Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 6

Cognition in Viewing and Talking about Art

Author(s): Judith Smith Koroscik


Source: Theory into Practice, Vol. 23, No. 4, Teaching the Arts (Autumn, 1984), pp. 330-334
Published by: Taylor & Francis, Ltd.
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1476389 .
Accessed: 23/06/2014 00:35

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Taylor & Francis, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Theory into
Practice.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JudithSmith Koroscik

Cognition in Viewing and Talking About Art

From the time the arts were introduced into the terest in enhancing the quality of students' under-
schools, it was assumed their contributionto ed- standing of art through activities involving critical
ucation was found in their ability to enhance af- discourse about art. This article explores these
fective growth. The arts were considered to be issues by highlightingfindings from several studies
subjects where feelings and emotions reign su- (Koroscik,1982, 1983; Koroscik& Blinn, 1983; Ko-
preme. Intellectualwork was reserved for subjects roscik, Desmond, & Brandon, in press; Koroscik &
like mathematics and science. Cognition and art Osman, 1984; Koroscik & Wiley, 1984).
were seen as falling at distant ends of a curriculum
that contrasted intellectual activities with artistic Levels of Processing Information
endeavors. This view was further reinforced when The study of cognition has produced theoretical
Krathwohl,Bloom, and Masia (1964) divided their models of human informationprocessing. According
taxonomies of educational objectives into separate to one model that has received much attention, the
realms known as the cognitive, affective, and psy-
chomotor domains. Recent research, however, calls processing of informationis a continuumcomprised
of a number of successive levels or stages (Craik
this view into question. & Lockhart, 1972). The first stage of processing
Regardless of the field from which knowledge involves the analysis of sensory or structural fea-
originates or the form in which it is presented, its tures of a stimulus (e.g., lines, angles, pitch, or
acquisition cannot occur without the perception, brightness); the second involves the application of
transformation,interpretation,retention, and appli- previously acquired knowledge in the interpretation
cation of information(Reynolds & Flagg, 1983). The of meanings that characterize semantic dimensions
delineation of these cognitive processes has be- of stimuli (e.g., representational features, symbol-
come the focus of much educational research in ism, etc.).
recent years. Art educators have pursued the study Memories are formed as the byproducts of
of cognition as it functions in the making of and
processing. Their persistence is presumed to be "a
responding to works of art. function of depth of analysis, with deeper levels of
One facet of this research has dealt with the analysis associated with more elaborate, longer
effect of verbalization on the art-viewing process. lasting, and stronger [memory] traces" (p. 675).
Relationships were investigated among structural, Informationprocessed from a single stimulus may
semantic, and verbal statements made about works be encoded into memory in terms of its structural,
of art. This research has come about because a semantic, or phonemic (verbal)characteristics. The
number of art teachers have shown increased in- durabilityof such stored informationcorresponds
Judith Smith Koroscik is assistant professor of art edu- to the depth to which it has been processed. This
cation at The Ohio State University. in turn is affected by such variables as the amount

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
of time available for encoding, the nature of task 1. This turbulent composition includes the repre-
demands, the characteristics of the stimulus itself, sentation of storm clouds, rain, a body of water,
and the compatibilityof the stimulus with what the a sailing vessel, and numerous animals.
viewer has already stored in memory. 2. The scene depicted in this artworkcorresponds
to the Biblical account of Noah's Ark and the
Levels of Processing Art Great Flood.
3. This artwork reflects a spiritualisticconcern for
Craikand Lockhart'slevels of processing model mankind'suniversalapostasy and corruptionand
provides a useful framework for research that fo- their devastating consequences.
cuses on the complexities associated with encoding
structural and semantic features of art. Each as- The difficulties often associated with deriving
sumes a different level of cognitive activity that meaning from art are evidenced in the frustration
varies with the type of structural and semantic experienced by many viewers who, without the aid
information processed by viewers. These differ- of contextual informationor priorknowledge of art,
ences are worth noting in some detail. gaze upon the structuralfeatures of highlyabstract
works only to ask: "What is it?" or "What does it
Structural InformationProcessing mean?" Althoughpainted in 1843, the subject mat-
ter represented in the Turner painting described
Structuralfeatures of art are those that phys- above is characteristicallyhigh in abstraction. Cas-
ically reside in an art object, and as such they are ual viewers often mistake it for a more contem-
readily available for encoding to any viewer with porary painting and are seldom successful in
normal vision. Levels of processing structural in- detecting its representation of animals let alone its
formationinclude(a) encoding an artwork'sphysical spiritual overtones. When subsequently informed
properties (e.g., its size, surface qualities, and me- that the work depicts Noah's Arkin the Great Flood,
dia),(b) discerningvisual elements that appear within representational features and expressive qualities
the work (e.g., colors, shapes, lines, and values), are readily discerned. This points up an important
and (c) observing relationshipsas they occur among research problem, i.e., how does verbal information
those elements. An example of such levels of struc- affect the formation of structuraland semantic en-
tural processing as they correspond to Joseph codings of art?
Turner's painting, Shade and Darkness: The Eve-
ning of the Deluge, are represented in the following Verbalization Effects on Processing Art
statements:
1. The artist applied oils to canvas when he pro- Theoretical debates persist when explanations
duced this artwork which measures approxi- are offered on how verbalizationaffects the proc-
mately 30 x 30 inches. essing of pictorial stimuli. Two models, the dual-
2. A large curving mass of browns and blacks encoding model and the sensory-semantic model,
borders the upper edge of this artwork. have been most useful in predictingthe role verbal
3. Values of brown arch over a large white mass mediation plays in structural and semantic proc-
in the upper half of this circularcomposition. essing. According to the dual-encoding model (Pai-
vio, 1971, 1978), the verbalization of a picture's
features increases the probabilitythat two codes
Semantic InformationProcessing are activated in the formationof stimulus memories.
In contrast, the processing of meaning de- The argument is that sensory features of pictures
mands that visual information present in an art are stored in imagery codes, while the products of
object be interpretedas "standing for" what is not verbalization are retained as verbal or linguistic
immediately present (Bartlett, 1932). The interpre- codes. The redundancyof stored informationin dual
tation of meaning can include (a) identifying rep- codes increases the likelihoodthat a picture will be
resentational features and expressive qualities, (b) remembered. Simply stated, two codes are better
recognizing instances of symbolic denotation, and than one.
(c) determining underlying religious, social, eco- The effect of verbalizationon the encoding of
nomic, philosophical, or political principles exem- pictorial stimuli is characterized somewhat differ-
plified by a work. These processing characteristics ently by the sensory-semantic model (Nelson, Reed,
are illustrated in the following statements about & McEvoy, 1977). In this view, pictures and their
Turner's painting: verbaldescriptions are assumed to evoke distinctive
VolumeXXIII,Number4 331

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
sensory representations in memory but access a Results of the experiments indicatedthe nature
common semantic system. Presumably,the analysis of processing task demands affected the amount
of a picture's semantic features is requisite to the of informationsubjects retained after viewing art-
formation of verbal descriptions or the derivation works of varyinglevels of abstraction. Verbaltasks
of meaning from those contextually presented. Ver- instructing students to consider an artwork in its
balizing about stimulus features ensures that se- entirety by generating a title that characterized the
mantic processing of the picture occurs, which work resulted in greater retention of structuraland
thereby increases the likelihood that semantic in- semantic features than all other verbal and non-
formationis maintainedas memories of the stimulus verbalprocessing tasks. Such findingssupportthose
(Warren & Horn, 1982). These assumptions are of an earlier investigation (Koroscik, 1982) and fol-
consistent with Craik and Lockhart's (1972) argu- low theoretical predictions that naming pictures en-
ment that deeper or more semantic levels of proc- sures semantic processing and thus promotes
essing result in the generation of more elaborate greater retention of stimulus information(Craik &
and longer lasting memory traces. Lockhart, 1972; Nelson et al., 1977).
Onlyrecently has empiricalevidence been gath- Interestingly, the findings also suggested the
ered to determine whether these predictions ac- occurrence of implicit verbalization or some form
of semantic processing under task conditions that
curately characterize the cognitive processes
associated with viewing art. In one study (Koroscik, did not demand semantic analysis for verbal re-
1982), verbal task demands directed viewing re- sponses to stimuli. This was indicated by low re-
tention scores for structuralinformationthat resulted
sponses to the six levels of structuraland semantic
when students viewed highly abstract works of art.
processing noted earlier. It was hypothesized that Structural encodings were apparently difficult to
the encoding and retention of visual art would be
retain when high levels of abstraction inhibitedtheir
significantlylower under task directives to verbalize association with representationalfeatures. The rel-
structuralfeatures than under task instructions re-
ative ease in translating subject matter of less ab-
questing the verbalization of semantic
stract works promotedgreater retention,presumably
characteristics.
Results verified this general hypothesis; how- by the concurrentprocessing of meaning and struc-
tural attributes.
ever, they also indicatedsome structuralprocessing This explanation follows an assumption of the
tasks produced retention rates that were unex-
sensory-semantic model of encoding that visual,
pectedly matched by those generated in response meaning, and phonemic features of pictorialstimuli
to semantic task demands. It was speculated that need not be fully integrated into complete memory
such findings may have resulted as a function of
representations before the processing of succeed-
testing procedures that allowed structural infor- ing features is initiated. "Instead, encoding is pre-
mation to be processed semantically in the ver- sumed to be a dynamic, continuous process
balizationof those structuralfeatures. Exclusive use
occurring through time. At any one point in time
of verbal processing tasks and verbal questions on different types of features may be in the state of
the retention measure precludedfurtherspeculation
being processed simultaneously" (Nelson et al.,
of verbalization effects from these findings. 1977, p. 487). This and other evidence lends sup-
Subsequent investigations (Koroscik & Blinn, port to the conclusion that verbal processing was
1983; Koroscik&Osman, 1984) examined structural largely responsible for improved retention of both
and semantic informationprocessing by direct com- structural and semantic features of art.
parison of viewing and testing responses that de- A later study (Koroscik, Desmond, & Brandon,
manded or did not demand verbalization.Processing in press) further examined the relationship among
tasks used in this research and in a later experiment structural, semantic, and verbal encodings of art
(Koroscik & Wiley, 1984) directed viewers to focus by displaying works of varied levels of abstraction
on a specified area of an artwork or to examine a with verbal contextual information. It was specu-
work in its entirety on either verbal or nonverbal lated that the encoding and retention of art is sub-
terms. Assessment of encoding and retention spec- ject to the type of contextual informationgiven to
ificity could thus be obtained by analyzing differ- viewers at presentation. Verbal labels with literal
ences in retention scores for information from references to the objects, persons, or events de-
incidental and intentional areas of artworks in the picted in an artwork ought to evoke semantic en-
restricted viewing conditions. codings that differfromthose generated in response
332 Theory Into Practice

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
to verbal references pertaining to the work's ex- 9. If meanings are to be processed from highly
pressive qualities and/or other nonliteralaspects of abstract art, will verbal or visual contextual
the depicted content. Accordingly,it was presumed information be provided to facilitate their
that persons with prior knowledge of art carry par- interpretation?
allel informationto art viewing experiences in the 10. What willensure that students comprehendthe
form of previously stored cognitions. Contextual correspondence between contextual informa-
informationmay function merely as redundant in- tion and the structural and semantic features
formation for such viewers, or it may provide re- of art?
trieval cues that access pertinent memories. A test The research reported in this article provides
of these assumptions was conducted by manipu- only a small sample of existing evidence that chal-
lating verbal context conditions for groups of art lenges a noncognitive view of art. A more tenable
and non-art college students. view is one which assumes that all realms where
Results of the study were consistent with pre- learningoccurs, includinglearningin the arts, have
vious findings. They indicated that the retention of their cognitive, affective, and psychomotor aspects.
structural informationwas linked to the retention To assume that the objectives pursued in the ac-
of its meanings. The presentation of verbalcontexts quisition of a given subject are all of one type is
ensured that meaning was processed to some ex- to misconstrue the insights beginningto emerge in
tent, thus facilitating the retention of semantic in- the psychology of cognition.
formation which in turn promoted the retention of
structural features. Results also indicated that ac- References
curate interpretationof meaning was a function of
the level of abstraction that characterized each Bartlett,F.C. (1932). Remembering.London:Cambridge
UniversityPress.
artwork and of the type of contextual information Craik,F.I.M.,& Lockhart,R.S. (1972).Levelsof process-
given at input. Even more sophisticated viewers ing: A frameworkfor memoryresearch.Journalof
with priorknowledge of art benefited from the pres- VerbalLearningand VerbalBehavior, 11, 671-684.
entation of verbal contextual information. Koroscik,J.S. (1982). The effects of priorknowledge,
presentationtime, and task demandson visualart
processing. Studies in Art Education, 23 (3), 13-22.
Educational Implications Koroscik, J.S. (1983). Structuralinformationretention in
These findings offer direction for curriculum visual art processing. (ERICDocument Reproduction
ServiceNo. ED 231 699)
development through the identificationof variables Koroscik,J.S., & Blinn,L.M.(1983). The effect of ver-
that affect the acquisition of knowledge in art view- balizationon visual art processingand retention.
ing contexts. Questions derived from such research Studies in Art Education, 25 (1), 23-31.
ask practitioners to consider the following when J.S., Desmond,K.K.,&Brandon,S.M.(inpress).
Koroscik,
in proc-
The effect of verbalcontextualinformation
designing art learningexperiences for their students: essing visual art. Studies in Art Education.
1. What is the nature of artworks selected for Koroscik,J.S., & Osman,A. (1984).[The effect of ver-
presentation to students? balizationon visualart processing:A culturalcom-
2. What level of abstraction characterizes the parisonof Americanand Egyptianviewers].College
of the Arts ResearchGrantProject,The OhioState
works to be shown? University.Unpublished raw data.
3. How much time will students be given to ex- Koroscik,J.S., & Wiley,K. (1984).[Theeffect of verbal-
amine each work of art? izationon structural-semantic processingof visual
4. Are the artworks to be presented similar or art]. Spencer FoundationResearchGrantProject,
The OhioState University.Unpublished rawdata.
much different in structure and meaning from Krathwohl,D., Bloom,B., & Masia,B. (1964). The tax-
those previously examined by students? onomy of educational objectives. New York: David
5. What requisite knowledge of art and of the McKayCo.
world is assumed in the study of particular Nelson,D.L.,Reed,V.S., &McEvoy,C.L.(1977).Learning
works? to orderpicturesandwords:A modelof sensoryand
semantic encoding. Journal of Experimental Psy-
6. What is the nature of tasks students are to chology: Human Learning and Memory, 3 (5), 485-
perform when viewing the artworks presented 497.
to them? Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New
7. Willtasks direct students to an artwork'sstruc- York:Holt,Rinehart& Winston.
tural features or meanings? Paivio,A. (1978).Mentalcomparisonsinvolvingabstract
attributes. Memoryand Cognition, 6, 199-208.
8. Will tasks demand verbal or nonverbal re- Reynolds,A.G., & Flagg, P.W. (1983). Cognitivepsy-
sponses from students? chology.Boston:Little,Brown,and Company.
VolumeXXIII,Number4 333

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Warren, L.R., & Horn, J.W. (1982). What does naming a ognition of identical and same-name alternative.
picture do? Effects of prior picture naming on rec- Memory and Cognition, 10 (2), 167-175.

tip

334 Theory Into Practice

This content downloaded from 91.229.248.134 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 00:35:15 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi