Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 14

Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2005, 18, 281–294

The Changing Epidemiology of Autism


Eric Fombonne
Department of Psychiatry, Montreal Children’s Hospital, McGill University, Montreal, Canada

Accepted for publication 6 June 2005

This article reviews epidemiological studies of autism vasive developmental disorders in then provided. It is
and related disorders. Study designs and sample charac- concluded that most of the increase is accounted for by
teristics are summarized. Currently, conservative preva- changes in diagnostic concepts and criteria, and by
lence estimates are: 13/10000 for autistic disorder, improved identification. Whether or not there is, in
21/10000 for pervasive developmental disorders not addition to these factors, a true increase in the incidence
otherwise specified, 2.6/10000 for Asperger disorder, of the disorder cannot be examined from available data.
and 2/100000 for childhood disintegrative disorder.
Newer surveys suggest that the best estimate for the Keywords: asperger disorder, autism, childhood disinte-
prevalence of all autistic spectrum disorders is close to grative disorder, epidemiology, incidence, pervasive
0.6%. A detailed analysis of time trends in rates of per- developmental disorder, prevalence

information on rates of autistic disorder, three studies


Introduction
provided estimates only on all PDDs combined, and three
The aims of this article are to provide an up-to-date studies provided data only on high-functioning PDDs.1
review of the methodological features and substantive
results of published epidemiological surveys. This chap-
Survey Descriptions
ter updates my previous review (Fombonne 2003a) with
the inclusion of eight new studies made available since Surveys were conducted in 14 countries and half of the
then. The specific questions addressed in this chapter results have been published since 1997. Details on the
are: (a) what is the range of prevalence estimates for precise sociodemographic composition and economic
autism, and related disorders?; (b) is the incidence of activities of the area surveyed in each study were gener-
autism increasing?; (c) what are the correlates of autis- ally lacking; most studies were, however, conducted in
tic-spectrum disorders?; (d) what is the role, if any, of predominantly urban or mixed areas, with only two sur-
cluster reports in causal investigations of autism? veys (studies 6 and 11) carried out in predominantly
rural areas. The proportion of children from immigrant
families was generally not available and very low in five
Selection of Studies
surveyed populations (studies 11, 12, 19, 23 and 26).
The studies were identified through systematic searches Only in studies 4, 34 and 38 was there a substantial
in the major scientific literature databases (MEDLINE, minority of children with either an immigrant or differ-
PSYCINFO) and from prior reviews (Fombonne 1999, ent ethnic background living in the area. The age range
2003a). Only studies published in the English language of the population included in the surveys was from
were included. Surveys which relied only on a question- birth to early adult life, with an overall median age of
naire-based approach to define caseness were also exclu- 8.0. Similarly, there was a huge variation in the size of
ded as the validity of the diagnosis is unsatisfactory in the populations surveyed, with a median population
these studies. Overall, 43 studies published between 1966 size of 63 860 subjects (mean 255 000) and about half of
and 2004 were selected which surveyed pervasive devel-
opmental disorders (PDDs) in clearly demarcated, 1
Studies are referred in the text by the numbers used in
non-overlapping samples. Of these, 37 studies provided Tables 1 and 4 or later in the text to index each of them.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2005.00266.x


282 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

the studies relying on targeted populations ranging in examination to the use of batteries of standardized meas-
size from approximately 16 000 to 167 000. ures. The Autism Diagnostic Interview (Le Couteur et al.
1989) and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observational
Schedule (Lord et al. 2000) were used in the most recent
Study Designs
surveys. The precise diagnostic criteria retained to define
A few studies have relied on existing administrative caseness vary according to the study and, to a large
databases (i.e. studies 34 and 40) or on national registers extent, reflect historical changes in classification systems.
(study 35) for case identification. Most investigations Thus, Kanner’s (1943) criteria, Lotter’s (1966, 1967) and
have relied on a two-stage or multistage approach to Rutter’s (1970) definitions were used in the first eight
identify cases in underlying populations. The first surveys (all conducted before 1982), whereas diagnostic
screening stage of these studies often consisted of send- and statistical manual (DSM)-based definitions took over
ing letters or brief screening scales requesting school thereafter as well as international classification of dis-
and health professionals to identify possible cases of eases (ICD)-10th edition since 1990. Kielinen et al. (2000)
autism. Each investigation varied in several key aspects have shown that a two- to threefold variation in rates of
of this screening stage. First, the coverage of the autism can result from applying different diagnostic cri-
population varied enormously from one study to teria to the same survey data.
another. In addition, the surveyed areas varied in terms
of service development as a function of the specific edu-
Characteristics of Autistic Samples
cational or healthcare systems of each country and of
the year of investigation. Second, the type of informa- Data on children with autistic disorder were available in
tion sent out to professionals invited to identify children 37 surveys (studies 1–37; see Table 1). The number of
varied from simple letters including a few clinical de- subjects considered to suffer from autism ranged from
scriptors of autism-related symptoms or diagnostic six to 5038 across studies (median 48; mean 209). An
checklists re-phrased in non-technical terms, to more assessment of intellectual function was obtained in 21
systematic screening based on questionnaires or rating studies. The median proportion of subjects without intel-
scales of known reliability and validity. Third, participa- lectual impairment is 29.6% (range 0–60%)2. The corres-
tion rates in the first screening stages provide another ponding figures are 29.3% (range 6.6–100%) for mild to
source of variation in the screening efficiency of surveys moderate intellectual impairments, and 38.5% (range 0–
although refusal rates tend to be very low. 81.3%) for severe to profound level of intellectual disabil-
Only two studies (studies 1 and 30) provided an esti- ity. Gender re-partition among subjects with autism was
mate of the reliability of the screening procedure. The reported in 32 studies totalling 6963 subjects with aut-
sensitivity of the screening methodology is also difficult ism, and the mean male:female ratio was 4.3:1. Gender
to gauge in autism surveys. The usual approach which differences were more pronounced when autism was not
consists of sampling, at random, screened negative sub- associated with intellectual disability. In 13 studies (865
jects in order to estimate the proportion of false negat- subjects) where the sex ratio was available within the
ives has not been used in these surveys for the obvious normal band of intellectual functioning, the median sex
reason that, because of the very low frequency of the ratio was 5.5:1. Conversely, in 12 studies (813 subjects),
disorder, it would be both imprecise and very costly to the median sex ratio was 1.95:1 in the group with autism
undertake such estimations. As a consequence, preval- and moderate to severe intellectual disability.
ence estimates must be seen as underestimates of ‘true’
prevalence rates. The magnitude of this underestimation
Prevalence Estimations
is unknown in each survey.
Similar considerations about the methodological vari-
Autistic disorder
ability across studies apply to the intensive assessment
phases. Participation rates in these second-stage assess- Prevalence estimates ranged from 0.7/10 000 to 72.6/
ments were generally high. The source of information 10 000 (Table 1). Prevalence rates were negatively corre-
used to determine caseness usually involved a combina- lated with sample size (Spearman’s r ¼ )0.73; P < 0.01);
tion of informants and data sources, with a direct small-scale studies tended to report higher prevalence
assessment of the person with autism in 21 studies.
The assessments were conducted with various diag- 2
Study 23 which relied upon different IQ groupings has been
nostic instruments, ranging from a classical clinical excluded.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Table 1 Prevalence surveys of autistic disorder

Number of % with
Study Size of target subjects Diagnostic normal Gender ratio Prevalence
no. Reference Country Area population Age with autism criteria IQ (M:F) rate/10 000 95% CI

1 Lotter 1966 UK Middlesex 78 000 8–10 32 Rating scale 15.6 2.6 (23/9) 4.1 2.7–5.5
2 Brask 1970 Denmark Aarhus County 46 500 2–14 20 Clinical – 1.4 (12/7) 4.3 2.4–6.2
3 Treffert 1970 USA Wisconsin 899 750 3–12 69 Kanner – 3.06 (52/17) 0.7 0.6–0.9
4 Wing et al. 1976 UK Camberwell 25 000 5–14 171 24 items 30 16 (16/1) 4.82 2.1–7.5
rating
scale of
Lotter
5 Hoshino et al. 1982 Japan Fukushima-Ken 609 848 0–18 142 Kanner’s – 9.9 (129/13) 2.33 1.9–2.7
criteria
6 Bohman et al. 1983 Sweden County of 69 000 0–20 39 Rutter 20.5 1.6 (24/15) 5.6 3.9–7.4
Västerbotten criteria
7 McCarthy et al. 1984 Ireland East 65 000 8–10 28 Kanner – 1.33 (16/12) 4.3 2.7–5.9
8 Steinhausen et al. 1986 Germany West Berlin 279 616 0–14 52 Rutter 55.8 2.25 (36/16) 1.9 1.4–2.4
9 Burd et al. 1987 USA North Dakota 180 986 2–18 59 DSM-III – 2.7 (43/16) 3.26 2.4–4.1
10 Matsuishi et al. 1987 Japan Kurume City 32 834 4–12 51 DSM-III – 4.7 (42/9) 15.5 11.3–19.8
11 Tanoue et al. 1988 Japan Southern Ibaraki 95 394 7 132 DSM-III – 4.07 (106/26) 13.8 11.5–16.2
12 Bryson et al. 1988 Canada Part of 20 800 6–14 21 New RDC 23.8 2.5 (15/6) 10.1 5.8–14.4
Nova-Scotia
13 Sugiyama & Abe 1989 Japan Nagoya 12 263 3 16 DSM-III – – 13.0 6.7–19.4
14 Cialdella & France 1 Department 135 180 3–9 61 DSM-III – 2.3 4.5 3.4–5.6
Mamelle 1989 (Rhône) like
15 Ritvo et al. 1989 USA Utah 769 620 3–27 241 DSM-III 34 3.73 (190/51) 2.47 2.1–2.8
16 Gillberg et al. 19914 Sweden South-West 78 106 4–13 74 DSM-III-R 18 2.7 (54/20) 9.5 7.3–11.6

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Gothenburg +
Bohuslän County
17 Fombonne & France 4 Regions 274 816 9–13 154 Clinical- 13.3 2.1 (105/49) 4.9 4.1–5.7
du Mazaubrun 1992 14 departments ICD-10 like
18 Wignyosumarto Indonesia Yogyakarita 5120 4–7 6 CARS 0 2.0 (4/2) 11.7 2.3–21.1
et al. 1992 (SE of Jakarta)
19 Honda et al. 1996 Japan Yokohama 8537 5 18 ICD-10 50.0 2.6 (13.5) 21.08 11.4–30.8
20 Fombonne et al. 1997 France 3 Departments 325 347 8–16 174 Clinical 12.1 1.81 (112/62) 5.35 4.6–6.1
ICD-10-like
21 Webb et al. 1997 UK South Glamorgan, 73 301 3–15 53 DSM-III-R – 6.57 (46/7) 7.2 5.3–9.3
Wales
22 Arvidsson et al. 1997 Sweden Mölnlycke 1941 3–6 9 ICD-10 22.2 3.5 (7/2) 46.4 16.1–76.6
(West coast)
23 Sponheim & Norway Akershus County 65 688 3–14 34 ICD-10 47.13 2.09 (23/11) 5.2 3.4–6.9
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 283

Skjeldal 1998
Table 1 Continued

Number of % with
Study Size of target subjects Diagnostic normal Gender ratio Prevalence
no. Reference Country Area population Age with autism criteria IQ (M:F) rate/10 000 95% CI

24 Taylor et al. 1999 UK North Thames .490 000 0–16 427 ICD-10 – – 8.7 7.9–9.5
25 Kadesjö et al. 1999 Sweden Karlstad 826 6.7–7.7 6 DSM-III-R/ 50.0 5.0 (5/1) 72.6 14.7–130.6
(Central) ICD-10
Gillberg’s
criteria
(Asperger
syndrome)
26 Baird et al. 2000 UK South-East Thames 16 235 50 ICD-10 60 15.7 (47/3) 30.8 22.9–40.6
27 Powell et al. 2000 UK West Midlands 25 377 62 Clinical/ – – 7.8 5.8–10.5
ICD10/
DSM-IV
28 Kielinen et al. 2000 Finland North (Oulu & 152 732 187 ICD-8/ 49.8 4.12 (156/50) 12.2 10.5–14.0
Lapland) ICD-9/
284 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

ICD-10
29 Bertrand et al. 2001 USA Brick Township, 8896 36 DSM-IV 36.7 2.2 (25/11) 40.5 28.0–56.0
New Jersey
30 Fombonne et al. 2001 UK England & Wales 10 438 5–15 27 DSM-IV/ 55.5 8.0 (24/3) 26.1 16.2–36.0
ICD-10
31 Magnusson & Iceland Whole Island 43 153 5–14 57 Mostly 15.8 4.2 (46/11) 13.2 9.8–16.6
Saemundsen 2001 ICD-10
32 Chakrabarti & UK Staffordshire 15 500 2.5–6.5 26 ICD10/ 29.2 3.3 (20/6) 16.8 10.3–23.2
Fombonne 2001 (Midlands) DSM-IV
33 Davidovitch et al. 2001 Israel Haiffa 26 160 7–11 26 DSM-III-R/ – 4.2 (21/5) 10.0 6.6–14.4
DSM-IV
34 Croen et al. 2002a USA California DDS 4 950 333 5–12 5038 CDER«Full 62.85 4.47 (4116/921) 11.0 10.7–11.3
syndrome»
35 Madsen et al. 2002 Denmark National register 63 859 8 46 ICD-10 – – 7.2 5.0–10.0
36 Chakrabarti & UK Staffordshire 10 903 4–7 24 ICD-10/ 33.3 3.8 (19/5) 22.0 14.4–32.2
Fombonne 2005 (Midlands) DSM-IV
37 Fombonne et al., Canada Montreal Island 27 749 4–17 61 DSM-IV – 5.1 (51/10) 22.0 16.8–28.2
submitted (Quebec)

1
This number corresponds to the sample described in Wing & Gould (1979).
2
This rate corresponds to the first published paper on this survey and is based on 12 subjects amongst children aged 5–14 years.
3
In this study, mild mental retardation was combined with normal IQ, whereas moderate and severe mental retardation were grouped together.
4
For the Goteborg surveys (Gillberg 1984; Steffenburg & Gillberg 1986; Gillberg et al. 1991), a detailed examination showed that there was overlap between the samples
included in the three surveys; consequently only the last survey has been included in this table.
5
This proportion is likely to be overestimated and to reflect an underreporting of mental retardation in the CDER evalautions.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 285

rates. When surveys were combined in two groups definition. It should be clear from these figures that they
according to the median year of publication (1994), the represent a very substantial group of children whose
median prevalence rate for 18 surveys published in the treatment needs are likely to be as important as those of
period 1966–1993 was 4.7/10 000, and the median rate children with autism.
for the 18 surveys published in the period 1994–2004
was 12.7/10 000. Indeed, the correlation between preva-
Asperger syndrome and childhood disintegrative
lence rate and year of publication reached statistical sig-
disorder
nificance (Spearman’s r ¼ 0.65; P < 0.01); and the results
of the 22 surveys with prevalence rates over 7/10 000 The reader is referred to recent epidemiological reviews
were all published since 1987. These findings point for these two conditions (Fombonne 2002; Fombonne &
towards an increase in prevalence estimates in the last Tidmarsh 2003). In brief, epidemiological studies of
15–20 years. The interpretation of this trend is discussed Asperger syndrome (AS) are sparse, probably because
below. In order to derive a best estimate of the current of the fact that it was acknowledged as a separate
prevalence of autism, it was therefore deemed appropri- diagnostic category only recently in both ICD-10 and
ate to restrict the analysis to 28 surveys published since DSM-IV. Only two epidemiological surveys have been
1987. The average rate was 16.2/10 000 and the median conducted which specifically investigated its prevalence
rate was 11.3/10 000. Similar values were obtained (Ehlers & Gillberg 1993, study 41; Kadesjö et al. 1999,
when slightly different rules and time cutpoints were study 42). However, only a handful (n < 5) of cases
used, with median and mean rates fluctuating between were identified in these surveys, with the resulting esti-
10–13 and 13–18/10 000 respectively. From these results, mates being extremely imprecise. By contrast, other
a conservative estimate for the current prevalence of recent autism surveys have consistently identified smal-
autistic disorder is most consistent with values lying ler numbers of children with AS than those with autism
somewhere between 10/10 000 and 16/10 000. For fur- within the same survey. In eight such surveys (studies
ther calculations, the mid-point of this interval was arbi- 23–27, 32 reviewed in Fombonne & Tidmarsh 2003; and
trarily adopted as the working rate for autism studies 36 and 37), the ratio of autism to AS rates in
prevalence, i.e. the value of 13/10 000. each survey was >1, suggesting that the rate of AS was
consistently lower than that for autism (Table 2). How
much lower is difficult to establish from existing data,
Unspecified PDDs – PDD-NOS
but a ratio of 5 to 1 would appear an acceptable, albeit
Several studies have provided useful information on conservative, conclusion based on this limited available
rates of syndromes similar to autism but falling short of evidence. This translates into a rate for AS which would
strict diagnostic criteria for autistic disorder (see be one-fifth that of autism. We therefore used an esti-
Fombonne 2003a). Different labels have been used to mate of 2.6/10 000 for AS for subsequent calculations. A
characterize them such as the triad of impairments recent survey of high-functioning PDDs in Welsh main-
involving impairments in reciprocal social interaction, stream primary schools has yielded a relatively high
communication and imagination (Wing & Gould 1979). (uncorrected) prevalence estimate of 14.5/10 000 (Webb
These groups would be overlapping with current diag- et al. 2003, study 43) but no separate rate was available
nostic labels such as atypical autism and PDD-NOS. for Asperger disorder specifically.
Fourteen of the 37 surveys yielded separate estimates of Few surveys have provided data on childhood disinte-
the prevalence of these developmental disorders, with grative disorder (CDD) (Table 3). Prevalence estimates
11 studies showing higher rates for the non-autism dis- ranged from 1.1 to 9.2/100 000. The pooled estimate
orders than the rates for autism. The ratio of the rate of based on seven identified cases and a surveyed popula-
non-autistic PDD to the rate of autism had a mean value tion of 358 633 children, was 1.9/100 000. The upper-
of 1.6, which, assuming a 13/10 000 rate for autistic dis- bound limit of the associated confidence interval (4.15/
order, translates into an average prevalence estimate of 100 000) indicates that CDD is a very rare condition, with
20.8/10 000 for PDD-NOS. This group has been much one case occurring for every 65 cases of autistic disorder.
less studied in previous epidemiological studies but
increasing recognition of its importance and relevance to
Prevalence for combined PDDs
autism has led to changes in the design of more recent
epidemiological surveys (see below) that are now Taking the aforementioned conservative estimates, the
designed to include these less-typical forms in their case prevalence for all PDDs is at least 36.4/10 000 [i.e. the

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Table 2 Asperger syndrome (AS) in recent autism surveys

Asperger
Assessment Autism syndrome

Size of Age Rate Rate Autism/


No. Reference population group Informants Instruments Diagnostic criteria n (per 10 000) n (per 10 000) AS Ratio

23 Sponheim & 65 688 3–14 Parent Parental Interview + ICD-10 32 4.9 2 0.3 16.0
Skjeldal 1998 Child direct observation,
childhood autism
rating scale (CARS),
autism behaviour
checklist (ABC)
24 Taylor et al. 490 000 0–16 Record Rating of all data ICD-10 427 8.7 71 1.4 6.0
1999 available in child record
25 Kadesjö et al. 826 6.7–7.7 Child Autism diagnostic DSM-III-R/ICD-10 6 72.6 4 48.4 1.5
286 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

1999 Parent interview-revised (ADI-R), Gillberg’s criteria


Professional Griffiths Scale or Wechsler (Asperger syndrome)
intelligence scale for
children (WISC), Asperger
Syndrome Screening
Questionnaire
27 Powell et al. 25 377 1–4.9 Records ADI-R Available data DSM-III-R 54 – 16 – 3.4
2000 DSM-IV
ICD-10
26 Baird et al. 16 235 7 Parents ADI-R Psychometry ICD-10 45 27.7 5 3.1 9.0
2000 Child DSM-IV
Other data
32 Chakrabarti & 15 500 2.5–6.5 Child ADI-R, 2-week ICD-10 26 16.8 13 8.4 2.0
Fombonne 2001 Parent multidisciplinary DSM-IV
Professional assessment, Merrill-
Palmer, Wechsler
preschool scale of
intelligence (WPPSI)
36 Chakrabarti & 10 903 2.5–6.5 Child ADI-R, 2-week ICD-10 24 22.0 12 11.0 2.0
Fombonne 2005 Parent multidisciplinary DSM-IV
Professional assessment, Merrill-
Palmer, WPPSI

Overall 614 123 5.0

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 287

sum of estimates for autism (13/10 000), PDD-NOS


(per 100 000)
(20.8/10 000), and AS (2.6/10 000)]. This global estimate

0–58.6
0.16–35.9

0.87–4.15
0.13–3.4

0.04–8.5

0.3–8.4
95% CI

is derived from a conservative analysis of existing data.


However, nine recent epidemiological surveys yielded
even higher rates in seven instances (Table 4). The two
surveys that did not probably underestimated the rates.
(per 100 000)

In the Danish investigation (study 35), case finding


Prevalence
estimate

depended upon notification to a National Registry, a


1.11

1.52

2.34
method which is usually associated with lower sensitiv-
6.4

9.2

1.9
ity for case finding. The Atlanta survey by the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC; study 39) was
M/F

2/–

2/–

1/–

1/–

6/–
based on a very large population (which typically yields
?

lower prevalence: see above) and age-specific rates were


in fact in the 40–45/10 000 range in some birth cohorts
n

7 (Fombonne 2003b). In most other studies, the case


definition chosen for these investigations was that of a
ADI-R, 2-week multidisciplinary

ADI-R, 2-week multidisciplinary


ADI-R, CARS and psychological

PDD as opposed to the narrower approach focusing on


Structured parental interview

Parental interview and direct

assessment, Merrill-Palmer,

assessment, Merrill-Palmer,
available – DSM-III criteria

autistic disorder that was typical of previous surveys.


WPPSI – ICD-10/DSM-IV

WPPSI – ICD-10/DSM-IV
observation (CARS, ABC)

Investigators were concerned with any combination of


and review of all data

tests – mostly ICD-10

severe developmental abnormalities occurring in one or


more of the three symptomatic domains defining PDD
and autism. Case-finding techniques employed in these
Assessment

surveys were proactive, relying on multiple and repea-


ted screening phases, involving both different inform-
ants at each phase and surveying the same cohorts at
different ages, which certainly maximized the sensitivity
of case identification. Assessments were performed with
2.5–6.5

2.5–6.5
3–14
2–18

5–14
group

standardized diagnostic measures (i.e. autism diagnostic


Age

interview (ADI)-R and autism diagnostic observational


schedule (ADOS)) which match well the more dimen-
sional approach retained for case definition. The size of
population

65 688

15 500

10 903

358 633
180 986

85 556

targeted populations was reasonably small (between


Size of
target
Table 3 Surveys of childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD)

9000 and 28 000), allowing for the most efficient use of


research resources. Conducted in different regions and
countries by different teams, the convergence of esti-
Iceland (whole island)
USA (North Dakota)

mates around 60/10 000 for all PDD combined is stri-


Country (region/state)

Norway (Akershus

UK (Staffordshire,

UK (Staffordshire,

king especially when coming from studies with


improved methodology. This estimate is now the best
estimate for the prevalence of PDDs currently available.
Midlands)

Midlands)
County)

Time Trends
The debate on the hypothesis of a secular increase in
Saemundsen 2001

Pooled estimates

rates of autism has been obscured by a lack of clarity in


Fombonne 2001

Fombonne 2005
Burd et al. 1987

Chakrabarti &

Chakrabarti &
Magnusson &

the measures of disease occurrence used by investiga-


Skjeldal 1998
Sponheim &

tors, or rather in their interpretation. In particular, it is


Reference

crucial to differentiate prevalence (the proportion of indi-


viduals in a population who suffer from a defined disor-
der) from incidence (the number of new cases occurring
in a population over a period of time). Prevalence is use-
No.

23

31

32

36

ful to estimate needs and plan services; only incidence


9

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
288 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

Table 4 Newer epidemiological surveys of PDDs

Autism PDDNOS + AS All PDDs

Study Rate Male/female % IQ Rate Male/female % IQ Rate


no. Author Age (per 10 000) ratio normal (per 10 000) ratio normal (per 10 000)

26 Baird et al. 2000 7 30.8 15.7 60 27.1 4.5 – 57.9


29 Bertrand et al. 2001 3–10 40.5 2.2 37 27.0 3.7 51 67.5
32 Chakrabarti & 4–7 16.8 3.3 29 44.5 4.3 94 61.3
Fombonne 2001
35 Madsen et al. 2002 8 7.7 – – 22.2 – – 30.0
36 Chakrabarti & 4–7 22.0 4.0 33.3 35.8 8.7 91.6 58.7
Fombonne 2005
37 Fombonne et al. 2005 4–17 22.0 5.1 – 42.9 4.7 – 65.2
38 Scott et al. 2002 5–11 – – – – – – 58.31
39 Yeargin-Allsopp 3–10 – – – – – – 34.0
et al. 2003
40 Gurney et al. 2003 6–11 – – – – – – 52.0

1
Computed by the author.

rates can be used for causal research. Both prevalence July 2002, a change of +75.8%. Second, the focus on the
and incidence estimates will be inflated when case defi- year-to-year changes in absolute numbers of subjects
nition is broadened and case ascertainment is improved. known to California state-funded services detracts from
Time trends in rates can therefore only be gauged in more meaningful comparisons. For example, as per
investigations which hold these parameters under strict December 2002, the total of subjects diagnosed with
control over time. These methodological requirements PDD was 17 748 in the 0–19-year age group (including
must be borne in mind whilst reviewing the evidence for 16 108 autism codes 1 and 2 and 1640 other PDDs; Cali-
a secular increase in rates of PDDs. Five approaches to fornia Department of Developmental Services 2003). The
assess this have been used in the literature. population of 0–19-year olds of California was
10 462 273 in July 2002. If one applies a rather conserva-
tive PDD rate of 30/10 000, one would expect to have
Referral statistics
31 386 subjects within this age group with a PDD living
Increasing numbers of children referred to specialist ser- in California. These calculations do not support the ‘epi-
vices or enrolled in special education registers have demic’ interpretation of the California DDS data. Rather,
been taken as evidence for an increased incidence of they suggest that children identified in the California
autism-spectrum disorders. However, trends over time DDS database were only a subset of the population pre-
in referred samples are confounded by many factors such valence pool and that the increasing numbers reflect
as referral patterns, availability of services, heightened merely an increasing proportion of children accessing
public awareness, decreasing age at diagnosis and chan- services. Third, no attempt was ever made to adjust the
ges over time in diagnostic concepts and practices, to trends for changes in diagnostic concepts and defini-
name only a few. Failure to control for these confound- tions. However, major nosographical modifications were
ing factors was obvious in some recent reports introduced during the corresponding years with a gen-
(Fombonne 2001), such as the widely quoted reports eral tendency in most classifications to broaden the con-
from California educational services [California Depart- cept of autism (as embodied in the terms ‘autism
ment of Developmental Services (DDS) 1999, 2003]. First, spectrum’ or ‘pervasive developmental disorder’).
these reports applied to numbers rather than rates, and Fourth, age characteristics of the subjects recorded in
failure to relate these numbers to meaningful denomina- official statistics were portrayed in a confusing manner
tors left the interpretation of an upward trend vulner- where the preponderance of young subjects was presen-
able to changes in the composition of the underlying ted as evidence of increasing rates in successive birth
population. For example, the population of California cohorts (see Fombonne 2001). The problems associated
was 19 971 000 in 1970 and rose to 35 116 000 as on 1 with disentangling age from period and cohort effects in

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 289

such observational data are well known in the epidemi- from a study of the California DDS database population
ological literature and deserve better statistical handling. to the California population (Fombonne 2003b). The con-
Fifth, the decreasing age at diagnosis leads, in itself, to clusions of this report were therefore simply unwar-
increasing numbers of young children being identified ranted. Some evidence of ‘diagnostic switching’ was
in official statistics or referred to already busy specialist produced in California (Eagle 2004) and in the UK by
services. Earlier identification of children from the pre- Jick & Kaye (2003) who showed that the incidence of
valence pool may result in increased service activity; specific developmental disorders (including language
however, it does not mean increased incidence. disorders) decreased by about the same amount that the
Another study of this data set was subsequently incidence of diagnoses of autism increased in boys born
launched to demonstrate the validity of the ‘epidemic’ during 1990–1997. On the whole, evidence from these
hypothesis (MIND Institute 2002). The authors used referral statistics is very weak and proper epidemiologi-
DDS data and aimed at ruling out changes in diagnostic cal studies are needed to assess secular changes in the
practices, immigration into California as factors explain- incidence of a disorder.
ing the increased numbers. While immigration was rea-
sonably ruled out, the study comparing diagnoses of
Comparison of cross-sectional epidemiological surveys
autism and mental retardation over time was impossible
to interpret in the light of the extremely low (<20%) As shown earlier, epidemiological surveys of autism
response rates. Furthermore, a study based only on each possess unique design features which could
cases registered for services cannot rule out that the pro- account almost entirely for between-studies variations in
portion of cases within the general population who rates, and time trends in rates of autism are therefore
registered with services has changed over time. For difficult to gauge from published prevalence rates. The
example, assuming a constant incidence and prevalence significant correlation previously mentioned between
at two different time points (i.e. there is no epidemic), prevalence rate and year of publication could merely
the number of cases known to a public agency deliver- reflect increased efficiency over time in case identifica-
ing services could well increase by 200% if the propor- tion methods used in surveys as well as changes in
tion of cases from the community referred to services diagnostic concepts and practices (Webb et al. 1997;
rises from 25 to 75% in the interval. In order to rule out Kielinen et al. 2000; Magnusson & Saemundsen 2001).
this (likely, see above) explanation, data over time are The most convincing evidence that method factors could
needed both on referred subjects and on non-referred (or account for most of the variability in published preva-
referred to other services) subjects. Failure to address lence estimates comes from a direct comparison of eight
this possibility precludes any inference to be drawn recent surveys conducted in the UK and the USA

Table 5 Study design impact on prevalence

PDD rate
Location Size Age group Method (per 10 000)

UK studies
Chakrabarti & Fombonne 2001 Staffordshire 15 500 2 12–6 1
2 Intense screening + assessment 62.6
Baird et al. 2000 South East Thames 16 235 7 Early screening + follow-up 57.9
identification
Fombonne et al. 2001 England & Wales 10 438 5–15 National household survey of 26.1
psychiatric disorders
Taylor et al. 1999 North Thames 490 000 0–16 Administrative records 10.1

US studies
Bertrand et al. 2001 Brick Township, NJ 8896 3–10 Multiple sources of 67
ascertainment
Sturmey & Vernon (2001) Texas 3 564 577 6–18 Educational services 16
California Department of California 3 215 000 4–9 Educational services 15
Developmental Services 1999
Hillman et al. 2000 Missouri – 5–9 Educational services 4.8

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
290 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

(Table 5). In each country, four surveys were conducted Fombonne 2005), suggesting no upward trend in overall
around the same year and with similar age groups. As rates of PDDs during the study’s time interval.
there is no reason to expect huge between-area differ-
ences in rates, prevalence estimates should therefore be
Successive birth cohorts
comparable within each country. However, an inspec-
tion of estimates obtained in each set of studies shows a In large surveys encompassing a wide age range, increas-
sixfold variation in rates for UK surveys, and a 14-fold ing prevalence rates among the most recent birth cohorts
variation in US rates. In each set of studies, high rates could be interpreted as indicating a secular increase in
are derived from surveys where intensive population- the incidence of the disorder, provided that alternative
based screening techniques were employed whereas explanations can confidently be ruled out. This analysis
lower rates were obtained from studies relying on was used in two large French surveys (Table 1: studies
administrative methods for case finding. As no passage 17 and 20). The surveys included birth cohorts from 1972
of time was involved, the magnitude of these gradients to 1985 (735 000 children, 389 of whom had autism), and,
in rates can only be attributed to differences in case pooling the data of both surveys, age-specific rates
identification methods across surveys. Thus, no infer- showed no upward trend (Fombonne et al. 1997).
ence on trends in the incidence of PDDs can be derived A recent analysis of special educational disability from
from a simple comparison of prevalence rates over time, Minnesota showed a 16-fold increase in the number of
as studies conducted at different periods are likely to children identified with a PDD from 1991–1992 to 2001–
differ even more with respect to their methodology. 2002 (Gurney et al. 2003; study 40). The increase was not
specific to autism as during the same period an increase
of 50% was observed for all disability categories (except
Repeat surveys in defined geographical areas
severe mental handicap), especially for the category
Repeated surveys, using the same methodology and including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
conducted in the same geographical area at different (ADHD). The large sample size allowed the authors to
points in time, can potentially yield useful information assess age, period and cohort effects. Prevalence
on time trends provided that methods are kept relat- increased regularly in successive birth cohorts; for exam-
ively constant. The Göteborg studies (Gillberg 1984; Gill- ple, amongst 7-year olds, the prevalence rose from 18/
berg et al. 1991) provided three prevalence estimates 10 000 in those born in 1989, to 29/10 000 in those born
which increased over a short period of time from 4.0 in 1991 and to 55/10 000 in those born in 1993, suggest-
(1980) to 6.6 (1984) and 9.5/10 000 (1988), the gradient ive of birth cohort effects. Within the same birth cohorts,
being even steeper if rates for the urban area alone are age effects were also apparent as, for children born in
considered (4.0, 7.5 and 11.6/10 000) (Gillberg et al. 1989, the prevalence rose with age from 13/10 000 at age
1991). However, comparisons of these rates is not 6 years, to 21/10 000 at age 9 years and 33/10 000 at age
straightforward as different age groups were included 11 years. As argued by the authors, this pattern is not
in each survey. Second, the increased prevalence in the consistent with what one would expect from a chronic
second survey was explained by improved detection non-fatal condition diagnosed in the first years of life.
among those with intellectual disability, and that of the Their analysis also showed a marked period effect that
third survey by cases born to immigrant parents. That identified the early 1990s as the period where rates star-
the majority of the latter group was born abroad sug- ted to go up in all age and birth cohorts. Gurney et al.
gests that migration into the area could be a key explan- (2003) further argued that this phenomenon coincided
ation. Taken in conjunction with a change in local closely with the inclusion of PDDs in the federal Individ-
services and a progressive broadening of the definition ual with Disabilities Educational Act (IDEA) funding
of autism over time acknowledged by the authors (Gill- and reporting mechanism in the US. A similar interpret-
berg et al. 1991), these findings do not provide evidence ation of upward trends had been put forward by Croen
for an increased incidence in the rate of autism. et al. (2002a) in their analysis of the California DDS data.
Two separate surveys of children born in 1992–1995
and 1996–1998 in Staffordshire in the UK (Table 1: stud-
Incidence studies
ies 32 and 36) were performed with rigorously identical
methods for case definition and case identification. The Only three studies provided recent incidence estimates
prevalence for combined PDDs was comparable and not (Powell et al. 2000; Kaye et al. 2001; Smeeth et al. 2004).
statistically different in the two surveys (Chakrabarti & All studies showed an upward trend in incidence over

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 291

short periods of time. In the largest study of 1410 sub- 10 000 for this group when compared with 4.4/10 000
jects, there was a 10-fold increase in the rate of first for the rest of the population (Wing 1993). However, the
recorded diagnoses of PDDs in UK general practice wide confidence intervals associated with rates from this
medical records from 1988–1992 to 2000–2001 (Smeeth study (Table 1) indicate no statistically significant differ-
et al. 2004). The increase was more marked for PDDs ence. In addition, this area of London had received a
other than autism but the increase in autism was also large proportion of immigrants from the Caribbean
obvious. However, none of these studies could deter- region in the 1960s and, under circumstances of migra-
mine the impact of changes over time in diagnostic cri- tion flux, estimation of population rates should be
teria, improved awareness and service availability on viewed with much caution. Similarly, the findings from
the upward trend. the Göteborg studies paralleled an increased migration
flux in the early 1980s in this area (Gillberg 1987). In the
Icelandic survey (study 31), 2.5% of the parents of aut-
Conclusion on time trends
istic children were of non-European origin compared
The available epidemiological evidence does not with a 0.5% corresponding rate in the whole population,
strongly support the hypothesis that the incidence of but it was unclear if this represented a significant differ-
autism has increased. As it stands now, the recent ence. In Norway (study 23), the proportion of children
upward trend in rates of prevalence cannot be directly with autism and a non-European origin was marginally
attributed to an increase in the incidence of the disorder. but not significantly raised as compared to the popula-
There is good evidence that changes in diagnostic cri- tion rate of immigrants (8% versus 2.3%) but this was
teria, diagnostic substitution, changes in the policies for based on a very small sample (2 children on non-Euro-
special education and the increasing availability of servi- pean origin). A UK survey found comparable rates in
ces are responsible for the higher prevalence figures. areas contrasting for their ethnic composition (Powell
Most of the existing epidemiological data are inadequate et al. 2000). In the Utah survey (study 15), the autism
to properly test hypotheses on changes in the incidence parents showed no deviation from the racial distribution
of autism in human populations. Moreover, because of of this state. The analysis of a large sample (N ¼ 4,356)
the low frequency of autism and PDDs, power is seri- of Californian PDD children showed a lower risk of aut-
ously limited in most investigations and variations of ism in children of Mexico born mothers and a similar
small magnitude in the incidence of the disorder are risk for children of mothers born outside the USA as
very likely to go undetected. compared to California born mothers (Croen et al.
2002b). In this study, the risk of PDD was raised in Afri-
can-American mothers with an adjusted rate ratio of 1.6
Correlates of Autism
(95% CI: 1.5–1.8); by contrast, the prevalence was similar
in white, black and other races in the population based
Associated medical conditions
survey of Atlanta (Yeargin-Allsopp et al. 2003) where
Overall, the proportion of cases of autism which could case ascertainment is likely to be more complete than in
be causally attributed to known medical disorders was the previous study.
low. Tuberous sclerosis and fragile X disorder are the Taken altogether, the combined results of these reports
medical disorders with the strongest association with should be interpreted in the specific methodological con-
autism. The fraction of cases of autism with any known text of these investigations. Most studies had low num-
potentially aetiologically significant medical condition bers of identified cases, and especially small numbers of
ranged from 0% to 16.7%, with a mean value of 5.9% autistic children born from immigrant parents. Statistical
(see Fombonne 2003a for further details). testing was not rigorously conducted and doubts could
be raised in several studies about the appropriateness of
the comparison data which were used (see Fombonne
Autism, race and immigrant status
2003a). The hypothesis of an association between immi-
Some investigators have mentioned the possibility that grant status or race and autism, therefore, remains lar-
rates of autism might be higher among immigrants gely unsupported by the empirical results. Most of the
(Wing 1980; Gillberg 1987; Gillberg et al. 1991, 1995). claims about these possible correlates of autism derived
Five of the 17 children with autism identified in the from post hoc observations of very small samples and
Camberwell study were of Caribbean origin (study 4; were not subjected to rigorous statistical testing. Large
Wing 1980) and the estimated rate of autism was 6.3/ studies have generally failed to detect such associations.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
292 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

support the hypothesis of a secular increase in the inci-


Autism and social class
dence of autism but the power to detect time trends is
Twelve studies provided information on the social class seriously limited in existing data sets. Whilst it appears
of the families of autistic children. Of these, four stud- that prevalence estimates have gone up over time, this
ies (1, 2, 3 and 5) suggested an association between increase most likely represents changes in the concepts,
autism and social class or parental education. The year definitions, service availability and awareness of autis-
of data collection for these four investigations was tic-spectrum disorders in both the lay and professional
before 1980 (Table 1), and all studies conducted there- public. To assess whether or not the incidence has
after provided no evidence of the association. Earlier increased, method factors which account for an import-
findings were probably due to artifacts in the availabil- ant proportion of the variability in rates must be tightly
ity of services and in the case-finding methods, as controlled. Nevertheless, the current prevalence figure
already shown in other samples (Schopler et al. 1979; of 0.6% carries straightforward implications for current
Wing 1980). and future needs in services and early educational inter-
vention programmes.
Cluster Reports
Disclosure
Occasional reports of space or time clustering of cases of
autism have raised concerns in the general public. In Dr Fombonne is an expert witness for vaccine manufac-
fact, only one such report has been published in the pro- turers in the thimerosal litigation.
fessional literature (Baron-Cohen et al. 1999) which des-
cribed seven children with either autism or PDD-NOS
Correspondence
living within a few streets from each other in a small
town of the Midlands (UK). The cluster was first identi- Any correspondence should be directed to Eric
fied by a parent and the subsequent analysis was unin- Fombonne, Canada Research Chair in Child Psychiatry,
formed with proper statistical procedures and McGill University, Department of Psychiatry, The
inconclusive as to whether or not this cluster could have Montreal Children’s Hospital, 4018 St. Catherine St.,
occurred by chance only. W. Montreal, QC H3Z 1P2, Canada (e-mail: eric.fombonne@
Cluster alarms are likely to represent random occur- mcgill.ca)
rence in most instances, as illustrated by several recent
investigations of cluster alarms for other rare disorders
References
of childhood. Cluster alarms in autism have not been
investigated with scientific rigour whereas research Arvidsson T., Danielsson B., Forsberg P., Gillberg C., Johansson
strategies and ad hoc statistical procedures exist for that M. & Kjellgren G. (1997) Autism in 3–6 year-old children in a
purpose. The approach to such cluster alarms should be suburb of Goteborg, Sweden. Autism 2, 163–173.
to confirm the alarm in the first place, using the avail- Baird G., Charman T., Baron-Cohen S. et al. (2000) A screening
instrument for autism at 18 months of age: a 6 year follow-
able techniques to assess the significance of clusters and
up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
to exclude random noise in spatial and time distribution
cent Psychiatry 39, 694–702.
of the disorder. It is only when an alarm has been con- Baron-Cohen S., Saunders K. & Chakrabarti S. (1999) Does aut-
firmed, more complex epidemiological investigations ism cluster geographically? A research note. Autism 3, 39–43.
should be set up to investigate risk factors and causal Bertrand J., Mars A., Boyle C. et al. (2001) Prevalence of autism
mechanisms. in a United States population: the Brick Township, New Jer-
sey, investigation. Pediatrics 108, 1155–1161.
Bohman M., Bohman I. L., Björck P. O. & Sjöholm E. (1983)
Conclusion Childhood psychosis in a northern Swedish county: some
Epidemiological surveys of autism and PDDs have now preliminary findings from an epidemiological survey. In: Epi-
been carried out in several countries. Methodological demiological Approaches in Child Psychiatry (eds M. H. Schmidt
& H. Remschmidt), pp. 164–173. Georg Thieme Verlag,
differences in case definition and case-finding proce-
Stuttgart.
dures make between survey comparisons difficult to
Brask B. H. (1970) A prevalence investigation of childhood
perform. However, from recent studies, a best estimate psychoses. In: Nordic Symposium on the Care of Psychotic
of 0.6% can be confidently derived for the prevalence of Children, pp. 45–153. Barnepsychiatrist Forening, Oslo.
autism-spectrum disorders. Current evidence does not

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 293

Bryson S. E., Clark B. S. & Smith I. M. (1988) First report of a Fombonne E. & Tidmarsh L. (2003) Epidemiological data on
Canadian epidemiological study of autistic syndromes. Asperger disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 4, 433–445. North America 12, 15–21.
Burd L., Fisher W. & Kerbeshan J. (1987) A prevalence study of Fombonne E., du Mazaubrun C., Cans C. & Grandjean H.
pervasive developmental disorders in North Dakota. Journal (1997) Autism and associated medical disorders in a large
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 26, French epidemiological sample. Journal of the American Acad-
700–703. emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 1561–1569.
California Department of Developmental Services (1999) Chan- Fombonne E., Simmons H., Ford T., Meltzer H. & Goodman R.
ges in the population of persons with autism and pervasive (2001) Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders in
developmental disorders in California’s Developmental the British nationwide survey of child mental health. Journal
Services System: 1987 through 1998 Report to the Legislature, of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 40,
1 March 1999; 19 p. (http://www.dds.ca.gov). 820–827.
California Department of Developmental Services (2003) Aut- Gillberg C. (1984) Infantile autism and other childhood psycho-
ism Spectrum Disorders: Changes in the California caseload– ses in a Swedish region: epidemiological aspects. Journal of
an update 1999 through 2002. April 2003 (http://www.dds. Child Psychology and Psychiatry 25, 35–43.
ca.gov/Autism/pdf/AutismReport2003.pdf). Gillberg C. (1987) Infantile autism in children of immigrant
Chakrabarti S. & Fombonne E. (2001) Pervasive developmental parents. A population-based study from Göteborg, Sweden.
disorders in preschool children. Journal of the American Medi- British Journal of Psychiatry 150, 856–858.
cal Association 285, 3093–3099. Gillberg C., Steffenburg S. & Schaumann H. (1991) Is autism
Chakrabarti S. & Fombonne E. (2005) Pervasive developmental more common now than ten year ago? British Journal of Psy-
disorders in preschool children: high prevalence confirmed. chiatry 158, 403–409.
American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 1133–1141. Gillberg C., Schaumann H. & Gillberg I. C. (1995) Autism in
Cialdella P. H. & Mamelle N. (1989) An epidemiological study immigrants: children born in Sweden to mothers born in
of infantile autism in a French department. Journal of Child Uganda. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 39, 141–144.
Psychology and Psychiatry 30, 165–175. Gurney J. G., Fritz M. S., Ness K. K., Sievers P., Newschaffer C.
Croen L. A., Grether J. K., Hoogstrate J. & Selvin S. (2002a) The J. & Shapiro E. G. (2003) Analysis of prevalence trends of aut-
changing prevalence of autism in California. Journal of Autism ism spectrum disorder in Minnesota (comment). Archives of
and Developmental Disorders 32, 207–215. Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 157, 622–627.
Croen L. A., Grether J. K. & Selvin S. (2002b) Descriptive epi- Hillman R., Kanafani N., Takahashi T. & Miles J. (2000) Preva-
demiology of autism in a California population: who is at lence of autism in Missouri: changing trends and the effect of
risk? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32, 217– a comprehensive State autism project. Missouri Medicine 97,
224. 159–163.
Davidovitch M., Holtzman G. & Tirosh E. (2001) Autism in the Honda H., Shimizu Y., Misumi K., Niimi M. & Ohashi Y. (1996)
Haifa area – an epidemiological perspective. Israeli Medical Cumulative incidence and prevalence of childhood autism in
Association Journal 3, 188–189. children in Japan. British Journal of Psychiatry 169, 228–235.
Eagle R. S. (2004) Commentary: Further commentary on the Hoshino Y., Yashima Y., Ishige K., Tachibana R., Watanabe M.,
debate regarding increase in autism in California. Journal of Kancki M., Kumashiro H., Ueno B., Takahashi E. & Furu-
Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 87. kawa H. (1982) The epidemiological study of autism in
Ehlers S. & Gillberg C. (1993) The epidemiology of Asperger FukushimaKen. Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japonica 36,
syndrome. A total population study. Journal of Child Psychol- 115–124.
ogy and Psychiatry 34, 1327–1350. Jick H. & Kaye J. A. (2003) Epidemiology and possible causes
Fombonne E. (1999) The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psy- of autism. Pharmacotherapy 23, 1524–1530.
chological Medicine 29, 769–786. Kadesjö B., Gillberg C. & Hagberg B. (1999) Autism and Asper-
Fombonne E. (2001) Is there an epidemic of autism? Pediatrics ger syndrome in seven-year old children. A total population
107, 411–413. study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 327–
Fombonne E. (2002) Prevalence of childhood disintegrative dis- 331.
order (CDD). Autism 6, 147–155. Kanner L. (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact.
Fombonne E. (2003a) Epidemiological surveys of autism and Nervous Child 2, 217–250.
other pervasive developmental disorders: an update. Journal Kaye J., Melero-Montes MdM. & Jick H. (2001) Mumps,
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33, 365–382. measles, and rubella vaccine and the incidence of autism
Fombonne E. (2003b) The prevalence of autism. Journal of the recorded by general practitioners: a time trend analysis.
American Medical Association 289, 1–3. British Medical Journal 322, 0–2.
Fombonne E. & du Mazaubrun C. (1992) Prevalence of infantile Kielinen M., Linna S. L. & Moilanen I. (2000) Autism in North-
autism in 4 French regions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric ern Finland. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 9, 162–
Epidemiology 27, 203–210. 167.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294
294 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

Le Couteur A., Rutter M., Lord C., Rios P., Robertson S., Sponheim E. & Skjeldal O. (1998) Autism and related disorders:
Holdgrafer M. & MacLennan J. (1989) Autism Diagnostic epidemiological findings in a Norwegian study using ICD-10
Interview: a standardized investigator-based instrument. Jour- diagnostic criteria. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 19, 363–387. ders 28, 217–227.
Lord C., Risi S., Lembrecht L., Cook E. H. Jr, Leventhal B., Steffenburg S. & Gillberg C. (1986) Autism and autistic-like con-
DiLavore P. C., Pickles A., Rutter M. (2000) The Autism Diag- ditions in Swedish rural and urban areas: a population study.
nostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of British Journal of Psychiatry 149, 81–87.
social and communication deficits associated with the spec- Steinhausen H.-C., Göbel D., Breinlinger M. & Wohlloben B.
trum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (1986) A community survey of infantile autism. Journal of the
30, 205–223. American Academy of Child Psychiatry 25, 186–189.
Lotter V. (1966) Epidemiology of autistic conditions in young Sturmey P. & Vernon J. (2001) Administrative Prevalence of
children: I. Prevalence. Social Psychiatry 1, 124–137. Autism in the Texas School System. Journal of the American
Lotter V. (1967) Epidemiology of autistic conditions in young Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40, 621.
children: II. Some characteristics of the parents and children. Sugiyama T. & Abe T. (1989) The prevalence of autism in
Social Psychiatry 1, 163–173. Nagoya, Japan: a total population study. Journal of Autism and
Madsen K. M., Hviid A., Vestergaard M., Schendel D., Wohl- Developmental Disorders 19, 87–96.
fahrt J., Thorsen P., Olsen J., Melbye M. (2002) A population- Tanoue Y., Oda S., Asano F. & Kawashima K. (1988) Epidemiol-
based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccination and ogy of infantile autism in Southern Ibaraki, Japan: differences
autism. New England Journal of Medicine 347, 1477–1482. in prevalence in birth cohorts. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Magnusson P. & Saemundsen E. (2001) Prevalence of autism in mental Disorders 18, 155–166.
Iceland. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31, 153– Taylor B., Miller E., Farrington C. P., Petropoulos M-C., Favot-
163. Mayaud I., Li J. & Waight P. A. (1999) Autism and measles,
Matsuishi T., Shiotsuki M., Yoshimura K., Shoji H., Imuta F. & mumps, and rubella vaccine: no epidemological evidence for
Yamashita F. (1987) High prevalence of infantile autism in a causal association. The Lancet 353, 2026–2029.
Kurume City, Japan. Journal of Child Neurology 2, 268–271. Treffert D. A. (1970) Epidemiology of infantile autism. Archives
McCarthy P., Fitzgerald M. & Smith M. A. (1984) Prevalence of of General Psychiatry 22, 431–438.
childhood autism in Ireland. Irish Medical Journal 77, 129–130. Webb E. V.J., Lobo S., Hervas A., Scourfield J. & Fraser W. I.
MIND Institute (2002) Report to the Legislature on the Principal (1997) The changing prevalence of autistic disorder in a
Findings from the Epidemiology of Autism in California. A Com- Welsh health district. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-
prehensive Pilot Study. 17 October, University of California, ogy 39, 150–152.
Davis, CA. Webb E., Morey J., Thompsen W., Butler C., Barber M. & Fraser
Powell J., Edwards A., Edwards M., Pandit B. S., Sungum-Pali- W. I. (2003) Prevalence of autistic spectrum disorder in chil-
wal S. R., Whitehouse W. (2000) Changes in the incidence of dren attending mainstream schools in a Welsh education
childhood autism and other autistic spectrum disorders in authority. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 45, 377–
preschool children from two areas in the West Midlands, UK. 384.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 42, 624–628. Wignyosumarto S., Mukhlas M. & Shirataki S. (1992) Epidemio-
Ritvo E. R., Freeman B. J., Pingree C., Mason–Brothers A., Jorde logical and clinical study of autistic children in Yogyakarta,
L., Jenson W. R., McMahon W. M., Petersen P. B., Mo A. & Indonesia. Kobe Journal of Medical Sciences 38, 1–19.
Ritvo A. (1989) The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic Wing L. (1980) Childhood autism and social class: a question of
survey of autism: prevalence. American Journal of Psychiatry selection? British Journal of Psychiatry 137, 410–417.
146, 194–199. Wing L. (1993) The definition and prevalence of autism: a
Rutter M. (1970) Autistic children: infancy to adulthood. Semi- review. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2, 61–74.
nars in Psychiatry 2, 435–450. Wing L. & Gould J. (1979) Severe impairments of social interac-
Schopler E., Andrews C. E. & Strupp K. (1979) Do autistic chil- tions and associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology
dren come from upper-middle-class parents? Journal of Aut- and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
ism and Developmental Disorders 9, 139–151. orders 9, 11–29.
Scott F. J., Baron-Cohen S., Bolton P. & Brayne C. (2002) Brief Wing L., Yeates S. R., Brierly L. M. & Gould J. (1976) The
report: prevalence of autism spectrum conditions in chil- prevalence of early childhood autism: comparison of admin-
dren aged 5–11 years in Cambridgeshire, UK. Autism 6, istrative and epidemiological studies. Psychological Medicine 6,
231–237. 89–100.
Smeeth L., Cook C., Fombonne E., Rodrigues L., Smith P. & Yeargin-Allsopp M., Rice C., Karapurkar T., Doernberg N.,
Hall A. (2004) Rate of first recorded diagnosis of autism and Boyle C. & Murphy C. (2003) Prevalence of autism in a US
other pervasive developmental disorders in United Kingdom metropolitan area. Journal of the American Medical Association
general practice, 1988 to 2001. BMC Medicine 2, 39. 289, 49–55.

Ó 2005 BILD Publications, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 281–294

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi