Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 16

Ó 2013 Federation of European Psychophysiology Societies

Article

Semantic Conflict Processing


in the Color-Word Stroop
and the Emotional Stroop
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Event-Related Potential (ERP) Correlates


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Montserrat Zurrón, Marta Ramos-Goicoa, and Fernando Díaz


Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Santiago de Compostela,
Galicia, Spain

Abstract. With the aim of establishing the temporal locus of the semantic conflict in color-word Stroop and emotional Stroop phenomena, we
analyzed the Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) elicited by nonwords, incongruent and congruent color words, colored words with positive and
negative emotional valence, and colored words with neutral valence. The incongruent, positive, negative, and neutral stimuli produced
interference in the behavioral response to the color of the stimuli. The P150/N170 amplitude was sensitive to the semantic equivalence of both
dimensions of the congruent color words. The P3b amplitude was smaller in response to incongruent color words and to positive, negative, and
neutral colored words than in response to the congruent color words and colored nonwords. There were no differences in the ERPs induced in
response to colored words with positive, negative, and neutral valence. Therefore, the P3b amplitude was sensitive to interference from the
semantic content of the incongruent, positive, negative, and neutral words in the color-response task, independently of the emotional content of
the colored words. In addition, the P3b amplitude was smaller in response to colored words with positive, negative, and neutral valence than in
response to the incongruent color words. Overall, these data indicate that the temporal locus of the semantic conflict generated by the
incongruent color words (in the color-word Stroop task) and by colored words with positive, negative, and neutral valence (in the emotional
Stroop task) appears to occur in the range 300–450 ms post-stimulus.

Keywords: color-word Stroop, emotional Stroop, temporal principal components analysis, Event-Related Potentials (ERPs), semantic conflict,
attention

The color-word Stroop effect or interference effect The hypothesis underlying studies that use the emotional
describes the slower response to the color in which incon- Stroop is that the semantic content of colored words with
gruent color words are printed (e.g., the word ‘‘blue’’ negative emotional valence preferentially captures the
printed in red) than to the color of colored nonwords (col- attention of the participants (because it provides an adaptive
ored Xs, colored symbols, or colored marks), or to the color advantage) and therefore should generate more interference
of congruent color words (e.g., the word ‘‘red’’ printed in than colored words with neutral or positive valence (see
red). The phenomenon was first described by J.R. Stroop MacLeod, 1991; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996).
in 1935 and was attributed to interference from the word The Parallel Distributed Processing model (PDP;
in the task of responding to the color. This is one of the Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990), which is widely
most robust phenomena in the field of Psychology (for a accepted by the scientific community, assumes that both
review, see MacLeod, 1991). dimensions of the stimuli (the meaning and the color of
As colored words with meanings unrelated to the color the word) are processed in parallel but with different
also caused interference (see MacLeod, 1991) and there strength, and that both lines of processing may come into
was interest in studying the attentional components of the conflict in one or more phases when the two dimensions
emotions, several researchers began to use colored words do not coincide, as with incongruent color words or when
with emotional valence in a task named the ‘‘emotional the colored word has emotional valence.
Stroop.’’ In such tasks, participants are required to respond With respect to the color-word Stroop, De Houwer
to the color of colored word stimuli with different emo- (2003) and Van Veen and Carter (2005) demonstrated that
tional valences, while ignoring the meaning of the word. the interference effect was generated by two conflicts, one

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000100
150 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

semantic and the other related to the response, thus integrat- components identified in this latency range. Several authors
ing to the two main hypotheses: the response conflict have identified (particularly at frontal electrodes) a deflec-
hypothesis (Dyer, 1973; Morton, 1969; Morton & Cham- tion denominated N450 or Ni or medial frontal negativity,
bers, 1973; Posner & Snyder, 1975) and the semantic con- although it is not usually negative (Chen, Bailey, Tiernan,
flict hypothesis (Luo, 1999; Seymour, 1977). The semantic & West, 2011; Kray, Eppinger, & Mecklinger, 2005;
conflict occurs between the unit that semantically processes McNeely, West, Christensen, & Alain, 2003; Rebai, Ber-
the word and the unit that semantically processes the color; nard, & Lannou, 1997; West, 2003, 2004; West & Alain,
the response conflict occurs between the unit that prepares 2000a). However, other authors have identified the P300
the response to the word and the unit that prepares the component (Ilan & Polich, 1999, Potter, Jory, Bassett,
response to the color of the incongruent stimuli. Barrett, & Mychalkiw, 2002), and yet others the P300 fol-
According to the PDP model, in the emotional Stroop lowed by the N450 (West & Alain, 2000b; West, Jakubek,
effect, the interference will be caused only by the semantic Wymbs, Perry, & Moore, 2005). Therefore, it is important
conflict (the emotional Stroop does not produce a response to clarify the component(s) identified in this latency range
conflict because the meaning of the words is not related to in studies involving color-response Stroop tasks.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the response); the greater interference from colored words With regard to the emotional Stroop, several ERP stud-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with negative emotional valence is derived from the fact ies have been carried out with healthy adult subjects
that negative emotional semantic content will be processed (Franken, Gootjes, & van Serien, 2009; Frhholz,
with greater strength than positive or neutral emotional Jellinghaus, & Herrmann, 2011; Gootjes, Coppens, Zwaan,
content. Franken, & van Strien, 2011; Li, Zinbarg, & Paller, 2007;
The Stroop test is widely used in neuropsychological McNeely, Lau, Christensen, & Alain, 2008; Sass et al.,
evaluation to determine the capacity of a person to direct 2010; Stewart et al., 2010; Taake, Jaspers-Fayer, & Liotti,
their attention toward relevant information, while inhibiting 2009; Thomas, Johnstone, & Gonsalvez, 2007; van Hooff,
irrelevant information. The time between presentation of Dietz, Sharma, & Bowman, 2008). Negative and neutral
the stimulus and the behavioral response is measured in colored words were presented in these studies, and positive
the test. The response times are measures of the speed of colored words were also included in some of the studies.
final processing and although they have been demonstrated Nevertheless, the semantic conflict involving colored words
to be of great use, techniques are now available that provide with positive, negative, and neutral valence has not been
more specific indices of the temporal course of the conflict considered. To study this conflict, the ERP elicited by posi-
processes that the Stroop task generates. Therefore, in sev- tive, negative, and neutral colored words must be compared
eral studies, Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) have been with those elicited by a condition in which the stimuli are
recorded in response to Stroop-type color word stimuli. colored nonwords. However, colored nonwords have not
ERPs provide information about the neural responses that been evaluated.
take place during information processing with a temporal The objective of these studies was to determine whether
resolution of milliseconds, which enables researchers to fol- ERP data provide evidence for preferential neural process-
low the temporal course of processing. Therefore, ERPs ing of colored words with negative emotional valence over
have helped to identify the temporal locus of the semantic the positive and neutral colored words. However, the results
conflict generated in the Stroop test. for the ERP data from emotional Stroop differ widely and
Specifically, in a recent study (Zurrn, Pouso, Lindn, are confusing as regards both identification of the ERP
Galdo, & Daz, 2009), ERP data elicited by congruent components and their topographical distribution, mainly
and incongruent color words were recorded in a task in for components in the temporal window between 300 and
which the participants had to judge the congruence/incon- 500 ms. Thus, a positive parietal wave (P290) and a nega-
gruence of color word stimuli (by stating whether or not tive going wave (N450) (which was not always negative)
the chromatic and semantic dimensions of the stimuli have been identified at fronto-central sites (and with a
matched each other, which minimizes the conflict between wider distribution) in this temporal window, as have the
responses). A temporal Principal Components Analysis LPP (Late Positive Potential) component and the P3 com-
(tPCA) was applied to the ERP data, and it was found that ponent (Franken et al., 2009; Frhholz et al., 2011; Gootjes
the amplitude of the P300 component (P3b), in the 300– et al., 2011; Li et al., 2007; McNeely et al., 2008; Sass
450 ms interval, was sensitive to the congruence/incongru- et al., 2010; Stewart et al., 2010; Taake et al., 2009;
ence of the stimuli. As the difference between the two stim- Thomas et al., 2007; van Hooff et al., 2008).
uli was that one (incongruent) generated a semantic conflict In the present study, colored nonwords, congruent and
and the other (congruent) did not, it was concluded that the incongruent color words, and positive, negative, and neutral
temporal locus of the semantic conflict may be in the 300– colored words were presented, and the participants had to
450 ms time window, in which the P300 component was respond to the color of the stimuli. The Reaction Time
identified. (RT), the number of errors, and the ERPs were recorded
In tasks in which participants were asked to respond to for each of the six types of stimuli. In order to determine
the color of the stimuli, the ERP amplitudes in the 300– the characteristic components in the waveforms obtained
450 ms interval were also found to differ between congru- for the six types of stimuli, the ERP data were analyzed
ent and incongruent color word stimuli, also indicating that by tPCA.
the semantic conflict may occur in this time window, The objectives of this study in relation to the color-word
although there is some controversy concerning the ERP Stroop were as follows: (1) to determine which ERP

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 151

component(s) are sensitive to the semantic conflict gener- were also mixed to prevent a reading strategy in the former,
ated by the incongruent color words, and (2) to confirm that to ensure that the subject responded to the color of the
the temporal locus of this conflict may occur between 300 printed words. In the third part, colored words with positive
and 450 ms, as found in previous studies. The objectives of emotional valence (e.g., the word tesoro [treasure] printed
the study in relation to the emotional Stroop included the in blue), negative emotional valence (e.g., the word veneno
following: (3) to determine which ERP component(s) are [poison] printed in blue), and neutral valence (e.g., the word
sensitive to the semantic conflict generated by the positive, repisa [ledge] printed in blue), were presented in three sep-
negative, and neutral colored words, (4) to identify the tem- arate blocks to avoid a carry-over effect: participants take
poral locus of the semantic conflict generated by the three longer to respond to stimuli that are preceded by stimuli
types of colored words, and (5) to test whether the behav- with negative valence (McKenna & Sharma, 2004; Waters,
ioral and/or ERP data indicate preferential processing of Sayette, Franken, & Schwartz, 2005). Each block included
negative colored words relative to the positive and/or neu- 60 stimuli (20 of each color). The three blocks of colored
tral words in tasks with response to the color of the words (positive, negative, and neutral) were counterbal-
stimulus. anced among participants. In all three parts of the session
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(involving presentation of nonwords, congruent/incongru-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ent color words, and positive/negative/neutral colored


words), the order of presentation of the stimuli was
Experiment I pseudorandom, so that more than three consecutive stimuli
of the same color were never shown.
Material and Method The participants were asked to press a different button
of a response box, depending on the color of the stimulus
Participants (red, blue, or green), with the fingers (index, middle, and
ring) of their dominant hand, as quickly and accurately as
Twenty-six university students (19 female) of mean age possible; participants were also asked to ignore the meaning
20.12 years (SD = 2.48) participated as volunteers in the of any words presented. The electroencephalographic
study. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (but (EEG) activity, reaction times (RTs), and the number of
not contact lenses). All were healthy, with no history of errors were recorded throughout the session.
neurological or psychiatric disorders or drug abuse and In order to ensure that the participants understood the
were not taking any medication at the time of participating instructions and responded correctly, and to familiarize
in the study. The participants were also required to abstain them with the task, a training block of 30 colored nonword
from drugs, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine prior to testing, stimuli was presented before the first part of the experimen-
and none reported fatigue caused by lack of sleep. All par- tal session; the behavioral data and electroencephalographic
ticipants gave their informed consent prior to taking part in data for these 30 stimuli were not recorded.
the study. Twenty-four participants were self-reported We presented the colored nonword stimuli first, fol-
right-handed and two were left-handed. lowed by the congruent/incongruent color words and finally
the colored words with emotional valence, because it has
been found that in the task of responding to the color, the
Procedure RT to incongruent stimuli decreases with practice, due to
less interference from the semantic processing of the word
The participants took part in a single experimental session (Davidson, Zacks, & Williams, 2003; Dulaney & Rogers,
divided in three parts, with brief rest periods between each. 1992; Edwards, Brice, Craig, & Penri-Jones, 1996; Fein-
In the first part of the session, 60 colored nonwords (con- stein, Brown, & Ron, 1994; MacLeod, 1998; Stroop,
sisting of four letters ‘‘x’’ -xxxx-, with 20 stimuli for each 1935). Thus, the order of presentation would interfere with
of three colors: red, green, or blue) were presented. These the objectives of the present study. Rest intervals were pro-
nonword stimuli were presented first and separately from gramed between each of the three parts of the session with
the other stimuli to ensure that they were processed only the aim of avoiding any possible effects of repetition of the
as chromatic stimuli and were not influenced by semantic stimuli on the amplitude of P3b, as in oddball tasks of long
expectations (in most studies involving the Stroop task, a duration the amplitude of P3b tends to decrease throughout
chain of letters ‘‘x’’ is the nonword stimulus most com- the session, but brief periods of rest between blocks have
monly used and is presented first). In the second part, 60 been shown to enable maintenance of the values of the
congruent color words were presented (20 examples of amplitude of P3b at initial levels (Lindn, Zurrn, & Daz,
the word rojo [red] printed in red, 20 of the word verde 2004).
[green] printed in green, and 20 of the word azul [blue] The stimuli were designed with the Stim system (ver-
printed in blue), mixed with another 60 incongruent color sion 3.0.15) from Neuroscan. The images were presented
words (20 examples of the word rojo [red] printed in green on a 1900 screen placed at a distance of 1 m from the partic-
or blue, 20 of the word verde [green] printed in red or blue, ipants. The words were presented in the center of the
and 20 of the word azul [blue] printed in red or green). screen, with lowercase Rom 1 letter style available in the
More than three consecutive words belonging to the same Stim software (e.g., the ‘‘x’’ letter was 2.5 cm high and
category (congruent or incongruent) were never shown in 2.4 cm wide). The background of the screen was always
this part of the task. Congruent and incongruent color words black, even during the interval between stimuli. Each

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


152 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

stimulus was displayed for 250 ms, with an interstimuli Identification of the ERP Components
interval (ISI) of 2,500 ms.
The positive, negative, and neutral words (see The ERP data were analyzed by tPCA to ensure correct
Appendix) were selected from the version of the Affective identification of the ERP components. Temporal PCA pro-
Norms for English Words (ANEW; Bradley & Lang, 1999) vides a statistical decomposition of the brain electrical pat-
adapted for the Spanish population (Redondo, Fraga, terns superimposed on the scalp-recorded data (Dien &
ComesaÇa, & Padrn, 2007). Three lists of 30 different Frishkoff, 2004). The waveforms obtained in response to
words were elaborated for each category (each list was pre- the six types of stimuli (Figure 1) were inspected visually
sented twice to produce the 60 stimuli corresponding to the and found to be similar. A covariance-matrix-based tPCA
three types of color words). The words selected are nouns, was then used for the six conditions. The decision regarding
verbs, and adjectives between four and six letters long, and selection of the numbers of components (or factors) was
all are common words in Spanish (the frequency and famil- based on the scree test (Cattel, 1966). Extracted factors
iarity values were obtained from the LEXESP-CORCO were then subjected to Promax rotation. Dien (1998)
database; Sebastin-Galls, Mart, Cuetos, & Carreiras, showed that the use of Promax rotation improves the accu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2000). According to the data provided by Redondo et al. racy of the results and reduces problems such as misalloca-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(2007), the words with positive and negative emotional tion of variance (see also Dien & Frishkoff, 2004).
valence had a high arousal value (positive: mean = 6.6; Temporal PCA provides two matrices: one for factor load-
negative: mean = 6.8), a mean emotional valence of 7.4 ings and another for factor scores. The first shows the load-
for positive words and of 1.9 for negative words. The neu- ing of each factor over time, and the second provides
tral words elicited little arousal (mean = 4.3) and interme- information about the extent to which each factor is present
diate valence (mean = 4.9), indicating that they lack in the averaged ERPs (i.e., at each electrode site). The fac-
emotional content. The colors were assigned at random to tor scores are transformed values of the original voltages
words with positive, negative, and neutral valence. and can therefore be used as a measure of the amplitude.
A positive factor score indicates a voltage higher than
0 lV, so that the higher the score, the larger the amplitude;
EEG Recordings a negative factor score indicates a negative voltage.
The tPCA identified six temporal factors (TF), which
The participants were seated in a comfortable chair in an elec- explain 87.43% of the total variance (Figure 2). Taking into
trically shielded laboratory with attenuated sound and light- account the temporal range of the factor loadings and
ing levels, and they were instructed not to move during the the factor scores at the different electrodes, TF 3, 4, 5,
recording sessions and to look at the center of the screen. and 6 corresponded to ERP components, whereas TF 1
Each participant was fitted with a cap with nose reference and 2 (with temporal range of the factor loadings above
and frontopolar ground, to record the EEG activity at 30 elec- 700 ms) did not correspond to any identifiable component
trode sites corresponding to the 10–20 system (with the Neu- in the ERP waveforms and were not taken into account
roScan 4.1 EEG recording system). Eye movements were in the subsequent analysis. TF 3, 4, 5, and 6 corresponded
recorded simultaneously by additional electrodes placed to ERP components as follows: TF6 corresponded to the
above and below the left eye (VEOG) and at the outer canthus frontal P150/occipital N170 complex, which is character-
of each eye (HEOG). All impedances were reduced to 5 kX ized as a positive wave with maximum amplitude at frontal
or less to obtain good quality recordings, and care was taken electrodes and a negative wave at occipital electrodes, with
to avoid damaging the subject’s scalp. peak latencies within the 120–190 ms window (Joyce &
The EEG signals were passed through a 0.1–30 Hz Rossion, 2005 demonstrated that both waves comprised a
band-pass filter and amplified at 500 K, and the digitalized single component); TF5 corresponded to the negative ante-
signal was stored. Pre-stimulus (200 ms) and post-stimulus rior N2 component, clearly identifiable at fronto-central
(1,400 ms) segments were extracted from the EEG and the electrodes with maximum amplitude at Fz and peak latency
pre-stimulus interval was defined as the baseline. within 230–300 ms; TF4 corresponded to the positive pari-
Signals exceeding €100 lV were automatically etal P3b (P300 or P3) component within 300 and 450 ms;
excluded from the averages and only the epochs corre- and finally, TF3 corresponded to the centro-parietal Posi-
sponding to the stimuli with correct responses were taken tive Slow Wave (PSW), with maximum amplitude at Pz
into account. Ocular artifacts were corrected separately by and peak latency within 450–580 ms following P3b.
an off-line analysis with the algorithm of Gratton, Coles,
and Donchin (1983). The ERP waveforms of participants
were obtained by averaging 50–55 epochs for each type Statistical Analysis
of stimulus (average epochs for each stimulus: Xs = 53.1;
congruent = 51.8; incongruent = 51.0; positive = 54.3; Color-Word Stroop
negative = 52.5; neutral = 51.4). There were no differences
in the number of averaged epochs between the different Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVAs), in
types of stimuli. Eight of the twenty-six participants were which the stimuli were considered as the within-subject fac-
excluded from the study because of an insufficient number tor (colored nonwords, congruent color words, and incon-
of epochs, and therefore the final ERP sample comprised 18 gruent color words), was used to compare the RT and the
participants. number of errors.

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 153
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Temporal Factor (TF) loadings from the


temporal Principal Components Analysis of the ERP data
from Experiment I.

anterior N2, P3b, and PSW, or four levels: Fz, Cz, Pz,
and Oz, for the P150/N170 complex).
The peak latencies of the ERP components were mea-
sured manually at the electrode with the largest amplitude
(frontal P150 at Fz, occipital N170 at Oz, P3b at Pz, ante-
rior N2 at Fz, and PSW at Pz) in the latency ranges indi-
cated in the previous section, ‘‘Identification of the ERP
components.’’ The peak latencies were analyzed by
repeated measures ANOVAs, with one within-subject fac-
tor: stimulus (with three levels).

Emotional Stroop

The behavioral data were analyzed by repeated measures


ANOVAs with four levels of the stimulus factor (nonwords
and colored words with positive, negative, and neutral
valence), for reaction time and number of errors.
The temporal factor scores were subjected to repeated
measures ANOVAs with two within-subject factors: stimulus
(four levels: nonwords and colored words with positive, neg-
ative, and neutral valence) and electrode (with three or four
levels); the peak latencies of the ERP components were exam-
ined by ANOVAS with four levels of the stimulus factor.
When the ANOVA revealed significant differences,
pairwise comparison of means was carried out to identify
the source of the differences (Bonferroni’s correction was
applied). Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied to
the degrees of freedom in all cases in which the condition
of sphericity was not met. In these cases, the original
degrees of freedom are shown together with the correct p
Figure 1. ERP waveforms elicited by nonwords (pink), and epsilon (e) values. Differences were considered signif-
congruent color words (black), incongruent color words icant at p  .05. All statistical analyses were carried out
(red), and colored words with positive (green), negative with SPSS (version 15.0)
(blue), and neutral (brown) valence in Experiment I.
x-Axis: time in ms; y-axis: microvolts. Waveforms (up to Results
down): Fz, Cz, Pz, and Oz
Color-Word Stroop
The temporal factor scores were examined by ANOVAs
with two within-subject factors: stimulus (with three levels) Behavioral data (see Table 1). As expected, the RTs to the
and electrode (with three levels: Fz, Cz, and Pz, for the different stimuli differed significantly, F(2, 48) = 36.2,

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


154 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

Table 1. Mean (and standard deviation) reaction times (ms) and number of errors corresponding to the following types of
stimuli: colored nonwords (XXXX), congruent color words, incongruent color words, colored words with
positive, negative, and neutral emotional valence in Experiment I
Nonwords Congruent Incongruent Positive Negative Neutral
Reaction time 499 (83) 551 (107) 621 (132) 560 (102) 549 (99) 542 (114)
Number of errors 2.6 (2.5) 1.9 (1.65) 3.7 (2.9) 1.6 (2.0) 3.7 (1.7) 2.1 (2.1)

p < .001, e = 0.678; specifically, the RTs were signifi- scores were significantly larger at the Fz electrode than at
cantly longer in response to incongruent color words than the Cz, Pz, and Oz electrodes, and larger at the Cz and
in response to congruent color words and nonwords, and Pz electrodes than at the Oz electrode.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

they were significantly longer to congruent than to nonword The TF5 scores (anterior N2) did not differ significantly
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

stimuli (p < .001 for all paired comparisons). for the different stimuli or electrodes, and it there wasn’t
The number of errors also differed significantly, interaction effect.
F(2, 50) = 10.1, p < .001; specifically, significantly more The TF4 scores (P3b) were significantly larger for the
errors were obtained in response to the incongruent color congruent and nonword stimuli than for the incongruent stim-
words than in response to the congruent (p < .001) and to uli, and were significantly larger at the Pz electrode than at
the nonword stimuli (p < .01). the Cz and Fz electrodes, and larger at Cz than at Fz. Given
Therefore, the incongruent color words generated inter- that the P3b amplitude to the incongruent words was signifi-
ference in the response to the color of the stimuli. cantly different than to congruent words and nonwords, it
Event-Related Potential data (see Table 2 and Figures appears that the P3b amplitude was sensitive to interference
1, 3–5). The TF6 scores (frontal P150/occipital N170) were from incongruent color words in the response to color.
significantly larger for the congruent color word stimuli The TF3 scores (PSW) were significantly larger for
than for the incongruent color word and for the nonword nonwords than for congruent and incongruent stimuli, and
stimuli. There were no significant differences between the they were significantly larger at the Pz electrode than at
scores for incongruent stimuli and the nonwords. The Cz and Fz electrodes, and larger at Cz than at Fz.

Table 2. F values for the repeated measures ANOVAs for the scores of TF6 (P150/N170), TF5 (anterior N2), TF4 (P3b),
and TF3 (PSW) obtained for: congruent (C) and incongruent (I) color words, and colored nonwords (X)
Factor F (df) Paired comparison
TF6 (P150/N170) Stimulus 5.1 (2, 34)* e = 0.743 C > I***
C > X*
Electrode 19.3 (3, 51)*** e = 0.6 Fz > Cz**
Fz > Pz*
Fz > Oz***
Cz, Pz > Oz***
Interaction 3.4 (6, 102)* e = 0.4 Fz: C > I*
Cz: C > I**
Pz: C > I**
Oz: C > I, X***
TF5 (anterior N2) Stimulus 1.1 (2, 34)
Electrode 2.9 (2, 34) e = 0.6
Interaction 1.3 (4, 68) e = 0.7
TF4 (P3b) Stimulus 6.2 (2, 34)* e = 0.7 C, X > I**
Electrode 63.7 (2, 34)*** Pz > Cz, Fz***
Cz > Fz***
Interaction 1.1 (4, 68) e = 0.6
TF3 (PSW) Stimulus 4.0 (2, 34)* X > C, I*
Electrode 28.4 (2, 34)*** e = 0.7 Pz > Cz, Fz***
Cz > Fz***
Interaction 2.4 (4, 68)
Notes. The degrees of freedom (df) are shown in parentheses. e = epsilon value. *p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001.

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 155

Table 3. F values for the repeated measures ANOVAs for the scores of TF6 (P150/N170), TF5 (anterior N2), TF4 (P3b),
and TF3 (PSW) obtained for: words with positive (P), negative (Ng), and neutral (N) emotional valence, and
colored nonwords (X)
Factor F (df) Paired comparison
TF6 Stimulus 0.2 (3, 51)
(P150/N170) Electrode 16.0 (3, 51)*** e = 0.5 Fz > Cz**
Fz > Oz***
Cz > Oz**
Pz > Oz***
Interaction 3.2 (9, 153)** e = 0.5 Not significant
TF5 Stimulus 1.2 (3, 51)
(anterior N2) Electrode 2.2 (2, 34) e = 0.6
Interaction 2.1 (6, 102) e = 0.6
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

TF4 Stimulus 14.5 (3, 51)*** X > P, Ng, N***


(P3b) Electrode 44.6 (2, 34)*** Pz > Cz**
Pz > Fz***
Cz > Fz**
Interaction 7.7 (6, 102)*** Fz: X > P **; X > Ng*; X > N***
Cz: X > P, Ng, N***
Pz: X > P, Ng, N***
TF3 Stimulus 1.4 (3, 51)
(PSW) Electrode 28.7 (2, 34)*** Pz > Fz, Cz**
Cz > Fz***
Interaction 0.2 (6, 102) e = 0.6
Notes. The degrees of freedom (df) are shown in parentheses. e = epsilon value. *p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001.

Finally, no significant differences were observed The TF4 scores (P3b) were significantly larger for non-
between stimuli in relation to the peak latency of the com- words than for colored words with positive, negative, and
ponents studied. neutral valence, and were significantly larger at Pz than
at Cz and Fz, and at Cz than at Fz. Thus, P3b was sensitive
to interference from positive, negative, and neutral words in
Emotional Stroop the color-response task. By contrast, there were no signifi-
cant differences between the scores for the three types of
Behavioral data (see Table 1). The stimulus factor had a colored words with valence (positive, negative, and
significant effect on the RTs, F(3, 72) = 10.3, p < .001, neutral).
e = 0.649: the participants’ RTs were significantly longer The TF3 scores (PSW) were significantly larger at the
in response to stimuli with positive (p < .01), negative Pz electrode than at Cz and Fz, and larger at Cz than at Fz.
(p < .01), and neutral valence (p < .05) than in response No significant differences were observed in relation to
to nonwords, as expected. Therefore, the positive, negative, the peak latencies of the components studied.
and neutral words generated interference in the response to
color.
There were also significant differences in the number of Spatial PCA on TF4
errors between stimuli, F(3, 72) = 8.9; p < .001: signifi-
cantly more errors were produced in response to stimuli Given that TF4 (P3b) was sensitive to interference from the
with negative emotional valence than in response to those incongruent color words and to interference from the posi-
with positive and neutral valence (p < .001 for both pairs tive, negative, and neutral colored words, a spatial PCA
of comparisons). Thus, the number of errors was sensitive (sPCA) was carried out for TF4, because ERPs often differ
to the negative relative to the positive and neutral content topographically in relation to the experimental conditions.
in the color-response task. The sPCA provides a reliable division of the scalp into
Event-related potentials (see Table 3, and Figures 1, 3, different recording regions, and each region or Spatial Fac-
and 5). The TF6 scores (frontal P150/occipital N170) were tor (SF) is formed with the scalp points where recordings
significantly larger at the Fz electrode than at Cz and Oz, tend to covary. Also in this case, selection of the number
and larger at the Cz and Pz electrodes than at Oz. of SFs was based on the scree test, and extracted factors
The TF5 scores (anterior N2) did not differ significantly were submitted to promax rotation. Each SF can be quanti-
for the different stimuli or electrodes, and it there wasn’t fied through the spatial factor score, a single parameter that
interaction effect. reflects the amplitude of the whole spatial factor. The sPCA

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


156 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 3. Voltage maps for each of the ERP components in each task condition in Experiment I.

extracted four SFs that explained 90% of the variance. The significantly in relation to type of stimulus (SF1:
SF1 was parietal, the SF2 frontal, the SF3 central, and SF4 F(2, 34) = 5.46, p < .05, e = 0.74; SF2: F(2, 34) = 3.5,
left frontal-temporal. p < .05; SF3: F(2, 34) = 4, p < .05; SF4: F(2, 34) =
In accordance with previous results, in the color-word 8.53, p < .01, e = 0.67). The pairwise comparisons showed
Stroop, the factor scores for four SFs of the TF4 differed that the scores were significantly higher for nonwords than

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 157
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 5. Mean scores for TF4 (P3b) at Pz for the


Figure 4. Mean scores for TF6 at Fz (P150) and Oz nonword stimuli (XS), congruent color word stimuli (CS),
(N170) for nonword stimuli (XS), congruent color word incongruent color word stimuli (IS), and colored word
stimuli (CS), and incongruent color word stimuli (IS) in stimuli with emotional valence (EVS; positive, negative,
Experiment I. and neutral) in Experiment I.

for the incongruent stimuli (p < .05 in all four SF), and they neutral valence than in response to incongruent and congru-
were also significantly higher for the congruent than for the ent color words, so that the differences observed in the first
incongruent stimuli (p < .05 in SF4). experiment could not be attributed to the order of presenta-
As expected, in the emotional Stroop, the four SFs of tion of the stimuli.
the TF4 also differed significantly with respect to the type
of stimulus (SF1: F(3, 51) = 6.6, p < .01, e = 0.33; SF2:
F(3, 51) = 9.5, p < .001; SF3: F(3, 51) = 11.2, p < .001; Material and Method
SF4: F(3, 51) = 10.84, p < .001). Specifically, in the four
SFs, the factor scores were significantly higher for the non- Participants
words than for the positive, negative, and neutral colored
words (p < .05 for all paired comparisons). No significant Sixteen university students of mean age 20.9 years
differences were found between the positive, negative, (SD = 3.83) participated as volunteers in the study. The
and neutral words in any of the four spatial factors of new participants met the same criteria as described for
TF4. the first experiment.

Comparison of the TF4 (P3b) Scores Obtained for the


Procedure
Positive/Negative/Neutral Colored Words With the
Scores Obtained for the Congruent and Incongruent
Two blocks of 150 stimuli were presented (30 congruent
Color Words (Figure 5)
color words, 30 incongruent color words, 30 colored words
with positive emotional valence, 30 colored words with
Given that the TF4 scores did not differ significantly for the
negative emotional valence, and 30 neutral color words).
positive, negative, and neutral color words, the mean values
The order of presentation of the stimuli was pseudorandom,
for the three types of color words were calculated at Pz, and
as stimuli of the same color or of the same type (incongru-
these mean values were compared with the values for con-
ent, congruent, positive, negative, and neutral) never
gruent and incongruent color words, by paired t tests: the
appeared more than three times in succession. The charac-
TF4 scores (P3b) for the positive, negative, and neutral col-
teristics of the stimuli were the same as in the first experi-
ored words were significantly smaller than for congruent
ment, and the participants were also required to respond to
color words (t(17) = 5.23, p < .001) and for incongruent
the color of the stimuli.
color words (t(17) = 3.87, p < .01).
As in the previous experiments, 30 practice stimuli (col-
ored ‘‘xxxx’’) were presented before the blocks of test stim-
uli to familiarize the participants with the task.

Experiment II
EEG Recordings
A second experiment was carried out to confirm that the
amplitude of the P300 (P3b) component was smaller in The recording characteristics were the same as in the first
response to colored words with positive, negative, and experiment.

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


158 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

to the incongruent color word stimuli than in response to


the nonwords and the congruent color words. In the emo-
tional Stroop, the reaction times to positive, negative, and
neutral stimuli were significantly longer than the reaction
times to nonwords. The incongruent words and the positive,
negative, and neutral words interfered in the behavioral
response to color.
No facilitation effects were observed. The facilitation
effect, which occurs when the two dimensions of a color
word stimulus coincide and are therefore easier to process,
consists of shorter reaction times to congruent color words
than to colored stimuli (nonwords). The facilitation effect is
less robust than the interference effect and depends on sev-
eral variables. In this respect, MacLeod (1991) reported the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Figure 6. ERP waveforms at Pz elicited by congruent repercussions of presenting congruent and incongruent
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

color words (black), incongruent color words (red), and color words in the same block or in different blocks. When
colored words with positive (green), negative (blue), and congruent color words are presented alone in one block, the
neutral (brown) valence in Experiment II. x-Axis: time in participants may adopt a strategy of reading the words,
ms; y-axis: microvolts. which decreases the reaction times in response to these
stimuli, thus favoring facilitation. In the present study, the
congruent and incongruent color words were presented
Identification of the ERP Components pseudorandomly in the same block, with the same probabil-
ity of appearance. This precluded a reading strategy, as
Only the peak amplitude of P3b at the Pz electrode site was attention was divided between the two dimensions of the
measured in this experiment. The component was identified stimuli: color and meaning of the words (MacLeod,
between 300 and 450 ms post-stimulus. 1991), and explains the absence of a facilitation effect
and the longer reaction time in response to congruent stim-
uli than in response to nonwords.
Statistical Analysis There were no differences in the reaction times to the
three types of stimuli with valence: positive, negative, and
The P3b amplitude was examined by repeated measures neutral. A longer RT to color words with negative valence
ANOVAs with the stimulus as the within-subject factor than for color words with neutral valence is characteristic
(congruent color words, incongruent color words, and posi- of subjects with different clinical disorders and of highly
tive, negative, and neutral colored words). anxious, but otherwise healthy, individuals (see Bair-
Haim, Lami, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van
IJzendoorn, 2007), but is not commonly observed in healthy
Results subjects with low or intermediate levels of anxiety (Phaf &
Kan, 2007). Sharma and McKenna (2001) indicated that
The results of Experiment II (see Figure 6) were consistent when the number of stimuli per time unit increases, negative
with those of Experiment I. The stimulus factor had a sig- aspects of the stimulus have been found to receive greater
nificant effect on the P3b amplitude, F(4, 60) = 9.6; weighting. Thus, it is possible that in healthy participants,
p < .001, e = 0.516: the amplitude was significantly larger the interval between stimuli is important for finding differ-
for congruent and incongruent color words than for colored ences in RT to color words with different emotional valence.
words with positive, negative, and neutral valence (p < .01, In this respect, reaction times to stimuli with negative emo-
for each pair of comparisons), and there were no significant tional valence were longer than in response to stimuli with
differences between the amplitudes for the three types of neutral valence with an ISI between approximately 1,000
colored words with emotional valence. and 1,150 ms (Gootjes et al., 2011), and with an ISI of
approximately 900 ms (40 ms between the response and pre-
sentation of the next stimulus, van Hooff et al., 2008),
although this was not observed when the ISI were longer,
approximately 1,300 ms (van Hooff et al., 2008, 500 ms
Discussion between the response and the following stimulus). In tasks
in which ISIs > 1,300 ms were used (i.e., Frhholz et al.,
Behavioral Data: Color-Word and Emotional 2011, with an ISI of 1,500 ms, McNeely et al., 2008, with
Stroop an ISI of 3,000 ms, Sass et al., 2010, with an ISI of
2,000 € 225 ms, and in the present study, with an ISI of
In the color-word Stroop, the reaction times were signifi- 2,500 ms), the emotional valence was not observed to have
cantly longer in response to incongruent color words than any effect on the RT.
in response to nonwords and congruent color words. More errors were produced in response to colored words
Furthermore, the participants made more errors in response with negative valence than in response to those with

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 159

positive and neutral valence, and therefore this variable was (Ciesielski et al., 2011; Silton et al., 2010; West et al.,
found to be more sensitive to the negative emotional con- 2005), and in the emotional Stroop (Stewart et al., 2010;
tent than the RT, which is the measure traditionally used Thomas et al., 2007).
in studies involving the emotional Stroop task. The results The Stroop task is a cognitive control task, and in these
for the number of errors appear to indicate that words with tasks the anterior N2 amplitude has been associated with
negative emotional valence are processed differently than the conflict between responses, that is, the conflict between
positive and neutral words. different representations of responses to a stimulus
(Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Folstein
& van Petten, 2008; Van Veen & Carter, 2002). In the
color-word Stroop, the incongruent stimuli generated con-
ERP Data: Color-Word Stroop and Emotional flict between responses, but the anterior N2 was not sensi-
Stroop tive to the conflict between responses. This is possibly
because, unlike in other cognitive control tasks, the conflict
In the color-word Stroop, the P3b amplitude was signifi- between responses in the color-word Stroop task is poster-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cantly smaller for incongruent stimuli than for congruent ior to the semantic conflict (two representations of different
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and nonword stimuli. Moreover, the P3b amplitude was sig- responses can only be generated if there is a semantic con-
nificantly smaller for positive, negative, and neutral colored flict stage). As the semantic conflict is associated with
words than for nonwords. The P3b amplitude for positive, P300, the conflict between responses in the color-word
negative, and neutral colored words was significantly smal- Stroop must be posterior to P300 and therefore to N2. In
ler than that for incongruent and congruent color words, the case of the emotional Stroop, there were no differences
and there were no differences in the amplitude between in amplitude between stimuli because the positive, nega-
the three types of stimuli with valence (positive, negative, tive, and neutral colored words did not generate any con-
and neutral). Thus, the temporal locus of the semantic con- flict between responses.
flict in both the incongruent color word and positive/nega- The component corresponding to TF4 is P3b (P300 or
tive/neutral color word may be within the P300 latency P3), as shown by the parietal distribution, positive polarity,
range. morphology, and latency between 300 and 450 ms. The
Following the temporal order of the ERP components, component identified in the ERP waveforms elicited by
significant differences were observed in the P150/N170 the color word stimuli between 300 and 450 ms post-
complex between congruent and incongruent stimuli, and stimulus is P3b.
between congruent stimuli and nonword stimuli. However, In the color-word Stroop used in the present study, the
no differences were observed between the incongruent and amplitude of the P3b component was smaller for incongru-
nonword stimuli; therefore, the P150/N170 complex is not ent color-word stimuli than for congruent and nonword
associated with interference from incongruent color words, stimuli, as also found in previous studies (Houston, Bauer,
and it appears that this complex may be sensitive to the & Hesselbrock, 2004; Ilan & Polich, 1999; Mager et al.,
semantic equivalence of both dimensions of the congruent 2007; Potter et al., 2002; West & Alain, 2000b; West
stimulus. This interpretation is consistent with studies that et al., 2005; Zurrn et al., 2009). Furthermore, the P3b
associate N170 with the lexical-semantic processing of amplitude for the colored words with positive, negative,
visually presented words (Hauk, Davis, Ford, Pulvermller, and neutral valence was also smaller than for the nonwords.
& Marslen-Wilson, 2006; Hauk & Pulvermller, 2004; Thus, the amplitude of P3b was sensitive to interference
Sereno & Rayner, 2003; Sereno, Rayner, & Posner, 1998). that incongruent words and positive/negative/neutral words
In the emotional Stroop, no differences in the P150/ generate in response to color.
N170 complex were observed between the different types The amplitude of the P3b for positive, negative, and neu-
of stimuli, which supports the above-mentioned interpreta- tral colored words was smaller than for incongruent color
tion as in the emotional Stroop no stimuli in which both words and congruent color words. In addition, there was a
dimensions were semantically equivalent were presented. gradation from larger to smaller amplitude of the P3b compo-
Furthermore, McNeely et al. (2008) did not observe any nent to the three types of color words: congruent, incongru-
differences in the amplitude of the posterior N170 compo- ent, and words with valence (positive, negative, and
nent between colored words with positive, negative, and neutral). The P300 amplitude decreases in double tasks
neutral valence, and Frhholz et al. (2011) did not find (Isreal, Wickens, Chesney, & Donchin, 1980; Kramer, Sire-
any difference in N170 between colored words with nega- vaag, & Braune, 1987; Sirevaag, Kramer, Coles, & Donchin,
tive and neutral valence. 1989; Wickens, Kramer, Vanasse, & Donchin, 1983), and
In the present study, for both the color-word Stroop and therefore the gradation from larger to smaller amplitude
the emotional Stroop, no significant differences were observed in the present study may be related to the processing
observed in the amplitude of the anterior N2 component demands of the secondary task generated in the color-
(corresponding to TF5) for the different stimuli, which is response version of the Stroop task. In this type of Stroop
consistent with the findings of West et al. (2005) and task, the participants carried out the target task, to respond
Thomas et al. (2007). The anterior N2 component (fron- to the color of the printed word, and a second task, to read
to-central negative component at around 200–300 ms) the word. The target task was the same for congruent and
was previously identified in the numerical Stroop (West, incongruent color words and for positive, negative, and
Bowry, & McConville, 2004), in the color-word Stroop neutral colored words. However, the second task generates

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


160 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

a semantic conflict that is very different for congruent and word, but when they focused their attention on another attri-
incongruent words and words with valence (positive, nega- bute of the word, the results were much more variable. In
tive, and neutral). the emotional Stroop, the attention of the subject is directed
Congruent words do not generate conflict, as the seman- toward the color of the stimulus, which may explain the
tic codes that are activated (one by the word and the other results of the present study.
by the color) are the same. Incongruent stimuli and those In this study, the insensitivity of the ERP components to
with positive, negative, and neutral valence both generate the emotional content of the colored words contrasts with
a semantic conflict, and whereas for the incongruent stimuli the results obtained for the number of errors, which indicate
the participants had to access two different semantic codes that the negative colored words may receive some type of
belonging to the same semantic category (color indicated preferential processing with respect to positive and neutral
by the word and the color in which the word was printed), words. Preferential processing of negative words may be
for positive, negative, and neutral colored words they had to confined to the ERP components related to the response
access two semantic codes belonging to different categories rather than those related to perception and evaluation of
(meaning of the word and meaning of the color in which it the colored-word stimuli.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

was printed). Moreover, the meanings of the words with No differences in the latency of P3b were found in rela-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

positive, negative, and neutral valence were very diverse, tion to the different stimuli, which is consistent with the
as 30 different words of each type were included. Thus, results of other studies (Atkinson, Drysdale, & Fulham
the semantic processing load of the words with positive, 2003; Duncan-Jonhson & Kopell, 1980, 1981; Grapperon,
negative, and neutral valence appears greater than the Vidal, & Leni, 1998; Ilan & Polich, 1999; Lavoie, 1999;
semantic processing load of the incongruent color words. Rosenfeld & Skogsberg, 2006; Szcs & Soltsz, 2010;
This explanation might also be extended to the nonword West & Alain, 2000a; Zurrn et al., 2009). The latency
stimuli: the amplitude of P3b elicited by nonwords was of P3b has traditionally been considered as an indicator
the largest because the stimuli were only chromatic. Over- of the time required to evaluate the stimuli (Donchin,
all, these findings suggest that the temporal locus of the 1981; Kutas, McCarthy, & Donchin, 1977). Thus, it has
semantic conflict (i.e., the stage at which are compared been reported that such results may indicate that the pro-
the semantic code of the word and the semantic code of cesses related to stimulus evaluation do not intervene in
the color in which it is printed) occurs in the P3b latency the inference effect (Atkinson et al., 2003; Duncan-Jonhson
range of 300–450 ms in the case of incongruent color & Kopell, 1981; Ilan & Polich, 1999; Lavoie, 1999; Rosen-
words and positive, negative, and neutral colored words. feld & Skogsberg, 2006). However, this interpretation does
It might be argued that the gradation in the P3b ampli- not appear adequate as it is now considered that the P3b
tude (observed in Experiment I) is not the result of the latency may be an indicator of the time taken to evaluate
semantic conflict, but rather an effect of practice, as a result the stimuli in complex tasks but not in simple tasks (Pfef-
of the order of presentation of the three parts of the exper- ferbaum, Christensen, Ford, & Kopell, 1986; Verleger,
imental session (presentation of nonword, congruent/incon- 1997; Verleger, Jaskowski, & Wascher, 2005), and the
gruent, and positive/negative/neutral words). In Experiment Stroop task is characterized precisely by the stimuli being
II of the present study, the congruent and incongruent color of very different degrees of complexity (see Zurrn et al.,
word stimuli and positive, negative, and neutral colored 2009).
words were presented within the same block, and the results As also reported by Mager et al. (2007) and Zurrn et al.
were similar to those obtained in Experiment I. Thus, the (2009), in the present study, a positive central-parietal com-
data on the P3b amplitude obtained in Experiment I cannot ponent, with a latency of around 500 ms, was observed
be attributed to practice or to the order of presentation of after P3b and was denominated PSW (corresponding to
the different blocks of stimuli. TF3). This component is similar to the late positive compo-
We did not find any differences in the amplitude of P3b nent identified in the classic study by Squires, Squires, and
among the three stimuli with valence positive, negative, and Hillyard (1975), after P300 (P3b) and with maximum posi-
neutral, as also reported by Sass et al. (2010). However, tive amplitude at Pz. Because of its polarity, scalp distribu-
Franken et al. (2009), Li et al. (2007), and Stewart et al. tion, and approximate latency, this component appears to
(2010) observed differences between negative colored be the same as that denominated Slow Potential (SP)
words and neutral/positive colored words. Thomas et al. by West and colleagues (Markela-Lerenc et al., 2009;
(2007) observed differences in the amplitude of P300 for McNeely et al., 2003; West, 2003, 2004; West & Alain,
colored words with negative valence and for neutral color 1999, 2000a, 2000b; West et al., 2005) and believed to
words, in a task involving identification of words with neg- be associated with response selection, and appears to be
ative emotional valence, but in a task in which the partici- the same as the LPP component identified by Franken
pants had to indicate the color of the stimulus, the et al. (2009), Frhholz et al. (2011), and Gootjes et al.
differences between both types of color words decreased. (2011) in the emotional Stroop.
Kissler, Herbert, Winkler, and Junghofer (2009) reported In the color-word Stroop, the amplitude of the PSW
that in studies of visual word processing, the emotional component was larger for nonwords than for congruent
valence affected the amplitude of a positive component at and incongruent color words, although there were no differ-
around 400–500 ms, which was denominated P3, P3b, ences between the latter. This is consistent with the results
LPP, or LPC, and that this effect was robust when the sub- reported by Zurrn et al. (2009) for a congruence/incongru-
jects directed their attention to the emotional content of the ence judgment task. In the emotional Stroop, no differences

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 161

between stimuli were observed. The PSW did not appear to De Houwer, J. (2003). On the role of stimulus-stimulus and
be modulated by semantic interference processes or by the stimulus-response compatibility in the Stroop effect. Mem-
response selection processes, as was suggested by West ory and Cognition, 31, 353–359.
Dien, J. (1998). Addressing misallocation of variance in
et al. (2005). principal component analysis of event-related potentials.
In summary, the behavioral data confirm that incongru- Brain Topography, 11, 43–55.
ent color words and positive, negative, and neutral colored Dien, J., & Frishkoff, G. A. (2004). Principal components
words generate interference in response to the color. More- analysis of ERPs data. In T. C. Handy (Ed.), Event-related
over, the ERP data may indicate that the temporal locus of potentials: A methods handbook (pp. 189–207). Cambridge,
the semantic conflict generated by the incongruent color UK: MIT Press.
Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise! . . . Surprise? Psychophysiology,
words and by the positive, negative, and neutral colored 18, 493–513.
words may be located in the latency range between 300 Dulaney, C. L., & Rogers, W. A. (1992). Mechanisms under-
and 450 ms post stimulus, in relation to the P3b lying reduction in Stroop interference with practice for
component. young and old adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 470–484.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Duncan-Jonhson, C. C., & Kopell, B. S. (1980). The locus of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Acknowledgments interference in the Stroop task: When you read ‘‘blue’’, do


you see ‘‘red’’? Psychophysiology, 17, 308–309.
This work was financially supported by funds from the Duncan-Jonhson, C. C., & Kopell, B. S. (1981). The Stroop
Spanish Ministerio de Economa y Competitividad effect: Brain potentials localize the source of interference.
(PSI2010-22224-C03-03), and from the Galician Govern- Science, 214, 938–940.
Dyer, F. N. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the
ment: Consellera de Economa e Industria (10 PXIB study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes.
211070 PR), and Consellera de Educacin e Ordenacin Memory and Cognition, 1, 106–120.
Universitaria (Axudas para a consolidacin e estruturacin Edwards, S., Brice, C., Craig, C., & Penri-Jones, R. (1996).
de unidades de investigacin competitivas do sistema uni- Effects of caffeine, practice, and mode of presentation on
versitario de Galicia. Modalidade: Grupos con potencial Stroop task performance. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and
de crecemento. Ref: CN 2012/033). Behavior, 54, 309–315.
Feinstein, A., Brown, R., & Ron, M. (1994). Effects of practice
of serial test of attention in healthy subjects. Journal of
Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 436–447.
References Folstein, J. R., & van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive
control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A
Atkinson, C. M., Drysdale, K. A., & Fulham, W. R. (2003). review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152–170.
Event-related potential to Stroop and reverse Stroop stimuli. Franken, I. H., Gootjes, L., & van Serien, J. W. (2009).
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 47, 1–21. Automatic processing of emotional words during an emo-
Bair-Haim, Y., Lami, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, tional Stroop task. Neuroreport, 20, 776–781.
M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related Frhholz, S., Jellinghaus, A., & Herrmann, M. (2011). Time
attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A course of implicit processing and explicit processing of
meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. emotional faces and emotional words. Biological Psychol-
Botvinick, M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & ogy, 87, 265–274.
Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive Gootjes, L., Coppens, L. C., Zwaan, R. A., Franken, I. H. A., &
control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. van Strien, J. W. (2011). Effects of recent word exposure on
Bradley, M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English emotion-word Stroop interference: An ERP study. Interna-
words (ANEW): Instructions manual and affective ratings. tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 79, 356–363.
Gainesville, FL: Centre for Research in Psychophysiology Grapperon, J., Vidal, F., & Leni, P. (1998). The contribution of
University of Florida. cognitive evoked potentials to knowledge of mechanism on
Cattel, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. the Stroop test. Clinical Neurophysiology, 28, 207–220.
Behavioral Research, 1, 245–276. Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method
Ciesielski, K. T., Rowland, L. M., Harris, R. J., Kerwin, A. A., for off-line removal ocular artefact. Electroencephalography
Reeve, A., & Knight, J. E. (2011). Increase anterior brain and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484.
activation to correct response on high-conflict Stroop task in Hauk, O., Davis, M. H., Ford, M., Pulvermller, F., & Marslen-
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, Wilson, W. D. (2006). The time course of visual word
122, 107–113. recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP
Chen, A., Bailey, K., Tiernan, B. N., & West, R. (2011). Neural data. Neuroimage, 30, 1383–1400.
correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2-1 Hauk, O., & Pulvermller, F. (2004). Effects of word length and
mapping stroop task. International Journal of Psychophys- frequency on the human event-related potential. Clinical
iology, 80, 129–138. Neurophysiology, 115, 1090–1103.
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the Houston, R. B., Bauer, L. O., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (2004).
control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed Effects of borderline personality disorder features and a
processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological family history of alcohol or drug dependence on P300 in
Review, 97, 332–361. adolescents. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 53,
Davidson, D. J., Zacks, R. T., & Williams, C. C. (2003). Stroop 57–70.
interference, practice, and aging. Neuropsychology, Devel- Ilan, A. B., & Polich, J. (1999). P300 and response time from a
opmental, and Cognition. Section B, Aging Neuropsychology manual Stroop task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110,
and Cognition, 10, 85–98. 367–373.

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


162 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

Isreal, J., Wickens, C., Chesney, G., & Donchin, E. (1980). The latency depend on the task. Electroencephalography and
event-related potentials as an index of display monitoring Clinical Neurophysiology, 64, 424–437.
workload. Human Factors, 22, 211–224. Phaf, R. H., & Kan, K. J. (2007). The automaticity of emotional
Joyce, C., & Rossion, B. (2005). The face-sensitive N170 and Stroop: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
VPP components manifest the same brain processes: The Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 184–199.
effect of reference electrode site. Clinical Neurophysiology, Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive
116, 2613–2631. control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and
Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Emotion and attention in visual word processing – An ERP Potter, D. D., Jory, S. H., Bassett, M. R. A., Barrett, K., &
study. Biological Psychology, 80, 75–83. Mychalkiw, W. (2002). Effect of mild head injury on event-
Kramer, A. F., Sirevaag, E. J., & Braune, R. (1987). A related potential correlates of Stroop task performance.
psychophysiological assessment of operator workload dur- Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8,
ing simulated flight missions. Human Factors, 29, 145–160. 828–837.
Kray, J., Eppinger, B., & Mecklinger, A. (2005). Age differ- Rebai, M., Bernard, C., & Lannou, J. (1997). The Stroop test
ences in attentional control. An event-related potential evokes a negative brain potential, the N400. International
approach. Psychophysiology, 42, 407–416. Journal of Neuroscience, 91, 85–94.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kutas, M., McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting Redondo, J., Fraga, I., ComesaÇa, M., & Padrn, L. (2007). The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mental chronometry: The P300 as a measure of stimulus Spanish adaptation of ANEW (affective norms for English
evaluation time. Science, 197, 792–795. words). Behavior Research Methods, 39, 600–605.
Lavoie, M. E. (1999). Toward a functional explanation of the Rosenfeld, J. P., & Skogsberg, K. R. (2006). P300-based Stroop
locus of the Stroop Interference: A psychophysiological study with low probability and target Stroop oddballs: The
study. Brain and Cognition, 40, 167–170. evidence still favours the response selection hypothesis.
Li, W., Zinbarg, R. E., & Paller, K. (2007). Trait anxiety International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 240–250.
modulates supraliminal and subliminal threat: Brain poten- Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Edgar, J. C.,
tial evidence for early and late processing influences. Fisher, J. E., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Time course of
Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, attentional bias in anxiety: Emotion and gender specificity.
25–36. Psychophysiology, 47, 247–259.
Lindn, M., Zurrn, M., & Daz, F. (2004). Changes in P300 Sebastin-Galls, N., Mart, M. A., Cuetos, F., & Carreiras, M.
amplitude during an active standard auditory oddball task. (2000). LEXESP: Lxico informatizado del espaÇol [LEXESP:
Biological Psychology, 66, 153–167. Lexicon of Computer Science in Spanish Language].
Luo, C. R. (1999). Semantic competition as the basis of Stroop Barcelona, Spain: Edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona.
interference: Evidence from color-word matching tasks. Sereno, S. C., & Rayner, K. (2003). Measuring word recognition
Psychological Science, 10, 35–40. in reading: Eye movements and event-related potentials.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 489–493.
Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K., & Posner, M. I. (1998). Stablishing a
109, 163–203. time-line of word recognition: Evidence from eye move-
MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Training on integrated versus separated ments and event-related potentials. Neuroreport, 9, 2195–
Stroop task: The progression of interference and facilitation. 2200.
Memory and Cognition, 26, 201–211. Seymour, P. H. (1977). Conceptual encoding and locus of the
Mager, R., Bullinger, A. H., Brand, S., Schmidlin, M., Schrli, H., Stroop effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Mller-Spahn, F., . . . Falkenstein, M. (2007). Age-related Psychology, 29, 245–265.
changes in cognitive conflict processing: And event-related Sharma, D., & McKenna, F. P. (2001). The role of time pressure
potential study. Neurobiology of Aging, 28, 1925–1935. on the emotional Stroop task. British Journal of Psychology,
Markela-Lerenc, J., Schmidt-Kraepelin, C., Roesch-Ely, D., 92, 471–481.
Mundt, C., Weisbrod, M., & Kaiser, S. (2009). Stroop Silton, R. L., Heller, W., Towers, D. N., Engels, A. S.,
interference effect in schizophrenic patients: An electro- Spielberg, J. M., Edgar, J. C., . . . Miller, G. A. (2010). The
physiological approach. International Journal of Psycho- time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and
physiology, 71, 248–257. anterior cingulated cortex during top-down attentional
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional control. Neuroimage, 50, 1292–1302.
Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: The role of Sirevaag, E. J., Kramer, A. F., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E.
fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental (1989). Resource reciprocity: An event-related brain poten-
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, tial analysis. Acta Psychologica, 70, 77–97.
382–392. Squires, N. K., Squires, K., & Hillyard, S. A. (1975). Two
McNeely, H. E., Lau, M. A., Christensen, B. K., & Alain, C. varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpre-
(2008). Neurophysiological evidence of cognitive inhibition dictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalography
anomalies in persons with major depressive disorder. and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38, 387–401.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 1578–1589. Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Sass, S. M., Fisher, J. E., Edgar, J.
McNeely, H. E., West, R., Christensen, B. K., & Alain, C. C., Heller, W., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Attentional bias to
(2003). Neurophysiological evidence for disturbances of negative emotion as a function of approach and withdrawal
conflict processing in patients with schizophrenia. Journal anger styles: An ERP investigation. International Journal of
of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 679–688. Psychophysiology, 76, 9–18.
Morton, J. (1969). Categories of interference: Verbal mediation Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal
and conflict in card sorting. British Journal of Psychology, reactions. Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
60, 329–346. Szcs, D., & Soltsz, F. (2010). Stimulus and response conflict
Morton, J., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Selective attention to in the color-word Stroop task: A combined electro-myog-
words and colours. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental raphy and event-related potential study. Brain Research,
Psychology, 25, 387–397. 1325, 63–76.
Pfefferbaum, A., Christensen, C., Ford, J. M., & Kopell, B. S. Taake, I., Jaspers-Fayer, F., & Liotti, M. (2009). Early frontal
(1986). Apparent response incompatibility effects on P3 responses elicited by physical threat words in an emotional

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing


M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop 163

Stroop task: Modulation by anxiety sensitivity. Biological West, R., & Alain, C. (2000b). Effects of task context and
Psychology, 81, 48–57. fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting
Thomas, S. J., Johnstone, S. J., & Gonsalvez, C. J. (2007). performance of Stroop task. Brain Research, 873, 102–111.
Event-related potentials during an emotional Stroop task. West, R., Bowry, R., & McConville, C. (2004). Sensitivity of
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 221–231. medial frontal cortex to response and nonresponse conflict.
van Hooff, J. C., Dietz, K., Sharma, D., & Bowman, H. (2008). Psychophysiology, 41, 739–748.
Neural correlates of intrusion of emotion words in a West, R., Jakubek, K., Wymbs, N., Perry, M., & Moore, K.
modified Stroop task. International Journal of Psychophys- (2005). Neural correlates of conflict processing. Experimen-
iology, 67, 23–34. tal Brain Research, 167, 38–48.
Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action- Wickens, C., Kramer, A., Vanasse, L., & Donchin, E. (1983).
monitoring processes in the anterior cingulated cortex. Performance of concurrent task: A psychophysiological
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593–602. analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing
Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2005). Separating semantic resources. Science, 221, 1080–1082.
conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A Williams, J. M., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The
functional MRI study. Neuroimage, 27, 497–504. emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological
Verleger, R. (1997). On the utility of P3 latency as an index of Bulletin, 120, 3–24.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mental chronometry. Psychophysiology, 34, 131–156. Zurrn, M., Pouso, M., Lindn, M., Galdo, S., & Daz, F. (2009).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Verleger, R., Jaskowski, P., & Wascher, E. (2005). Evidence for Event-related potentials with the Stroop colour-word task:
an integrative role of P3b in linking reaction to perception. Timing of semantic conflict. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 19, 165–181. Psychophysiology, 72, 246–252.
Waters, A. J., Sayette, M. A., Franken, I. H. A., & Schwartz, J.
E. (2005). Generalizability of carry-over effects in the
emotional Stroop task. Behaviour Research Therapy, 43, Accepted for publication: May 28, 2013
715–732. Published online: November 14, 2013
West, R. (2003). Neural correlates of cognitive control and
conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks.
Neuropsychologia, 41, 1122–1135. Montserrat Zurrn
West, R. (2004). The effects of aging on controlled attention and
conflict processing in the Stroop task. Journal of Cognitive Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology
Neuroscience, 16, 101–113. University of Santiago de Compostela
West, R., & Alain, C. (1999). Event-related neural activity Campus Sur s/n
associated with the Stroop task. Cognitive and Brain 15782 Santiago de Compostela, Galicia
Research, 8, 157–164. Spain
West, R., & Alain, C. (2000a). Age-related decline in inhibitory Tel. +34 981 563-100, ext. 13707
control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in Fax +34 981 528-071
older adults. Psychophysiology, 37, 179–189. E-mail montserrat.zurron@usc.es

Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164


164 M. Zurrn et al.: ERPs in the Color-Word and Emotional Stroop

Appendix
List of Spanish words with positive, negative, and neutral
emotional valence presented in the task (with English
translation in brackets). The words are ordered alphabeti-
cally in Spanish.

Positive Negative Neutral


Afecto (Affection) Agona (Agony) Banco (Bench)
Amado (Loved) Bomba (Bomb) Barril (Barrel)
Amor (Love) Brutal (Brutal) Brazo (Arm)
Anhelo (Desire) Cncer (Cancer) Cable (Cord)
nimo (Cheer) Celos (Jealousy) Calle (Street)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Beso (Kiss) Choque (Crash) Carro (Wagon)


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Chiste (Joke) Clera (Rage) Cesta (Basket)


Coche (Car) Diablo (Devil) Codo (Elbow)
Coito (Intercourse) Dolor (Pain) Corcho (Cork)
Dlar (Dollar) Enfado (Anger) Cuenco (Bowl)
xito (Success) Estrs (Stress) Dedo (Finger)
Fama (Fame) Guerra (War) Esfera (Sphere)
Fiesta (Party) Horror (Horror) Farol (Headlight)
Ganar (Win) Hostil (Hostile) Fase (Phase)
Genial (Terrific) Infiel (Unfaithful) Hbito (Habit)
Gloria (Glory) Ladrn (Thief) Heno (Hay)
Guapa (Pretty) Loco (Mad) Jarra (Jug)

ntimo (Intimate) Miedo (Afraid) Lpiz (Pencil)
Jfflbilo (Joy) Odio (Hatred) Llano (Plain)
Lder (Leader) Rabia (Rabies) Motor (Engine)
Madre (Mother) Rehn (Hostage) Olla (Kettle)
Pareja (Couple) RiÇa (Quarrel) Parte (Part)
Pasin (Passion) Temor (Fear) Pieza (Item)
Rico (Riches) Terror (Terrorist) Pster (Poster)
Risa (Laughter) Txico (Toxic) Puerta (Door)
Sexo (Sex) Trauma (Trauma) Repisa (Mantel)
Sexy (Sexy) Tumor (Tumor) Silla (Chair)
Tesoro (Treasure) lcera (Ulcer) Taxi (Taxi)
Viaje (Travel) Veneno (Venom) Tinta (Ink)
Vida (Life) Violar (Rape) Venda (Bandage)

Journal of Psychophysiology 2013; Vol. 27(4):149–164 Hogrefe Publishing

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi