Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 16

Psychological Medicine, 1999, 29, 351–366.

Printed in the United Kingdom


# 1999 Cambridge University Press

Mild mental retardation : psychosocial functioning in


adulthood
B. M A U G H AN," S. C O L L I S H A W    A. P I C K L E S
From the MRC Child Psychiatry Unit and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre,
Institute of Psychiatry, London

ABSTRACT
Background. Evidence on the adult adaptation of individuals with mild mental retardation (MMR)
is sparse, and knowledge of the factors associated with more and less successful functioning in
MMR samples yet more limited.
Method. Prospective data from the National Child Development Study were used to examine social
circumstances and psychosocial functioning in adulthood in individuals with MMR and in a non-
retarded comparison group.
Results. For many individuals with MMR, living circumstances and social conditions in adulthood
were poor and potential stressors high. Self-reports of psychological distress in adulthood were
markedly elevated, but relative rates of psychiatric service use fell between childhood and
adulthood, as reflected in attributable risks. Childhood family and social disadvantage accounted
for some 20–30 % of variations between MMR and non-retarded samples on a range of adult
outcomes. Early social adversity also played a significant role in contributing to variations in
functioning within the MMR sample.
Conclusions. MMR appears to be associated with substantial continuing impairment for many
individuals.

relation to mild mental retardation (MMR)


INTRODUCTION
some commentators have argued that associated
Current diagnostic criteria for mental retar- impairments may be essentially time-limited,
dation (World Health Organization, 1992 ; and confined to the years of schooling. As
American Psychiatric Association, 1994) involve environmental demands become more varied
two elements : first, significantly below average and flexible with the transition to adulthood,
intellectual functioning, and second, limitations and a wider range of social ‘ niches ’ becomes
in adaptive functioning – impairments in coping available, so adaptive problems are likely to
with common life demands, and in the skills decrease (Kushlick & Blunden, 1974 ; Mac-
required for personal independence. The in- Millan, 1982).
clusion of social adaptation as a defining Evaluating this possibility has proved difficult.
criterion has long been a matter of debate, on In childhood, epidemiological studies have pro-
both conceptual and methodological grounds vided a clear picture of the functional impair-
(MacMillan et al. 1993). Developmental issues ments associated with MMR : increased rates of
are among the key considerations here : adaptive neurological abnormalities ; speech, language
capacities may change across development, and reading difficulties ; and high rates of
varying with the demands and supports of emotional and behaviour problems (see e.g.
differing environmental contexts. Indeed, in Birch et al. 1970 ; Rutter et al. 1970 ; Broman et
al. 1987). Evidence on functioning in adulthood
is much more limited. In terms of service use,
" Address for correspondence : Dr Barbara Maughan, MRC Child
Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London
rates of registration for special services are
SE5 8AF. known to fall between childhood and adulthood
351
352 B. Maughan and others

(Scott, 1994), but it is also clear that many stances and adaptation of individuals with
individuals not regarded as requiring additional MMR in adult life, using data from the second
support may still experience considerable diffi- of the British birth cohort studies, the National
culties in day-to-day functioning. More detailed Child Development Study (NCDS). Although
assessments have primarily focused on pre- not designed as a study of mental retardation,
viously institutionalized or specially educated NCDS allows for the identification of children
samples (see Widaman et al. 1991, for a review), with IQ levels in the MMR range, including
and very little is known about the later ad- those educated in both special and mainstream
aptation of young people educated in main- schools. The NCDS cohort has been tracked
stream schools (Bruininks et al. 1990). This is from birth to age 33, and the data-set includes
likely to be an important gap. Many children indicators of many key aspects of adult func-
with MMR are supported in mainstream tioning : cognitive competence (reading, writing
schooling, and epidemiological studies suggest and numeracy skills), independent living, oc-
that, although less impaired than their special cupational skills, and social and personal
school counterparts, they too often face con- relationships. Emotional distress, behaviour
siderable difficulties. In the Isle of Wight problems and psychiatric disorder constitute
epidemiological studies, for example, half of the further important outcome domains (Jacobson,
‘ mainstream ’ MMR group showed severe read- 1982 ; Borthwick-Duffy & Eyman, 1990). In a
ing problems, and rates of neurological ab- recent review, Borthwick-Duffy (1994) noted
normality were twice as high, and behaviour that although there is a general consensus that
problems four times higher, than in a non- people with mental retardation are at increased
retarded comparison group (Rutter et al. 1970). risk of emotional disorders, prevalence estimates
Any comprehensive picture of the adult func- vary widely, and few studies have reported
tioning of individuals with MMR clearly needs comparisons with non-retarded controls. Based
to take account of this important group. on a nationally representative birth cohort,
Although samples, methods and measures NCDS also provides for assessments in this
vary widely, a number of investigations have area.
now concluded that around half of individuals In addition to information on patterns of
with MMR are likely to show significant functioning in adulthood, a second need is for
difficulties in coping in adult life (Weaver, 1946 ; more detailed exploration of factors that influ-
Granat & Granat, 1978 ; Edgerton, 1984 ; Ross ence adult outcomes. Early social adversity may
et al. 1985). The most comprehensive UK be of particular importance here. All epidemio-
investigation to date derives from the Aberdeen logical studies have noted strong overlaps
surveys (Birch et al. 1970). The MMR sample between MMR and social disadvantage in
included all children in a 5-year age-cohort childhood (Roeleveld et al. 1997), and more
placed in special schools in the city before age 16 detailed reports have pointed to much increased
(N l 221), along with a non-retarded compari- risks of disrupted care-giving and distressed
son group individually matched for age, sex and family circumstances among children with mild
childhood social class. A follow-up at age 22 mental impairments (Richardson et al. 1985 b).
painted a gloomy picture for many of the young Although MMR is associated with identifiable
people with MMR (Richardson & Koller, 1992, pathological causes in a minority of cases
1996). Although only 20 % were receiving special (Simonoff et al. 1996), the two-group model of
services, much larger proportions faced prob- mental retardation, whereby mild impairments
lems at work (Richardson et al. 1988) and in are regarded as primarily subcultural or familial,
their personal and social relationships (Koller et has largely stood the test of time. Perhaps
al. 1988 a, b ; Richardson et al. 1993), and over a surprisingly, however, there have been few
third showed moderate to severe behaviour attempts to examine how far any later difficulties
disturbances (Richardson et al. 1985 a). Overall, faced by individuals with MMR are attributable
only 27 % were considered to be functioning to early social adversity, rather than to mental
adequately in all of these areas. impairments per se. Thus, our second aim was to
Our first aim in the present study was to assess how far early adversity, along with other
provide further evidence on the social circum- childhood risks, might account for variations in
Mild mental retardation 353

adult functioning between MMR and non- high reliability (Kuder-Richardson formula 0n94
retarded samples. (Douglas, 1964)). Secondly, as the test showed
Finally, since many individuals with MMR least discrimination at the extreme of the range,
do achieve satisfactory levels of adaptation in school placement data were used to exclude
adulthood, we also need to know more about children with severe intellectual retardation from
predictors of more and less successful outcomes the MMR sample : all those reported as attending
within MMR samples. Here, current evidence is special schools or units for the severely retarded
restricted to special school samples, and suggests at ages 11 or 16 were excluded from the MMR
that IQ level (Ross et al. 1985), family stability category.
in childhood, and brain disorder (Richardson & Of the 13 473 children in the original birth
Koller, 1992) are among the most important cohort with the relevant test and school place-
predictors of general adaptive functioning. ment data, this approach identified 48 as severely
Childhood behaviour problems – well-known to retarded (a rate of 3n6 per 1000), 275 as falling
show continuities to a range of poor adult into the MMR range (20n4 per 1000), and the
outcomes in non-retarded samples (see e.g. remainder (N l 13 150) as non-retarded. In a
Robins, 1978 ; Zoccolillo et al. 1992) – have been recent review of prevalence studies of MMR,
examined in relation to later behavioural dis- Roeleveld et al. (1997) noted the wide range of
turbance in individuals with MMR (Richardson prevalence estimates currently reported. They
et al. 1985 a), but their effects on wider patterns calculated a tentative average prevalence rate of
of functioning have yet to be assessed. Therefore, 29n8 per 1000 for children in the 50–70 IQ range,
our third aim was to examine the contributions slightly above Penrose’s (1963) theoretical value
of a range of childhood predictors in con- of 22n7 per 1000 for mental retardation as a
tributing to variations in adult functioning whole. The rate of MMR identified in the
within an MMR sample. NCDS cohort fell well within the range reported
in the more satisfactory epidemiological studies
undertaken to date (Roeleveld et al. 1997).
METHOD
Samples Measures
NCDS is a prospective study of all children in Childhood
Britain born in 1 week (3–9 March) in 1958 – Birth circumstances
some 17 000 in all. After the initial birth survey, The Perinatal Mortality Survey (Butler & Bon-
childhood follow-ups took place at ages 7 (Davie ham, 1963) included indicators of obstetric and
et al. 1972), 11 (Wedge, 1969) and 16 years perinatal complications (induced labour and
(Fogelman, 1983), and included a range of foetal distress), together with the child’s birth-
assessments : medical examinations and edu- weight.
cational testing of the study child, interviews
with parents, and data collection from teachers Social and family circumstances
and schools. The adult contacts, at ages 23 and Indicators of family size, social class, parental
33, involved interviews and questionnaires com- education and housing tenure were taken from
pleted by the cohort members themselves (Ferri, the age 11 sweep, contemporaneously with the
1993). group definition measures. A binary indicator of
MMR status was defined on the basis of data periods spent in institutional or other substitute
from the 11 and 16 year-old contacts, using a care up to age 16 was used to index family
two-stage approach. First, children were classi- breakdown\severe parenting problems. For
fied as falling into the mentally retarded range if analyses within the MMR samples, these
they scored at or below 1n94 .. units below the measures were combined to produce an eight-
mean (equivalent to IQ scores of  70) on a point social disadvantage index, including mea-
standardized 80 item group-based general ability sures of childhood social class, family size,
test administered to cohort members at age 11. housing tenure and receptions into care,
The test was designed by the National Foun- weighted according to their approximate power
dation for Educational Research, included alter- in predicting poor adult outcomes in the non-
nate verbal and non-verbal items, and showed retarded comparison group.
354 B. Maughan and others

were combined to identify any contacts with


Sensory and neurological problems child guidance, psychiatric and other specialist
Data from the medical examinations at each services up to age 16.
childhood sweep were used to derive indicators
of any severe or handicapping sensory impair- Court appearances
ments, epilepsy, and minor and more severe
In a similar way, teacher and parent reports
neurological abnormalities.
were combined to identify children who had
School placement appeared before the courts as a result of
delinquency up to age 16.
School placement was recorded at each of the
childhood sweeps. Special school placements
were reported for children with sensory and Adult status and outcomes
physical as well as intellectual impairments, and Data from the cohort member interviews at age
for those with multiple difficulties. All of these 33 were used to derive a range of measures of
types of provision were included in the special adult status and functioning, as follows.
school category.
Social class at age 33
School attainments
Registrar General’s occupational groupings :
Standardized reading comprehension and math-
non-manual and skilled occupations were con-
ematics tests devised by the National Foun-
trasted with semi-skilled\unskilled jobs. Subjects
dation for Educational Research were admini-
who had not participated in the labour market
stered at age 11. Both showed high reliability
in the past 10 years were included in the second
(reading, Kuder-Richardson formula 0n82 ;
(unskilled) category. For women in stable
maths, 0n92 (Fogelman, 1980)).
marital\cohabiting relationships, social class
Behaviour problems was assigned according to their partner’s
occupational status.
At age 11, standardized teacher behaviour
ratings were made using the Bristol Social
Adjustment Guides (BSAG, Stott, 1966), adap- Employment status at age 33
ted for use in NCDS. Although now largely Two measures were derived : the first contrasting
superseded, scores on the BSAG correlate highly cohort members currently employed (including
(r l 0n92) with the more widely used Rutter B full-time, part-time and self-employment) with
scale (Yule, 1968). From a large pool of items, those who were unemployed and out of the
teachers were asked to endorse behavioural labour market, the second contrasting the
descriptors appropriate to each study child. currently unemployed with the employed and
Responses were then grouped to provide 12 those out of the labour market.
syndrome scores, along with a total score. For
the present analyses, syndrome scores were Employment history ages 23–33
combined here to construct scales reflecting anti-
Four measures were derived, covering the
social behaviour (including stealing, destruct-
number of jobs of 1 month or longer, the
iveness and aggression), emotional problems
number of periods of unemployment, the length
(shy, withdrawn and depressed behaviours) and
of first period of unemployment and any
restlessness. Cut-offs as close as possible to the
dismissals.
90th percentile on the antisocial and the
emotional factors, and the 95th percentile on
the restlessness factor, were used to identify Receipt of benefits at age 33
children showing high (problem) scores. Included reports of receipt by the cohort member
or his\her partner of state benefits reflecting low
Psychiatric and other special service use in family income (unemployment benefit, supple-
childhood mentary benefit\income support, family income
To avoid under-reporting from any individual supplement\family credit, one parent benefit, or
data-source, parent, teacher and medical reports housing benefit).
Mild mental retardation 355

were derived to index the availability of support


Housing tenure at age 33 from a friend, a parent or parent-in-law, and
Owner-occupation was contrasted with all other more than one person.
tenure categories. Cohort members living in
their parents’ homes, or with other relatives, Psychological well-being and help-seeking for
were classified as ‘ other ’. emotional problems
The Malaise Inventory (Rutter et al. 1970), a 24-
Non-independent living at age 33 item self completion scale, was used to assess
Cohort members were treated as not living in- affective symptomatology at age 33 ; interviewers
dependently if they were unmarried and living were instructed to read the items for any subjects
with their parent(s)\family of origin. Those reporting marked reading difficulties. A total
living with a marital\cohabiting partner but score of six or more has been shown to
sharing accommodation with parents were classi- discriminate those with and without an interview
fied as living independently on this measure. diagnosis of depression (Rutter et al. 1976).
Recent studies with both adolescent (Manikam
Family life at age 33 et al. 1995) and adult samples (Lindsay et al.
Two measures were derived : whether cohort 1994) have confirmed the internal consistency of
members were currently living with a partner ; self-reports of anxiety and depressed mood
and ever fathered\mothered a child. among individuals with mild and moderate
mental impairments. In addition, the adult
Disability in adulthood interviews included self-reports of emotional
Two measures were derived : self-reports of any difficulties and contacts with GPs or specialists
longstanding illness or disability serious enough over emotional problems between ages 23 and
to limit daily life, and\or registration as disabled. 33, and the CAGE (Mayfield et al. 1974),
a brief screening questionnaire for alcohol
Literacy and numeracy skills problems, used in relation to both past and
These were derived from cohort members reports current drinking. Scores of two or more on the
of reading, writing, and numeracy problems in past version of the scale have been shown to
adulthood. provide a good indicator of alcohol problems
in community samples (King, 1986).
Relationship histories
Four measures were derived : whether the cohort Response rates and sample attrition
member had ever lived as married with a partner Complete data on the childhood measures
for  1 month ; if so, their age at the start of selected for use in the analyses were available for
their first partnership, and whether they had 238\275 individuals in the MMR sample
experienced a partnership breakdown ; and if (86n5 %) and 12 033\13 150 (91n5 %) in the non-
they had separated\divorced, whether they had retarded comparison group. Of these, 122
formed a second or subsequent partnership. (51n3 %) and 8554 (71n1 %) provided data at the
age 33 follow-up. Occasional missing data on
Parenting histories individual measures from the age 33 contact
Two measures were derived : cohort members further reduced the numbers with complete data
ages at the birth of their first child, and their to 100 and 7205 respectively.
number of children by age 33. As these figures suggest, data losses over the
period of the follow-up were significantly greater
Social support in the MMR than the comparison group. Known
Six hypothetical scenarios were used to assess death rates did not differ significantly between
the availability of support from family, friends the groups (3n2 % and 1n7 % respectively), but
and other. We focused here on the three scenarios individuals with MMR were less likely to be
that tapped personal\emotional difficulties (an traced in adulthood, and if traced, were also less
important change in your life, problems in likely to agree to participate. This pattern was
relationships with a partner, and feeling down not unexpected : in the 1946 British birth cohort,
or depressed). In each case, summary measures Wadsworth et al. (1992) found that childhood
356 B. Maughan and others

ability scores were among the strongest pre- Tables show odds ratios (OR), 95 % confidence
dictors of adult response rates. In NCDS, intervals (CI) and associated significance levels
complete data cases also tended to under- for between-group comparisons on dichotomous
represent men, subjects from lower social class measures, and means, standard deviations and F
backgrounds in childhood, those who had spent tests for comparisons on continuous measures.
periods in care, and those with childhood On two measures (age at partnership formation,
behaviour problems. In general, these factors and age at birth of first child), a minority of
appeared to affect response in both the MMR cohort members had not made the relevant role
and comparison groups in similar ways ; in some transitions by age 33. These measures were thus
instances, however, there were suggestions that analysed using methods for censored regression
these additional factors had a rather greater and Wald z tests. The quoted significance levels
impact on subject attrition within the MMR are indicative only, as no corrections have been
sample. made for multiple testing or non-independence
We used weights to tackle this sample attrition among tests.
problem (Brick & Kalton, 1996 ; Dunn, 1997).
Inverse probability weights were calculated from
RESULTS
the predicted response probabilities derived from
a logistic regression model of response to the Background
cohort member interview at age 33. Two main Table 1 provides an overview of the charac-
criteria were used in selecting effects for inclusion teristics of the MMR and non-retarded samples
in the weights : first, statistical significance within at birth and in late childhood and adolescence.
the logistic regression, and secondly, the extent MMR was associated with low birth-weight in
to which the selected weighting scheme sat- girls, with increased rates of neurological prob-
isfactorily reproduced the means and prevalence lems and epilepsy in boys, and with higher than
rates of cases with complete childhood data on expected levels of sensory handicap in children
key variables at age 11. The prediction equation of both sexes. As numerous other studies have
included effects of gender, parental education, reported, MMR was much more common
childhood social class (no male head of house- among children from lower social class back-
hold versus other, and classes IV and V versus grounds, and in large families. Parents of
other), general ability at age 11, teacher rated children in the MMR sample were less likely to
behaviour scores, reception into care and special have completed any post-compulsory education
school placement. To take account of possible than those in the remainder of the cohort (12 %
differential impact of these factors in the MMR v. 30 %), and fewer families of children with
and comparison groups, interactions between MMR owned their own homes (24 % v. 47 %).
each of these factors and MMR\comparison The odds of family problems severe enough to
group status were also included. Finally, one necessitate the child’s reception into care ap-
rather extreme weight assigned to an individual proached four times higher than those in the
subject was trimmed to that of the next lowest non-retarded sample.
weight to improve precision of the estimates By age 11, somewhat under a third of the
(Potter, 1988). children with MMR were placed in special
schools, and rates of special schooling rose
Statistical analysis further (to 35n8 % for boys and 28n3 % for girls)
The weights were included throughout the by age 16. As expected, the special school
analyses by the use of weighted likelihood subgroup included many of the most intel-
estimation and the robust or sandwich parameter lectually impaired children ; at the same time,
covariance matrix estimator of Huber (1967), as the data made clear that children from the full
implemented in STATA (StataCorp, 1997). This MMR ability range were also supported in
approach is discussed and compared with other mainstream schools. Mean reading levels and
methods in Pickles et al. (1995). Tests for mathematics attainments in late childhood were
dichotomous outcomes were undertaken using severely depressed in the MMR samples : only
logistic regression analyses, and those for con- 2 % scored above the 25th percentile in math-
tinuous measures using multiple regression. The ematics, and 25 % in reading. Finally, both girls
Mild mental retardation 357

Table 1. Childhood characteristics


MMR Comparison

Boys Girls Boys Girls Odds ratio (95 % CI)


(N l 49)† (N l 51) (N l 3482) (N l 3723)
% % % % Boys Girls

Birth
Low birth weight ( 2500 g) 7n2 19n4 6n8 8n1 1n1 (0n4–2n7) 2n7 (1n4–5n6)**
Foetal distress 14n4 4n2 9n9 7n8 1n5 (0n6–4n0) 0n5 (0n2–1n7)
Induced labour 13n3 14n3 13n3 13n1 1n0 (0n4–2n6) 1n1 (0n4–3n0)
Family background
Social class IV or V 32n5 53n2 21n4 21n9 1n8 (0n9–3n4) 4n1 (2n2–7n5)***
Large family ( 4 children) 57n1 62n2 28n7 28n6 3n3 (1n8–6n2)*** 4n1 (2n2–7n6)***
Family disruption – ever 13n4 11n2 4n0 3n1 3n7 (1n3–10n2)* 4n0 (1n5–10n9)**
placed in care by age 16
Medical conditions
Moderate\severe sensory 12n7 15n5 3n3 2n8 4n3 (1n6–11n7)** 6n5 (2n5–16n9)***
handicap
Epilepsy 7n9 3n6 0n7 1n0 12n2 (3n3–44n8)*** 3n8 (0n5–28n8)
Neurological 8n3 1n4 1n4 1n0 6n6 (1n5–28n4)* 1n4 (0n2–10n4)
Behaviour problems age 11
Antisocial 30n6 21n7 12n8 8n3 3n0 (1n5–6n2)** 3n1 (1n2–7n6)*
Hyperactive 14n9 11n4 5n7 2n8 2n9 (1n2–6n8)* 4n5 (1n9–10n6)**
Emotional 21n5 23n6 8n9 6n9 2n8 (1n4–5n7)** 4n1 (1n9–8n9)***
Psychiatric service contact by 44n9 13n2 6n9 4n5 11n0 (5n8–20n8)*** 3n2 (1n2–8n7)*
age 16
Schooling age 11
Special schooling 31n2 23n9 0n8 0n6 52n9 (21n7–128n7)*** 55n8 (19n1–162n9)***

Age 11 school attainment Mean (p..) Mean (p..) Mean (p..) Mean (p..) F (df) F (df)

Reading score 5n7 (p5n1) 5n3 (p4n2) 16n3 (p6n2) 16n3 (p5n7) 213n3 (1)*** 240n8 (1)***
Maths score 2n4 (p3n8) 1n9 (p2n2) 17n5 (p10n3) 16n9 (p9n8) 159n5 (1)*** 150n5 (1)***

* P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01 ; *** P 0n001.


† Unweighted Ns. Weighted Ns used in analyses : MMR, boys l 78, girls l 60 ; Comparison, boys l 3657, girls l 3509.

and boys in the MMR samples showed elevated with MMR, living circumstances and material
rates of disruptive\antisocial behaviours, rest- conditions were poor. Among the men, a quarter
lessness, and emotional problems. Children in were receiving benefits, and half were in lower
special schools showed especially high rates of class occupations. Unemployment was four
disruptive behaviours and of sensory\neuro- times as common as in the comparison group,
logical difficulties. Rates of psychiatric service and home ownership only half as likely. Almost
contact in childhood were high, especially among a quarter of men with MMR were living with
boys. More detailed tests showed that referrals their families of origin at age 33, and con-
for psychiatric help were more strongly asso- siderably lower proportions than in the re-
ciated with sensory and neurological problems mainder of the cohort were in marital or
than with behavioural disturbance in the MMR cohabiting relationships. One in six men with
sample. Data on court appearances (available MMR reported that they suffered from a
on slightly reduced numbers) suggested that longstanding illness or disability, and one in 13
boys were also at increased risk of officially were registered disabled.
adjudicated delinquency in their early and mid Rates of handicapping illness and disability
teens : 19 % of boys with MMR, by contrast were reported at similarly high levels among
with only 10 % in the comparison group, had women with MMR, but official registrations for
appeared in court by age 16. disability were much less common. Women in
the MMR sample were more likely to have
Social circumstances at age 33 children in their early thirties than their peers,
Table 2 gives an initial overview of the groups’ but somewhat less likely to be living with a
situations in adulthood. For many individuals partner at the time of interview. They themselves
358 B. Maughan and others

Table 2. Circumstances at age 33


MMR Comparison
Odds ratio (95 % CI)
Men Women Men Women
% % % % Men Women

Class and material circumstances


Class IV or V, or no work 49n4 60n0 16n3 21n2 5n0 (2n7–9n3)*** 5n6 (3n0–10n3)***
Employed (FT, PT, or self) 68n5 38n6 92n1 68n7 0n2 (0n1–0n4)*** 0n3 (0n2–0n5)***
Unemployed 19n9 1n8 5n3 2n2 4n4 (1n9–10n1)*** 0n8 (0n1–5n9)
Receipt of benefits 27n5 48n1 9n7 16n4 3n5 (1n7–7n4)** 4n7 (2n5–8n9)***
Housing tenure : owner 38n2 41n7 73n3 74n0 0n2 (0n1–0n4)*** 0n3 (0n1–0n5)***
occupied house
Family circumstances at age 33
Not living independently 23n9 3n8 9n0 4n7 3n2 (1n6–6n5)** 0n8 (0n2–3n5)
Currently living with a partner 61n8 74n7 80n4 82n6 0n4 (0n2–0n7)** 0n6 (0n3–1n2)
Has children 63n4 90n2 66n0 76n4 0n9 (0n5–1n7) 2n8 (1n2–6n9)*
Longstanding illness\disability
Any limiting daily life 16n2 15n3 5n7 5n4 3n2 (1n3–8n0)* 3n1 (1n1–8n6)*
Registered disabled 7n4 1n3 1n1 0n6 7n4 (1n8–29n5q** 2n1 (0n3–16n1)

*P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01 ; *** P 0n001.

Table 3. Self-reported literacy and numeracy problems, age 33


MMR Comparison
Odds ratio (95 % CI)
Men Women Men Women
% % % % Men Women

Reading problems 38n7 33n0 4n5 2n1 13n4 (6n8–26n4)*** 22n7 (11n1–46n3)***
Writing problems 45n5 34n4 11n9 5n8 6n2 (3n3–11n6)*** 8n5 (4n3–16n7)***
Numeracy problems 17n0 8n5 1n7 2n5 11n9 (4n7–29n9)*** 3n6 (1n4–9n6)*
Any literacy or numeracy 55n0 43n7 13n3 8n0 8n0 (4n3–14n7)*** 8n9 (4n7–16n7)***
problems since leaving school

*P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01 ; *** P 0n001.

were less likely to be in employment than other CI l 1n0–4n4, P l 0n05). Among women, similar
women, and their family social class (rated controls reduced group differences in housing
wherever appropriate on the basis of their tenure by 43 %, moving the OR closer to the null
partner’s occupation) was if anything more value of 1 (OR l 0n44, CI l 0n22–0n89, P l
depressed than that of men with MMR. Almost 0n02).
half of the women were receiving welfare benefits Within the MMR sample, variations in out-
in their early thirties. come also appeared to reflect some degree of
We undertook a series of analyses to assess continuity from childhood disadvantage. Re-
how far these differences between the MMR and ceipt of welfare benefits and housing tenure
comparison groups were associated with vari- showed no significant links with childhood
ations in other childhood risks : low birthweight, general ability scores, early sensory\neurological
sensory\neurological impairments, early social problems or behavioural difficulties, but each
disadvantage (low social class, large family size, point on the eight-point index of childhood
poor housing and receptions into care) and disadvantage increased the odds of poor adult
behaviour problems. Controls for these factors circumstances by 1" times. For women only,
#
reduced, but failed to eliminate, the between- adult social status showed significant links (P l
group differences. Among men, for example, 0n05) with tested ability levels in childhood ;
controlling for childhood background factors these associations were only marginally reduced
reduced group differences in dependence on by the inclusion of other childhood background
benefits by 41 %, to an odds ratio of 2n1 (95 % factors.
Mild mental retardation 359

Table 4. Partnership and parenting histories – age 33


MMR Comparison
Odds ratio (95 % CI)
Men Women Men Women
% % % % Men Women

Partnership histories
Ever in a stable 79n5 94n7 89n3 93n9 0n5 (0n2–1n0)* 1n2 (0n4–4n0)
cohabitation
Breakdown of relationship 33n7 45n5 27n3 30n7 1n4 (0n7–2n7) 1n9 (1n0–3n6)
(ever)†
Multiple partnerships† 15n8 31n8 20n3 22n3 0n7 (0n3–1n7) 1n6 (0n8–3n4)
Mean age at first partner (23n6) (19n3) (23n4) (21n5)
(years)†
Parenting histories
Teenage parent 6n4 39n5 3n0 12n1 2n2 (0n6–7n6) 4n7 (2n4–9n2)***
Number of live births ever
0 36n6 9n8 34n0 23n6 1n0 1n0
1 or 2 43n5 46n9 52n5 57n5 0n8 (0n4–1n5) 2n0 (0n8–5n0)
3 19n9 43n3 13n4 18n9 1n4 (0n5–3n5) 5n5 (2n1–14n5)**
Mean age at first child (24n0) (20n7) (25n9) (24n1)
(years)‡

* P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01 ; *** P 0n001.


† Of those ever in a relationship ; ‡ of those who have had a child.

Literacy and numeracy skills in adulthood up to age 33. Here, findings varied in some
Literacy and numeracy skills are of central important ways by gender. Among women, the
importance to many aspects of adult functioning. great majority in both the MMR and com-
In the non-retarded comparison group, around parison groups had established marital\
10 % of cohort members reported difficulties in cohabiting relationships by their early thirties,
these areas in adult life. As Table 3 shows, over and most had had at least one child. The two
40 % of women and more than half of the men groups differed markedly, however, in the timing
with MMR reported problems of this kind. of these early adult role transitions. Women in
These self-ratings showed strong continuities the MMR sample began their first partnerships
with childhood reading and mathematical skills. over 2 years earlier than was the norm for
MMR-comparison group differences in levels of women in the remainder of the cohort, and had
adult literacy\numeracy problems were un- their first child almost 5 years earlier ; almost
related to childhood social background, be- 40 % became mothers in their teens. Censored
haviour, or sensory\neurological problems, but regression analyses confirmed the highly sig-
were reduced to non-significance by controls for nificant differences between the groups in age at
age 11 general ability scores. Within the MMR first partner (z lk3n52, P 0n001) and age at
sample, childhood reading and maths scores first child (z lk6n04, P 0n001). Group dif-
proved more important than measured general ferences in age at first childbearing were reduced
ability levels in accounting for variation in adult by 39 % by controls for childhood social
literacy and numeracy skills, and remained disadvantage and medical problems, and by a
significant predictors of adult difficulties (read- further 8 % by the inclusion of childhood
ing, P l 0n02 ; maths, P l 0n04) in the presence behaviour problems. Including all of these
of controls for sensory\neurological problems, factors, however, the group differences remained
childhood social disadvantage and behaviour highly significant (P 0n001). Within the MMR
problems. sample, variations in age of childbearing were
significantly associated with early social dis-
advantage (z lk2n28, P l 0n02), though not
Independent living, relationship and family with any of the other childhood factors ex-
formation amined. This pattern of early adult role trans-
Table 4 gives details of patterns of marital\ itions was much less marked for men with
cohabiting relationships and family formation MMR, and no significant group differences in
360 B. Maughan and others

Table 5. Employment histories – ages 23–33


MMR Comparison
Odds ratio (95 % CI)
Men Women Men Women
% % % % Men Women

No jobs age 23–33 7n7 25n4 0n8 5n6 10n5 (2n6–41n7)** 5n7 (2n8–12n0)***
 5 jobs age 23–33 14n3 5n3 16n3 14n3 0n9 (0n3–2n2) 0n3 (0n1–1n1)
Any period of 56n8 14n9 28n2 21n9 3n3 (1n8–6n2)*** 0n6 (0n3–1n4)
unemployment age 23–33
Multiple periods of 26n3 3n3 11n1 6n7 2n8 (1n4–5n9)** 0n5 (0n1–2n0)
unemployment age 23–33
1st period of employment :
 12 months† 31n0 12n8 25n6 28n3 1n3 (0n5–3n3) 0n4 (0n1–3n1)
Any sacking‡ 3n8 0n0 4n0 1n4 1n0 (0n2–4n2) —

* P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01 ; *** P 0n001.


† Of those ever unemployed ; ‡ of those who have worked.

Table 6. Social support


MMR Comparison

Men Women Men Women Odds ratio (95 % CI)


(N l 40)† (N l 44) (N l 3309) (N l 3594)
% % % % Men Women

Sources of support for :


An important life change
Friend 31n3 23n4 29n7 45n8 1n1 (0n5–2n2) 0n4 (0n2–0n7)**
Parent\in-law 81n4 61n1 63n5 72n3 2n5 (1n2–5n4)* 0n6 (0n3–1n1)
More than one person 81n8 68n2 76n6 86n8 1n4 (0n6–3n0) 0n3 (0n2–0n6)**
A relationship problem
Friend 24n6 38n2 35n3 60n4 0n6 (0n3–1n3) 0n4 (0n2–0n8)**
Parent\in-law 68n0 49n1 52n8 54n6 1n9 (1n0–3n7) 0n8 (0n4–1n5)
More than one person 55n0 52n5 57n8 73n8 0n9 (0n5–1n8) 0n4 (0n2–0n7)**
Feeling down or depressed
Friend 34n4 41n8 36n5 64n3 0n9 (0n4–1n9) 0n4 (0n2–0n8)**
Parent\in-law 51n2 45n2 37n5 46n4 1n7 (0n9–3n4) 1n0 (0n5–1n8)
More than one person 62n8 65n4 61n1 82n2 1n1 (0n5–2n1) 0n4 (0n2–0n8)**

* P 0n05 ; ** P 0n01.
† Unweighted Ns. Weighted Ns used in analyses : MMR, men l 64, women l 48 ; Comparison, men l 3461, women l 3378.

either age at first partnership or first child


remained once the censored nature of the data Employment histories
had been taken into account. Instead, possibly Table 5 shows indicators of employment his-
more salient for the men was the lower overall tories between ages 23 and 33. For women, the
rate of marriage\cohabitation in the MMR only marked differences between the MMR and
sample, and, as noted earlier (see Table 2), the comparison groups arose in the proportions
much increased likelihood that men with MMR who had not worked at all outside the home
would be living alone with their family of origin during this period. These remained significant
in their early thirties. In this latter area, group after controls for all childhood factors, and,
differences were reduced by controls for child- with the exception of a significant (P l 0n03)
hood behaviour problems and social disad- effect of childhood social disadvantage, showed
vantage, but still remained significant (OR l few clear links with childhood background
2n6, CI l 1n1–5n7, P l 0n02). Within the MMR indicators within the MMR sample.
sample, neither general ability levels nor any of Among the men, one in twelve in the MMR
the other childhood factors significantly pre- sample had not worked at all between ages 23
dicted variations in independent living in the and 33. Over half of this group (58 %) were
early thirties. registered disabled. Even taking account of
Mild mental retardation 361

disability, however, men with MMR were still availability of multiple sources of support.
significantly more likely to have remained out of Instead, the main MMR-comparison group
the labour market in their twenties and early contrasts arose in relation to support from
thirties (OR l 7n8, CI l 2n6–23n8, P l 0n001). parents. This was reported at significantly higher
Those in the job market had considerably levels by men in the MMR sample on one
increased risks of multiple periods of unem- scenario (discussion of a major life change), and
ployment, but showed few other major em- followed a similar though non-significant trend
ployment problems on the measures available in the other two. As outlined earlier, men with
here. They were no more likely than men in the MMR were much more likely to be living with
remainder of the cohort to have suffered long- their parents in adulthood ; even among those
term unemployment, to have made large num- living independently, however, group differences
bers of job changes, or to have been dismissed in reliance on parents remained at almost exactly
from jobs. The increased vulnerability to re- similar levels.
peated spells of unemployment in the MMR Across all three scenarios, MMR-comparison
sample did suggest, however, that men with group contrasts in patterns of social support
MMR faced greater difficulties in re-establishing seemed relatively independent of childhood
themselves in work if they were made redundant background factors. Neither early disadvantage
or chose to leave a job. Group differences in nor behaviour problems seemed implicated in
rates of repeated unemployment were reduced between-group differences for either sex, and the
by 34 %, and to non-significance (OR l 1n9, childhood factors showed few clear links with
CI l 0n9–3n9, P l 0n10) when controlled for variations in availability of support within the
childhood social disadvantage, and for sensory\ MMR sample.
neurological and behavioural problems. Within
the MMR sample, repeated unemployment was Adult psychiatric morbidity and service use
unrelated to general ability levels, but was Women in the MMR sample reported strikingly
significantly associated with early social ad- high levels of depressed mood at the time of the
versity (P l 0n001), and with lower levels of adult interviews, with over half scoring above a
childhood behaviour problems (P l 0n04). Once cut-point on the Malaise Inventory suggesting
again, these findings gave little reason to assume clinically significant disorder (Table 7). Malaise
that repeated unemployment reflected unsat- scores were also much elevated among the men.
isfactory performance in the workplace. By contrast, self-reported rates of help-seeking
for emotional problems in the previous 10 years
Social support showed only minor group differences, and rates
Table 6 shows responses to items from the social of alcohol problems (as indexed by high scores
support questionnaire completed by cohort on the CAGE) were closely similar in the two
members at age 33. Within the non-retarded samples. For both women and men, MMR-
sample, responses varied consistently by gender, comparison group differences in Malaise scores
with more women than men reporting turning to were reduced (by 35 % and 42 % respectively) by
a friend for support, and also having multiple the addition of controls for childhood adversity,
sources of support available to them. Against sensory\neurological impairments and behav-
that background, a consistent pattern of MMR- iour problems. Taking all of these factors into
comparison group contrasts also emerged. account, however, the group differences re-
Women with MMR were markedly less likely to mained highly significant.
report support from a friend than women in the To enhance the power of the within-group
remainder of the cohort, and they were also analyses, predictors of high Malaise scores and
significantly less likely to feel that they had more past help-seeking within the MMR samples
than one person to turn to. Controls for were examined for men and women combined.
childhood disadvantage and behaviour problems Taking account of mean differences between the
reduced these differences by up to 20 %, but sexes, high scores on the Malaise Inventory
failed to eliminate them. For men, a quite showed some links with childhood sensory and
different pattern emerged, with no group dif- neurological problems (OR l 3n1, CI l 1n0–9n6,
ferences in support from friends, or in the P l 0n05) and social disadvantage (OR l 1n4,
362 B. Maughan and others

Table 7. Psychiatric morbidity – age 33


MMR Comparison
Odds ratio (95 % CI)
Men Women Men Women
% % % % Men Women

High Malaise score ( 6) 29n7 51n1 9n0 15n9 4n3 (2n1–8n7)*** 5n5 (3n0–10n3)***
Emotional problems ages 23–33
None 56n7 36n6 53n2 37n5 1n0 1n0
Some, but no help-seeking 30n7 17n0 33n3 33n0 0n9 (0n4–1n8) 0n5 (0n2–1n2)
Consulted GP 7n9 34n2 8n0 19n5 0n9 (0n3–2n5) 1n8 (0n9–3n7)
Consulted specialist 4n6 12n3 5n5 10n0 0n8 (0n2–2n6) 1n3 (0n4–4n0)
High CAGE score 18n1 10n4 16n4 7n7 1n1 (0n5–2n4) 1n4 (0n6–3n4)
Total Malaise score : mean (p..) 3n6 (p3n6) 5n8 (p4n5) 2n0 (p2n7) 2n8 (p3n2) F l 25n7 (1)*** F l 52n5 (1)***

*** P 0n001.

CI l 1n0–2n0, P l 0n07), but not with general MMR sample, the analyses were all controlled
ability levels or childhood behaviour ratings. for intake differences between the two school
Women with MMR were considerably more sectors in terms of cohort members’ measured
likely than men to report consulting GPs and\or ability in childhood, social background and
specialists over emotional problems in the behaviour problems, and sensory\neurological
previous 10 years (OR l 6n1, CI l 2n2–16n5, handicaps.
P 0n001). Taking these gender differences into On the majority of the outcome measures any
account, only childhood behaviour problems initial bivariate differences in adult outcomes
showed any associations with help-seeking between mainstream and specially educated
(P l 0n07). groups were reduced to non-significance (P 
The data on service contacts also made it 0n2) by controls for intake variations between
possible to examine attributable fractions for the two school sectors. Some marginal asso-
service use associated with MMR across the ciations with school placement did, however,
cohort as a whole. As shown in Table 1, rates of remain on three indicators : housing tenure (P l
psychiatric and other specialist service contacts 0n10), earlier age at first child among women
were much elevated in the MMR sample in (P l 0n09), and Malaise scores (P l 0n04). In
childhood, especially among boys ; based on each case, young people placed in special schools
these data, the percentage of childhood service had fared less well than their peers in mainstream
use attributable to MMR was 11n1 % for boys schooling.
and 3n4 % for girls. In adulthood, service use
among men with MMR was much reduced. DISCUSSION
Among women, rates of contact increased over
those reported in childhood, but to a lesser Mental retardation has been identified as a
extent than for women in the comparison group. much under-researched area in relation to the
Service use attributable fractions thus fell be- size of the clinical burden involved (Medical
tween childhood and adulthood for both sexes. Research Council, 1993). Recent estimates sug-
gest that some 1n6 million people in the UK are
Special and mainstream schooling likely to be affected, the great majority showing
Finally, in view of the widespread debates on mild mental impairments comparable with those
the advantages and disadvantages of mainstream examined here. Among the many aspects of this
schooling for children with mild mental impair- field requiring fuller attention, developmental
ments (see e.g. Howlin, 1994), it seemed im- studies, and especially those tracing longer-term
portant to attempt some assessment of the patterns of adaptive functioning, have been
extent to which adult outcomes within the MMR especially sparse.
sample varied according to the type of school Against that background, this study was
cohort members had attended. As it was clear designed to capitalize on data collected in the
that special schools had admitted some of the course of the National Child Development Study
most severely impaired children within the to characterize key aspects of adult functioning
Mild mental retardation 363

in MMR samples, and to provide some initial constructed to allow not only for main effects of
pointers to the childhood influences that might these factors, but also for possible variations in
be involved. Before discussing the findings, it is their impact between the two main study groups.
important to consider both the strengths and the Our first aim was to contrast the MMR and
limitations of the NCDS database for a study of comparison samples on a range of measures of
this kind. On the positive side, it allowed for the adult status and functioning. With few excep-
identification of a relatively sizeable sample of tions, individuals with MMR appeared to be
children functioning intellectually within the facing considerably elevated rates of difficulty.
MMR range, and provided markers of their For many, living circumstances and social
adaptation in a range of aspects of adult conditions in adulthood were poor, and potential
functioning. Almost all previous follow-up stressors high. Many of the women had married
studies have focused on specially educated or and begun childbearing at very young ages ; by
referred samples, and few have included non- their early thirties they were markedly more
retarded comparison groups. NCDS offered the likely to be caring for large families than the
major advantage of providing data on the majority of their peers, and to be reliant on
important group of mainstream-educated chil- welfare benefits to supplement or maintain their
dren with MMR and of allowing for com- incomes. In terms of social support, most women
parisons with non-retarded samples throughout in the cohort saw friends as key resources at
the analyses. times of emotional or relationship problems.
Alongside these strengths, two main limi- Women with MMR were significantly less likely
tations must also be borne in mind in interpreting to report supports of this kind, and their levels
the findings. First, individual IQ tests were not of emotional distress at the time of the adult
available within the data-set, and we relied interviews were exceptionally high.
instead on results from a group-based general For men, outcome profiles differed in some
ability test to distinguish the MMR and non- important ways, most obviously in relation to
retarded comparison groups, and on educational indicators of independent living. Almost a
placement records to exclude those with severe quarter of the men with MMR were living with
impairments. More refined approaches to group their parents in their early thirties, and regardless
definition would undoubtedly have been de- of living situation, men in the MMR sample
sirable. In practice, however, the rate of MMR were more likely than others to see their parents
identified using this approach (20n4 children per as key sources of social support. On the measures
1000) fell well within the range of estimates available in NCDS, overt labour market diffi-
reported in the more satisfactory epidemio- culties in the MMR sample were confined to two
logical studies undertaken to date (Roeleveld et main areas : men with MMR were more likely to
al. 1997) and the childhood correlates of MMR have faced multiple periods of unemployment
closely paralleled those identified by previous than their peers in their twenties and early
investigations. The second limitation concerned thirties, and a small but higher proportion had
loss of data. Subject attrition, along with the remained completely out of the labour market
requirement for complete data on measures throughout this period. Finally, like their female
from a number of study sweeps, and from counterparts, men with MMR showed much
different reporters, considerably reduced the increased risks of psychiatric morbidity in
numbers available for the main analyses, par- adulthood.
ticularly among members of the MMR sample. Mild mental retardation is widely charac-
We identified a series of factors that differ- terized as cultural\familial in origin, with social
entiated complete from incomplete data cases : disadvantage and genetic factors assumed to
cognitive ability, male gender, family disruption play key contributory roles. The strong overlaps
and disadvantage in childhood, restricted par- between MMR and social adversity in childhood
ental education, and childhood behaviour prob- raise important questions about the basis for
lems. These variables affected the availability of problems in adult functioning, and the extent to
data in the MMR and comparison groups in which poor adult outcomes might be attributable
broadly similar ways. Nonetheless, the weights to socially disadvantaged conditions in early
used to take account of sample attrition were life. Comparisons between MMR and non-
364 B. Maughan and others

retarded samples in NCDS suggested that factors suggest that sensory\neurological problems,
of this kind played a non-trivial, but by no along with early adversity, played significant
means predominant, role. Taking account of the roles in both between and within-group differ-
higher rates of sensory and neurological prob- ences, but nonetheless accounted for only modest
lems within the MMR sample, measures of proportions of the risks observed. Perhaps
childhood social disadvantage (low social class, surprisingly, continuities from teacher-rated be-
large family size, poor housing circumstances haviour problems in childhood showed little
and family breakdown) accounted for some impact in the within-group analyses of current
20–30 % of differences between the MMR and symptomatology. To an extent, this may have
non-retarded groups across a range of adult reflected methodological limitations : the BSAG
outcome measures. The main exceptions were teacher ratings, although widely used at the time
indicators of adult social support in both sexes, of the childhood studies, undoubtedly provided
and lack of independent living among the men, less sensitive indicators of childhood behaviour
both of which seemed relatively unrelated to problems than more recently developed assess-
early disadvantage. ment tools. More generally, however, it seems
Probably the most striking aspect of the clear that more detailed studies of the factors
findings concerned the high rates of emotional that contribute to the greatly elevated risks of
distress reported by both men and women with emotional distress among young adults with
MMR in adult life. A considerable body of mild mental impairments are greatly needed.
evidence suggests that adults with mental re- Measures of service use highlighted other
tardation are at increased risk of emotional issues. Between group differences in reports of
disorders (Borthwick-Duffy, 1994). To date, contacts with health and psychiatric services
however, few studies have been able to provide were much reduced between childhood and
direct comparisons with similar assessments in adulthood, but with little indication that levels
non-retarded samples. Our results suggest that of distress had moderated in parallel ways. In
in both relative and absolute terms, individuals childhood, attributable risks for service use
with MMR are likely to experience high rates of associated with MMR were considerably el-
affective symptomatology in adulthood. So far evated ; similar estimates in adulthood showed
as we could ascertain, these findings are unlikely barely any increased risk among women and
to reflect methodological artefacts. Other invest- lower than expected rates among men. These
igators (Lindsay et al. 1994 ; Manikam et al. contrasts need to be seen against the background
1995) have shown that adolescents and adults of levels of contact with psychiatric and health
with mild–moderate mental impairments are services at different developmental periods in the
able to provide consistent reports of feelings of cohort as a whole. By age 11, for example, under
depression and anxiety on self-report instru- 5 % of girls in the comparison group had been
ments similar to the Malaise Inventory, and referred to child guidance or other psychiatric
Malaise scores showed no systematic relation- services, but in early adulthood some 10 % of
ships with variations in intellectual ability within women reported referral to a specialist for mental
the MMR sample studied here. The high rates of health problems and much larger proportions
emotional distress reported also stood in marked had consulted their GPs. Among men, rates of
contrast to cohort members’ reports of alcohol specialist referral were higher in childhood and
problems, which were in no sense elevated in the were maintained at broadly similar rates in adult
MMR sample. life. Within the MMR samples, the findings
To date, the mechanisms underlying increased followed very different trends, suggesting that
risks for psychiatric disorder among individuals relative rates of access to service use declined
with MMR remain little understood ; brain across development. We can only speculate on
pathology, early family adversity, feelings of reasons for this pattern. Under-reporting of
inadequacy in educational and social roles, service contacts by individuals with MMR
intrinsic associations between cognitive and cannot, of course, be excluded. In addition,
behavioural\emotional functioning, and genetic however, it seems likely that referrals from
factors are among the most widely-canvassed teachers and others constituted an important
possibilities (Simonoff et al. 1996). Our findings avenue for access to services in childhood ; lack
Mild mental retardation 365

of comparable ‘ third party ’ initiators of service Butler, N. R. & Bonham, D. G. (1962). Perinatal Mortality.
Livingstone : Edinburgh.
contact in adulthood may have contributed in Davie, R., Butler, N. & Goldstein, H. (1972). From Birth to Seven.
no small way to the relatively low levels of help- The Second Report of the National Child Development Study.
seeking reported in adult life. Longman in association with the National Children’s Bureau :
London.
As noted at the outset, a number of commen- Douglas, J. W. B. (1964). The Home and the School. MacGibbon &
tators have argued that the school years may Kee : London.
prove the most demanding for individuals with Dunn, G. (1997). Compensating for missing data in psychiatric
surveys. Epidemiologia e Psichiatrica Sociale 6, 159–162.
MMR, and that problems in adaptive func- Edgerton, R. B. (ed.) (1984). Lives in Process : Mildly Retarded
tioning may decrease as environmental demands Adults in a Large City. AAMD : Washington, DC.
become more flexible in adult life. Our findings Ferri, E. (ed.) (1993). Life at 33 : The Fifth Follow-up of the National
Child Development Study. National Children’s Bureau : London.
suggest a different, and in many ways less Fogelman, K. (1980). NCDS Tests : Reliability and Other Aspects of
optimistic, conclusion. For many individuals Reading and Mathematics Tests at 11 and 16 Years. NCDS User
with MMR, the demands of the school years Support Group. Mimeograph.
Fogelman, K. (1983). Growing up in Great Britain. Macmillan :
seem likely to be replaced by other stressors in London.
adulthood, new challenges presenting themselves Granat, K. & Granat, S. (1978). Adjustment of intellectually below-
at each new life-stage. From a theoretical average men not identified as mentally retarded. Scandinavian
Journal of Psychology 19, 41–51.
perspective, our findings argue the need for Howlin, P. (1994). Special educational treatment. In Child and
more detailed studies of the developmental Adolescent Psychiatry : Modern Approaches, 3rd edn. (ed. M.
pathways involved here. From a practice view- Rutter, E. Taylor and L. Hersov), pp. 1071–1088. Blackwell
Scientific : Oxford.
point, appropriate and accessible supportive Huber, P. J. (1967). The behaviour of maximum likelihood estimates
services in adulthood would appear to be urgent under non-standard conditions. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley
needs for many individuals with MMR. Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability 1, 221–233.
Jacobson, J. W. (1982). Problem behavior and psychiatric impairment
within a developmentally disabled population : I. Behavior fre-
This study was supported by a grant from the Medical quency. Applied Research in Mental Retardation 3, 121–139.
Research Council (G.9538070). We are grateful to King, M. (1986). At risk drinking among general practice attenders.
Peter Shepherd and colleagues from the Social Validation of the CAGE questionnaire. Psychological Medicine 16,
213–217.
Statistics Research Unit, City University for ad- Koller, H., Richardson, S. A. & Katz, M. (1988 a). Marriage in a
ditional help with the NCDS database ; to our young adult mentally retarded population. Journal of Mental
colleagues Robert Goodman and Emily Simonoff for Deficiency Research 32, 93–102.
their comments and advice during the course of the Koller, H., Richardson, S. A. & Katz, M. (1988 b). Peer relationships
study ; to Denise Shields for help in preparation of the of mildly retarded young adults living in the community. Journal
of Mental Deficiency Research 32, 321–331.
manuscript ; and to Michael Rutter, whose concern Kushlick, A. & Blunden, R. (1974). The epidemiology of mental
for a better understanding of the needs of individuals subnormality. In Mental Deficiency : the Changing Outlook 3rd
with mental impairments provided the main stimulus edn (ed. A. M. Clarke and A. D. B. Clarke), pp. 31–41. Methuen :
for this work. London.
Lindsay, W. R., Michie, A. M., Baty, F. J., Smith, A. H. W. &
Miller, S. (1994). The consistency of reports about feelings and
emotions from people with intellectual disability. Journal of
REFERENCES Intellectual Disability Research 38, 61–66.
MacMillan, D. L. (1982). Mental Retardation in School and Society,
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical 2nd edn. Little, Brown : Boston.
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric
MacMillan, D. L., Gresham, F. M. & Siperstein, G. N. (1993).
Association : Washington, DC.
Conceptual and psychometric concerns about the 1992 AAMR
Birch, H. G., Richardson, S. A., Baird, D., Horobin, G. & Ilsley, R.
definition of mental retardation. American Journal on Mental
(1970). Mental Subnormality in the Community : A Clinical and
Retardation 98, 325–335.
Epidemiological Study. Williams and Wilkins : Baltimore.
Borthwick-Duffy, S. A. (1994). Epidemiology and prevalence of Manikam, R., Matson, J. L., Coe, D. A. & Hillman, N. (1995).
psychopathology in people with mental retardation. Journal of Adolescent depression : relationships of self-report to intellectual
Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62, 17–27. and adaptive functioning. Research in Developmental Disabilities
Borthwick-Duffy, S. A. & Eyman, R. K. (1990). Who are the dually 16, 349–364.
diagnosed ? American Journal on Mental Retardation 94, 586–595. Mayfield, D., McCleod, G. & Hall, P. (1974). The CAGE
Brick, J. M. & Kalton, G. (1996). Handling missing data in survey questionnaire. Validation of a new alcohol screening instrument.
research. Statistical Methods in Medical Research 5, 215–238. American Journal of Psychiatry 131, 1121–1123.
Broman, S., Nichols, P. L., Shaughnessy, P. & Kennedy, W. (1987). Medical Research Council (1993). Biological Psychiatry Field Review.
Retardation in Young Children : A Developmental Study of Cognitive Medical Research Council : London.
Deficit. Lawrence Erlbaum : Hillsdale, NJ. Penrose, L. S. (1963). The Biology of Mental Defect. Sidgwick &
Bruininks, R. H., Thurlow, M. L., McGrew, K. S. & Lewis, D. R. Jackson : London.
(1990). Dimensions of community adjustment among young adults Pickles, A., Dunn, G. & Vasquez-Barquero, J. L. (1995). Screening
with intellectual disabilities. In Key Issues in Mental Retardation for stratification in two-phase epidemiological surveys. Statistical
Research (ed. W. I. Fraser), pp. 435–448. Routledge : London. Methods in Medical Research 4, 73–89.
366 B. Maughan and others

Potter, F. J. (1988). Survey of procedures to control extreme sampling Rutter, M., Tizard, J., Yule, W., Graham P. & Whitmore, K. (1976).
weights. Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods, Research Report : Isle of Wight Studies 1964–1974. Psychological
American Statistical Association pp. 453–458. Medicine 6, 313–332.
Richardson, S. A. & Koller, H. (1992). Vulnerability and resilience in Scott, S. (1994). Mental retardation. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry :
adults who were classified as mildly mentally handicapped in Modern Approaches 3rd edn (ed. M. Rutter, E. Taylor and L.
childhood. In Vulnerability and Resilience in Human Development Hersov), pp. 616–646. Blackwell Scientific : Oxford.
(ed. B. Tizard and V. Varma), pp. 102–123. Routledge and Keegan Simonoff, E., Bolton, P. & Rutter, M. (1996). Mental retardation :
Paul : London. genetic findings, clinical implications and research agenda. Journal
Richardson, S. A. & Koller, H. (1996). Twenty-two Years. Causes and of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37, 259–280.
Consequences of Mental Retardation. Harvard University Press : StataCorp (1997). Stata Statistical Software : Release 5.0. Stata
Cambridge. Corporation : College Station, TX.
Richardson, S. A., Koller, H. & Katz, M. (1985 a). Continuities and Stott, D. H. (1966). The Social Adjustment of Children. Manual to the
change in behavior disturbance : a follow-up study of mildly Bristol Social Adjustment Guides 3rd edn. University of London
retarded young people. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 55, Press : London.
220–229. Wadsworth, M. E. J., Mann, S. L., Rodgers, B., Kuh, D. J. L.,
Richardson, S. A., Koller, H. & Katz, M. (1985 b). Relationship of Hilder, W. S. & Yusuf, E. J. (1992). Loss and representativeness in
upbringing to later behavior disturbance of mildly mentally
a 43 year follow-up of a national birth cohort. Journal of
retarded young people. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 90,
Epidemiology and Community Health 46, 300–304.
1–8.
Weaver, T. R. (1946). The incidence of maladjustment among mental
Richardson, S. A., Koller, H. & Katz, M. (1988). Job histories in
defectives in military environments. American Journal of Mental
open employment of a population of young adults with mental
Deficiency 51, 238–246.
retardation : I. American Journal on Mental Retardation 92,
483–491. Wedge, P. (1969). The second follow-up of the National Child
Richardson, S. A., Katz, M. & Koller, H. (1993). Patterns of leisure Development Study. Concern 3, 34–39.
activities of young adults with mild mental retardation. American Widaman, K. F., Borthwick-Duffy, S. A. & Little, T. D. (1991). The
Journal on Mental Retardation 97, 431–442. structure and development of adaptive behaviors. International
Robins, L. N. (1978). Sturdy childhood predictors of adult antisocial Review of Research in Mental Retardation 17, 1–54.
behavior : replications from longitudinal studies. Psychological World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of
Medicine 8, 611–622. Mental and Behavioural Disorders : Clinical Descriptions and
Roeleveld, N., Zielhuis, G. A. & Gabree$ ls, F. (1997). The prevalence Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO : Geneva.
of mental retardation : a critical review. Developmental Medicine Yule, W. (1968). Identifying maladjusted children. Paper presented at
and Child Neurology 39, 125–132. ‘ The Child and the Outside World ’, 29th Biennial Conference of
Ross, T. T., Begap, M. J., Dondis, E. H., Gianpiccolo, J. S. & the Association for Special Education, Coventry, August 1968.
Meyers, C. E. (1985). Lives of the Mentally Retarded : A Forty-year Zoccolillo, M., Pickles, A., Quinton, D. & Rutter, M. (1992). The
Follow-up Study. Stanford University Press : Stanford, CA. outcome of childhood conduct disorder : implications for defining
Rutter, M., Tizard, J. & Whitmore, K. (eds.) (1970). Education, adult personality disorder and conduct disorder. Psychological
Health and Behaviour. Longman and Green : London. Medicine 22, 971–986.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi