Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 16

Acquisition in the Natural Approach: The Binding/Access Framework

Author(s): Tracy David Terrell


Source: The Modern Language Journal, Vol. 70, No. 3 (Autumn, 1986), pp. 213-227
Published by: Blackwell Publishing on behalf of the National Federation of Modern Language Teachers
Associations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/326936
Accessed: 30/10/2010 13:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=black.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Blackwell Publishing and National Federation of Modern Language Teachers Associations are collaborating
with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Modern Language Journal.

http://www.jstor.org
Acquisition in the Natural Approach:
The Binding/Access Framework
TRACY DAVID TERRELL

THE NATURAL APPROACH TO LANGUAGE TEACHING hensible input at the appropriate level, the
suggests the incorporation of two sources of grammatical structures and forms will be ac-
knowledge of the target language into the teach- quired in a "natural" order.
ing context.1 Following "second language ac- The purpose of this paper is to explore L2
quisition theory" (henceforth "L2 theory") as theory in more detail in the context of language
formulated by Krashen, the terms "acquisition" teaching in a classroom. I will focus on the ac-
and "learning" are used to refer to these two quisition process, attempting to describe how
sorts of linguistic knowledge.2 Acquisition is I view the functions and interaction of acqui-
the process which is said to lead to subconscious sition and learning for the NA class. Claims in
knowledge about language, a "feel"for correct- this paper are not based on empirical data
ness, while the product of learning is said to gathered in experimental fashion, but rather on
result from conscious attention to some part of personal experiences teaching Spanish with
the target language. Accordingly, in a Natural NA, observing others acquire with NA (Span-
Approach (NA) class, we attempt to provide ish, French, Dutch, German, Greek, Japanese,
the students with opportunities for both sorts Mandarin Chinese), and introspection of my
of language experiences. own recent experiences in NA Greek classes
Emphasis in NA is given to acquisition. This here in the US and in Greece. No claims are
decision is based on L2 theory. Krashen hy- made for those who acquire languages in natu-
pothesizes that fluency, specifically the produc- ral acquisition environments, or for students
tion of utterances, depends primarily on ac- using methodologies other than NA.
quired knowledge, while learned knowledge is
useful to a speaker mainly as a "monitor," a DEFINITIONS
mental processor which enables the speaker to
I take the acquisition of a form of the tar-
make corrections in an utterance before and as
it is spoken. According to this view, most get language to be the process which leads to
the ability to understand and produce that form
speech production is based on acquired knowl-
correctly in a communicative context.4 A "form"
edge, since speakers are not able to monitor to
may be monomorphemic (tree), polymor-
any great degree while engaged in normal con-
versation. Thus even if some rule of grammar phemic (running), or grammatical (the). I will
consider a form to be acquired only when it can
is explained, practiced, and "learned" in a lan-
be both understood and produced. This posi-
guage class, this knowledge is not readily avail- tion coincides with the "working" definition of
able to most speakers in normal conversation.
Krashen maintains that acquisition takes acquisition by second language researchers.5
Consequently, I consider first the development
place whenever the acquirer interacts with com- of listening comprehension skills and then pro-
prehensible input in the target language.3 Ac- ceed to speaking skills.
cording to this hypothesis it is not necessary to
"program" specific grammar points in a lesson LISTENING COMPREHENSION
since, if the acquirer receives enough compre-
The ability to comprehend utterances in the
input can be examined by looking at two com-
The Modern Language Journal, 70, iii (1986)
ponents: 1) the use of context-dependent strate-
0026-7902/86/00003/213 $1.50/0
?1986 The Modern Language Journal gies to determine meaning of unknown forms
in the input; 2) the association of meaning with
214 TracyDavid Terrell
these new forms. These component skills inter- maintain the focus on the comprehension of
act and depend on each other as the ability to input.
comprehend grows. Let us first examine the ConcreteAssociation. The activities in the first
process which associates meaning with form. two stages of NA language acquisition always
include the possibility of making concrete asso-
ciation within a meaningful context. The class
activities include Asher's "Total Physical Re-
BINDING
sponse" (association with body movement),
Binding is the term I propose to describe the input centered on visuals (usually magazine
cognitive and affective mental process of link- pictures or other realia), and input centered on
ing a meaning to a form. The concept of bind- characteristics of the students themselves.8
ing is what language teachers refer to when they Speech Techniques.NA instructors draw stu-
insist that a new word ultimately be associated dents' attention to key words by means of
directly with its meaning and not with a trans- various speech techniques: by pronouncing
lation.6 In our native language we bind forms words slowly, by varying the tone and volume
to their meaning so strongly that most of us of the voice, by reentry, by pointing out a
never reflect on the fact that meaning and form word's referent with each use, and, of course,
are linked arbitrarily. by liberal use of gestures and other sorts of body
The binding of meaning with form clearly language. In addition, the focus during all ac-
is experiential in the development of our first tivities is on the meaning of the utterances in
language. For example, a child playing in the the input.
yard comes across a flower. Mother provides Students in NA classes have reported various
input such as: "Isn't that a pretty flower? Do strategies for associating meaning with form.
you like the flower?" (Child pulls off the blos- For example, in a demonstration lesson of
som.) "No, don't pick the flower. We are sup- Greek, I introduced the word paputsia (shoes)
posed to look at flowers, not pick them." (Child pointing to my own and then later to pictures
begins to eat the flower.) "No, don't eat the of shoes.9 The participants later reported a
flower, smell it. Doesn't the flower smell nice? variety of association techniques. A few looked
It's a pretty flower, isn't it?" Through such ex- at the referent and attempted to link the new
periences, the child uses several senses to de- form directly. Others reported using the word
velop the concept "flower" and to begin the papoos as an association. Still others used the
binding of this concept to the correct form in other languages they knew: Spanish pantuflas
its native language. and French pantouffles (slippers). Frequently
The facilitation of binding is a major focus students report that at first they recognize some
of activities in a beginning NA class. Many of words only by the initial syllable, or even by
the specific NA techniques are explicitly de- the initial consonant. Sometimes the length of
signed to create situations and experiences the word helps. In a TPR activity, some stu-
which will aid the student in binding meaning dents reported distinguishing the command
to form. I will review three characteristics of perpatiste (walk) from trekste (run) simply by
NA which appear to facilitate the binding pro- length plus the initial consonant. Thus it ap-
cess. pears that binding takes place in stages for most
Acquisition in Stages. Past descriptions of NA students. At first, a new form is recognized as
emphasize that students in a NA class are "something familiar," but the meaning (or more
allowed to proceed through stages of language accurately, meanings) may not be recalled.
acquisition.' Three important stages for be- Later meaning is slowly recognized. Eventually
ginners are posited: comprehension (prepro- the form is recognized and interpreted within
duction), early speech (one-word responses), the phonological stream. Binding is complete
and speech emergence (sentence production). when the form evokes the meaning without
The existence of a "pre-speech" stage gives the delay and the form finally "sounds like what it
students an opportunity to concentrate on bind- means."
ing without the added pressures for speech. In The binding of form to meaning is not
addition, even when students do start speaking, equivalent to listening comprehension, but
one-word responses are encouraged in order to rather is a building block of it. Listening com-
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 215

prehension includes other, more complex, pro- tors (such as salience and frequency) in the
cesses such as the interpretation of the relation- input. If the form to be accessed is not strongly
ships between elements in an utterance. Thus, bound (for whatever reason, perhaps low fre-
in Stage One (Comprehension) the goals of NA quency in the input), then access will be diffi-
include not only the facilitation of the binding cult. Phonological complexity doubtless plays
of key forms in the input to meaning but also a role. If the form is long and contains several
the development of basic comprehension strate- sounds radically different from those in the
gies including "contextual guessing." The fol- native language, the word will be difficult to
lowing is a translation of typical input in one produce even if it is strongly bound to its mean-
of my early Greek lessons (students' responses ing. For example, I recognize the meaning of
are in parentheses).'0 the word anapsitikd(soft drink) immediately if
Look at this picture. What do we see? A woman? it is used by others, but I have difficulty pro-
(Yes.) Yes, that's right, there is a woman in the ducing it, perhaps, because of length and stress
picture. What is this? (Students shake heads.) This patterns. It is reasonable to assume that facility
is a hat. The woman is wearing a hat. This picture in access is also related to frequency of oppor-
of the woman who is wearing the hat is for Yvonne. tunities to access a specific form in a meaning-
Ok? Now who has the picture of the woman who ful context. This assumption is, in part, a re-
is wearing the hat? (Yvonne.)11 statement of the traditional idea that meaning-
ful practice in speaking leads to increased
We understood key elements of the input by
fluency.
matching the parts of the input corresponding In order to test my hypothesis (that speed of
to the concepts represented by the English
access is related to opportunities to access a spe-
words - look,picture, woman, hat, and who- with
cific form), we carried out an experiment with
the help of contextual cues for meaning. In this
students of German as a foreign language. 14A
way students are encouraged to develop a sort
of "strategic competence," interpreting the group of third-quarter elementary German stu-
dents at the University of California, San
Greek corresponding to Whohas thepictureof the
woman wearing a hat? by focusing on the target Diego, participated in a TPR (Total Physical
Response) activity in which the instructor
language forms for who, woman, hat, and the modeled commands such as Husten Sie (cough),
communicative context.
PfeifenSie (whistle), KlatschenSie einmaldie Hiinde
(clap your hands once). In the first session stu-
dents listened only and carried out the instruc-
SPEECH
tions. In the second session, half of the students
The production of an utterance in the tar- gave the commands to the entire group. Thus,
get language also involves at least two compo- one-half of the group listened only, while the
nents: the ability to express a particular mean- other half was required to access and vocalize
ing with a particular form and the ability to the forms. Later each student was tested indi-
string forms together in appropriate ways. The vidually on his/her ability to access these forms.
former I call access, the latter involves produc- As predicted by Krashen's input hypothesis,
tion strategies. access itself was not a problem: all of the stu-
Access. The term I use to refer to the produc- dents in both groups were able to access most
tion of an appropriate form to express a spe- of the forms. However, speed of access was dif-
cific meaning in an utterance is access.12 Be- ferent. The students who actually gave the
ginning students create utterances by access- commands had consistently shorter access times
ing forms and then stringing them together in than those who had never meaningfully vocal-
utterances.'3 Access does not follow automati- ized the commands. The average time required
cally from binding. Even when a form has been for recall for the silent group was 2.23 seconds,
bound to a meaning and is recognized immedi- while for the speaking group it was 1.68 sec-
ately in the input, students do not necessarily onds. The conclusion is that meaningful com-
produce it automatically and easily. munication experiences in which students are
Facility in access may be controlled by sev- given the opportunity to produce forms which
eral factors. One factor is the strength of the have been (or are in the process of being)
binding process, itself controlled by several fac- bound, should result in increased fluency.
216 TracyDavid Terrell
Three methodological characteristics of NA sary: 1) comprehensible input which results in
aid in the development of access skills: 1) acqui- binding; 2) opportunities to access these bound
sition in stages; 2) reduced response activities; forms to express ideas in meaningful contexts.
3) expansion of short responses. The binding of meaning to monomorphemic
Acquisitionin Stages. As with binding, the de- forms such as uninflected nouns, verbs, and
velopment of good access skills is aided by the adjectives, is relatively simple, both in theory
fact that NA students have different sorts of de- and in classroom practice. The binding of
mands put on them in the early stages of lan- meaning to grammatically complex words and
guage acquisition. In the comprehension stage, to the grammatical morphemes themselves is
access is not demanded at all since all efforts somewhat complicated. Individual students
are directed toward binding a large number of probably vary somewhat in their binding and
words. As binding develops, however, the in- access of words that contain grammatical mor-
structor begins to provide opportunities within phemes. The following remarks are, therefore,
the input for single word responses from the unavoidably speculative and are based again
students. The responses always consist of a on personal experiences in NA classes.
form which has been bound. There are four im-
portant eliciting techniques: 1) yes-no ques-
BINDING GRAMMATICAL MORPHEMES
tions- Is therea dog in this picture? 2) either-or
questions-Is this a dog or a cat? 3) Wh-ques- Morphemes which do not undergo complex
tions - What (Where, When, Who) is this? 4) allomorphy and which refer to a relatively
Open-ended statements - The dog is in the . . . . simple semantic concept are most easily
Reduced-ResponseActivities. Certain sorts of bound. 16EFL and ESL instructors universally
communicative activities require only reduced report that the progressive -ing, when referring
responses from the students. Three used fre- to ongoing action, is quickly bound by their stu-
quently in NA are: 1) open dialogs; 2) inter- dents. The Spanish preposition de (of), which
views; and 3) information searching.'5 indicates possession, is bound quickly and
Expansion of Short Responses. NA instructors easily by most English-speaking students. How-
expand student responses to provide for com- ever, as any ESL teacher affirms, the English
prehensible input. For example, in the inter- verb marker -s seems particularly resistant to
change: Is the dog eating? (No.) What'she doing? binding (and access). Perhaps this resistance is
(Running.) Yes,he'srunning. Whereis he running? because the verb morpheme -s signals no real
(Park.) Yes, he's running in a park-the student meaning, but, rather, marks a co-occurrence
was asked to access the items runningand park restriction with singular NP subjects. This sec-
but not he's, since presumably the former, but tion describes the binding process of grammati-
not the latter, had been bound in previous ac- cal morphemes in NA classes using as examples
tivities. At the same time the exchange allows plural markers on nouns and person-number
for the opportunity to access running,it also pro- endings on verbs. It draws on my experience
vides an opportunity to enhance the binding teaching Spanish to English speakers in the
of he's (and perhaps the, in, and a). United States and on that as a student in an
Access is not equivalent to speech produc- NA Greek class.
tion. Rather, as in the case of binding, it is a Plural. Spanish plural nouns are bound very
building block of production which ultimately quickly by English-speaking students doubtless
involves other skills such as the generation of because the plural marker -(e)s also signals
utterances using the syntax of the target lan- plurality in English. After only two or three ex-
guage as a frame for the forms which have been amples in the input, mesa/mesas(table/tables),
accessed. Unfortunately, we know little about drbol/drboles(tree/trees), students react to forms
the sort of speech production strategies used by ending in -(e)s as plural.
students in NA classes. The plural morpheme on noun modifiers is
The binding and access of forms, as a sum- not bound as quickly although it is formally
mary to this point, are seen as basic processes identical to the plural marker on nouns. Ad-
of both listening and speaking skills - the goals jectives, for example, grande/grandes (big/big-PL)
of an NA class. It follows, then, that for acqui- appear to be processed by attending to the stem
sition to take place, two conditions are neces- for meaning and ignoring the ending. The
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 217

obvious explanation is that speakers of English (speaks), como (I eat), and come (eats) are fre-
have nothing in their language which would quent in the input and bound relatively soon."7
cause them to focus on adjectives as a signal As the number of forms for a single person
of plurality. Thus the concept "big-PL" is new number are bound, say first person singular,
and it takes some time to bind the form grandes the morpheme itself becomes increasingly
to the meaning "b i g-PL," that is, for the word salient. Students who have bound verb forms
grandes to "sound" plural. However, with such as hablo, como, llego, salgo, pongo, vivo,
enough instances of plural adjectives in the estudio,practico,juego, duermo(I + verb meaning)
input, both forms, grande and grandes, are even- subsequently bind the morpheme -o to "I."This
tually bound and interpreted correctly. binding is evidenced by the fact that they can
Plural forms in Greek are more difficult than correctly understand new first person singular
Spanish to bind because several subclasses of forms encountered in the input. Students report
nouns are pluralized with different morphemes. that the binding of -o to "first person singular"
In my own acquisition of Greek, bound I words is fairly rapid, and is quickly followed by the
as either singular or plural according to the con- binding of -mos to "first person plural." The
text in which I first heard them. Thus paptitsia binding of -s to "second person familiar singu-
(shoes) was heard as plural and bound as such lar" takes somewhat longer as does the binding
without knowing exactly what part of the word of -n to "third person plural."
signaled plurality; this word was never heard Anecdotal evidence suggests that the binding
in its singular form. Other words were bound of grammatical morphemes is similar to that
as singular without a corresponding plural of full lexical items. My hypothesis is that in
form: kefdli(head), pdrta(door), taksl'(taxi). Still both cases students begin by binding a com-
others were heard in the input in both forms: plete word to a meaning. Subsequently the
aftokinito (automobile) and aftokinita (auto- grammatical marker becomes salient as a num-
mobiles), yinika (woman) andyindkes (women). ber of words with the same marker are bound.
These double forms were also bound as sepa- This leads to the possibility of binding the
rate lexical items. However, as my experience morpheme itself to the appropriate meaning
with Greek input increased, numerous plural and to the ability to understand new forms
words appeared which ended in -es or -a. Thus which carry the morpheme.
in addition to binding each separate form with
-a or -es, the morphemes themselves were even-
tually bound to the notion of plurality. And BINDING GRAMMATICAL
NA TECHNIQUES
MORPHEMES:

after they were bound, I was able to recognize


them as signaling plurality on new words in the L2 theory predicts accurately that, for natu-
input. I have not completed the acquisition of ral acquirers, grammatical morphemes will be
the rule(s) in Greek for plurality, however, acquired as expected (in a natural order) pro-
since I have not yet bound enough forms to vided that enough comprehensible input is
have "figured out" the restrictions on the dis- available. For example, NA instructors in pri-
tribution of each allomorph. I also cannot mary school ESL, immersion, or bilingual pro-
access and produce all of the plural forms that grams do not usually program the inclusion of
I can understand. In addition I have bound a specific grammatical morphemes in their
few other forms which are plural and which do speech; this task would not be easy even if they
not use these plural markers: prdovlima/provli'mata attempted to control their input to such a
(problem, problems). Presumably more input degree. The situation is different in NA foreign
will result in the acquisition of a generalization language classes for adolescents or adults.
concerning these plural forms also. Four potential problems exist: 1) time; 2)
Person-NumberVerbMorphemes.Beginning NA lack of saliency; 3) redundancy; and 4) the pos-
Spanish students report that at first they do not sibility of comprehension without use of gram-
hear or notice person-number affixes on verbs. matical morphemes. First, the number of in-
Thus hablo (I speak) and habla (he/she/you structional hours in a typical elementary col-
speak(s)) are at first simply variants interpreted lege foreign language course ranges from
as "speak." However, in certain "association" ac- seventy-five to 150. Given random input for
tivities, forms such as hablo (I speak) and habla these 100 or so hours, it is not clear that most
218 TracyDavid Terrell

grammatical morphemes would reappear in the the -o itself is bound to "I"(or using grammati-
input enough to permit complete binding. Even cal terminology to "first-person singular present
worse, the opportunities to access these mor- tense"). Once the morpheme -o is bound, it may
phemes in meaningful contexts are quite be understood (and subsequently produced) on
limited, especially in classes with more than fif- novel forms in the input.
teen or so students. Second, grammatical mor- Another example helps clarify this two-step
phemes are often not salient. Typically, gram- process for the binding of a grammatical mor-
matical morphemes are not stressed and they pheme. In Greek I bound the following verb
are not in salient positions in sentences. They forms to their meaning: mildte(you speak), dxete
are also usually redundant, especially when dis- (you have), pinete (you drink), and so forth. In
course and contextual information is taken into other words, the verb forms I heard and under-
consideration. Finally, students can use global stood in the classroom input were processed
listening strategies, which avoid attending at and stored as words rather than as forms con-
all to grammatical markers, and still under- sisting of a stem and a grammatical morpheme.
stand most of the input. (This had to be the case since additional gram-
In order to minimize the negative impact of matical information was not available to help
these factors, in certain NA activities we try to me analyze these forms.) However, as an in-
make maximal use of important grammatical creasing number of these forms with similar
morphemes. Examples of these teaching tech- affixes are stored, the binding of -te as a mean-
niques are given below for verbal person-num- ingful unit begins. Suppose now that I have
ber morphemes in Spanish and Greek. bound a form like fordi (he/she wears). Later
Spanish verb forms can give rise to multiple I hear a form fordite(you wear) and recognize
and complex contrastive patterns; in addition that it refers to "you." Given enough of these
to person-number forms, one must bind tense, opportunities to interpret new forms with -te,
aspect, and mood morphemes to their mean- I will be able to complete the binding of the
ings. It is not realistic to think that students will morpheme -te to its meaning "second person."
bind (and later skillfully access) the forty or so My hypothesis concerning the binding of
forms of each Spanish verb simply from listen- meaning to grammatical morphemes is that
ing to random input for the one hundred or so complex items are first processed by students
hours of a typical elementary Spanish course. in an NA class as simple words. Then, as the
On the other hand, the memorization of verb number of words containing the same gram-
paradigms most certainly guarantees neither matical morpheme increases, they are able to
binding nor access. attach meaning to the morpheme itself. The
NA activities use a two-step approach in- teaching approach of NA is to give opportuni-
tended to create optimal conditions for the ties to hear the same suffix used repeatedly in
binding of verb suffixes. The first step consists a communicative context in which meanings
of giving input which allows binding meaning can be clearly and easily associated with the
to a number of words containing the same mor- stem and the suffix.18
pheme. Assume the topic of the input in a be-
ginning Spanish class to be "daily activities of ACCESS OF GRAMMATICAL MORPHEMES
the instructor." The teacher describes his/her
daily routine and the students understand Let us now consider forms containing gram-
forms like me levanto(I get up), me baifo(I bathe), matical morphemes from the point of view of
desayuno(I eat breakfast), trabajo(I work), etc. their access.'19Apparently at first NA students
The fist step is the binding of meaning to the assign a general meaning to several forms. For
polymorphemic form, i.e., the students bind example, tengo(is) and tiene (3s) are bound to
the meaning which includes "I" to the entire "have"with no person-number association. This
form. This task is relatively simple since first- incomplete binding is possible since, in most
person singular forms are concentrated in the utterances, even in cases of subject deletion in
input because of the particular topic chosen Spanish, the subject of the sentence is well de-
("What I do each day"). As the number of forms fined by context. However, when access is re-
including this same morpheme increases, step quired of forms for which binding is not com-
two is carried out: the forms are "analyzed," and plete, students produce utterances like *yo tiene
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 219

tarea(*I has work) as well as the correctyo tengo notion of the acquisition of speech in natural
tarea. In addition, it appears that these kinds stages, and certain instructional techniques
of agreement errors persist even after the per- which provide naturally for the concentration
son-number affixes are bound-that is, after of grammatical morphemes in communicative
students consistently recognize the meaning of activities, are aimed specifically at developing
tengo (I have) and tiene (has). The persistence binding and access-i.e., the basic building
of these sorts of morphological errors supports blocks of acquisition.
the notion that access does not automatically
L2 THEORY: THE INSTRUCTOR'S VIEW
follow from binding.
A complicating factor is the existence of Krashen's L2 acquisition theory is based pri-
grammatical agreement. The task of the stu- marily on research which looks at language ac-
dent is not just to recognize and produce singu- quisition in natural environments (accom-
lar and plural verb forms which correspond to panied or not by classroom instruction). How-
the meaning they want to express, but to access ever, certain problematic areas exist in L2 ac-
them attending to co-occurrence restrictions, quisition theory when applied to classroom in-
i.e., hablan(speak-pl) if the subject is Juan and struction- especially to foreign language in-
Carmen. The final stages of the acquisition of struction. I will examine four of them and in
the rule of person-number agreement in such each case show how Krashen's acquisition
cases may be seen as the sorting out of the framework, which includes the concepts of
access of correct forms at the same time. 20Pre- binding and access as proposed here, resolves
sumably as the students process input, the these problems.
binding of grammatically complex forms be- Vocabulary.L2 researchers often talk about
comes increasingly precise. Then with experi- "the acquisition of a second language," but "ac-
ence in producing forms in meaningful con- quisition" in L2 research and theory has been
texts, the access of correct forms is both faster applied mostly to morphology, syntax, and
and more correct. sometimes phonology. Indeed, most of the re-
search Krashen cites to support L2 theory con-
PROMOTING CORRECT LEXICAL ACCESS sists of the studies which focus on the acquisi-
IN NA CLASSES
tion of particular morphemes such as English
As in the case of binding, in order to foster -ing, verbal -s, plural -s, the copula, and so
correct access we attempt to create situations forth. Some syntactic studies of areas such as
in which certain morphemes are concentrated sentence negation and relative clause formation
in an activity in a single meaning-form rela- do exist.23 Increasing attention is currently
tionship.21 In an interview activity, for ex- being paid to the acquisition of discourse skills.
ample, the student might be asked a series of On the other hand almost no work has been
questions like: Wheredidyou go? Who didyou see? done on the acquisition of simple monomor-
What didyou do? Didyou eat? Wheredidyou work? phemic words. This lack of focus on the acqui-
The answer to all of these questions can involve sition of simple vocabulary items is somewhat
a verb form in the first-person singular of the peculiar from the point of view of the instruc-
past tense. Students report that communicative tor of an elementary L2 class. And, of course,
experiences in which they are given opportuni- from the point of view of the beginning students
ties to access many forms with the same form- trying to communicate with a native speaker
function relationship facilitates the access pro- of the target language, the most important com-
cess itself.22 ponent of that language is its lexicon. In the
discussion to this point, I have hypothesized
SUMMARY
that the concepts of binding and access, hence
To this point we have attempted to examine acquisition, are applicable both to monomor-
the acquisition process in NA class activities. phemic lexical items and to grammatical mor-
We have looked at listening comprehension and phemes. Indeed, most of the activities in the
speech production, and at certain components first two stages in an NA class are designed spe-
of these skills: binding, access, listening strate- cifically to focus on the binding and access of
gies, and production strategies. I have sug- key nouns, verbs, and adjectives.
gested that characteristics of NA, especially the Speech in the NA Class. L2 researchers nor-
220 TracyDavid Terrell
mally measure acquisition by some criterion of almost all exercises are designed to force the
production. We say, for example, that speakers students to produce forms they have had little
have acquired the -ing morpheme when they or no opportunity to bind. However, the effect
produce it in ordinary speech at or above, say, of forced production of unbound forms on the
a ninety percent accuracy level in obligatory binding process itself remains an open ques-
occasions.24 Krashen's Input Hypothesis claims tion. Certainly non-meaningful production
that acquisition results from understanding would not have any effect on binding. On the
messages. Theoretically, one could make the other hand, the production of unbound items
claim that acquisition takes place without any in a meaningful context by one student might
speech production. Such a claim would have serve as comprehensible input to other students
no practical implications since the principal and aid the latter in the binding process. How-
source of comprehensible input must be oral ever, it is not clear that the mechanical produc-
interaction with speakers of the target lan- tion of unbound forms, the goal of most gram-
guage. Native speakers are not usually known matical exercises, has more than a minimal
to pursue conversation with those who do not effect on binding for the student who produces
or cannot respond. Within the binding-access them.
framework proposed here, acquisition is de- Because of our skepticism regarding the
fined as both the ability to comprehend and to value of production of unbound forms, NA ac-
produce meaningful utterances in the target tivities are designed to give the students oppor-
language. By definition then, acquisition is com- tunities to bind forms before being asked to
plete only when the student can both compre- access and produce them. In any natural inter-
hend and produce the target language. It fol- change, however, students inevitably try to ex-
lows that comprehensible input is the primary ceed their range of competence and attempt to
source for acquisition, since comprehensible access and produce words not yet bound. This
input provides both the opportunities and the attempt is, in part, necessary in order to de-
data for the binding process and the develop- velop strategic competence. Thus some access
ment of listening comprehension strategies. of unbound forms will be found in any NA class
However, although acquisition begins with activity. However, if one adheres to the prin-
comprehensible input, students must have ciple of "stages"of acquisition described above,
opportunities to access and produce linguistic most language the students will be asked to pro-
elements which have been bound. Within the duce will have been bound and the occasional
binding-access framework, this amounts to the access of unbound forms probably does no
claim that the student must have real two-way harm. In any case comprehensible input, not
conversational experiences for complete acqui- the production of unbound forms, is the prin-
sition to take place.25 cipal vehicle for binding. And indeed forced
Since the focus of L2 theory and of the first production of forms which have not yet been
two stages of NA is on comprehensible input, bound results in numerous errors and perhaps
teachers frequently ask about the role of speech is a contributing factor to the fossilization of
in acquisition. Within the binding-access inaccurate forms. The remedy for fossilization
framework this question becomes more precise: prescribed by most proponents of "cognitive"
what role does access play in binding? We approaches is error correction. Unfortunately,
assume that increased opportunities for access- whatever the benefits of error correction dur-
ing bound forms will, of course, lead to more ing mechanical drills, direct and "interruptive"
skill in access. However, a different question error correction of grammatical details during
remains: does forcing the student to access meaningful communication clearly disrupts the
words which have not yet been bound, espe- acquisition process.
cially words containing grammatical suffixes, Conscious and Subconscious Knowledge. In
help to bind those elements? It should be noted Krashen's L2 theory (and in the version of the
that this position is precisely that of the "cog- NA presented in Krashen and Terrell's The
nitive" approach in which language lessons are Natural Approach, note 1 below) acquisition is
organized in the following way: 1) explain; 2) a subconscious process and occurs only when
practice; and 3) apply. And, in fact, most texts input is comprehended within a communica-
assume that binding is aided by access since tive context. Learning is a conscious process.
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 221
While merely terms, conscious and subcon- the concept "table." This is also true for adults
scious may be useful for explaining second-lan- in natural acquisition situations. One evening
guage acquisition in a natural environment; in a Greek taverna, I asked the person sitting
they are definitely problematic for the class- next to me what kind of work he did. The
room L2 instructor.26 Basing a definition of ac- answer, stratiotis, meant nothing to me. How-
quisition on this contrast has caused many in- ever, explanation and gestures led me to the
structors to reject L2 theory on the basis that concept (soldier) and even to the place of work
the terms "conscious" and "subconscious" are (Santorini). My efforts to link stratiotisand con-
difficult, if not impossible, to work with. Stu- cept "soldier" were indeed quite conscious and
dents normally pay conscious attention to lan- deliberate.
guage during classroom instruction. If the in- As the students' competence advances, bind-
structor emphasizes acquisition, L2 acquisition ing becomes more precise; other information
theory has it that one is forced to presume much about the word, its range, its connotations, is
of the instruction will be directed toward some- bound either consciously or subconsciously. For
thing happening subconsciously in the minds example, some students consciously note that
of the students; that is not a comfortable stance Spanish esquinarefers only to an exterior corner
for most instructors. In addition, many teachers such as on a street, while rincdnrefers only to
resist the idea that students should not be ex- an interior one such as the corner of a room.
plicitly encouraged to pay attention to the pro- Other students bind this distinction subcon-
duction of "correct"target language. L2 theory sciously, recognize the meaning, and access the
recommends focus only on the message, not on two forms correctly without thinking explicitly
grammar, but while this is presumably the of the distinction.
norm in natural acquisition contexts, it is often Conscious attention also plays a role in NA
difficult to achieve in the foreign language class- activities designed for the binding of gram-
room. matical morphemes. For example, the Spanish
Higgs asserts that the problem stems from plural marker -(e)s is used by English-speaking
a failure to distinguish the process of acquisi- students for interpreting utterances, and this
tion from its product.27 Within the binding- knowledge is usually available for conscious in-
access framework proposed here this problem trospection. What varies among adults is the
is resolved: the product, i.e., acquired lan- level of grammatical sophistication (metalan-
guage, is available for subconscious automatic guage) available to discuss their knowledge.
production. However, this availability does not My claim is that in an NA class various levels
imply that the process of acquisition by adults of conscious attention may be present during
in a classroom must take place entirely on a the binding process itself. Indeed, one could de-
subconscious level. Students in NA classes fine binding as a mixture of conscious and/or
claim that indeed much of the acquisition pro- subconscious attempts at connecting a form
cess appears to take place on a conscious level. with meaning by attending both to the mean-
To see the merit of this claim, let us return to ing and context of the utterance in which the
the concepts of binding and access. I will again form is heard. Keep in mind, however, that
use examples first of monomorphemic words conscious attention to forms does not imply any
and then grammatical morphemes. particular level of grammatical sophistication.
In the previous discussion I imply that items Such conscious attention is also not a prerequi-
of major lexical categories may be bound con- site for the binding of grammatical mor-
sciously; that is, many students may attend phemes.28 My rejection of the conscious/sub-
consciously to the association of target language conscious distinction does not support the so-
forms with their meanings. I even suggest vari- called "communication drills" in which students
ous conscious strategies that students use for are forced to attend to creative communication
the binding process. This conscious binding and monitor grammatical forms at the same
certainly applies to the core denotative mean- time.
ing of an item. If, in a classroom during an EFL What about access and the conscious/sub-
lesson, I point to a table and say to a student conscious distinction? Within a binding-access
This is a table, most language students make a framework we may view the production of
conscious effort to bind the form they hear with utterances as the accessing and arrangement of
222 TracyDavid Terrell
forms using (morpho-)syntactic rules. Adult been present in the input or for whatever rea-
learners in an NA class report that access fre- son it may not have been salient enough to have
quently, but not always, involves conscious been bound. After some thought, the incorrect
mental "searching" for a form to express a par- *venieronmay be created if the student remem-
ticular meaning. This claim seems especially bers the endings which have been memorized,
to hold true with beginning students. And, if or the correct vinieron,if he/she also remembers
access of a particular item has not occurred to make the correct stem vowel change in this
often, that item will often be "hard" to access. form. Finally the student produces Mis amigos
In early speech, many students report that most vinierona visitarme(My friends came to visit me).
access and production of single word responses Thus the student falls back on the rules which
is done consciously. Such was my case in all he/she has learned and creates the form. The
encounters with native speakers in Greece: only result is a correct utterance, but vinieron still
after multiple opportunities to access a given does not "sound" like "they came"; rather, the
word in a communicative context was I able student knows consciously that this form means
to do so without conscious attention. Of course, "they came" to a native speaker of Spanish.
to say that early access may be either conscious One of the reasons monitoring is so difficult
or subconscious does not imply that students is that the speaker has no way to judge output
are focussed only on grammar, but rather that except consciously by rule. Since the forms
they are focussed on accessing a form to express have not yet been bound, they do not neces-
a desired meaning. If the access is for forms sarily "feel"correct. The view of monitoring as
which have been bound, the process is easier. the accessing of unbound forms also predicts
The more experience accessing a particular that the students may generate forms they
form in communicative contexts, the more would not necessarily even understand in a
fluent the speech, and the more subconscious communicative context. This happened fre-
the accessing of particular forms becomes. quently to me in Greece. I would carefully
Monitoring in NA. Using the binding-access memorize a word from my "Greek for tourists"
we may now redefine Monitoring without rely- book and even produce it successfully on occa-
ing on the conscious-subconscious distinction. sion. However, when the same word was used
Krashen defines monitoring as the use of later by natives, I often would not recognize it.
"learned" knowledge to edit our production. Monitoring defined without appealing to the
Presumably, then, the use of any knowledge conscious/subconscious distinction is now
which had been obtained with the aid of con- clearly separate from the notion of "attending"
scious attention would be termed monitoring. to speech. I have assumed that binding occurs
Within the context of the classroom, this defi- when the students attendto the elements of the
nition of monitor use is clearly too broad. Using input, i.e., that they actively try to associate
the binding-access framework it is not neces- meaning and form and subsequently, in their
sary to use the conscious/subconscious distinc- production, try to make the elements in their
tion and we may define monitoring as the output match as closely as possible the input.
access of forms learned (studied, memorized), My claim is that whether this "attention" is pri-
but not yet bound. Thus, monitoringis the use marily conscious and/or subconscious is essen-
of a form which one knows to be correctvia studyand tially irrelevant.29 As long as meaning and form
exercises, but which has not yet been "experienced" are associated experientially, acquisition is tak-
enoughto "soundlike"what it means. For example, ing place.
a student of Spanish may answer the question, In general we would prefer that our students
cQudpasd anoche?(What happened last night?) use their acquired system while speaking. They
by beginning with mis amigos (my friends) . . . would thus comprehend enough bound forms
and they pausing, trying to generate the third- in the utterance to follow the message; in their
person plural form of the past tense of venir speech they would access forms they bound
(come). Although various forms of venir may through input. However, in certain cases, a
have been bound, venir (to come), vengo (I grammatical morpheme may be particularly re-
come), and viene (comes), and perhaps a few sistant to binding (English verbal -s, for ex-
others of the forty or so possible forms, the ample) or the system itself may consist of so
third-person plural past vinieronmay have not many forms that it is difficult to hear each often
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 223

enough to bind the meaning of a particular one. quisition, and it does not becomeacquisition
In these instances, the students who wish to ex- without the necessary comprehensible input.
press themselves using forms not yet completely However, just the fact that the students under-
bound have the choice of either making errors stand that verb forms may vary according to
or monitoring to improve correctness. The verb their subjects may speed up binding and access
affix system of Romance languages is notori- of these forms during subsequent acquisition
ously difficult to bind since thirty to fifty forms activities. The claim then is not that learning
exist for each verb. Each of several thousand becomes acquisition, since binding is still con-
common verb forms must be repeatedly heard ditional based on comprehensible input, but
in a meaningful context to be completely rather that such knowledge may aid in mak-
bound. Indeed, it is not unreasonable to expect ing the forms to be bound and accessed more
that complete binding of these forms takes sev- salient in the input itself.
eral years of high quality input. In these cases, Second, explicit study and learning of new
it is not unreasonable to expect students to forms could aid in the inference of global mean-
monitor their speech to improve grammatical ing from utterances containing a large number
accuracy. Indeed, in The NaturalApproach(note of unbound elements. This sort of learned
1 below), we suggest that the goal of an NA knowledge would seem to aid the acquisition
class is to produce "optimal"monitors, low-level process in a general way even if it does not
monitoring which improves accuracy but does necessarily affect the particular items being
not interfere with communication. studied. Suppose, for example, I learn that
Greek yethira is equivalent to bridge. I may
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LEARNING AND repeat the word aloud several times or con-
IN THE CLASSROOM
ACQUISITION
sciously try to associate something with it to
Various researchers have discussed the rela- help retention. If asked for the Greek equiva-
tionship between acquisition and learning.30 lent for bridge,I can produceyethira in the same
Many, including Brown and Bialystok, for ex- way that tourists often memorize and produce
ample, propose that learning and acquisition phrases such as How muchdoesit cost?However,
are the ends of a single continuum. Still others yethira is not yet bound to the concept repre-
such as Sharwood-Smith hypothesize that sented by English bridge, I simply "know" (ab-
learning aids acquisition.31 Krashen rejects the stractly, theoretically) that it refers to bridge.To
proposition that learning becomes acquisition, bind the concept to the Greek word I still need
while noting the possibility that something to hear yethira used in a meaningful context.
learned may later be acquired.32 Suppose the instructor now tells a story in
I have no evidence to clarify claims and whichyethira is used several times. Since I have
counterclaims for natural second language ac- previously learned the meaning of yethira, the
quisition. However, in the context of NA, the word is salient for me and I will doubtless con-
question is easy to answer: learning in some sciously recognize it when said. And, if I
cases aids acquisition and, in others, impedes visualize a bridge in the context of a story, it
it. Let us look at examples of each. is likely that I will be able to bind the meaning
Most students in an NA class agree that to the word: the formyethira will begin to "sound
some learning via mechanical drills aids the ac- like" its meaning.
quisition process. Learning of grammatical This sort of "prelearning" of vocabulary may
forms and new words might aid acquisition in indeed aid acquisition, but it is not very effi-
two ways. First, learning focusses attention on cient. It is notoriously difficult to bind mean-
a new element so that, when it appears in the ing to form when both are associated arbitrarily
input, it might be recognized and, therefore, in a "vocabulary list" or a mechanical drill. Per-
salient. For example, it appears to be helpful haps such learning affects only short-term
to students of Spanish and French to discuss memory. On the other hand, it is relatively
explicitly the concept of subject-verb agree- simple to bind meaning to form when both are
ment. Such a discussion might be profitably fol- used in communicative contexts. As I noted
lowed up by an exercise to verify comprehen- above, the only possible advantage to prelearn-
sion of the concept. The explanation and exer- ing is that it may in some cases speed up the
cise are aimed at learning. Learning is not ac- binding process by making the new forms
224 TracyDavid Terrell
salient in the input. In NA we do not depend tribution and co-occurrence) may also speed up
much on prelearning; we prefer rather to intro- their acquisition. In Spanish and French the
duce all new items in a communicative context nonstressed natural and preverbal placement
first. However, individual learning styles vary of the clitic pronouns make their acquisition
greatly and some students report "reading quite problematic; most students report that,
ahead" in the text in order to preview new at first, they do not even hear them and simply
material. infer their presence by context. It does not seem
Conscious review of new words and gram- unreasonable that an explanation of their form
matical forms heard only a few times in the and placement followed by short exercises to
input may also speed up the binding process. verify comprehension of the explanation would
In one class period, my Greek instructor be helpful in making these forms more salient
showed a picture of a bay with a bridge at one and thereby more understandable in the input.
end. New words introduced in the input in- However, the study of grammar is more
cluded the Greek equivalents of bridge, bay, problematic than is the learning of simple vo-
ships, clouds, water, buildings, and highway. Each cabulary. In order for the study of grammar
of these words was used several times, and he to be effective, one would want to study a par-
asked questions like: Is therea bridgein this pic- ticular morpheme at the beginning of the bind-
ture? Whereare the ships? How many ships do you ing process. Unfortunately, it is not always
see? What colorare the clouds?Naturally the bind- clear exactly when binding begins. If grammar
ing of each new word was not completed in a is studied too soon, this knowledge may indeed
single class activity. The next day I reviewed interfere with comprehension of utterances and
the new words introduced in the previous class thereby retard the binding process.
period, pronouncing each while trying to During the first hour of my Greek instruc-
visualize its referent. The instructor had given tion, the instructor used visuals, made state-
good comprehensible input at the correct level. ments about the content of pictures, and then
I engaged in conscious study consisting of a asked yes/no questions to verify comprehen-
review of the new words outside of class. I can- sion. To answer I simply had to shake my head
not, of course, prove that the review helped yes or no. I recall a picture of a woman cross-
speed up binding. However, I am convinced ing a street. The instructor stressed the words
that at least for me, consciousstudy during my yineka (woman), forema (dress), and ble (blue) in
review of these words made them more salient; the input as the key lexical items to be bound
and during the following classes, when these in order to comprehend the input. The ques-
words were again used in a meaningful con- tions to verify comprehension were relatively
text, my recognition of their meaning was quick simple, my task mostly being to recognize these
and easy. three phonological shapes among the "noise":
One could argue correctly that the learning Is this a woman? Is this a dress?Is the dress blue?
practice was unnecessary - with enough input One must keep in mind I did not (nor could
the words would have been acquired in any I) attend to the forms corresponding to English
case. While this assertion is probably true (and is, this, or the, nor even wearing, have, and pic-
is most likely the normal way second-language ture. But I was successful in comprehending the
acquirers in natural situations gain new questions and was able to respond correctly to
vocabulary), sufficient class time does not exist them.
for the necessary amount of reentry of each new In this and later input I also began to hear
word; materials which could supply compre- occasionally the word ine. I asked what it
hensible input outside of class are not readily meant. The instructor replied that it is the
available. Learning in the form of moderate third-person singular of the verb to be. One
amounts of (pre- and) post-study may speed up would assume that such knowledge would help
the binding of lexical items in situations in bind since the utterances would contain one less
which there would not be enough time or op- unknown element. Unfortunately, at that time
portunity for the necessary reentry in com- this new knowledge caused me a great deal of
prehensible input.33 trouble because I would subsequently focus on
The (pre- and) post-study of grammatical ine and often miss another more important item
morphemes (and their associated rules of dis- in the input. Thus in a question like Is the dress
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 225

that this woman is wearing blue?I would focus on CONCLUSION

ine and miss fdrema (dress) and was unable to Certain problems in the application of
answer the question. The problem was that, Krashen'sL2 theory to foreign-languageclass-
initially, I was unable to attend to much more room teaching led me to re-examine the func-
than one form per utterance. This stage did not tion of acquisition and learning for language
last long, of course, and within a few class hours students. In this paper I redefine the terms "ac-
I was able to process two or three words per
quisition"and "learning"for the classroomcon-
utterance and ine was bound in due time to text in what I call the binding/accessframework
"copula" as indeed was afto to "demonstrative." for acquisition. The redefinition does not
Although knowledge of target language change the essence of Krashen's L2 theory.
grammar may generally aid in the acquisition However, the new definitionshave advantages
process, the danger for instructors is to stress for the language instructor: 1) they treat both
too much grammar too soon. A common prac- lexical and grammatical acquisition; 2) they
tice in French, Spanish, and German classes
clarifythe relationshipbetween comprehension
is to introduce the forms of the definite and in- and speech in the acquisition process; and 3)
definite articles within the first hour of instruc-
they differentiatebetween acquiredand learned
tion. This practice is a mistake. The recogni-
knowledge without using a conscious-subcon-
tion of articles does nothing to aid comprehen- scious distinction. Within this framework, ac-
sion; and the probability of binding meaning quisition as a process is seen as a mixture of
to the various articles and sorting out their conscious and subconscious attempts at bind-
proper distribution so soon is very low. Spe- ing form and meaning and then accessingthose
cific focus on verb affixes also usually happens forms for a communicative purpose. Learning
too soon; knowledge of verb paradigms plays exercises may in some cases aid the acquisition
little or no role in beginners' comprehension -
process for many students.35
which in early stages depends mostly on the
recognition of the verb stems.34

4Leonard Newmark (personal communication) points out


NOTES
correctly that my focus is quite restrictive: acquisition does
not just occur form by form; rather, acquirers tend to
"chunk," to acquire pieces of routines, patterns, and under-
1Stephen D. Krashen and Tracy D. Terrell, The Natural standing and to produce them through various "approxi-
Approach: Language Acquisition in the Classroom (Oxford: mative" stages. I have chosen to restrict my attention to
Pergamon; Hayward, CA: Alemany, 1983). See also Tracy the acquisition of forms and will not discuss the acquisition
D. Terrell, "A Natural Approach to the Acquisition and of syntax (word order, transformations, etc.) here.
Learning of a Language," Modern Language Journal, 61 5To my knowledge all scholars in second language acqui-
(1977), pp. 325-36; "The Natural Approach to Language sition use this definition. Comprehension is assumed but
Teaching: An Update," Modern LanguageJournal, 66 (1982), not usually measured.
pp. 121-32; Stephen D. Krashen, Tracy D. Terrell, 6My use of binding is related to the Sausurrean concepts
Madeline Ehrman & Martha Herzog, "A Theoretical Basis of signification and unrelated to Chomsky's use of the term
for Teaching the Receptive Skills," ForeignLanguageAnnals, in syntactic theory. See, for example, Noam Chomsky, Lec-
17 (1984), pp. 261-75; Tracy D. Terrell, "The Natural Ap- tures on Governmentannd Binding (Foris: Dorecht, 1981).
proach in Bilingual Education," Schooling and Language 7See notes 1 and 2 above.
Minority Students: A TheoreticalFramework (California State sSee James J. Asher, Learning Another Language through
Department of Education, Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dis- Actions: The Complete Teacher'sGuide (Los Gatos, CA: Sky
semination and Assessment Center, California State Uni- Oaks, 1977), and Krashen & Terrell (note 1 above), pp.
versity, Los Angeles, 1982). 75-84.
2Stephen D. Krashen, Second Language Acquisition and 9I use my own broad phonetic transcription for Greek.
Second Language Learning (Oxford: Pergamon, 1981), and 10The NA Greek class was taught by Evris Tsakirides.
Principles and Practice in SecondLanguageAcquisition (Oxford: "1Superficially these input sequences appear to be no
Pergamon, 1982). more than modified audiolingual drills or grammatical exer-
3Certain other conditions are not directly relevant to this cises. Transcribed speech always seems somewhat strange.
discussion: the acquirer must be focussed on the message, In the case of early input sequences in NA, the repetition
the input must be at a certain level (i + 1), and the acquirer which seems perfectly natural in context "looks funny" when
must have a low "affective filter." See Krashen (note 2 above) written down. However, when used with interesting visuals,
for details. with good intonation patterns and accompanying body lan-
226 TracyDavid Terrell
guage, these simple input sequences are normally quite English. They state explicitly (their note 10) "it is possible
interesting for the students. However, such sequences are that some regular past tense forms, particularly of high fre-
not, and are not meant to be, representative of real com- quency verbs, are also stored in the lexicon, but we have
munication between native speakers of the target language. found no evidence in the current research to support this
Herein lies a difference between the communicative ap- possibility" (p. 268).
proach and NA. In the former, all input texts should be 201 view agreement rules in terms of lexical access, not
representative of natural, communicative events, hence the morpheme addition. In this view los automdvilesnuevos (the
emphasis on authentic texts. In NA, on the other hand, new cars) is produced by accessing three plural forms los,
we aim at input which is a reduced code (teacher talk) automdviles, and nuevos, not by accessing stems and then
parallel to the interaction between a parent and small child adding suffixes while generating an utterance. Lexical access
(motherese) or between a native and a nonnative speaker implies that all forms in a paradigm are stored lexically and
(foreigner-talk) both of which correspond to Krashen's i + 1. accessed as such. In addition, a speaker also stores rules
The Greek sequence, while inauthentic as a text between and can use them to generate novel forms, either those
adult speakers, would be perfectly natural between a mother which have never occurred in the input or those which have
and daughter looking at a picture book. occurred so infrequently that they have not yet been stored
121have defined access in this way so that it will be related lexically (see also note 19 above). In this view, number
directly to acquisition. The term "access"could be used more agreement is a co-occurrence restriction on access of plural
broadly than I have defined it here. Other sorts of access forms.
exist. For example, in audiolingual drills students are forced 21I should, in all fairness, point out that one is not forced
to access forms they are learning; in translation exercises to view the acquisition of grammatical morphemes by for-
students are forced to access words they have not yet eign language students as an important goal. In that case,
studied. Presumably these mental searches would aid learn- no reason would exist to try to control the input. Certainly,
ing more than acquisition. traditional syllabi, texts, materials, and even methodolo-
13For the most part, the syntax of beginning students gies viewed the learning of grammar as the principal (and
appears to be that of their native language. However, there often, only) goal in a foreign language course.
may be simple pragmatic modes in which, for all practical 220ne might object that L2 theory as currently formu-
purposes, there is no syntax but rather a stringing together lated is incompatible with the idea that instructor manipula-
of ideas in the order they are thought of. For some data tion of input can speed up the acquisition of a particular
and discussion, see John Schumann, "Utterance Structure morpheme since these are presumably acquired in a
in Basilang Speech," Pidgen and CreoleLanguages. Essays in "natural" order. However, there are reasons to reject such
Memory ofJohn E. Reinecke, ed. Glenn Gilbert (Ann Arbor: a position. Factors must exist which operate in the acqui-
Karoma, 1983). sition process to cause a natural order of acquisition. Al-
'4Leonora Hoskins, "The Effects of Vocalization on though we do not know exactly what these factors are, we
Recall," unpublished paper read in a graduate seminar at can hypothesize that they might include such things as:
the University of California, San Diego, spring 1983. semantic transparency, allomorphy, usefulness, redun-
'5See Krashen & Terrell (note 1 above), chapter 4. dancy, salience, frequency, and so forth. These factors (or
16E. V. Clark & R. A. Berman, "Structure and Use in others) must interact in rather complex ways to produce
the Acquisition of Word Formation," Language, 60 (1984), the natural order found in so many studies. If these are
pp. 542-90, refer to these properties as "semantic trans- the factors that control the natural order, then it follows
parency" and "formal simplicity." They add two other gen- that acquisition might be speeded up, or the order even
eral principles for the mastery of a word formation reper- changed if we could manipulate some of the controlling
toire: "productivity" and "conventionality." factors. Although such control of input is difficult, it cer-
"'These activities are described in Tracy D. Terrell, tainly is not impossible in a foreign language context: the
Magdalena Andrade, Jeanne Egasse & E. Miguel Mufioz, language instructor often completely controls the input in
Dos Mundos.:Instructor'sEdition (New York: Random House, the target language. The factors most "controllable" are fre-
1986). quency, salience, and usefulness. By radical control of the
'8See also my essay "A Natural Approach to the Teaching input and the topics of the communicative interactions, the
of Verb Forms and Functions in Spanish," ForeignLanguage instructor could conceivably speed up at least the acquisi-
Annals, 13 (1980), pp. 129-36. tion of certain morphemes and in some cases even change
191view the production of grammatically complex forms their natural order of acquisition. The issue is somewhat
(in this case stem + affix) in the following way. Complex uninteresting for language instructors since, in most cases,
forms are stored as complete words in the lexicon, i.e., we do not know what the natural order is anyway and there-
hablamos(we speak) is stored and accessed as a single unit. fore have no way of measuring the effect of the strategies
However, acquirers also formulate (and store) generaliza- proposed here.
tions about grammatically complex words. With these 23Heidi Dulay, Marina Burt & Stephen Krashen, Lan-
generalizations they are able to understand and generate guage Two (Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press, 1982).
forms not previously encountered in the input. This view 24Roger Brown, A First Language (Cambridge: Harvard
is based on Joan L. Bybee & D. I. Slobin, "Rules and Univ. Press, 1973).
Schemas in the Development and Use of the English Past," 25Several colleagues have pointed out that this view of
Language, 60 (1982), Ip. 265-89. However, Bybee & Slobin acquisition implies that even the first (dominant) language
posit this analysis only for irregular past tense forms in is never completely acquired. For example, there exist many
Acquisition in the Natural Approach 227
words we understand but which we do not (or even could 30Michael Long, "Does Second Language Instruction
not) produce easily. Within the binding/access framework, Make a Difference? A Review of Research," TESOL Quar-
these forms would be considered to be only partially ac- terly, 17 (1983), pp. 359-82, examines the effect of formal
quired, i.e., bound, but not accessed. To take another second language instruction on language proficiency and
example, second generation "natives" who can understand, concludes that we have "considerable evidence to indicate
but not produce their "mother" tongue, must be said to have that SL instruction does make a difference." He shows that
only partially acquired that language. the effect of instruction holds: 1) for children as well as
26H. Douglas Brown, "The Consensus: Another View," adults; 2) for intermediate and advanced students, not just
Foreign Language Annals, 17 (1984), pp. 177-80, notes: beginners; 3) on integrative as well as discrete-point tests;
"Psychologists are still pursuing definitions of conscious- and 4) in acquisition-rich as well as acquisition-poor envi-
ness and have not reached any sort of explanatory adequacy ronments. Long claims (p. 374): "Instruction is supposed
in that pursuit - at least none that psychologists can agree to result in learning, defined by Krashen as conscious
on." However, my proposal in this paper to define acqui- knowledge of rules of SL." His study leads him to suggest
sition and learning without appealing to a "conscious-sub- a redefining of the construct learning, which "currently
conscious" dichotomy does not lead in the direction Brown seems too narrow." Although, in principle, I agree with
proposes, namely that acquisition and learning are the final Long's conclusions, my approach in this paper has been
points of a single continuum. the opposite, i.e., a broadening of the concept of acquisi-
27Theodore V. Higgs (San Diego State Univ.), personal tion and a narrowing of what is considered to be learning
communication. (although perhaps a broadening of its role in acquisition).
28My preliminary work with native speakers of English The problem is Krashen's characterization of acquisition
who have acquired Spanish in the Dominican Republic as a subconscious process. If acquisition, as defined in the
without formal instruction or study indicates that conscious binding-access framework, can include conscious attention
binding strategies are used far less by "natural" acquirers to language and language forms, then much of what goes
than by students in a classroom. A typical comment was on in formal instruction can be thought of as an aid to acqui-
"I didn't really make a conscious effort to remember a new sition directly. Therefore, I do not assume that formal lan-
word since I knew that I would hear it many times a day guage classes must necessarily equate with learning
and it would just sink in by itself." (although this is still too often the case in foreign language
29The lack of careful attention to the input may be the classes).
principal cause of fossilization among adult L2 acquirers. 31See H. Douglas Brown (note 26 above); M. Sharwood-
In L1 acquisition, children imitate, first, adults and, later, Smith, "Consciousness-Raising and the Second Language
their peers. This "imitation" also applies to language: chil- Learner," Applied Linguistics, 11 (1981), pp. 159-68.
dren want to "sound like" everyone else around them. I 32See Krashen, 1982 (note 2 above). I do not support
believe that, precisely because of this strong drive toward in either case the position that learning "becomes" acquisi-
imitation, children attend to details in the input which are tion. Learning may indeed aid in binding and in access,
not strictly necessary for the communication of informa- but the conditions posited for acquisition by Krashen's L2
tion. Evidence for this hypothesis comes from children's theory (see note 2 above) still hold even after the form has
awareness of the difference between the input and their pro- been learned, i.e., it must be used in comprehensible input
duction. For example, my four-year-old niece became very in a communicative context such that binding can take place
upset when, using her pronunciation, I asked about her and the student must have ample opportunities to access
trip on the "erplane." She was aware of the difference and produce the form to communicate messages.
between her pronunciation and the pronunciation in the 330n the other hand, time spent on the study of vocabu-
input and she correctly interpreted that I was making fun lary would clearly have been better spent listening to addi-
of her inability to imitate a model. Adults, in most cases, tional comprehensible input. For example, recorded texts
do not have the same strong drive to assimilate completely at my level of competence containing the words used in
and "be like" natives of L2. I do not mean to imply that class would have been much more helpful than simple rote
such an integrative motivation is necessary for perfect L2 review and study. Thus, even though I claim that learning
acquisition, but rather that such motivation produces a can aid acquisition, I do not assume that learning is the
greater "attention" to the input and that this attention (not most desirable way to enhance acquisition for all students
the motivation per se) results in accurate binding and later in all contexts.
facility in access. In "Communicative Competence: Some 34Barry Taylor, "Teaching ESL: Incorporating a Com-
Rules of Comprehensible Input and Comprehensible Out- municative, Student-Centered Component," TESOL Quar-
put in its Development" (presented at the University of terly, 17 (1983), pp. 69-88, offers other reasons for explicit
Southern California, 1983), Merrill Swain makes a similar grammar instruction: 1) to supplement communicative
proposal in her attempt to explain why Canadian French experiences; 2) to make up for the lack of input; 3) to use
immersion students lag behind their native French coun- the target language in the teaching of grammar; and 4) to
terparts despite many years of presumably good compre- satisfy a perceived need of the students.
hensible input. She says: "the claim, then, is that produc- 35I am grateful for comments and suggestions which were
ing the target language may be the trigger that forces the made on early drafts of this paper by Trisha Dvorak, James
learner to pay attention to the means of expression needed Lee, Bill VanPatten, Theodore Higgs, Stephen Krashen,
in order to successfully convey his or her own intended Robin Scarcella, Roni LeBauer, Betty Leeman, Irene
meaning." Daniels, Robert Maple, and Leonard Newmark.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi