Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Attitudes toward organ donation and donor

behavior: a review of the international literature


Objective—To conduct a systematic review of published research that assessed Claire E. Wakefield, PhD,
the predictors of attitudes toward deceased organ donation, willingness to donate, Kaaren J. Watts, PhD,
and donor behavior.
Judi Homewood, PhD,
Data Sources—MEDLINE, PreMEDLINE, PsycInfo, and the Cumulative Index
to Nursing and Allied Health Literature were searched for the period from 1988 to Bettina Meiser, PhD,
2009. Laura A. Siminoff, PhD
Study Selection—Eligible studies included members of the general public (ie, not University of New South Wales, Sydney,
transplant recipients, donor families, or health professionals) and used multivariate and Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick,
analyses for quantitative assessment of predictors.
Australia (CEW, KJW, BM), Macquarie
University, North Ryde, Australia (JH),
Data Extraction—The search identified 33 relevant studies. Owing to heterogeneity Virginia Commonwealth University,
in populations and measures, results were summarized rather than subjected to Richmond (LAS)
meta-analysis.
Data Synthesis—Research suggests that individuals who are younger, female, have Corresponding author: Dr Claire
higher education levels and socioeconomic status, hold fewer religious beliefs, have Wakefield, Psychosocial Research
high knowledge levels, know others with positive attitudes, are more altruistic, and Group, Dickinson 3, Department of
have fewer concerns about manipulation of the body of the deceased donor are more Medical Oncology, Prince of Wales
likely to have positive attitudes toward donation and are more willing to donate Hospital, Randwick, NSW 2031,
their organs. Sydney, Australia (e-mail:
Conclusions—The review revealed the complexity of individuals’ attitudes toward
c.wakefield@unsw.edu.au)
donation and the need for more sophisticated future studies of the interactions between To purchase electronic or print reprints,
the broader factors influencing donation (such as social norms and existing legislation contact:
in each country) and individual factors, such as attitudes and beliefs. (Progress in The InnoVision Group
Transplantation. 2010;20:380-391) 101 Columbia, Aliso Viejo, CA 92656
Phone (800) 809-2273 (ext 532) or
(949) 448-7370 (ext 532)
Fax (949) 362-2049
E-mail reprints@aacn.org

O ne deceased organ and tissue donor can save, or


dramatically improve the quality of, up to 10 peo-
ple’s lives.1,2 Organ transplantation can treat a wide
intention to donate organs or attempt to address
deficits in public knowledge about organ donation.
Under the opt-in model of organ donation operating
range of end-stage, life-threatening illnesses, including within the United States, the United Kingdom, and
diseases of the heart, kidney, liver, lung, and pancreas.2 Australia, where the intention to donate one’s organs
If enough organs were available, many thousands of must be voluntarily registered, attitudes toward and
patients worldwide could benefit from transplantation beliefs about organ donation are likely to be an impor-
medicine. Unfortunately, even in countries with high tant component of an individual’s decision to register
transplantation success rates, many patients in need or not. Previous research suggests that people’s atti-
will not receive a transplant because of the low rates of tudes and beliefs toward organ donation contribute sig-
donation around the globe.2 Moreover, thousands more nificantly to willingness to donate. For example, Parisi
could benefit from a transplant should enough organs and Katz3 found that individuals who held strong pos-
become available and the numerous restrictions itive attitudes and weak negative attitudes toward
applied to current waiting lists be relaxed.2 organ donation were the most committed to signing a
Public campaigns to promote organ donation gen- donor card. In order to develop more effective public
erally either encourage individuals to declare their health campaigns, it is critical to understand more

380 Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010


Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

about the attitudes and beliefs that either increase or outcome measures were similar to those of interest).37
decrease people’s willingness to donate and their com- In the current context, observational studies that use
mitment to register their intentions. sound methods provide the best available evidence.
Randomized controlled trials are not relevant here
Objective because this article is not intended to review interven-
This review aimed to identify any study published tions designed to influence donor attitudes or behav-
in a peer-reviewed journal that assessed predictors of iors. Observational studies that used sound methods,
attitudes toward deceased organ donation and donor including random sampling, were classified as “high”
behaviors. quality. A study’s grade was decreased to “moderate”
if it used a convenience sample or if it provided insuf-
Methods ficient information about the study measures or the
The inclusion criteria for studies were as follows: data analyses. Note, however, that the “convenience
(1) Quantitative research methods were used and pre- sample” criterion is strict and that potential confound-
dictors were assessed by using multivariate statistical ing variables were usually controlled for in the multi-
analyses and (2) Study participants were members of variate analyses. The assessing authors completed the
the general public (ie, not transplant recipients, donor assessment separately and the interrater agreement was
families, or health professionals). 95.2%, with only 11 discrepancies across 231 assess-
ment criteria. Discrepancies were discussed with the
Core Variables larger team before a consensus was reached for each
Studies measuring any of the following core vari- criterion. Studies were not excluded from the review
ables were included in the review: respondent’s attitude on the basis of quality; however, methodological qual-
toward deceased organ donation (assessed by using a ity of the studies is discussed later.
validated or purposely designed quantitative attitudes
survey), self-reported willingness to donate (assessed by Results
using a validated or purposely designed survey), and In total, 788 articles were initially identified, with
donor behavior (defined as having joined an organ 33 meeting all inclusion criteria (Table 3). Nineteen
donor registry, having signed and/or carrying an organ articles originated from the United States, 4 from
donor card, or having indicated their willingness to Spain, 3 from The Netherlands, and the remainder from
become an organ donor on their driver’s license). Hong Kong (n = 2), China (n = 2), Pakistan (n = 1), and
Switzerland (n=1). One article described a collaborative
Data Sources study involving the United States, Japan, and Korea.
The databases MEDLINE, PreMEDLINE, PsycInfo,
and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Quality Assessment
Health Literature were searched from 1988 to 2009, The GRADE approach37 was used to guide assess-
limited to studies published in English and involving ment of the quality of the evidence presented in the
human subjects. included studies. The quality of the observational
studies was generally high (Table 4). More than 50%
Study Selection (17/33 = 52%) of the studies were classified as provid-
The free-text search term “organ don$” was entered ing direct evidence because of their use of random
individually. Next, the terms “psych$,” “attitude,” and population samples rather than convenience samples.
“belief” were entered individually and grouped to form Most studies used at least 1 valid and reliable assess-
a stem group. This stem was combined with the initial ment tool, although many also used purposely created
search and deduplicated. The remaining abstracts were unvalidated tools as well. However, only 30% explic-
screened, and studies not fitting the inclusion criteria itly reported using a theoretical framework to guide
were discarded. Table 1 provides a summary of the their research.37
excluded studies. The included articles were checked
by a second reviewer to determine whether they met Predictors of Attitudes Toward Deceased
inclusion criteria. We used the techniques described by Organ Donation
Oxman4 to guide analysis of the findings. Sociodemographic Predictors. Younger per-
Each included study was assessed for quality by sons11,16,18 and those with higher levels of education and
2 authors (C.W., K.W.) who used the criteria outlined socioeconomic status11,13,26 are more likely to have pos-
in Table 2 (based on the validated GRADE system37). itive attitudes toward organ donation. In one study,10
The 4 key elements that were considered when judging researchers reported a significant influence of sex on
the quality of evidence were: study design, study qual- attitudes, with female students in a Chinese university
ity, consistency of estimates of effect across studies, being more likely to hold positive attitudes toward
and directness (ie, the extent to which the people and donation than their male counterparts. In only 2

Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010 381


Wakefield et al

Table 1 Exclusion criteriaa

No. of studies
Exclusion criterion rejected
Live donation 79
Xenotransplantation 6
Qualitative study 40
Correlates of knowledge and attitudes about organ donation and donor behaviors 61
Attitudes and beliefs of health professionals and medical or allied health students 156
Studies of transplant recipients or organ donors 14
Process of practice of organ donation, or organ donation law (including financial incentives) 48
Bereaved families and predictors of their decision to donate (or not to donate) a loved one’s organs; 49
personality correlates of willingness to donate; family discussion about organ donation
Attitudes toward brain death, or the concept of death, and attitudes toward non-heart-beating donors 26
Care of bereaved families and the grieving process 24
Organ donation for research purposes 5
Other medical conditions 11
Review 33
Editorial comment, letter, or opinion piece 73
Public education about organ donation, or interventions designed to change attitudes toward organ donation 45
Donation of specific organs including kidneys, bone marrow, tissue, blood, and face transplantation 19
Case study 12
Unpublished dissertation 16
Pediatric donation 8
Abstract unavailable 5
Other, including duplication of previous data or a topic unrelated to organ donation 20
No. of studies 788
No. of studies excluded 755
No. of studies included 33

a When a study met 2 or more exclusion criteria, each excluded study was coded by its first, or main, exclusion reason so that each study was only included in the

table once.

studies8,26 did researchers report a significant influence Familial Influences. Two studies11,23 showed a sig-
of ethnicity or religious beliefs in attitudes toward nificant influence of family and significant others on
organ donation. The larger of these studies8 (involving individuals’ attitudes about donation. For example,
426 students from 3 countries) reported that stronger Rios et al23 recently surveyed 3155 adults in Spain and
religious beliefs or spiritual concerns were predictive reported that having a partner who is in favor of organ
of less favorable attitudes toward donation. Interest- donation is predictive of more positive attitudes. These
ingly, researchers in that study also reported that the authors also found that those who had discussed dona-
perception that a spiritual connection exists between tion with their family had more positive attitudes.23
the donor and the recipient was predictive of more
positive attitudes.8 Other Predictors. In 2 studies, researchers reported
that individuals with altruistic tendencies19 and those
Knowledge. In 8 studies,11-13,16,19,26,31 researchers who trusted the attending physician had more positive
assessed knowledge about organ donation, and all attitudes toward donation.5 Also, 3 studies11-13 showed
reported that increased knowledge about organ dona- that individuals who have previously undertaken other
tion was predictive of more positive attitudes. Not sur- prosocial activities, such as blood donation, are more
prisingly, having a previous personal experience with likely to have a favorable attitude toward donation.
organ donation, or knowing a donor or recipient, also Finally, 4 studies investigated the influence of atti-
is predictive of more favorable attitudes.26 tudes toward the human body on attitudes toward

382 Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010


Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

Table 2 Quality assessment of studies included in review (N = 33)

Reference Other modifying Overall


and year Study design Study quality Consistency Directness factors quality
Alden and Observational SSQ plus 1 No important Direct High
Cheung,5 (survey, cross-sectional) validated measure inconsistencies
2000 No theoretical frame-
work specified
Alvaro et al,6 Observational SSTS No important Direct High
2005 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies
work specified
Boulware et Observational SSTS No important Direct High
al,7 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies
2002 work specified
Bresnahan et Observational SSQ plus 2 No important Indirect (CS) Mod
al,8 (survey, cross-sectional) validated inconsistencies
2007 measures
Theoretical frame-
work specified
Brug et al,9 Observational SSQ Negative expec- Indirect (CS) Mod
2000 (survey, cross-sectional) Theoretical frame- tations stronger
work specified predictor of
willingness to
donate than
positive
Chen et al,10 Observational No theoretical frame- No important Indirect (CS) Limited information Mod
2006 (survey, cross-sectional) work specified inconsistencies on study measures
Conesa et al,11 Observational SSQ No important Direct Limited detail Mod
2003 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies on multivariate
work specified analyses variables
Conesa et al,12 Observational SSQ No important Direct Limited detail on Mod
2004 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies multivariate analy-
work specified ses variables
Conesa et al,13 Observational SSQ No important Direct Limited detail Mod
2006 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies on multivariate
work specified analyses variables
Feeley and Observational SSQ plus 2 No important Indirect (CS) Multicollinearity was Mod
Servoss,14 (survey, cross-sectional) validated measures inconsistencies controlled for
2005 No theoretical frame-
work specified
Horton and Observational SSQ plus 1 validated No important Direct High
Horton,15 (survey, cross-sectional) measure inconsistencies (community)
1990 Theoretical frame- Indirect
work specified (CS, students)

Horton and Observational SSQ plus 3 validated No important Indirect (CS) Mod
Horton,16 (survey, cross-sectional) measures inconsistencies
1991 Theoretical frame-
work specified
Li et al,17 Observational SSTS No important Direct High
2001 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies
work specified
Miles and Observational SSTS No important Direct Limited measure of Mod
Frauman,18 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies attitudes
1988 work specified

Continued

Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010 383


Wakefield et al

Table 2 Continued

Reference Other modifying Overall


and year Study design Study quality Consistency Directness factors quality
Morgan et al,19 Observational SSQ plus 1 validated No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2003 (survey, cross-sectional) measure inconsistencies
Theoretical frame-
work specified
Morgan,20 Observational SSQ plus 1 validated No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2006 (survey, cross-sectional) measure inconsistencies
Theoretical frame-
work specified
Park and Observational SSQ No important Indirect (CS) Mod
Smith,21 (survey, cross-sectional) Theoretical frame- inconsistencies
2006 work specified
Reubsaet et Observational SSQ Negative out- Direct High
al,22 (survey, cross-sectional) Theoretical frame- come beliefs
2001 work specified were strongest
predictors of
willingness
to donate
Rios et al,23 Observational SSQ, validated in No important Direct Limited detail on Mod
2007 (survey, cross-sectional) previous studies in inconsistencies actual data col-
same geographical lection process
area
No theoretical frame-
work specified
Romero et al,24 Observational SSQ plus 1 validated No important Direct (Hispanic Limited measure Mod
2001 (survey, cross-sectional) measure inconsistencies and non-Hispanic of willingness
No theoretical frame- elderly) to donate
work specified
Rubens et al,25 Observational SSQ (some items No important Indirect (CS) Mod
1998 (survey, cross-sectional) adapted from inconsistencies
Gallup poll, and pilot
tested before use)
No theoretical frame-
work specified
Rumsey et al,26 Observational ODAS validated as No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2003 (survey, cross-sectional) part of this study inconsistencies
No theoretical frame-
work specified
Saleem et al,27 Observational SSQ No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2009 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies
work specified
Saub et al, 28 Observational SSQ No important Direct Limited informa- Mod
1998 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies tion about
work specified measures
Schulz et al,29 Observational SSTS No important Direct Good level of High
2006 (survey, cross-sectional) No theoretical frame- inconsistencies detail on scale
work specified attributes
provided
Siminoff et al,30 Observational SSQ, items based on No important Direct High
2006 (survey, cross-sectional) focus group results inconsistencies
No theoretical frame-
work specified
Sirois et al,31 Observational SSQ plus 2 validated No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2005 (survey, cross-sectional) measures inconsistencies
No theoretical frame-
work specified
Continued
Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

Table 2 Continued

Reference Other modifying Overall


and year Study design Study quality Consistency Directness factors quality
Skowronski,32 Observational SSQ plus 2 validated Negative beliefs Indirect (CS) Mod
1997 (survey, cross-sectional) measures discriminated
No theoretical frame- more clearly
work specified between
donors than
positive beliefs
Van den Berg Observational SSQ No important Indirect (CS) Mod
et al,33 2005 (survey, prospective, No theoretical frame- inconsistencies
6 month follow-up) work specified
Weber et al,34 Observational SSQ plus 1 validated No important Indirect (CS) Included meas- Mod
2007 (survey, cross-sectional) measure inconsistencies ure of actual
Theoretical frame- organ donor
work card signing
behavior
Yeung et al,35 Observational SSTS. No theoretical No important Direct High
2000 (survey, cross-sectional) framework specified inconsistencies
Wu and Tang,36 Observational SSQ, 1 validated No important Indirect (CS) Mod
2009 (survey, cross-sectional) measure, and inconsistencies
adaptations of
existing measures
Theoretical frame-
work
a Study design refers to the basic study design (observational studies or randomized trials); Study quality refers to the detailed study methods and execution;

Consistency refers to the similarity of estimates of outcomes across studies; Directness refers to the sample and outcome measures are similar to those of interest;
Other modifying factors include additional factors that may lower (or enhance) the quality of evidence, including imprecise or sparse data and a very strong effect or
association, respectively; and Overall quality ranks the quality of the evidence. The grades range from “very low” (any estimate of effect is very uncertain) to “high”
(further research is unlikely to change confidence in the estimate of the effect or outcome). These definitions are sourced from the GRADE system.4

Abbreviations: CS, convenience sample; Mod, moderate; ODAS, organ donation attitude scale; SSQ, study-specific questionnaire; SSTS, study-specific telephone survey.

organ donation. Two studies12,23 showed that individu- particularly among African Americans. 2 0 In one
als who were less concerned about the manipulation study27 conducted in Pakistan, consisting of predomi-
of the body of the deceased donor (eg, autopsy) were nantly Muslim participants, the perception that reli-
more likely to have a favorable attitude toward dona- gion allowed organ donation was a positive predictor
tion. Conversely, believing that the integrity of the body of motivation to donate (Table 5).
should be maintained after death appears to be predic-
tive of negative attitudes toward organ donation.5,11,13 Knowledge. Twelve studies14-16,19,20,25,27-31,43 involved
assessment of knowledge about, or awareness of,
Predictors of Willingness to Donate and/or organ donation, and all reported that individuals with
Donor Behaviors greater knowledge were more willing to donate. Six
Sociodemographic Predictors. Six studies7,13,16,29,30,35 studies6,13,14,25,28,29 showed that knowing an organ donor,
showed that younger persons are more willing to transplant recipient, someone waiting for an organ, or
donate their organs, and 3 of those studies16,29,30 were someone willing to donate was predictive of willing-
assessments of actual donor behavior (ie, having ness to donate and donor behaviors (Table 5).
signed or carrying a donor card) (Table 5). Six stud-
ies7,13,24,27,30,35 showed that persons with higher socioeco- Familial Influences. Perceived favorable atti-
nomic status and education are more willing to donate, tudes of a significant other toward organ donation, or
and 4 studies6,10,22,42 showed that women may be more believing that a significant other would advocate or
willing to donate than are men. In 3 studies, influence one’s decision about becoming an organ
researchers reported that African American7,18 and His- donor, was predictive of willingness or intention to
panic American ethnicity24 were predictive of lower donate,19,21,22,28 willingness to communicate about
willingness to donate. Strong religious beliefs also organ donation with others,19,21,42 and donor behavior
appear to be predictive of lower willingness to donate,7,32 (Table 5).19,20,31

Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010 385


Wakefield et al

Table 3 List of studies included in review (N = 33)

Reference,
date, origin Outcome measures Sample characteristics Measures Findings
Alden and Possession of donor 425 adults, aged 16-60 (mean, SSQ Positive predictor: trust in doctors;
Cheung,5 card 41.0) y; random sampling negative predictor: belief in main-
2000, USA taining integrity of body after death
Alvaro et al,6 Family discussion 1203 Hispanic Americans, SSTS Positive predictors: female sex, fam-
2005, USA and donor status aged 18-90 (mean, 41.0) y; ily discussion, knowing an organ
random sampling donor, positive beliefs about
organ donation
Boulware et al,7 Willingness to 385 adults, aged 18-75 (mean, SSTS Negative predictors: older age,
2002, USA donate, donor card NR); random sampling African American ethnicity, being
unemployed, religion, mistrust in
hospitals
Bresnahan et al,8 Intention to register Convenience sample of students; SSQ plus Positive predictors: family commu-
2007, US, as a donor mean age NR knowledge nication, positive attitude; negative
Japan, Korea and attitudes3,15 predictor: spiritual concerns
Brug et al,9 Willingness to 145 high school students, SSQ Positive predictors: past donor
2000, The register as a donor aged 15-21 (mean, 16.2) y; behavior, perceiving that significant
Netherlandsa convenience sample others would want the respondent
to donate; negative predictor: neg-
ative beliefs about organ donation
Chen et al,10 Willingness to donate 922 Chinese undergraduates, SSQ Positive predictors: female sex,
2006, China aged 16-37 (mean, 20.8) y being a blood donor; reasons for
organ donation: altruism; reasons
against: concerns about integrity
of body and organ misuse
Conesa et al,11 Attitudes toward 2000 adults, >15 years (mean, 30-item PADQ3,15 Positive predictors: Younger age,
2003, Spain organ donation 41.2 y); random sampling higher education, previous experi-
and transplantation ence with organ donation, knowl-
edge, partner attitude; reasons for
donation: solidarity, reciprocity;
reasons against: fear of apparent
death, against corpse manipulation
Conesa et al,12 Attitudes toward 210 adolescents, aged 15-19 SSQ Positive predictors: being a student,
2004, Spaina organ donation (mean NR); random stratified positive attitude toward blood
and transplantation sample donation or body manipulation,
knowledge; reasons for donation:
solidarity, reciprocity; reasons
against donation: fear of apparent
death
Conesa et al,13 Attitudes toward 181 adults, mean age 41.1 y; SSQ Positive predictors: higher education,
2006, Spain organ donation random sampling previous experience with organ
donation, blood donation, agreeing
with corpse manipulation; reasons
for donation: solidarity, reciprocity;
reasons against: against manipu-
lation of the deceased’s body, fear
of apparent death
Feeley and Donor status 502 university students; Mean SSQ, plus Positive predictors: knowing a
Servoss,14 age, NR; convenience sample knowledge,38 donor, recipient, or someone on a
2005, USA attitudes16 waiting list
Horton and Donor behaviorb 481 students, aged 17 to 43 SSQ plus atti- Positive predictor: Knowledge.
Horton,15 (mean, 20) y; 465 members tudes scale39 Potential barriers: misconceptions
1990, USA of public, aged 15 to 85 y about Western religious support
for organ donation, death criteria,
process of signing up as a donor.
Horton and Donor behavior 295 students, mean age SSQ plus values, Positive predictors: Helpful values,
Horton,16 19.9 y; convenience sample knowledge, knowledge. Favourable attitudes
1991, USA attitudes15,39 predicted willingness to donate,
which predicted carrying of donor
card.
Continued
Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

Table 3 Continued

Reference,
date, origin Outcome measures Sample characteristics Measures Findings
17
Li et al, 2001, Willingness to donate 1018 public (mean age, 32.6 y), SSTS Positive predictor: being a blood
Hong Kong and donor status 1227 blood donors (mean age, donor; for blood donors and
28.9 y); random sampling members of the public, main con-
cern about organ donation was
mutilation of the body after death
Miles and Attitudes toward 585 adults, aged 19-91 (mean, SSTS Positive predictors: younger age,
Frauman,18 organ donation 45) y; random sample. being white, rather than African
1988, USA American
Morgan,19 Donor status 310 African Americans, mean SSQ Positive predictors: knowledge,
2006, USA age 45 y favorable attitudes, and lower lev-
els of religiosity
Morgan et al,20 Donor behaviors 798 adults; mean age, NR; SSQ plus Positive predictors: knowledge,
2003, USA convenience sample attitudes16 altruism, perceived positive atti-
tudes of significant others
Park and Intention to register 261 university students, mean SSQ Positive predictors: positive atti-
Smith,21 and to talk with age, 20.5 y tudes toward organ donation and
2006, USA family perceived positive opinion of sig-
nificant others
Reubsaet et al,22 Intention to register 1836 adolescents, aged 16 to SSQ Positive predictors: positive beliefs,
2001, The 18 y (mean, NR) female sex, knowledge, having
Netherlandsa previously thought about or dis-
cussed organ donation, positive
opinions of significant others
Rios et al,23 Attitudes toward 1155 British and Irish adults SSQ Positive predictors: having discussed
2007, Spain organ donation living in Spain; 2000 native the issue with family, being uncon-
Spaniards (mean age, 45 y) cerned about corpse mutilation,
positive attitude of partner, belief
that respondent might need a
transplant in the future
Romero et al,24 Willingness to donate 883 adults (mean age, 74 y) SSQ, Iowa Self- Positive predictors: higher education
2001, USA organs Assessment and interpersonal trust; negative
Inventory predictors: Hispanic background,
poorer English skills
Rubens et al,25 Donor status 742 students; mean age, NR; SSQ Positive predictors: having knowl-
1998, USA convenience sample edge about organ donation system,
knowing someone with organ
donor card
Rumsey et al,26 Attitudes toward 197 students, aged 17-48 y Organ Donation Positive predictors: better education
2003, USA organ donation (male mean, 20.5 y; female Attitude Scale about organ donation, knowing an
mean, 20 y) organ donor or recipient; negative
predictor: strong religious beliefs
Saleem et al,27 Attitudes toward organ 408 adults, aged 18-60 y SSQ adminis- Positive predictors: higher socioe-
2009, Pakistan donation (motiva- (mean age, male: 32 y, female: tered conomic status, knowledge, and
tion to donate) 33.5 y); convenience sample in a face-to- perceived allowance of organ dona-
face interview tion in religion; reasons for organ
donation: to save life, compassion
/sympathy, monetary benefits,
responsibility; reasons against
organ donation: religious beliefs,
fear that organs will be wasted/
mistreated
Saub et al,28 Willingness to donate 378 adults; mean age, 44 y; Positive predictors: knowing how to
1998, USA convenience sample SSQ arrange organ donation, being
willing to accept an organ, belief
body appears normal after organ
removal, knowing an organ donor;
reasons against: wants body to
remain intact, not wanting body
used for science, fears life may
not be saved
Continued
Wakefield et al

Table 3 Continued

Reference,
date, origin Outcome measures Sample characteristics Measures Findings
29
Schulz et al, Willingness to donate, 1509 adults; mean age, NR; SSTS Positive predictors: positive affect,
2006, signing donor card stratified random sample knowing another donor card
Switzerland holder, knowledge about organ
donation; negative predictors:
negative affect, older age
Siminoff et al,30 Willingness to donate 1283 adults, mean age, SSQ Positive predictors: younger age,
2006, USA (signed donor card) 43.8 y; random sample higher education, agrees with pre-
sumed consent, belief that hospi-
tals need not ask family
permission for organ donation;
higher trust in medical system
was predictive of donor status for
whites, but not African Americans
Sirois et al,31 Donor status (donor 137 parent-adolescent dyads, SSQ, attitudes, Adolescent and parental positive
2005, USa or nondonor) mean age adolescents: 15.5, knowledge3,16 (and negative) attitudes toward
parents: 43.6). Convenience and knowledge about organ dona-
sample. tion, and frequency of communi-
cation about organ donation
distinguished between adolescent
donors and nondonors.
Skowronski,32 Willingness to donate Study 1: 1247 students SSQ Negative beliefs about organ dona-
11997, USA (median age: 19 y) tion discriminated more clearly
Study 2: 2491 students between donors than positive
(median age, NR) beliefs; negative beliefs included:
desire not to think about death,
concern about needing organs
after death, disfigurement and
death being declared prematurely
Van den Berg et Attitude, willingness 464 students; mean age, NR; SSQ More positive evaluation of organ
al,33 2005, The to receive info 85 in 6 month follow-up; donation 6 months earlier was
Netherlands convenience sample predictive of potential donors
agreeing to receive more informa-
tion about organ donation
Weber et al,34 Intention to donate 370 students; mean age, NR SSQ plus atti- Positive predictors: positive atti-
2007, USA and donor behavior tudes scale40 tudes toward organ donation,
belief that friends and family
would endorse organ donation;
intention to donate was strongest
predictor of signing a donor card
Yeung et al,35 Willingness to donate 284 adults, aged 15-64 y (mean, SSTS Positive predictors: younger age,
2000, Hong NR); probability sampling having a professional occupation,
Kong altruism, understanding about
organ donation, confidence in
doctors, and an absence of strong
beliefs about being buried whole
Wu and Tang,36 Attitudes toward organ 290 adults, aged 25-87 y (mean, SSQ plus death Negative predictor: death anxiety
2009, China donation and donor 46.56); convenience sample anxiety scale41 indirectly reduced the likelihood of
behavior signing a donor card through low-
ered perceived self-efficacy toward
signing a donor card
a Studies on adolescent samples.
b Donor behavior reported as either having signed or carrying an organ donor card.

Abbreviations: NR, not reported; PADQ, Psychosocial Aspects of Donation Questionnaire; SSQ, study-specific questionnaire; SSTS, study-specific telephone survey.

Altruism and Beliefs About Bodily Integrity. Indi- signed an organ donor card or to have enrolled in an
viduals who have altruistic beliefs about helping oth- organ donation registry.32 Positive attitudes toward
ers and about assisting science appear more willing to manipulation of the body of the deceased donor, or
discuss organ donation,6,20 to donate,32 and/or to have having low levels of concern about maintaining the

388 Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010


Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

Table 4 Summary of predictors of attitudes toward deceased organ donation

Variable
(No. of studies showing
significant association) Results
Sex (1/31) Female sex is predictive of more positive attitudes10
Age (3/31) Younger age is predictive of positive attitudes11,16,18
Education/Socioeconomic Higher level of education and socioeconomic status are predictive of more favorable attitudes12,14,27,28
status (3/31)
Ethnicity/Religion (3/31) Stronger religious beliefs are predictive of less favorable attitudes8,26
Organ donation is predictive of more favorable attitudes27
Knowledge (8/31) Knowledge of organ donation is predictive of more favorable attitudes11-13,15,16,19,26,27,31
Knowing a donor or recipient is predictive of more favorable attitudes26
Attitudes of others (2/31) Favorable attitudes of significant others are predictive of an individual’s attitudes toward donation11,23
Communication (1/31) Discussing organ donation with a significant other is predictive of favorable attitudes toward donation23
Altruism/Trust (2/31) Altruism and trust of the attending physician is predictive of positive attitudes14,19
Attitude toward body Being unconcerned about manipulation of the deceased’s body is predictive of more favorable atti-
tudes12,23
Believing that the integrity of the body should be maintained is predictive of negative attitudes5,11,13
Belief that one might need an organ in the future is predictive of positive attitudes23
Prosocial activities (3/31) Experience with or having a positive attitude toward prosocial activities (eg, blood donation) is
predictive of favorable attitudes11-13

Table 5 Summary of predictors of willingness to donate and donor behaviors

Variable
(No. of studies showing
significant association) Results
Sex (4/31) Female sex is predictive of willingness to donate6a,10a,22a,42a
Age (6/31) Younger age is predictive of willingness to donate7a,13,16c,29ac,30c,35a
Education/Socioeconomic Higher socioeconomic status7a,27,30c,35a and education13,24a,30c are predictive of willingness
status (6/31)
Ethnicity/Religion (5/31) African American7a,18a and Hispanic American24a ethnicity is predictive of lower willingness
Strong religious beliefs are predictive of lower willingness,7a,32ac particularly among African Americans20c
Perception that religion allows organ donation is predictive of willingness to donate27
Knowledge (11/31) (6/31) Greater knowledge or awareness of organ donation is predictive of willingness14c,15c,16c,19a,20c,22a,25c,27,28a,29ac,30a,31c
Knowing someone needing or receiving an organ is predictive of willingness6bc,13a,14a,25c,28a,29a
Attitudes of others (7/31) Favorable attitudes of a significant other is predictive of willingness to donate19abc,20c,21ab,22a,28a,31c,42b
Communication (2/31) Communication about organ donation with significant others is predictive of willingness6a,14c
Altruism (4/31) Altruism and higher levels of interpersonal trust are predictive of willingness to donate6a,20c,24a,32ac
Attitude toward body Low levels of concern about maintaining integrity of the body after death are predictive of increase
(3/31) willingness to donate19,20c
Concern about disfigurement of the body is a negative predictor of willingness to donate32ac
Prosocial activities (3/31) Prosocial activities are predictive of willingness to donate10a,17ac,30c (African Americans)
a Willingness/intention
to donate.
b Willingnessto communicate about organ donation.
c Donor behavior (has signed and/or carries an organ donor card).

Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010 389


Wakefield et al

integrity of the body after death,20 and believing that and the multiplicity of influences on these acts could
the body appears normal after organ removal also not be fully captured by using the systematic review
appear to be predictive of willingness to donate.19 methods adopted for this study. Broader factors such
Concern about disfigurement of the body is a negative as prevailing social and cultural norms and existing
predictor of willingness to donate (Table 5).32 organ donation policies in different countries also sig-
nificantly influence organ donation decisions. Impor-
tantly, the actions of staff of emergency departments
Donate, and Donor Behavior. Researchers in numer- and intensive care units,44,45 and the attitudes of family
Relationship Between Attitudes, Willingness to

ous studies have reported that individuals who have a members of the deceased46 also have a strong impact
positive attitude toward donation are more likely to be on organ donation rates, and the contribution of these
willing to donate6,8,19,29,33,36 and/or to sign a donor card factors was not assessed in the studies identified in
or enroll in an organ donation registry.5,15,16,19,21,22,31,34 this review. The relative contribution of each of these
Interestingly, researchers in 3 studies9,21,32 found that factors is unknown, but given the substantial differ-
negative attitudes and/or beliefs were a stronger pre- ences between countries with regards to organ dona-
dictor of willingness to donate than were positive tion rates, it is likely that attitudinal differences are
attitudes and/or beliefs. Finally, researchers in 4 stud- only one of many factors that influence donation rates.
ies8,22,31,42 have investigated the relationship between
reported willingness to donate and actual donor Future Directions
behavior, all of which showed that reported willing- Future research would benefit from more sophis-
ness to donate was a good predictor of actually sign- ticated analysis of the reasons for the low numbers of
ing a donor card (Table 5). organ donations worldwide. The older, more conven-
tional, public health assumptions that generalized edu-
Conclusions cation will correct “ignorance” and therefore “improve
Before addressing the implications of the findings behavior” may be significantly flawed. It is possible,
of this review, the factors that compromised its ability for example, that previous education campaigns have
to provide a comprehensive assessment of predictors successfully altered community attitudes but did not
of attitudes toward organ donation should be men- increase actual donor numbers because previous cam-
tioned. These limitations include the heterogeneity of paigns have polarized donation, that is, they permitted
the study populations in the published literature, the and endorsed community acceptance of making a firm
heterogeneity of study measures and outcome vari- decision against donation, rather than increasing the
ables adopted by the included articles (which severely number of individuals making a firm positive deci-
limited our ability to compare results directly), the sion.47 It is also important for future research to begin
small sample sizes of many studies, the use of unvali- to separate out the more modifiable individual atti-
dated measures in many studies, and the low quality of tudes that could reasonably be addressed by commu-
the majority of studies. Also, the majority of studies in nity educational intervention and those attitudes that
the review originated in the United States, and this are based in more fundamental belief systems, such as
may limit the generalizability of findings from these cultural and religious beliefs.
studies to countries with different health systems and
cultural contexts. Acknowledgments
In this review of studies on predictors of individ- The authors acknowledge the contributions of David
uals’ attitudes toward organ donation, willingness to Cairns (Department of Psychology, Macquarie University),
donate, and donor behaviors published between 1988 Tim Mathew (Kidney Health Australia), Alison Barn-
and 2009, we found 32 relevant articles from 9 coun- well (Bereavement Services, Red Cross Organ Donation
Network), and Elizabeth Lobb (Calvary Health Care and
tries. The review demonstrated that individuals who
Cunningham Centre for Palliative Care).
are younger, are female, have higher educational lev-
els and/or socioeconomic status, hold fewer religious
Financial Disclosures
beliefs, have higher knowledge levels, know others Claire E. Wakefield is supported by a postdoctoral
with positive attitudes, are more altruistic and trusting, training fellowship from the National Health and Medical
and have fewer concerns about manipulation of the Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) (ID 510421). Bet-
body of the deceased donor are more likely to have tina Meiser is supported by a career development award
positive attitudes toward donation and are more will- from the NHMRC (ID 350989).
ing to donate. Thus this review shows that the predic-
tors of organ donation attitudes and behavior are References
multifaceted and wide-ranging. 1 Reubsaet A, Brug J, Nijkamp MD, Candel MJ, van Hooff JP,
van den Borne HW. The impact of an organ donation regis-
The complexity of the actual donation procedures tration information program for high school students in the
that govern organ donation within different countries Netherlands. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(7):1479-1486.

390 Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010


Attitudes toward organ donation and donor behavior

2 Australia and New Zealand Organ Donation Registry. organs: report on a biethnic medicare population. Med Am.
Monthly Report on Organ Donation in Australia. Adelaide, 2001;2(2):120-125.
South Australia; 2009. http://www.anzdata.org.au/anzod 25. Rubens AJ, Oleckno WA, Ciesla JR. Knowledge, attitudes, and
/updates/anzod1989toCurrent.pdf. Accessed May 31, 2010. behaviors of college students regarding organ/tissue donation
3. Parisi N, Katz I. Attitudes toward posthumous organ donation and implications for increasing organ/tissue donors. Coll Stud
adn commitment to donate. Health Psychol. 1986;5(6):565-580. J. 1998;32(2):167-178.
4 Oxman A. Preparing and maintaining systematic reviews. In: 26. Rumsey S, Hurford DP, Cole AK. Influence of knowledge and
Oxman A, ed. The Cochrane Collaboration, The Cochrane religiousness on attitudes toward organ donation. Transplant
Collaboration Handbook. London, United Kingdom: BMJ Proc. 2003;35:2845-2850.
Publishing Group; 1995. 27. Saleem T, Ishaque S, Habib N, et al. Knowledge, attitudes and
5. Alden DL, Cheung AHS. Organ donation and culture: a com- practices survey on organ donation among a selected adult pop-
parison of Asian american and European American beliefs, atti- ulation of Pakistan. BMC Med Ethics. 2009;10:5.
tudes, and behaviors. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2000;30(2):293-314. 28. Saub EJ, Shapiro J, Radecki S. Do patients want to talk to their
6. Alvaro EM, Jones SP, Robles AS, Siegel J. Predictors of physicians about organ donation? Attitudes and knowledge
organ donation behavior among Hispanic Americans. Prog about organ donation: a study of Orange County, California res-
Transplant. 2005;15(2):149-156. idents. J Commun Health. 1998;23(6):407-417.
7. Boulware LE, Ratner LE, Sosa JA, Cooper LA, LaVeist TA, 29. Schulz PJ, Nakamoto K, Brinberg D, Haes J. More than nation
Powe NR. Determinants of willingness to donate living and knowledge: cultural micro-diversity and organ donation in
related and cadaveric organs: identifying opportunities for Switzerland. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;64(103): 294-302.
intervention. Transplantation. 2002;73(10):1683-1691. 30. Siminoff LA, Burant CJ, Ibrahim SA. Racial disparities in pref-
8. Bresnahan M, Lee SY, Smith SW, et al. A theory of planned erences and perceptions regarding organ donation. J Gen Intern
behavior study of college students’ intention to register as Med. 2006;21(9):995-1000.
organ donors in Japan, Korea, and the United States. Health 31. Sirois BC, Sears SF Jr, Marhefka S. Do new drivers equal new
Commun. 2007;21(3):201-211. donors? An examination of factors influencing organ donation
9. Brug J, Van Den Borne B, Brouwers A, Van Hooff H. Pre- attitudes and behaviors in adolescents. J Behav Med. 2005;
dictors of willingness to register as an organ donor among 28(2):201-212.
Dutch adolescents. Psychol Health. 2000;15(3):357-368. 32. Skowronski J. On the psychology of organ donation: attitudinal
10. Chen JX, Zhang TM, Lim FL, et al. Current knowledge and atti- and situational factors. Basic Appl Soc Psychol. 1997;
tudes about organ donation and transplantation among Chinese 19(4):427-456.
university students. Transplant Proc. 2006;38(9): 2761-2765. 33. van den Berg H, Manstead ASR, van der Plight J, Wigboldus
11. Conesa C, Rios A, Ramirez P, et al. Psychosocial profile in favour DHJ. The role of affect in attitudes toward organ donation and
of organ donation. Transplant Proc. 2003;35(4):1276-1281. donor-relevant decisions. Psychol Health. 2005;20(6):789-802.
12. Conesa C, Rios Zambudio A, Ramirez P, Canteras M, 34. Weber K, Corrigan M, Martin MM, Members of COMM 160.
Rodriguez MdM, Parrilla P. Socio-personal profile of Real donors, real consent: testing the theory of reasoned action
teenagers opposed to organ donation. Nephrol Dialy Trans- on organ donor consent. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2007;
plant. 2004;19(5):1269-1275. 37(10):2435-2450.
13. Conesa C, Rios A, Ramirez P, Canteras M, Rodriguez M, Par- 35. Yeung I, Kong SH, Lee J. Attitudes towards organ donation in
rilla P. Attitudes toward organ donation in rural areas of Hong Kong. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50(11):1643-1654.
southeastern Spain. Transplant Proc. 2006;38(3):866-868. 36. Wu AM, Tang CS. The negative impact of death anxiety on
14. Feeley TH, Servoss TJ. Examining college students’ intentions self-efficacy and willingness to donate organs among Chinese
to become organ donors. J Health Commun. 2005;10(3):237-249. adults. Death Stud. 2009;33(1):51-72.
15. Horton RL, Horton PJ. Knowledge regarding organ donation: 37. Atkins D, Best D, Briss PA, et al. Grading quality of evidence and
Identifying and overcoming barriers to organ donation. Soc strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2004; 328(7454):1490.
Sci Med. 1990;31(7):791-800. 38. Morgan S, Miller J. Communicating about gifts of life: the
16. Horton R, Horton P. A model of willingness to become a effect of knowledge, attitudes, and altruism on behavior and
potential organ donor. Soc Sci Med. 1991;33(9):1037-1051. behavioral intentions regarding organ donation. J Appl Com-
17. Li PK, Lin CK, Lam PKW, et al. Attitudes about organ and mun Res. 2002;30(2):163-178.
tissue donation among the general public and blood donors in 39. Goodmonson C, Glaudin V. The relationship of commitment free
Hong Kong. Prog Transplant. 2001;11(2):98-103. behavior and commitment behavior: a study of attitude toward
18. Miles M, Frauman A. Public attitudes toward organ donation. organ transplantation. J Soc Issues. 1971;27(4):171-183.
Dial Transplant. 1988;17(2):74-76. 40. McCroskey JC, Richmond VA. Introduction to Interpersonal
19. Morgan SE, Miller JK, Arasaratnam LA. Similarities and dif- Communication. Edina, MN: Burgess International Group; 1992.
ferences between African Americans’ and European Ameri- 41. Wu AMS, Tang CSK, Yan ECW. Assessment of death anxiety
cans’ attitudes, knowledge, and willingness to communicate among Chinese. Adv Psychol Res. 2003;28:119-128.
about organ donation. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33(4):693-715. 42. Breitkopf CR. Perceived consequences of communicating
20. Morgan SE. Many facets of reluctance: African Americans organ donation wishes: an analysis of beliefs about defending
and the decision (not) to donate organs. J Natl Med Assoc. one’s decision. Psychol Health. 2006;21(4):481-497.
2006;98(5):695-703. 43. Reubsaet A, Brug J, Van den Borne B, Van Hooff H. Predictors
21. Park HS, Smith SW. Distinctiveness and influence of subjec- of organ donation registration among Dutch adolescents.
tive norms, personal descriptive and injunctive norms, and Transplantation. 2001;72(1):51-56.
societal descriptive and injuntive norms on behavioral intent: 44. Floden A, Kelvered M, Frid I, et al. Causes why organ donation
a case of two behaviors critical to organ donation. Human was not carried out despite the deceased being positive to dona-
Commun Res. 2006;33(2):194-218. tion. Transplant Proc. 2006;38(8):2619-2621.
22. Reubsaet A, Van den Borne B, Brug J, Pruyn J, Van Hooff H. 45. Schweiger M, Wasler A, Prenner G, et al. Improving the rate of
Determinants of the intention of Dutch adolescents to register organ donation. Transplant Proc. 2004;36(9):2543-2545.
as organ donors. Soc Sci Med. 2001;53(3):383-392. 46. Simpkin A, Robertson L, Barber V, Young J. Modifiable fac-
23. Rios A, Cascales P, Martinez L, et al. Emigration from the tors influencing relatives’ decision to offer organ donation: sys-
British Isles to Southeastern Spain: a study of attitudes toward tematic review. BMJ. 2009;338(21):b991-b991.
organ donation. Am J Transplant. 2007;7:2020-2030. 47. Lawlor M, Billson F. Registering wishes about organ donation
24. Romero LJ, Vigil JJ, Lindeman RD. Willingness to donate may decrease the number of donors. Med J Aust. 2007;186(3):156.

Progress in Transplantation, Vol 20, No. 4, December 2010 391


Copyright of Progress in Transplantation is the property of North American Transplant Coordinators
Organization, Inc. (NATCO) and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi