Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

Nutritional Neuroscience, Volume 6 Number 5 (October 2003), pp.

269–275

Effects of An Oral Mixture Containing Glycine, Glutamine


and Niacin on Memory, GH and IGF-I Secretion in
Middle-aged and Elderly Subjects
L.I. ARWERT a, J.B. DEIJEN b,* and M.L. DRENT a
a
Department of Endocrinology, VU University Medical Center, de Boelelaan 1117, 1081 HVAmsterdam, The Netherlands;
b
Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Free University, van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands

(Received 26 March 2003; Revised 9 May 2003; In final form 22 July 2003)

Aging is associated with declining activity of the growth In addition, progressive large age-associated cogni-
hormone-insulin-like growth factor-I (GH-IGF-I) axis and tive impairments have been found after the age of 40,
with a decrease in cognitive function. The stimulatory as compared to subjects aged between 20 and 30
effect of an orally administered nutritional supplement,
mainly containing glycine, glutamine and niacin on the years (Woodruff-Pak, 1997). Several studies have
GH-IGF-I axis and on mood and cognition was investi- shown associations between GH, IGF-I and cognitive
gated. Forty-two healthy subjects (14 men and 28 women, function. Besides having somatic effects, growth
aged 40 – 76 years) were enrolled in a randomised, double hormone (GH) also affects (neuro)psychological
blind, placebo-controlled trial. They received 5 g of a functioning. Adults with growth hormone deficiency
nutritional supplement or placebo, twice daily orally for a
period of 3 weeks. At baseline and after 3 weeks, blood (GHD), a state characterized by low GH and low
was collected for measurement of serum GH and IGF-I IGF-I levels, feel less energetic, are emotionally more
levels and mood and cognitive function were tested. The labile, and experience disturbances in sex life and
nutritional supplement ingestion for 3 weeks was found feelings of social isolation at a significantly higher
to increase serum GH levels with 70% relatively to frequency than controls. They are also found to have
placebo, whereas circulating IGF-I levels did not change.
Mean GH (6 SD) increased in this group from 3.23 (64.78) significant cognitive deficits. Adult men, with child-
to 4.67 mU/l (6 5.27) ( p 5 0.03). GH increase was not hood-onset GH deficiency had lower memory
associated with improvement in mood or memory. function compared to controls, which improved to
Correlation analyses, however, revealed that individual normal scores within 1 year of GH replacement
increases in IGF-I, but not GH, were associated with therapy (Deijen et al., 1996; 1998). Moreover, the
improved memory and vigour. It is concluded that an oral
mixture of glycine, glutamine and niacin can enhance GH changes in memory performance are positively
secretion in healthy middle-aged and elderly subjects. correlated to the GH-induced changes in serum
IGF-I concentration. Effects on memory can be
Keywords: Cognition; Glutamine; Glycine; Growth hormone; explained by the presence of a high concentration
Insulin-like growth factor-I; Niacin of GH receptors in the hippocampus, which plays a
major role in memory processes (Nyberg, 2000).
It has also been observed that GH treatment affects
quality of life (QoL) in patients with GHD (Burman
INTRODUCTION et al., 1995). A further support for a relationship
between GHD and impaired cognitive functioning is
It is well known that aging is associated with both a given by studies on IGF-I plasma level and
decrease in cognitive function and declining activity intellectual functioning. For instance, serum IGF-I
of the growth hormone-insulin-like growth factor I concentration in GH-deficient males correlated
(GH-IGF-I) axis. From age 40 to 80 the amount of positively with IQ score and education level.
growth hormone in men progressively decreases. In healthy subjects, IGF-I plasma levels of elderly

*Corresponding author. Tel.: þ 31-20-4448756. Fax: þ 31-20-4448971. E-mail: jb.deijen@psy.vu.nl

ISSN 1028-415X print/ISSN 1476-8305 online q 2003 Taylor & Francis Ltd
DOI: 10.1080/10284150310001612195
270 L.I. ARWERT et al.

men and women were found to be positively GH levels was found after 3 weeks of ingestion
associated with Mini Mental State Examination this food supplement (personal communication
(MMSE) scores (Rollero et al., 1998). Similarly, IGF-I of Dr Rimon, American Medical Laboratories,
levels in elderly healthy men were found to be Herzylia, Israel).
associated with better performance in tests sensitive In the current study, the effect of the adminis-
to the effects of aging, especially speed of infor- tration of an oral nutritional supplement mainly
mation processing (Aleman et al., 1999). In addition, containing glycine, glutamine and niacin on GH and
higher serum total IGF-I levels in healthy elderly IGF-I levels, mood and cognitive function in healthy
subjects (above 55 years) have been found to be subjects above 40 years was studied. As mental
associated with less cognitive decline over the function and GH release declines after the age of 40,
following 2 years (Kalmijn et al., 2000). Thus, the we hypothesized that this nutritional supplement
GH-IGF-I axis may play a role in the level of, in could increase GH and IGF-I release, thereby
particular, cognitive functioning in GHD patients improving cognitive function in these middle-aged
and healthy persons. As the activity of the GH-IGF-I and elderly subjects.
axis and cognitive function both decline with age, it
may well be possible to maintain or improve
cognitive function by increasing the activity of this MATERIALS AND METHODS
axis.
A number of studies have been performed Subjects
concerning the interactions between nutrients and
The effect of a nutritional food supplement on GH
the GH-IGF-I axis. Proteins and vitamins appear to
and IGF-I plasma levels and cognitive function was
control GH-stimulated IGF-I expression. The level
examined in 42 healthy non-obese volunteers, which
of protein and calorie intake regulates plasma IGF-
were recruited by advertisements in daily papers.
I and insulin-like growth factor binding proteins
The number of males was 14 and of females 28, they
(IGFBPs) levels. Glycine plays an important role in
were aged between 40 and 76 years. None of the
the control of hypothalamic-pituitary function.
subjects had any history of serious psychiatric
When glycine (250 ml, 0.3 M) was administered
disease or were taking any medication at the time of
into the duodenum in non-obese subjects, there
the experiment. Exclusion criteria were professional
was a significant increase in serum GH value
sports participants, alcohol consumption . 3 uni-
(Kasai et al., 1978). In healthy subjects doses of 4, 8
ts/day, vegetarians, dieting, use of drugs, hypnotics,
or 12 g glycine intravenous resulted in a dose-
antidepressants, the presence of sensory or motor
dependent GH release (Kasai et al., 1980). This
handicaps and the use of nutritional supplements
demonstrated that glycine is one of the stimulatory
within 1 month prior to start of the study. The level
agents inducing the pituitary to secrete GH.
of education was assessed by a scale comprising five
Arginine is also known to stimulate the GH
education levels. These levels are defined in
release and is used in GH stimulation tests. In a
conformity with the levels of education specified
study in sheep, glutamine intravenously (0.25 M
by the European Economic Community (Schuhfried,
glutamine solution at a rate of 3 mmol/kg during
1992). The study was conducted according to the
30 min) was one of the amino acids that increased
Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the
GH secretion (Kuhara et al., 1991). Niacin
Ethics Committee of the VU University Medical
(40 mg/kg body weight) infused 2 h prior to
Center. All subjects gave their written informed
growth hormone releasing factor (GRF) injection
consent before inclusion in the study.
has been found to enhance GRF-induced GH
release in sheep (Sartin et al., 1988) and a dose
between 100 and 200 mg acipimox, a nicotinic acid
Procedure
analog, stimulated GH secretion in pre-pubertal
children (Lanes et al., 2000). However, the Subjects received a powder of amino acids and
mechanisms underlying the amino acid/vitamin- vitamins for 3 weeks or placebo treatment, which
induced GH release are not fully known yet. was also administrated as a powder. Pairs of subjects
In addition, there are no studies available on the were formed, each pair matched for age, sex and
association between nutrition, GH-IGF-I and education. Subsequently, each one of the two was
cognition. Concerning the possibility of enhancing randomly assigned to the placebo or treatment
GH status of healthy subjects by means of a group, according to a double blind procedure. All the
nutritional supplement one study has been carried volunteers were asked to ingest 5 g of powder with
out before with a natural food supplement water twice a day on an empty stomach. Intake was
composed of amino acids and vitamins (Vitalinw, in the morning and before bedtime, with the last
10 g daily). In 12 volunteers (6 men and 6 women, meal or calorie containing food or drink consump-
aged 40 – 75 years), a mean increase of 367% blood tion at least 3 h before. No food was allowed to be
ORAL SUPPLEMENTS AND MEMORY 271

consumed for at least 45 min after taking the powder. rate themselves with respect to their feelings over
This regime was maintained for 6 days followed by the previous 3 days. The shortened Dutch version of
1 day of rest. This day off was included to make it 32 items was used, which consists of five sub-scales:
easier for the subjects to comply with the protocol. Depression, Anger, Fatigue, Vigour and Tension.
Five grams of the nutritional supplement was Responses were made by choosing from five
composed of a proprietary mixture of glycine and response alternatives. Score: The rating (1 –5) on
glutamine (4.92 g), niacin (20 mg) with added each item and the scale to which each item
vitamin A (1000 IU), vitamin C (10 mg), vitamin E contributes is recorded.
(5 IU), selenium (15 mcg), green tea leaf powder
(7 mg), bilberry fruit powder (7 mg), quercitin (5 mg),
Memory
alpha-lipoic acid (5 mg) and coQ 10 (10 mg) (Vitalinw,
Revita Health Corp., USA). In the present study, Short-term memory: Associative learning task. Nine
niacin was administered in two separate doses of word pairs consisting of a name and an occupation
20 mg, each dose being equivalent to the rec- were displayed on a computer screen at a constant
ommended daily allowances (RDA). This is a safe rate of one pair per 3 s. After these pairs were
dose that was expected not to induce side effects and presented, the subject was asked to choose one of
being high enough (two times the RDA) to stimulate the nine occupation alternatives. After each answer
GH secretion. The dose of the mixture of glycine and the results were given in the following manner:
glutamine is comparable to doses found in earlier “Yes, John is a grocer”, or “No, Susan is a teacher”.
studies to enhance GH secretion (Kasai et al., 1980). Three trials were given in which the subject has to
The mixture of these three GH stimulating agents learn as many paired names and occupations as
can be expected to stimulate GH secretion substan- possible. By means of this recognition procedure
tially. The placebo powder preparation was a short-term verbal learning is measured. Score: The
commercially available sweetener with low caloric number of correct associations on each trial
value (25 kcal/5 g). Five grams was composed of (maximum score: 9).
3.23 g maltodextrine, 0.1 g aspartame, 0.83 g cane Visual digit span task. This test for the assessment of
sugar and 0.83 g soft brown sugar. This preparation short-term memory, concentration and attention has
was chosen because of the identical appearance and two parts. A sequence of digits was presented one at
caloric value to Vitalinw powder, calculated to be the time to the subject. After the whole sequence was
19 kcal/5 g. As oral doses of aspartame (0.534 g) have presented, the subject was required to enter the
shown to have no effect on serum growth hormone sequence on the computer keyboard. Increasingly
levels (Carlson and Shah, 1989), this sweetener is not longer spans of digits were presented, until the
likely to affect GH secretion or cognitive functioning. subject made two errors at a span length. In the
Subjects were examined individually in a quiet second testing condition, the subject was asked to
room. The sequence of the tests was the same for all respond with the order of digits reversed (back-
subjects and the test procedure as a whole took about ward). Score: The lengths of the longest span
45 min. Two testing assistants, who were not aware answered correctly forward and backward were
which treatment the subjects received, administered recorded.
the tests. On the day before starting the consumption Symbol-digit substitution. This test was used to
of the food supplement a 5-cc venous blood sample measure the coding-speed. Nine symbols and nine
was drawn after an overnight fast for determination digits were paired (in a “key”) at the top of the
of serum GH and IGF-I levels. Second blood samples screen. The subject was required to press the digits
and cognitive tests were performed after 3 weeks of on the keyboard corresponding to a test set of the
ingestion the powder, 1– 1.5 h after taking the last nine symbols presented in scrambled order. Six sets
powder on an empty stomach. The blood samples of nine symbol-digits pairs were presented. Score:
were centrifuged (10 min, 3500 rpm at 48C) and the response latency for each of the nine items in
serum was stored at 2 208C until analysis. each trial and the number of digits incorrectly
matched with the test symbols was recorded.
Intermediate memory: Associative learning delayed
Psychological Tests
recognition task. To assess intermediate recall a single
The following tests were selected from the Neuro- recognition trial of the nine names to be matched
behavioral Education System (NES) (Letz, 1993) and with one of the nine occupations used in the
were administered by means of a PC. associate learning test was administered at the end
of the testing session. The approximate 30 min delay
between this final testing and the associate learning
Mood
task comprises a retention task for intermediate
The Profile of Mood States (POMS) is a self- memory. Score: The number of correct responses
administered questionnaire in which the subjects (maximum score: 9).
272 L.I. ARWERT et al.

TABLE I Characteristics of the study subjects one-tailed, based on the hypothesis that the
nutritional supplement would enhance GH status,
No. of Education mood and memory. Data are presented as mean ^
No. of female/ Age level
subjects male ðmean ^ SDÞ ðmean ^ SDÞ standard deviation (SD), unless specified otherwise.
Treatment 22 13/9 60.3 ^ 9.0 3.3 ^ 0.9
Placebo 19 14/5 61.5 ^ 6.8 3.4 ^ 0.7
Total 41 27/14 60.9 ^ 8.0 3.3 ^ 0.8 RESULTS

There were no dropouts during the study. One


Hormone Assays female in the food supplement group was excluded
from the analysis because of extreme and varying
Serum GH and IGF-I levels were measured using
values of serum growth hormone concentration (GH
commercially available assays (GH, immunometric
session 1: 67 mU/l, GH session 2: 2.1 mU/l).
assay, Sorin Biomedica, Saluggia Italy; IGF-I,
Characteristics of the remaining volunteers are
Chemoluminiscentic, Nichols Institute Diagnostics,
shown in Table I. Their serum IGF-I values inclusive
San Juan Capristrano, USA). The detection limits for
reference values for age and sex are shown in Table II.
GH and IGF-I are 1.0 mU/l (0.5 mg/l) and
Mean age and education level were similar for
6.0 nmol/l, respectively. The intra-assay coefficient
people who had the nutritional supplement powder
of variation (CV) for GH is 4% at serum GH of
and placebo.
4 mU/l. The interassay CV for GH is 9% at serum
GH of 2.5 mU/l and 8% at serum GH of 8.6 mU/l.
For IGF-I the intra-assay CV is 3% at serum IGF-I of Side Effects
20 nmol/l and the inter assay CV is 6% at serum
IGF-I of 33 nmol/l and 8% at serum IGF-I of After 3 weeks of the ingestion of the mixture there
7 nmol/l. Normal values for IGF-I differ with age were no subjects reporting any side effects. Thus,
and sex as shown in Table II. also blushing and heat waves, known side effects of
niacin amongst very sensitive people, were not
reported.
Statistical Analysis
All calculations were performed with the Statistical
Treatment Effects on GH, IGF-I and Cognitive
Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). As the data
Functions
were not normally distributed differences between
conditions were determined by means of the Regarding GH concentration, the Wilcoxon test
Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test. The Spearman indicated that the post-treatment GH concentration
correlation coefficient was calculated to assess in the treatment group was significantly higher
possible relationships between serum hormone than the pre-treatment values ðp ¼ 0:03Þ: Mean GH
levels and scores on cognitive and mood scales. increased in this group from 3.23 (^ 4.75) to 4.67
Difference-scores (D) were computed for GH/IGF-I (^ 5.27) mU/l. In the placebo group mean GH did
values and psychological scores ½ðscore session 2 2 not change (baseline, 3.92 (^ 7.24) mU/l; post-
score session 1Þ=score session 1: As these D treatment, 2.91 (^ 2.96) mU/l ðp ¼ 0:4Þ (Fig. 1).
scores were normally distributed, we calculated The mean percentage of GH increase in the treatment
the Pearson correlation coefficient. Significance group relative to the placebo group is 70%.
level was set as p , 0:05: All statistical tests were The ranges of GH change for all the individual

TABLE II Mean IGF-I levels of males and females in the present study and normal IGF-I values

Normal values in Normal values in Females IGF-I (nmol/l) Males IGF-I (nmol/l)
female (nmol/l) male (nmol/l)
(P5–P95) (P5–P95) Baseline Post-treatment Baseline Post-treatment
40– # 51 years 8.7–30.3 8.7–30.1 Mean ^ SD 17.7 ^ 4.2 19.7 ^ 5.7 19.0 ^ 0.0 16.0 ^ 1.4
Range 13.0–21.0 15.0–26.0 19.0–19.0 15.0–17.0
Number 3 3 2 2
52– # 61 years 7.8–25.5 7.8–25.8 Mean ^ SD 19.1 ^ 7.2 18.3 ^ 4.1 19.7 ^ 0.6 18.3 ^ 3.2
Range 10.0–31.0 11.0– 23.0 19.0–20.0 16.0–22.0
Number 8 8 3 3
.61 years 10.3–19.0 10.3–19.0 Mean ^ SD 18.1 ^ 5.4 17.8 ^ 4.8 15.2 ^ 6.1 16.4 ^ 5.7
Range 10.0–30.0 12.0–28.0 9.5–29.0 7.3 –29.0
Number 16 16 19 19
ORAL SUPPLEMENTS AND MEMORY 273

FIGURE 1 Mean GH concentration (^SEM) at sessions 1 and 2 for the treatment and placebo groups. (*p , 0:05).

subjects in the nutritional supplement group were of subjects, IGF-I concentration correlated positively
between 2 90 and 2100%. The ranges for males were with tension (r ¼ 0:35; p ¼ 0:01). That means that a
between 2 90 and 1200%, for females between 2 65 higher IGF-I concentration was associated with a
and 2100%. In the placebo group these ranges were higher score on the tension scale of the POMS.
between 2 80% and 600%. The largest GH changes Difference-scores (D) were computed for GH/
were found in subjects with very low baseline GH IGF-I values and psychological scores [(score
levels (, 1.0 mU/l). session 2 2 score session 1)/score session 1]. Delta
With respect to the IGF-I concentration, no IGF-I correlated positively with digit-span back-
significant differences between post-treatment and wards (r ¼ 0:41; p ¼ 0:004). This means that a higher
pre-treatment were found. This indicates that the increase in IGF-I across sessions is associated with a
IGF-I concentration did not change across sessions larger increase in working memory. Regarding
and no difference is seen between the treatment and mood, D IGF-I correlated positively with D vigour
placebo group. IGF-I in the treatment group did not (r ¼ 0:39; p ¼ 0:006), indicating that the more IGF-I
change (pre-treatment, 17.47 (^ 4.88) nmol/l; post- increases the more vigour improves. Thus, although
treatment, 17.65 (^ 4.66) nmol/l). In the placebo no significant increase in IGF-I was found in the
group IGF-I also did not change (pre-treatment, 18.11 treatment group, these data indicate that on
(^ 6.28) nmol/l; post-treatment, 17.68 (^ 4.64) individual level an increase in IGF-I is associated
nmol/l). with an improvement of memory and vigour.
The intra-subject correlation of pre- and post Regarding GH, no relations were found between D
treatment IGF-I concentration is high (r ¼ 0:84; GH and D mood/cognition.
p , 0:0005). There was an association between pre-
treatment GH and pre-treatment IGF-I (r ¼ 0:39;
p ¼ 0:006). Also post-treatment GH and post-treat- DISCUSSION
ment IGF-I concentrations were significantly related
(r ¼ 0:31; p ¼ 0:02). Pre-treatment GH did correlate The results of the present study indicate that the use
significantly with post-treatment IGF-I (r ¼ 0:44; of an oral mixture of amino acids and vitamins,
p ¼ 0:002). mainly containing glycine, glutamine and niacin,
Regarding cognitive functions, no significant during 3 weeks increased the mean level of GH
treatment effects were found. compared to placebo. Because GH is secreted in a
pulsatile fashion, with 2/3 of the daily secretion
taking place at night, a single determination of the
Correlation’s of GH and IGF-I with Cognitive
GH concentration is assumed not to be very reliable.
Function
Therefore, the measurement of 24 h GH-profiles to
Scores on the cognitive tests were found to be related assess the GH status would have been more
to GH and IGF-I values. The pre-treatment and informative. The intra-subject pre-and post-treat-
post-treatment correlations were calculated for the ment GH correlation however is quite high ðr ¼ 0:60Þ
whole group of subjects ðn ¼ 41Þ: and the pre-treatment GH concentration correlates
Pre-treatment: No significant correlations of significantly with the pre- and post-treatment IGF-I
cognitive parameters were found with GH and concentrations (r ¼ 0:39 and r ¼ 0:44; respectively).
IGF-I values. Post-treatment: Within the whole group Previous studies have shown that, because of
274 L.I. ARWERT et al.

homeostatic control mechanisms within the GH-IGF- GH and IGF-I at both pre- and post-treatment
I axis, the day to day mean mass of GH secretion per indicating that higher GH is associated with a higher
burst and the serial orderliness of the GH release is IGF-I at both sessions, support this idea.
strongly preserved in healthy men across a wide A second explanation for the absence of an
span of ages and body-compositions (Friend et al., increase in IGF-I may that some subjects exceeded
1996). In a study in which every 10 min blood the normal age-adjusted values before treatment.
samples were drawn in healthy men (age range, The phenomenon of “regression towards the mean”
29 –77 years) for 48 h, it was found that the mean will increase the chance of having lower post
mass of GH secretion per burst was highly conserved treatment values. Six out of seven subjects, who
across sessions and consecutive days within subjects had a pre-treatment IGF-I concentration that
(r ¼ 0:93 and r ¼ 0:92; respectively). Thus, the exceeded the normal value for age and sex, had a
orderliness of GH release and the mean mass of lower post-treatment IGF-I concentration. Thus, this
GH secreted per burst are highly conserved in decrease in IGF-I concentration may be the con-
individual men on successive days. The above sequence of the high values before treatment.
findings make it probable that the GH samples in Another explanation for our finding that GH levels
our study are reliable and representative for the GH were higher after 3 weeks in the nutritional
status of the subjects. supplement group without an effect seen in IGF-I
The difference in ranges of GH levels between levels could be found in the study design. Subjects
males and females suggest that females respond had ingested the powder 1 –1.5 h before the blood
more to the nutritional supplement preparation than sample was drawn in the post-treatment situation.
males. The larger number of females within the Possibly we measured an acute effect of the powder
sample may explain this. It may also suggest that on serum GH levels, with no effect on IGF-I levels.
females are more sensitive for GH stimulation than This is also seen in an animal study, in which the
males. Indeed it is known that there is a difference in intravenously administration of 17 amino acids on
secretion and sensitivity for GH in men and women. the secretion of GH in six castrated male sheep was
The GH production in healthy women is about thrice investigated. The amino acids caused an increase in
that in men, whereas the IGF-I levels are similar GH secretion, while no changes in plasma IGF-I were
(Span et al., 2000). The mean serum GH concen- found (Kuhara et al., 1991).
trations over 24 h are higher in women than in men. The effects of GH on cognition are probably not
Also women exhibit higher maximal serum GH only mediated by IGF-I, but also by a direct effect of
concentration peak values than men (van den Berg GH itself on GH receptors in the brain. It is reported
et al., 1996). Higher maximal and incremental serum that there is a negative correlation between the
GH peak amplitudes account for the higher GH density of GH-binding sites and increasing age in
concentrations in women. IGF-I levels are similar various brain regions (Lai et al., 1993). The exact
however, suggesting a lower sensitivity to GH in mechanism how GH and IGF-I influence cognition
women. Gender has a significant influence on the still has to be investigated. In this study, the amino
human GH axis in the pre-menopausal age group. It acids and vitamins preparation did not improve
appears that estrogens regulate the physiological cognitive and/or mood parameters. It may well be
output both quantitatively (a higher daily secretion true that the short treatment period of 3 weeks
rate) and qualitatively (a less orderly pattern of GH induces only subtle changes not leading to measur-
release) (Veldhuis, 1998). Our sample mainly able effects. Our data however, indicate that on an
consisted of post-menopausal women, only two individual level an increase in IGF-I was correlated
women being pre-menopausal. The effect of the with an improvement of memory and vigour.
mixture on GH was still significant ðp ¼ 0:04Þ after In contrast, we found no correlations between an
removing these females from analysis. increase in GH and memory or mood.
Regarding IGF-I we found no significant increase The present study indicated that 3 weeks of
after the nutritional supplement use. It is known that nutritional supplement ingestion increased GH
IGF-I levels rise more slowly than GH levels. concentration, but not IGF-I, in healthy people
Increments in serum IGF-I occurred 4 – 6 h after above 40 years. As the secretion of GH decreases
intravenous GH exposure in GH-deficient patients with age, starting in the third decade of life, the
(Jorgensen et al., 1991). In case of an oral nutritional increase of GH secretion in elderly persons may
supplement a longer administration time could be possibly halt or reverse the age-related catabolic state
necessary to increase IGF-I levels. As our treatment resulting in physical and cognitive changes. The
period was 3 weeks, this period may be too short to specific beneficial effects found in other studies are
result in a significant rise of IGF-I. As a consequence, an increase of lean body mass, aerobic capacity,
the use of the food supplement for a period longer immune function, and possibly also cognitive
than 3 weeks possibly leads to an increase in IGF-I. function (Khorram, 2001). Other effects are a
The significant positive correlations found between decrease in fat mass and possibly cholesterol.
ORAL SUPPLEMENTS AND MEMORY 275

However, future studies are needed to determine Kasai, K., Suzuki, H., Nakamura, T., Shiina, H. and Shimoda, S.I.
(1980) “Glycine stimulated growth hormone release in man”,
whether a nutritional supplement can cause a long- Acta Endocrinologica 93, 283–286.
term increase in GH secretion and whether normal- Khorram, O. (2001) “Use of growth hormone and growth hormone
ization of GH and IGF-I levels in healthy adults will secretagogues in aging, help or harm”, Clinical Obstetrics and
Gynecology 44, 893 –901.
lead to improvements in their psychological func- Kuhara, T., Ikeda, S., Ohneda, A. and Sasaki, Y. (1991) “Effects of
tional capacity and QoL. intravenous infusion of 17 amino acids on the secretion of GH,
glucagon, and insulin in sheep”, American Journal of Physiology
260, E21–E26.
Lai, Z., Roos, P., Zhai, O., Olsson, Y., Fholenhag, K., Larsson, C. and
References Nyberg, F. (1993) “Age-related reduction of human growth
hormone-binding sites in the human brain”, Brain Research
Aleman, A., Verhaar, H.J.J., de Haan, E.H.F., de Vries, W.R., 621, 260–266.
Samson, M.M., Drent, M.L., van der Veen, E.A. and Lanes, R., Lunar, L., Carrillo, E., Villaroel, O., Gunczler, P. and
Koppeschaar, H.P.F. (1999) “Insulin-like growth factor-I and Palacios, A. (2000) “Acipimox, a nicotinic acid analog,
cognitive function in healthy older men”, Journal of Clinical stimulates growth hormone secretion in short healthy pre-
Endocrinology and Metabolism 84, 471–475. pubertal children”, Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology and
Burman, P., Broman, J.E., Hetta, J., Wirklund, I., Erfurth, E.M., Metabolism 13, 1115 –1120.
Hagg, E. and Karlsson, F.A. (1995) “Quality of life in adults Letz, R. (1993) NES2 User’s Manual, version 4.6. Copyright 1993,
with growth hormone (GH) deficiency, response to treatment Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.
with recombinant human GH in a placebo-controlled 21 Nyberg, F. (2000) “Growth hormone in the brain, characteristics of
month trial”, Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism specific brain targets for the hormone and their functional
80, 3585–3590. significance. Front”, Neuroendocrinology 21, 330 –348.
Carlson, H.E. and Shah, J.H. (1989) “Aspartame and its constituent Rollero, A., Murialdo, G., Fonzi, S., Garrone, S., Gianelli, M.V.,
amino acids, effects on prolactin, cortisol, growth hormone, Gazzerro, E., Barreca, A. and Polleri, A. (1998) “Relationship
insulin, and glucose in normal humans”, American Journal of between cognitive function, growth hormone and insulin-like
Clinical Nutrition 49, 427–432. growth factor I plasma levels in aged subjects”, Neuropsycho-
Deijen, J.B., de Boer, H., Blok, G.J. and van der Veen, E.A. (1996) biology 38, 73–79.
“Cognitive impairments and mood disturbances in growth Sartin, J.L., Barthol, F.F., Kemppainen, R.J., Dieberg, G., Buxton, D.
hormone deficient men”, Psychoneuroendocrinology 21, and Soyoola, E. (1988) “Modulation of growth hormone-
313– 322. releasing factor stimulated growth hormone secretion by
Deijen, J.B., de Boer, H. and van der Veen, E.A. (1998) “Cognitive plasma glucose and free fatty acid concentrations in sheep”,
changes during growth hormone replacement in adult men”, Neuroendocrinology 48, 627 –633.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 45– 55. Schuhfried, G. (1992) The PC/S Vienna Test System.
Friend, K., Iranmanesh, A. and Veldhuis, J.D. (1996) Test Management Program. Dr. G. Schuhfried, Modeling,
“The orderliness of the growth hormone (GH) release process Austria.
and the mean mass of GH secreted per burst are highly Span, J.P.T., Pieters, G.F.F.F., Sweep, C.G.J., Hermus, A.R.M.M. and
conserved in individual men on successive days”, Journal of Smals, A.G.H. (2000) “Gender difference in insulin-like
Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 81, 3746– 3753. growth factor I response to growth hormone (GH)
Jorgensen, J.O.L., Blum, W.F., Moller, N., Ranke, M.B. and treatment in GH-deficient adults, role of sex hormone
Christiansen, J.S. (1991) “Short-term changes in serum replacement”, Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism
insulin-like growth factors (IGF) and IGF binding protein 3 85, 1121–1125.
after different modes of intravenous growth hormone (GH) van den Berg, G., Veldhuis, J.D., Frolich, M. and Roelfsema, F.
exposure in GH-deficient patients”, Journal of Clinical (1996) “An amplitude-specific divergence in the pulsatile
Endocrinology and Metabolism 72, 582–587. mode of growth hormone (GH) secretion underlies the gender
Kalmijn, S., Janssen, J.A.M.J.L., Pols, H.A.P., Lamberts, S.W.J. and difference in mean GH concentrations in men and pre-
Breteler, M.M.B. (2000) “A prospective study on circulating menopausal women”, Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and
insulin-like growth factor I (IGF-I), IGF-binding proteins, and Metabolism 81, 2460–2467.
cognitive function in the elderly”, Journal of Clinical Veldhuis, J.D. (1998) “Neuroendocrine control of pulsatile growth
Endocrinology and Metabolism 85, 4551–4555. hormone release in the human, relationship with gender”,
Kasai, K., Kobayashi, M. and Shimoda, S.I. (1978) “Stimulatory Growth Hormone and IGF Research 8, 49 –59.
effect of glycine on human growth hormone secretion”, Woodruff-Pak, D.S. (1997) The Neuropsychology of Aging (Blackwell
Metabolism 27, 201–208. Publishers Ltd., Oxford), Chapter 11.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi