Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 20

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

Human Perception and Performance 0096-1523/97/$3.00


1997, Vol. 23, No. 1, 116-135

Music Performance and the Perception of Key

William Forde Thompson L o l a L. Cudd),


Atkinson College, York University Queen's Univermty

The effect of music performance on perceived key movement was examined. Listeners
judged key movement in sequences presented without performance expression (mechanical)
in Experiment 1 and with performance expression in Experiment 2. Modulation distance
varied. Judgments corresponded to predictions based on the cycle of fifths and toroidal
models of key relatedness, with the highest correspondence for performed versions with the
toroidal model. In Experiment 3, listeners compared mechanical sequences with either
performed sequences or modifications of performed sequences. Modifications preserved
expressive differences between chords, but not between voices. Predictions from Experiments
1 and 2 held only for performed sequences, suggesting that differences between voices are
informative of key movement. Experiment 4 confmned that modifications did not disrupt
musicality. Analyses of performances further suggested a link between performance expres-
sion and key.

In this article we illustrate the effects of music perfor- been directed at a detailed description of the characteristics
mance on the perception of key movement. Music perfor- of skilled performance. It is well documented that when a
mance typically includes systematic variations in timing, skilled performer attempts to interpret a piece of music, he
dynamics, articulation, and other components of motor ex- or she will introduce discrepancies from the printed score.
pression that are linked to compositional structure. We first Analyses of recorded performances have yielded statistical
review selected research findings related to this topic. We descriptions of discrepancies in timing, intensity, pitch, and
then describe the construct of musical key and key move- timbre (for overviews, see Gabrielsson, 1987; Sloboda,
ment, along with theoretical models of the mental represen- 1988; Sundberg, 1983). These discrepancies are referred to
tation of key relationships. Finally, we report experimental as "performance variation" or "performance expression."
evidence that performance expression influences the ab- Performance variation may sometimes reflect muscular
straction of key relationships in short musical excerpts. constraints, but it also reflects musical knowledge embed-
ded in the motor programs for music performance. Exami-
Music Performance nation of the role of such knowledge in motor programs
suggests that performance expression is systematic, inten-
Our experience of music and our ideas about music mainly tional, and rule based (Clarke, 1985; Palmer, 1988, 1989;
derive from listening to performed music . . . . The perfor- Repp, 1992; Sloboda, 1985; Todd, 1985). Expressive ac-
mance of a piece of music represents the overt, auditory
manifestation of the composer's and/or the performer's inten- tions may be used to emphasize or convey certain aspects of
tions, ideas, feelings, and whatever is being communicated in musical structure and meaning over others.
music. It is the direct stimulus to the listener and will pro- For example, performance expression is related to the
foundly affect.., perceiving and understanding. (Gabriels- segmentation of music into units called phrases (Clarke,
son, 1988, p. 27) 1982, 1985; Palmer, 1989; Todd, 1985). As a phrase bound-
ary is approached, the performed duration of events in-
Motivated by an attempt to discover how such musical
creases beyond the notated duration; the loudness also may
communication takes place, current research efforts have
decrease. Phrases may be separated by pauses. Moreover, a
performer is capable of adopting alternative interpretations
This research was supported by research grants from the Natural of the phrasing of a piece of music and of adjusting perfor-
Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. Technical mance expression to match the intended interpretation
assistance was provided by Alistair Mackinnell, Don Sinclair, and (Palmer, 1992).
Alex Wiebe. We are grateful to Yuval Fichman for performing the A second example is the presence of timing asynchronies
musical excerpts, to Shulamit Mor for assisting in the selection and in performance expression when the musical notation indi-
harmonization of the musical materials, and to Gabriel Radvansky cates simultaneity (Palmer, 1988; Rasch, 1979). These
and Murry Stainton for helpful suggestions.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to asynchronies tend to be applied to the notes designated by
William Forde Thompson, Department of Psychology, Atkinson the performer as the melody. Playing each note of the
College, York University, 4700 Keele Street, North York, Ontario, melody slightly ahead of its accompaniment is consistent
Canada M3J IP3, or to Lola L. Cuddy, Department of Psychology, with the principles outlined by Bregman and colleagues
Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada K7L 3N6. regarding auditory segregation and distinctiveness (e.g.,
116
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 117

Bregman, 1990, pp. 492, 496). Performance expression are 2, 2, 1, 2, 2, 2, and 1 semitones. The tonic, or key note,
treats the melody as a distinctive voice among the other of a major scale may be any one of the 12 chromatic tones;
voices of the music. the remaining tones are determined by the successive steps
Much less is known about whether performance expres- of this interval pattern of semitones. Thus, for any given
sion elucidates the pitch structure of Western tonal music-- major scale, some of the tones of the chromatic scale do not
more specifically, whether performance expression conveys appear. These tones, called nonscale tones, are least impor-
information about the stractural relationships among notes, tant in the hierarchy of tone functions for the key. (They
chords, and keys in a piece of music. Of particular relevance may occur infrequently in actual music, but their function is
to the current research is that there is no evidence available normally subordinate to the scale tones.)
on the question of whether listeners' perception of key is A similar hierarchical structure applies to the minor keys
facilitated (or possibly masked) by performance expression. in the Western idiom. In particular, the tones of both major
Theory and data strongly argue that perceptual sensitivity to and minor keys are organized about a focal tone--the ton-
key and key relationships, in conjunction with other levels ic--which has a special status. The role of the tonic has
of pitch structure, contributes to the encoding, representa- been likened to that of a cognitive referent, or prototypic
tion, and recall of musical materials (Krumhansl, 1990, and element: "These elements are given priority in processing,
references cited therein). Thus, in light of Gabrielsson's are most stable in memory, and are important for linguistic
(1988) quote, it is important to determine whether perfor- description" (Knm~ansl, 1990, p. 18).
mance expression may influence the perception of key. The second principal structure deals with the relationships
In this research we examined the influence of perfor- among the 24 keys. Each of the 12 chromatic tones may
mance expression on the perception of key movement. function as the tonic of a key. Thus, there are 24 possible
Before describing the experimental approach, we review keys: 12 major and 12 minor. A traditional description of
some basic musical terms and concepts. the relationships among keys is the cycle of fifths shown in
Figure 1. The figure shows the relationships among major
keys; a similar cycle represents relationships among minor
Perception of Key and Key Relationships keys. The note names on the cycle, C, G, D, and so on, refer
to the tonic notes of the 12 major keys. The distance
The perception of key and key movement in Western between keys is defmed by the number of steps on the cycle.
tonal music involves sensitivity to a complex system of Thus, the key of C is most closely related (least distant) to
relationships among tones and chords. Traditional music the neighboring keys of G and F, one step clockwise and
theory offers detailed descriptions of these relationships one step counterclockwise on the cycle, respectively. It is
(e.g., Piston, 1941/1962), and an extensive body of empir- most distant from the key of F sharp, six steps around the
ical research has scrutinized, verified, and in some aspects
refined and elaborated on the descriptions of music theory
(for reviews, see Butler, 1992; Dowling & Harwood, 1986;
Handel, 1989; Krumhansl, 1990). Research suggests that
listeners over a wide spectrum of age, musical training, and
musical exposure have acquired rich mental representations
of the underlying pitch relationships characteristic of the
Western idiom.
The representations guiding the perception of musical key
may be described in terms of two principal structures: one
that references relationships among tones within a key and
BI'/AD C

one that references relationships among keys. These struc-


tures are outlined in turn.
First, a musical key involves a specified collection of
EI'/D#L mV?A
tones (sometimes called pitch classes) and a hierarchy of
functions of tones. The most familiar collection outlining a AVG# = E
musical key is the major scale: doh-re-mi-fah-sol-la-ti.
Within this collection, or scale, the first note, doh, is said to
have the most important function. It is called the tonic, tonal D /C# B
center, or key note. Other pitches have subordinate func-
tions to the tonic; the next most important is the fifth note of
Gb/F#
the scale, sol, the next is the third note mi, followed by the
remaining scale notes.
The collection itself is a subset of the set of tones obtained CYCLE OF FIFTHS
by dividing the octave, or frequency ratio 2:1, into 12 equal
logarithmic steps. Adjacent steps form the interval called a Figure 1. The cycle of fifths model of key relationships. The
semitone, and a succession of 12 semitones is called the figure shows the relationships among major keys; the note names
chromatic scale. For the major scale, the successive steps refer to the key notes of the 12 major keys.
118 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

cycle. Neighboring keys share all but one scale tone of their angle in Figure 2. (Note that in Figure 2, the axes represent
respective major scales; the number of tones shared between angular measures around a circle; thus, the top and bottom
keys decreases as the distance in steps between keys edges of the rectangle are identified, as are the fight and left
increases. edges.)
Figure 1 also shows roman numeral codes for selected The two models--the cycle of fifths and the toroidal
keynotes with respect to the key of C. The roman numeral model--provide different measures of key distance. The
code is a traditional description of the position of the key cycle of fifths measures the distance between keys in terms
note within the scale of C. Thus, the code for G is V, of the number of steps separating the keys on the cycle,
meaning that G is the fifth note of the scale of C; the code reflecting the number of scale tones shared between the
for D is II, meaning that D is the second note of the scale of keys. The toroidal model measures the distance between
C, and so on. The code for F sharp is IV#; the note F sharp keys, not merely in terms of the number of shared scale
does not occur in the scale of C, so the code indicates that tones but also in terms of the degree of correspondence
the key note is a semitone above the fourth note of the scale between the tonal hierarchies of keys. That is, the quantifi-
of C. Roman numeral codes are used to describe the stim- cation of distance between keys on the torus takes into
ulus sequences in the current experiments. account the degree to which keys share structurally impor-
In 1982, Krumhansl and Kessler produced an empirically tant scale tones. We examine and compare both measures of
derived spatial map of the relationships among all 24 major key distance in the analyses of our data.
and minor keys. The mapping preserved the cycle of fifths
(see Figure 1) but also quantified the relation between major
and minor keys. Derivation of the map involved an empir-
ical strategy called the probe-tone technique (Krumhansl & Performance and the Perception of Key
Shepard, 1979) adapted for use with a variety of musical
contexts (Krumhansl & Kessler, 1982). The technique in- To our knowledge, no study has identified an influence by
volved the presentation of a context followed by one of the performance variation on the perception of key movement.
12 tones of the chromatic scale. The listener rated each However, results of studies of performance expression have
probe tone for goodness of fit with the context. The set of 12 established strong links between performance variation and
ratings, one for each probe tone, was called the probe-tone levels of pitch structure other than key (Palmer, 1988,
profile for that context. 1989); still other evidence, cited next, suggests a direct link
For both major and minor keys, two types of context between music performance and key structure. In studies in
produced probe-tone profiles that were highly intercorre- which performance expression was not used, the role of key
lated. These contexts were chords consisting of the first, structure in musical pitch processing has been clearly es-
third, and fifth tones of the scale, called tonic triads, and tablished (e.g., Bartlett & Dowling, 1980; Cohen, 1991;
chord progressions, called cadences. The profiles corre- Cuddy, Cohen, & Mewhort, 1981; Cuddy & Thompson,
sponded closely to the organization of the tonal hierarchy 1992; Krumhansl & Kessler, 1982; Thompson, 1993;
described by traditional music theory: The highest ratings Thompson & Cuddy, 1989, 1992). In view of this research,
were assigned to the tonic, the next highest were assigned to it seems likely that performance expression can influence
the other two notes of the tonic triad, the next highest were the perception of key movement, and it was the aim of our
assigned to the remaining four notes of the scale, and the research to identify and study this influence.
lowest ratings were assigned to the five nonscale notes. The Results of previous studies of music performance have
profiles for the major-key contexts were averaged to form suggested that knowledge of pitch structure is embodied in
the standardized profile for the major key, and the profiles performance variation. Performers' articulation of the me-
for the minor-key contexts were averaged to form the stan- lodic line already has been mentioned: Palmer (1989) ex-
dardized profile for the minor key. amined chord asynchronies, when voices were notated as
A standardized profile for each of the 24 major and minor simultaneous, for performances of Mozart's Sonata in A
keys was constructed, and a matrix of correlations among all Major, K. 331. Notes of the melodic line preceded other
24 keys was obtained. Multidimensional scaling was ap- voices by an average of 20 ms, suggesting that chord
plied to the intercorrelation matrix. The intent was to pro- asynchronies were used to signal the performers' identifi-
vide a low-dimensional space, or map, in which geometric cation of the melody.
distances between keynotes reflected perceptual distances. Research by Palmer and van de Sande (1993) suggests
The result was a four-dimensional solution. Two dimen- that k e y structure is one of the levels of pitch structure
sions accounted for relationships according to the cycle of involved in the motor programs for music performance. The
fifths. The other two dimensions accounted for the relation- approach by Palmer and van de Sande (1993) was to exam-
ships between major and minor keys; this pattern of rela- ine errors in performance rather than intended expressive
tionships also was circular and was termed the cycle of actions. Performance errors were coded in terms of the level
thirds. The mathematical description of the four- of pitch strneture implicated. Errors involving one note were
dimensional space is a torus. coded as single-note units, and errors involving multiple
It is possible to illustrate the torus in two dimensions by simultaneous notes were coded as chord units. Errors in
plotting the angular locations on both the cycle of fifths and single-note units occurred most often for polyphonic music
the cycle of thirds. This configuration is shown as a reet- (containing strong within-voice associations), and errors in
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 119

C# A F [

f c# a f

Ab E C

a e c
!

b
E B G Eb

b
e b g

F# D B

m
bb f# d
.
, C# ,A F

Angular Displacement - Cycle of Fifths

Figure 2. The toroidal model of key relationships. The two axes represent angular measures
around a circle; thus, the top and bottom edges of the rectangle represent equivalent positions, and
the fight and left edges represent equivalent positions. Adapted from 'q'racing the Dynamic Changes
in Perceived Tonal Organization in a Spatial Representation of Musical Keys," by C. M. Krumhansl
and E. Kessler, 1982, Psychological Review, 89, p. 346. Copyright 1982 by the American Psycho-
logical Association.

chord units occurred most often for homophonic music and Kessler (1982) were able to track the changing sense of
(containing strong across-voice associations). For both key for a number of modulating chord sequences. In a series
polyphonic and homophonic music, diatonic (same-key) of studies, Thompson and Cuddy (1989, 1992) and Cuddy
errors were more common than nondiatonic (different-key) and Thompson (1992) demonstrated listeners' sensitivity to
errors. These fmdings suggest that motor programs for the direction and distance of modulation in excerpts from
music performance reflect knowledge of multiple levels of Bach chorales. Listeners were able to judge the direction
pitch structure: voices, chords, and keys. and distance of modulations of 0, 1, or 2 steps on the cycle
In addition to the aforementioned work, performance of fifths in both clockwise and counterclockwise directions.
expression and key structure have been linked on theoretical Thompson (1993) extended the procedure to the study of
grounds. The performance principles of "melodic charge" harmonized versions of melodies composed by Hindemith
and "harmonic charge," proposed by Sundberg, Fryden, and (1968) for modulations of 0, 2, 4, and 6 steps clockwise on
Askenfelt (1983), include relating the loudness and duration the cycle of fifths. Listeners' judgments of perceived key
of notes and chords, respectively, to their tonal stability movement discriminated between nonmodulating and mod-
within the current key. Research has shown that computer- ulating sequences and also showed some ability to discrim-
delivered melodies were judged to be more musical when
inate differences in the distance of modulation for modulat-
such principles were applied than when they were not
ing sequences.
(Thompson, Sundberg, Friberg, & Fryden, 1989). However,
In this investigation, we examined whether performance
although Thompson et al. (1989) suggested that the princi-
ples reflect "a performer's way of signalling his or her expression would influence judgments of key movement. In
sensitivity to key relationships" (p. 69), their investigations Experiments 1 and 2, judgments of key movement were
did not address whether performance expression influences compared for harmonized sequences presented mechani-
perceptual judgments of key. caUy, that is, without performance expression (Experiment
Results of several studies attest to the psychological va- 1) and sequences presented with performance expression
lidity of key structure for musical excerpts presented with- (Experiment 2). To anticipate the results, we found that
out performance variation. One procedure concerns listen- listeners' judgments corresponded more closely to theoret-
ers' ability to discern whether an excerpt contains a single, ical predictions when sequences were performed than when
unambiguously specified key or whether the excerpt con- sequences were presented mechanically. These results
talns movement from one key to another, called a key guided two additional experiments and an analysis of per-
modulation. Using the probe-tone technique, Krumhansl formance expression in the music performances.
120 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

Experiment 1 Sequences
In Experiment 1, we examined judgments of key move- Figure 3 displays the eight harmonized sequences used by
ment in the harmonized sequences used by Thompson Thompson (1993). They were based on harmonization exercises
(1993). Specifically, the experiment represented an attempt taken from a textbook written by Hindemith (1968, pp. 105, 109).
to replicate the results of the third experiment in Thomp- Each exercise required the harmonization of a melodic sequence of
four measures (two phrases); the exercise specified whether har-
son's study with a different experimental paradigm. Thomp-
monization must involve nonmodulation or a modulation to a
son presented listeners with a single harmonized sequence particular key.
on each trial and instructed them to rate the psychological Harmonizations of melodic sequences were composed by a
distance between the first and final keys on a 7-point scale. musician with more than 10 years of formal training in music
As predicted by the cycle of fifths model of key relation- performance and Western tonal composition. We and one other
ships, mean ratings were lowest for nonmodulation, higher highly trained musician checked that the following criteria were
for modulation to II, and higher still for modulation to HI. satisfied: (a) All harmonizations ended on the key specified by
Surprisingly, however, the mean rating for modulation to Hindemith (1968) for the exercise; (b) conventions of traditional
IV# (6 steps on the cycle of fifths) was lower (but not harmonic practice were obeyed; (c) modulation in a modulating
significantly) than mean ratings for modulation to III (4 sequence was initiated in the third measure; and (d) all sequences
ended with a conventional V-I cadence in the final key of the
steps). This finding contrasts with predictions by the cycle
sequence.
of fifths model and demonstrates that the perception of key The eight sequences represented two sets of four sequences
distance is not entirely determined by the extent to which each: Set A and Set B. Within each set, the first two bars of each
the keys have overlapping scale notes. sequence were identical. In the third and fourth bars, the four
Although Thompson (1993) found that mean ratings were sequences were identical in rhythm and similar in the contour of
not significantly different for modulation to III and modu- the soprano voice, but they differed in chord progression and key
lation to IV#, it is possible that listeners would have dis- structure. The four sequences of each set represented four types of
criminated between these modulation types if they had been modulation: (a) nonmodulating (nonmodulation); (b) modulating
asked directly to compare these two types of key movement. two steps on the cycle of fifths to the key of the supertonic
Thus, in Experiment 1 listeners were presented with a pair (modulation to II); (c) modulating four steps on the cycle of fifths
to the key of the mediant (modulation to IR); and (d) modulating
of sequences on each trial and were instructed to judge the
six steps on the cycle of fifths to the key of the sharpened
relative distance o f key movement in the two sequences subdominant (modulation to IV#).
(i.e., to judge which sequence contained the greater dis-
tance). Therefore, Experiment 1 offered a more direct as-
sessment of the listeners' ability to discriminate between Apparatus and Stimuli
two different distances of key movement.
MIDI files for the eight sequences were created with Composer
Similar to the procedure adopted by Thompson (1993), software (Professional Composer; Mark of the Unicorn, Cam-
sequences were presented with no expressive variation in bridge, MA). The onset-to-onset timing and tone durations were
timing and loudness, a manner of presentation that is re- coded precisely as notated. The loudness for each tone was gen-
ferred to here as mechanical. In the musical instrument erated by setting the MIDI keypress velocity at 64. (Velocity
digital interface (MIDI) files that generated the sequences, values are scaled from 1 to 127.) The tempo of the sequences was
durational values conformed strictly to the notated values of 120 quarter-tone beats per minute, or 0.5 s/beat. A Macintosh
the sequences, and all keypress velocity values, the MIDI SE-30 computer, using customized software, controlled the pre-
analog of loudness, were the same (64 units on a scale sentation of sequences to fisteners. The computer was connected
ranging from 1 to 127). ~ Thus, as well as a test of the by a MIDI interface to a Roland U-20 multitimbral keyboard with
equal temperament tuning. Sequence tones were the sampled piano
reliability of the earlier findings, the current experiment
sounds of the Roland U-20. They were delivered to listeners
provided a baseline against which the influence o f expres- through Sennheiser HD-480 headphones.
sive variation in performance (Experiment 2) could be Each experimental trial consisted of a pair of sequences sepa-
evaluated. rated by a 1-s pause. After each trial, a response cue was displayed
on the computer monitor, and responses were entered directly into
the computer. Trials were self-paced.
Method
Design and Procedure
Listeners
For each sequence set, 12 different conditions were created, as
Ten adult listeners from the York University student population shown in the left side of Table 1. The 12 conditions consisted of
participated in the experiment. All listeners reported normal hear- six types of sequence pairs and two orders of each pair. The six
ing. Listeners were musically trained, having a minimum of 5
years of formal training on a musical instrument and a maximum
of more than 10 years of formal musical training. Listeners were 1 A musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) is a device used
naive, however, with respect to the purpose of the experiment; to record, edit, or play music under computer control. A MIDI file
moreover, none were familiar with the relevant experimental lit- is a file of musical information that may be stored on computer.
erature on the perception and quantification of key distances. The information contained in a standard MIDI file includes pitch,
Listeners were paid $7 for their participation in the experiment. duration, keypress velocity, attack time, and release time.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 121

Figure 3. Harmonized sequences used as stimuli in the experiments (Set A and Set B). The type
of key movement is listed above each sequence.
122 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

types of sequence pairs represented all possible combinations of 1, respectively, of the transformed scale. The judgment that
the four different types of modulation within a given set. The two the second sequence moved farther than the first followed
orders represented, for each pair, the order in which the two types by ratings of 1, 2, or 3 became the ratings 4, 5, and 6 of the
of modulation were presented. For Order 1, the modulation involv- transformed scale. Ratings were subjected to an analysis of
ing fewer steps on the cycle of fifths was presented first; for Order
variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures on three vari-
2, the modulation involving more steps on the cycle of fifths was
presented first. ables: sequence set with two levels (Sets A and B), sequence
The 12 conditions for Set A and 12 conditions for Set B yielded pair with six levels (see Table 1), and order within the
a total of 24 trials. The order of the 24 trials was randomly and sequence pair with two levels (see Table 1).
independently determined for each listener. Listeners were asked No reliable effects attributable to sequence set were ob-
to compare the distance of key movement of the two sequences of tained, so the results are presented collapsed across se-
each pair. First, they were to judge which sequence, the first or the quence set. Figure 4 displays mean ratings for the six types
second presented, involved the greater distance. Second, they were of sequence pairs and for the two orders of presentation
asked to compare the distances involved in the two sequences, and within pairs. For five of the six types of sequence pairs,
assign a rating from 1 (distances are similar) to 3 (distances are ratings were higher if the key movement in the second
very different). Practice trials, selected randomly from the set of
sequence involved a greater number of steps than the first
experimental trials, were provided to acquaint the listeners with the
task. During the practice trials, each listener adjusted the overall sequence on the cycle of fifths. The mean rating for pairs in
loudness to a comfortable listening level (about 65-70 dB SPL). which the second sequence involved a greater number of
Listeners were allowed to quit the practice trials and begin the steps (Order 1) was 4.33, whereas the mean rating for pairs
experimental trials at any time. Thus, the number of practice trials in which the first sequence involved a greater number of
varied from participant to participant. Listeners were not given steps (Order 2) was 3.05. There was a main effect of order
feedback on their judgments either for the practice or experimental on ratings, F(1, 19) = 49.14, p < .0005, reflecting an
trials. overall ability of listeners to discriminate different distances
of key movement.
Results and Discussion For one type of sequence pair (modulation III with mod-
ulation IV#) mean ratings were opposite to predictions
For purposes of data analysis, the two judgments pro- based on the cycle of fifths but consistent with the results of
vided by each listener on each trial were transformed to a Thompson (1993) for these modulation types. More gener-
single rating ranging from 1 to 6. The rating scale reflected ally, listeners discriminated key movements better for some
the perceived extent of key movement of the second se- sequence pairs than for others. This result led to a signifi-
quence of each pair relative to the fLrst. On the transformed cant interaction between order and sequence pair, F(5,
scale, a rating of 1 corresponded to the judgment that the 45) = 10.88,p < .0001. We examined this interaction using
perceived movement of the second sequence was much less six planned comparisons.
than the first, and a rating of 6 corresponded to the judgment For each type of sequence pair, mean ratings were com-
that the perceived movement of the second sequence was pared for both orders in which the two sequences were
much greater than the first. Thus, the listener's judgment presented. The ability to discriminate distance of key move-
that the fn'st sequence moved farther than the second fol- ment for a given pair of sequence types was demonstrated if
lowed by ratings of 1, 2, or 3 became the ratings 3, 2, and mean ratings were significantly different for the two orders

Table 1
Different Conditions for Each Sequence Set Consisting of Six Types of Sequence Pairs and Two Orders for Each Type
Order of presentation Standardized predictions
Type of
sequence pair First Second Cycle of fifths Toroid
Order 1: Fewer lst-greater 2nd
1 Nonmod MOd II 0.35 1.07
2 Nonmod MOd 11I 1.04 1.17
3 Nonmod Mod IV# 1.73 1.59
4 Mod II MOd HI 0.35 0.10
5 Mod II Mod IV# 1.04 0.52
6 Mod HI MOd IV# 0.35 0.42
Order 2: Greater 1st-fewer 2nd
1 MOd II Nonmod -0.35 - 1.07
2 Mod HI Nonmod - 1.04 - 1.17
3 Mod IV# Nonmod - 1.73 - 1.59
4 Mod HI MOd H -0.35 -0.10
5 Mod IV# MOd II -1.04 -0.52
6 Mod IV# Mod HI -0.35 -0.42
Note. Standardized predictions of perceived difference in key movement between members of each pair are based on the cycle of fifths
and toroidal models. Nonmod = nonmodulation; MOd = modulation.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 123

for individual sequences. In spite of differences in task


demands, listeners in both studies did not perceive a mod-
ulation to IV# to involve a significantly greater distance of
key movement than a modulation to HI.
The pattern of results reported here could be predicted
extremely well from the results reported by Thompson
(1993, Experiment 3). A set of predicted ratings for the
current experiment was derived from the mean ratings of
individual sequences reported by Thompson. For each type
of sequence pair, the predicted rating was taken as the mean
rating for the first modulation type minus the mean rating
for the second modulation type. This set of predicted ratings
then was compared with mean ratings obtained in Experi-
ment 1. The correlation between the predicted and the
current ratings was .97 (df = 10, p < .001). This extremely
close correspondence between the current results and those
reported by Thompson (1993) provides strong support for
the validity of the response procedure used in Experiment 1.
Correlation and multiple regression were next conducted
to explore further the perceptual validity of the cycle of
fifths model of key relationships and to compare this model
with the toroidal model of key relationships developed by
Krnmhansl and Kessler (1982). Predicted ratings of key
distances for each of the 12 conditions were obtained for
both the cycle of fifths model and the toroidal model.
Predictions for the cycle of fifths model were based on the
difference in the number of steps of key movement between
the two sequences of each pair. Predictions for the toroidal
Figure 4. Mean ratings for the six types of mechanically pre- model were based on the difference in Euclidian distances
sented sequence pairs, and for the two presentation orders col- traversed between the two sequences of each pair. Euclidian
lapsed across the two sequence sets. Nonmod = nonmodulation; distances were calculated using the coordinates of keys on
Mod = modulation; solid bar = Order 1; hatched bar = Order 2. the toms listed by Krumhansl (1990). Standardized values
of the predictions for both models are shown in Table 1.
Examination of Table 1 reveals some of the differences
of presentation. In general, listeners were better able to between these two models of key distance. For the cycle of
discriminate between nonmodulating and modulating se- fifths model, theoretical distances are linearly related to the
quences than between two different types of modulating number of steps traversed on the cycle of fifths. For exam-
sequences. ple, modulations from C major to D major (2 steps), C
Significant differences were found between orders of major to E major (4 steps), and C major to F-sharp major (6
presentation for four types of sequence pairs (the first four steps) represent equal increases in distance on the cycle of
listed in Table 1): nonmodulating with modulating to II (2 fifths. For the toroidal model, however, theoretical distances
steps on the cycle of fifths), F(1, 9) = 47.21, p < .0001; are not linearly related to the number of steps traversed on
nonmodulating with modulating to HI (4 steps), F(1, 9) = the cycle of fifths. For example, modulations from C major
42.51, p < .0001; noumodulating with modulating to IV# (6 to D major (2 steps) and from C major to E major (4 steps)
steps), F(1, 9) = 47.09, p < .0001; and modulating to II (2 represent similar distances on the toroidal model, and they
steps) with modulating to III (4 steps), F(1, 9) = 81.00, p < represent a shorter distance than the distance from C major
.0001. Significant differences were not obtained for the to F-sharp major (6 steps). (Note that projection of the toms
remaining two types of sequence pairs: modulating to II (2 as two-dimensional [see Figure 2] introduces some distor-
steps) with modulating to IV# (6 steps), F(1, 9) = 1.42, p > tion of the distances; the predicted values in Table 1 were
.25, and modulating to III (4 steps) with modulating to IV# not obtained from Figure 2 but were calculated as the
(6 steps), F(1, 9) = 0.82, p > .35. Euclidian distance in four dimensions.)
The pattern of results within the interaction is contrary to The mean ratings for the 12 conditions were significantly
both the cycle of fifths and the toroidal model of key correlated with the predictions of both models. The corre-
relationships. For both, greater distance in key movement lation between mean ratings and predictions based on the
was associated with a modulation to IV# (6 steps) than cycle of fifths model was .78 (df = 10, p < .01). The
either a modulation to II (2 steps) or a modulation to III (4 correlation between mean ratings and predictions based on
steps). However, the pattern of ratings is consistent with the toroidal model was .87 (df = 10, p < .001).
results reported by Thompson (1993, Experiment 3). In that Multiple regression then was conducted to assess the
study, listeners rated the perceived distance of modulation relative predictive power of the two models. Both predictors
124 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

were entered, and the significance of the beta weight (re- he wished until he was reasonably satisfied. The performer also
gression coefficient) for each predictor was tested. The beta was given the opportunity to edit the keypress velocity, onset, and
weight for the cycle of fifths model, - . 1 1 , was not signif- duration information in the MIDI files. These minor adjustments
icantly different from zero. The beta weight for the toroidal ensured that the final version was perfectly consistent with the
performer's intentions. For example, if the performer felt that a
model, .98, was significant, t(9) = 2.4, p < .05. This result
specific note was too loud, he could reduce the MIDI velocity
suggests that the toroidal model had predictive power sig- value for that note. (Playing on the Roland keyboard did not
nificantly beyond what could be explained by the cycle of always result in a performance that was satisfactory to the per-
fifths model. former. The Roland keyboard is touch sensitive, but it is not
The results provide support for the toroidal model over weighted the same as an acoustic piano keyboard.) The pianist did
the cycle of fifths model as a perceptual model of key not know the precise aims of the research.
relationships. Both models, however, make certain predic- In a final step, the tempo of performed sequences was adjusted
tions that are incompatible with the experimental results. As so that it was perceptually equivalent to the tempo of sequences
noted earlier, both models predict that a modulation to IV# presented in Experiment 1. This step was achieved by setting the
(6 steps on the cycle of fifths) should induce greater per- tempo of each performed sequence such that the duration of the
ceived key movement than a modulation to III (4 steps). fLrSt half of the performed sequence was equal to the duration of
the first half of the mechanical version. Because performed se-
However, listeners were unable to discriminate between the quences involved a dtardando (i.e., a slowing of tempo) at the end
key movement involved in these two types of sequences. of the second phrase, the total duration of performed sequences
ranged from 1 to 3 s longer than the total duration of mechanical
Experiment 2 sequences. For each sequencel we and one other musically trained
listener verified that the adjustment resulted in perceived equiva-
Experiment 1 established that listeners were able to per- lence of tempo between the mechanical and performed versions.
ceive differences in key movement for four of the six types
of sequence pairs tested. However, modulation to IV# was
Description of Performance Expression
not perceived to involve a greater distance than either mod-
ulation to II or modulation to IH. This finding corroborates The MIDI files of performed sequences were analyzed to check
the results reported by Thompson (1993, Experiment 3) and that the timing and loudness patterns were consistent with expres-
raises questions about the models of key relationships sive patterns typically observed in music performances (e.g., see
tested. Palmer, 1989). More extensive analyses of the MIDI files, moti-
Experiment 2 replicated the procedure of Experiment 1, vated by the experimental results, are presented later. Two types of
with one important difference: The MIDI files for the se- expressive variation were examined in the initial MIDI analysis:
quences were recorded from the keyboard performance of a expressive differences between chords and melodic emphasis.
Expressive differences between chords describe overall patterns of
highly trained musician. The second experiment, in con-
timing (accelerando and ritardando) and loudness (crescendo and
junction with the first, addressed the question whether the diminuendo). Melodic emphasis may occur through timing or
expressive actions o f a performer may influence the percep- loudness.
tion of key movement. In particular, we examined the Expressivedifferencesbetweenchords. Within each sequence,
possibility that when performance expression is present, timing and loudness data were collected for 14 chord locations,
listeners' judgments may correspond more closely to theo- referenced by bar and beat numbers. Each sequence consisted of
retical representations of key distance. four bars (separated by bar lines), and each bar consisted of four
(quarter-note) beats. Chords occurred on Beats 1-7 (Bars 1 and 2)
and on Beats 9-15 (Bars 3 and 4). Bars 1 and 2 are referred to as
Method Phrase 1, and Bars 3 and 4 are referred to as Phrase 2.
The expressive use of timing for the 14 chords was assessed with
Listeners reference to the interonset interval (IOI) ratio. The IOI ratio is the
ratio between the IOIs in performed sequences and the correspond-
Ten adult listeners from the York University community, fitting ing IOIs in mechanical sequences. The IOI between successive
the same description as the listeners in Experiment 1, participated chords was calculated as the time interval between the average
in this experiment. None had participated in Experiment 1. onset time for voices in the current chord and the average onset
time for voices in the subsequent chord, disregarding notes not
occurring on Beats 1-7 or Beats 9-15. Because no IOI can be
Sequences calculated for the last chord of a sequence, the IOI ratio for this
The sequences were the same as those used in Experiment 1 (see chord was replaced with the ratio between the average duration of
Figure 3). voices in performed sequences and the average duration of voices
in mechanical sequences.
Figure 5 plots the IOI ratio for the 14 chords in the sequences
Apparatus and Stimuli averaged across sequences. The pattern of IOI ratio values indi-
cates a decrease in tempo at the end of each phrase, particularly at
The apparatus was the same as that used in Experiment 1. The the end of the second phrase. This pattern of timing, referred to as
sequences were performed on the Roland U-20 keyboard by a a ritardando, typically is used to convey a sense of closure at
professional pianist and recorded as MIDI files on computer. The phrase boundaries.
pianist was asked to produce a musically acceptable performance Examination of IOI ratio values in individual sequences indi-
of each sequence and was allowed to produce as many versions as cated that the use of timing was consistent among the eight
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 125

1.4" loudness of each chord was approximated as the average keypress


velocity for all voices in the chord. Notes not occurring on Beats
1-7 or Beats 9-15 were excluded from these calculations.
O 1.3 Figure 6 plots the average keypress velocity for the 14 chords in
the sequences averaged across all sequences. Most evident from
Figure 6 is a general tendency to shape each phrase with a
m crescendo (i.e., an increase in loudness) followed by a diminuendo
1.2

,//
(i.e., a decrease in loudness). Compared with the initial phrase, the
final phrase involved a greater range of loudness values. This
0
m 1.1
increased range in loudness may have helped to establish a stron-
ger sense of closure in the final phrase.
Examination of keypress velocity values in each sequence indi-
cated a moderate consistency in the use of loudness among the

al 1.0
9) eight performed sequences. Twenty-five of the 28 correlation
:!
values in the intercorrelation matrix were significant at the .05
level (df = 12). Correlation coefficients ranged from .18 to .91,
0.9 I ' | ' J ' | ' | ' | ' | ' | ' | ' l ' | ' l ' l ' l ' l ' l "
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
and the average correlation between sequences was .60.
Phrm 1 ~rm 2
Melodic emphasis. The soprano voice or melody typically is
emphasized through both timing (melodic asynchrony) and loud-
Beat ness. In general, notes in the soprano voice tend to occur before (or
Figure 5. Mean interonset interval (IOI) ratio of chords occur- "lead") other notes at the same notated position (Palmer, 1989).
ring on each quarter-note beat in the four bars averaged across type This feature of musical performance, known as melodic asyn-
of modulation and sequence set. Chords occur on Beats 1-7 chrony, also was found in the current performances. Melodic
(Phrase 1) and on Beats 9-15 (Phrase 2). asynchrony was examined by calculating the difference between
the onset of each note in the soprano voice and the average onset
for other voices at the same notated position. On average, the
soprano voice was played 16.67 ms before other voices, which was
performed sequences. The set of 14 IOI ratio values for each significantly different from zero, t(l14) = 11.37, p < .0001.
sequence was highly correlated with the set of IOI ratio values for
Loudness also is typically used to emphasize the melodic line
all other sequences. The 28 correlation values in the complete
(Palmer, 1989). Consistent with this feature of musical perfor-
intercorrelation matrix were all significant at the .01 level (df =
mance, keypress velocities in the current performances were in-
12). Correlation coefficients ranged from .75 to .97, and the
average correlation between sequences was .87. deed greater in the soprano voice than in other voices. The average
The expressive use of loudness for the 14 chords was measured keypress velocity of notes in the soprano voice was 64.29, whereas
by keypress velocity. Keypress velocity is strongly related to the average keypress velocity of notes in other voices was 43.38,
perceived loudness and is measured on a scale from 1 to 127. The t(494) = 16.62, p < .0001.

~3

&=
U
~= 60
U
m
0

I
m
Q

0
,Id
C
m
0
| | - | - ! | - i | | J - i . i - | - i o i i i

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Phrase I Phrase 2
Beat
Figure 6. Mean keypress velocity of chords occurring on each quarter-note beat in the four bars
averaged across type of modulation and sequence set. Chords occur on Beats 1-7 (Phrase 1) and on
Beats 9-15 (Phrase 2).
126 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

Design and Procedure quence involved a greater number of steps was 2.81, F(1,
9) = 120.86, p < .0001.
The design and procedure were identical to those used in Ex- As expected, the size of the effect of order depended on
periment 1. the type of sequence pair. This result led to a significant
interaction between sequence pair and order, F(5, 45) =
Results and Discussion 8.71, p < .0001, which was examined further using six
planned comparisons.
Judgments again were transformed to ratings on a scale Significant differences again were found between orders
ranging from 1 to 6. As before, ratings were subjected to an of presentation for the three types of sequence pairs that
ANOVA with repeated measures on three variables: se- included a nonmodulating sequence (the first three listed in
quence set with two levels (Sets A and B), sequence pair Table 1): nonmodulating with modulating to II, F(1, 9) =
with six levels, and order within sequence pair with two 35.27, p < .0001; nonmodulating with modulating to III,
levels. F(1, 9) = 46.10, p < .0001; and nonmodulating with
No reliable effects attributable to sequence set were ob- modulating to IV#, F(I, 9) = 270.00, p < .0001. The results
tained, so the results are presented collapsed across se- for sequence pairs that involved two modulating sequences
quence set. Figure 7 displays mean ratings for the six types were different from the results of Experiment 1. Contrary to
of sequence pairs and for the two orders of presentation the results of Experiment 1, significant differences were
within pairs. As in Experiment 1, there was a main effect of obtained between orders of presentation for the two types of
order of presentation within sequence pairs. The mean rat- sequence pairs that included a modulation to IV#: modulat-
ing for pairs in which the second sequence involved a ing to II with modulating to IV#, F(1, 9) = 13.79, p < .01,
greater number of steps on the cycle of fifths was 4.46, and modulating to BI with modulating to IV#, F(1, 9) =
whereas the mean rating for pairs in which the first se- 13.85, p < .01. The remaining sequence pair, modulating to
II with modulating to HI, which had yielded a significant
difference between orders of presentation in Experiment 1,
did not yield a significant difference in this experiment, F(I,
9) = 2.37, p > .15 (although the difference was in the same
direction).
This pattern of results corresponds closely to predictions
based on the toroidal model. First, the toroidal model pre-
dicts similar distances for modulation to II (2 steps) and
modulation to liI (4 steps), and listeners indeed did not
significantly discriminate between these modulation types.
Second, both the toroidal model and the cycle of fifths
predict that modulation to IV# (6 steps) involves greater
distance than other modulation types, and listeners did make
this discrimination.
The mean ratings for the 12 conditions were significantly
correlated with the predictions of both the cycle of fifths
model and the toroidal model (see Table 1). The correlation
between mean ratings and predictions based on the cycle of
fifths model was .92 (dr = 10, p < .001). The correlation
between mean ratings and predictions based on the toroidai
model was .98 (dr = 10, p < .001). Multiple regression then
was conducted to assess the relative predictive power of the
two models. The beta weight for the cycle of fifths model,
.10, was not significantly different from zero. The beta
weight for the toroidal model, .89, was significant, t(9) =
5.61, p < .0005. This result indicates that the toroidal model
had predictive power significantly beyond what could be
explained by the cycle of fifths model. In fact, the toroidal
model provided an excellent description of perceived rela-
tionships between keys.
Three statistical analyses of combined data from the two
experiments confirmed that judgments of key movement
were significantly different for performed and mechanical
Figure 7. Mean ratings for the six types of performed sequence versions of sequences. First, a mixed-design ANOVA was
pairs and for the two presentation orders collapsed across the two conducted, treating the two experiments as a between-
sequence sets. Nonmod = nonmodulation; Mod = modulation; subjects variable (mechanical vs. performed versions).
solid bar = Order 1; hatched bar = Order 2. Within-subjects variables were sequence set with two lev-
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 127

els, sequence pair with six levels, and order within sequence thereby increasing the correspondence between judgments
pair with two levels. As found when results for Experiments and theoretical predictions.
1 and 2 were analyzed separately, there was a main effect of A second effect of performance expression was to de-
order, F(1, 18) = 153.78, p < .001, and an interaction crease perceived key movement in sequences modulating to
between order and sequence pair, F(5, 90) = 16.70, p < III (relative to other sequences), resulting in poorer discrim-
.001. The main effect of order indicated an overall ability of ination between sequences modulating to II and sequences
listeners to discriminate different distances of key move- modulating to III. Sequences modulating to III were judged
ment. The interaction between order and sequence pair to involve greater key movement than all other modulation
indicated that listeners discriminated key movements better types when they were presented mechanically. However,
for some sequence pairs than for others. Most importantly, the toroidal model predicts that perceived key movement for
however, there was an interaction among order, sequence this modulation type should be similar to that for modula-
pair, and experiment, F(5, 90) = 3.20, p < .011, confnTning tion to II and less than that for modulation to IV# (see Table
that performance expression influenced judgments of key 1). Because performance expression decreased perceived
movement. The means from Experiments 1 and 2 are listed key movement in sequences modulating to 111, judgments
in the Appendix. for performed sequences were compatible with this predic-
Second, correlations between theoretical predictions and tion. Listeners did not judge modulation to HI to involve
the mean ratings for mechanical sequences (Experiment 1) significantly greater key movement than modulation to II,
were compared with correlations between theoretical pre- but they did judge modulation to III to involve significantly
dictions and mean ratings for performed sequences (Exper- less key movement than modulation to IV#.
iment 2). For the cycle of fifths, the correlation between Experiment 3 was designed to confirm these two effects
predicted ratings and mean ratings of performed sequences of performance expression by asking listeners to make
(r = .92) was significantly higher than the correlation direct comparisons between mechanical and performed ver-
between predicted ratings and the mean ratings of mechan- sions of each sequence. That is, listeners were presented a
ical sequences (r = .78), t(9) = 2.24, p < .05. Similarly, for mechanical version, followed by a performed version, and
the toroidal model, the correlation between predicted ratings they were asked to indicate whether the performance ex-
and mean ratings of performed sequences (r = .98) was pression increased, decreased, or did not change the per-
significantly higher than the correlation between predicted ceived degree of key movement. We predicted that perfor-
ratings and mean ratings of mechanical sequences (r = .87), mance expression should increase perceived key movement
t(9) = 3.26, p < .01. most for sequences modulating to IV# and least for se-
quences modulating to HI.
Finally, we obtained the correlation between ratings by
individual participants and theoretical predictions based on
the cycle of fifths and toroidal models. The set of 10
correlation coefficients for mechanical sequences (one for Experiment 3
each participant in Experiment 1) then was compared with
Experiment 3 provided a within-subjects test of the influ-
the set of 10 correlation coefficients for performed se-
ence of performance expression. Each modulating mechan-
quences (Experiment 2). For the cycle of fifths model,
ical sequence was paired with the performed version of the
correlations between predicted and actual ratings were sig-
same sequence, and listeners were asked to judge the effect
nificantly higher when performed sequences were presented of performance expression on their perception of the key
(mean r = .79) than when mechanical sequences were movement. The mechanical version always was presented
presented (mean r = .62), t(18) = 2.28, p < .05. Similarly, first, as a standard, and the listener judged whether the
for the toroidal model, correlations between predicted and effect of performance expression was to increase or to
actual ratings were significantly higher when performed decrease the perceived distance of key movement.
sequences were presented (mean r = .85) than when me- Nonmodulating sequences were excluded from Experi-
chanical sequences were presented (mean r = .68), t(18) = ment 3 for the following reason: To verify theoretical pre-
2.29, p < .05. dictions, listeners were asked to judge the effects of perfor-
A comparison of Figures 4 and 5 reveals two important mance expression on perceived distance of key movement.
shifts that led to a greater correspondence, for performed Nonmodulating sequences, by definition, do not involve key
sequences, between judgments of key movement and theo- movement. Thus, excluding nonmodulating sequences en-
retical predictions. First, performance expression had the sured that presentations were consistent with task demands.
effect of increasing perceived key movement in sequences On the basis of a comparison of the results of Experi-
modulating to IV# (relative to other sequences), resulting in ments 1 and 2, we predicted that the effect of performance
greater discrimination between sequence pairs involving expression would vary with the type of modulation. In
this type of key movement. Key movement to IV# was particular, performance expression should lead to the great-
underestimated (with respect to theoretical models) when est increase in perceived key movement for sequences mod-
sequences were presented mechanically. Because perfor- ulating to IV# and the least increase (or the most decrease)
mance expression increased perceived key movement for in perceived key movement for sequences modulating to III.
these sequences, performed versions of the sequences were Performance expression should have an intermediate effect
no longer underestimated with respect to theoretical models, on judgments of sequences modulating to II.
128 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

Experiment 3 also explored the effects of modified per- versions preserved the between-chords performance expression of
formance expression on perceived key movement. The ra- the sequences presented in Experiment 2. That is, they preserved
tionale for the modification was based on a finding reported differences in onset timing and loudness for the 14 chords of each
sequence. However, the MIDI values for time of onset and key-
by Thompson (1993). In that study, ratings of perceived key
press velocity for each note were set to the average value of the
movement for harmonized sequences did not differ for four notes of each chord. Thus, the modification preserved the
modulations to III and modulations to IV# (a finding rep- chord-to-chord expression found in performed versions, but it did
licated in the current Experiment 1). However, when indi- not preserve expressive differences between individual voices (so-
vidual voices of those sequences were presented (i.e., so- prano, alto, tenor, bass). The methods for presenting stimuli and
prano, alto, tenor, bass), ratings for these two types of collecting responses were the same as those used in the previous
modulation were in fact differentiated. Thompson argued experiments.
that individual voices may carry informative cues about key
structure that are masked when voices are combined to form Design and Procedure
mechanically presented harmonized sequences.
It may be expected that such cues are less masked in Each trial consisted of a mechanical version of a sequence,
performed sequences, when independence of individual followed by a performed or a modified version of the same
voices is emphasized through expressive cues (Palmer, sequence. The pairing of each of six mechanical versions of the
1989). If individual voices provide important cues about modulating sequences with the performed version of the sequence,
key relationships, then removing expressive differences be- or the modified version of the sequence, yielded 12 pairs. Six
additional altered versions of performances, again created by ed-
tween individual voices from performed versions should
iting the MIDI fries, also were included in the experimental session
reduce or eliminate the effect of performance expression on but are not discussed.2 The order of presentations was determined
perceived key movement. Alternatively, if expressive dif- randomly and independently for each listener.
ferences between voices do not provide important cues Listeners f'trst were asked to judge whether the performance
about key relationships, then removing these differences expression involved in the second member of a pair enhanced or
should give rise to the same differences found with full reduced the perceived distance of key movement compared with
performance expression. the mechanical version. Second, listeners indicated the degree of
Modified versions of sequences were created by editing the effect on a scale from 1 (notmuch) to 3 (a great deal). Practice
the MIDI files of performed versions. The modification trials were provided as in the previous experiments.
preserved only the performance expression between chords
(simultaneities) and therefore still involved deviations from Results and Discussion
the notated, or mechanical, versions in loudness and timing.
However, modified versions involved no expressive differ- Similar to the procedure of Experiments 1 and 2, the two
ences between individual voices. Each modified version of judgments were transformed to a score on a scale from 1 to
a sequence was paired with the mechanical version of that 6, where 1 indicated that the expression used greatly re-
sequence, and listeners were asked to judge the effect of duced the perceived distance of key movement and 6 indi-
performance expression on perceived distance of key move- cated that the expression used greatly increased the per-
ment. The question addressed was whether the results for ceived distance of key movement. The data were subjected
modified versions would correspond to the results for per- to a two-variable repeated measures ANOVA. The two
formed versions. variables were modulation type (modulation to II, modula-
tion to HI, and modulation to IV#) and comparison type
(performed and mechanical, and modified and mechanical).
Method Figure 8 shows the mean ratings for comparisons of
performed and mechanical versions and for comparisons of
Participants modified and mechanical versions. Data are shown for the
Ten adult listeners from the York University community, fitting three types of modulation and are collapsed across the two
the same description as the listeners in Experiment 1, participated sequence sets.
in this experiment. None had participated in Experiments 1 and 2. All comparisons produced mean ratings above the mid-
point of the scale (3.5), a result suggesting that both per-
formed and modified versions enhanced the sense of key
Sequences movement to some degree. Moreover, the mean rating for
The six modulating sequences shown in Figure 3 were used. performed versions (4.53) was significantly higher than the

Apparatus and Stimuli 2 The condition was included only for exploratory purposes.
These versions were created by shifting the onset time and velocity
The apparatus was the same as that used in Experiment 1. The of each note such that the average onset time and velocity of all
stimuli were 18 pairs, in which the first member of the pair was a chords were identical. Thus, these versions maintained relative
mechanical version of one of the six modulating sequences, and differences between notes within chords but involved no overall
the second member was either a performed or modified version of differences between chords. In addition to eliminating chord-to-
that sequence. Mechanical and performed versions were the stim- chord expression, the procedure distorts the phrasing of individual
uli presented in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. Modified voices. No reliable differences were found for this condition.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 129

chord-to-chord expression alone was not sufficient to rep-


licate the effect found for fully performed versions.

Experiment 4

The results of Experiment 3 confLrm that performance


expression influenced perceived key movement in the di-
rection of the toroidal model. These effects were found for
performed versions, but not for modified versions in which
expressive differences between individual voices were re-
moved. The latter finding suggests that the effects of per-
formance expression depended on expressive differences
between voices.
There is, however, an alternative explanation for this
potentially important finding. It is possible that removing
expressive differences between voices seriously degraded
the musicality of the sequences and listeners were unable to
judge the effects of such expression on perceived key move-
ment. If this were the case, it would be difficult to draw
conclusions regarding the role of expressive differences
between voices in conveying key movement. However, if
modified and performed versions are judged to be similar in
overall musicality, one could assume that expressive differ-
ences between voices specifically affect the perception of
key movement. Thus, Experiment 4 was conducted to con-
firm that the modification to the performed versions did not
merely degrade the perceived musicality of the stimuli.

Figure & Mean ratings of the influence of performance expres- Me~od


sion on perceived key movement for performed and modified
versions and for each type of modulation (Mod) averaged across Participants
the two sequence sets. Bars in each set are as follows: left = Mod
II, middle = Mod HI, and right = Mod IV#. Ten adult listeners from the York University community, fitting
the same description as the listeners in Experiment 1, participated
in this experiment. None had been involved in the previous
experiments.
mean rating for modified versions (3.85), F(1, 9) = 10.74,
p < .01, indicating that the expressive differences between
voices generally enhanced perceived key movement. Sequences
Inspection of Figure 8 reveals that only for comparisons
involving performed versions were the results compatible The eight sequences shown in Figure 3 were used.
with the prediction based on the previous two experiments.
For comparisons involving performed versions, the mean Apparatus and Stimuli
rating for sequences modulating to IV# was highest, the
mean rating for sequences modulating to HI was lowest, and The apparatus was the same as that used in Experiment 1.
the mean rating for sequences modulating to II was inter- Different versions of each sequence were used--mechanical, per-
mediate. A planned trend analysis supported this observa- formed, and modified versions---following the method outlined in
tion; the linear trend (comparing modulation to IV# with Experiment 3. Methods for stimulus presentation and response
collection were the same as those used in the previous experi-
modulation to HI) was significant, F(1, 9) = 6.74, p < .03,
ments. All modulation types were included in the experiment:
whereas the quadratic trend (comparing modulating to II nonmodulation and modulation to II, III, and IV#.
with the average of the other two types of modulation) was
not (F < 1). No effects of type of modulation were found
for comparisons involving modified versions. Thus, the Design and Procedure
effects found for performed versions were not found for
Each trial consisted of the presentation of one version of one of
modified versions.
the sequences. After each presentation, tisteners were asked to rate
The results of Experiment 3 confirm that performance the musicality of the performance on a scale from 1 (unmusical) to
expression influenced judgments such that they corre- 7 (very musical). The 24 trials--Eight Sequences Three Ver-
sponded more closely to the toroidal model of key relation- sions--were presented in an order that was determined randomly
ships. However, expressive differences between individual and independently for each listener. Eight other altered versions of
voices must be present for this influence to be effective; performances, also created by editing the MIDI files, were in-
130 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

cluded in the experimental session but are not discussed.3 Practice next phase of our research involved an analysis of MIDI
trials were provided as in previous experiments. files of performances, with the aim of identifying perfor-
mance variables that may have been responsible for the
Results and Discussion experimental effects (thus, these analyses went beyond the
Figure 9 displays mean ratings for each of the three general descriptions of performances provided in Experi-
versions for each type of modulation. Data were averaged ment 2). The analyses in this section were motivated by the
across the two sequence sets. Ratings were subjected to an discovery that performance expression could influence
ANOVA with repeated measures on three variables: perfor- judgments of key movement (Experiments 1-3) and that
mance version with three levels, type of modulation with this influence depended on expressive differences between
four levels, and sequence set (A vs. B). individual notes, not on expressive differences between
There was a significant main effect of performance ver- chords (Experiments 3 and 4).
sion, F(2, 18) = 11.97, p < .001. Examination of Figure 9 Three variables available from MIDI fries were consid-
indicates that mechanical versions were perceived to be ered: melodic asynehrony, IOI ratio, and keypress velocity.
relatively unmusical compared with the performed and The aim of the analysis was to assess the possibility that the
modified versions. Post hoc comparisons indicated that the loudness and/or timing of individual notes reflected (among
former versions were assigned significantly lower ratings other aspects of musical structure) the tonal stability of
than the latter versions, F(1, 9) = 12.70, p < .01. However, notes with respect to overall key. By conveying tonal sta-
there was no significant difference between the performed bility through loudness, timing, or both, performance ex-
and modified versions. These findings confirm that, al- pression might provide listeners with an additional source of
though modifications disrupted the effect of performance information to determine key and key movement.
expression on judgments of key movement, modifications The tonal stability of notes was estimated using tonal
had no effect on the musical quality of performances. hierarchy values reported by Krumhansl and Kessler (1982)
Of secondary interest, there was a significant main effect and described in detail by Krumhansl (1990). These tonal
of type of modulation, F(3, 27) = 3.28, p < .05. Examina- hierarchy values can be used to derive both the cycle of
fifths and the toroidal models of key relationships (Krum-
tion of Figure 9 suggests that listeners perceived sequences
modulating to IV# to be the least musical of all sequences. hansl, 1990). Therefore, a link between expressive variables
Mean ratings for the other three types of modulation were and tonal hierarchy values could account for the correspon-
similar. There was no significant effect of sequence set. dence between judgments of key movement in performed
sequences and predictions based on the cycle of fifths and
toroidal models.
Performance Variation Reflects Knowledge o f Key:
Tonal hierarchy values were assigned to notes by deter-
Evidence F r o m an Analysis o f MIDI Files mining the scale degree of all notes relative to the current
o f Performances key (i.e., tonic, sharpened tonic, etc.). The key of Phrase 1
was identified as C major for all eight sequences. The key
The results of the experiments indicate that performance of Phrase 2 was identified differently depending on the type
expression influenced judgments of key movement. The of modulation. For nonmodulating sequences, the key of
Phrase 2 was identified as C major. For sequences modu-
7-
lating to H, HI, and IV#, the key of Phrase 2 was identified
as D major, E major, and F-sharp major, respectively:

Melodic Asynchrony
11
5
c First, the complete set of 115 melodic asynchrony values
el was compared with the corresponding set of tonal hierarchy
'- 4

| 3 These versions were identical to the altered versions included


II 3 in Experiment 3 (see Footnote 2). Judgments for these versions
were not included in the statistical analysis. However, it is notable
that these versions and mechanical versions were assigned similar
2 ratings of musicality, indicating that judgments of musicality were
not based on the presence of deviations from mechanical
presentation.
i i i !
4 For modulating sequences, we also considered the possibility
Nonmod Mod II Mod III Mod IV#
that the performer may have treated the beginning of Phrase 2 as
the key of C major, and it may be appropriate to identify the point
Version
of key change at a later temporal position in the sequence (e.g.,
Figure 9. Mean ratings of musicality for each of the three ver- when the fwst nondiatonic note occurs). However, alternative
sions (mechanical [0], performed [Iq], and modified [lid for each interpretations of the precise time of the key change within Bar 3
type of modulation (MOd) averaged across the two sequence sets. had no or little effect on the overall correlation between expressive
Nomod = nonmodulation. variables and the tonal hierarchy.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 131

values. There was no correlation between the degree of A multiple regression analysis indicated that for soprano
melodic asynchrony and the tonal hierarchy value of notes, and bass voices, the tonal hierarchy had predictive power
r(l13) = .01, ns. Two additional analyses also failed to beyond what was explained by predictors based on phrasing
reveal a relationship between melodic asynchrony and the and metric stress. We evaluated several predictors related to
tonal hierarchy. First, the tonal hierarchy provided no phrasing and metric stress: The phrasing predictor with the
unique l~redictive power in a number of multiple regression greatest predictive power was created by coding notes at
models. Second, average asynehrony values for the 12 phrase boundaries as 1 and other notes as 0. The metric
scale degrees (collapsed across all soprano notes in all eight stress predictor was created by coding notes at the first,
sequences) were uncorrelated with the corresponding tonal second, third, and fourth beats of each bar as 3, 1, 2, and 1,
hierarchy values, r(10) = .16, ns. respectively (see Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983). We then
entered these predictors and the tonal hierarchy predictor
into a multiple regression analysis. The multiple correlation
I 0 1 Ratio for this three-predictor model was .34 for the soprano voice
(p < .01) and .45 for the bass voice (p < .01). Beta weights
Next, we examined the IOI ratio of notes separately for for the phrase boundary predictor were significant for the
each individual voice. The IOI for a given note was calcu- soprano voice (/8 = .290, p < .01), but not significant for
lated as the time interval between the onset of the note and the bass voice (/8 = .180, p < .09). Beta weights for the
the onset of the subsequent note. The IOI ratio of each note metric stress predictor were significant for both the soprano
in performed sequences was obtained by dividing the I0I of voice (18 = -.282, p < .01) and the bass voice (18 = -.342,
the note by the IOI of the corresponding note in mechanical p < .001). Most importantly, beta weights for the tonal
sequences. Because no IOI can be calculated for the final hierarchy predictor were significant for both the soprano
note of any sequence, the IOI ratio for these notes was voice (/3 = . 156, p < .05) and the bass voice (18 = .313, p <
replaced with the ratio between the duration of the note in .001).
the performed sequence and the duration of the note in the Finally, the average IOI ratio for each of the 12 scale
mechanical sequence. Because the tempi of performed and degrees was calculated (collapsed across all sequences), and
mechanical sequences were equated, the ratios provided an these average values were compared with the corresponding
appropriate measure of the relative lengthening and short- tonal hierarchy values in a correlation analysis. This ap-
ening of durations in performances. Tonal hierarchy values proach was adopted to diminish the effects of local influ-
were assigned to notes in the manner described previously. ences (e.g., metric stress, intervalllc movement, fingering)
Evaluation of IOI ratio was done in two ways, as had been and to highlight the overall relationship between IOI ratio
done for the analysis of melodic asynchrony. First, we and tonal stability. The soprano voice contained instances of
considered the complete set of IOI ratio values for each all 12 scale degrees. Alto, tenor, and bass voices contained
voice in all eight sequences. This analysis considered notes 9, 11, and 10 of the 12 scale degrees, respectively. Average
in both phrases, but we also examined notes only in Phrase IOI ratio values were correlated significantly with the tonal
2. (The type of modulation always was established in Phrase hierarchy for the soprano voice, r(10) = .71, p < .01, and
2, so information related to key should have been clearly for the bass voice, r(8) = .62, p < .05. Average IOI ratio
evident in this phrase.) Second, we considered the average values were not correlated significantly with the tonal hier-
IOI ratio values for each of the 12 scale degrees (i.e., tonic, archy for the alto voice, r(7) = -.20, ns, or for the tenor
sharpened tonic, etc.), collapsed across all sequences. voice, r(9) = .44, ns.
For the soprano voice, there was a small but significant
correlation between the set of IOI ratio values and the tonal Keypress Velocity
hierarchy values, r(154) = .20, p < .05. The correlation was
increased slightly when only notes within the second phrase As with IOI ratio, evaluation of keypress velocity was
were considered, r(82) = .23, p < .05. For the bass voice, done in two ways. We first considered the complete set of
there also was a significant correlation between the set of keypress velocity values for each voice in all eight se-
IOI ratio values and the tonal hierarchy values, r(118) = quences. We next considered average keypress velocity
.32, p < .001. Again, this correlation was increased when values for each of the 12 scale degrees (collapsed across
only notes within the second phrase were considered, sequences).
r(54) = .42, p < .001. There were no significant correla- For the soprano voice, the correlation between the com-
tions between IOI ratio and the tonal hierarchy for inner plete set of keypress velocity values and the tonal hierarchy
voices. values was not significant, r(154) = - . 1 4 , p < .08. A small
This correspondence between IOI ratio and tonal hierar- but significant correlation was found when only notes
chy values, for soprano and bass voices, also held when
correlations were examined separately for each of the eight
5 Multiple regression was explored to assess the possibility that
sequences. For the soprano voice, the mean correlation was a relationship between melodic asynchrony and the tonal hierarchy
.20, which was significantly different from zero, t(7) = was suppressed by other covariates (e.g., metric stress, notated
4.90, p < .002. For the bass voice, the mean correlation was duration, loudness). However, even after statistically controlling
.36, which also was significantly different from zero, t(7) = for the effects of other variables, there was no evidence for any
6.66, p < .00t. relationship between melodic asynchrony and the tonal hierarchy.
132 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

within the second phrase were considered, r(82) = -.26, performance expression serves to enhance or modify (or
p < .05. For the bass voice, the correlation between key- both) information that is already present in the score. Nev-
press velocity and the tonal hierarchy was significant, ertheless, the effects of performance expression are system-
r(l18) = -.27, p < .01. This correlation was increased atic and significant, thus implicating performance as a cru-
when only notes within the second phrase were considered, cial component of musical communication.
r(54) = -.36, p < .001. There were no significant corre- A comparison of the results of Experiments 1 and 2
lations associated with alto or tenor voices. suggests ~that judgments of key movement were not the
This correspondence between tonal hierarchy values and same for performed versions as they were for mechanical
keypress velocity in the bass voice also held when correla- versions of the same sequences. In particular, performance
tions were examined separately for each of the eight se- expression brought judgments in line with theoretical pre-
quences. The mean correlation for the eight bass voices dictions based on the cycle of fifths and toroidal models of
was -.27, which was significantly different from zero, key relationships. In Experiment 3, we confirmed this effect
t(7) = -3.48, p < .01. of performance expression by having listeners compare
A multiple regression analysis indicated that for the bass directly mechanical and performed versions of the same
voice, the tonal hierarchy had predictive power beyond what sequences.
was explained by predictors based on phrasing and metric The results of Experiment 3 also indicate that if per-
stress. We entered predictors based on phrasing and metric formed versions were modified by removing expressive
stress (described earlier) with the tonal hierarchy predictor differences between voices, the effect of performance ex-
into a multiple regression analysis. The multiple correlation pression on judgments was eliminated. Modified versions
for this three-predictor model was .38 (p < .01). The beta that did not involve expressive differences between individ-
weight was significant for the phrase boundary predictor ual voices were comparable to mechanical versions in terms
(/3 = -.309, p < .01) and marginally significant for the of their impact on perceived key movement. This finding
tonal hierarchy predictor (/3 = -.177, p < .056). The beta suggests that expressive differences between individual
weight was not significant for the metric stress predictor voices were responsible for the effects of performance ex-
(/3 = .077, p < .40). pression on judgments of key movement.
The average keypress velocity for each of the 12 scale The results of Experiment 4 reveal that, unlike judgments
degrees then was calculated (across sequences), and these of key movement, judgments of musicality did not depend
average values were compared with tonal hierarchy values on expressive differences between individual voices. Mod-
in a correlation analysis. The correlation between average ified versions that preserved between-chords expression
keypress velocities and the tonal hierarchy values was not were judged to be as musical as performed versions by
significant for the soprano voice, r(10) = .49, ns, or the moderately trained listeners. This finding suggests that ex-
tenor voice, r(9) = -.30, ns. However, average keypress pressive differences between individual voices are not es-
velocity values were significantly correlated with tonal hi- sential to the overall musical quality of performed versions
erarchy values for the alto voice, r(7) = -.61, p < .05, and even though they provide essential cues for the perception
the bass voice, r(8) = -.84, p < .01. It may be noted that of key movement.
the negative coefficients associated with loudness indicate a An analysis of MIDI fries for performed versions identi-
tendency to emphasize tonally unstable notes and deem pha- fied two possible sources of information about key structure
size stable notes, as Sundberg et al. (1983) predicted. in performances. First, there was a significant relationship
between the timing (IOI ratio) of individual notes and the
tonal hierarchy, particularly for the soprano voice. Second,
Summary of MIDI Analysis there was a significant relationship between the keypress
velocity of individual notes and the tonal hierarchy, partic-
The analysis of the MIDI files revealed that within indi- ularly for the bass voice. In addition, for IOI ratio values in
vidual voices, timing and loudness cues were correlated the soprano and bass voices, and for velocity values in the
with the tonal stability of notes. Notes that were tonally
stable within a key tended to be played for longer duration
in the soprano and bass voices and softer in the bass voice. 6 For the bass voice, both interonset interval (IOI) ratio and
keypress velocity were correlated with the tonal hierarchy. This
finding raises the question of whether IOI ratio and keypress
General Discussion velocity are related variables. Considering notes in the bass voice,
the correlation between IOI ratio and loudness values was -.20
The results of this research indicate that performance (df = 118, p < .05). Considering notes in all four voices, the
expression influences the perception of key movement. The correlation between IOI ratio and loudness values was again -.20
performer's use of loudness and timing significantly af- (df = 494, p < .001). Although these correlations were statistically
reliable, the coefficients indicated that each variable accounted for
fected listeners' judgments of key movement. The finding less than 5% of the variance of the other variable. Thus, IOI ratio
highlights the importance, in theoretical discussions of mu- and loudness reflect largely distinct sources of variance in perfor-
sic cognition, of performance expression in shaping listen- mance expression. A multiple regression revealed that both had
ers' perceptions of musical pitch structure. The effects of predictive power for tonal stability in the bass voice (R = .38),
performance expression are subtle because performance it- with beta weights for both IOI ratio and keypress velocity signif-
self must conform to the static elements of a musical score; icant beyond the .01 level.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 133

bass voice, the tonal hierarchy provided predictive power pression to articulate a phrase, knowledge of tonal structure
beyond what could be explained by predictors based on may be involved implicitly.
phrasing and metric stress. Finally, when average values for The effects reported here also are relevant to previous
each scale degree were considered, the tonal hierarchy was investigations of key perception. In earlier reports (Thomp-
highly correlated with IOI ratio (especially for the soprano son, 1993; Thompson & Cuddy, 1989, 1992), we argued
voice) and velocity (especially for the bass voice). Overall, that the perception of key from a sequence of chords in-
the MIDI analysis strongly suggested that information about volves different principles than the perception of key from
key structure is available to listeners in a performer's use of an individual voice or melody. Our proposal was contrasted
both loudness and timing. with a strict hierarchical model of melody, harmony, and
key, in which an individual voice or melody conveys key
structure by implying an underlying chord progression. Al-
Implications for Production and Perception though the current research was not designed to reexamine
this issue, the findings provide support for our previous
Two possible production mechanisms may be invoked to claims. A strict hierarchical model predicts that information
account for the reported effects. First, performance expres- concerning key should be associated most strongly with the
sion may directly reflect a performer's knowledge of key. chord progression. Thus, performance variation that acts to
That is, performers may emphasize or deemphasize notes in emphasize certain chords over others might be expected to
a way that references their knowledge of key. influence judgments of key movement. However, we found
This possibility is consistent with theoretical suggestions that chord-to-chord expression, in the absence of expressive
made by Sundberg and his colleagues (Sundberg et al., differences between voices, had no influence on judgments
1983; Thompson et al., 1989). Sundberg et al. (1983) pro- of key movement. Expressive differences between voices
posed a set of "rules" relating performance expression to were essential to the effects of performance expression on
musical structure. Two of the rules--melodic and harmonic judgments. This f'mding is compatible with our earlier claim
chargemimply a direct link between knowledge of key and that individual voices are able to reference key structure in
the motor commands that give rise to performance expres- a way that cannot be explained merely in terms of implied
sion. Specifically, tonally "charged" events (i.e., unstable chord progression.
events with respect to the overall key) are thought to be Our findings also may add to the understanding of the
emphasized, usually by increases in loudness. Our finding process by which musical key is abstracted. It has been
of an inverse relationship between loudness and tonal sta- argued that the abstraction of key involves a process of
bility thus provided support for the rules of melodic and assessing the similarity between musical input and an es-
harmonic charge. However, these rules were only partially tablished internal representation of key acquired through
supported by the results of our research. Melodic and har- long-term exposure to Western tonal music (e.g., Krum-
monic charge also predicted a negative correlation between hansl & Schmuckler, cited in Krumhansl, 1990). In the
tonal stability and the expressive lengthening of durations. current research, theoretical models of key relationships
By contrast, we found that IOI ratio and tonal stability were were better able to predict judgments of key movement in
correlated positively. performed sequences than in mechanical sequences. Our
A second possibility is that performance expression does interpretation of this finding is that performance expression
not reflect the performer's knowledge of key in spite of the modified the nature of the musical input, such that it cor-
influence of performance expression on judgments of key responded more closely to the performer's internal repre-
movement. Instead, performance expression may have been sentation of keys.
directed at another dimension of structure, such as the The convergence of our judgment data with the toroidal
grouping of events into phrases or metric stress. Because model of key relationships provides strong support for the
phrase structure and metric stress are compositionaUy re- psychological validity of the tonal hierarchy as an internal
lated to tonal stability, performance expression might have representation of key (for a related description, see Lerdahl,
had an incidental effect of reflecting key structure. For 1988). The toroidal model of keys, which was derived from
example, phrase endings in Western tonal music are typi- the tonal hierarchy values, provided excellent predictions of
cally associated with tonally stable events, and performers judgments for performed versions. The toroidal model of
tend to end each phrase with a diminuendo and a ritardando. keys was a more successful predictor of judgments than was
This coincidence of tonal structure and performance expres- the cycle of fifths model of keys. This comparison suggests
sion could lead to a negative correlation between loudness that perceived key distance reflects the similarity between
and tonal stability and a positive correlation between IOI tonal hierarchies of keys, not merely the degree of overlap
ratio and tonal stability, consistent with our findings. How- of diatonic (scale) notes.
ever, in the series of multiple regression analyses described
earlier, we confirmed that the tonal hierarchy had predictive
power beyond what was explained by predictors based on Parallels With Speech
phrase boundaries and metric stress. Moreover, it is impor-
tant to recognize that the perceptual abstraction of phrase The effects of performance expression on perceptual
boundaries and metric structure may be cued partially by the judgments may reflect general perceptual-motor principles
tonal stability of notes. Thus, when a performer uses ex- that are not limited to the domain of music. In particular, the
134 THOMPSON AND CUDDY

effects reported are comparable to effects of intonation on imental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7,
language comprehension. Research has established that in- 869-883.
tonation is an important source of verbal information. Pro- Cuddy, L.L., & Thompson, W.F. (1992). Asymmetry of per-
sodic cues, in combination with syntactic cues, have been ceived key movement in chorale sequences: Converging evi-
dence from a probe-tone analysis. Psychological Research, 54,
found to influence sentence segmentation (Geers, 1978).
51-59.
Accurate segmentation, in turn, is an important determinant Dowling, W. J., & Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music cognition. San
of verbal comprehension. Our analysis of music perfor- Diego, CA: Academic Press.
mances revealed comparable cues to segmentation. Phrase Gabrielsson, A. (Ed.). (1987). Action and perception in rhythm
endings were associated with decreases in loudness and and music. Stockholm, Sweden: Royal Swedish Academy of
increases in duration. It is likely that such performance cues Music.
contribute to the perceptual segmentation of sequences into Gabrielsson, A. (1988). Timing in music performance and its
phrases. relations to music experience. In J. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative
When verbal material contains new information, compre- processes in music (pp. 27-51). Oxford, England: Clarendon
hension is quicker if the new information is accented by Press.
intonation than if it is not (Bock & Mazzelia, 1983). The use Geers, A. E. (1978). Intonation contour and syntactic sttatcture as
predictors of apparent segmentation. Journal of Experimental
of intonation stress to signal new information has been
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 273-
observed in young children and may be viewed as a cross-
283.
cultural phenomenon (MacWhinney & Bates, 1978). The Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of
accenting of new verbal information with intonation is auditory events. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
analogous to the Sundberg et al. (1983) hypothesis on music Hindemith, P. (1968). A concentrated course in traditional har-
performance, supported by our MIDI analysis, that unex- mony: Book L (Rev. ed.). New York: Sehott.
pected pitches are emphasized in performance with in- Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch.
creases in loudness. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
These parallel effects of speech intonation and music Krumhansl, C.L., & Kessler, E. (1982). Tracing the dynamic
performance implicate a general phenomenon in which mo- changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representa-
tor production mechanisms reference knowledge of struc- tion of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89, 334-368.
tural levels derived from the surface characteristics of Krumhansl, C. L., & Shepard, R. N. (1979). Quantification of the
hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of
events. Speech production mechanisms may access knowl-
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
edge of new or contrasting information, phrase and sentence
5, 579-594.
structure, and emotional connotation. Similarly, production Lerdald, F. (1988). Tonal pitch space. Music Perception, 5,
mechanisms involved in music performance may access 315-349.
knowledge of melodic structure, rhythmic structure, style, Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal
emotional tone, and, as our findings indicate, key relation- music. Cambridge, MA: M1T Press.
ships. In both cases, levels of structure are clarified and MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for con-
enhanced by production mechanisms. veying givenness and newness: A cross-cultural developmental
study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17,
539-558.
Palmer, C. (1988). Timing in skilled music performance. Unpub-
References
lished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY.
Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1980). The recognition of trans- Palmer, C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical perfor-
posed melodies: A key-distance effect in developmental per- mance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
spective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percep- and Performance, 15, 331-346.
tion and Performance, 6, 501-515. Palmer, C. (1992). The role of interpretive preferences in music
Bock, J.K., & Mazzella, J.R. (1983). Intonational marking of performance. In M. R. Jones & S. Holleran (Eds.), Cognitive
given and new information: Some consequences for comprehen- bases of musical communication (pp. 249-262). Washington,
sion. Memory & Cognition, 11, 64-76. DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: Palmer, C., & van de Sande, C. (1993). Units of knowledge in
M1T Press. music performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Butler, D. (1992). The musician's guide to perception and cogni- Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1-14.
tion. New York: Schirmer. Piston, W. (1962). Harmony (3rd ed.). New York: Norton. (Orig-
Clarke, E.F. (1982). Timing in the performance of Erik Satie's inal work published 1941).
"Vexations." Acta Psychologica, 50, 1-19. Rasch, R. A. (1979). Synchronization in performed ensemble mu-
Clarke, E. F. (1985). Structure and expression in rhythmic perfor- sic. Acustica, 43, 121-131.
mance. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.), Musical Repp, B. (1992). A consl~aint on the expressive timing of a
structure and cognition (pp. 209-236). San Diego, CA: Aca- melodic gesture: Evidence from performance and aesthetic judg-
demic Press. ment. Music Perception, 10, 221-242.
Cohen, A.J. (1991). Tonality and perception: Musical scales Sloboda, J.A. (1985). Expressive skill in two pianists: Metrical
primed by excerpts from "rhe Well Tempered Clavier" of J. S. communication in real and simulated performances. Canadian
Bach. Psychological Research, 53, 305-314. Journal of Psychology, 39, 273-293.
Cuddy, L. L., Cohen, A. J., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (1981). Percep- Sloboda J. A. (Ed.). (1988). Generative processes in music. Ox-
tion of structure in short melodic sequences. Journal of Exper- ford, England: Clarendon Press.
PERFORMANCE AND KEY PERCEPTION 135

Sundberg, J. (Ed.). (1983). Studies of music performance. Stock- change in chorale sequences: A comparison of single voices and
holm, Sweden: Royal Swedish Academy of Music. four-voice harmony. Music Perception, 7, 151-168.
Sundberg, J., Fryden, L., & Askenfelt, A. (1983). What tells you the Thompson, W. F., & Cuddy, L. L. (1992). Perceived key move-
player is musical? An analysis-by-synthesis study of music perfor- ment in four-voice harmony and single voices. Music Percep-
mance. In J. Sundberg (Ed.), Studies of music performance (pp. tion, 9, 427-438.
61-75). Stockholm, Sweden: Royal Swedish Academy of Music. Thompson, W. F., Sundberg, J., Friberg, A., & Fryden, L. (1989).
Thompson, W.F. (1993). Modeling perceived relationships be- The use of rules in the performance of melodies. Psychology of
tween melody, harmony, and key. Perception & Psychophysics, Music, 17, 63-82.
53, 13-24. Todd, N. P. (1985). A model of expressive timing in tonal music.
Thompson, W.F., & Cuddy, L.L. (1989). Sensitivity to key Music Perception, 3, 33-59.

Appendix
Mean Ratings and Standard Errors for Each Pair Type in Both Orders for Experiment 1 (Mechanical
Sequences) and Experiment 2 (Performed Sequences)
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
Pair type Order 1 Order 2 Order 1 Order 2
Nonmod 4.60 2.75 4.50 2.45
Mod II 0.24 0.23 0.26 0.26

Nonmod 5.10 2.30 4.85 2.70


Mod HI 0.23 0.33 0.21 0.29

Nonmod 4.50 2.60 5.00 2.00


Mod IV# 0.36 0.38 0.21 0.25

Mod II 4.05 3.00 3.85 3.20


MOd HI 0.38 0.31 0.34 0.39

MOd II 3.95 3.40 4.40 3.30


Mod IV# 0.36 0.42 0.33 0.41

Mod Ill 3.80 4.25 4.20 3.20


Mod IV# 0.42 0.41 0.30 0.35
Note. Ratings near 6 indicate that the second sequence involved greater distance than the first sequence. Nonmod = nonmodulation;
MOd = modulation.

Received December 14, 1994


Revision received April 18, 1995
Accepted October 9, 1995

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi