Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232222451

Time for a view on screen time

Article in Archives of Disease in Childhood · October 2012


DOI: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

29 890

1 author:

Aric Sigman
Dr Aric Sigman
4 PUBLICATIONS 55 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

It is not a 'project' it is a paper View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Aric Sigman on 26 January 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Leading article

have a television, computer or video game


Time for a view on screen time equipment in his or her bedroom.’13 The
French Government prohibits French
channels from airing all TV programmes
Aric Sigman —educational and otherwise—aimed at
children under 3 years of age.14
INTRODUCTION viewing is distinct from other forms of What is the basis for this concern?
In Britain today, children by sedentary behaviour, and point to the
the age of 10 years have potential public health benefits of a MORTALITY AND MORBIDITY
regular access to an average of reduction in ST. It is proposed that Numerous well designed prospective
five different screens at home. Britain and Europe’s medical establish- cohort studies continue to find a highly
In addition to the main family ments now offer guidance on the average significant dose-response association
television, for example, many very young number of hours per day children spend between ST and risk of type 2 diabetes,
children have their own bedroom TV viewing screen media, and the age at cardiovascular disease (CVD) and all-
along with portable handheld computer which they start. cause mortality among adults, with ST
game consoles (eg, Nintendo, Playstation, identified as an independent risk factor
Xbox), smartphone with games, internet LEVEL OF CONSUMPTION: DOSE with biological plausibility.15–17
and video, a family computer and a Children of all ages today, are watching Wijndaele et al16 recently reported
laptop and/or a tablet computer (eg, more screen media than ever before.5 that every 1 h/day increase in television
iPad). Children routinely engage in two Excluding any viewing time outside the viewing was associated with a 6%
or more forms of screen viewing at the home, such as hand-held screen activities, increased hazard for total fatal or non-
same time, such as TV and laptop.1 the average ST in the home for young fatal CVD, and an 8% increased hazard
Viewing is starting earlier in life. Nearly British adolescents is 6.1 h/day, and this for coronary heart disease, independent
one in three American infants has a TV is rising significantly.6 Canadian children of gender, age, education, smoking,
in their bedroom, and almost half of all spend 7.8 h/day, and American children alcohol, medication, diabetes status, CVD
infants watch TV or DVDs for nearly 7.5 h: 55% of their waking lives.7 8 The family history, sleep duration and phys-
2 h/day.2 ongoing EU ToyBox study group has just ical activity energy expenditure.
Across the industrialised world, watch- reported, ‘In general, parents had no At the same time, others report that
ing screen media is the main pastime of formal rules about TV viewing… for participants engaging in 4 h/day of
children. Over the course of childhood, Remarkably, in most countries, parents of recreational ST (eg, TV, computers) rela-
children spend more time watching TV a low SES had almost no rules regarding tive to <2 h/day, there was a 48%
than they spend in school.3 When includ- watching TV. Children can watch TV all increased hazard for all-cause mortality,
ing computer games, internet and DVDs, day long or whenever they want.’9 and a 125% increased hazard for CVD
by the age of seven years, a child born However, ST is no longer merely a cul- events of which 25% was explained by
today will have spent one full year of tural issue about how children spend cardiometabolic biomarkers.18
24 h days watching screen media. By the their leisure time, nor is it confined to In children and adults, ST has also
age of 18 years, the average European concern over the appropriate/inappropri- been found to have an unfavourable
child will have spent 3 years of 24 h days ate content of what is on the screen. It dose-response association with a range of
watching screen media; at this rate, by has become a medical issue, often exhi- biomarkers for CVD, type 2 diabetes and
the age of 80 years, they will have spent biting a dose-response relationship with metabolic syndrome (MetS) including
17.6 years4 glued to media screens. disease risk. LDL/HDL/total cholesterol, triglyceride,
Yet, irrespective of the content or edu- fibrinogen, systolic/diastolic blood pres-
cational value of what is being viewed, SCREEN TIME GUIDELINES sure (BP) and C-reactive protein.16 19
the sheer amount of average daily screen The US Department of Health and Examining television/DVD/video and
time (ST) during discretionary hours Human Services now cites reducing ST as computer use and metabolic risk, Hardy
after school is increasingly being consid- one of its key ‘health improvement prior- et al20 found that adolescent boys who
ered an independent risk factor for ities’ in achieving its ‘national 10-year exceed 2 h/day of STwere more than twice
disease, and is recognised as such by health promotion and disease prevention as likely to have abnormal levels of insulin
other governments and medical bodies objective’: ‘to increase the proportion of chil- and homeostatic model assessment of
but not, however, in Britain or in most of dren aged 0 to 2 years who view no television insulin resistance (HOMA-IR), suggesting
the EU. To date, views of the British and or videos on an average weekday, and increase an increased risk of insulin resistance.
European medical establishments on the proportion of children and adolescents Another study involving 13–18.5-year-olds
increasingly high levels of child ST aged 2 years through 12th grade [18 years] in five Spanish cities found those watching
remain conspicuous by their absence. who view television, videos, or play video more than 3 h of TV per day had ‘signifi-
This paper will highlight the dramatic games for no more than 2 hours a day.’10 The cantly less favourable levels’ of HDL-
increase in the time children today spend Australian Department of Health and cholesterol, glucose, apolipoprotein A1 and
watching screen media. It will provide a Ageing has issued similar guidelines, as overall CVD risk scores.21
brief overview of some specific health has the American Academy of Pediatrics
and well-being concerns of current (AAP), adding: ‘media—both foreground VIEWING IS NOT MERELY
viewing levels, explain why screen and background—have potentially nega- ‘SEDENTARY BEHAVIOUR’
tive effects and no known positive effects Nevertheless, the association between ST
Correspondence to Dr A Sigman, Office 444, 91
for children younger than 2 years.’11 12 and health risk appears not as simple as
Western Road, Brighton BN1 2NW, UK; aric@ The Canadian Paediatric Society has gone ST merely being a sedentary behaviour.
aricsigman.com further, ‘No child should be allowed to First, sedentary behaviour as a risk factor

Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0 1


Leading article

is distinct from too little moderate-to- children who had used Information and In a randomised crossover study, video
vigorous physical activity (MVPA) in all Communication Technology (ICT) equip- game playing was found to significantly
age groups, with the two possibly being ment for an average of 3 h the preceding increase food intake in adolescents imme-
separate constructs involving different day (only half the UK child norm) diately after playing ‘and was not com-
metabolic pathways contributing to showed a significantly reduced cortisol pensated for during the rest of the day.’34
disease.19 20 22 23 Prolonged ST elevates increase 1 h after waking compared with One hour of playing a video game
health risk independent of the level of children who had not used ICT at all, or resulted in a daily energy surplus of
MVPA people routinely engage in.22 A for less than 1 h. ‘The results suggest a stress 163 kcal, a rate of 60 000 kcal/year, which
recent analysis of the ongoing US response as a consequence of a long period of could help manufacture almost 8 kg of
National Institutes of Health Diet and ICT use… [which] can persist over night and body fat per year.
Health Study observed that even among have an impact on the regulation of All these effects are taking place at a
people with high levels of MVPA, high HPA-activity even the next morning.’ They time in our history when 68% of dinners
amounts of television viewing remain suggest that child ST day after day may in the UK are eaten while watching
associated with a 47% increased risk for ‘predispose some adolescents to the devel- television.5
all-cause, and a 100% increased risk for opment of allostatic load.’ There is Interestingly, a randomised controlled
cardiovascular mortality.17 Another study already concern that even HPA changes clinical intervention trial divided 4–
of body composition and abdominal within the normal range may be subtle 7-year-olds into two groups: one had its
obesity in children across 10 European early indicators of, and contributors to, TV and computer viewing reduced by
cities has recently concluded that phys- unfavourable physical health outcomes in half; the other did not. After 3 years,
ical activity does not attenuate the adolescence and adulthood. there had been a significant reduction in
obesity risk associated with ST.24 The educational value of screen mater- the BMI of those who had halved their
Moreover, children’s ST may be some- ial being viewed does not preclude the screen viewing, and relatively little in
what distinct from other forms of seden- significant associations reported above those who had not.35
tary behaviour in its influence on between ST and morbidity, mortality and The above findings may have signifi-
biological risk factors for disease.23 25 26 associated biomarkers. cant public health implications. Children
For example, examining a range of seden- 9–12 years with a high BMI are more
tary behaviours, screen activities and BP likely to have high BP, cholesterol and
in young adolescents, Gopinath et al25 OBESOGENIC MECHANISMS blood insulin levels by adolescence.36
reported a dose-response relationship, Increased TV viewing has been consist- Even marginally elevated BMI in adoles-
‘each hour per day spent in screen time, ently shown to be linked to increased cence constitutes a substantial risk factor
watching TV and playing video games body mass index (BMI) in both children for early occurrence of angiography-
was associated with a significant increase and adults independent of physical activ- proven coronary heart disease.37 The EU
in diastolic BP of 0.44 ( p=0.0001), 0.99 ity.22 29 The association appears stronger ToyBox study has in its ‘Evidence-based
( p<0.0001) and 0.64 mm Hg ( p=0.04), in young children. A cross-sectional study recommendations for the development of
respectively. By contrast, each hour per assessing fat mass by Jackson et al30 obesity prevention programs targeted
day spent reading was associated with a found a dose-response relationship: ‘Each at preschool children’ just called for
decrease…’ In prepubertal children, TV extra hour of watching TV was asso- ‘Limitation of leisure screen time to
viewing and total ST, but not computer ciated with an extra 1 kg of body fat… <1 h/d (or the amount of time recom-
use, have been found to be positively Preschool children who watch more TV mended by appropriate national guide-
associated with both systolic and dia- are fatter and are less active … the rela- lines, if less than 1 h/d)’.29
stolic BP, while painting or sitting were tion between TV viewing and fatness is
not.26 Additionally, different screen activ- not mediated by physical activity…’. BRAIN AND COGNITION
ities may have differing independent ST is clearly associated with unhealthy ST is associated in a dose-response
associations with biomarkers and chronic dietary behaviours in children, adoles- manner with subsequent attention pro-
disease risk in youth.23 26 27 cents and adults.31 However, in addition blems in a variety of age groups. A longi-
A cross-sectional study of a large to the influence of food advertising, tudinal study of 2623 children reported
sample of overweight and obese adoles- studies of children’s ingestive behaviour that children who watched television at
cents concluded that time spent playing in direct response to screen viewing ages 1 and 3 years had a significantly
seated video games was the only type of suggest it can act as a distraction away increased risk of developing attentional
ST associated with increased BP and total from vital satiation food cues toward problems by the time they were 7 years
cholesterol/HDL ratio.27 Carson and non-food cues (screen), thereby disrupt- old. For every hour of television a child
Janssen found in a representative popula- ing the development of habituation to watched per day, there was a 9% increase
tion of 6–19-year-olds that time spent food and, therefore, increasing energy in subsequent attentional problems con-
watching TV was predictive of a higher intake while children are viewing.32 sistent with a diagnosis of ADHD.38
score of an aggregated or clustered Eating a meal while viewing screens is A longer-term dose-response association
measure of cardio-metabolic risk, but rec- also thought to disrupt the encoding and has been found between television
reational computer time was not.23 While memory formation of the meal. Impaired viewing at the ages of 5 and 11 years,
in prepubertal children, Martinez-Gomez memory for recent eating may increase and subsequent attention problems in
et al26 found that TV viewing but not food intake hours after viewing stops. A adolescence independent of early atten-
computer time was associated with study in the journal Appetite found that tion problems and other confounders.39
increased BP. the ‘effects of television watching on Similar associations have been reported
Studies of HPA stress-regulation and ST food intake extend beyond the time of in 14–22-year-olds, and in a study of
shed further light on underlying mechan- television watching to affect subsequent 8–24-year-olds, Swing et al concluded,
isms. Wallenius et al28 found school-aged consumption’.33 ‘Viewing television and playing video

2 Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0


Leading article

games each are associated with increased physical activity (MVPA) or overall sed- the nonverbal nuances of others’ emo-
subsequent attention problems in child- entary time.’50 A cross-sectional analysis tions involve similar learning processes
hood… late adolescence and early of 9–10-year-old girls found ST to be which appear to have a neural basis. For
adulthood…’.40 41 ‘negatively associated with self-esteem’.49 example, the insular cortex has been iden-
In addition to ST, screen ‘speed’—the A survey of 3461 North American girls tified as a key brain mechanism involved
pace of editing and degree of novelty aged 8–12 years found a significant asso- in experiencing the emotional states of
within screen material—is increasingly ciation between ST as well as measures of others and is thought to underlie egalitar-
thought to be an important factor in the media multitasking, and negative psycho- ian behaviour in humans.56 Feeling
above findings. Lillard and Peterson social well-being. empathy for a friend’s emotional suffer-
‘found that 9 minutes of viewing a Conversely, face-to-face communica- ing activates affective pain regions (dorsal
popular fast-paced fantastical television tion was strongly associated with posi- anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and
show immediately impaired 4-year-olds’ tive psychosocial well-being.48 A study of insula) associated with having firsthand
EF [executive function], a result about Japanese children aged 5–14 years reports experience of the same suffering.57 The
which parents of young children should ST had a strong association with negative learning effects of routinely experiencing
be aware.’ 42 feeling upon awakening and recommends such social emotions are reflected neuro-
Dopamine is central to the ability to guidelines for child ST as a preventive logically. The ‘deliberate cultivation of
pay attention and implicated in attention measure.47 compassion’ through ‘compassion train-
problems. It is produced in response to Adjusting for pre-existing individual ing’ for empathic responses are associated
screen novelty. Significant dopamine and family factors, a prospective longitu- with changes in functional
release within the striatum is found to dinal study of 1314 Canadian children neuroplasticity.58
occur quickly in young adult brains while found significant negative associations At the same time, researchers conduct-
playing computer games.43 44 Dopamine between ST at 29 and 53 months of age, ing functional magnetic resonance
is also a key component of the brain’s and psychosocial well-being at age imaging (fMRI) research have expressed
reward system, and is heavily implicated 10 years. Each 1 h increase in early child- concern that when using the internet, for
in the formation and maintenance of hood exposure corresponded to a 7% unit example, the areas of the brain associated
addictions. There are growing concerns decrease in classroom engagement and with empathy showed virtually no
that extensive computer game playing 10% unit increase in victimisation in increase in stimulation, concluding
may lead to long-term changes in the middle childhood. Researchers reported ‘Young people are growing up immersed
reward circuitry that resemble the effects ‘Higher levels of early childhood televi- in this technology and their brains are
of substance dependence.44 In addition to sion exposure predicted greater chances of more malleable, more plastic and chan-
computer games, screen viewing by peer rejection experiences such as being ging than with older brains … As the
youngsters begets more viewing and, teased, assaulted, or insulted by other brain evolves and shifts its focus towards
unlike other sedentary behaviours, most students … our results suggest that new technological skills, it drifts away
of the criteria of substance dependence reduced time for critical social interac- from fundamental social skills.’59 60
apply to people with higher levels of tions in early childhood owing to dis-
ST.45 Screen ‘addiction’, once a populist placed time spent watching television Mirror neurons
catch phrase, is increasingly being used may present later specific risks of devel- A child’s brain is also believed to have
by physicians to describe the growing oping inadequate social skills.’52 multiple mirror neuron systems that spe-
number of children engaging in screen In understanding the above associa- cialise in carrying out and understanding
activities in a dependent manner.46 tions, several mechanisms have been pro- not just the actions of others, but their
posed. Humans require a certain amount intentions—the social meaning of their
PSYCHOSOCIAL HEALTH of ‘co-presence’—regular eye-to-eye behaviour and their emotions.
ST is strongly associated with measures contact for optimal physical and mental People who rank high on a scale meas-
of child mental well-being.47–50 The AAP health.53 Moreover, the fundamental uring empathy have particularly active
has recently published a report on The ability to relate to others is dependent on mirror neuron systems. A study of the
Impact of Social Media on Children, social and emotional skills that are learnt brain activity of 10-year-olds who
Adolescents and Families, which contains through regular social interaction. observed and imitated emotional expres-
a section entitled ‘Facebook Depression’, Face-to-face conversations confer linguis- sions and social skills found a direct rela-
which is ‘defined as depression that devel- tic skills, along with the ability to have tionship between the level of activity in
ops when preteens and teens spend a conversations—to know when and how the children’s mirror neuron systems and
great deal of time on social media sites, to listen and contribute. This learning ‘two distinct indicators of social func-
such as Facebook, and then begin to process is highly technical and time con- tioning in typically developing children’:
exhibit classic symptoms of depression.’51 suming.54 For example, during empathy and social skills. The authors
A British study found that children who face-to-face interaction, in addition to concluded that the importance of observ-
spent more than 2 h/day watching televi- hearing a voice and accompanying facial ing and copying everyday social beha-
sion or using a computer ‘were at [60%] expressions of the speaker’s face, the viours and the mirror neuron system
increased risk of high levels of psycho- speech sounds produce tiny bursts of ‘may indeed be relevant to social func-
logical difficulties and this risk increased aspiration—air pressure which hit the tioning in everyday life during typical
if the children also failed to meet physical child’s skin—tactile information contrib- human development’.61
activity guidelines. …Both television uting to auditory perception.55 It may be that children must exercise
viewing and computer use are important specific brain areas and systems regularly
independent targets for intervention for Neurobiology of socialisation and extensively in situ, in order to
optimal well-being for children, irrespect- The development of empathy and com- develop crucial social and emotional skills
ive of levels of moderate/vigorous passion requiring subtle skills of reading or deficits will emerge later.

Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0 3


Leading article

A meta-analysis of 72 studies on TV viewing or screen-media use’.66 premature exposure to screen media.


empathy conducted between 1979 and Research by the Centers for Disease Screen viewing should be delayed, or
2009 among almost 14 000 university Control and Prevention (CDC), along minimised, until age 3 years.
students ‘found the biggest drop in with other studies, has found parental ▸ Encourage no screens in children’s
empathy after the year 2000. College kids rules and limits on ST effectively reduce bedrooms.
today are about 40 per cent lower in ST, as does not having screens in bed- ▸ Parents of younger children should be
empathy than their counterparts of 20 or rooms.67 68 The CDC study also found a advised to choose screen material with
thirty years ago, as measured by standard negative dose-response relationship a slower pace, less novelty and more
tests of this personality trait.’ The between weekly physical activity and the of a single narrative quality.
authors believe that the sheer increase in risk of exceeding recommended ST limits, ▸ Parents should be encouraged to
child and adolescent ST during this time recommending the promotion of physical monitor and control the time their
could be one very important factor, and activity as an additional means of redu- children spend on hand-held computer
concluded that the rise of social media cing ST. games/media.
may also play a role in the decline in Role modelling is another important ▸ Ideal discretionary ST limits are:
empathy: ‘The ease of having ‘friends’ factor. Parent and child levels of screen – 3–7 years: 0.5–1 h/day
online might make people more likely to viewing are strongly related: children – 7–12 years: 1 h
just tune out when they don’t feel like who live in a household that promotes – 12–15 years: 1.5 h
responding to others’ problems, a behav- TV-watching (TV is on when the child – 16+ years: 2 h
iour that could carry over offline.’ They comes home from school, and meals are Parents must take into consideration
also believe electronic media has contrib- eaten in front of the TV) are more likely how much time their children are spend-
uted to a social environment that works to watch excessive amounts of television ing doing homework on computers
against slowing down and listening to themselves. If parents watch television before coming to a decision on discretion-
someone who requires sympathy.62 for more than 4 h/day, their son and ary ST for their child.
Returning to the potential role of ST daughter, respectively, will be 10.5 and ▸ Parents should be aware of the role-
in psychosocial learning, it is known that three times more likely to watch it for modelling influence their own viewing
younger children experience considerable more than 4 h/day too.69 habits may have on their children
difficulty when translating to real life along with the potential influence of
what they see on a screen. Children learn RECOMMENDATIONS background or ‘passive’ media.
tasks better from a live demonstration The associations between ST and health ▸ Information about infants and tod-
than from an equivalent televised demon- risks are reported to occur generally dlers watching screens should be
stration, a problem referred to as the video beyond exposure of 2 h/day. Yet the included within maternity ward ‘birth
deficit.63 This effect becomes more pro- average child is exposed to three times packs’ given to mothers.
nounced and may persist at older ages as this amount. Therefore, reducing total ▸ Health visitors should be aware of
the task complexity increases—and psy- daily ST for children, and delaying the medical evidence and advise new
chosocial tasks, such as perceiving and age at which they start, could provide sig- parents.
interpreting other’s actions, emotions nificant advantages for their health and ▸ Nurseries and day care centres should
and intentions are highly complex. well-being. Although popular phrases make parents aware of this issue, as is
Regarding the daily time available for such as ‘striking a balance’ or ‘everything the case in Belgium and France.
children to learn psychosocial skills in moderation’ may sound reassuringly ▸ Schools should adopt a position on
through face-to-face interactions, studies sensible, one of the main obstacles in the amount of time children spend in
at Stanford University have led to a ‘dis- encouraging parents to reduce their chil- front of a screen in and out of school
placement’ theory of internet use: dren’s ST is the vagueness of the terms and communicate this to pupils and
‘moderation’ and ‘excessive’. parents.
In short, no matter how time online is
Paediatricians must now define these
measured and no matter which type of
social activity is considered, time spent terms by first considering ST as simply DISCUSSION
on the Internet reduces time spent in another form of consumption measured The study of ST as a public health subject
face-to-face relationships… an hour on in units of hours/minutes consumed per is relatively new, and the associations
the Internet reduces face-to-face time day: a simple public health concept to between ST and health risk cited in the
with family by close to twenty- grasp and act upon. Considering the observational studies above do not prove
four minutes.64 existing empirical research and position direct causation. While it is not possible
of medical bodies and governments in within the confines of this paper to
Sigman reported that between 1987
other countries, the following guidance describe and compare the design and inde-
and 2007 the number of hours per day of
on recreational ST (eg, before and after pendent variables, including the demo-
face-to-face social interaction declined
school) are only ideals for parents. Even graphic profile of each study, others are
markedly as the use of electronic media
if they are not adhered to, it is important beginning to do so. For example, a
has increased.65
to establish such ideals as a reference meta-analysis of relevant studies, includ-
point to work from. ing some of the above, involving
INTERVENTIONS ▸ Eighty per cent of adult brain size European, US and Australian populations
There is good evidence that children’s ST growth occurs during a child’s first on ST and risk of Type 2 Diabetes, CVD
can be reduced through simple measures, 3 years, when they may be most vul- and all-cause mortality carried out at the
mainly raising parental awareness. A sys- nerable to the effects of screen media. Harvard School of Public Health, included
tematic review by researchers at Harvard There should be a buffer zone in the large sample sizes, long durations of
Medical School recently reported that 29 early years, whereby this stage of child follow-up, and well established prospect-
studies ‘achieved significant reductions in development is ‘cordoned off ’ from ive studies. In addition, their pooled

4 Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0


Leading article

estimates were based on prospective ana- parents who were ignored or unacknow- they spent in focused attention during
lyses with detailed adjustment for a wide ledged on their return home because chil- play. Researchers concluded, ‘These find-
range of confounding variables. After dren were ‘otherwise engaged in [screen] ings have implications for subsequent
adjusting for BMI or other obesity mea- activity… comprised a substantial percent- cognitive development.’73 Researchers
sures and dietary variables, the associa- age of observed behavior’. The high level have also found both the quantity and
tions with health risk still remained of being unacknowledged ‘encountered by the quality of interactions between
highly significant.15 fathers when they reunited with their chil- parents and children decline markedly
The various studies finding associations dren was particularly striking… These when background TV is on, which ‘may
between ST and subsequent attention latter results are particularly noteworthy. have a negative impact on develop-
problems were predominantly prospective Social scientists have long documented the ment’.74 Current US estimates for chil-
and controlled for relevant confounding near universality of positive behavior in dren 8 months to 8 years of age are 3.9 h
variables including pre-existing attention the form of greetings when two or more of background TV per day.75 The report
problems.38–41 Several of the above people reunite after being apart for a by AAP ‘recommends that parents and
studies on ST and psychosocial well-being period of time. Greetings recognize a caregivers … recognize that their own
used cross-sectional designs, making it person’s arrival, status and display positive [background] media use can have a nega-
more difficult to determine whether intentions that universally facilitate the tive effect on children’. The AAP consid-
higher levels of ST were associated with transition into social interaction with ers claims by media industry executives
the development of negative well-being, another.’ ST has changed this.71 that educational media programmes are
or negative well-being preceded higher Although this paper is concerned with meant to be watched by both the parent
levels of ST.47–50 For example, whether ST, when one includes screen content— and the child to facilitate social interac-
children with psychological difficulties, which may be very much at odds with tions and the learning process as having
such as extreme shyness, are more likely the values and desires of parents, but quite the opposite effect.12
to gravitate towards ST over more soci- which children may be absorbing for There are limitations in simply advis-
able activities. many hours a day, the distorting effect ing parents to reduce children’s ST. As
However, the prospective longitudinal on the parental role in imparting their with many preventative health initia-
Canadian study controlling for pre- own values and providing boundaries for tives, such advice may be least effective
existing psychosocial problems found sig- adolescence could be considerable. A in families where it matters most.
nificant associations with ST.52 decade ago, in their ‘Analysis of However, it is wrong to assume that it is
There are additional concerns not Children’s Programming Provision’, the simply the underprivileged families being
addressed above, regarding safeguarding British Government’s Broadcasting referred to. The AAP recently reported
children from online exploitation and Standards Commission concluded ‘The ‘Parents who believe that educational
exposure to inappropriate material. There television is almost like a member of the television is “very important for healthy
are also emerging concerns about the family in its own right’. With subsequent development” are twice as likely to keep
potential effect of 3D screens on the birth of multiple screens, today extensive the television on all or most of the
development of the child’s stereoscopic ST appears to have created the three- time.’12 Most parents want to reduce
vision and depth perception through pos- parent family. health risks for their children, provided
sible alterations in the development of Both within and outside the family, they understand the rationale behind the
the ‘fusion center’ of the brain. children learn the rules of relationships advisory, and a clear course of action is
Interestingly, in their ‘Nintendo 3DS— through extensive routine face-to-face offered. For example, between 1996 and
Parents’ Information, 3D Concerns’, experience. Yet, as social media consumes 2007, children in England, exposed to
Nintendo of America states, ‘children 6 a larger portion of a child’s daily life, this passive smoking, declined by nearly 70%.
and under should not use the 3D feature is likely to create difficulties in extrapolat- Most interestingly, declines were greatest
of the Nintendo 3DS system.70 ing on-line ‘un-boundaried’ relationships in the 2 years immediately preceding legis-
The simple arithmetic of hours of to those of the ‘real’ world, thereby lation as the result of, it is thought, the
eye-to-screen contact versus eye-to-eye affecting those relationships. media campaigns and greater publicity on
contact are making clear the profound The family environment has been the impacts of passive smoking during
implications of such extensive ST for described as an ecosystem of interruption this period. Subsequent research has
family and social relationships. In 2007, technologies. Background media that is found that ‘absolute declines in exposure
when children had access to far fewer not being actively viewed by the child is were greater in those most exposed at the
screens, a large-scale study by Britain’s increasingly associated with developmen- outset … including those living in lower
Children’s Society found that television tal risks. A study of 5–6-year-olds found sociodemographic status households’.76
alone was already displacing the parental that both active TV viewing and back- At the moment, most parents and phy-
role, eclipsing ‘by a factor of five or ten ground ‘passive’ TV exposure was related sicians are unaware of ST as a health
the time parents spend actively engaging to shorter sleep duration, sleeping disor- issue, and if they do harbour concerns,
with children’. ders and overall sleep disturbances. they remain unaware of the rationale or a
An ongoing study of families in situ by Moreover, passive exposure to TV of more clear rule of thumb as a course of action.
the University of California–Los Angeles than 3 h/day was strongly related to It is, therefore, an imperative to first
has found that social disengagement is sleep disturbances, therefore, ‘parents redress this knowledge void. Given that
rapidly increasing, as side-by-side and should control the quantity of TV many parents have grown to rely on ST
eye-to-eye human interactions in the viewing and … limit children’s exposure as a means of occupying their children—‘the
home are being displaced by the to passive TV.’72 A study of 1–3-year-olds electronic babysitter’—any guidance on
eye-to-screen relationship. It was reported found that background TV significantly ST would benefit from the inclusion of
that ‘family members seldom came reduced the amount of time they played suggestions for developing constructive
together as a group.’ The number of with their toys, and the amount of time practical alternatives to ST.

Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0 5


Leading article

Although children may be regularly child health. Few seem to have spotted outcomes. As health risks are reported to
involved in various screen activities and the most salient point: their focus is not occur beyond exposure of 2 h of ST per
using different screen devices, most ST is on child health, but as one leading insti- day, although the average child is exposed
recreational, that is, discretionary, and it tution advising successive British govern- to three times this amount, a robust ini-
is this ST that should be the primary ments states, ‘research focuses on tiative to encourage a reduction in daily
public health target for reduction. There children’s and young people’s interactions recreational ST could lead to significant
may be scope for further reductions. with [author’s italics] electronic media, improvements in child health and devel-
Preschool and infant schools, if made and on media education.’ opment. Britain and European medical
aware of this issue, could easily reduce establishments should consider ST as a
ST, as much of it is predominantly discre- Information hygiene separate entity from sedentary behaviour,
tionary at that stage of education. Junior Moreover, while medical journals, such as and offer an advisory on the average
and secondary schools could reconsider the BMJ group have instituted measures number of hours per day young children,
the extent to which entertainment TV to identify competing interests and in particular, are viewing screen media,
programmes and films are used as lessons sources of funding, particularly regarding and the age at which they start.
and as ‘golden time’. There is a concern the pharmaceutical industry, in order to
Contributors Author and guarantor: AS.
that not to expose young children to minimise such influence within their
screen technology puts them at a devel- publications, thereby maintaining a high Funding None.
opmental and educational disadvantage. standard of information hygiene, the Competing interests The author declares: no support
Furthermore, it is often assumed that if culture and standard of hygiene with from any organisation for the submitted work; no
financial relationships with any organisations that might
children do not ‘get used to’ screen tech- respect to research on screen media and
have an interest in the submitted work in the previous
nology early on, they will in some way children is very different in media, psych- 3 years.
be intimidated by it, or be less competent ology and education journals.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned;
at using it later. However, research has As the most valid impartial arbiter of externally peer reviewed.
found that even Rhesus monkeys are child health, British and European paedi-
Accepted 11 July 2012
comfortable with, and capable of using, atric medicine must in future be at the
the same screen technology that children centre of research, public understanding Arch Dis Child 2012;0:1–8
doi:10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196
are exposed to.77 The charity, Orangutan and policy formation on ST.
Outreach, has been conducting trials In other areas of child health and
gauging iPad interaction with their apes. development, when considering the REFERENCES
1. Jago R, Sebire SJ, Gorely T, et al. ‘I’m on it 24/7 at
The director has reported ‘It’s not really potential effects of profound new devel-
the moment’: a qualitative examination of
toy-like because they are engaging with opments, the paediatric health profes- multi-screen viewing behaviours among UK 10–
them as devices … it’s definitely going in sion instinctively adopts a principle of 11-year-olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activ 2011;8:85.
the cognitive direction.’ 78 precaution. Yet, to date, the increasingly 2. Zero to Eight: Children’s Media Use in America.
excessive levels of child ST have been Common Sense Media. 2011. http://www.
commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/research/
Political implications met with relative silence. While many zerotoeightfinal2011.pdf
Perhaps because ST is not a dangerous questions remain regarding the precise 3. American Academy of Child and Adolescent
substance or a visibly risky activity, it has nature of the association between Psychiatry. Children & watching TV. Facts Fam
eluded the scrutiny that other health ST and adverse outcomes, the advice 2001;54:1–2.
4. Sigman A. The impact of screen media on children:
issues attract. Additionally, there is little from a growing number of both research-
a Eurovision for parliament. In: Clouder C, et al., eds.
funding and public gratitude in looking ers and other medical associations and Improving the quality of childhood in Europe 2012.
for the negative effects of the world’s government health departments else- Vol 3. European Parliament Working Group on the
favourite pastime. In 2006, American where is becoming unequivocal: reduce Quality of Childhood in the European Union,
paediatric researchers writing in the ST.10–17 21 24 29 35 38 41 80–83 Irrespective 2012:88–121.
5. TV Licencing. TeleScope: A focus on the nation’s
American Medical Association’s Archives of whether this will endear paediatricians viewing habits from TV Licensing. UK, 2011. http://
of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine asked to the public or not, Britain and Europe’s www.tvlicensing.co.uk/resources/library/BBC/
rhetorically: ‘Why is it that something medical establishments, including the MEDIA_CENTRE/TeleScope_report.pdf
that is widely recognised as being so RCPCH, EAP and Department of Health, 6. OfCom. Children and parents: media use and
attitudes report. 2011. http://stakeholders.ofcom.org.
influential and potentially dangerous has should now formally express some
uk/binaries/research/media-literacy/oct2011/
resulted in so little effective action? To be concern over ST. Children_and_parents.pdf
sure, there has been some lack of political A drop in viewing figures may be bad 7. Leatherdale ST, Ahmed R. Screen-based sedentary
will to take on the enormously powerful for TV executives’ blood pressure, but behaviours among a nationally representative sample
and influential entertainment industry … may prove to be very good for child of youth: are Canadian kids couch potatoes? Chron
Dis Injuries Canada 2011;31:141–6.
[Screen] media need to be recognised as a health. 8. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
major public health issue’.79 Obesity and Overweight for Professionals: Childhood:
CONCLUSION A Growing Problem. 2011. p 3. http://www.cdc.gov/
A problem of provenance Children of all ages are spending more obesity/childhood/problem.html
9. De Decker E, De Craemer M, De Bourdeaudhuij I,
Rather than paediatricians, it has been time than ever before, watching screen
et al., ToyBox-study group. Influencing factors of
the academics in ICT, media literacy, edu- media. Viewing screen media is physiolo- screen time in preschool children: an exploration of
cation, marketing and social and cogni- gically distinct from other forms of sed- parents’ perceptions through focus groups in six
tive psychology, whose research is often entary behaviour, with average daily ST European countries. Obes Rev 2012;13:75–84.
funded by TV, software and technology increasingly considered an independent 10. U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services. Healthy People 2020, Objective PA-8:
industries, who have dominated research, risk factor, often exhibiting a dose- Increase the proportion of children and adolescents
public and professional understanding of, response relationship with disease and who do not exceed recommended limits for screen
and policy considerations on, ST and unfavourable child development time. http://healthypeople.gov/2020/

6 Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0


Leading article

topicsobjectives2020/objectiveslist.aspx?topicId=33 28. Wallenius M, Hirvonen A, Lindholm H, et al. 48. Pea R, Nass C, Meheula L, et al. Media use,
(accessed 10 Mar 2012). Salivary cortisol in relation to the use of Information face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and
11. Australian Department of Health and Ageing. and Communication Technology (ICT) in school-aged social well-being among 8-to 12-year-old girls. Dev
Education and Prevention: Non-productive sedentary children. Psychology 2010;1:88–95. Psychol 2012;48:327–36.
behaviour. Australian Government. http://www.health. 29. Summerbell CD, Moore HJ, Vögele C, et al. 49. Racine EF, DeBate RD, Gabriel KP, et al. The
gov.au/internet/publications/publishing.nsf/Content/ Evidence-based recommendations for the relationship between media use and psychological
gug-carer-toc~gug-carer-sedentary (accessed 24 development of obesity prevention programs and physical assets among third- to fifth-grade girls.
Feb 2012). targeted at preschool children. Obes Rev J Sch Health 2011;81:749–55.
12. American Academy of Pediatrics. Media use by 2012;13:129–32. 50. Page AS, Cooper AR, Griew P, et al. Children’s
children younger than 2 years. Council on 30. Jackson DM, Djafarian K, Stewart J, et al. screen viewing is related to psychological difficulties
communications and media. Pediatrics 2011;128. Increased television viewing is associated with irrespective of physical activity. Pediatrics 2010;126:
doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1753 elevated body fatness but not with lower total e1011–17.
13. Canadian Paediatric Society, Psychosocial energy expenditure in children. Am J Clin Nutr 51. Schurgin O’Keeffe G, Clarke-Pearson K, Council On
Paediatrics Committee. Impact of media use on 2009;89:1031–6. Communications And Media. Clinical report—the
children and youth: Recommendations. http://www. 31. Pearson N, Biddle SJH. Sedentary behavior and impact of social media on children, adolescents, and
cps.ca/english/statements/CP/pp03-01.htm dietary intake in children, adolescents, and adults: a families. Pediatrics 2011;127:799–804.
(accessed 11 Mar 2012). systematic review. Am J Prev Med 2011;41:178–88. 52. Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick CF, Barnett TA, et al.
14. France bans marketing television programmes 32. Temple JL, Giacomelli AM, Kent KM, et al. Prospective associations between early childhood
targeted at under threes. The Telegraph. 21 Television watching increases motivated responding television exposure and academic, psychosocial, and
August 2008. http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/ for food and energy intake in children. Am J Clin Nutr physical well-being by middle childhood. Arch Pediatr
worldnews/europe/france/2595495/France-bans- 2007;85:355–61. Adolesc Med 2010;164:425–31.
marketing-television-programmes-targeted-at-under- 33. Higgs S, Woodward M. Television watching during 53. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social
threes.html (accessed 22 Feb 2012). lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic
15. Grøntved A, Hu FB. Television viewing and risk of women. Appetite 2009;52:39–4. review. PLoS Med 2010;7:e1000316.
type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and all- 34. Chaput JP, Visby T, Nyby S, et al. Video game 54. Abu-Akel A. The psychological and social dynamics
cause mortality: a meta-analysis. JAMA playing increases food intake in adolescents: a of topic performance in family dinnertime
2011;305:2448–5. randomized crossover study. Am J Clin Nutr conversation. J Pragmatics 2002;34:1787–806.
16. Wijndaele K, Brage S, Besson H, et al. Television 2011;93:1196–203. 55. Gick B, Derrick D. Aero-tactile integration in speech
viewing and incident cardiovascular disease: 35. Epstein LH, Roemmich JN, Robinson JL, et al. A perception. Nature 2009;462:502–4.
prospective associations and mediation analysis in randomized trial of the effects of reducing television 56. Dawes CT, Loewen PJ, Schreiber D, et al. Neural
the EPIC norfolk atudy. PLoS ONE 2011;6:e20058. viewing and computer use on body mass index in basis of egalitarian behavior. PNAS
17. Mathews CE, George SM, Moore SC, et al. young children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2012;109:6479–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118653109
Amount of time spent in sedentary behaviors and 2008;162:239–45. 57. Meyer ML, Masten CL, Ma Y, et al. Empathy for the
cause-specific mortality in US adults. Am J Clin Nutr 36. Lawlor DA, Benfield L, Logue J, et al. Association social suffering of friends and strangers recruits
2012;95:437–45. between general and central adiposity in childhood, distinct patterns of brain activation. Soc Cogn Affect
18. Stamatakis E, Hamer M, Dunstan DW. Screen- and change in these, with cardiovascular risk factors Neurosci Published Online First: 21 February 2012.
based entertainment time, all-cause mortality, and in adolescence: prospective cohort study. BMJ doi: 10.1093/scan/nss019
car-diovascular events: population-based study with 2010;341:c6224. 58. Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Lamm C, et al. Functional
on- going mortality and hospital events follow-up. 37. Tirosh A, Shai I, Afek A, et al. Adolescent BMI neural plasticity and associated changes in positive
J Am Coll Cardiol 2011;57:292–9. trajectory and risk of diabetes versus coronary affect after compassion training. Cereb Cortex
19. Pinto Pereira SM, Ki M, Power C. Sedentary disease. N Engl J Med 2011;364:1315–25. Published Online First: June 1 2012. doi: 10.1093/
behaviour and biomarkers for cardiovascular disease 38. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, cercor/bhs142
and diabetes in mid-life: the role of television- et al. Early television exposure and subsequent 59. Immordino-Yang MH, McColla A, Damasioa H,
viewing and sitting at work. PLoS ONE 2012;7: attentional problems in children’. Pediatrics et al. Neural correlates of admiration and
e31132. 2004;113:708–13. compassion. Proc Nat Acad Sci 2009;106:8021–6.
20. Hardy LL, Denney-Wilson E, Thrift AP, et al. 39. Landhuis CE, Poulton R, Welch D, et al. Does 60. Small G. Research shows that Internet is Rewiring
Screen time and metabolic risk factors among childhood television viewing lead to attention our Brains. UCLA Faculty and Staff News. 15
adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med problems in adolescence? Results from a prospective October 2008. http://www.today.ucla.edu/portal/ut/
2010;164:643–9. longitudinal study. Pediatrics 2007;120:532–7. 081015_gary-small-ibrain.aspx
21. Martinez-Gomez D, Rey-López JP, Chillón P, et al. 40. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, et al. Extensive 61. Pfeifer JH, Iacobonia M, Mazziottaa JC, et al.
Excessive TV viewing and cardiovascular disease risk television viewing and the development of attention Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and
factors in adolescents. The AVENA cross-sectional and learning difficulties during adolescence. Arch interpersonal competence in children. NeuroImage
study. BMC Public Health 2010;10. Pediatr Adolesc Med 2007;161:480–6. 2008;39:2076–85.
22. Inoue S, Sugiyama T, Takamiya T, et al. Television 41. Swing EL, Gentile DA, Anderson CA, et al. 62. Konrath S, O’Brien EH, Hsing C. Changes in
viewing time is associated with overweight/obesity Television and video game exposure and the dispositional empathy in American college students
among older adults, independent of meeting physical development of attention problems. Pediatrics over time: a meta- analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Rev
activity and health guidelines. J Epidemiol 2010;126:213–21. 2011;15:180–98.
2012;22:50–6. 42. Lillard AS, Peterson J. The immediate impact of 63. Elizabeth Zack E, Barr R, Gerhardstein P, et al.
23. Carson V, Janssen I. Volume, patterns, and types of different types of television on young children’s Infant imitation from television using novel
sedentary behaviour and cardio-metabolic health in executive function. Pediatrics 2011;128:644–9. touch screen technology. Br J Dev Psychol
children and adolescents: a cross-sectional study. 43. Koepp MJ, Gunn RN, Lawrence AD, et al. Evidence 2009;27:13–26.
BMC Public Health 2011;11:274. for striatal dopamine release during a video game. 64. Nie NH, Stepanikova I, Pals H, et al. Ten years after
24. Rey-López J, Ruiz JR, Vicente-Rodríguez G, et al. Nature 1998;393:266–8. the birth of the internet: how do Americans use the
Physical activity does not attenuate the obesity risk 44. Weinstein AM. Computer and video game Internet in their daily lives? Report: Stanford Institute
of TV viewing in youth. Pediatr Obes 2012;7:240–50 addiction-a comparison between game users and for The Quantitative Study Of Society. Stanford
25. Gopinath B, Baur LA, Hardy LL, et al. Relationship non-game users. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse University; 2005.
between a range of sedentary behaviors and blood 2010;36:268–76. 65. Sigman A. Well connected? The biological
pressure during early adolescence. J Hum Hypertens 45. Kubey R, Csikszentmihalyi M. ‘Television addiction is implications of social networking. The Biologist
2011;26:350–6. no mere metaphor’. Sci Am Special Addition 2009;56:14–20.
26. Martinez-Gomez D, Tucker J, Heelan KA, et al. 2004;14:48–55. 66. Schmidt ME, Haines J, O’Brien A, et al. Systematic
Associations between sedentary behavior and blood 46. Lin F, Zhou Y, Du Y, et al. Abnormal white matter review of effective strategies for reducing screen time
pressure in young children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med integrity in adolescents with internet addiction among young children. Obesity 2012;20:1338–54.
2009;163:724–30. disorder: a tract- based spatial statistics study. PLoS 67. Carlson SA, Fulton JE, Lee SM, et al. Influence of
27. Goldfield GS, Baur LA, Hardy LL, et al. Video game ONE 2012;7:e30253. limit-setting and participation in physical activity on
playing is independently associated with blood 47. Kondo Y, Tanabe T, Kobayashi-Miura M, et al. youth screen time. Pediatrics 2010;126:e89–96.
pressure and lipids in overweight and obese Association between feeling upon awakening and 68. Ramirez ER, Norman GJ, Rosenberg DE, et al.
adolescents. PLoS ONE 2011;6:e26643. use of information technology devices in Japanese Adolescent screen time and rules to limit screen time
children. J Epidemiol 2012;22:12–20. in the home. J Adolesc Health 2011;48:379–85.

Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0 7


Leading article

69. Jago R, Fox KR, Page AS. Parent and child physical 74. Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, et al. 78. Ape versus machine: Do primates enjoy computer
activity and sedentary time: do active parents The impact of background television on games? BBC Nature, 8 February 2012. http://www.
foster active children? BMC Public Health parent–child interaction. Child Dev bbc.co.uk/nature/16832378 (accessed 8 Jul 2012).
2010;10:194. 2009;80:1350–9. 79. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ. Media as a public
70. Nintendo of America. Nintendo 3DS—Parents’ 75. Lapierre M, Piotrowski M, Linebarger D. Background health sssue. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
Information, 3D Concerns. 2012. http://www.nintendo. television in the homes of American children. Paper 2006;160:445–6.
com/consumer/systems/3ds/en_na/parents.jsp presented at the International Communication 80. Mark AE, Janssen I. Relationship between screen
71. Campos B, Graesch AP, Repetti R, et al. Opportunity Association’s Annual Conference; 24–28 May 2012, time and metabolic syndrome in adolescents.
for interaction? A naturalistic observation study of Phoenix, AZ, USA. J Public Health 2008;30:153–60.
dual-earner families after work and school. J Fam 76. Bauld L. The impact of smokefree legislation in 81. Dunstan DW, Barr ELM, Healy GN, et al. Television
Psychol 2009;23:798–807. England: evidence review. University of Bath, 2011. view- ing time and mortality. The Australian Diabetes,
72. Paavonen EJ, Pennonen M, Roine M, et al. TV Central Office of Information. http://www.dh.gov.uk/ Obesity and Lifestyle Study (AusDiab). Circulation
exposure associated with sleep disturbances in en/Publicationsandstatistics/Publications/ 2010;121:384–91.
5-to 6-year-old children. J Sleep Res PublicationsPolicyAndGuidance/DH_124961 82. Paavonen EJ, Roine M, Korhonen P, et al. Media and
2006;15:154–61. (accessed 2 Aug 2012). children’s well-being. Duodecim 2011;127:1563–70.
73. Schmidt ME, Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, et al. The 77. The Tulane National Primate Research Center 83. Wijndaele K, Brage S, Besson H, et al. Television
effects of background television on the toy play How Smart are Monkeys? Tulane University. 2006. viewing time independently predicts all-cause and
behavior of very young children. Child Dev http://www.tnprc.tulane.edu/public_faq.html#20 cardiovascular mortality: the EPIC Norfolk study. Int J
2008;79:1137–51. (accessed 8 Jul 2012). Epidemiol 2011;40:150–9.

8 Arch Dis Child Month 2012 Vol 0 No 0


View publication stats

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi