Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 4

Pediatric Diabetes 2004: 5: 59—62 Copyright # Blackwell Munksgaard 2004

Printed in Denmark. All rights reserved


Pediatric Diabetes

Editorial

Does moderately severe hypoglycemia cause


cognitive dysfunction in children?

Corresponding author:
Christopher M. Ryan, PhD
Department of Psychiatry
University of Pittsburgh School of
Medicine
3811 O’Hara Street
Pittsburgh
PA 15213
USA
Tel: þ1-412-246-5392
e-mail: ryancm@upmc.edu

Multiple case reports have demonstrated that clinically neurophysiological event-related potential (P300)
significant brain damage may result following an studies (6) have also failed to demonstrate brain dysfunc-
extended period of profound hypoglycemia — generally tion in adults who had one or more hypoglycemic comas,
characterized as blood glucose values below whereas magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have
1.5—1.0 mmol/L for several hours. Neuroimaging and been equivocal, with some (7), but not all (8) showing
neuropathological studies show extensive neuronal loss cortical atrophy following recurrent hypoglycemia.
bilaterally in the cerebral cortex, particularly in mesial Longitudinal neuropsychological studies have similarly
temporal regions, as well as in the hippocampus, basal failed to find robust evidence of cognitive deterioration
ganglia, and brain stem (1). Although neurocognitive in older adolescents or adults following moderately
assessments have been conducted only infrequently in severe recurrent hypoglycemia (9), and learning and
these cases, the extant data indicate that learning and memory skills are almost invariably reported to be
memory skills are most likely to be compromised, as normal (10).
one would expect given the well-established association Is it possible that children are more susceptible to the
between hippocampal/mesial temporal lobe damage neurocognitive sequelae of hypoglycemia than adults?
and memory dysfunction (2). Researchers have now identified at least one subgroup
Most individuals with diabetes never experience an that is particularly vulnerable: children with an early
episode of profound hypoglycemia, but a large propor- onset of diabetes. Studies conducted over the past
tion of diabetic children and adults will experience one 20 yrs have repeatedly demonstrated that children and
or more moderate to severe hypoglycemic events (i.e., adolescents diagnosed before 5 or 6 yrs of age have a
blood glucose values between 2.2 and 3.3 mmol/L) greatly elevated risk of experiencing severe hypoglyce-
(3). Remarkably, however, there is little compelling mia and of manifesting significant brain dysfunction, as
evidence — particularly from studies conducted within evidenced by abnormalities on electroencephalograms
the past 10 yrs — that this degree of hypoglycemia leads (EEGs) (11) and by poor performance on a variety of
to significant brain dysfunction. For example, serum cognitive tasks, including measures of learning and
markers of acute neuronal damage (neuron-specific memory (12). On the other hand, children who are
enolase and protein S-100) remain within normal limits first diagnosed during elementary or high school and
following a single episode of moderate to severe hypo- who subsequently experience an episode of moderate to
glycemia in adults; in contrast, elevations of these severe hypoglycemia do not ordinarily manifest neuro-
markers, consistent with brain cell death, are seen in cognitive sequelae (13, 14). Why is an early onset of
patients who experienced an episode of profound hypo- diabetes such a potent predictor of hypoglycemia-
glycemia accompanied by permanent neurological signs induced cognitive dysfunction? It is now widely
or death (4). Positron emission tomography (5) and believed that the first 5 yrs of life constitute an
59
Ryan

especially critical period for brain development (15), information in the classroom and in the workplace,
and the occurrence of either hypoglycemia-associated one would also predict that children experiencing severe
neuroglycopenia and/or chronic hyperglycemia during and/or recurrent hypoglycemia to be at an academic
that critical period may induce structural and or vocational disadvantage. Again, there is little
functional brain abnormalities that interfere with evidence to support that prediction. We know that
normal cognitive function (16). As a corollary, when over time, diabetic children do tend to manifest a relative
diabetes onset occurs after that critical period, hypogly- deterioration in performance on measures of verbal
cemic events are unlikely to have any significant impact intelligence or academic achievement, but that decline
on brain function unless the blood glucose levels fall seems more related to poor metabolic control and/or
below 1.5 mmol and persist for an extended period of duration of diabetes (14, 20); there is no compelling
time. evidence that recurrent hypoglycemia per se is associated
In this issue, Hershey and her associates provide with cognitive decline.
some very intriguing data suggesting that the view In their report, Hershey and her associates have
outlined above may be incorrect. During a 15-month focused on the occurrence of one or more episodes of
observation period, children who experienced at least intercurrent hypoglycemia, and argued that it is the
one episode of severe hypoglycemia subsequently hypoglycemic event that is likely to be responsible for
performed less well on a spatial working memory task the very subtle performance differences seen on meas-
regardless of their age at diagnosis, as compared to ures of spatial memory. But another interpretation
their diabetic peers without intercurrent hypoglycemia. comes to mind: perhaps cognitive decline is largely
Although multiple learning and memory tests were secondary to chronic hyperglycemia. A growing body
administered, the only task adversely affected by a of literature on adults with type 2 diabetes has shown
hypoglycemic event was an unfamiliar, cognitively clear evidence of cognitive deterioration in the
demanding, non-verbal task known to tap mnemonic absence of severe hypoglycemia (21), and both cross-
skills that may be largely ‘hard-wired’ and dependent sectional and longitudinal studies of adults with
on intact hippocampal systems. juvenile-onset type 1 diabetes have also reported
Hershey’s group has now used this task with several cognitive differences (8) or decrements over time
different groups of children and adults and has repeat- (22) that are related to poor metabolic control and/or
edly demonstrated a link between conditions thought to the development of complications, rather than
to affect the hippocampus (e.g. temporal lobe epilepsy to recurrent hypoglycemia. Recent studies of children
and severe hypoglycemia) and spatial working memory with diabetes have also highlighted the possible adverse
(17, 18). Thus, there is little doubt about the reprodu- effects of chronic hyperglycemia on cognitive function
cibility of their observation. What remain unknown, (14, 23).
however, are the implications (if any) of this observa- Indeed, it is noteworthy that as a group, the children
tion for the child with diabetes. After all, the magnitude studied by Hershey et al. who had an episode of severe
of this effect is quite small. For example, children who hypoglycemia during follow-up also had nearly twice as
experienced an episode of hypoglycemia did not show a many episodes of severe hyperglycemia (6.5 vs. 3.4)
worsening of performance over time. Rather, they prior to baseline testing than those who experienced
showed no change, as compared to diabetic children no hypoglycemic episodes. While neither group differed
who did not experience hypoglycemia in the interim in hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) levels at either the baseline
and subsequently improved their accuracy on the task. visit or at the follow-up visit, during the 15-month
Moreover, performance decrements on this task were observational period, those subjects who experienced
not associated with declines on any other cognitive severe hypoglycemia also had higher HbA1c levels
tasks, raising questions about the validity of spatial (8.6% vs. 8.2%). Consistent with the link between
working memory as a meaningful measure of cognitive chronic hyperglycemia and cognitive dysfunction are
function in children. the between-group differences in performance at
If, on the other hand, the spatial memory task is baseline on several measures, including the magnitude
conceptualized as a marker of hippocampal integrity — of the error made by the severe hypoglycemia group on
consistent with both the animal literature and some of the spatial memory task (38.0 vs. 31.2 mm) and their
Hershey’s earlier work (18), and if a single episode of longer response times on the Grooved Pegboard test
hypoglycemia can affect this brain system, then one (92.1 vs. 80.6 s).
would expect recurrent episodes to produce extensive Spatial working memory tasks, similar to the task
and permanent hippocampal damage with resultant used by Hershey et al. have also been found to be
profound memory problems. Yet, there is little evidence sensitive to chronic hyperglycemia — at least in rats
to support that prediction: clinically significant memory (24). Maze learning, which necessarily relies on spatial
disorders are infrequent in diabetic children or adults memory, is also disrupted in diabetic animals, with the
with recurrent hypoglycemia (19). Because learning magnitude of the effect associated with the duration of
and memory skills are fundamental to acquiring new diabetes. Importantly, animals with diabetes of longer
60 Pediatric Diabetes 2004: 5: 59—62
Editorial

duration also showed a significant decline in neuronal References


densities in the hippocampus, and these changes could
1. AUER RN, HUGH J, COSGROVE E, CURRY B. Neuropatho-
not be attributed to insulin-induced hypoglycemia (25). logic findings in three cases of profound hypoglycemia.
Other animal research has demonstrated that during Clin Neuropathol 1989: 8: 63—68.
the performance of a spatial maze, there is a 32% 2. MARKOWITSCH HJ. Neuroanatomy of memory. In:
decline in extracellular glucose levels in the hippo- TULVING E, CRAIK FIM, eds. The Oxford Handbook of
Memory. New York: Oxford University Press 2000,
campus, suggesting that certain types of cognitive 465—484.
tasks — particularly those with a spatial component — 3. DAVIS EA, KEATING B, BYRNE GC et al. Impact of
can deplete hippocampal glucose (26). Diabetic individ- improved glycaemic control on rates of hypoglycaemia
uals in poor metabolic control may thus have more in insulin dependent diabetes mellitus. Arch Dis Child
1998: 78: 111—115.
difficulty performing those types of cognitive tasks, 4. STRACHAN MWJ, ABRAHA HD, SHERWOOD RA et al.
particularly if chronic hyperglycemia triggers a down- Evaluation of serum markers of neuronal damage
regulation of glucose transporters at the level of the following severe hypoglycaemia in adults with insulin-
blood—brain barrier (27), thereby reducing the avail- treated diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Metab Res Rev 1999:
ability of glucose to hippocampal neurons. Healthy 15: 5—12.
5. CHABRIAT H, SACHON C, LEVASSEUR M et al. Brain metabol-
non-diabetic adults with reduced glucose tolerance ism after recurrent insulin-induced hypoglycemic episodes:
also show an inverse relationship between glucose levels a PET study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994: 57:
during a 2-h intravenous glucose tolerance test, and 1360—1365.
both the size of the hippocampus — as measured by 6. KRAMER L, FASCHING P, MADL C et al. Previous
episodes of hypoglycemic coma are not associated
MRI, and performance on verbal memory tests (28). with permanent cognitive brain dysfunction in IDDM
Taken together, these studies argue for a link between patients on intensive insulin treatment. Diabetes 1998:
elevated glucose levels and poor cognition which may 47: 1909—1914.
be particularly evident on tests sensitive to hippo- 7. PERROS P, DEARY IJ, SELLAR RJ et al. Brain abnormal-
ities demonstrated by magnetic resonance imaging in
campal function. adult IDDM patients with and without a history of
Does a single episode of moderately severe hypogly- recurrent severe hypoglycemia. Diabetes Care 1997: 20:
cemia disrupt brain function in children with diabetes? 1013—1018.
The excellent study of Hershey and her colleagues 8. FERGUSON SC, BLANE A, PERROS P et al. Cognitive
ability and brain structure in type 1 diabetes: relation
makes it clear that those children who experienced to microangiopathy and preceding severe hypoglycemia.
severe hypoglycemia are more likely to perform some- Diabetes 2003: 52: 149—156.
what more poorly on spatial memory tasks, but as 9. DIABETES CONTROL and COMPLICATIONS TRIAL RESEARCH
should be evident from these comments, the patho- GROUP. Effects of intensive diabetes therapy on neuro-
psychological function in adults in the Diabetes Control
physiological mechanism is by no means self-evident, and Complications Trial. Ann Intern Med 1996: 124:
nor are the clinical implications. The ‘confound’ that 379—388.
investigators need to be mindful of is that individuals 10. RYAN CM, WILLIAMS TM. Effects of insulin-dependent
who experience severe hypoglycemia are also more diabetes on learning and memory efficiency in adults.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1993: 15: 685—700.
likely to have extended periods of chronic hyperglyce- 11. BJøRGAAS M, SAND T, GIMSE R. Quantitative EEG in
mia, as indexed by poor metabolic control, and this type 1 diabetic children with and without episodes of
variable is also known to be associated with neuro- severe hypoglycemia: a controlled, blind study. Acta
cognitive dysfunction. An additional issue that is Neurol Scand 1996: 93: 398—402.
12. RYAN C, VEGA A, DRASH A. Cognitive deficits in
sometimes, but not invariably, taken into account adolescents who developed diabetes early in life.
by researchers is the fact that severe hypoglycemia Pediatrics 1985: 75: 921—927.
is also more common in individuals with an early onset 13. WYSOCKI T, HARRIS MA, MAURAS N et al. Absence of
of diabetes, who may as a consequence have diabetes- adverse effects of severe hypoglycemia on cogitive func-
tion in school-aged children with diabetes over 18
associated abnormalities in brain development. months. Diabetes Care 2003: 26: 1100—1105.
Until all of these issues are sorted out and we are in a 14. SCHOENLE EJ, SCHOENLE D, MOLINARI L, LARGO RH.
position to make accurate attributions of causality, Impaired intellectual development in children with
patients should be counseled to prevent the occurrence type 1 diabetes. association with HbA1c, age at
of hypoglycemia but at the same time maintain their diagnosis, and sex. Diabetologia 2002: 45: 108—114.
15. CAVINESS VS, KENNEDY DN, BATES JF, Makris N.
blood glucose levels as close to the normal range as The developing human brain: a morphometric profile.
possible. Individualized therapy, with frequent blood In: THATCHER RW, LYON GR, RUMSEY J, KRASNEGOR N,
glucose monitoring, remains the only way to reduce eds. Developmental Neuroimaging: Mapping the
the likelihood that children (or adults) with diabetes Development of Brain and Behavior. San Diego:
Academic Press 1997, 3—14.
will develop cognitive complications. 16. RYAN CM. Effects of diabetes mellitus on neuro-
psychological functioning: a lifespan perspective. Semin
Christopher M. Ryan Clin Neuropsychiatry 1997: 2: 4—14.
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh 17. HERSHEY T, BHARGAVA N, SADLER M et al. Conventional
School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, USA vs. intensive diabetes therapy in children with type 1

Pediatric Diabetes 2004: 5: 59—62 61


Ryan

diabetes: effects on memory and motor speed. Diabetes diabetes 6 years after disease onset. Diabetes Care 2001:
Care 1999: 22: 1318—1324. 24: 1541—1546.
18. HERSHEY T, CRAFT S, GLAUSER TA, HALE S. Short-term 24. POPOVIç M, BIESSELS G-J, ISAACSON RL, GISPEN WH.
and long-term memory in early temporal lobe dysfunc- Learning and memory in streptozotocin-induced
tion. Neuropsychology 1998: 12: 52—64. diabetic rats in a novel spatial/object discrimination
19. RYAN CM. Memory and metabolic control in children. task. Behav Brain Res 2001: 122: 201—207.
Diabetes Care 1999: 22: 1242—1244. 25. LI Z-G, ZHANG W, GRUNBERGER G, SIMA AAF.
20. MCCARTHY AM, LINDGREN S, MENGELING MA et al. Hippocampal neuronal apoptosis in type 1 diabetes.
Factors associated with academic achievement in Brain Res 2002: 946: 221—231.
children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2003: 26: 26. MCNAY EC, FRIES TM, GOLD PE. Decreases in
112—117. rat extracellular hippocampal glucose concentration
21. STRACHAN MWJ, DEARY IJ, EWING FME, FRIER BM. Is associated with cognitive demand during a spatial task.
type 2 (non-insulin dependent) diabetes mellitus asso- Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000: 97: 2881—2885.
ciated with an increased risk of cognitive dysfunction? 27. MCCALL AL, MILLINGTON WR, WURTMAN RJ.
Diabetes Care 1997: 20: 438—445. Metabolic fuel and amino acid transport into the brain
22. RYAN CM, GECKLE MO, ORCHARD TJ. Cognitive effi- in experimental diabetes mellitus. Proc Natl Acad Sci
ciency declines over time in adults with type 1 diabetes: USA 1982: 79: 5406—5410.
effects of micro- and macrovascular complications. 28. CONVIT A, WOLF OT, TARSHISH C, DE LEON MJ. Reduced
Diabetologia 2003: 46: 940—948. glucose tolerance is associated with poor memory per-
23. NORTHAM EA, ANDERSON PJ, JACOBS R et al. Neuro- formance and hippocampal atrophy among normal
psychological profiles of children with type 1 elderly. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003: 100: 2019—2022.

62 Pediatric Diabetes 2004: 5: 59—62

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi