Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/241843512

The need inventory of sensation seeking (NISS)

Article in European Journal of Psychological Assessment · January 2011


DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000085

CITATIONS READS

11 766

2 authors, including:

Philipp Hammelstein
Psychiatrische Dienste Aargau AG
28 PUBLICATIONS 330 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Philipp Hammelstein on 27 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


M. Roth & P. Hammelstein:
European JournalThe Need Inventory
of PsychologicalA of Sensation
ssessment
© 2011
2012; Seeking
Hogrefe
Vol. (NISS)
28(1):11–18
Publishing

Original Article

The Need Inventory of


Sensation Seeking (NISS)
Marcus Roth1 and Philipp Hammelstein2
1
University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany, 2University of Düsseldorf, Germany

Abstract. Based on the conception of sensation seeking as a need rather than a temperamental trait (Hammelstein, 2004), we present a
new assessment method, the Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS), which is considered to assess a motivational disposition. Three
studies are presented: The first examined the factorial structure and the reliability of the German versions of the NISS; the second study
compared the German and the English versions of the NISS; and finally, the validity of the NISS was examined in a nonclinical study
and compared to the validity of conventional methods of assessing sensation seeking (Sensation Seeking Scale – Form V; SSS-V).
Compared to the SSS-V, the NISS shows better reliability and validity in addition to providing new research possibilities including
application in experimental areas.

Keywords: sensation seeking, assessment, need theory

Introduction raun, 1996; Roth, 2003; Roth et al., 2007). Major points of
criticism include the lack of a theoretical foundation for the
The concept of sensation seeking can be traced back to the SSS-V items; the lack of a conceptual differentiation be-
works of Zuckerman (1971), yet its definition has changed tween sensation seeking and impulsivity; the use of items
over time. Zuckerman (1979) initially described sensation which are dependent on age, culture, and socioeconomic
seeking as a “trait defined by the need for varied, novel and status (e.g., “skiing,” “mountaineering”); and the inclusion
complex sensations and experiences and the willingness to of items that themselves describe dysfunctional behavior
take physical and social risks for the sake of such experi- (e.g., the use of marijuana) and that therefore potentially
ence” (p. 10). Sensation seeking was conceptualized as an lead to a confounding of predictor (sensation seeking) and
age- and gender-dependent trait (for details, see Roth, criterion (substance abuse).
Hammelstein, & Brähler, 2007; Roth, Schumacher, & The present studies do not aim to repeat these critical
Brähler, 2005). In later years, however, Zuckerman reject- issues. An alternative concept of sensation seeking was
ed his own definition of sensation seeking as a need and presented by Hammelstein and Roth (Hammelstein, 2004;
instead referred to it as a trait involving the “seeking of Roth et al., 2007; Roth, 2009), and a theoretical discussion
varied, novel, complex and intense sensations and experi- between Zuckerman and the exponents of an alternative
ences” (Zuckerman, 1994, p. 27). This modification was model has been published elsewhere (Roth, Hammelstein,
made on account of the implication of “compulsion” (p. 26) & Brähler, 2009). Rather, we intend only to outline our
conveyed by the term “need.” It is, however, almost impos- theoretical model inasmuch as necessary for an understand-
sible to avoid conceiving of sensation seeking as a need; ing of the development of an alternative assessment meth-
an exclusively behavioral definition without a motivational od. With respect to the theoretical foundations and a corre-
component would result in a completely descriptive con- sponding discussion of this alternative method, we refer to
ceptualization without an explicative function. Even the publications cited above.
Zuckerman himself sometimes continues to speak of a Here, sensation seeking is defined as a need for novelty
“need” for sensations (1994, p. 374). Apart from this and intensity which develops its motivational power via the
breach of logical explication (definition of “sensation seek- registration of discrepancies between the as-is and the to-be
ing” as “seeking”), problems arise when defining and op- state. This definition focuses on the aim (or function) of a
erationalizing sensation seeking based on highly specific specific behavior (e.g., feeling of excitement in the body)
behavior via the Sensation Seeking Scale – Form V (SSS- rather than the behavior or behavior pattern itself (e.g.,
V; Zuckerman, 1994). mountain climbing). In our view, it is thus essential that a
There has been much critique of both the conceptualiza- given behavior pursue experiences of novelty or intensity
tion and the assessment of sensation seeking using the SSS- before it can be spoken of as sensation seeking. The con-
V (e.g., Arnett, 1994; Hammelstein, 2004; Jackson & Ma- crete form this behavior takes (e.g., partying, sexual behav-

© 2011 Hogrefe Publishing European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18
DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000085
12 M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS)

ior, gambling, etc.; items of the SSS-V) is not essential to 1994) and Arnett (1994). Items should reflect a physical
defining sensation seeking. While, in our view, specific be- or psychological sensation as a goal state rather than a
haviors such as mountain climbing can represent a search concrete behavior. This is the distinction between the goal
for experiences of intensity, they might also represent aims (i.e., sensation) and the means (i.e., the concrete behavior
that have nothing to do with the construct of sensation seek- or the concrete kind of stimulation to reach this sensation-
ing. An instrument is therefore needed to assess the under- al goal). In the first step the authors generated 27 items
lying motivational aims and not the concrete behavior, as that were then sent to four colleagues who had to rate
is the case with the SSS-V and even the Arnett Inventory whether the items reflected a concrete behavior/stimula-
of Sensation Seeking (AISS; Arnett, 1994). tion or a state of experience. At least the behavior had to
Temperamental traits are defined by a specific behav- be described very broadly so that different kinds of behav-
ior pattern and can therefore be best measured based on ior could lead to this state (e.g., “I enjoy doing exciting
concrete behavior (e.g., introversion by reserved, less and stimulating activities with others”). Only if all four
outgoing, and less sociable behavior). In contrast, needs subjects rated the items as sensation oriented the item had
are defined by the aims that drive behavior (stimulation, not been cancelled (e.g., the item “Finding myself in un-
sex, control, attachment, etc.) and therefore cannot be familiar situations is exciting” was rated as reflecting con-
measured via the concrete behavior itself. Grosse Holt- crete behavior and was omitted, whereas the item “I like
forth and Grawe (2000) developed a German question- feeling totally charged” was rated as reflecting a state of
naire for clinical purposes to measure different needs. In sensation). A total of 10 items were omitted during this
this questionnaire, participants are asked how important step, so that 17 items remain (see Appendix).
it is for them to experience specific target states (e.g., Respondents are required to rate how often they have
having control, being complimented on something, etc.). felt this way in the past 6 months on a 5-point Likert scale
The basis of this questionnaire is constituted by 94 items (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always). Thus, the first study
from the statements of people in treatment. Everyday examined the factorial structure as well as the reliability of
goals, such as avoiding criticism and seeking the ad- the NISS comprising the items generated as described
miration by others, are grouped into avoidance and above.
approach motives. This questionnaire aims to assess in-
terpersonal motives that are relevant for treatment plan-
ning. Methods
Similar to this questionnaire, the Need Inventory of Sen-
sation Seeking (NISS) was developed as an assessment Participants
method to ask respondents to state how pleasant a specific
An e-mail was sent to students and employees of a German
stimulation situation (target state) is to them. The higher
university. It contained an internet link to the questionnaire,
number of situations, or the more pleasant the instance of
which in turn was linked to a MySQL database. Since it
stimulation is rated, the greater the need for stimulation.
was not possible to record e-mail addresses or IP addresses,
The following studies aim to test the reliability and validity
anonymity was guaranteed. A total of 555 respondents
of the NISS. Study 1 examines the factorial structure and
completed the questionnaire.
the reliability of the German version. Study 2 presents the
Participants’ mean age was M = 29.40 years (SD = 9.42)
English version of the NISS and examines its comparability
with a range of 17 to 68 years. 313 participants were wom-
with the German version. Study 3 tests the validity of the
en (54.6%). The sample was almost completely character-
German version and examines the possibility of using the
ized by a high level of education, with 519 (93.5%) having
NISS to predict the willingness of individuals to change
attended school for 13 years (Abitur).
their job.

Materials
Study 1: Factorial Structure, Item Participants were administered the German version of the
Analysis, and Preliminary Evidence of NISS comprising the 17 items (see Appendix).
Validity
In the first step, items were generated that reflect pleasure Results
in the target state of stimulation or in the opposite state Exploratory Analysis of the Dimensional Structure
(rest, calmness). Example items include “I like the feeling of the NISS
of excitement in my body” or “I enjoy it when there’s
nothing going on for a while.” The background of the gen- The factorial structure of the NISS was examined using a
eration of items was shaped by the descriptions of “high principal component analysis (PCA) with oblimin rota-
sensation seekers” in the publication of Zuckerman (1979, tion. Since the Kaiser criterion (eigenvalues greater than

European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18 © 2011 Hogrefe Publishing
M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS) 13

Table 1. Results of PCA with oblimin-rotation and item analysis concerning the NISS of the German Sample A (n = 555)
Descriptives PCA Item analysis
Item no. M SD F1 F2 h² ris rit
05 2.67 0.98 .77 –.16 .59 .69 .58
04 2.63 1.06 .75 –.31 .59 .67 .66
13 2.66 0.93 .74 –.03 .57 .65 .49
16 2.92 0.89 .73 –.20 .53 .65 .57
07 2.30 1.00 .71 –.10 .50 .62 .51
14 2.62 1.02 .67 –.16 .44 .58 .52
06 2.90 0.98 .66 –.17 .44 .57 .52
03 2.76 1.06 .65 –.06 .43 .56 .46
01 2.70 0.97 .60 –.11 .37 .52 .47
15 2.64 0.92 .56 –.22 .32 .48 .48
02 2.81 1.12 .54 –.14 .29 .46 .45
11 3.13 1.01 –.14 .82 .68 .70 .63
09 3.47 0.98 –.31 .80 .66 .67 .70
08 3.80 0.90 –.26 .75 .57 .62 .64
10 3.08 0.99 –.04 .74 .56 .60 .52
17 3.09 0.95 –.21 .72 .53 .58 .60
12 2.74 1.02 –.07 .52 .27 .38 .43
λ 5.2 3.5
Note. λ = Eigenvalue; ris = item to scale correlation; rit = item to total correlation. Highest loadings are presented in bold.

Discussion
1) frequently leads to an overestimation of the number of
factors to be retained, a parallel analysis (PA) was per- Reliability of the German NISS-version is considerably
formed in the sense of Horn (1965). Using PA, the PCA higher than that found for other measures of sensation
results in two factors which explain 49.0% of the total seeking (SSS-V: e.g., Ball, Farnhill, & Wangeman, 1984;
variance (for two factors: λempirical = 2.8332 > λrandom = Ferrando & Chico, 2001; AISS: e.g., Ferrando & Chico,
1.2627; for three factors: λempirical = 1.2071 < λrandom = 2001; Roth, 2003). The two-dimensional structure is in
1.2072). Loadings and communalities (h²) are presented line with Grawe’s (2002) theoretical considerations con-
in Table 1. The first factor contains items describing stim- cerning fundamental needs. In his view, schemes of ap-
ulation as aims of approaching according to Grawe proach and avoidance emerge for every fundamental need.
(2002); this factor is referred to as “need for stimulation” However, it should be noted that all items of the second
(NS). The second factor consists of all negatively poled factor (referred to as “avoidance of rest and tranquility”)
items and is therefore called “avoidance of rest (or tran- are negatively poled. It is therefore questionable whether
quility)” (AR). The factor structure also remains stable this factor actually reflects the aim of avoidance or rather
when applying common factor analysis instead of PCA a different aim of approach (see General Discussion).
and varimax instead of oblimin rotation.

Study 2: Confirmatory Factor Analysis


Item Analysis
and Validity
Table 1 shows the Item-scale correlations (ris) and item-to- To ensure that the structure of the German NISS, as re-
tal correlations (rit). Internal consistency is high with α = vealed by factor analyses in the German sample, is equiv-
.88 for the subscale NS and α = .82 for AR, and α = .86 alent to that of the English version of the instrument, we
for the total scale. The two scales have a correlation of r = crossvalidated the factor model (Study 1) with the data of
.25. The correlation between the NS and the total score is an English sample. We furthermore examined the relation-
r = .78, and the correlation between the AR and the total ship between the NISS and the conventional method of
score is r = .80. measuring sensation seeking (SSS-V). Correlations be-

© 2011 Hogrefe Publishing European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18
14 M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS)

Figure 1. Final path diagram (stan-


dardized coefficients) of the NISS by
sample C (n = 146). NS = Need for
stimulation, AR = Avoidance of rest.

tween the two scales were expected to be midrange; higher


correlations were not expected because of the different as- Results
pects (aims of behavior vs. concrete behavior patterns) as-
Data were analyzed using EQS 5.7 (Bentler, 1995). Since
sessed by the instruments.
χ² values are highly susceptible to variations in sample size
(Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Browne & Cudeck, 1993), we
used the comparative fit index (CFI), which is widely in-
Methods dependent of sample size. Furthermore, the root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA; Jöreskog, 1993)
Participants and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR)
were provided as indices. As recommended by Bentler
Participants were recruited via different internet sport fo- (1995), we interpreted a CFI value of 0.90 or above as
rums. This online method of recruitment would seem ap- showing adequate fit and a value of 0.95 as showing excel-
propriate for obtaining a homogeneous, nonclinical, Eng- lent fit. In line with Browne and Cudeck (1993), we con-
lish-speaking sample (methodical limitations associated sidered values of RMSEA below 0.08 to reflect adequate
with this recruitment strategy are referred to in the discus- fit and values below 0.05 good fit. Values of the SRMR are
sion). Internet users were informed that they had to be Eng- expected to stay below 0.10. Parameters were estimated
lish native speakers to take part in the study. using the robust maximum-likelihood (ML) estimation
The sample consisted of 146 participants (51 female, method.
34.9%) aged from 14 to 63 years (M = 33.1, SD = 11.7). The tested NISS model was specified according to the
103 subjects came from the United States (70.5%), 12 from results of Study 1, which revealed two dimensions of sen-
Canada (8.2%), 8 from the United Kingdom (5.5%), 8 from sation seeking (see in detail Figure 1). The fit indices asso-
New Zealand (5.5%), and 15 from other countries (10.3%). ciated with this model indicate a suboptimal fit when ap-
The average level of education was rather high, ranging plied to the English sample (χ² = 240.26, p < .001, df = 118;
from 4 to 20 years of education (M = 16.0, SD = 3.8). CFI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.08; SRMR = 0.09). However,
when allowing correlations between the unique variance of
items 4 and 5 (see Figure 1), as was indicated by the mod-
ification indices (MIs) and standardized expected parame-
Materials ter changes (SEPCs), the fit indices indicate an reasonable
fit to the data of the English sample: While χ² is significant
Participants were administered the English version of the (χ² = 220.61, df = 117, p < .001), the other fit indices now
NISS as well as the original version of the SSS-V (Zucker- indicate reasonable model fit (CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.07,
man, 1994). The SSS-V is a 40-item, forced-choice ques- SRMR = 0.09). Based on content analysis, it would seem
tionnaire that measures four factors: thrill and adventure appropriate to allow the unique variances of the both items
seeking (TAS), experience seeking (ES), disinhibition to correlate: the common variance of items 4 and 5 may be
(Dis), and boredom susceptibility (BS). interpreted as constituting a factor described as “stimula-

European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18 © 2011 Hogrefe Publishing
M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS) 15

Table 2. Correlations of the subscales of the SSS-V and the tor assesses something other than the need for sensation
NISS (n = 146) seeking.
TAS Dis BS ES SSS-V
total
NS .54** .40** .34** .15 .57**
Study 3: Validity in a Nonclinical
AR .08 .03 .06 –.19 –.02
NISS total .41** .28* .27* –.02 .37** Context
Notes. TAS = Thrill and Adventure Seeking; Dis = Disinhibition; BS
= Boredom Susceptibility; ES = Experience Seeking; SSS-V total = By conceptualizing sensation seeking as a basal need for
total score of the SSS-V; NS = Need for stimulation; AR = Avoidance stimulation that may be expressed in multiple areas of a
of rest; NISS total = total score of the NISS. *p < .05, **p < .01 (after person’s life, the present study examined the validity of the
Bonferroni adjustment).
NISS in comparison with the SSS-V by applying a criterion
other than various types of risk behavior. In keeping with
Arnett’s suggestion (1994) to explore the sensation seeking
tion of the body” that goes beyond the congeneric factor of
trait in relation to work, we employed the willingness for
a general need for stimulation.
occupational change as criterion with the following mani-
Furthermore, measurement equivalency was tested us-
festations: “intention to change workplace” and “wanting
ing multiple group comparisons with nested models. As
to learn or work in a new job.” It was expected that, com-
proposed by Raju, Lafitte, and Byrne (2002), invariant fac-
pared to low sensation seekers, the general need for stim-
tor loadings between the English and German groups were
ulation of high sensation seekers would be manifested in a
used as criteria for measurement invariance. Since Δχ² has
comparatively high willingness to change workplace. This
been criticized on account of its dependence on sample size
assumption is also supported by Zuckerman’s (1994) de-
(e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1998), we used ΔCFI as an indicator
scription of high sensation seekers as preferring changing
in the comparison of models. According to Cheung and
surroundings and low sensation seekers as preferring envi-
Rensvold (2002), values greater than .01 indicate a signif-
ronments that are highly stable and predictable. Because
icant drop in fit. Multigroup CFA indicate that the structure
the intention to change jobs is influenced by the current
and the factor loadings are invariant across the two coun-
situation of the employment market, we chose to examine
tries (unconstrained model: χ² = 793.77, df = 234; model
a sample of hairdressers, as hairdressing is one of the few
with equal factor loadings: χ² = 824.71, df = 249; ΔCFI =
occupations in Germany in which vacancies exist.
0.004) showing the stability of the NISS over the two sam-
ples.
Internal consistency was similar to that found for the
original German version. Cronbach’s α ranged from .79 Methods
(AR) to .91 (NS; .88 for the total scale). The correlation Participants
coefficients between the SSS-V and the NISS are described
in Table 2. As can be seen, the subscale NS moderately The participants in this study were female employees of 60
correlates with the subscales of the SSS-V and corresponds hairdressing salons in Germany. Altogether, 146 people
to the correlations between the intensity subscale of the work for this company, who were asked to complete a ques-
AISS and the subscales of the SSS-V (see Arnett, 1994; tionnaire. The scales were administered by the same person
Ferrando & Chico, 2001). There is, however, no relation- in all cases (including an introduction).
ship between the AR subscale and the SSS-V. Of the 146 employees invited to take part in the study,
118 participated. Responses from participants who had
more than three missing answers were excluded (n = 11).
Male hairdressers (N = 2) were excluded due to their small
Discussion number. This left a final sample of 105 female hairdressers
aged between 19 and 65 years (M = 40.83, SD = 13.39).
Overall, the results of the confirmatory factor analyses sup-
port the bidimensional structure of the English instruments.
This appears to imply a clear crosscultural and crosslingual Measures
stability of the factorial structure of the NISS. Furthermore,
as indicated by the Cronbach’s α coefficients, we can as- In addition to questions relating to demographic informa-
sume that the psychometric properties of the English adap- tion, participants completed the German form of the NISS
tations of the NISS are adequate to excellent. These results and the German version of the SSS-V by Beauducel, Stro-
support the applicability of the English versions. Correla- bel, and Brocke (2003; see description above). Respon-
tions between NISS and SSS-V are midrange, as expected. dents were also requested to indicate how often they intend-
However, the subscale AR does not correlate with any of ed to change their work place during their professional life
the SSS-V scales, which might suggest that this second fac- and whether they would like to learn or switch to a new

© 2011 Hogrefe Publishing European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18
16 M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS)

Table 3. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of NISS and SSS-V scores in participants who do not intend to change
their workplace (No-ICW; n = 71) vs. participants who intent to do so (ICW; n = 34), and results of MANOVA
and MANCOVA (covariate: age)
No ICW ICW MANOVA MANCOVA
M SD M SD F ε² F ε²
NISS total 41.17 6.61 47.01 8.41 14.53*** .13 10.66** .10
NS 23.26 5.57 28.80 7.41 17.68*** .15 13.27*** .12
AR 15.92 3.62 16.04 3.19 <0.01 .00 0.23 .00
SSS-V total 51.44 4.64 55.41 6.65 12.18*** .11 2.44 .02
Dis 12.17 1.66 13.68 2.66 11.32*** .10 5.10* .05
BS 12.44 1.42 13.00 1.72 2.94 .03 0.33 .00
TAS 12.77 2.42 14.18 2.61 7.44** .07 0.51 .01
ES 14.06 1.38 14.56 2.05 2.67 .03 0.37 .00
Notes. NISS total = total score of the NISS; NS = Need for stimulation; AR-Trait = Avoidance of rest; SSS-V total = total score of the SSS-V;
Dis = Disinhibition; BS = Boredom Susceptibility; TAS = Thrill and Adventure Seeking; ES = Experience Seeking. *p < .05, **p < .01; ***p
< .001.

Table 4. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of NISS and SSS-Vscores in participants who would like to learn or
switch to a new job (LNJ; n = 38) vs. participants would not like to do so (No-LNJ; n = 61), and results of
MANOVA and MANCOVA (covariate: age)
LNJ No-LNJ MANOVA MANCOVA
M SD M SD F ε² F ε²
NISS total 45.73 8.39 41.18 6.78 8.55 ** .08 5.63 * .06
NS 27.56 7.38 23.20 5.74 10.56 ** .10 7.34 ** .07
AR 16.05 3.78 15.99 3.41 0.01 .00 0.12 .00
SSS-V total 54.45 5.70 51.67 5.24 6.00 * .06 0.59 .01
Dis 13.29 2.30 12.23 1.91 5.83 * .06 2.12 .02
BS 12.79 1.74 12.54 1.48 1.09 .01 <0.01 .00
TAS 14.18 2.53 12.69 2.33 8.20 ** .08 1.36 .01
ES 14.18 1.74 14.21 1.53 0.01 .00 1.19 .01
Notes. NISS-total score of the NISS; NS = Need for stimulation; AR = Avoidance of rest; SSS-V-total = total score of the SSS-V; Dis =
Disinhibition; BS = Boredom Susceptibility; TAS = Thrill and Adventure Seeking; ES = Experience Seeking. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

job. Response options were never and . . . times for the first termine the meaning of the differences. Dichotomization of
question and yes and no for the second question. the variable ICW was necessary due to its highly skewed
distribution. As shown in Table 3, significant group differ-
ences were found in the total scale of the NISS and the
Results subscale NS-Trait, with ε² of .13–.15. We also found sig-
nificant differences in the total scale of the SSS-V and the
To test our assumption that sensation seeking is related to Dis and TAS subscales with somewhat smaller effects.
the willingness for occupational change, we examined However, respondents who intend to change their work-
whether participants with greater willingness had higher place are also younger than those who have no such inten-
scores on the sensation-seeking measures as compared to tions (ICW: M = 31.38, SD = 10.15; no-ICW: M = 45.35,
participants with low willingness. Analyses were conduct- SD = 12.70; ANOVA: F = 32.16, p < .001, ε² = .24). Fur-
ed separately for the variables “intention to change work- thermore product-moment correlations between age and
place” (response distribution: never: n = 71; once: n = 19; sensation-seeking scores reveal significant correlations be-
twice: n = 15) and “wanting to learn or work in a new job” tween age and the SSS-V scales (SSS-V total: r = –.40, p
(response distribution: yes: n = 38; no: n = 61). < .001; Dis: r = –.26, p < .01; ES: r = –.22, p < .05; BS: r
We first divided the sample into “intention to change = –.20, p = .05, TAS: r = –.41, p < .001), but not between
workplace” (ICW; n = 34) and “no intention to change age and the NISS scales (NS total: r = .18, ns, NS: r = –.19,
workplace” (no-ICW; n = 71). Differences between ICW ns; AR: r = –.05, ns). It is therefore not surprising that en-
and no-ICW were examined using multivariate analyses of tering age as a covariate causes the differences in SSS-V
variance (MANOVA) with ICW/no-ICW as independent scores between ICW and no-ICW to disappear (see Table
variable and the SSS-V and NISS scores as dependent vari- 3, right-hand column), whereas differences on the NISS-
ables. We also calculated the effect size ε² in order to de- scales remain stable.

European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18 © 2011 Hogrefe Publishing
M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS) 17

The same result pattern was found when respondents sesses something that is not assessed by the SSS-V and is
were asked whether they would like to learn or switch to a not related to the intention to change job (furthermore, it
new job. Here, the sample was divided into participants may be a factor that measures an intention to relax as a kind
who would like to have a new job (LNJ; n = 38) and those of antipole to the need for stimulation).
with no such desires (no-LNJ; n = 61). As shown in Table The second possible interpretation is to consider the fac-
4, differences on the SSS-V scales again disappeared when torial structure as a methodical artifact. This interpretation
age was controlled for, whereas differences with respect to seems likely in light of the fact that the two-factor structure
the NISS total score and the subscale NS remained stable. corresponds to differences in item wording (the negatively
poled items all load onto Factor 2). The second factor may
thus be a result of response behavior to items measuring sen-
Discussion sation seeking. In this case, the NISS would assess sensation
seeking by means of a single factor, which would be in line
The results demonstrate the validity of the NISS with regard with the conception of sensation seeking as a one-dimension-
to the willingness for occupational change. Furthermore, the al basic need. Which of the two interpretations applies re-
results indicate that the relationship between the NISS and mains unclear: While the first factor is assumed to be an in-
the willingness for occupational change was not due to con- dicator of the need for sensation seeking, the meaning of the
founding effects of age. In contrast, there was no relationship second factor requires clarification in further studies.
between sensation seeking measured using the SSS-V and the Estimates of validity were consistently satisfactory
willingness for occupational change when age was controlled throughout the studies presented in this paper for this first
for. With respect to several items of the SSS-V which refer to factor, referred to as “need for stimulation.” The advantage
strenuous physical activities (e.g., “mountain climbing”), we of this scale for measuring sensation seeking can be sum-
cannot be certain whether the SSS-V truly measures sensa- marized as follows: First, it shows hardly any age depend-
tion seeking or rather a merely age-related activity status. ence (in contrast to the SSS-V); second, it does not contain
However, the subscale AR of the NISS also does not differ- any description of risk situations and can therefore be used
entiate between the two examined groups. This again raises for both functional as well as dysfunctional aspects of sen-
the question as to whether the AR-subscale really describes sation seeking; and third, due to the lack of concrete de-
the aim of avoiding tranquility (see General Discussion). scriptions of behavior, the NISS can be used in a diverse
range of contexts. However, if the expression of sensation
seeking is of interest, the SSS-V seems to be an adequate
method. It depends on the research questions being asked,
General Discussion which instrument is more appropriate.
Despite the present lack of clarity regarding the role of the
Against the backdrop of a conceptual criticism of the con- second factor of the NISS, this new method of measuring
ventional view of sensation seeking as a temperamental sensation seeking clearly shows some advantages over the
trait (see Arnett, 1994; Roth, 2003) as well as critical con- SSS-V and opens up new possibilities for research on sensa-
siderations concerning the SSS-V and the AISS (e.g., Fer- tion seeking. However, the limitations of the present study lie
rando & Chico, 2001), a new instrument, the Need Inven- in selected samples (users of sport forums, women hairdress-
tory of Sensation Seeking (NISS), was presented which ers) and the fact that only self-report measures are used.
conceives of sensation seeking as a need. The analyses con- Therefore, future research should choose more samples and
ducted reveal high reliability for both the German and the behavioral measures that allow a differential prediction of
English version of the NISS compared to other measures NISS and SSS-V. In this context it could also be useful to use
of sensation seeking. Reliability of the NISS is somewhat “openness to experience” as a covariate when analyzing how
higher than that found for the SSS-V and even the AISS. the NISS predicts outcomes over and above the SSS-V.
However, the interpretation of the factorial structure is
somewhat unclear. Both PCA and CFA reveal a two-di-
mensional structure with all positively poled items loading
onto the first factor and all negatively poled items loading
onto the second factor. At least two interpretations of these References
findings are possible. The first is to consider these two fac-
tors in the sense of Grawe (2002), namely, that the first Arnett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a
factor assesses the aim of approach (“need for stimulation”) new scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 289–296.
and the second the aim of avoidance (“avoidance of rest”). Ball, I. L., Farnhill, D., & Wangeman, J. F. (1984). Sex and age dif-
This interpretation is challenged when taking into account ferences in sensation seeking: Some national comparisons. Brit-
the following results: The second subscale does not corre- ish Journal of Psychology, 75, 257–267.
late in any way with the SSS-V and does not differentiate Beauducel, A., Strobel, A., & Brocke, B. (2003). Psychometrische
between individuals who do and do not intend to change Eigenschaften und Normen einer deutschsprachigen Fassung der
their workplace. It may, however, be the case that AR as- Sensation Seeking-Skalen, Form V [Psychometric properties

© 2011 Hogrefe Publishing European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18
18 M. Roth & P. Hammelstein: The Need Inventory of Sensation Seeking (NISS)

and norms of a German version of the Sensation Seeking Scales, Roth, M. (2003). Validation of the Arnett Inventory of Sensation
Form V]. Diagnostica, 49, 61–72. Seeking (AISS): Efficiency to predict the willingness toward oc-
Bentler, P. M. (1995). Structural equations program manual. En- cupational chance, and affection by social desirability. Personal-
cino, CA: Multivariate Software. ity and Individual Differences, 35, 1307–1314.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and good- Roth, M. (2009). Social support as a mediator in the relation between
ness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological sensation seeking (need for stimulation) and psychological ad-
Bulletin, 88, 588–606. justment in older adults. Personality and Individual Differences,
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (Eds.). (1993). Alternative ways of 47, 798–801.
assessing model fit. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Roth, M., Hammelstein, P., & Brähler, E. (2007). Beyond a youthful
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of- behavior style – age and sex differences in sensation seeking
fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equa- based on need theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,
tion Modeling, 9, 233–255. 1839–1850.
Ferrando, P. J., & Chico, E. (2001). The construct of sensation seek- Roth, M., Hammelstein, P., & Brähler, E. (2009). Toward a multi-
ing as measured by Zuckerman’s SSS-V and Arnett’s AISS: A methodological approach in the assessment of sensation seeking.
structural equation model. Personality and Individual Differenc- Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 247–249.
es, 31, 1121–1133. Roth, M., Schumacher, J., & Brähler, E. (2005). Sensation seeking
Grawe, K. (2002). Psychological therapy. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe. in the community: Sex, age and sociodemographic comparisons
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Grawe, K. (2000). Fragebogen zur Analyse on a representative German population sample. Personality and
Motivationaler Schemata (FAMOS) [Questionnaire for the Anal- Individual Differences, 39, 1261–1271.
ysis of Motivational Schemas]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psycho- Zuckerman, M. (1971). Dimensions of sensation seeking. Journal
logie und Psychotherapie, 29, 170–179. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 36, 45–52.
Hammelstein, P. (2004). Faites vos jeux! Another look at sensation Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level
seeking and pathological gambling. Personality and Individual of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Differences, 37, 917–931.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in
of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press.
factor analysis. Psychometrica, 30, 179–185.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure
modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecifi- Published online: September 16, 2011
cation. Psychological Methods, 3, 424–453.
Jackson, J. S. H., & Maraun, M. (1996). The conceptual validity of
empirical scale construction: The case of the sensation seeking
scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 103–110. Marcus Roth
Jöreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A.
Bollen & J. S. Lang (Eds.), Testing structural equation models University of Duisburg-Essen
(pp. 294–317). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Campus Essen
Raju, N. S., Lafitte, L. J., & Byrne, B. M. (2002). Measurement Department of Psychology
equivalence: A comparison of methods based on confirmatory 45117 Essen
factor analysis and item response theory. Journal of Applied Psy- Germany
chology, 87, 517–529. E-mail marcus.roth@uni-due.de

Appendix: NISS (English version)


Directions: Below is a set of statements regarding attitudes 8. I like to take time out to relax.
and behavior. Read each statement carefully. Please state 9. I enjoy it when there’s nothing going on for a while.
how often in the past 6 months you have felt this way (1 = 10. It can be pleasant to close yourself off from the outside
almost never; 2 = seldom; 3 = occasionally; 4 = frequently; world.
5 = almost always). Please do not leave any answer blank. 11. I like to just sit back and enjoy a peaceful moment.
12. I know the feeling of being stimulated so much by ex-
1. I like to find myself in situations which make my heart ternal influences that I want to retreat.
beat faster. 13. I like to be aware of the excitement in my body.
2. I like to test my body’s limits. 14. I like to feel boisterous and in high spirits.
3. I know the feeling of wanting to be pepped up and stim- 15. There are situations in which a can never get enough
ulated somehow. external stimulation.
4. I like feeling totally charged. 16. I like to be exposed to strong stimulation.
5. I like the feeling of excitement in my body. 17. I sometimes enjoy doing and experiencing nothing.
6. I prefer strong and impressive experiences. Note. The German version of the questionnaire is available
7. I sometimes need a certain “kick” in order to feel good. from the corresponding author.

European Journal of Psychological Assessment 2012; Vol. 28(1):11–18 © 2011 Hogrefe Publishing

View publication stats

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi