Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 12

Attachment and Psychosomatic Medicine: Developmental Contributions to Stress

and Disease
ROBERT G. MAUNDER, MD, FRCP(C), AND JONATHAN J. HUNTER, MD, FRCP(C)
Objective: The object of this study was to evaluate the evidence linking attachment insecurity to illness. Attachment
theory describes lifelong patterns of response to threat that are learned in the interaction between an infant and his
or her primary caregiver. Despite its biopsychosocial domain, attachment theory has only recently been applied to
psychosomatic medicine. Method: MEDLINE and PsychInfo databases were searched from 1966 to 2000 for English
language papers with key words “attachment” and “object relations.” Papers and their cited references were
reviewed if they were directly related to physical illness, symptoms, or physiology. A hypothetical causal model
was developed. Results: Direct and indirect evidence from survey studies supports an association between
attachment insecurity and disease. Animal studies and human experiments suggest that attachment contributes to
individual differences in physiological stress response. There is also less robust support for insecure attachment
leading to symptom reporting and to more frequent health risk behaviors, especially substance use and treatment
nonadherence. Evidence supports the prediction from attachment theory that the benefits of social support derive
more from attachment relationships than nonattachment relationships. Conclusions: Although the available data
are suggestive rather than conclusive, the data can be organized into a model that describe attachment insecurity
leading to disease risk through three mechanisms. These are increased susceptibility to stress, increased use of
external regulators of affect, and altered help-seeking behavior. This model warrants further prospective
investigation. Key words: Attachment, disease model, stress, development, biopsychosocial.

result of predicted and measurable individual differ-


ences. The attachment model explains how repeated
ACTH ⫽ adrenocorticotropin; CSF ⫽ cerebrospinal
crucial interactions between infant and caregiver re-
fluid; HbA1c ⫽ glycated hemoglobin; HPA ⫽ hypotha-
lamic-pituitary-adrenal; NE ⫽ norepinephrine; SOC ⫽ sult in lifelong patterns of stress-response, receptivity
sense of coherence; UC ⫽ ulcerative colitis. to social support, and vulnerability to illness.

ATTACHMENT THEORY
There is convincing evidence that social support (1),
depression, (2, 3) and stress (4) have important effects When Bowlby described attachment theory (10 –12),
on the progress of some diseases, although the rela- he provided a biological basis for understanding close,
tionship between these psychosocial factors and dis- protective relationships. A central innovation of at-
ease processes is complex and often inconsistent be- tachment theory was the recognition that a child’s
tween studies (1, 2, 4). Furthermore, there is a growing desire for proximity to his or her mother is a biological
body of evidence describing a high prevalence of past drive which has been selected in evolution, rather than
psychological trauma, especially sexual abuse, in per- a behavior which is learned to satisfy other biological
sons with a variety of physical conditions including drives such as hunger. If maintaining proximity is a
gastrointestinal disorders (5), fibromyalgia, (6, 7) and fundamental need, attachment behavior can be under-
pain syndromes (8, 9). One of the central questions for stood as a set of strategies that have been learned to
any model that ascribes disease risk to relatively com- achieve optimal proximity. An infant, defenseless on
mon events such as trauma, loss, isolation, or to the its own, maintains proximity to her or his mother
ubiquitous experience of stress is how the model ac- through a complex system of communications and be-
counts for individual differences in psychosocial haviors, which increase its chances of survival. At-
susceptibility. tachment behavior, such as smiling, vocalizing, crying,
This paper explores the possibility that attachment and approaching, is the normal, adaptive response of a
theory could be extended to a biopsychosocial model mammal to threat. Organized patterns of attachment
of development and health that explains inconsisten- behavior emerge at a relatively fixed time for each
cies in the stress and social support literature as the species, in humans the second half of the first year
(13).
Before the emergence of organized attachment be-
From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Mount havior (ie, before learning) infant signaling of needs is
Sinai Hospital, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. innate and responsive to immediate stimuli. In this
Address reprint requests to: Dr. R. Maunder, Dept. of Psychiatry, pre-attachment period appropriate proximity relies on
Mount Sinai Hospital, 600 University Ave, Toronto, Ontario, Can-
ada, M5G 1X5. Email: rmaunder@mtsinai.on.ca
parental anticipation of infant needs and sensitivity to
Received for publication May 8, 2000; revisions received Novem- infant signals. In the transition to organized attach-
ber 21, 2000. ment behavior, proximity is increasingly dependent

556 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


0033-3174/01/6304-0556
Copyright © 2001 by the American Psychosomatic Society
ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

on patterns of approach and withdrawal that are rein- Developmental Psychobiology of Attachment
forced and amplified by the parent. This operant con-
The psychobiological basis of attachment has been
ditioning results in the laying down of procedural
studied in animals, where it is possible to observe
memory in the infant. Procedural memory (learning
extremes of attachment system failure. The first such
“how to”) is encoded learning which occurs develop-
set of observations is of primates raised with various
mentally before the ability for conscious recall. Since
degrees of maternal deprivation (18). The second is a
the neurological systems required for the laying down
series of experiments with rats that has elegantly elu-
of declarative memories will only be fully developed
cidated the “hidden regulators” of infant physiology
by age 2 to 3, the behavioral patterns learned at this
critical stage are not available for conscious recall (14). that occur in the mother-infant interaction (19).
Attachment behaviors occur in patterns that show Brief separation of mammalian infants from their
substantial differences between infant-parent pairs but mothers results in protest behavior, such as high-in-
occur in typical clusters. Infants can be reliably clas- tensity vocalization, agitated searching, and excessive
sified in an attachment typology based on Ainsworth’s self-grooming (19). Persistent separation results in re-
standardized Strange Situation in which the infant is duced responsiveness, decreased heart rate, and low-
presented with stressful situations that include the ered body temperature often referred to as despair (18,
presence of a stranger, separation of the infant from his 19). Monkeys that are kept in isolation through critical
or her primary caretaker, and reunion (13). Avoidant periods of infancy demonstrate gross behavioral and
infants cry minimally during separation, treat the biological consequences. The behavioral conse-
stranger similarly to the parent, and avoid contact on quences of isolation rearing represent a disorganiza-
reunion. Secure infants show distress at separation tion of innate social behaviors. What these animals are
and actively seek contact at the time of reunion, which missing, according to attachment theory, are the re-
is soothing. Angry-ambivalent infants similarly show peated experiences of parental amplification and rein-
separation distress and proximity-seeking on reunion, forcement of innate behavioral patterns that result in
but combine proximity-seeking with angry resistance social norms becoming encoded in procedural mem-
(13). The fourth category, disorganized attachment, ory (14). The biological consequences of isolation rear-
was created later to include subjects who present no ing include abnormalities of neurotransmitter and hor-
such coherent pattern. mone function (18). Further consequences include
The origin of these attachment phenotypes is reductions in cerebellar (20) and motor cortical (21)
thought to reside in a complicated “goodness of fit” dendritic branching. These deficits illustrate the de-
between parent, child, and environment. Although gree to which normal brain development and normal
most attention has been paid to the parental contribu- endocrine and neurotransmitter regulation are depen-
tions to attachment style it is also recognized that dent on the postnatal influence of crucial social
parental attitudes are shaped by the temperamental interactions.
needs and abilities of the infant and by environmental The large conceptual gap between observations of
stresses and supports (15). Despite this complexity, maternally deprived primates and the normal experi-
some generalizations are useful. In general, parenting ence of parents and infants is partially bridged by the
that is unresponsive relative to the needs of a child or description of a continuum of rehabilitation in de-
intolerant of a child’s distress predisposes to avoidant prived primates. In particular, Kraemer (18) has de-
attachment. The child learns that seeking proximity scribed how the consequences of isolation rearing can
through crying and clinging is futile. On the other be partially compensated for by substitute mothers,
hand, independence is reinforced and valued. As a ranging from inanimate surrogates to peers to same-
result attachment behaviors are relatively deactivated. species foster mothers. In a stepwise progression, the
Maintaining a degree of protective proximity depends degree of residual impaired function in isolation-
on avoiding communications of dependence (16). reared monkeys depends on the extent to which ma-
Angry-ambivalent attachment typically follows ternal functions are replaced by a surrogate and by the
from interrupted or inconsistent parental care (17). age of the monkey at the time of replacement. Inani-
Confidence in oneself to respond appropriately to mate surrogates lead to increased attachment behavior
threats does not develop adequately. The value of turn- if the surrogates bear qualities such as feeling soft and
ing to the attachment figure for security is intermit- furry, warmth, mobility, and rocking motion. Inani-
tently reinforced, but the need to be vigilant for the mate surrogates will not suffice to correct the greatly
presence and loss of the other is more strongly rein- decreased CSF NE levels of the isolates, but peer sur-
forced. Proximity, once obtained, is often not soothing, rogates are partially effective. Peer surrogates rehabil-
resulting in persistent anxiety. itate behavior to a greater degree, such that isolated

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 557


R. G. MAUNDER AND J. J. HUNTER

monkeys can become indistinguishable from peers sponse to external threat and in a set of expectations
when in familiar social situations. To achieve near related to threat.
normal levels of CSF NE requires same-species adult Expectations learned after the development of de-
female surrogates. Importantly, none of these surro- clarative memory form a cognitive schema that defines
gates is able to provide isolates with the ability to what sorts of events are threatening, how one should
perceive social relationships, to respond well to social react to threats, and what one can expect of others
cues or to behave normally when competing for re- under these circumstances. On the other hand, earlier
sources. Thus primate isolation rearing provides learning encoded in procedural memory does not form
strong evidence of both biological and behavioral ef- a cognitive “expectation” but a nonverbal working de-
fects of early social deprivation and of the partial plas- scription of what counts as a threat and what is done
ticity of these effects to rehabilitation by improved when one is detected. Together, this set of expecta-
social relationships. tions relating to threat, proximity, and separation are
Hofer’s rat model brings us closer to the regulatory called the internal working model.
processes that occur between mother and infant under
more nearly normal conditions. The infant rat depends
Adult Attachment Patterns and the Internal
on its mother as a regulator of a number of functions
Working Model
critical to survival, including nutrition and regulation
of temperature and activity (19). This body of work Any relationship in which proximity to the other
reveals two factors that are important to understanding affects felt security is an attachment relationship (23).
the association of attachment to health. First, the The primary example of adult attachment is between
mother is understood to be supplying necessary regu- committed sexual partners, but many other relation-
latory functions for the infant. Understanding that the ships may include a degree of attachment, including
mother serves as an external regulator of the infant’s the relationship of patient and clinician.
physiology allows us to see that the loss of mother at In Bowlby’s theory, the internal working model pro-
this critical stage directly constitutes a loss of regula- vides the continuity of attachment style over time.
tion. More than just a stress, maternal loss constitutes Models of self and other vary from very positive ex-
an actual undoing of the system. Second, the nature of pectations (resiliency of self, responsiveness of other)
these regulatory functions is highly specific and not to very negative expectations (fragile or incompetent
intuitively obvious. For example, infant activity is in- self, unavailable or unreliable other). Since expecta-
creased by warmth (provided by close proximity to tions of self and other are relatively independent (24),
mother) but decreased by tactile contact with mother. an individual’s attachment status can be described by
Furthermore the degree of contact with and stimula- their position on each of these dimensions, which has
tion by mother is regulated by infant activity, such that led to a four-category, two-dimension model of attach-
the mother-infant pair taken together comprises a bal- ment (25).
anced feedback loop. Hofer’s experiments demonstrate In the four-category model a person who has posi-
the mechanisms by which proximity-maintaining tive expectations of both self and other is secure. Se-
communications occur developmentally before the cure attachment is associated with an internalized
feelings that are recognized, in older humans, as “se- sense of being worthy of care, of being effective in
curity.” Attachment is first and foremost a biologically eliciting care when required, and a sense of personal
based behavioral system. efficacy in dealing with most stressors independently
The development of autonomous function requires (23). Secure individuals are described as adaptable,
a transition from the interdependent mother-infant capable, trusting, and understanding (26). In this de-
regulatory system to relatively independent internal scription, we are choosing to emphasize aspects of
self-regulation of physiological functions. This transi- attachment that relate to security, threat, and stress
tion depends on the unfolding of genetically pro- and to de-emphasize feelings related to loveability and
grammed neurological development under the con- comfort. This is consistent with Bowlby’s psychobio-
stant modification of social interactions (22). Thus, the logical model.
same processes of mutual engagement and interaction Insecure attachment can be categorized as preoccu-
that initially serve to maintain proximity as an end in pied, dismissing, and fearful. A person who expects to
itself are the crucial prerequisites of both emotional cope with stress inadequately but whose expectations
attachment security and physiological self-regulation. of others are more positive is preoccupied. Preoccu-
These intertwined developmental processes ulti- pied attachment, the adult correlate of infant angry-
mately result in an organism that can independently ambivalent attachment, is associated with excessive
maintain homeostasis and mount a physiological re- care-seeking, separation protest, and fear of loss (23).

558 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

Although the preoccupied individual seeks care, the the first attachment. Attachment type is then under-
soothing that results from contact is partial and tran- stood as a disposition toward certain perceptions of
sient. Preoccupied individuals are described as anx- others, certain perceptions of self, and certain pre-
ious, dependent, emotional, impulsive, and approval- ferred strategies that will be triggered by the presence
seeking (26). of a perceived threat. The emergence of attachment
People who distrust the effectiveness of social sup- behavior is largely context dependent, a state phenom-
ports but have a positive view of themselves empha- enon, but the internal working model that provides
size independence and are classified as dismissing. A consistency to the pattern of behavior that emerges is a
self-sufficient and undemanding attitude is often trait.
highly valued, but the associated distrust and avoid-
ance of intimacy communicates the underlying inse- METHODS
curity. Situations that demand relinquishing control
MEDLINE and PsychInfo databases were searched from 1966 to
and depending on others, such as hospitalization for
2000 for English language papers with key words “attachment” and
acute illness, may result in crisis. Dismissing attach- “object relations.” Peer-reviewed papers and their cited references
ment is characterized by coldness to others and com- were included for review if they were directly related to physical
petitiveness (25). illness, symptoms, or physiology, and not included if they ad-
Finally, a person whose expectations of both self dressed only psychopathology. This literature was first reviewed for
evidence for and against an overall association of insecure attach-
and other are negative is fearful. Fearful attachment is
ment and disease. Possible causal mechanisms linking insecure
the least-studied category, having been introduced by attachment to disease were then reviewed. A causal model was
Bartholomew in 1991 (25). Fearful individuals are de- developed and the evidence for and against hypotheses contained in
scribed as cautious, doubting, self-conscious, shy, and the model was reviewed.
suspicious (26).
Dismissing and fearful attachment taken together ATTACHMENT INSECURITY AND DISEASE
are the adult correlate of infant avoidant attachment.
To assess whether there is an overall association of insecure
Most studies do not distinguish between dismissing attachment and disease there are four relevant aspects of attachment
and fearful attachment, often labeling the composite literature. The evidence for an association of attachment and disease
category “avoidant.” To maintain consistency, the in childhood, an association in adulthood, an association of child-
term avoidant will be used to describe dismissing and hood precursors of insecure attachment and adult disease, and the
association of secure attachment and later health will be reviewed.
avoidant styles (except where studies specifically dis-
tinguish dismissing and fearful styles), whereas “pre-
occupied” will be used to designate the attachment Is Childhood Attachment Insecurity Associated
type that is variously referred to as preoccupied, anx- With Childhood Disease?
ious, ambivalent, and resistant. The clinical observation that parents of ill infants experience
Studies of attachment types in different age groups anxiety that might interfere with reliable and consistent parenting
have demonstrated that the prevalence of avoidant has led to the study of attachment security in several diagnostic
groups. Remarkably, no excess of attachment insecurity has been
attachment is fairly constant in all age cohorts (about
found in premature infants (28, 29), in preschoolers with develop-
20% to 25%) whereas the prevalence of preoccupied mental delays (29), or in infants with cleft lip and palate, (30)
attachment is less in older cohorts (from about 20% at compared with either healthy controls or community norms.
college age to 5% to 8% in middle age) (26, 27). Re- On the other hand, toddlers with certain chronic illnesses have
cently, a 31-year longitudinal study in women sup- been found to have a higher prevalence of insecure attachment.
Children with recurrent otitis media over six months exhibit more
ported the view that these changes represent trends in
attachment insecurity than children without recurrent otitis media
individuals over time, rather than cohort effects, by (31). Twenty-six children aged 12 to 22 months with failure to thrive
showing a modest decline in preoccupied attachment, exhibited less secure attachment and more anxious, disorganized
a similar increase in secure attachment, and stable attachment in the Strange Situation, regardless of organic or nonor-
avoidance attachment scores over adult life (26). ganic etiology of failure to thrive (32). Cystic fibrosis is associated
with a higher prevalence of disorganized insecurity than found in
The importance of the internal working model for
healthy controls (33). Epilepsy is associated with a higher preva-
understanding attachment and health is that it clarifies lence of insecure attachment than cerebral palsy or normal controls
the dilemma of whether attachment insecurity should (34). A higher prevalence of insecure attachment has also been
be understood as an emotional state or a personality reported in asthmatic preschool children (35).
trait. An attachment type is the result of an internal In congenital heart disease there is a significant excess of
avoidant insecurity in patients (37%) compared with healthy con-
working model that guides affects and behavior when
trols (9%) (36). Of further interest, functional severity of heart dis-
a threat is perceived. Presumably the neurological sub- ease was not different between secure and insecure infants at diag-
strate for this internal working model has developed nosis but insecure infants were significantly less likely to show
through the effects of highly salient conditioning in improvement at follow-up at age 1 year.

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 559


R. G. MAUNDER AND J. J. HUNTER

Is Adult Attachment Insecurity Associated With lows causal inferences to be made about attachment in the absence
Adult Disease? of prospective studies. A very large study of primary practice med-
ical patients provides strong evidence for an association between
Adult attachment pattern can be reliably classified using the precursors of attachment insecurity and adult diseases. The risk
Adult Attachment Interview, while dimensions of attachment secu- factors assessed were psychological, physical, or sexual abuse, vio-
rity are measured with a number of self-report instruments (37). lence against mother, and living with family members who were
While adult attachment has been extensively studied in relationship substance abusers, mentally ill, suicidal, or imprisoned. The disease
to psychopathology (38) there has been much less investigation of outcomes studied were ischemic heart disease, cancer, chronic lung
the relationship between adult attachment and physical illness. In disease, skeletal fractures, and liver disease. In 9,508 patients, a
idiopathic spasmodic torticollis, avoidant attachment, measured by graded relationship was found between the number of retrospec-
the Attachment Q-sort, is over-represented compared with normal tively reported childhood family risk factors and adult disease (47).
controls (39). In UC, avoidant attachment (compulsive self-reliance)
measured by the Reciprocal Attachment Questionnaire is more prev-
alent in a subgroup of patients who lack an antibody marker than in Is Secure Attachment Associated With Adult
a clinically similar subgroup of UC patients with the marker. This Health?
finding is taken as tentative confirmation of a hypothesis of distinct
If insecure attachment is associated with disease it should also be
etiological paths in these two groups (40).
true that secure attachment is associated with health. Support for
SOC is a construct that describes a psychological predisposition
this association can be inferred from the 50-year prospective study
to life stress response (41). SOC describes a person’s confidence in
of a cohort of men recruited at university age (48). Adult attachment
their ability to understand and manage stimuli from their internal
was not explicitly measured at the onset of this study because the
and external environments and, thereby, cope effectively with stres-
concept had not been described in 1942. However, childhood
sors. SOC is very similar in description to the secure end of the
strengths were measured in several ways that taken together suggest
expectation of self dimension of the two-dimension, four-category
that secure attachment may be a predictor of physical health. Spe-
model of attachment. An association of low SOC and coronary heart
cifically, the cohesiveness of the home before age 20 and whether or
disease risk has been found in white-collar men, but not in blue-
not the boy’s relationships with his mother and father were condu-
collar men (42).
cive to trust, autonomy, and initiative were predictors of the man’s
health at age 50 (49). At age 60, early adult closeness to siblings
Are Childhood Risk Factors for Attachment became the more significant predictor. Maturity of defenses at age 20
to 47 also predicts physical health at 60 (48).
Insecurity Associated With Adult Disease?
The best prospective test of the influence of insecure attachment
on adult disease would be to determine attachment security in
MODEL OF INSECURE ATTACHMENT
children and follow them until adulthood. Such studies are not yet CONTRIBUTING TO DISEASE
available in humans, despite the fact that some cohorts of children
Because there is suggestive evidence from a range of studies
observed in the Strange Situation are now old enough that their
favoring an overall association of attachment insecurity and illness,
adult health status could be investigated. There are animal studies
we wish to examine the possibility that insecure attachment con-
and less direct human studies, however, which address this
tributes to disease processes. In reviewing the literature, we have
question.
developed a model describing paths by which insecure attachment
In animal studies, premature maternal separation is an experi-
could affect the course of disease. Figure 1 describes potential psy-
mental analogue of the naturally occurring parental behaviors that
chosomatic mechanisms that could account for a correlation of in-
contribute to attachment insecurity. In a strain of rats that is genet-
secure attachment and disease. The model describes how insecure
ically susceptible to stress-induced gastrointestinal ulceration, early
attachment may be associated with disturbances of stress regulation
weaning results in significantly more stress-induced ulceration after
(Path 1), use of external regulators of affect (Path 2), and nonuse of
the pups have grown to maturity (43). Inconsistent results have been
protective behaviors (Path 3). These possible paths organize the
reported in cancer-prone rats. In one study separation of rat pups
literature review that follows. The theoretical reasoning behind each
from their mothers for 20 hours per day for 18 days was associated
path is followed by an empirical review.
with longer survival time after inoculation of Erlich carcinoma (44).
In another study of the same animal model, rats that were com-
pletely weaned from their dams prematurely showed longer survival Path 1: Insecure Attachment May Affect Stress
after implantation of Erlich carcinoma if they were paired with a Regulation
littermate after weaning (45).
In humans, one useful strategy to examine this relationship is to Of the three proposed paths by which insecure attachment could
look at childhood events that are often precursors of insecure attach- affect disease, the role of stress is of particular interest from a
ment. Evidence supports the clinical observation that experiences of physiological point of view. Attachment and stress are related de-
trauma, neglect, parental loss, and separation increase a child’s risk velopmentally because, on the one hand, the stress response has
of insecure attachment (46). While attachment security is the result been selected in evolution to be triggered by environmental threat
of a complex interaction between a child and his or her caretakers while, on the other hand, the function of the attachment system is to
and environment, and not all traumatized children become inse- increase security in the face of environmental threat. We propose
curely attached, trauma can be understood as the most provocative three ways by which attachment may determine individual differ-
test of the resiliency of the infant-caretaker system. Study of the ences in the stress response.
potential precursors of attachment insecurity is important because Attachment insecurity may increase perceived stress. The inter-
events like trauma and parental death can be reported retrospec- nal working model describes the evaluative processes by which an
tively with more reliability than reports of the subjective quality of individual deals with threat by negotiating proximity to trusted
parent-child attachment. Studying the precursors of insecurity al- others. Included in this evaluation is a determination of what con-

560 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

Fig 1. Model of hypothesized mechanisms by which attachment security could contribute to disease.

stitutes a threat, which may involve both qualitative experiential tionship of attachment insecurity to the intensity of the cardiovas-
features of the environmental situation and a threshold for triggering cular and cortisol response to acute stress in children and in adults.
attachment behavior. Preoccupied attachment involves a self-per- Sroufe and Waters (55) measured changes in heart rate in children
ception of vulnerability, which may lead to a lower threshold for during the Strange Situation. They reported that all children show
activating attachment behavior. In one model of hypochondriasis heart-rate increases during separation, which remain elevated until
and somatization, preoccupied persons have acquired a sense of reunion with the parent. At reunion secure infants exhibited a return
personal vulnerability and vigilance so intense that interoceptive to baseline heart rate in less than a minute. Both resistant (preoccu-
sensation (normal perception of physiological operations) is per- pied) and avoidant children exhibited elevations of heart rate much
ceived as a potential threat (50). Avoidant attachment, on the other longer into the reunion sequence in spite of clear differences in
hand, includes an attitude of interpersonal distrust that may result behavior (with the avoidant children displaying very little distress).
in situations that require intimacy or interdependence (including a Subsequent investigations have revealed a more complicated pic-
situation of apparent “social support”) being perceived as ture. Adrenocortical stress reactivity during the Strange Situation
threatening. has been reported to occur in ambivalently attached infants but not
Mikulincer and Florian (51) have studied the relationship of in secure or avoidant infants (56). Salivary cortisol may be elevated
adult attachment to perceived threat and distress under two condi- only in those insecurely attached infants who also display behav-
tions of real-life stress. In 92 male military recruits undergoing four ioral inhibition (57).
months of combat training appraisal of the training in terms of threat In university students, a history of childhood bereavement and a
was more pronounced in ambivalent (preoccupied) persons than in self-report of disturbed family environment each exert a main effect
avoidant or secure persons. In a retrospective study of students who on the cardiovascular response to a laboratory stressor. In the same
had experienced missile attacks in the Gulf War, attachment type protocol, family disturbance also predicted an enhanced cortisol
had a significant effect on the intensity of perceived distress. All response to the stress of viewing a movie depicting the death of a
insecure subjects reported higher levels of hostility and somatiza- boy’s mother (58). Similarly, women with a history of childhood
tion than secure subjects, while ambivalently attached (preoccu- sexual abuse exhibit increase ACTH, cortisol, and heart rate re-
pied) subjects also reported more anxiety and depression distress sponses to a standardized stress compared with women without
(52). such a history (59). While these studies allow inferences to be drawn
Attachment insecurity may affect the intensity or duration of the about attachment insecurity, one study of stress response has di-
physiological stress response. The stress response is highly regu- rectly measured adult attachment. In idiopathic spasmodic torticol-
lated at multiple physiological levels. It is reasonable to assume that lis (but not in healthy controls), avoidant attachment measured by
this complexity allows that central factors (such as insecure attach- the Attachment Q-sort is associated with elevated salivary cortisol
ment) may modulate the system partially, allowing not only for response to the stress of discussing attachment relationships (39).
individual differences in the initiation or noninitiation of the stress [Attachment pattern may determine the success of social support
cascade but also for modification of its intensity or duration. in buffering stress.] Social support has been widely studied as a
In rats, an increase in maternal care elicited by separating an mediator of illness and is considered to be beneficial to a range of
infant mouse from the mother (“handling”) for a few minutes each health outcomes (60), despite controversy about the details of the
day in the first two weeks of life results in lifelong reduction in the association and its mechanism (61). Attachment theory may be re-
HPA response to stressors. If the separation is longer, mothers ignore quired for a coherent reading of the complexities of the relationship
the pups on their return and the pups suffer a lifelong increase in the of social support and health. Attachment theory, for example, pre-
reactivity of ACTH and cortisol to stress (53). Furthermore, a strong dicts that attachment relationships will provide “felt security” in the
correlation has been found between the amount of early maternal face of threat or loss whereas other (nonattachment) relationships
licking and grooming and the amount of messenger ribonucleic acid will not. Given the close developmental relationship between at-
for glucocorticoid receptors that is expressed in the hippocampal tachment style and stress response, it can be hypothesized that
regions, which are critical for negative feedback on the HPA axis relationships that provide “felt security” are also more effective in
(54). buffering stress. West (62) has used this prediction to explain why
There is also human experimental evidence to support the rela- close, intimate, and confiding relationships buffered women against

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 561


R. G. MAUNDER AND J. J. HUNTER

the psychiatric sequelae of stressful life events in Brown’s Camber- cohol consumption, alcoholism, and drug abuse (47, 75). Direct
well study, when no such affect was found with other close affiliate studies of attachment reveal that adults with avoidant attachment
relationships, even to first degree relatives (63). Furthermore, attach- drink alcohol to enhance positive affect (76). Adolescent substance
ment theory predicts that the degree of buffering provided by an users value social conformity less than nonusers (74, 77, 78), which
attachment figure would be greater for secure individuals than in- is closely related to parental support (77) and strength of emotional
secure ones and greater in preoccupied insecurity than in avoidant bonds to family and friends (74). Drug use in young adults is also
insecurity. related to parent-child attachment (79).
It is expected that attachment style will also influence the degree Eating behavior. Patterns of food consumption are thought to be
to which social contact is sought at times of stress. Secure attach- related to developmental relationships, and food consumption has
ment is thought to be associated with a greater willingness to seek physiological and affective regulatory effects. It is predicted, there-
support and a related expectation that support will be available and fore, that insecure attachment will be a risk factor for disturbances of
effective. Avoidant attachment is associated with a desire for inter- eating behavior. Insecure attachment is associated with weight con-
personal distance and self-reliance (23), whereas preoccupied at- cern and lower self-esteem in adolescents (80). Parental overprotec-
tachment may be associated with less support-seeking because of a tion is associated with oral control and anorectic behavior in ado-
fear of unavailability or rejection. lescent girls (81). Eating behavior is also associated with smoking
A longitudinal study of 60 recently widowed and 60 married and alcohol consumption (81). External regulation through food
men and women supports the prediction that attachment relation- intake has also been suggested as a mechanism responsible for
ships are special, in that the emotional loneliness resulting from the obesity. One study has provided confirmatory evidence (82), al-
loss of an attachment partner cannot be compensated by nonattach- though this has not yet been replicated with better-validated mea-
ment social support (64). With respect to stress and health variables, sures of attachment insecurity.
the ability of the proximity of an attachment figure to reduce phys- Sexual behavior. It is expected that attachment style will have a
iological stress responses has received support from both animal and strong influence on sexual behavior (83). Unfortunately, the litera-
human studies. In guinea pigs the presence of a bonded partner is ture currently available is quite modest. As with substance use,
more effective than the presence of another same species animal in social conformity and peer bonding have been associated with lower
reducing cortisol response (65). In humans, partner support results sexual activity in teenagers (81). Unsafe sexual behavior has been
in lower cortisol reactivity to a laboratory stress in men, compared associated with impaired object relations (84).
with stranger support or no support (66). Family emotional involve-
ment has a stronger association to cardiovascular health behavior
than other forms of social support (67). Support from a spouse or Path 3: Insecure Attachment May Alter Use of
family member is more effective in long-term reduction of diastolic Protective Factors
blood pressure than other social support (68).
The hypothesis that different patterns of attachment are associ- In addition to an excess of health risk behaviors, insecure attach-
ated with different patterns of using social support has also been ment may also contribute to illness through the failure or nonuse of
studied. In students, both the perception of available support and protective factors. One of the most important protective factors,
the extent to which support is sought at times of stress were greater social support, has been described above. Additionally insecure
in secure subjects than in avoidant or ambivalent (preoccupied) attachment may affect treatment adherence.
subjects (69). In 92 military recruits avoidant attachment was asso- Treatment adherence. Diabetes is an example of a disease in
ciated with less support-seeking than secure attachment (51). Secure which treatment adherence is crucial. Viederman and Hymowitz
women are more likely than avoidant women to seek emotional (85) proposed a model of diabetic control in which “the effective-
support from partners in an anxiety-provoking laboratory experi- ness of [the patient’s] engagement and participation in treatment” is
ence (70). In 81 healthy young adults dismissing and fearful attach- determined by early relationship experience. The experience of de-
ment styles were associated with much less support seeking than veloping autonomy, mastery, and reliable warm support during the
secure and preoccupied styles. Support-seeking in this study was toddler phase provides a positive body image and sense of self-
significantly mediated by the perception of available support (71). control that persists through life and allows for sensitivity to bodily
needs and for appropriate self-control at times of health crisis. In the
absence of such a sense of autonomy and flexibility, health crises
Path 2: Insecure Attachment May Result in produce defensiveness, especially denial. The mobilization of mal-
Altered Use of External Regulators of Affect adaptive defenses during a health crisis results in an inability to
autonomously monitor blood sugar and respond adaptively by ad-
The next proposed etiological pathway concerns regulation of justing insulin and diet, and the inability to benefit from the avail-
affect. Since insecure attachment results in deficits in internal affect ability of supportive resources. Recently this proposition has been
regulation (72, 73), it is expected that insecurity will be associated empirically studied using HbA1c as a measure of diabetic control in
with greater use of external regulators. A number of behavioral 15 type 1 diabetics. HbA1c is an established biological index that is
strategies that are used to regulate dysphoric affect (to soothe, to elevated in poorly controlled diabetes. Subjects with avoidant at-
distract, or to excite) are also risk factors for disease. Included among tachment had significantly higher levels of HbA1c than those with
these are smoking tobacco, drinking alcohol, using other psychoac- secure or preoccupied attachment (86). While it is presumed that
tive drugs, over-eating, under-eating, and engaging in risky sexual this worse control is due to nonadherence, it is also possible that
activity. This path focuses on regulatory behavioral strategies, some- increased glucocorticoids in avoidantly attached persons contribute
what arbitrarily distinguishing these from the relationship factors to insulin resistance.
(social support) that have been described above. Symptom reporting. Two studies directly support the link be-
Substance use. The relationship between adolescent coping, ad- tween attachment insecurity and symptom reporting. In students
olescent-parent relationship, and substance use is fairly well estab- who were making the transition from home to living at university, a
lished, although the inferences that can be drawn regarding attach- relationship of avoidant attachment and physical symptoms was
ment are generally indirect. Childhood abuse and trauma predict found, mediated by emotional self-control and emotion-focused
adolescent smoking and alcohol abuse (74) and adult smoking, al- coping (87). In another study an excess of medically unexplained

562 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

symptoms was found in fearful and preoccupied Hepatitis C patients attachment behavior. Because illness events can rep-
compared with securely attached individuals with the same disease resent loss, threat, isolation, and dependency, they are
(88).
There is also indirect support for this association. In university
precisely the types of stress that will initiate attach-
students who report a low SOC there is a positive correlation be- ment behaviors. The concept of the internal working
tween negative life events and physical symptoms (89). In addition, model helps to clarify the distinction between attach-
low SOC was found to predict sickness-absences from work in ment behaviors, which will often be elicited by illness
women, but not in men, over a four-year longitudinal study of stresses, and attachment patterns, which are more en-
municipal workers (90). Further indirect evidence comes from stud-
ies of adults who were sexually abused in childhood who have more
during. Nonetheless attitudes that indicate attachment
pain complaints, at more sites and greater health care utilization insecurity may well be easier to elicit when the attach-
than nonabused controls (9). ment system is mobilized during illness stress. Clearly
there is a need for prospective clinical studies and
controlled experimental studies to test the causal pro-
DISCUSSION
cesses that link correlated states.
This review finds supportive evidence for the hy- We propose that the deleterious health effects of
pothesis that attachment insecurity contributes to attachment insecurity are not universal. Given the
physical illness. A model is presented that describes ubiquity of stress and the high prevalence of insecure
how attachment insecurity may contribute to three attachment (35% to 40% of the general population
mechanisms that increase disease risk: altered stress (27)), attachment insecurity cannot logically be a suf-
physiology, increased use of external regulators of af- ficient condition for any moderately uncommon dis-
fect, and altered use of health-protective behaviors. ease. Furthermore, it would not make evolutionary
Thus attachment style may be a predictor of stress- sense for a highly conserved behavioral pattern (91) to
vulnerability and disease risk for a number of diseases. be a sufficient condition for major illness. It is more
A rationale for differentiating between individuals persuasive that attachment insecurity contributes to
who are highly susceptible to stress, physiologically disease by adding risk as a “second hit” on a preexist-
and behaviorally, and those whose response to stress is ing vulnerability, such as would be provided by ex-
well-regulated is crucial for etiological research. Since pression of a gene or by an infectious agent, which
heterogeneity in stress susceptibility is likely to dilute provides the disease specificity.
true stress relationships in population-based research, The evidence reviewed here provides modest sup-
this rationale should provide new research approaches port for each of the hypotheses examined. Considered
to the several diseases in which stress is thought to be together, the direct and indirect evidence that favors a
a risk factor, but for which research on this association role for attachment in disease forms a coherent group
has resulted in inconsistent results. of literature that calls for careful evaluation. The
The attachment paradigm provides a truly biopsy- model that has been described and reviewed is com-
chosocial model of disease. The model describes how plex, but each of the several hypotheses that it gener-
repeated, critical interactions occur at key develop- ates seems to be open to be operationalized and tested.
mental periods between a child and his or her care- The association of attachment insecurity (and its
taker (each of whom come to the interaction with their precursors) and the physiological stress response has
own set of more or less determined temperamental been more thoroughly studied than the association of
needs and vulnerabilities). While the need for such attachment security and the behavioral risk factors that
interactions and the basic “vocabulary” of interaction we were able to identify. As a result there is more
is determined for a species over an evolutionary time- robust evidence for the physiological impact of attach-
scale, within this domain there is much flexibility ment insecurity than for its behavioral impact cur-
allowing an individual to develop patterns of behavior rently, which may run counter to intuition. Investiga-
and physiological response which provide the best fit tion of the role of attachment insecurity in treatment
between the individual, his or her caretaker, and their nonadherence and other health risk behaviors should
environment. The consequences of this developmental be a priority.
setup are both physiological and behavioral, and help The model has significant implications. If attach-
to explain the wide range of expression of distress and ment insecurity contributes to disease course or onset,
coping behavior that is found in humans. then this may lead to treatment opportunities. One can
This review has focused on the possibility that pat- speculate that physiologic dysregulation which ac-
terns of attachment influence disease risk. It must be companies insecure attachment could lead, for exam-
recognized, however, that the relationship of attach- ple, to testing of benzodiazepines, or modulators of
ment to illness is very likely to be bidirectional. Illness corticotrophin-releasing factors to reduce disease risk
events are near-perfect triggers for the mobilization of in persons with a specific vulnerability to a particular

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 563


R. G. MAUNDER AND J. J. HUNTER

illness (for example those at genetic risk) and/or those fering predictions of a neuroticism model and an at-
of a specific attachment type. tachment model in this regard are empirically testable.
An attachment perspective might modify behavioral Finally, methodological concerns limit confidence
interventions for external regulators of affect that are in the original research reviewed in this paper. At this
also disease risk factors. Increasing treatment adher- early stage of development of this literature most of the
ence in avoidantly attached individuals, for example, studies that have been published are positive studies,
might be facilitated by strategies that enhance per- which introduces a potential bias. In addition, with
ceived personal control and respect interpersonal dis- the exception of one large general practice cohort (47),
tance, whereas interventions to reduce excessive help- two prospective studies (36, 48), and the experiments
seeking in individuals with preoccupied attachment (18, 19, 58, 65), these studies report on relatively small
would take a very different approach. samples and are cross-sectional in design. Thus no
Several caveats must be emphasized. One is that the conclusions can be drawn about the validity of the
association of attachment insecurity with major de- model.
pression (92) has been given insufficient attention in In an effort to promote rigorous testing of a series of
the literature reviewed here. This association is impor- hypotheses that are important to psychosomatic med-
tant to the study of physical disease for a number of icine, the methodology of this review has been inclu-
reasons. First, depression is often associated with a sive and synthetic. It is hoped that this exercise in
worse course of physical illness (2, 3). Second, depres- hypothesis generation will encourage the development
sion may, in some cases, be a direct consequence of of a literature robust enough to allow for review that is
severe attachment disruption. Third, there is an impor- exclusive and analytic in the future. In particular, pro-
tant overlap in the physiological features of attach- spective and longitudinal research is required to con-
ment insecurity and depression. Depression, chronic trol for bidirectional influences between (context-de-
stress, and insecure attachment share an association to pendent) attachment behavior and illness events and
HPA dysregulation (54, 58, 93) in which it is concep- to determine if attachment types which are construed
tually unclear which entity is primary. Thus, the bio- to be traits are, in fact, present as predisposing factors
psychosocial concepts of depression and insecure at- before disease onset.
tachment cannot be cleanly separated. At a minimum,
the inter-relationship needs to be addressed by mea-
REFERENCES
suring and controlling for depressive symptoms in
studies of attachment insecurity and disease. 1. Broadhead WE, Kaplan BH, James SA, Wagner EH, Schoenbach
VJ, Grimson R, et al. The epidemiologic evidence for a relation-
It is also worth noting the differences between at-
ship between social support and health. Am J Epidemiol 1983;
tachment and two other psychological constructs that 117:521–37.
have been invoked to explain the relationship between 2. Wulsin LR, Vaillant GE, Wells VE. A systematic review of the
distressing experience and physical symptoms. SOC is mortality of depression. Psychosom Med 1999;61:6 –17.
descriptively similar to the secure end of the self- 3. Watson M, Haviland JS, Greer S, Davidson J, Bliss JM. Influence
of psychological response on survival in breast cancer: a popu-
appraisal dimension of attachment in the four-category
lation-based cohort study. Lancet 1999;354:1331– 6.
model. Although this observation has not been directly 4. Chrousos GP, Gold PW. The concepts of stress and stress system
tested, it is possible that there is considerable overlap disorders. Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis.
between these constructs. It remains problematic that Jama 1992;267:1244 –52.
SOC captures only half of the attachment construct. 5. Drossman DA, Talley NJ, Leserman J, Olden KW, Barreiro MA.
Sexual and physical abuse and gastrointestinal illness. Review
The “appraisal of other” dimension appears not to
and recommendations. Ann Intern Med 1995;123:782–94.
overlap with SOC. This is important because the ap- 6. McBeth J, Macfarlane GJ, Benjamin S, Morris S, Silman AJ. The
praisal of other dimension is crucial to understanding association between tender points, psychological distress, and
the avoidant stance which appears to be important in adverse childhood experiences: a community-based study. Ar-
treatment adherence (86), in symptom reporting (87), thritis Rheum 2000;42:1397– 404.
7. Walker EA, Keegan D, Gardner G, Sullivan M, Bernstein D,
and in the ability to benefit from the buffering poten-
Katon WJ. Psychosocial factors in fibromyalgia compared with
tial of social supports. Neuroticism has also been pos- rheumatoid arthritis: II. Sexual, physical, and emotional abuse
tulated to mediate between the experience of child- and neglect. Psychosom Med 1997;59:572–7.
hood abuse and adult disease (94). Neuroticism, which 8. Golding JM. Sexual assault history and headache: five general
describes a perceived inability to process or cope with population studies. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999;187:624 –9.
9. Fillingim RB, Wilkinson CS, Powel T. Self-reported abuse his-
intense affect, is not identical with insecure attach-
tory and pain complaints among young adults. Clin J Pain 1999;
ment. The neuroticism construct does not distinguish 15:85–91.
between attachment and nonattachment relationships 10. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol 1: Attachment. New York:
in their ability to provoke and buffer stress. The dif- Basic Books; 1969.

564 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

11. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol 2: Separation: anxiety and ment security: a path model. Early Education & Development
anger. New York: Basic Books; 1973. 1999;10:517–34.
12. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol 3: Loss: sadness and depres- 32. Ward MJ, Kessler DB, Altman SC. Infant-mother attachment in
sion. New York: Basic Books; 1980. children with failure to thrive. Infant Mental Health Journal
13. Ainsworth M, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of 1993;14:208 –20.
attachment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Hill- 33. Simmons RJ, Goldberg S, Washington J, Fischer-Fay A. Infant-
sdale (NJ): Erlbaum; 1978. mother attachment and nutrition in children with cystic fibro-
14. Kandel ER. Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: a new sis. J Dev behav Pediatr 1995;16:183– 6.
intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. Am J Psychiatry 34. Marvin RS, Pianta RC. Mother’s reaction to their child’s
1999;156:505–24. diagnosis: relations with security of attachment. J Clin Child
15. Belsky J, Rosenberger K, Crnic K. The origins of attachment Psychol 1996;25:436 – 45.
security: “Classical” and contextual determinants. In: Goldberg 35. Mrazek DA, Casey B, Anderson I. Insecure attachment in se-
S, Muir R, Kerr J, editors. Attachment theory: social develop- verely asthmatic preschool children: is it a risk factor? J Am
mental, and clinical perspectives. Hillsdale (NJ): The Analytic Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1987;26:516 –20.
Press; 1995. 36. Goldberg S, Simmons RJ, Newman J, Campbell K, Fowler RS.
16. Main M. Avoidance in the service of attachment: A working Congenital heart disease, parental stress and infant-mother re-
paper. In: Immelman K, Barlow G, Main M, Petrinovitch L, lationships. J Pediatr 1991;119:661– 6.
editors. Behavioral Development: The Bielefeld Interdiscipli- 37. Crowell JA, Fraley RC, Shaver PR. Measurement of individual
nary Project. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1981; differences in adolescent and adult attachment. In: Cassidy J,
651–93. Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment. New York:
17. Bowlby J. Making and breaking of affectional bonds: 1. aetiology Guilford; 1999. p. 434 – 65.
and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. Brit 38. Dozier M, Stovall KC, Albus KE. Attachment and psychopathol-
J Psychiatry 1977;130:201–10. ogy in adulthood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of
18. Kraemer GW. A psychobiological theory of attachment. Behav Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New
Brain Sci 1992;15:493–511. York: Guilford Press, 1999;497–519.
39. Scheidt CE, Waller E, Malchow H, Ehlert U, Becker-Stoll F,
19. Hofer MA. Hidden regulators: implications for a new under-
Schulte-Moenting J, Lucking CH. Attachment representation
standing of attachment, separation and loss. In: Goldberg S,
and cortisol response to the Adult Attachment Interview in
Muir R, Kerr J, editors. Attachment theory: social developmen-
idiopathic spasmodic torticollis. Psychother Psychosom 2000;
tal, and clinical perspectives. Hillsdale (NJ): The Analytic Press,
69:155– 62.
1995.
40. Maunder RG, Lancee WJ, Greenberg GR, Hunter JJ, Fernandes
20. Floeter MK, Greenough WT. Cerebellar plasticity: modification
BJ. Insecure attachment in a subgroup with ulcerative colitis
of Purkinje cell structure by differential rearing in monkeys.
defined by ANCA status. Dig Dis Sci 2000; 45:2127–32.
Science 1979;206:227–9.
41. Antonovsky H, Sagy S. The development of a sense of coherence
21. Struble RG, Riesen AH. Changes in cortical dendritic branching
and its impact on responses to stress situations. J Soc Psychol
subsequent to partial social isolation in stumptailed monkeys.
1986;126:213–25.
Dev Psychobiol 1978;11:479 – 86.
42. Poppius E, Tenkanen L, Kalimo R, Heinsalmi P. The sense of
22. Schore AN. Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self. Hills-
coherence, occupation and the risk of coronary heart disease in
dale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 1994.
the Helsinki Heart Study. Soc Sci Med 1999;49:109 –20.
23. West ML, Sheldon-Kellor AE. Patterns of Relating: An Adult 43. Ackerman SH, Hofer MA, Weiner H. Age at maternal separation
Attachment Perspective. New York: Guilford Press 1994. and gastric erosion susceptibility in the rat. Psychosom Med
24. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of 1975;37:180 – 4.
adult attachment: An integrative overview. In: Simpson JA, 44. LaBarba RC, White JL. Maternal deprivation and the response to
Rholes WS, editors. Attachment theory and close relationships. Erlich carcinoma in BALB/c mice. Psychosom Med 1971;33:
New York: Guilford; 1998. 458 – 61.
25. Bartholomew K, Horowitz LM. Attachment styles among young 45. LaBarba RC, Lazar JM, White JL. The effects of maternal separa-
adults: a test of a four-category model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1991; tion, isolation and sex on the response to Erlich carcinoma in
61:226 – 44. BALB/c mice. Psychosom Med 1972;34:557–9.
26. Klohnen EC, John OP. Working models of attachment: A theory- 46. Lyons-Ruth K, Jacobvitz D. Attachment disorganization: Unre-
based prototype approach. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, editors. solved loss, relational violence, and lapses in behavioural and
Attachment theory and close relationships. New York: Guilford; attentional strategies. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Hand-
1998. book of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications.
27. Mickelson KD, Kessler RC, Shaver PR. Adult attachment in a New York: Guildford; 1999;520 –54.
nationally representative sample. J Pers Soc Psychology 1997; 47. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM,
73:1092–106. Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse
28. Frodi A, Thompson R. Infants’ affective responses in the Strange and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of
Situation: effects of prematurity and quality of attachment. deaths in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACE)
Child Dev 1985;56:1280 –90. study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14:245–58.
29. Goldberg S. Risk factors in infant-mother attachment. Canadian 48. Vaillant GE, Vaillant CO. Natural history of male psychological
Journal of Psychology 1988;42:173– 88. health, XII: a 45-year study of predictors of successful aging at
30. Speltz ML, Endriga MC, Fisher PA, Mason CA. Early predictors age 65. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147(1):31–7.
of attachment in infants with cleft lip and palate. Child Dev 49. Vaillant GE. Natural history of male psychological health, II:
1997;68:12–25. some antecedents of healthy adult adjustment. Arch Gen Psy-
31. McCallum MS, McKim MK. Recurrent otitis media and attach- chiatry 1974;31:15–22.

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 565


R. G. MAUNDER AND J. J. HUNTER

50. Stuart S, Noyes R. Attachment and interpersonal communica- role of attachment styles. J Pers Soc Psychology 1992;62:
tion in somatization. Psychosomatics 1999;40:34 – 43. 434 – 46.
51. Mikulincer M, Florian V. Appraisal of and coping with a real- 71. Ognibene TC, Collins NL. Adult attachment styles, perceived
life stressful situation: the contribution of attachment styles. social support, and coping strategies. J Soc Pers Relations 1998;
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 1995;21:406 –14. 15:323– 45.
52. Mikulincer M, Florian V, Weller A. Attachment styles, coping 72. Mikulincer M. Adult attachment style and affect regulation:
strategies, and posttraumatic psychological distress: the impact strategic variation in self-appraisals. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;75:
of the Gulf War in Israel. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993;64:817–26. 420 –35.
53. Levine S, Haltmeyer GC, Kaas GG, Penenberg VH. Physiological 73. Kobak R, Sceery A. Attachment in late adolescence: working
and behavioral effects of infant stimulation. Physiol Behav 1967; models, affect regulation, and representation of self and others.
2:55– 63. Child Dev 1999;59:135– 46.
54. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, 74. DeFronzo J, Pawlak R. Effects of social bonds and childhood
Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, experience on alcohol abuse and smoking. J Soc Psychol 1993;
hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and the hypothalamic- 133:635– 42.
pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 1997;277: 75. Springs FE, Friedrich WN. Health risk behaviors and medical
1659 – 62. sequalae of childhood sexual abuse. Mayo Clin Proc 1992;67:
55. Sroufe LA, Waters E. Heart rate as a convergent measure in 603– 4.
clinical and developmental research. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 76. Magai C. Affect, imagery and attachment. In: Cassidy J, Shaver
1977;23:3–27. PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory research and clin-
56. Spangler G, Schieche M. Emotional and adrenocortical re- ical applications. New York: Guilford Press; 1999.
sponses of infants to the strange situation: the differential func- 77. Wills TA, Cleary SD. How are social support effects mediated? A
tion of emotional expression. Int J Behav Dev 1998;22:681–706. test with parental support and adlescent substance use. J Pers
57. Nachmias M, Gunnar MR, Mangelsdorf S, Parritz RH, Buss K. Soc Psychol 1996;71:937–52.
Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: the moderating role 78. McBride CM, Curry SJ, Cheadle A, Anderman C, Wagner EH.
of attachment security. Child Dev 1996;67:508 –22. School-level application of a social bonding model to adolescent
58. Luecken LJ. Childhood attachment and loss experiences affect risk-taking behaviour. J Sch Health 1995;65:63– 8.
adult cardiovascular and cortisol function. Psychosom Med 79. Brook JS, Whiteman M, Finch S, Cohen P. Mutual attachment,
1998;60:765–72. personality, and drug use: pathways from childhood to young
59. Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, Graham YP, Wilcox M, Bonsall R, adulthood. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs
Miller AH, Nemeroff CB. Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic re- 1998;124:492–510.
sponses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in 80. Sharpe TM, Killen JD, Bryson SW, Shisslak CM, Estes LS, Gray
childhood. Jama 2000;284:592–7. N, Crago M, Taylor CB. Attachment style and weight concerns in
60. House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and preadolescent and adolescent girls. Int J Eat Disord 1998;23:
health. Science 1988;241:540 –5. 39 – 44.
61. Uchino BN, Cacioppo JT, Kielcolt-Glaser JK. The relationship 81. Lavik NJ, Clausen SE, Pedersen W. Eating behaviour, drug use,
between social support and physiological processes: a review psychopathology, and parental bonding in Norway. Acta Psy-
with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for chiatr Scand 1991;84:387–90.
health. Psychol Bull 1996;119:488 –531. 82. Raynes E, Auerbach C, Botyanski NC. Level of object represen-
62. West M, Livesley WJ, Reiffer L, Sheldon A. The place of attach- tation and psychic structure deficit in obese persons. Psychol
ment in the life events model of stress and illness. Can J Psy- Rep 1989;64:291– 4.
chiatry 1986;31:202–7. 83. Feeney JA, Raphael B. Adult attachments and sexuality: impli-
63. Brown GW, Bhrolchain MN, Harris T. Social class and psychi- cations for understanding risk behaviours for HIV infection.
atric disturbance among women in an urban population. Soci- Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1992;26:399 – 407.
ology 1975;9:225–54. 84. Bassman LE. Object relations and self-reported AIDS self-care
64. Stroebe W, Stroebe M, Abakoumkin G. The role of loneliness behaviour. Psychol Rep 1991;68:915–23.
and social support in adjustment to loss: A test of attachment 85. Viederman M, Hymowitz P. A developmental-psychodynamic
versus stress theory. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;70:1241–9. model for diabetic control. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1988;10:34 – 40.
65. Sachser N, Matthias D, Hirzel D. Social relationships and the 86. Ciechanowski P, Katon W, Hirsch I. Attachment style and ad-
management of stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998;23: herence in the diabetic patient [abstract]. Psychosom Med 1999;
891–904. 61:110.
66. Kirschbaum C, Klauer T, Filipp SH, Hellhammer DH. Sex- 87. Kotler T, Buzwell S, Romeo Y, Bowland J. Avoidant attachment
specific effects of social support on cortisol and subjective re- as a risk factor for health. Br J Med Psychol 1994;67:237– 45.
sponses to acute stress. Psychosom Med 1995;57:23–31. 88. Ciechanowski P, Dwight M, Katon W, Rivera-Ball D. Attachment
67. Franks P, Campbell TL, Shields CG. Social relationships and classification associated with unexplained medical symptoms
health: the relative roles of family functioning and social sup- in patients with chronic hepatitis C. Proceedings of the Second
port. Soc Sci Med 1992;34:779 – 88. International Attachment and Psychopathology Conference;
68. Levine DM, Green LW, Deeds SG, Chwalow J, Russell RP, Finlay 1998 Toronto, Canada.
J. Health education for hypertensive patients. Jama 1979;241: 89. Jorgensen RS, Frankowski JL, Carey MP. Sense of coherence,
1700 –3. negative life events and appraisal of physical health among
69. Florian V, Mikulincer M. Effects of adult attachment style on the university students. Personality and Individual Differences
perception and search for social support. J Psychol 1995;129: 1999;27:1079 – 89.
665–76. 90. Kivimaki M, Feldt T, Vahtera J, Nurmi JE. Sense of coherence
70. Simpson JA, Rholes WS, Nelligan JS. Support seeking and sup- and health: evidence from two cross-lagged longitudinal sam-
port gicing within couples in an anxiety provoking situation: the ples. Soc Sci Med 2000;50:583–97.

566 Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001)


ATTACHMENT AND DISEASE

91. Simpson JA. Attachment theory in modern evolutionary per- 93. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The impact of early adverse experi-
spective. In: Cassidy J, editor. Hanbook of Attachment: Theory ences on brain systems involved in the pathophysiology of
Research and Clinical Applications. New York: Guilford, 1999; anxiety and affective disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2000;46:
115– 61. 1509 –22.
92. West M, Rose MS, Verhoef MJ, Spreng S, Bobey M. Anxious 94. Talley NJ, Boyce PM, Jones M. Is the association between irri-
attachment and self-reported depressive symptomatology in table bowel syndrome and abuse explained by neuroticism? A
women. Can J Psychiatry 1998;43:294 –7. population-based study. Gut 1999;42:47–53.

ANNOUNCEMENT

Minority Research Training in Psychiatry


The Program for Minority Research Training in Psychiatry (PMRTP) is funded by the National Institute of
Mental Health. Through it, the American Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education sponsors training
of minority medical students, Psychiatric residents, and fellows who are interested in research by providing
advice, placement assistance, tuition, stipends, travel and other expenses. The director of the Program is
James Thompson, M.D., M.P.H.; the project manager is Ernesto Guerra. For more information call the toll
free number for the PMRTP, 1-800-852-1390, or 202-682-6225, email eguerra@psych.org, or write to PMRTP
at the American Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education, 1400 K Street, NW, Washington, DC
20005.

Psychosomatic Medicine 63:556 –567 (2001) 567

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi