Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 6

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology Copyright 2004 by

2004, Vol. 33, No. 1, 8287 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Commentary: Differentiated Measures of Temperament


and Multiple Pathways to Childhood Disorders
Mary K. Rothbart
University of Oregon

Provided is a commentary on articles written for a special section on temperament


and childhood disorders. Temperaments contributions to the development of child-
hood disorders are considered both generally and specifically. Questions are raised
about the use of terminology in the field, particularly the term difficult. Differentiation
of outcomes and predictors is discussed, with a view to identifying multiple pathways
to adaptation or disorder.

At the 1978 International Conference on Infant 1. Individual differences in temperament that in


Studies, Jack Bates posed a set of questions to charac- the extreme may constitute the psychopathol-
terize the early state of our work: Infant temperament: ogy or a disposition toward it.
How shall we define the concept, measure it, and apply 2. Temperament characteristics evoking reactions
it in practice? In the 25 years since that meeting, con- in others that can increase or buffer the risk of
siderable progress has been made in measures, con- psychopathology.
structs, and communication of information about tem- 3. Characteristics influencing the persons choice
perament, although major issues in measurement of settings and relationships that may create
remain. With the publication of the articles in this spe- greater or lesser risk for psychopathology.
cial section, it is clear that new and more refined ques- 4. Temperamental differences influencing the
tions have now been posed, further integrating temper- form of a disorder, its course, the likelihood of
ament with the development of behavior problems. its recurrence, or a combination of these.
Temperament identifies variations in affective- 5. Temperamental biases in information process-
motivational and attentional adaptations that are both ing that can influence cognitions about the
genetically inherited and shaped by experience (Der- world, self, and others.
ryberry & Rothbart, 1997). We have defined tempera- 6. Temperamental regulation or buffering against
ment as constitutionally based individual differences the effects of risk factors or stress.
in reactivity and self-regulation, observed in the do- 7. Heightened or lower responsiveness to envi-
mains of emotionality, motor activity, and attention ronmental events.
(Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). By reactivity, we mean 8. Interaction among temperamental characteris-
characteristics of the individuals responsivity to tics that can influence outcomes as tempera-
changes in stimulation shown at multiple levels (e.g., ment develops.
behavioral, autonomic, neuroendocrine) and through 9. Different temperamental dispositions that may
parameters of latency, rise time, peak intensity, and re- shape different developmental pathways to a
covery time of reaction. By self-regulation, we mean given outcome, and individual dispositions that
processes modulating this reactivity, including ap- may influence multiple outcomes (Cicchetti &
proach, avoidance, inhibition, and attentional self-reg- Cohen, 1995).
ulation. As children develop, initially more reactive 10. Temperament characteristics and caregiving
systems become increasingly regulated, as systems environments that may make independent con-
like fearful inhibition and attentional effortful control tributions to outcomes, or may interact in in-
develop. creasing or decreasing risk of disorder.
In an earlier attempt to summarize research on de- 11. Dysfunctional states themselves that may change
velopment and psychopathology (Rothbart, Posner, & aspects of temperament and create further risk.
Hershey, 1995), we speculated about ways in which
temperament might be involved in development of risk At the time of the 1995 review, only a very few of
for psychopathology. The list included these possibilities had been a topic of research, and
what is striking about the reviews in this special section
Requests for reprints should be sent to Mary K. Rothbart, 1227 is the number of these possibilities that have now been
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 974031227. E-mail: maryroth@ explored. To make links between these general topics
darkwing.uoregon.edu and some of the research and ideas in this section, I re-

82
COMMENTARY: DIFFERENTIATED MEASURES OF TEMPERAMENT

fer to items in the list by number in the following. In sponses to novelty and (b) more general irritability or
addition to these topics, the articles in this section distress proneness (Bates, Freeland, & Lounsbury,
stress issues of emotional regulation and dysregulation 1979). General irritability predicted later externalizing
that I believe will be basic to future advances in the and internalizing problems; fear of novelty predicted
field. A focus on emotion regulation suggests these internalizing problems to a greater degree than ex-
kinds of questions: When fear and avoidance are elic- ternalizing problems (see review by Rothbart & Bates,
ited, how may their expression be reinforced or regu- 1998). Batess research indicated that the two varieties
lated? How can soothing of negative affect occur from of negative emotionality are differentially related to
others (Harman, Rothbart, & Posner, 1997) or from the specific behavior problems that develop later, and this
self (Rothbart, Ziaie, OBoyle, 1992)? What kinds of important distinction is lost in the more general con-
coping can modulate or enhance emotional reactions? struct of difficultness as often operationalized.
When does the coping itself become maladaptive? Another way of looking at difficulty is to say that
some children are labeled by their caregivers as prob-
lem children at an early age, and this labeling is likely
Difficult Temperament: influenced by the childs temperament. The terms easy
Confusion in Terminology and difficult may be also used by parents of more than
one child to compare children (i.e., the first was easy,
Before discussing the articles in this section indi- but the second was difficult). The terms may be ap-
vidually, I would like to comment on the use of the plied in an endearing way, or in an attempt to identify
difficult child construct in several of the articles. the source of problems in family relationships. In addi-
Please consider these concerns along with the excellent tion, however, labels communicated to the child may
ones voiced by Frick and Morris in their article (this is- contribute to the quality of the childs adaptation. The
sue). Thomas, Chess, and associates (Thomas, Chess, study of social attributions, values, and labeling is im-
& Birch, 1968; Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig, & portant, but it takes us to a level of analysis beyond the
Korn, 1963), in their groundbreaking work on temper- simple measurement of temperament.
ament, identified nine dimensions of temperament Over recent years, the list of nine dimensions of
along with three categories of infants, designated as temperament (activity level, mood, intensity, thresh-
easy, difficult, and slow to warm up. The difficult old, approachwithdrawal, attention spanpersistence,
and easy categories were based on a factor analysis of distractibility, adaptability, and rhythmicity) proposed
data from the original New York Longitudinal Study, by Thomas, Chess, and their associates has undergone
which identified a cluster of five temperament dimen- revision (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Recent research,
sions: (a) approachwithdrawal, (b) mood, (c) inten- for example, has identified a general structure of tem-
sity, (d) adaptability, and (e) rhythmicity, that they la- perament in childhood that includes broad factors of
beled difficult (Thomas et al., 1963). In algorithms for surgencyextraversion, negative affectivity, and effort-
assessing difficulty developed by Carey and McDevitt ful control (Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001).
(1978), Fullard, McDevitt, and Carey (1984), and oth- In our research, surgencyextraversion is defined pri-
ers, difficult and easy are derived from extreme marily by scales assessing positive emotionality and
scores on all of these five dimensions. approach, including positive anticipation, high-in-
There are a number of problems in the use of the diffi- tensity pleasure (sensation-seeking), activity level,
culty construct in temperament research (see also impulsivity, smiling and laughter, and a negative load-
Plomin, 1982; Rothbart, 1982). One is the wide variabil- ing from shyness. A negative affectivity factor includes
ity of operationalizations of the construct. For example, positive loadings for shyness, discomfort, fear, an-
factor analyses of data collected on the New York Longi- gerfrustration, and sadness, and a negative loading
tudinal Study dimensions have frequently failed to rep- from soothability-falling reactivity. The effortful con-
licate loadings of rhythmicity on the difficulty factor trol factor is defined by positive loadings from in-
(Bates, 1980). As a result, studies often delete rhythm- hibitory control, attentional focusing, low intensity
icity measurements or use varying composites to assess (nonrisk-taking) pleasure, and perceptual sensitivity.
a difficult temperament, depending on the outcome of These broader factors suggest a hierarchical structure
their own factor analysis. This inconsistency in the use to temperament, with more specific characteristics re-
of difficulty creates serious problems for knowing what lated to each other at a higher level in the hierarchy.
is meant by the term in any given study.
Bates (1980) argued that the core variable of mea-
sures of difficulty is the presence of negative emotion- A Comment on the Articles
ality, and his work utilized measures assessing this in this Special Section
variable. Factor-analytic work by Bates and others also
identified two kinds of negative emotionality in in- Lonigan, Vasey, Phillips, and Hazen (this issue)
fancy: (a) negative affect related to childrens re- have reported extraction of similar factors to those de-

83
ROTHBART

scribed previously, from childhood self-reported items ration) is basic to our understanding. These studies also
assessing emotionality and attention. In turn, Lonigan have much to offer to our general understanding of de-
et al. have found that measures of temperament show velopment.
differential relations to measures of depression and It is likely that differential predictions of psycho-
anxiety, with negative affectivity predicting both de- pathology using the two kinds of negative affect found
pression and anxiety and effortful control predicting by Bates et al. (1979) will add further understanding
lower levels of both problems. Surgencypositive af- (9). For example, research increasingly suggests that
fectivity predicts unique variance of depression only, one kind of negative affectfearcan act as a protec-
with lower levels of surgency predicting more likeli- tive factor for development of the externalizing disor-
hood of depression. Like other studies with adults re- ders (Rothbart & Bates, 1998), and Frick and Morris
viewed in Lonigan et al.s article, their research indi- (this issue) present data to suggest this is the case (8).
cates that both reactive (positive and negative affect) In addition, links between conduct disorder and de-
and regulative (effortful control) temperament charac- pression suggest that a developmental analysis of the
teristics are related to psychopathology (1 and 8 in the order of appearance of symptoms will be important.
previous list). Lonigan et al. also bring up the auto- The article by Frick and Morris (this issue) makes
matic attentional biases linked to negative affectivity, excellent use of what is currently known about temper-
which may influence both the daily experiences of ament, demonstrating the effectiveness of considering
children and their proneness to future negative experi- differentiated outcomes as well as differentiated tem-
ences (5 and 7). Lonigan et al.s important findings fur- peramental predictors to describe possible multiple
ther raise the question of how these dispositions may pathways to conduct problems (4 and 9). They distin-
interact in influencing development of internalizing guish between reactive and more instrumental aggres-
outcomes. It also will be of interest to link problem sion, suggesting that emotional reactivity and lack of
outcomes to affect at lower levels of the hierarchy (e.g., emotional regulation are likely to be closely tied to the
fear, anger, sadness, soothability) and to specific expe- reactive symptoms. They also note that the develop-
riences of the child and related cognitions. ment of conscience is linked to regulation of instru-
Compas, Connor-Smith, and Jaser (this issue) begin mental aggression and that Kochanskas (1995) dem-
to fill in possible developmental pathways to depres- onstration of connections between temperamental fear,
sion by examining basic links between stress, coping, effortful control, and the development of conscience
and adaptive and maladaptive outcomes. They demon- has identified important links between temperament
strate relations between negative affectivity and rela- and the development of morality. Frick and Morris ob-
tively automatic avoidance coping (3 and 5) and sug- servation that affective aspects of conscience can coun-
gest that attentional control may be related to the use of teract callous and unemotional responses to others is
secondary coping, such as cognitively reframing a situ- very helpful. Although temperamental building blocks
ation or intentionally buffering more automatic stress like effortful control can provide the capacity to regu-
reactions (6). Their work brings up basic issues of late action, the direction of that regulation is not speci-
emotion regulation that are also discussed in other arti- fied: It will depend on the values and goals of the per-
cles in the section. Compas et al. also point out that son, one of which is the desire not to hurt others. The
temperament can directly influence the experiences of values and goals of the child will also be influenced by
children, with stronger dispositions to negative affect the culture (Putnam, Sanson, & Rothbart, 2002) and by
mapping onto dysphoric symptoms and low positive the set of experiences and relationships the child has
affectivity mapping onto anhedonic symptoms (1). had.
Children prone to negative affect, for example, will ex- Nigg, Goldsmith, and Sachek (this issue) also con-
perience stronger stress reactions than less highly dis- sider more differentiated outcomes as well as more dif-
tress-prone children, given the same environmental ferentiated negative affectivity predictors of attention
stressor (7). These observations are related to an im- deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). These authors
portant point made by Escalona (1968) about effec- make important strides in identifying possible multiple
tive experience: measuring an environmental stressor pathways (9), stressing the importance of development
is not sufficient for us to understand what a child is ex- in shaping both temperament and psychopathology
periencing, given temperamental variability and vari- (8). Their brief review of genetics findings suggests
ability in the use of coping. Future research is likely to that shared environment may be relatively unimportant
make more direct links between affectivity, coping, in the development of ADHD, but important for
and the cognitive appraisals that have been linked to ADHD with conduct disorder. This is an intriguing
depression (Beck, 1976). Issues of emotion regulation finding, suggesting socialization studies comparing
and dysregulation thus appear to be of major impor- the two outcomes.
tance for the development of psychopathology, and in- If we put together the analysis of Frick and Morris
formation about coping at all stages of reactivity (prior (this issue) with the review of Nigg et al. (this issue),
to aspects of the reaction, at peak, and in relation to du- we can see that studies on the development of conduct

84
COMMENTARY: DIFFERENTIATED MEASURES OF TEMPERAMENT

disorder in relation to the attentional capacities of the orientation, emotional regulation, and delay of gratifi-
child will be of great interest. Differentiating anger and cation. Thus, it is not clear whether the differences
irritability from fear will also lead to interesting ques- between Wills and Dishions self-control and tempera-
tions about fear as a control mechanism: Is fear control mental effortful control are more than terminological.
mainly inhibitory, or is avoidance also involved? What Their construct of poor self-control, however, is defined
is the role of punishment in fear control? Kochanska by aspects of impatience, impulsiveness, orientation
(1995) and Asendorpf (1990) have already made im- toward the present, inability to delay gratification, and
portant advances in looking at fear in the development coping with problems through anger. This is clearly
of conscience and the possibility of two kinds of shy- a more complex personality construct, although sur-
ness, one more temperamental and one likely related to gentextraverted characteristics and low effortful con-
social punishment. Thinking of fear as a control system trol may serve as developmental building blocks for it.
does not neatly fit within our heuristic distinction be- This construct appears to be linked to low emotional and
tween reactivity and self-regulation (Rothbart & behavioral regulation and is likely related to the expres-
Derryberry, 1981). Fear is a reactive system, but it in- sive aspects of conduct disorder described by Frick and
cludes regulatory aspects. Effortful control, on the Morris.
other hand, is chiefly self-regulatory, and the inhibition Questions about relations between temperament
or avoidance of fear and surgency reactions is often ac- and attachment have been considered for many years
complished through effortful control. (see review by Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Zeanah and
Elsewhere (Rothbart, 1999), I have commented that Fox (this issue) raise a new set of issues about the pos-
one of the most confusing aspects of applying tempera- sible relation between temperament and reactive at-
ment constructs has been the tendency for different re- tachment disorder. Reactive attachment disorder is an
searchers to use different names for the same process. unusual disorder, in that a history of pathogenic care is
In a volume on fear, shyness, and social phobia, I found criterial to diagnosis and is evidently assumed to be
that five different chapters used five different terms de- causal. An added criterion is the presence of one of two
scribing individual differences in fear (Rothbart, types of symptoms: (a) highly inhibited behavior, am-
1999). Nigg et al.s negative approach term is an exam- bivalent or contradictory behavior to the caregiver; and
ple of this phenomenon. There are important con- (b) indiscriminate sociability to a wide range of adults
current and longitudinal links between anger and and lack of selectivity to attachment figures. Zeanah
surgencyextraversion (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Her- and Fox suggest that the first set of syndromes maps
shey, 2000), but combining approaching aspects of onto negative affectivity, and the second set possibly
surgency with the kind of affect (anger) that occurs onto surgencyextraversion (4). They suggest that in-
when our goals are blocked is not very helpful. I dividual differences in irritability may be the basic mo-
strongly believe, however, that if we address the kinds tivator of avoidance. However, the symptoms de-
of differentiating questions suggested by Nigg et al., scribed seem closer to individual differences in fearful
and those listed previously, we will be better able to as- inhibition. They make the point that empirical research
sign terms to our constructs so that we can know what in this area will be valuable, and this research may well
they mean when we use them. profit from a differentiated approach to assessing nega-
Wills and Dishion (this issue) put forward a differen- tive affect. Zeanah and Fox note that temperament it-
tiated model specifying temperament and self-control self may be affected by pathogenic rearing environ-
as moderators for the relations between parenting, peer ments (11), citing studies of institutionalized children
groups, and substance use (3, 10). They have found tem- described as higher in negative affect, more shy, and
peramental positive mood and task orientation to be re- less active and sociable than home-reared children. In
lated to supportive relationships with parents and low addition, they note attentional problems that appear to
substance abuse, whereas negative emotionality and ac- be associated with neglect in caregiving (2).
tivity level are related to parentchild conflict and sub-
stance abuse, and inversely related to parent support (2).
Wills and Dishion differentiate between self-control
and poor self-control and further differentiate those con- Summary of Critical Issues
structs from extraversionsurgency and effortful con- and Future Directions
trol. Nevertheless, there appear to be overlap among
these constructs. Wills and Dishion define self-control 1. By differentiating within broad temperament di-
as involving self-monitoring, planning, future orienta- mensions, or even within more narrow temperament
tion, delay of gratification, and emotional regulation. dimensions such as fear, we may advance our under-
We (Posner & Rothbart, 1998) have proposed that standing of the development of psychopathology.
effortful control is supported by the executive attention 2. Temperament may be linked to multiple path-
system, and both executive attention and effortful con- ways to childhood disorders, and differentiating symp-
trol are involved in planning, self-monitoring, future toms of the disorders facilitates this effort.

85
ROTHBART

3. Further study of emotional regulation, dysreg- depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical
ulation, and coping will benefit our understanding of Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 2131.
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful
disorder and adaptation. processes in the organization of temperament. Development
4. Effortful control, a broad temperament dimen- and Psychopathology, 9, 633652.
sion based on the development of executive attention, Escalona, S. K. (1968). The roots of individuality: Normal patterns
is a key to adaptive and maladaptive development and of development in infancy. Chicago: Aldine.
to the design of prevention and intervention strategies. Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004/this issue). Temperament and de-
velopmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical
5. Goals of empathy and caring for the self and oth- Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 5468.
ers can set the motivational framework for the applica- Fullard, W., McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1984). Assessing tem-
tion of effortful control. perament in one- to three-year-old children. Journal of Pediat-
ric Psychology, 9, 205217.
Harman, C., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1997). Distress and at-
tention interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Emotion,
Developing Attention-Training 21, 2743.
Interventions Kochanska, G. (1995). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline,
and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging in-
ternalization. Child Development, 66, 597615.
Given the importance of executive attention and Lonigan, C. J., Vasey, M. W., Phillips, B. M., & Hazen, R. A.
effortful control in so many developmental processes, (2004/this issue). Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of
attention-based interventions may prove helpful in pre- threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adoles-
venting or helping to treat childhood disorders. Mi- cent Psychology, 33, 820.
Nigg, J. T., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sachek, J. (2004/this issue). Tem-
chael Posner and I have now completed a trial of an at-
perament and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The de-
tention-training program with normal 4-year-olds. We velopment of a multiple pathway model. Journal of Clinical
developed a computerized joystick-controlled program Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 4253.
based on primate attention training originally devel- Plomin, R. (1982). The difficult concept of temperament: A response
oped by Washburn and Rumbaugh (1992). In our first to Thomas, Chess, and Korn. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 28,
2533.
test of the approach with a small sample of children,
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self-regulation,
we found that training was related to increases in chil- and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal So-
drens IQ, especially their matrix performance, not ciety of London, B(353), 19151927.
found for controls (Rothbart & Rueda, 2003). There Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tem-
were further indications that childrens performance perament and parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
on conflict tasks had improved. Future research will in- parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp.
255277). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
vestigate whether these results replicate and whether Rothbart, M. K. (1982). The concept of difficult temperament: A
they are helpful for children whose attentional func- critical analysis of Thomas, Chess, & Korn. MerrillPalmer
tioning has been compromised. One feature of this ap- Quarterly, 28, 3540.
proach is that it is based on a neuroscience analysis of Rothbart, M. K. (1999). Temperament, fear, and shyness. In L. A.
the processes involved in executive attention, and this Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social
phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and clinical outcomes
approach may prove helpful for others in the area of di- (pp. 8893). New York: Oxford University Press.
agnosis and treatment. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K., & Fisher, P. (2001). Inves-
tigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Childrens
Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon
References (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
chology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). Development of inhibition during child- ment (5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.
hood: Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individ-
model. Developmental Psychology, 26, 721730. ual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown
Bates, J. E. (1980). The concept of difficult temperament. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp.
MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 26, 299319. 3786). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Mea- Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of
surement of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to
794803. parent report at seven years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the
New York: International Universities Press. life span (pp. 85119). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the Infant Tem- ciates, Inc.
perament Questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61, 375379. Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. (1995). Temperament,
Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (1995). Perspectives on developmental attention, and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti
psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Devel- & J. D. Cohen (Eds.), Manual of developmental psychopa-
opmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. thology (Vol. 1, pp. 315340). New York: Wiley.
320). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (in press). The development of
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., & Jaser, S. S. (2004/this issue). effortful control. Festschrift in honor of Michael I. Posner.
Temperament, stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

86
COMMENTARY: DIFFERENTIATED MEASURES OF TEMPERAMENT

Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & OBoyle, C. G. (1992). Self-regulation guage Research Centers Computerized Test System. Behavior
and emotion in infancy. In N. Eisenberg & R. A. Fabes (Eds.), Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 24, 157164.
Emotion and its regulation in early development: New direc- Wills, T. A., & Dishion, T. J. (2004/this issue). Temperament and ad-
tions for child development (pp. 723). San Francisco, CA: olescent substance use: A transactional analysis of emerging
Jossey-Bass. self-control. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychol-
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. G. (1968). Temperament and be- ogy, 33, 6981.
havior disorders in children. New York: New York University Zeanah, C. H., & Fox, N. A. (2004/this issue). Temperament and at-
Press. tachment disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S. Psychology, 33, 3241.
(1963). Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York:
New York University Press.
Washburn, D. A., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1992). Testing primates with Received April 20, 2003
joystick-based automated apparatus: Lessons from the Lan- Accepted August 26, 2003

87

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi