verbalization. Imitation had to occur simul-
and ask questions, (c) use of modeling and taneously (e.g., the older sibling and the
role play to practice using the strategies, and younger sibling smashed play dough at the
(d) a second opportunity to discuss and ask same time; the younger sibling pretended to

questions. Typically, the teaching part of the feed a doll and the older sibling pretended to
sessions lasted about 20 min and was briefer feed a doll too).
during the latter portions of each interven- For verbal responding, siblings were
tion condition. The next 10 min involved taught contingent verbal responsiveness and
a play-based interaction between the older verbal commenting. For contingent verbal
sibling and the younger sibling. This in- responsiveness, the older siblings were taught
teraction was videotaped. While siblings were to respond verbally to both verbal and
asked to play together for 10 min, data were nonverbal acts of intentional communication
collected from the first full 5 min of the tape. performed by the younger siblings. To
The decision to code only 5 min of each simplify the intervention for the older sib-
session was made to keep data collection lings, they were taught to respond only
feasible and because the first full 5 min were following overt acts of intentional communi-
thought to be the most representative of the cation : (a) verbalizations by the younger
older siblings abilities to implement the sibling directed toward the older sibling, (b)
responsive interaction strategies. Younger attempts by the younger sibling to show the
siblings often became fatigued or less in- older sibling something by pointing to an
terested in a single activity toward the end of object or event, (c) attempts by the younger

276
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
sibling to give the older sibling an object, and a maximum of 10 praise statements per 5 min
(d) attempts by the younger sibling to session. Praise described the siblings appro-
communicate using sign language. Addition- priate behaviors (e.g. &dquo;good job mirroring,&dquo;
al, less overt acts of intentional communica- &dquo;great job responding to everything&dquo;).
tion (e.g., smile, reach) by the younger Generalization. Generalization sessions
siblings were coded and counted as commu- were conducted during approximately every
nication attempts, but the older siblings were fourth baseline and intervention session.
not expected to respond to them. Older During generalization sessions, siblings were
siblings were taught to respond by (a) asked to prepare a snack together. General-
verbally repeating any part of what their ization sessions were typically 10 to 15 min in
younger siblings said, or (b) verbally de- length. Generalization sessions were similar
scribing the activities in which the two to baseline sessions in that the trainer
siblings were participating. As part of the provided no prompts or praise.
responsiveness training, older siblings also Follow up. A follow-up session was con-
were taught to pause for at least 5 s after ducted approximately a month after comple-
each of their own verbal turns to allow their tion of the intervention. These sessions were
younger siblings opportunity to respond.
an identical to baseline sessions in which siblings
Correct verbal responding episodes were were asked to play together for 10 min with
scored when the older siblings responded to the toys that had been provided. No
a communicative attempt by their younger prompts, directions, or feedback were given
siblings and then paused for 5 s. during these sessions.
Older siblings also were taught to use
verbal comments. That is, they were taught Measures
to take another turn if their younger siblings Three classes of behavior were measured
did not attempt to communicate, either using the sibling interaction code: (a) use of
verbally or nonverbally, (a) within the 5 s, responsive interaction strategies by the older
or (b) following a verbal or nonverbal turn siblings, (b) acts of intentional communica-
taken by their younger siblings during the 5-s tion by the younger siblings, and (c) verbal
period. Older siblings were taught to com- prompts and praise provided by the trainer
ment by using descriptive statements about during training sessions. Specific codes for
something they themselves were doing in the the acts of intentional communication by the
activity, what their younger siblings were younger siblings were adapted from a code
doing, or what they were doing together. created by Yoder (2003). Observational data
Correct verbal commenting episodes were were collected on these behaviors by coding
scored when, following a 5-s pause, the older the videotapes of individual sessions. Video-
sibling made a descriptive comment followed tapes were viewed and scored using Proco-
by another 5-s pause. derDV (Tapp, 2003). Continuous event re-
Throughout the intervention, the trainer cording was used to measure all of the above
was present to provide prompts to the older behaviors except for mirroring. Momentary
siblings when necessary. Prompts consisted time sampling with 10-s intervals was used to
of verbal directives spoken aloud and were measure mirroring due to the difficulty in
limited to two prompts per min for a maxi- segmenting different acts of mirroring. Pro-
mum of 10 prompts per 5-min session. coderDV was used to code (a) frequencies of
Prompting was used to remind older siblings responding by the older siblings following
to mirror and verbally respond and to a turn by the younger sibling or a 5-s pause,

suggest ways of mirroring and verbally (b) frequencies of acts of intentional com-
responding. Praise for correct use of the munication by the younger siblings, (c)
intervention strategies was used throughout frequencies of training and praise by the
all intervention sessions. Praise statements trainers, and (d) the percent of intervals
were limited to 2 statements per min for during which mirroring took place.

277
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
Table 2
Mean Percent of Interobserver Agreement for Each Behavior

Older sibling percentage of responsiveness older younger siblings, and trainer


siblings,
was calculated by determining the number of prompts. A trained observer watched and
times the younger siblings had intentional coded data from the videos of all experimen-
communications and the number of times the tal sessions for all dyads. IOA was assessed
older siblings responded to acts of intentional on approximately 25% of the baseline, in-
communication within a 5 s window. The tervention, and follow-up sessions for each
percentage responsiveness was calculated by dyad by having a second observer indepen-
dividing the sum of older siblings responses dently code the tapes. The percentage of
to acts of intentional communication by the agreement was calculated for each category
sum of acts of intentional communication by of behavior using the formula: number of
the younger siblings and multiplying this agreements divided by the number of agree-
quotient by 100. ments plus disagreements, with the quotient
Intentional communication, the primary multiplied by 100. For interval coding,
measure for the younger siblings, was divided ProcoderDV was used to estimate interval-
into two pragmatic categories: (a) comments by-interval interobserver agreement. The
and (b) requests. Acts of intentional commu- percentage agreement was calculated for
nication included acts that required coordi- mirroring using the formula number of
nated attention (i.e., nonword vocalizations, agreements divided by the number of agree-
reach, clap, smile, contact point, and touch ments plus disagreements, with the quotient
older sibling) and acts that did not require multiplied by 100. Coders were trained to
coordinated attention (i.e., referential word criterion (85% IOA) prior to the start of the
or sign, conventional gesture, give, show, current study. Results of IOA assessments
extend upturned palm to older sibling, distal are shown in Table 2. The IOA ranged

point, or a movement of the older siblings between 64% and 100% for all behaviors
hand to an object). These acts were then for all dyads across baseline, mirroring
labeled as comments or requests, depending training, responding training, and follow-up
on their function. An act of intentional sessions. Lower rates of IOA often corre-
communication was coded as a comment if sponded with lower frequencies of behaviors
the apparent intent was to direct the older or the presence of new behaviors that were

siblings attention, share positive affect, or not yet defined by the code.
share interest. An act of intentional commu- The procedural fidelity of each trainers
nication was coded as a request if the implementation of the training procedures
apparent intent was to request an action, was assessed during 25% of each dyads

object, help, comfort, or a label, or to baseline and intervention sessions for a total
maintain turn-taking or an ongoing routine. of 14 assessments. To assess procedural
fidelity, the primary observer used a 14-item
Interobserver Agreement ..... checklist of the intervention training compo-
Interobserver agreement (IOA) data were nents (e.g., use of pictorial manual, role
collected on coding the behaviors of the play). Percentage fidelity was calculated

278
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
using the formula: number of items scored Data prompted and spontaneous strategy
on
correct divided by the total number of items use are presented, but not separately. After
observed, with the quotient multiplied by the older siblings received training in the use
100. Procedural fidelity ranged between 86% of mirroring as a responsive interaction
and 100% for the three dyads across mirror- strategy, they were able to use mirroring
ing training and responding training sessions during play sessions. The number of inter-
(M = 96%). vals in which mirroring was used was
relatively low for the older siblings (TS 1,
Social Validity TS2, and TS3) throughout the baseline
Following the completion of the study, 10 condition. With the introduction of mirror-
masters level students in special education ing training, older sibling 1 showed an
participated in an assessment of social immediate increase in level of mirroring use
validity. The students viewed 1-min clips of and an accelerating trend. Older sibling 3s
each dyad in a baseline session and in an use of mirroring remained near baseline

intervention session. Clips were taken from levels at the first session of mirroring
the first baseline sessions and the last training but showed a significant increase
intervention sessions for each dyad. The 10 on the second mirroring training session.
students watched the videos clips at the same The use of mirroring by older sibling 1 and
time; thus, each student observed the video older sibling 3 remained stable and above
clips in the same order. Without knowing baseline levels throughout the responding
which sessions were baseline and which were condition. Older sibling 2 showed an in-
intervention, the students completed a 12- crease in level of mirroring use beginning at

item questionnaire on the behavior and the second mirroring training session. Use of
language of the sibling dyads in each clip. mirroring by older sibling 2 remained
The questionnaire asked students to de- primarily above baseline levels throughout
termine to how much they agreed (i.e., the mirroring and verbal responding training
strongly agree, agree, disagree, strongly phases; there were only a few data points in
disagree, or cant tell) the older sibling was the verbal responding phase that overlapped
using responsive interaction strategies with with baseline data.
the younger siblings, the younger siblings The numbers of contingent verbal re-
were communicating with the older siblings, sponses and comments were low, with limited
and the siblings were enjoying spending time variability, for the three dyads during both
together and were interacting positively. baseline and mirroring training conditions.
Example items included &dquo;the target sibling With the introduction of responding training,
mirrors or imitates the actions of the child older sibling 1 and older sibling 3 showed an
with Down syndrome&dquo;, &dquo;the sibling with immediate increase in level and an accelerat-
Down syndrome takes verbal turns (e.g. ing trend in their use of responding and
intelligible comments and unintelligible descriptive commenting. Older sibling 2
utterances) during the interaction&dquo;, and showed an increase in level of responding
&dquo;both siblings are positively interacting with and descriptive commenting following the
each other&dquo;. second responding training session. This
change maintained throughout the condition,
RESULTS with limited variability.
Percentage of responsiveness. Percentages
Older Sibling Behavior of responsiveness were low for older sibling
Older sibling behaviors during baseline and 1, older sibling 2, and older sibling 3
training conditions are shown in Figure 1. throughout the baseline and mirroring con-
Means and standard deviations of older ditions, with some variability in performance
sibling use of mirroring and responding across dyads (see Table 3). Following the
across conditions are shown in Table 3. introduction of responding training, all

279
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
Figure 1.
Number of intervals in which the older siblings used mirroring and the number of verbal responses
during baseline, mirroring training, responding training, and follow-up sessions. Triangles indicate
data from generalization sessions.

I
dyads showed immediate increase in level
an Behaviors of the Younger Siblings
of responding that maintained above baseline Effects of older their
sibling training on
levels throughout the condition. Older sibling younger siblings evident but
were somewhat
3 also demonstrated an accelerating trend in different for each of the dyads. The behaviors
percentage of responsiveness across the of the younger siblings across baseline and
condition. training conditions are shown in Figure 2

280
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
1-n
to
-s
71~
6
a
~Z6)
t4
z
s

>
zzz~
a
Ct)
.5
=
6
a

7#
CS
a

&dquo;J
t:),
$
W
z
.8
z
~ ~
Q 2
9 ~ -It
281
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
and Table 3. The frequencies of commenting to have generalized to the snack setting.
were high for younger sibling 1 and younger Older siblings use of the responsive in-
sibling 3 throughout the baseline and mirror- teraction strategies remained near baseline
ing conditions. With the introduction of the levels during generalization sessions at all
responding condition, commenting initially conditions of the intervention. The behaviors
remained stable for younger sibling 1 and of the younger siblings during generalization
increased in level toward the end of in- sessions were variable and near baseline
tervention. Younger sibling 3 also showed levels as well.
a slight increase in level of commenting

during the responding condition. Comment- Trainer Prompts and Praise


ing remained relatively low for younger Although the data on siblings uses of the
sibling 2 throughout baseline and mirroring. responsive interaction strategies include both
Following the introduction of the responding prompted and spontaneous uses, data on
condition, younger sibling 2 showed an trainer prompts suggested the trainers used
increase in level of commenting, with some few prompts and praise statements to sup-
overlapping data points. Requesting re- port siblings throughout the intervention.
mained relatively low for all three younger The trainers did not prompt during baseline,
siblings throughout the study. generalization, or follow-up sessions; prompts
and praise were used by the trainer only in
Follow Up intervention sessions.
Data from a 1-month follow-up observation
indicated that older siblings were able to
Social Validity
maintain use of the responsive interaction
In an assessment of social validity following
strategies during that period (see Figure I the completion of the study, 10 masters level
and Table 3). The number of intervals during
students rated video clips of the sibling dyads
which mirroring was used by older siblings in
the follow-up sessions was above baseline
in intervention as more positive and re-
levels and similar to the levels observed ciprocal than baseline video clips. Scores on
the social validity assessment could range
during intervention. The number of times from a minimum score of 0 to a maximum
that the older siblings described or repeated
score of 48. The mean ratings of baseline
remained high for older sibling 1 and older
video clips were 24.4, 27.9, and 34.2 for Dyad
sibling 3, but decreased for older sibling 2.
Percentage responsiveness also remained 1, Dyad 2, and Dyad 3, respectively. Mean
high for older sibling 1 and older sibling 3. ratings of intervention clips were 46.8, 33.0,
It decreased to 0% for older sibling 2, but and 44.6 for Dyad 1, Dyad 2, and Dyad 3,

given the low rate of commenting and respectively. In addition to the social validity
assessment, informal interviews with the
requesting by younger sibling 2, older sibling mothers of the siblings and the older siblings
2 did not have many opportunities to re-
spond. For younger sibling 2 and younger suggest that the mothers and the older
sibling 3, rates of commenting in follow up siblings enjoyed participating in the study
show overlap with rates of commenting in and found it to be a useful learning experi-
ence.
baseline. Younger sibling 1s rate of com-
menting remained above baseline levels at the
follow up but decreased in comparison to DISCUSSION
rates of commenting at the end of the
responding training phase (see Figure 2). All older siblings learned the responsive
interaction techniques quickly and used them
Generalization in interactions with their younger siblings
Generalization data are shown in Table 3. during intervention. The measures of the
The effects of the intervention do not appear communicative performance of the children

282
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
2.
Figure
Number of comments and requests made the younger siblings during baseline, mirroring training,
by
responding training, and follow-up Squares indicate comments and circles indicate
sessions.
requests. Closed triangles indicate comments and the open triangles indicate requests during
generalization sessions.

with Down syndrome revealed a slight in- intervention, is encouraging given that the
crease in the number of comments made in frequency of commenting increased without
each session. The increase in commenting, direct
prompting. While all younger siblings
evident following implementation of the commenting increased, younger sibling 2 had

283
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
much lower levels of commenting when This intervention might be better suited for
compared to younger sibling 1 and younger children with higher level play skills. Still,
sibling 3. Compared to the other younger the maintenance of change in behaviors for
siblings, younger sibling 2 had lower re- both the older siblings and those with
ceptive and expressive language scores as Down syndrome is encouraging. Mainte-
measured by the PPVT and EVT at baseline nance data for the current study further

(see Table 1). The differing characteristics of supported previous research that has sug-
the younger siblings might have influenced gested siblings can learn and continue to use
the response to intervention, as well as the intervention strategies over time (Celiberti
number of intervention sessions necessary to & Harris, 1993; Hancock & Kaiser, 1996;
increase commenting. The sibling dyads were James & Egel, 1986; Tekin & Kircaali-Iftar,
only in intervention for 12 to 15 sessions. Of 2002).
these 12 to 15 sessions, only 8 to 10 sessions The effects of the intervention in the play
took place in the responding training condi- setting did not readily generalize to the snack
tion of intervention in which younger siblings setting. Sibling interactions during general-
were likely to be hearing richer, more re- ization sessions were positive, but the siblings
sponsive language from their older siblings. took very asymmetrical roles. The younger
A longer intervention or an extended re- siblings often needed assistance preparing
sponding training condition might have their own snacks. The older siblings often
shown even stronger effects for the younger assumed the role of a teacher or helper,
siblings, especially for younger sibling 2 who instructing the younger siblings on how to
had lower rates of commenting as well as prepare the snack rather than being respon-
lower scores on language measures. sive to their acts of intentional communica-
No change occurred in the number of tion. Such behavior is not completely un-
requests made by the younger siblings. expected given that the older siblings are
Requesting, however, was not typical in this likely to be accustomed to assisting their
intervention. The younger siblings were not brothers/sisters with Down syndrome when
prompted to request nor were requests they have difficulty completing a task by
modeled by the older siblings. The respon- themselves. It is possible that generalization
siveness of the older siblings might have of the responsive interaction skills could have
actually suppressed rates of requesting. Giv- been facilitated by a simple reminder from
en the increased responsiveness to communi- the trainer to use the responsive interaction
cative attempts made by the younger siblings, strategies or by actually training the siblings
there might have been limited need or within generalization settings. It might be
opportunity for requesting to occur. unreasonable to assume that a young child
Generally, changes in the older siblings should be able to generalize a newly acquired
use of responsive interaction strategies and skill to an untrained setting without some
the verbal behaviors of the younger siblings assistance.
were maintained at the 1-month follow up Generalization of intervention effects
for all dyads. The rate of repeating and might also have been facilitated by training
describing and the percentage responsive- across settings. Older siblings were trained
ness decreased for older sibling 2. Given the only in one setting, a play setting. Training
low rate of commenting and requesting by siblings across a variety of settings and
younger sibling 2, however, older sibling 2 situations in which sibling dyads often spend
had limited opportunities to respond. Youn- time together (e.g., meal time, car rides),
ger sibling 2 had a low level of play skills; might have facilitated the ability of the
he often performed few actions with the siblings to generalize their use of the re-
toys provided. Thus, older sibling 2 also sponsive interaction strategies. Additionally,
was limited on the number of things he older siblings use of responsive interaction
could describe regarding his siblings play. strategies across a variety of settings might

284
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
have increased the effects of the intervention with a disability liked or disliked participat-
on the younger siblings. ing in the study or if they increased the
Another strategy that might have facilitat- amount of time spent with their typical
ed older siblings generalization and mainte- siblings as a result of the intervention. Future
nance of intervention effects would have been research might use questionnaires designed
a practice and self-management system. The to assess the sibling with a disabilitys opin-
trainers visited older siblings twice each ion of the intervention. Parents might also be
week. It is not known how often they involved in assessing the impact of the
practiced using the strategies between trainer intervention on the sibling with a disability.
visits. To promote practice in the future, Parents might be asked to complete daily
siblings could be given a calendar on which report forms on the amount of time siblings
to indicate which days they practice using the are spending together and the quality of

strategies during interactions with their those interactions.


younger brothers/sisters or they could be Finally, more maintenance data are need-
asked to keep a daily journal of interaction ed to determine whether typical siblings can
experiences. Siblings could be positively continue using responsive interaction strate-
reinforced with a preferred prize after prac- gies without support from the interventionist.
ticing a specified number of times. In this study and our previous study, siblings
In addition to the improvements in beha- demonstrated the ability to use responsive
viors by the older siblings and the younger interaction strategies at 1-month follow-up
siblings, the quality of interactions between assessments. Future research might consider
siblings appeared to improve from baseline assessing maintenance a later interval (i.e.
at
to intervention. Assessments of social valid- 3 months, 6 months) and having someone
ity indicated that outside observers found other than the interventionist conduct fol-
improvements in sibling interactions in in- low-up assessments. The presence of the
tervention. Mean ratings on intervention interventionist might serve as a discriminative
clips were higher than baseline video clips, stimulus for the typical siblings. If siblings
demonstrating the observers view of sibling are expected to use the responsive interaction
dyad interactions as more positive and re-
strategies over time and in different settings,
ciprocal in intervention. it is important to determine whether or not
they can use the strategies when presented
Implications with different stimuli.
Several implications for practice can be de- In conclusion, when older siblings know
rived from the present study findings. Teach- how to interact with their younger siblings
ing older siblings strategies to facilitate inter- with disabilities, they are more likely to enjoy
actions with their brothers or sisters with such interactions. Furthermore, children with
disabilities is likely to improve the quality of Down syndrome are likely to benefit from
the time that siblings spend together. Clinical increased interactions with their older, typi-
practitioners, home visitors, and teachers cally developing brothers and sisters. Inter-
could use strategies such as these to involve actions between children with disabilities and
siblings in the numerous visits they attend typically developing children likely provide
with their brothers and sisters with disabilities. increased opportunities for the indirect
Results of the research on siblings use of
teaching of communicative behaviors.
responsive interaction strategies leave ques-
tions for future researchers to investigate.
REFERENCES
First, little is known about the affect of
the intervention on the sibling with a disabil-
Celiberti, D. A., & Harris, S. L. (1993). Behavioral
ity. Communication skills were assessed in intervention for siblings of children with
this study and the previous RI study, but autism: A focus on skills to enhance play.
little is known about how much the sibling Behavior Therapy, 24, 573-599.

285
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody picture Tekin, E., & Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2002). Compari-
vocabulary test-third edition. Circle Pines, son of the effectiveness of efficiency of
MN: American Guidance Service. response prompting procedures delivered by
Girolametto, L. (1988). Improving the social- sibling tutors. Education and Training in
Mental Retardation and Developmental Dis-
conversational skills of developmentally de-
layed children: An intervention study. Journal abilities, 37, 283-299.
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 156-167. Trent, J. A., Kaiser, A. P., & Wolery, M. (2005).
Hancock, T. B., & Kaiser, A. P. (1996). Siblings Sibling use of responsive interaction strate-
use of milieu teaching at home. Topics in Early
gies. Topics in Early Childhood Special Edu-
Childhood Special Education, 18, 168-190. cation, 25, 107-118.
Weiss, R. S. (1981). INREAL intervention for
James, S. D., & Egel, A. L. (1986). A direct
language handicapped and bilingual children.
prompting strategy for increasing reciprocal Journal of the Division for Early Childhood, 4,
interactions between handicapped and non- 40-52.
handicapped siblings. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 19, 173-186. Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive vocabulary test.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Ser-
Kaiser, A. P., Hemmeter, M. L., Ostrosky, M. M., vice.
Fischer, R., Yoder, P., & Keefer, M. (1996).
The effects of teaching parents to use re- Yoder, P. (2003). Intentional communication cod-
sponsive interaction strategies. Topics in Early ing system. Unpublished document, Vander-
bilt
University.
Childhood Special Education, 16, 365-406.
Kaiser, A. P., & Delaney, E. M. (1998). Re-
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E.
(1992). Preschool language scale, third edition
sponsive conversation: Creating opportunities (PLS-3). San Antonio, TX: Psychological
for naturalistic language teaching. Young
Corporation.
Exceptional Children Monograph Series, 3,
13-23.
Kaiser, A. P., Hancock, T. R., & Hester, P. P. This research was supported in part by Vanderbilt
(1998). Parents co-interventionists: Re-
as Center Family Research grant 4-30-100-
Kennedy
search on applications of naturalistic language 9972, NICHD grant T32HD07226, and NICHD
teaching procedures. Infants and Young Chil- grant P50DC03282. It was conducted to fulfill
dren, 10(4), 46-55. a requirement for a doctoral degree in special

McReynold, L. V., & Kearns, K. P. (1983). Single- education from Vanderbilt University to the first
subject experimental designs in communicative author. The authors acknowledge the support of
disorders. Baltimore: University Park Press. Jessica Reddinger and other members of the sibling
research group at Peabody College for their help
Roid, G., & Miller, L. (1997). Leiter international and support.
performance scale -revised. Wood Dale, IL:
Stoelting. Correspondence concerning this article should be
Tapp, J. (2003). ProcoderDV [Computer software addressed to Alacia Trent-Stainbrook, Department
for data collection from digital media]. Nash- of Special Education, Vanderbilt University,
ville, TN: Vanderbilt Kennedy Center. Re- Nashville, TN 37203. E-mail.- alacia. trent@
trieved from http://procoderdv.com vanderbilt. edu

286
Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by sorina constandache on April 23, 2009

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi